Chapter 5
1
Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical
System
2
3
Although uncommon, when the perfect storm of intractable conflict hits, it can
undermine the security and well-being of families, communities, and societies
everywhere. When viewed through the lens of dynamical systems, both the low
frequency and highly destructive consequences of this perplexing phenomenon
become understandable. In general terms, conflict intractability develops when
social, psychological, and structural elements interact over time to promote the
emergence of a stable and coherent pattern of thought and behavior organized
around perceived incompatibilities. These patterns function as attractors in that
they “attract” the mental and behavioral dynamics of the parties to the conflict. As
the conflict becomes a primary focus of each party’s thoughts, feelings, and actions,
a wide variety of events and forces—even those that seem irrelevant to the
conflict—are framed in a way that intensifies or maintains the conflict. Metaphorically, the conflict acts like a gravity well into which the surrounding mental,
behavioral, and social-structural landscape begins to slide. Once trapped in such a
well, escape requires tremendous energy and commitment and thus becomes
difficult, if not seemingly impossible. Such dynamics are today apparent in Israel/
Palestine, the Colombia/FARC conflict, and even in the current low-intensity
conflict dynamics on the island of Cyprus.
Our aim in this chapter is to unpack this general idea in terms of the principles
and concepts introduced in the preceding chapters. We begin by describing social
relations in terms of attractor dynamics, and using this perspective to illustrate how
intractable conflict represents a special form of social dynamics in which basic
processes line up in an unlikely but potentially destructive manner. Not all conflicts
become intractable, so we discuss the dynamical processes that distinguish this
potentially destructive form of social relations. The focus in particular is on the
shifting balance between reinforcing and inhibiting feedback loops (relationships
between variables that amplify their original states or inhibit it) in the progression
from benign to malignant social relations.
With this perspective in hand, we describe three lines of research designed to
assess its validity and relevance to conflict in real-world contexts. First, we present
empirical studies designed to illustrate the emergence and expression of conflict
R.R. Vallacher et al., Attracted to Conflict: Dynamic Foundations of Destructive Social
Relations, Peace Psychology Book Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-35280-5_5,
# Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013
103
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
104
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
52
intractability in interpersonal contexts when moral values provide the focus of
interaction. We then discuss how the dynamics of intractable conflict can transform
the social relationships in an entire society, and present preliminary evidence from
field research in support of this scenario of spreading pervasiveness. The third line
of research also focuses on the spread of conflict in social systems, but it is based on
a mathematical model that is implemented in computer simulations. This model,
which exemplifies the approach of dynamical minimalism, is based on a few
assumptions introduced by Morton Deutsch (1973) in his Crude Law of Social
Relations.
The final section discusses the distinction between open and closed systems, and
relates this distinction to the dynamics associated with intractable conflict. We
argue that whether a social system characterized by the potential for conflict can
escape the pull of intractability depends on the degree to which the system is
impervious to outside influences and new circumstances (closed) or instead is
capable of incorporating and adapting to influences and changing circumstances
(open). We conclude the chapter on an optimistic note by suggesting that the secret
to transforming intractable conflicts can be found within the very framework—
dynamical systems theory—that gives rise to intractability.
53
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
Most social conflicts are temporary and tend to be resolved without devolving into
violence and destruction. Indeed, as emphasized from the outset, conflict is largely
a constructive process that promotes compromise and innovation at all levels of
social reality, from friendship formation and marriage to representative democracy
and international relations. Still, a small proportion of conflicts—about 5–7 %—
tend to become protracted and appear impervious to resolution (Coleman, 2011).
Like constructive conflicts, conflicts characterized by intractability can be observed
at all levels of social reality—from dysfunctional marriages to political polarization
and interstate warfare.
An intractable conflict seems paradoxical with respect to its dynamics. On the
one hand, it is characterized by considerable dynamism, with important events
unfolding on various timescales. On the other hand, an intractable conflict is
characterized by long-term stability, with the parties to the relationship seemingly
frozen in their mutual feelings and action orientations. The simultaneous expression
of forces promoting intense dynamism and equally intense resistance to change
suggests that intractable conflict can be investigated through the lens of nonlinear
dynamical systems. In particular, the defining features of conflict can be recast in
terms of key concepts such as self-organization, emergence, hysteresis, nonlinear
transitions, and attractor dynamics.
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
105
Attractor Dynamics in Intractable Conflict
73
Once initiated, intractable conflicts seem to have a life of their own. Even if the
specific issues that generated the conflict are resolved, the animosity between the
parties to the conflict may persist or even intensify over time. A host of new issues
may arise and fade in salience, but the negative perceptions, beliefs, feelings, and
actions defining the relationship between the parties show little if any change. It is
hard to understand such intransience in terms of the usual motivational
underpinnings of social life. A conflict with no end in sight hardly serves the
interests of the parties to the conflicts: to the contrary, it drains their respective
resources, wastes energy, and diminishes human capital. Faced with such futility
and dysfunction, a compromise that partially addresses the needs and interests of
the parties should be embraced once they realize that such a compromise represents
a far better deal than does maintaining a self-defeating pattern of behavior with no
end in sight.
The persistence of persistent negative mental and behavioral patterns can be
couched in terms of attractor dynamics. An attractor, as noted in preceding
chapters, is a restricted range of states toward which a dynamical system evolves
over time, and to which the system returns after it has been perturbed. Attempts to
move the system out of its attractor promotes forces that reinstate the system at its
attractor. Viewed through this lens, intractable conflict represents the emergence of
strong, stable attractors reflecting stable patterns of thought, feeling, and action on
the part of group members. Such an attractor, in effect, “attracts” a wide variety of
mental and behavioral states to a narrow set of negative and destructive states.
74
The Attractor Landscape
96
The attractor concept and the relevance of attractors for conflict can be represented
in a simple metaphor, depicted in Fig. 5.1. The valley represents the system’s
attractor and the ball represents the current state of the system. From its current
position, the ball will roll down the hill and come to rest at the bottom of the valley.
The system portrayed in Fig. 5.1 has two attractors (A and B), each with its own
basin of attraction, which is represented by the width of each valley. Because the
basin of attraction for Attractor A is wider than the basin of attraction for Attractor
B, a wider range of states will evolve toward Attractor A than toward Attractor B.
The two attractors also vary in their strength, which is depicted as the respective
depth of each valley. Note that B is stronger than A—that is, it has a deeper valley.
In terms of the gravity metaphor, a system captured by attractror B is more difficult
for it to be dislodged by external influence than is a system captured by attractor A.
These basis properties of attractors have clear relevance for the intractability of
conflict. The width of the basin of attraction represents the range of ideas and
actions that are likely to converge on the predominant mental and behavioral
pattern characterizing the system. For a negative attractor with a wide basin of
97
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
106
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Fig. 5.1 A dynamical system
with two attractors (A and B)
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
attraction, even positive information that contradicts the negative view of another
person or group is likely to be transformed by a variety of cognitive mechanisms in
line with the predominant view (cf. Nowak, Vallacher, Tesser, & Borkowski,
2000). Thus, a peaceful overture or gesture by an opposing party may become
reframed so that it provides evidence in support of, rather than in contradiction to,
the predominant negative response tendency of the other party.
The depth of an attractor, meanwhile, represents how difficult it is for the system
to escape the pull of malignant thoughts and behaviors. When a system is at a deep
attractor for negative relations (as in Attractor B), any attempt to resolve the conflict
corresponds to pushing the ball uphill. This not only requires considerable effort,
but it is also likely to be futile—once the pushing force is relaxed, the ball will roll
back to the attractor. This scenario is illustrated by the experience of Poland during
the transition from communism to democracy in the late 1980s. The Polish police
attempted to suppress people’s expression of political opinions, economic needs,
and general dissatisfaction, but their efforts proved to be ineffective. Each police
action, in fact, only served to fuel opposition and the will to resist. In effect, these
actions were tantamount to pushing the ball up the hill. Because the attractor for
anti-government sentiment was very strong, the police actions proved to be counterproductive, as they tended to activate forces contrary to the police efforts.
The gravity metaphor, however, suggests that efforts to move a system out of its
attractor can prove successful if sufficient force is applied and if another attractor is
available. Thus, the system depicted in Fig. 5.1 can escape Attractor B and settle in
Attractor A if enough force is employed to move the ball up the hill and into the
valley representing Attractor A’s basin of attraction. This simple has important
implications for change in a system characterized by intractable conflict. In particular, if a system has more than one attractor, the mental, affective, and behavioral
states associated with the system tend to sort themselves categorically, with each
category corresponding to a different attractor. If change occurs it such a system, it
is likely to occur in an abrupt, nonlinear, qualitative manner rather than in a slow,
linear, and incremental fashion (cf. Latané & Nowak, 1994).
This nonlinear scenario of change may be asymmetric, however, with many
mechanisms aligned to maintain a protracted conflict. Once the conflict reaches a
certain threshold, then, the attractor of destructive conflict is likely to be considerably stronger than any other attractor for the system. A considerably stronger force
may therefore be required to escape the conflict than was required to drag the
parties into the conflict. Moreover, even if a conflict appears to be resolved, the
strong attractor of conflict continues to represent a latent danger that can recapture
the mental, affective, and behavioral dynamics of the parties involved, creating the
potential for a rapid reinstatement of the conflict. The rapid regression to a negative
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
107
attractor was evident, for example, in the resumption of hostilities in the Middle
East after the collapse of the Oslo accords in the 1990s.
152
Manifest and Latent Attractors
154
Because a system spends most of its time in the vicinity of its current attractor, the
nature of this attractor can be inferred from observation of the system’s pattern of
behavior. The system’s behavior, however, may not reveal the existence of other
attractors for the system’s dynamics that are currently invisible. These latent
attractors, which specify an alternative range of possible behaviors for the system,
become visible and define the intrinsic tendencies of the system when the system
falls within their respective basins of attraction. Hence, the creation and destruction
of latent attractors can prove critical in shaping how the system will respond to
conditions and events that have yet to take place.
The existence of a latent attractor can set the stage for dramatic and unexpected
changes in social relations mired in conflict. Genocides provide an extreme example of the power of latent attractors. They are typically initiated in a very rapid
fashion, but they are invariably preceded by a period of changes in inter-group
attitudes and behaviors, often involving the humiliation of group members, negative stereotyping, and a denial of rights. These changes are not necessarily dramatic
by themselves, so opposition to them may be fairly minimal and contained.
Nonetheless, each change may create and then deepen a latent attractor that can
subsequently determine the fate of inter-group relations. Once an attractor for
conflict has been created, even relatively small provocations can move the system
into the basin of this attractor, with the entire system dragged into full-blown
destructive conflict. The assassination of Archduke Ferdinand in Sarajevo in
1914, for example, probably would not have provoked World War I if a latent
attractor for intense conflict had not been created by a configuration of international
interests and a series of prior events.
