Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
…
6 pages
1 file
Néstor Taboada Terán, El precio del estaño.
Néstor Taboada Terán, El precio del estaño.
Animals
In this study, we report a particular begging strategy by Andean dogs and by humans on the unsurfaced road between the villages of Parotani and Cahiuasi in Bolivia. The positions of the dogs and humans begging and their behavioral displays were recorded. In dogs, the distance from each other was also recorded. Begging locations, for both dogs and humans, were permuted with a score, according to the higher probability of receiving food. The highest scores were assigned to the positions where cars had to slow down and the subjects were well visible, thus meriting a score corresponding to the higher probability of receiving a treat. The occurrences of the correct positioning at the external and internal parts of the bend were compared by a chi-square test. On a range of 93.3 km, the dogs were observed to always lie down at the border of the road, mainly alone (96.3%) and on hairpin bends, present due to the steep slope, obliging the vehicles to slow down. Humans were observed mainly in...
Acta veterinaria, 2011
Feeding time of stray dogs on the first, second, third, fifth and seventh day of captivity in a municipal shelter was studied. The average value of the feeding time of dogs decreased from the first day (429.07±107.08 seconds) to the third day in captivity when it reached the lowest average value (229.53±95.18 seconds). The feeding time of dogs on the third day in captivity differs significantly from the same parameter on the first (429.07±107.08 seconds) and second day (372.73±100.58 seconds) in captivity (p<0.01). The highest average value of feeding time of stray dogs in the shelter was recorded on the first day. On the fifth and seventh day the feeding time of dogs increased compared to the third day. However, there were no significant differences of the average value of feeding time on the third day comparing with the fifth day (260.8±111.06 seconds) or the seventh day (301.33±119.89 seconds). Dogs ate their food more slowly on the fifth and the seventh day than on the third day, but more quickly than on the first and the second day (372.73±100.58 seconds). Significant differences were estimated comparing the feeding time of dogs on the first day with the third (p<0.01), fifth (p<0.01), and seventh day (p<0.05) of housing in the municipal shelter. Also, the feeding time of dogs on the second day differed significantly from the third (p<0.01) and the fifth day (p<0.05).
Cornell University - arXiv, 2022
Foraging and acquiring of food is a delicate balance between managing the costs (both energy and social) and individual preferences. Previous research on the solitary foraging of free-ranging dogs showed that they prioritized the nutritionally highest valued food patch first but don't ignore other less valuable food either, displaying typical scavenger behaviour. The current experiment was carried out on groups of dogs with the same setup to see the change in foraging strategies, if any, under the influence of social cost like intra-group competition. We found multiple differences between the strategies of dogs foraging alone versus in groups with competition playing an implicit role in the dogs' decision making when foraging in groups. Dogs were able to continually assess and evaluate the available resources in a patch and adjust their behaviour accordingly. Foraging in groups also provided benefits of reduced individual vigilance. The various decisions and choices made seemed to have a basis in the optimal foraging theory wherein the dogs harvested the nutritionally richest patch possible with the least risk and cost involved but was willing to compromise if that was not possible. This underscores the cognitive, quick decision-making abilities and adaptable behaviour of these dogs. Significance Statement The purpose of this study is to explore an aspect of ecoethology of free-ranging dogs, that of their scavenging behaviour. Despite their ubiquitous presence on Indian streets, not much is known about these dogs. This study is the first of its kind to explore in detail the foraging strategies of these dogs under different social conditions. We showed experimentally by simulating real world conditions that these dogs are quick thinkers and behaviourally flexible in their strategies displaying qualities of an efficient scavengers. We also found that these strategies show evidence of an optimal foraging model.
Journal of Critical Reviews, 2020
Various parts of the etho-ecology of the lost dog population in India were examined. Densities of somewhere in the range of 127 and 1304 lost dogs km−2 cover with thickness gauges announced for different populations. A male:female proportion of 2:1 is likewise in concurrence with prior investigations. Conduct perceptions uncovered that these dogs will sometimes frame groups with strength chains of command and shared guard of a territory. From the dependability of these groups, long haul subsidiary bonds obviously exist among bunch individuals. This findings conflicts with the acknowledged thought that urban stray dogs are asocial and don't frame stable social groups. I. INTRODUCTION Dogs occupy an exceptionally extraordinary situation in human social orders due to their unwaveringness as well as due to their diverse abilities in serving human creatures in different limits they are one of the most unique animals in their reaction to social prizes. In India the stray dogs are inescapable in both urban and rural environments. Their population is expanding at a disturbing rate in India and in most southeast Asian nations, India has an expected dog population of 80 million, more than 80 % of which are stray dogs. These free extending animals are likewise called untouchable dogs and live in relationship with human residences. Significantly, the canine species has advanced more than a huge number of years. For example, it currently flourishes with a diet wealthy in starch that is near the human diet, permitting closeness with humans. Perceptions of the day to day environments of dogs and how they sort out themselves regarding humans have prompted the ID of the accompanying dog categories restricted dogs, which are absolutely needy upon individuals: all their basic needs are deliberately met by humans, who likewise limit their development family dogs, which have proprietors on whom they depend, in spite of the fact that they might be allowed to meander. Their propagation is managed by humans stray dogs, which incorporate dogs living in a human-overwhelmed setting. This is a heterogeneous gathering: it incorporates dogs that despite everything have a social security with humans, potentially deserted or conceived in human settings, and which will in general partner food with specific family units, in any event, when the property holders guarantee that the dogs