One can also envision the converse scenario. Thus, a series of positive acts by
one group toward another group may be rejected at face value and have little or no
immediate effect on the relations between the groups. But such acts may set the
stage for a sudden escape from a negative attractor by creating a positive latent
attractor to which the system can move. Consider, for example, the Orange revolution in Ukraine, during which the relations between Poland and Ukraine changed
over the course of a few weeks from fairly negative to very positive. Although the
observable change was rapid, it was made possible by the positive interactions and
exchanges between various groups of people that took place many years before the
Orange revolution. Another example is provided by the rapid repair in relations
between the U.S. and the USSR after decades of the Cold War. This transition from
negative to fairly positive relations was likely fostered by a series of low-profile
citizen exchanges, such as the Dartmouth conferences that occurred in the years
leading up to Perestroika (Saunders, 1997).
155
153
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
108
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
214
The potential for latent attractors has important implications for the fate of social
relations characterized by long-term and seemingly intractable conflict (e.g.,
Coleman, Bui-Wrzosinska, Vallacher, & Nowak, 2006; Coleman, Vallacher,
Nowak, & Bui-Wrzosinska, 2007; Nowak, Vallacher, Bui-Wrzosinska, &
Coleman, 2007; Vallacher, Coleman, Nowak, & Bui-Wrzosinska, 2010). Factors
such as objectification, dehumanization, and stereotyping of outgroup members are
clearly preconditions for the development of intractable conflict (Deutsch, 1973),
but they not have an immediate impact on intergroup relations. However, they may
gradually create a latent attractor to which the system can abruptly switch in
response to a relatively minor provocation. Conversely, attempts to resolve conflict
may be ineffective in the short run but nonetheless create a latent attractor for
positive intergroup relations, thereby establishing a potential relationship to which
the groups can switch if other conditions permit. Even between groups with a long
history of conflict, a latent positive attractor can promote a rapid de-escalation of
conflict.
In sum, noteworthy and relatively sudden changes in a social system correspond
to changes in the attractor landscape of the system. Although a change within a
basin of attraction is likely to be short-lived and resisted by the system, a change in
the salience of a system’s attractors can reconfigure the ensemble of likely states
that can be adopted and thus alters the forces operating within the system. Changes
in a system’s attractor landscape can promote changes in behavior that are become
self-sustaining and self-enhancing over time.
215
Levels of Social Reality and Attractor Dynamics
216
Because attractors represent formal properties of dynamical systems, they characterize the dynamics of systems at all levels of personal and social reality (cf. Nowak
& Vallacher, 1998; Vallacher & Nowak, 2007). So although cognitive processes
and inter-group relations are very different phenomena, for example, both can be
described in terms of stable patterns (e.g., interpretation in cognition, social distance in inter-group relations) that provide coherence and stable platforms for
action. This suggests that conflict can be characterized with attractors at different
levels of social reality. Attractors at the level of the mind represent stable patterns of
thought and affect concerning the parties to the conflict (e.g., oneself and another
person in interpersonal conflict, the ingroup and outgroup in inter-group conflict).
Attractors at the interpersonal and inter-group levels, meanwhile, represent stable
patterns of relations between the parties to the conflict.
There is feedback among levels in a dynamical system, so an attractor that
initially develops at one level of reality is likely to forge attractors at other levels.
Two groups may come into conflict over issues of resource allocation, for example,
and the resultant negative interactions could promote psychological attractors (i.e.,
negative attitudes and feelings) that are consistent with, and serve to maintain, the
interpersonal conflict. Indeed, once a psychological attractor develops, the factors
that spawned the conflict (i.e., resource allocation) could be subsequently resolved
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
109
mitigated without resolving the conflict if the attractor is strong and has a wide
basin of attraction. Thus, a change in objective conditions could be interpreted in a
manner that reinforces rather than undermines the negative feelings toward the
other party. In conflict scenarios, then, an interpersonal or inter-group attractor is
likely to promote a social judgment attractor, which then can reinforces the
interpersonal attractor, and so on, in a reciprocal reinforcing feedback loop.
Because of the potential for feedback among levels, a conflict initially associated
with one level of reality can create the potential for overlapping and mutually
reinforcing attractors that serve to anchor the conflict. Moreover, the expansion of
attractors due to feedback among levels can become encoded in cultural beliefs and
traditions, which adds yet another level to the conflict, thereby promoting long-term
intransigence and resistance to attempts at conflict resolution. An example is
provided by the annual Orange walk in Northern Ireland each July, which
celebrates the Protestant victory of Prince William of Orange over King James II
in 1690. This event reinforces memories and narratives of past grievances and
losses associated with the protracted conflict over Ireland and thus holds potential
for triggering latent psycho-social attractors for more hostile Catholic-Protestant
relations.
235
The Emergence of Intractable Conflict
253
Once antagonistic relations between individuals, groups, or nations have become
intractable, it is easy to appreciate how they are maintained with respect to attractor
dynamics. But how do social relations get to this point in the first place? Conflict is
ubiquitous and largely constructive in human experience, after all, and only a small
proportion of conflicts become destructive and protracted over a long period of
time. What dynamic processes transform a normally benign feature of social
relations into something that is irrational and dysfunctional?
254
Dynamics of Self-Regulation
261
The development of intractable hostility in social relations can be looked upon as a
breakdown in the self-regulatory mechanisms that underlie normal behavior. The
capacity for monitoring, controlling, and changing one’s thoughts, feelings, and
actions is widely recognized as a defining feature of human functioning (cf.
Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996; Carver & Scheier, 1999; Miller, Galanter, &
Pribram, 1960; Powers, 1973; Vallacher & Nowak, 1999; Vallacher & Wegner,
1987; Wegner & Pennebaker, 1993; Weiner, 1948). In the dynamical framework,
the self-regulatory tendencies of a system reflect the nature of the feedback loops by
which elements influence one another.
Elements linked by reinforcing feedback loops bolster and amplify one another’s
current state. A favorable thought about a person or group, for example, elicits other
262
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
255
256
257
258
259
260
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
110
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
favorable thoughts or transforms neutral thoughts into favorable ones, and these
thoughts in turn reinforce the state of the original thought. Through repeated
iterations of this feedback loop, the separate thoughts coalesce into a global
judgment with positive valence. By the same token, reinforcing feedback can
promote the emergence of a derogatory judgment if an unfavorable thought about
a person or group recruits other unfavorable thoughts and influences neutral
thoughts to take on the same valence. In contrast, elements linked by inhibiting
feedback loops inhibit or reverse one another’s current state. Thus, a favorable
thought about a person or group recruits unfavorable thoughts or transforms neutral
thoughts into unfavorable ones, whereas an unfavorable thought recruits favorable
thoughts or transforms neutral thoughts into favorable ones.
Self-regulation involves both types of feedback (Vallacher & Nowak, 1999).
Social judgment generated solely by reinforcing feedback loops can transform a
single positive or negative thought about a person or group into a global evaluation
that is highly polarized in a positive or negative direction, respectively (e.g., Tesser,
1978; Vallacher, Nowak, & Kaufman, 1994). On the other hand, if social judgment
were generated solely by inhibitory feedback loops, an unequivocal evaluation of a
person or group would never be attained, as every valenced thought would be
contradicted by a thought with the opposite valence. Effective self-regulation thus
involves a balance between reinforcing and inhibitory feedback loops.
This balance shapes the trajectory of thought and behavior in everyday social
relations—including relations characterized by benign or constructive conflict. A
hostile thought about a person or group, for example, elicits other negative thoughts
or transforms neutral thoughts into negative ones, and through repeated iterations of
this linkage, the separate thoughts coalesce into a coherent negative judgment. In
most social contexts, however, this potential for the spread of negativity and
conflict escalation is restrained by inhibitory feedback among elements. A hostile
action toward a person, for example, might promote feelings of regret or guilt, or
perhaps a consideration of the person’s benign or positive qualities. Such thoughts
effectively restrain the negative judgment and hostile impulses, establishing a
tempered stance in the social relationship.
Achieving an optimal balance between reinforcing and inhibiting feedback loops
is not an automatic consequence of mind and action, but rather requires sufficient
controlled processes—conscious, deliberative, and reflective modes of thought—to
enable self-awareness, impulse control, reconsideration of judgments and
conclusions, and action monitoring (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1999; Gilbert, 1993;
Vallacher & Nowak, 1999; Wegner, 1994). Nonetheless, this mode of thought and
action provides a reliable foundation for personal, interpersonal, and collective
behavior in most circumstances. Even in situations of potential or manifest social
conflict, the dynamics of mind and action generally reflect effective self-regulatory
mechanisms. People can change their mind or reverse a course of action, provided
they function in accordance with controlled processes that strike a balance between
reinforcing and inhibitory feedback.
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
111
Breakdown in Self-Regulation
316
If balanced feedback is the norm for everyday thought and behavior, what is
responsible for the breakdown in self-regulatory dynamics that allows strong
attractors of negative thought, emotion, and action to develop? Self-regulatory
failure is actually a fairly hot topic in contemporary social psychology. Research
in several different paradigms has identified a host of factors that can undermine or
deplete the executive resources necessary for controlled processes (e.g.,
Baumeister, Vohs, & Tice, 2007; Vallacher & Nowak, 1999; Vohs & Heatherton,
2000; Wegner, 1994) and thus tip the balance in favor of reinforcing feedback at the
expense of inhibitory feedback. These factors, by extension, promote polarization
in mental, affective, and behavioral states by undermining people’s ability to inhibit
thoughts, feelings, and actions that are biased in a particular direction.
Stress is a primary factor in self-regulatory failure because it can seriously
compromise controlled processes (cf. Glass & Singer, 1972). Several lines of
research demonstrate that people find it hard to think clearly or correct a course
of action when they are under pressure, faced with tasks that exceed their selfperceived competence, or must decide between different courses of action with
unclear consequences (e.g., Tice, Bratslavsky, & Baumeister, 2001; Vohs,
Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005). Stress also increases people’s felt need for closure,
a desire to “seize” a global judgment without considering other perspectives and to
“freeze” on that judgment and demonstrate strong resistance to disconfirming
evidence and contrary ideas (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996). When people feel a
heightened need for closure, they are not only highly defensive when exposed to
contradictory judgments, they also demonstrate intolerance and antagonism toward
people who express such judgments. Stress, of course, is inherent in prolonged and
difficult conflict, so quite plausibly the pressures and tensions experienced in such
conflicts contribute to a breakdown in normal self-regulation and promote the
emergence of an attractor defined in terms of coherent and polarized thoughts,
feelings, and actions.
A special form of stress is associated with people’s awareness of their mortality,
an existential state that is likely to have heightened salience in a situation with
potential for violent conflict. According to terror management theory (TMT),
people embrace the values of their culture most vigorously when they are mindful
of their own death (cf. Greenberg et al., 1990). Because destructive conflicts can
devolve toward violent confrontations, the death of oneself or of one’s fellow group
members may be considered a very real possibility. Interestingly, research on TMT
has established that when people are made mindful of death, they become more
hostile to people who criticize their country (Greenberg et al., 1990), more hostile
to outgroups (Dechesne, Greenberg, Arndt, & Schimel, 2000), more punitive
toward those who challenge established laws and procedures (Rosenblatt,
Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989), and more reluctant to use
cultural artifacts such as a country’s flag for a mundane purpose such as a table
covering (Greenberg, Porteus, Simon, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1995). These
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
AU1
112
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
findings suggest that the emergence and maintenance of strong attractors for
hostility in interpersonal and intergroup relations might be a consequence of the
recognition that one’s life is at risk under conditions of intense conflict. The
possibility of death associated with destructive violence, in other words, can tip
the balance in self-regulation toward reinforcing feedback, which promotes global
judgments with unequivocal valence.
Stress is not the only source of self-regulatory failure. Simply attending to
multiple sources of information or engaging in parallel activities, for example,
can undermine the controlled processes necessary for effective self-regulation.
Under such conditions of cognitive load, people fail to correct unwarranted evaluative judgments of others, presumably because they do not take into account factors
in the situation that may have promoted the behavior in question (Gilbert, 1989).
Thus, negative actions are attributed to the actor’s personality or values despite
pressures or expectancies that would likely elicit the same behavior in anyone (e.g.,
Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988). Stereotypical beliefs, moreover, tend to predominate when judging members of outgroups if the perceiver is under cognitive load,
since stereotypes are over-learned ways of thinking that do not require controlled
processes of thinking (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). Cognitive load also undermines
people’s ability to suppress thoughts and feelings that are unwanted or inappropriate (cf. Wegner, 1994; Wegner, Erber, & Zanakos, 1993; Wegner, Schneider,
Carter, & White, 1987). A negative characterization of a person or group, for
example, may be difficult to ignore or discount, even if the characterization is
unfounded and should not be considered valid or appropriate.
If substantial cognitive load is experienced in the midst of a potentially destructive conflict, one might expect an uptick in shallow processing of information
regarding members of an outgroup, with the balance shifted toward reinforcing
feedback at the expense of inhibitory feedback. Although this possibility has not
been directly tested, it seems reasonable. When a conflict begins to occupy people’s
thoughts, after all, everyone must still devote mental energy to the more mundane
aspects of life. Adding thoughts about conflict to the controlled processing necessary to get through daily life, then, might serve to create a level of cognitive load
that undermines balanced self-regulation. If so, people consumed with conflict on
top of their everyday concerns might be prone to develop polarized thoughts and
action tendencies toward members of the antagonistic group. The emergence of
attractor dynamics in this fashion may set the stage for intractable conflict.
Self-regulatory failure may have a biological basis. Brain functions utilize a
special form of glucose, called glycogen, and this “mental fuel” is burned at a
particularly rapid rate when a person is engaged in the controlled processing
necessary for self-regulation (Gailliot et al., 2007). Compared to participants who
are given a natural sweetener that provides glycogen to the brain, for example, those
who are given an artificial sweetener that lacks glycogen-producing properties are
more likely to demonstrate subsequent lapses in self-regulation. Such lapses may be
manifest as selfish or even immoral action, undertaken without consideration of the
act’s larger meaning or long-term consequences. People who inhibit an impulse or
delay gratification, for example, are subsequently inclined to act selfishly rather
Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
113
than altruistically, and to behave dishonestly if such behavior enhances their
immediate self-interest (e.g., Vohs et al., 2008).
This perspective suggests that prolonged efforts at self-regulation can result in a
boomerang effect, with inhibitory feedback playing a weakened role in subsequent
thought and behavior (cf. Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998; Gailliot
et al., 2007). For example, in tense social relations characterized by sustained
pressures for compromise and perspective taking—higher-level mental activities
that deplete glycogen—there may be a sudden breakdown in inhibitory feedback,
with the emergence of highly polarized thoughts and feelings shaping how the
respective parties respond to one another. This is clearly a speculative scenario, but
one that seems worthy of exploration. If confirmed, it would suggest an ironic
prediction: heightened concern with compromise and problem solving in a conflict
scenario holds a risk of transforming a benign conflict into one characterized by
extreme judgments and non-compromising action orientations.
To this point, we have concentrated on generic factors in everyday life that can
disrupt the balance between reinforcing and inhibitory feedback in thoughts,
feelings, and actions. There are unique factors in social relations characterized by
conflict, however, and these may hold potential for undermining self-regulation and
setting parties to a conflict on the path toward intractability. Likely candidates
include power symmetry versus asymmetry, the degree of confined interdependence, the level of cooperation versus competition over resources, the degree to
which access to resources has a zero-sum nature, and the salience and magnitude of
differences in ideology and social identity. The relation of these features of conflict
to the emergence of intractability characterized by strong attractors for hostility has
yet to be fully investigated. This issue, however, is front and center at several points
in the following chapters.
404
A Hypothetical Scenario of the Emergence of Intractability
430
The dynamical systems framework can be made concrete by means of a hypothetical scenario in which a conflict escalates to intractability. In this scenario, the
conflict might start with someone’s thoughts concerning another person that center
on incompatibility with that person (however, it could also start top-down, such as
when a youth is socialized into a protracted conflict from birth). A structure begins
to form as separate (negative) thoughts begin to support each other. This structure
grows by assimilating a growing number of other psychological processes and
emotions. As a result, judgments of the person become increasingly undifferentiated and unidimensional, organized around incompatibility with the person. Eventually, hostile intentions are likely to be communicated and initiate similar
processes in the other person. At this moment, two systems of conflict existing on
individual levels reinforce each other and eliminate the possibility for positive
interaction. The thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of both people are processed
within the structure of conflict. Conflict now exists at the interpersonal level.
431
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
114
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
As both parties seek support by recruiting other members of their respective
groups, the conflict escalates on the social level, where it is sustained by links
between the groups and by positive (reinforcing) feedback within each group. The
sustainability of the conflict increases because even if the person who was the
source of the conflict tries to disassemble the conflict structure, feedback from
others in either group will reinstate the conflict. With the growth in intensity of the
conflict, hostility and violence become increasingly likely as a result of the mutual
feedback. The structure of conflict now permeates individuals, groups, and the
whole society, recruiting almost all processes and events and thereby eliminating
almost all chances of positive interactions between the opposing groups. At some
point, the multitude of feedback loops within and between levels that sustain and
support the conflict renders fruitless the attempt to disassemble even large parts of
the conflict because the conflict would become reinstated in other parts of the
structure.
Protracted existence of conflict at the societal level is likely to start shaping the
symbol system of those involved in the conflict. Identities are built around incompatibility. The terms used for referencing the outgroup become dehumanizing in
nature and preclude positive interactions. In effect, the protracted conflict becomes
embedded in the culture of the society. When this happens, the conflict acquires a
new means of maintenance and spreading. Anyone adopting the culture is likely to
adopt the conflict embedded within it. People who never had contact with the
outgroup members are nonetheless unlikely to form sustainable positive or even
neutral relationships with the members of the outgroup. In this way, conflict is
passed through generations.
Referring to intractable conflict as “malignant” (Coleman, 2003) is warranted
because the analogy between the two terms is direct. Cancer works by penetrating
the structure of the organism and enslaving essential elements of the body, which
lose their original functions and begin working in service of the structure of cancer.
The collapse of complexity associated with intractable conflict is visible in the same
process. The multitude of psychological and social processes necessary for the
maintenance of mental structures, religions, and societies become enslaved into the
one-dimensional structure of conflict. Their original functions essentially vanish.
As the machine of war is established and grows, it encompasses a growing number
of social, economic, and political processes, focusing them all in a single issue
within the conflict.
In such a structure, love, friendship, or even collaboration at work between
members of the opposing parties will not be seen in its true nature, but rather as
collaboration with the enemy, weakness, or treason. The richness and multidimensionality of all the processes occurring in a healthy society become entrained in the
structure, leaving no opportunity for positive interactions. Such structures of intractable conflict can sustain multiple states, such as attack, revenge, temporary truce,
and strategic withdrawal, but not co-existence of long-term cooperation.
The cost of conflict is not limited to suffering, loss, or deaths in the center of the
conflict. Indeed, the most costly long-term consequences are not associated with the
direct damage, but rather with the elimination of possibilities of positive events that
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
115
are necessary to the functioning of any healthy society. Conflicts dramatically
diminish the social capital of a society, an effect that inevitably results in a dramatic
social and economic decline. Such consequences, however, may not be observable
for a considerable period of time. Thus, the ability to diagnose the sustainable states
of the society is likely to provide an early measure of the potential long-term
damage to the society as well indicate the potential for a peace process. Such
diagnosis would also provide a measure of the effectiveness of various intervention
attempts before they actually impact the visible states of the conflict, and it would
allow one to estimate the damage done by aggravating events before the damage is
manifest.
From this perspective, the severity of intractable conflict can be characterized
with respect to both structure and dynamics. Structurally, we can describe which
elements are relevant and how these elements are inter-related. What is the nature
of the feedback loops among elements? At what level does the structure of the
conflict currently exist? What proportion of psychological and social processes are
organized into the structure of conflict? What proportion remains conflict-free?
Even in intense conflict, there may be some contexts and issues (e.g., sharing water
resources) for which the parties are able to build relations or even find solutions
independently of the conflict. The degree to which conflict permeates all psychological and social processes, thereby organizing all judgments and actions in terms
of incompatibility, provides a measure of the conflict’s intensity.
So while the severity of conflict may be related to the amount of violence
between groups, the intractability of conflict may be defined in terms of the
elimination of sustainable states of positive interaction. In dynamical terms, an
intractable conflict lacks attractors for positive states. The progression, transformation, and de-escalation of intractable conflict can be described in terms of the
dynamics of the ensemble of sustainable states for both positive and negative
interactions—that is, in terms of the attractor landscape of the system. Attempts
at conflict resolution are unlikely to succeed if they do not work toward the
achievement of sustainable states. They may result in a temporary ceasefire, but
not in long-term co-existence between the opposing parties. If no sustainable states
exist, the first step at intervention should be directed to changing the ensemble of
sustainable states. Only after such change has occurred can the system be effectively moved to a benign or positive state.