don't have a place with them (Type I) and dogs that display shifting degrees of dread/resilience toward humans (Type II). These dogs are pulled in to human settings by the accessibility of food and shelter, whether or not these assets are deliberately given by humans or just coolly connected with them. They have nearness to humans, and ask from them as opposed to scavenging. These two sorts of stray dogs, with two potential degrees of relationship toward humans, are once in a while alluded to as town dogs. They are found meandering in a significant number of the huge urban areas of the Mediterranean bowl (Istanbul, Alexandria); feral dogs, which incorporate all dogs living in a wild and free state with no immediate food or shelter deliberately provided by humans, demonstrating no proof of socialization with but instead evasion of humans and occupying essentially indigenous habitats. Dogs may likewise change categories throughout their lives. Social alteration, in view of learning, is one of the most particular intellectual highlights of the species, as psychological ethology routinely illustrates. For example, it has been demonstrated that when dogs are relinquished, they sort out themselves into packs, displaying the more friendly practices of the species. This backings the idea that dogs can show different types of sociality in a similar existence, with conspecifics, humans and most likely different species also. The presence of free-meandering/stray dogs has pulled in the consideration of ethologists working in both rural settings. The conduct of these dogs and their good ways from humans rely to a great extent upon the idea of human exercises in the distinctive urban territories, and likely likewise on human societies and practices, as their thickness shifts from city to city. These variables may influence the closeness or in any case of intra-and
Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2009
Pet Behaviour Science, 2019
The effective and quick assessment of food preference is important when attempting to identify foods that might function as effective reinforcers in dogs. In the current experiment, more highly preferred foods were expected to be associated with faster approaches in a subsequent runway task. Eight dogs experienced combinations of two of six types of raw food in a paired preference assessment. These included the dog’s staple diet, to identify a rank order of preference for the foods. A different raw food was offered as the staple in two preference tests. In the runway task, the dogs were required to walk five metres to obtain a small amount of their most preferred, least preferred or staple foods and latency of approach to the foods was recorded. The results showed that the staple foods were not preferred as highly as the other foods and that each dog displayed unique and stable preferences for the different foods. The approach latencies were faster for their most-preferred food compared to their least preferred and the staple foods. The use of a runway to assess reinforcer effectiveness combined an effortful behaviour to obtain food while also requiring the dogs to make a choice, thus precluding the need for more complicated and time-consuming methods of preference assessment. The application of this method for fast and effective identification of preferred reinforcers is currently being investigating further to inform pet owners and behavioural scientists better about simple methods that they might use to identify highly preferred foods for use as reinforcers in training and behavioural testing.
I know over the next two days we are going to be talking about important problems and issues, but often in very abstract ways. I want to take this time to put a more personal and human face on global food issues, by talking about my experience working over the last 35 years in one tiny part of the planet, which is at the same time completely unique and typical in facing the dilemmas of the 21 st century. Its always hard to talk at this point in dinner, especially if you have an unappetizing message. But if I do take some of your appetite away at the beginning, I hope to restore it in time for desert. Belize is an ex British colony, perching on the Caribbean edge of the Central American mainland. After the indigenous Mayan people were conquered by the Spanish, the area was settled by Buccaneers and pirates, the first of many immigrant groups, who give it today a population of less than 350,000. Founded mainly as a logging colony, Belize was from the very beginning a country which imported much of its food, depending on high value exports to pay the bill. When I started working in Southern Belize in 1976 it was isolated and out of the way, populated mostly by Qeqchi Maya people living a very self-sufficient life in small villages scattered through the rainforest. I had the privilege of spending a year living in one community, where I learned how their complex subsistence system worked, how they grew 52 different food crops in an annual cycle, hunting and gathering which was the quintessence of 'sustainability' even though we didn't know it because the word had not been invented yet. Over the years since then I have watched this way of life has slowly collapse, as Qeqchi people have gradually learned to become consumers, switching to cash crops like rice, cacao and even marijuana to get
A study with the use of dance movement therapy as a counseling approach in a chemical dependency setting was presented. The objective of the study was to find out if the use of dance movement therapy in male chemical dependency groups reduces overall anxiety.
This paper will briefly survey the monetary approach to poverty measurement - a set of techniques and methodologies, adopted mostly by economists, based on the identification of poverty with a shortfall in a monetary indicator and the "objective" derivation of a poverty line. In order to describe these methodologies, the underlying rationale and the factors which have contributed to the current shape of this "approach", we will start by discussing the origins of such an approach. This will allow us to substantiate our claim about the peculiar and measurement driven nature of this approach to the definition of poverty, as well as critically to assess the extent to which the "objectivity" of the methods makes the results immune from value judgements. Further, it will allow us to identify some crucial features of this approach, which will be discussed with respect to current developments in the literature. A particular emphasis will be placed on identifyin...
IAFOR Journal of Education, 2015
DergiPark (Istanbul University), 2022
Journal of Russian Law, 2020
Sustainable Agriculture Research, 2012
Caspian Journal of Internal Medicine, 2013
Itinerario, 2024
Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 1988
Nutrition reviews, 2009
Technology and Health Care, 2023
Journal of Lightwave Technology, 1997