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
The dynamical perspective on intractable conflict is intended to provide integrated
understanding of a phenomenon that is highly diverse and seemingly idiosyncratic
in its various manifestations. But for this perspective to be useful, it must be
heuristic as well, generating new hypotheses that are open to verification. Accordingly, we have developed three lines of research reflecting our multi-disciplinary
orientation. The first involves laboratory studies focused on moral conflicts, such as
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
116
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
542
those over abortion and the right to take one’s own life, which have a high
propensity for escalating and becoming polarized and protracted. These studies
provide empirical evidence of the constructive versus destructive effects of highcomplexity versus low-complexity cognitive, emotional, and behavioral dynamics.
The second approach is a conceptual model detailing the pervasive spread of
negativity that is commonly observed in situations of protracted conflict. This
model is illustrated with a qualitative study that examines the contagion effects in
the ongoing conflict between Israelis and Palestinians. The third approach involves
mathematical modeling and computer visualization of Deutsch’s Crude Law of
Social Relations (Deutsch, 1973), which has been identified by scholars as a
primary mechanism at the core of escalation, de-escalation, and intractability
(Nowak, Deutsch, Bartkowski, & Solomon, 2010).
543
High and Low Complexity Dynamics in Moral Conflicts
544
This research program tests the central proposition of our approach, that intractable
conflicts (ones that are destructive, persistent, and resistant to change) evidence
attractors that are characterized by low levels of complexity (e.g., fixed-point
attractors). Other scholars have found evidence of attractors for difficult social
dynamics. Research on marriage and divorce by Gottman (2002) has consistently
found that the relational phase-space of couples whose relationships end in divorce
tend to be characterized by relatively strong, coherent attractors for negative
emotions and weak attractors for positive emotions. Similarly, Losada (1999) has
found that the emotional and behavioral attractors of low-functioning business
strategy groups are very different from high-functioning groups. High-functioning
groups tend to display relatively complex and open patterns of communication that
allow for the expression of thoughts, feelings, and actions that are negative as well
as positive. In marked contrast, low-functioning groups display patterns of thought,
feeling, and action that are both more constrained and more negative in tone and
substance.
Both lines of research have begun to identify basic parameters that distinguish
between attractors leading to more unfavorable and negative social outcomes
versus more favorable and positive outcomes. The underlying assumption is that
attractors that evidence more complexity and openness in human thinking, feeling,
and acting are associated with more favorable social dynamics. The parameters
characteristic of higher complexity and openness found in empirical research to
date include higher ratios of positivity-to-negativity of emotions (Bales, 1950;
Gottman, Markman, & Notarius, 1977), more balance between internally- and
externally-focused references in groups (Losada, 1999; Losada & Heaphy, 2004),
and greater balance between inquiry acts (questioning) and advocacy acts (positioning) in problem-solving groups (Losada & Heaphy, 2004).
To further investigate these dynamics, we developed a laboratory paradigm that
allowed us to trace the temporal trajectories of constructive versus destructive
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
AU2
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
117
discussions over seemingly intractable moral issues (for a description of the study,
see Kugler, Coleman, & Fuchs, 2011). We hypothesized that destructive
discussions over moral conflicts would be characterized by attractors which
evidenced lower levels of complexity, whereas more constructive discussions
would evidence more high-complexity patterns. In the context of these studies,
complexity refers to the way humans respond to a potentially complex environment
(cf. Satish, 1997). It was assumed that most moral conflicts, being inherently
nuanced and multi-dimensional, require more complex responses to be managed
constructively and effectively.
We conducted two studies. In both studies, dyads of previously unacquainted
participants were brought together our lab to discuss an issue that held potential for
generating an intractable conflict. In particular, the individuals comprising each
dyad were matched according to their pre-measured opposing views on a topic such
as abortion, euthanasia, or the death penalty, and then were asked to discuss the
topic and write a joint position statement. Throughout the course of their interaction, we gathered data about their emotions, cognitions, and behaviors. We
hypothesized that conflicts leading to more tractable versus more intractable
outcomes would exhibit more complex cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
dynamics during the discussion. The concept of complexity was supplemented by
the concept of openness to the other party’s concerns, opinions, and emotional
experiences. Openness was presumed to be instrumental for gaining a more complex view of the conflict issue and a better understand the other party (e.g., Johnson
& Johnson, 2006).
In Study 1, we compared dyads that had concluded their discussion with
tractable outcomes (those showing a mutual and elaborated joint understanding of
the issue) with dyads that had ended with more intractable outcomes (those showing
no or an incompatible joint understanding of the issue). The comparison supported
the hypothesis that those dyads ending with tractable outcomes exhibited more
complex and open dynamics. Highly complex and open dynamics were
operationalized by: (1) high levels of integrative complexity (i.e., differentiation
and integration of perspectives and information) as well as openness to learning
new information from the other party; (2) a high ratio of positive-to-negative
emotions (i.e., positive emotions are thought to broaden the momentary action
and thought repertoire and are therefore allowing for more complexity, e.g.,
Fredrickson, 1998, 2001, 2006), as well as a higher level of emotional connectedness (indicating an openness and responsiveness to the other party, e.g., Gottman &
Silver, 1999; Gottman, Swanson, & Swanson, 2002); (3) a high pro-social/pro-self
ratio (pro-social motivated behaviors are related to approaching conflict situations
in more complex categories, Carnevale & Probst, 1998), as well as higher levels of
inquiry/advocacy ratios (inquiry exhibits openness to the other parties thinking, e.g.
Losada & Heaphy, 2004).
Study 2 was identical to Study 1 with one important change: We manipulated the
level of integrative complexity of both members of the dyads prior to their discussion (high complexity dyads versus low complexity dyads). High versus low levels
of integrative complexity was induced through two different versions of a written
572
573
AU3
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
AU4
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
AU5
608
AU6
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
118
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
643
text on the specific moral issue under discussion (abortion, euthanasia, etc.): one
was written according to the standards for high integrative complexity, the other
according to the standards for low integrative complexity (cf. Baker-Brown et al.,
1992). This manipulation had a significant impact on the conflict dynamics (cognitive, emotional, and behavioral complexity) as well as on the conflict outcomes
(tractable vs. intractable). During the discussion, high-complexity dyads showed
more complex thinking (higher levels of integrative complexity), and following the
discussion dyads showed more complex emotions (higher positivity/negativity
ratios), more complex behaviors (higher prosocial/proself ratios), and reached
more tractable outcomes (a more elaborated joint position statement) than did the
low complexity dyads.
However, we did not find the same effects for openness that were observed in the
first study. We assume that the manipulation, which deliberately influenced cognitive complexity (integrative complexity), had a stronger and more direct impact on
the complexity dynamics than on the openness dynamics. Even though we assume
that the level of complexity and openness are connected, their relationship does not
seem to be linear or direct: enhancing complexity did not automatically lead to
more openness.
The two studies offer support for the proposition that constructive conflict
processes over potentially intractable issues exhibit more complexity along with
more openness in disputants’ experiences and behaviors. Nevertheless, much work
remains to be done. First, more research is needed to verify the connection between
conflict intractability and attractor dynamics (see Coleman et al., 2007; Vallacher
et al., 2010b). How do these differences in conflict dynamics develop in time? How
are they maintained once they are manifest? How might the combination of high
and low-complexity conditions operate—for example, will dyads tend to move to
the lowest or highest common denominator?
644
The Spread of Conflict into Intractability
645
Most conflicts have relatively clear boundaries that delineate what they are and are
not about, who they concern and who they don’t, and when and where it is
appropriate to engage in the conflict. In situations where conflicts become more
intractable, the experience of threat can become so basic and intense that the
negative effects of the conflict spread and become increasingly pervasive, affecting
many or even all aspects of a person’s or a community’s social and political life
(Coleman, 2003; Musallam, Coleman, & Nowak, 2010; Rouhana & Bar-Tal, 1998;
Zartman, 2005). The core existential nature of these conflicts can impact everything
from policy-making, leadership, education, the arts, and scholarly inquiry down to
the most mundane decisions such as whether to shop and eat in particular public
places. The totality of such experiences can feel overwhelming.
Although hostilities may be most obvious at a particular level (e.g., between
groups), intractable conflicts often affect and are affected by elements at multiple
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
AU7
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
119
levels (from the psychological to the societal or geopolitical). These elements
interact in complex ways, leading to increased chaos and confusion. Intractable
conflicts also tend to attract more and more parties over time to the dispute. The
original disputants then come to operate within a network of other relationships that
may or may not be opposed. This can include formal and informal third parties,
elites, observers, bystanders, NGOs, business organizations, etc (see Crocker,
Hampson, & Aall, 1999; Diamond & McDonald, 1996; Staub, 2001). In Kashmir,
for example, relations between the Indian, Pakistani, and Kashmiri leadership are
dependent upon, among others, relations between the leaders and their general
publics, hawks and doves within both groups, formal and informal representatives,
extremist factions on both sides, the media and the public, business communities on
all sides, regional neighbors, the Muslim and Hindu worlds, and the international
community.
In the following account, the dynamical systems perspective is employed to
enhance our understanding of how negativity in protracted conflict spreads to
previously neutral issues, parties, and structures, and to suggest a need for assessing
how this form of contagion occurs in conflicts over time (Musallam et al., 2010).
658
Pervasiveness
675
The destructiveness of long-term geopolitical conflicts is typically assessed by their
intensity in quantitative terms, which typically includes one of three sets of criteria:
(1) counting the number, types, and magnitude of bombing attacks, battles,
fatalities, injuries, population displacement, etc.; (2) assessing their temporal
scope or how long they persist, or (3) assessing the direct consequences of conflicts,
which entails the examination of the physical and psychological welfare of those
caught up in the dynamics of conflict, and the damage to the social, political,
economic and educational infrastructure of the society.
However, in characterizing intractable conflicts, scholars such as Zartman
(2005) have concluded that “intractability is a dynamic, self-reinforcing condition;
digging an ever-deepening hole for itself and feeding itself like a vortex” (p. 55). In
other words, the longer violent conflicts persist, the more they spread into the
central fabric of societies, and therefore the longer they last. Zartman’s observation
on the nature of intractability captures the essence of how conflicts become
pervasive: “They [conflicts] are not static; they grow, in both degree and nature,
extending and defending themselves, so that efforts to overcome them must penetrate several layers and deal with their protective dynamics [. . ..] intractability feeds
on intractability and grows with the feeding [. . .] the importance of protraction lies
not in its numerical duration but in that duration’s effect (2005, p. 49).”
Other scholars have specified particular processes and outcomes related to the
pervasive spread of negativity in conflict. For example, Rouhana and Bar-Tal
(1998) described several aspects of public life that can become affected by conflict
when it is highly central and “total”. They write:
676
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
120
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
When conflicts are total] they penetrate the societal fabric of both parties and force
themselves on individuals and institutions. Leaders, publics, and institutions—such as
educational and cultural systems—become involved in the conflicts. At some stages of
the conflicts, intellectual life and scholarly inquiry become politicized as interest in the
other society originates in the motivation to “know your enemy” and inquires become
guided by security needs and considerations. . . [centrality] is reflected in the group
members’ preoccupation with the conflicts. Thoughts related to the conflict are highly
accessible and are relevant to various discussions within each society (Bar-Tal, Raviv, &
Freund, 1994). The centrality of such conflicts is further reflected in their saliency on the
public agenda. The media and the political and intellectual elites are greatly preoccupied
with the conflicts and their developments (p. 762).
710
718
Various conflict scholars have observed that destructive conflicts have a tendency to spread and thereby sustain their negative effects (Coleman, 2000, 2003;
Gray, Coleman, & Putnam, 2007; Kriesberg, 2005; Rouhana & Bar-Tal, 1998;
Zartman, 2005). Yet despite decades of research dedicated to the study of protracted
conflict dynamics (see Azar, 1983, 1990; Burgess & Burgess, 1996; Coleman,
2003, 2004, 2006; Crocker & Hampson, 2004; Crocker, Hampson, & Aall, 2005;
Deutsch, 1973; Druckman, 2001; Kelman, 1997; Kriesberg, 2005; Zartman, 1989),
to date little is known about the basic mechanisms that account for the spread of
conflict, or about the conditions that make it more or less likely to occur.
719
A Dynamical Model of Conflict Pervasiveness
720
For the purposes of this model, conflict pervasiveness is defined as the process
through which the “spread of negativity into previously independent states and
activities” transpires (Musallam et al., 2010). The spread of negativity is a process
in which neutral states that are previously perceived as independent of a conflict
begin to be associated with the conflict and contribute to intensifying it or spreading
its effects further, instead of maintaining their original neutral states. For instance,
in Israel/Palestine, healthcare delivery vehicles such as ambulance were once
considered constructive and necessary components of functioning society, until
they began to be used to transport weapons, at which point their presence took on a
qualitatively different meaning. Conflict pervasiveness is dynamical by nature; it is
an evolving set of cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and/or structural elements that
grow in number, connectedness, and size over time. It is similar to the spread of a
fire through a forest or an epidemic through a community. Therefore, it becomes
necessary to adopt a conceptual framework for studying it that can help better
understand these evolving, proliferating dynamics.
Biological and social models of processes such as epidemiology of diseases and
contagion and diffusion of attitudes reveal the utility of using frameworks informed
by complexity science, including complex systems theory, network models, and
dynamical systems theory. For example, such frameworks have been fruitfully
applied to the outbreak of epidemics (Morris, 2004), as well as to processes of
increased stereotyping (McClelland, Rumelhart, & Hinton, 1986; Operario & Fiske,
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
AU8
AU9
AU10
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
121
2001) and aggression (Read & Miller, 2002; Read, Vanman, & Miller, 1997; Shoda,
Tiernan, & Mischel, 2002).
To reiterate, dynamical systems theory concerns the study of complex systems
that evolve over time. A dynamical system is defined as a set of elements that
change over time as each element adjusts to the myriad influences from the other
elements that make up the system. These elements can be thoughts, feelings, and
actions at an individual level; people, groups and norms at a social level, or various
institutions such as families, the media, religious organizations, schools, etc. at a
broader level. Each element can be stimulated and perpetuated along its current
path through reinforcing feedback loops between elements, where one element
stimulates another along its current trajectory and this element, in turn, stimulates
the first—thus making a loop. We see this when a negative act by an outgroup
member links to negative memories and feelings from previous encounters and
increase a general sense of animosity toward the outgroup and the likelihood that
they will perceive future acts as negative. Elements can also obstruct or reverse one
another via inhibiting feedback loops where one element constrains another
(Coleman et al., 2006). We see this when a sense of guilt or compassion arises
within us and buffers our hostile intentions or actions.
As noted earlier, feedback loops can operate both within and between levels of a
system. For instance, the perception of an outgroup member as “evil” by an ingroup
member can be reinforced psychologically by other thoughts and feelings that are
consistent with this assessment, as well as socially by other ingroup members that
hold and thus reinforce similar beliefs. At a group level, individuals can mutually
influence and inform each other via reinforcing feedback in a process that creates
increasing attitude homogeneity within subgroups resulting in increased conformity, or they can contradict and inhibit each other (inhibiting feedback) through
processes such as criticism and dissent that eventually results in increased heterogeneity of opinion within groups.
Thus, the spread of malignancy (or positivity) over time in a conflict can be
characterized as an increase in reinforcing feedback loops and a decrease in
inhibiting feedback loops between various elements both within and between the
psychological, social, and structural levels of a social system. Psychologically,
loops become established between various negative conflict-related attitudes or
thoughts which link with other negative emotions within an individual and begin
to affect cognitions that were previously unrelated to the conflict. Under some
conditions, these elements can be transformed, such as when neutral or positive
thoughts about another person become consistently negative. Then, the activation
of this thought by external stimuli or events will lead to the activation of other
linked cognitions, thereby psychologically drawing the individual into the negativity of the conflict.
Such negative cognitive-emotional clusters have the capacity to spread beyond
the individual psychological level and begin to operate (and create reinforcing
loops) through behaviors on the interpersonal and group levels, and ultimately in
changes on a structural level. In other words, for any individual, the likelihood that
thought A leads to thought B and emotion C is usually guided by linkages and loops
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
122
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
among cognitions and feelings available to that individual (Shoda et al., 2002),
which can link to their behavioral responses to members of group D and be
particularly likely within the context of situation E. What is important to emphasize
is that the spread of conflict is not necessarily bottom up (individual to environment) or top down (environment to individual), but rather they can spread in either
or both dir[ections and be mutually reinforcing as long as the system lacks
sufficient inhibiting feedback loops to mitigate such mutual reinforcement.
There is anecdotal evidence supporting the spread and displacement of aggression from one target group into other groups. Wessells (2007) noted that in Angola,
a place torn by protracted civil wars, aggression became more and more acceptable
as a means of solving community problems, and over time spread to other domains
of life including friendships, martial relationships, and work relations. Similarly,
Martin-Baro noted that during war, the slaughter of individuals, the disappearance
of loved ones, and the constant threat to one’s life all come to be accepted as
increasingly normal despite their extreme natures (See Martin-Baro, as citied in
Aron & Corne, 1994, Chaps. 6 and 7, pp. 108–135; Comas-Dı́az, Lykes, & Alarcón,
1998). This normalization and spread of violence-related acts has been
demonstrated statistically in many conflict areas. Reports such as the United
Nations Population Fund (UNFPA) have consistently reported increased domestic
violence against women (UNFPA, 2006), increases in organized and gang-related
crimes such as kidnapping children (UNAMI, 2006), and increases in sexual abuse
and trafficking in zones of protracted conflict (UNAMI, 2006; UNFPA, 2006).
In addition to the spread of aggression, conflicts are usually characterized by
contagion or “spillover” of attitudes of intolerance toward outgroups or subgroups
that are not part of the ingroup with respect to a particular conflict (Shamir & SagivSchifter, 2006; Sullivan, Shamir, Walsh, & Roberts, 1985). For instance, in conflict
zones there are often reports of increased intolerance towards other ethnic and
religious minority groups (Shamir & Sagiv-Schifter, 2006), increased political
intolerance in comparison to other peaceful societies (Sullivan, Piereson, &
Marcus, 1993), and increased intolerance towards gay and lesbian communities
(UNAMI, 2006).
At a structural level, Rouhana and Bar-Tal (1998) have noted that in areas of
intractable conflict, cultural and entertainment institutions such as theater, film,
museums, and educational institutions all align together and further polarize
societies. For example, educational institutions can fuel conflict by perpetuating
societal beliefs and ideologies that instigate conflict (Bar-Tal, 1999) instead of
keeping to their original mission of educating the general population (Bar-Tal,
1998; DeCharms & Moeller, 1962; Luke, 1988). This is achieved through the use of
biased history and social studies textbooks as well as other curricular and pedagogical tools. In addition, we often observe a transformation in national history
museums and art exhibits that propagate the “official” conflict narratives and
often constitute collective memories for warring populations (Nets-Zehngut,
2008; Savelsberg & King, 2005).
AU11
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
123
An Exploratory Study on Pervasiveness
829
In 2009, an exploratory study was conducted in Israel-Palestine to investigate the
phenomenon of conflict pervasiveness, with the goal of identifying its basic
dimensions (Musallam et al., 2010). For the purposes of this study, conflict pervasiveness was defined for participants as the range of issues and social processes that
are organized into the structure of the conflict versus those that are “conflict free”.
The primary aim of the study was to try to distinguish those elements of day-to-day
life in this setting that, at any given time, serve to drive the conflict (move people
toward anger or aggression against outgroups) versus those that were perceived to
be irrelevant to the conflict, or those elements that served to constrain the conflict
(move people toward a sense of empathy or compassion with outgroups).
At the beginning of the study, participants were asked to think about a typical
day in their-life and to list those activities that were “related to the conflict” and
those that were “conflict free”. Both Israelis and Palestinians found the task to be
extremely challenging. For example, one of the Israeli participants argued: “When
you grow up in a conflict, conflict is your reality, that is life, that is the way things
are. It is so difficult for me to distinguish what is related and what is not related to
the conflict”. This experience was shared by others and illustrates how “conflict
saturated” both Israeli and Palestinian societies are. When participants were
presented with the statement: “As the situation deteriorates and persists, more
and more aspects of peoples’ daily life become affected by it and serve to trigger
or perpetuate a sense of anger/resentment/contempt”, they all agreed strongly with
it.
All participants expressed strong emotional experiences that included a sense of
helplessness, insecurity, hopelessness, frustration, anxiety, fear, and anger. For
example, a sense of insecurity by Israeli participants reinforced the need to use
violence against the Palestinians, whereas anger and hopelessness expressed by
Palestinian participants supported the use of violent tactics against Israelis. In
addition, some participants noted that as things escalated in their communities,
the degree to which they participated in certain activities changed. For example, the
number of times they checked news updates and the number of phone calls they
made during particular days changed as a function of conflict escalation. As one of
the participants put it “It is not only about what you do, but how often you do certain
things and for how long they last”.
Finally, both Israeli and Palestinian interviewees indicated that the degree to
which they were affected by the conflict depended to a large extent on their relative
physical proximity to high/low conflict intensity locations. Those residing closer to
places where violence occurred experienced a much broader sense of contagion
than those who lived in relatively more peaceful locations. However, Israelis
viewed conflict pervasiveness as something that was more related to specific
periods and incidents (e.g., when bombing attacks occurred), whereas Palestinians
perceived conflict pervasiveness more consistently in their every-day experiences.
In addition, for Israelis, “conflict pervasiveness” was associated mostly with
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
124
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
890
psychological dimensions (e.g., decision making processes), whereas for
Palestinians, conflict pervasiveness was associated with more tangible, mundane
matters (e.g., the ability to go to a hospital, a store, etc.).
This exploratory study revealed several important points. First, it provided some
preliminary empirical support of the subjective experience of the phenomenon of
pervasiveness. Second, it highlighted the difficulty experienced by participants in
distinguishing between what is related and what is unrelated to conflict in situations
of protracted ethnopolitical conflict. Third, it pointed to the qualitative differences
in experiences expressed by distinct populations. Finally, it highlighted the importance of understanding the various interrelated aspects of conflict pervasiveness,
which include behavioral, cognitive, and affective components.
Although substantial amount of work has been dedicated to the development of
measures for the assessment of conflict intensity, duration, and the many
consequences of conflict intractability, to date no measures have been developed
to assess their degree of pervasiveness. Developing such measures could have
important implications for assessing long-term damage in conflict, diagnosing
fragile and ripe peace processes (Coleman, 2000; Zartman, 1986, 2000, 2003),
and assessing the effectiveness of interventions before applying them within conflicted societies (Coleman, 2006).
891
A Dynamical-Minimal Model of the Crude Law of Social Relations
892
900
Deutsch’s Crude Law of Social Relations (1973) is a simple theoretical generalization of the results of a research program concerning the conditions that promote
cooperation versus competition and the consequences of cooperating versus competing. It is based on the observation from empirical studies that the consequences
of cooperation and competition are strikingly similar to their causes. In particular,
the Crude Law states that “the characteristic processes and effects elicited by a
given type of social relationship (e.g., cooperative or competitive) tend also to elicit
that type of social relationship; and a typical effect of any type of relationship tends
to induce the other typical effects of the relationship”.
901
Basic Features of the Model
902
Following the approach of dynamical minimalism (Nowak, 2004), we attempted to
model the Crude Law mathematically using the simplest possible assumptions
(Nowak et al., 2010). We adopted a generic formalism of growth—the Alphabet
Model (Nowak & Solomon, 2006)—which is an elaboration of the AB model of
Solomon and his collaborators (Shnerb, Louzoun, Bettelheim, & Solomon, 2000).
The model concentrates on the development of protracted conflict across time.
Following the main factors constituting Crude Law dynamics as outlined by
Deutsch (1973), the model is based on three types of elements:
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
125
• Competitive interdependence: The extent to which the interests of parties are
incompatible.
• Competitive behaviors: The momentary behaviors of individuals to move
against each other in order to obstruct the one another’s goal achievement.
• Competitive orientations: The attitudes underlying competition. Such
orientations have many components, such as low trust or negative perceptions
of others. Orientations evolve on a slower time scale than do behaviors.
910
Computer Simulations of the Model
917
In our computational model, a 25 25 square grid represents the space where the
conflict interactions occur. In the beginning of the simulation, a random number of
competitive behaviors are assigned for each cell of the grid. Five cells (marked by
the white dots in Fig. 5.2a) are randomly chosen as the places where competitive
interdependence (e.g., competition over scarce resources such as water) exist. To
model the progression of a conflict into intractability, the simulation starts with no
elements containing competitive orientations. The starting configuration is shown
in Fig. 5.2a.
The simulations are based on a few simple rules that operate locally (i.e., within
a single cell). All the rules represent control parameters of the model and are
applied with a specific probability, which can be adjusted. Thus, with a specific
probability:
918
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
911
912
913
914
915
916
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
Competitive behaviors elicit new competitive behaviors.
Competitive behaviors decay with a certain rate.
Competitive behaviors migrate to nearby locations.
Competitive behaviors produce competitive orientations.
The probability of competitive behaviors producing new competitive behaviors
grows with the local (same cell) presence of elements of competitive interdependence and also elements of competitive orientations.
6. With some small probability, each negative behavior migrates to a randomly
chosen neighboring cell. Competitive behaviors thus diffuse from their place of
origin.
930
Thus, in the presence of competitive interdependence, competitive behaviors
reproduce competitive behaviors in kind and also help foster competitive
orientations, which evolve at a slower rate. Competitive orientations also facilitate
the multiplication of competitive behaviors. Each process thus produces conditions
facilitating its occurrence at the place where the conditions are met—a process that
captures the essence of the Crude Law.
The initial configuration (Fig. 5.2a), which is maintained in the early stages of
the simulation (Fig. 5.2b), shows the initial random distribution of competitive
interdependence and competitive behaviors. The height and color intensity of a box
correspond to the number of competitive behaviors, with higher and darker colored
940
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
AU12
126
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Fig. 5.2 (a–d) XXX
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
cells representing greater competition. As noted above, the small white dots
correspond to the locations of competitive interdependence, or competing goals.
The initial dynamic is dominated by the decrease in the number of competitive
behaviors. In the cells with no competitive interdependence, competitive behaviors
vanish at a higher rate than such behaviors are produced. However, in the locations
where structures of competitive interdependence are present, the number of negative behaviors grows as each competitive behavior produces another element of
competitive behavior with a high probability. Because each competitive behavior
most likely produces other competitive behaviors, the growth of competitive
behaviors in the cells where structures of competitive interdependence exists is
exponential. The number of competitive behaviors in the locations of competitive
interdependence increases dramatically over time where there exists a longer
history of interaction. Because the number of locations where the growth of conflict
occurs is low, a general decrease in competitive behaviors can be observed.
Because of the exponential growth of competitive behaviors, after some time
there is a global increase in competitive behaviors in the locations with high
competitive interdependence (Fig. 5.2c). Due to the high number of competitive
behaviors in the cells with competitive interdependence, competitive social
orientations are eventually produced in these locations (Fig. 5.2d). Thus, islands
of more destructive conflict characterized by competitive interdependence,
behaviors, and orientations are established around these cells.
Figure 5.2c shows the formation of pools of destructive conflict. Figure 5.2d
illustrates creation of competitive social orientations, which are marked by the dark
dots. Note the high concentration of negative orientations in the lower left and
upper right corner of Fig. 5.2d. The elements of competitive orientations were
created in the locations where competitive interdependence existed. They were
AU13
Research on the Dynamics of Intractable Conflict
127
created by the high incidence of competitive behaviors, rather than directly by the
competitive interdependence.
976
Reinstatement of Conflict
978
To simulate the effects of attempts at conflict resolution, in the next round of
simulations the competitive behaviors were reset to the initial random configuration
and the elements of competitive interdependence were eliminated. The final configuration of the previous simulation (Fig. 5.2d) was used as input. From a rational
perspective, there is little reason for the conflict to exist any longer. The competitive
orientations, however, were left intact. Figure 5.3a portrays the reinstatement of
conflict in the area of high concentration of competitive orientations.
The final simulation was run for several steps. Figure 5.3b portrays the final
configuration. Note the high concentration of competitive orientations in the lower
left corner. Negative orientations act as a social memory of past conflict. The
conflict is reinstated only in one of the original areas where the concentration of
the competitive orientations was the highest. Through a reinforcing feedback loop,
competitive behaviors produce more elements of competitive orientations. At this
location, the conflict is self-sustaining and growing at an accelerated rate. However, in other areas where the conflict had existed previously, it was not reinstated.
This is because the intensity of negative orientations elsewhere was not sufficient to
sustain the process of competitive behavior growth.
The temporal evolution of the conflict, the intervention, and the reinstatement of
the conflict are portrayed in Fig. 5.4 below. As we can see, the existence of
competitive social orientations results in the conflict being reinstated almost immediately. Although the conflict was recreated in only one location, because of the fast
exponential growth it takes only five simulation steps to reinstate a high level of
competitive behaviors.
The results highlight two factors that are critical to escalation of conflicts: the
duration of a conflict and the local severity of competitive interdependence. Simply
put, a conflict will escalate the most if competitive interdependence exists in the
same location and continues for a long time. If, however, the same amount of
competitive interdependence ceases to exist in one place, and after a short time
reappears in another, the severity of conflicts in a society will be much smaller. This
is because in each location a conflict does not have enough time to change
psychological orientations in a significant way, so it is less likely to escalate to
intractability.
Thus, the model illustrates how conflicts tend to be recreated in places where
they once existed, even after a long period of being dormant and even if competitive
interdependence no longer exists. In other words, hostilities committed in a conflict
create competitive orientations that reinstate the conflict in the same place.
This scenario suggests that the timing of intervention is critical for transforming
conflicts. In the earlier stages of a conflict, intervention needs to be focused on
competitive behaviors in the few places with conflicts of interest. In later stages,
979
977
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
1001
1002
1003
1004
1005
1006
1007
1008
1009
1010
1011
1012
1013
1014
1015
1016
1017
128
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Fig. 5.3 (a, b) The initial and final configuration of the competitive orientations and competitive
behaviors
Reinstatement of Conflict
120
9000000
8000000
100
80
6000000
5000000
60
4000000
40
3000000
Orientations
Behaviors and
7000000
Behaviors
Orientations
2000000
20
1000000
0
0
1
3
5
7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 39 41 43 45 47 49
Time
Fig. 5.4 Time course of the creation and reinstatement of the conflict
resolution of the conflict of interest still may be effective, although the conflict may
have become global for the society and the levels of hostility are likely to be high.
1020 After some time, enough competitive orientations are created to sustain the conflict
1021 even in the absence of any conflict of interests. When the conflict becomes self1022 sustaining, it acquires the features of intractability.
1018
1019
1023
Coping and Survival in Open Versus Closed Systems
From a dynamical systems perspective, it is important to distinguish between closed
and open systems. In closed systems, dynamics depend entirely on the current state
1026 of the system (i.e., the current values of variables in the system) and are not
1027 influenced by input from the external environment. They may display emergent
1028 properties, but this tendency reflects only the self-organization of intrinsic dynam1029 ics without sensitivity to external factors that might promote the emergence of
1030 different properties. Open systems, in contrast, display dynamics that are respon1031 sive to environmental influences. Self-organization occurs in such systems, but
1032 some products of self-organization are better suited to environmental demands and
1033 are selected at the expense of less well-suited products of self-organization.
1024
1025
Coping and Survival in Open Versus Closed Systems
129
Not surprisingly, open systems have far greater capacity to cope and survive,
particularly in a changing external environment. The notion of complex adaptive
systems (e.g., Holland, 1975, 1995), for example, refers to the capacity of systems
to change their own structures and functions in order to adapt to environmental
factors. This is easy to appreciate in biological systems, which must be able to
capitalize on opportunities such as the availability of food and the presence versus
absence of predators. A plant that did not grow in the direction of sunlight, for
example, would be at a severe disadvantage in the struggle for survival. Within the
lifetime of a single organism, learning is the most efficient mechanism for adaptation. At the species level, evolution—based on mutation and natural selection—
provides a mechanism for adaptation on a longer time scale that improves the
fitness of successive generations of organisms. In short, systems that are closed
cannot adapt and lose their fitness. Eventually, they are unable to cope and survive
as the external environment changes.
The distinction between closed and open adaptive systems has important
consequences for understanding intractable conflicts. Social groups, organizations,
and societies that adapt to environmental influences have a clear advantage in
coping and survival. In contrast, social systems that are inflexible in response to
changing conditions are at risk for dysfunctional behavior and even extinction. The
dynamical tendencies associated with intractable conflict capture the nature of
closed systems. Indeed, the development of intractability may be described as the
closing up of a previously open and adaptive system. Once the dynamics of a
system are captured by a strong attractor, environmental events essentially represent perturbations that are actively resisted so as to maintain the system’s coherent
state (i.e., pattern of thoughts, feelings, and action tendencies). Thus, a social
relation characterized by intractable conflict is composed of two (or more)
parties—whether individuals, groups, or nations—that react towards one another
with suspicion and hostility, even if such reactions are unwarranted, counterproductive, and maladaptive. Outside interventions, too, tend to be resisted in
such systems, despite the long-term self-interest that may be associated with such
interventions. Even after the issues that initially triggered the conflict have been
resolved (or are no longer relevant), the parties to an intractable conflict remain
intransient in their beliefs, emotions, and action orientations.
Intractable conflicts impair the capacity for coping and survival in other ways as
well. As the conflict escalates and self-organizes into a stable attractor landscape, it
enslaves an increasing number of psychological and social processes. Thus, many
processes that usually serve basic needs of individuals and social groups lose their
original functionality as they acquire new functionality of bringing harm to the
opponent. As the system loses the functions necessary to fulfill its basic needs, it
loses the capacity for coping and survival. This process is tantamount to the loss of
complexity in system dynamics. Prior to the emergence of deep attractors for
conflict, individuals and groups have rich, multidimensional dynamics enabling
the achievement of various goals and the fulfillment of various functions. When the
social relation devolves into a system of conflict, however, it absorbs other,
1034
1035
1036
1037
1038
1039
1040
1041
1042
1043
1044
1045
1046
1047
1048
1049
1050
1051
1052
1053
1054
1055
1056
1057
1058
1059
1060
1061
1062
1063
1064
1065
1066
1067
1068
1069
1070
1071
1072
1073
1074
1075
1076
1077
130
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
previously functional processes, so that the dynamics of each party become
organized along a single dimension of antagonism toward the other party.
1080
This scenario can be seen at different levels of social reality. Consider marital
1081 conflict, for example. A married couple originally experiences rich dynamics
1082 organized along many dimensions, such as providing material support, caring for
1083 their children, organizing entertainment, cleaning their house, planning vacations,
1084 planning shopping, and organizing their social life. If a conflict develops that
1085 progresses toward intractability, however, these functions lose their independence
1086 and come to fuel rather than diffuse the conflict. Decisions concerning the children,
1087 for instance, may be governed by the desire to limit contact with the other parent
1088 rather than dictated by the child’s needs. When planning entertainment, meanwhile,
1089 the motive may be to limit the pleasures of the partner rather than to spend
1090 enjoyable time together. And cleaning the house may become an occasion to
1091 blame the partner for the mess and assigning motives and personality traits that
1092 justify the judgment. As an increasing number of activities become transformed and
1093 aimed at harming the partner, the original function of these activities are lost. As a
1094 result, the capacity of the couple to cope with the flux of life is likely to be
1095 dramatically reduced.
1096
International conflict that devolves into intractability can be seen in the same
1097 light. The large losses experience by warring nations to a large extent reflect a loss
1098 of functionality in each nation’s social processes and institutions. As a conflict
1099 escalates and becomes intractable, the scope of social processes and institutions
1100 relevant to the conflict increases. The educational system, for example, becomes a
1101 system of propaganda rather than one of teaching children. The priority of trans1102 portation changes from the delivery or food and supplies to the movement of
1103 soldiers and weapons. The function of the justice system changes from enforcing
1104 laws to prosecuting (and persecuting) enemies in the outgroup and members of the
1105 ingroup who are suspected of supporting the outgroup enemies. The main function
1106 of the economic system is altered to finance the machinery of war, rather than to
1107 provide the material well being of the society. As these different institutions and
1108 processes acquire new (dis)functionality related to conflict, they lose their original
1109 purpose and thus the society becomes less able to fulfill the needs of its citizens. In
1110 general, all social functions suffer as a society loses its ability to cope with the
1111 variety of social and economic challenges it faces.
1112
Yet another maladaptive consequence of intractable conflict concerns the tem1113 poral focus of the parties to the conflict. Effective adaptation requires that systems
1114 react to present events in the current state of their environment. When locked in
1115 intractable conflict, however, the parties tend often to focus on the past (e.g., past
1116 injustices and grievances, seminal events that are memoralized, the history of the
1117 relationship) rather than on the needs of the present. Fixation on the past decouples
1118 the system from its focus on the present, so that the respective parties react to events
1119 that are no longer current and cannot be changed. And when the parties fixate on the
1120 past, they are likely to ignore signals that the opposing party is ready for reconcili1121 ation, and may misinterpret peaceful overtures. This clearly limits the prospect for
1122 conflict transformation. Politicians during times of conflict often exploit the
1078
1079
References
131
tendency to fixate on the past so as to prevent reconciliation that may not serve their
personal ambitions and interests. Playing on people’s fears and hatred is a good way
to secure and maintain power in a time of crisis and strained international relations.
So, What Can Be Done?
1123
1124
1125
1126
In our account, intractable conflict has all the hallmarks of a system that has become
self-organized with respect to a strong attractor that provides a coherent way of
thinking, feeling, and acting. As is the case for any dynamical system, once a deep
attractor for conflict emerges in a social system, it is highly resistant to change.
Rather than accommodating to new information and being sensitive to changing
challenges and demands, a social system governed by a strong attractor reacts to
such influences as if they were perturbations that threaten the system’s integrity. A
host of cognitive and social mechanisms are employed to deflect or reinterpret such
perturbations, rendering the system incapable of responding appropriately and
rationally to changing circumstances or even acting in its own best long-term
interests. A relationship mired in intractable conflict is inflexible and closed—
hardly the characteristics of an adaptive system.
The dynamical scenario of intractable conflict seems gloomy, if not hopeless.
How can one possibly transform a system that has lost its complexity and openness
to change, and whose sole motive is the preservation of a fixed way of seeing the
world? Actually, this is far from the end of the story. Indeed, although it may seem
ironic, the strategies that may prove effective in transforming intractable conflict
follow from the principles of dynamical systems theory and research. This aspect of
dynamical systems—their potential for change in their basic processes—is the
focus of Chap. 6.
1127
References
1147 AU14
Aron, A., & Corne, S. (1994). Writings for a liberation of psychology. Boston, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Azar, E. E. (1983). The theory of protracted social conflict and the challenge of transforming
conflict situations. Monograph Series in World Affairs, 20(M2), 81–99.
Azar, E. E. (1990). The management of protracted social conflict. Hampshire, UK: Dartmouth.
Baker-Brown, G., Ballard, E. J., Bluck, S., deVries, B., Suedfeld, P., & Tetlock, P. (1992). The
integrative complexity coding manual. In C. Smith (Ed.), Handbook of thematic analysis (pp.
605–611). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bar-Tal, D. (1998). Societal beliefs in times of intractable conflict: The Israeli case. International
Journal of Conflict Management, 9, 22–50.
Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Freund, T. (1994). An anatomy of political beliefs: A study of their
centrality, contents, and epistemic authority. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24,
849–872.
1148
1149
1150
1151
1152
1153
1154
1155
1156
1157
1158
1159
1160
1161
1128
1129
1130
1131
1132
1133
1134
1135
1136
1137
1138
1139
1140
1141
1142
1143
1144
1145
1146
132
1162
1163
1164
1165
1166
1167
1168
1169
1170
1171
1172
1173
1174
1175
1176
1177
1178
1179
1180
1181
1182
1183
1184
1185
1186
1187
1188
1189
1190
1191
1192
1193
1194
1195
1196
1197
1198
1199
1200
1201
1202
1203
1204
1205
1206
1207
1208
1209
1210
1211
1212
1213
1214
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the
active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological
Inquiry, 7, 1–15.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355.
Burgess, H., & Burgess, G. (1996). Constructive confrontation: A transformative approach to
intractable conflicts. Mediation Quarterly, 13(4), 305–322.
Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. (1998). Social values and social conflict in creative problem solving
and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1300–1309.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Stress, coping and self-regulatory processes. In L. Pervin &
J. P. Oliver (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 553–575). New York,
NY: Guilford.
Coleman, P. T. (2000). Fostering ripeness in seemingly intractable conflict: An experimental
study. International Journal of Conflict Management, 11(4), 300–317.
Coleman, P. T. (2003). Characteristics of protracted intractable conflict. Peace and Conflict:
Journal of Peace Psychology, 9, 1–37.
Coleman, P. T. (2004). Paradigmatic framing of protracted, intractable conflict: Towards the
development of a meta-framework—II. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology,
10, 197–235.
Coleman, P. T. (2006). Conflict, complexity, and change: A meta-framework for addressing
protracted, intractable conflicts - III. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 12
(4), 325–348.
Coleman, P. T. (2011). The five percent: Finding solutions to (seemingly) impossible conflicts.
New York, NY: Public Affairs, Perseus Books.
Coleman, P. T., Bui-Wrzosinska, L., Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (2006). Protracted conflicts as
dynamical systems. In A. K. Schneider & C. Honeyman (Eds.), The negotiator’s fieldbook: The
desk reference for the experienced negotiator (pp. 61–74). Chicago, IL: American Bar Association Books.
Coleman, P. T., Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., & Bui-Wrzosinska, L. (2007). Intractable conflict as
an attractor: Presenting a model of conflict, escalation, and intractability. The American
Behavioral Scientist, 50, 1454–1475.
Comas-Dı́az, L., Lykes, M. B., & Alarcón, R. (1998). Ethnic conflict and psychology of liberation
in Guatemala, Perú, and Puerto Rico. The American Psychologist, 53, 778–792.
Crocker, C. A., & Hampson, F. O. (2004). Taming intractable conflicts: Mediation in the hardest
cases. Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace.
Crocker, C. A., Hampson, F. O., & Aall, P. (1999). Herding cats: Multiparty mediation in a
complex world. Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace.
Crocker, C. A., Hampson, F. O., & Aall, P. (2005). Grasping the nettle: Analyzing cases of
intractable conflict. Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace.
DeCharms, R., & Moeller, G. H. (1962). Values expressed in American children’s readers. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 136–142.
Dechesne, M., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J. (2000). Terror management and the
vicissitudes of sports fan affiliation: The effects of mortality salience on optimism and fan
identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 813–835.
Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive processes. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Diamond, L., & McDonald, J. (1996). Multi-track diplomacy: A systems approach to peace. West
Hartford, CT: Kumarian Press.
Druckman, D. (2001). Turning points in international negotiation: A comparative analysis.
Journal of Conflict Resolution, 45, 519–544.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2,
300–319.
References
133
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-andbuild theory of positive emotions. The American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Unpacking positive emotions: Investigating the seeds of human
flourishing. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 57–59.
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., et al.
(2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a
metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336.
Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference
process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189–211). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Gilbert, D. T. (1993). The assent of man: Mental representation and the control of belief. In D. M.
Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), The handbook of mental control (pp. 57–87). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of
stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person
perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
733–739.
Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. (1972). Behavioral aftereffects of unpredictable and uncontrollable
aversive events. American Scientist, 60, 457–465.
Gottman, J. (2002). A multidimensional approach to couples. In Comprehensive handbook of
psychotherapy: Cognitive behavioral approaches (Vol. 2, pp. 355–372). New York, NY:
Wiley.
Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977). The topography of marital conflict: A
sequential analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39,
461–477.
Gottman, J. M., & Silver, N. (1999). The seven principles for making marriage work. New York,
NY: Three Rivers Press.
Gottman, J. M., Swanson, C., & Swanson, K. (2002). A general systems theory of marriage:
Nonlinear difference equation modeling of marital interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(4), 326–340.
Gray, B., Coleman, P. T., & Putnam, L. L. (2007). Introduction: Intractable conflict—New
perspectives on the causes and conditions for change. The American Behavioral Scientist,
50, 1415–1429.
Greenberg, J., Porteus, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1995). Evidence of a terror
management function of cultural icons: The effects of mortality salience on the inappropriate
use of cherished cultural symbols. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1221–1228.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., et al.
(1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of mortality salience on
reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 58, 308–318.
Holland, J. (1975). Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of
Michigan Press.
Holland, J. (1995). Can there be a unified theory of complex adaptive systems? In H. J. Morowitz
& J. L. Singer (Eds.), The mind, the brain, and complex adaptive systems. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2006). Peace education for consensual peace: The essential role
of conflict resolution. Journal of Peace Education, 3(2), 147–174.
Kelman, H. C. (1997). Social-psychological dimensions of international conflict. In I. W. Zartman
& J. L. Rasmussen (Eds.), Peacemaking in international conflict: Methods and techniques (pp.
191–237). Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace Press.
1215
1216
1217
1218
1219
1220
1221
1222
1223
1224
1225
1226
1227
1228
1229
1230
1231
1232
1233
1234
1235
1236
1237
1238
1239
1240
1241
1242
1243
1244
1245
1246
1247
1248
1249
1250
1251
1252
1253
1254
1255
1256
1257
1258
1259
1260
1261
1262
1263
1264
1265
134
1266
1267
1268
1269
1270
1271
1272
1273
1274
1275
1276
1277
1278
1279
1280
1281
1282
1283
1284
1285
1286
1287
1288
1289
1290
1291
1292
1293
1294
1295
1296
1297
1298
1299
1300
1301
1302
1303
1304
1305
1306
1307
1308
1309
1310
1311
1312
1313
1314
1315
1316
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Kriesberg, L. (2005). Nature, dynamics, and phases of intractability. In C. A. Crocker, F. O.
Hampson, & P. Aall (Eds.), Grasping the nettle: Analyzing cases of intractable conflict (pp.
65–98). Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and
“freezing. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.
Kugler, K. G., Coleman, P. T., & Fuchs, A. M. (2011). Conflict, complexity, and openness:
constructive versus destructive dynamics of discussions over intractable issues (WOP Working
Paper No. 2011/3). http://www.psy.lmu.de/wirtschaftspsychologie/forschung/working_papers/
index.html
Latané, B., & Nowak, A. (1994). Attitudes as catastrophes: From dimensions to categories with
increasing involvement. In R. R. Vallacher & A. Nowak (Eds.), Dynamical systems in social
psychology (pp. 219–249). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Latané, B., Nowak, A., & Liu, J. H. (1994). Measuring emergent social phenomena: Dynamism,
polarization, and clustering as order parameters of social systems. Behavioral Science, 39(1),
1–24.
Losada, M. (1999). The complex dynamics of high performance teams. Mathematical and
Computer Modelling, 30, 179–192.
Losada, M., & Heaphy, E. (2004). The role of positivity and connectivity in the performance of
business teams: A nonlinear dynamics model. The American Behavioral Scientist, 47,
740–765.
Luke, A. (1988). Literacy, textbooks and ideology. London: Falmer.
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Parallel distributed processing:
Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Psychological and biological models, Vol. 2).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York,
NY: Holt.
Musallam, N., Coleman, P. T., & Nowak, A. (2010). Understanding the spread of malignant
conflict: A dynamical systems approach. Peace and Conflict. The Journal of Peace Psychology,
16, 127–151.
Nets-Zehngut, R. (2008). The Israeli national information center and collective memory of the
Israeli–Arab conflict. The Middle East Journal, 62(4), 653–670.
Nowak, A. (2004). Dynamical minimalism: Why less is more in psychology? Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 8(2), 183–193.
Nowak, A., Deutsch, M., Bartkowski, W., & Solomon, S. (2010). From crude law to civil relations:
The dynamics and potential resolution of intractable conflict. Peace and Conflict: Journal of
Peace Psychology, 16(2), 189–209.
Nowak, A., & Solomon, S. (2006). Alphabet model of social processes. Paper presented at the
conference “Complexity models of social and economic processes” Torino, Italy.
Nowak, A., & Vallacher, R. (1998). Dynamical social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., Bui-Wrzosinska, L., & Coleman, P. T. (2007). Attracted to conflict:
A dynamical perspective on malignant social relations. In A. Golec de Zavala & K. Skarzynska
(Eds.), Understanding social change: Political psychology in Poland (pp. 33–49). Hauppauge,
NY: Nova Science.
Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., Tesser, A., & Borkowski, W. (2000). Society of self: The emergence
of collective properties in self-structure. Psychological Review, 102, 39–61.
Operario, D., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). Ethnic identity moderates perceptions of prejudice: Judgments
of personal versus group discrimination and subtle versus blatant bias. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 27, 550–561.
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Read, S. J., & Miller, L. C. (2002). Virtual personalities: A neural network model of personality.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 357–369.
References
135
Read, S. J., Vanman, E. J., & Miller, L. C. (1997). Connectionism, parallel constraint satisfaction
processes, and Gestalt principles: (Re)introducing cognitive dynamics to social psychology.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 26–53.
Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for
terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate
or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690.
Rouhana, N. N., & Bar-Tal, D. (1998). Psychological dynamics of intractable ethnonational
conflicts: The Israeli–Palestinian case. The American Psychologist, 53, 761–770.
Satish, U. (1997). Behavioral complexity: A review. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27,
2047–2067.
Saunders, A. D. (1997). Channel defenses. London: BT Batsford.
Savelsberg, J., & King, R. D. (2005). Institutionalizing collective memories of hate: Law and law
enforcement in Germany and the United States. The American Journal of Sociology, 111(2),
579–616.
Shamir, M., & Sagiv-Schifter, T. (2006). Conflict, identity, and tolerance: Israel in the Al-Aqsa
Intifada. Political Psychology, 27, 569–595.
Shnerb, N. M., Louzoun, Y., Bettelheim, E., & Solomon, S. (2000). The importance of being
discrete: Life always wins on the surface. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 97,
10322.
Shoda, Y., Tiernan, L., & Mischel, W. (2002). Personality as a dynamical system: Emergence of
stability and distinctiveness from intra- and interpersonal interactions. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 6, 316–325.
Staub, E. (2001). Genocide and mass killing: Their roots and prevention. In D. J. Christie, R. V.
Wagner, & D. D. Winter (Eds.), Peace, conflict and violence: Peace psychology for the 21st
century (pp. 76–86). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Sullivan, J. L., Piereson, J., & Marcus, G. E. (1993). Political tolerance and American democracy.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Sullivan, J. L., Shamir, M., Walsh, P., & Roberts, N. S. (1985). Political tolerance in context:
Support for un-popular minorities in Israel New Zealand, and the United States. Boulder, CO:
Westview.
Tesser, A. (1978). Self-generated attitude change. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 1,
289–338.
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes
precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 53–67.
UNFPA. (2006). UNFPA state of the world population. United Nations population fund. New
York, NY: United Nations, Department of Public Information.
United Nations Assistance Mission for Iraq (UNAMI). (2006). Human rights report 1 May—30
June 2006. New York, NY: UNAMI.
Vallacher, R. R., Coleman, P. T., Nowak, A., & Bui-Wrzosinska, L. (2010). Rethinking intractable
conflict: The perspective of dynamical systems. The American Psychologist, 65(4), 262–278.
Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (1999). The dynamics of self-regulation. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.),
Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, pp. 241–259). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (2007). Dynamical social psychology: Finding order in the flow of
human experience. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook
of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 734–758). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., & Kaufman, J. (1994). Intrinsic dynamics of social judgment.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 20–34.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Self-regulation and self-presentation:
Regulatory resource depletion impairs impression management and effortful self-presentation
depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 632–657.
1317
1318
1319
1320
1321
1322
1323
1324
1325
1326
1327
1328
1329
1330
1331
1332
1333
1334
1335
1336
1337
1338
1339
1340
1341
1342
1343
1344
1345
1346
1347
1348
1349
1350
1351
1352
1353
1354
1355
1356
1357
1358
1359
1360
1361
1362
1363
1364
1365
1366
1367
1368
1369
136
1370
1371
1372
1373
1374
1375
1376
1377
1378
1379
1380
1381
1382
1383
1384
1385
1386
1387
1388
1389
1390
1391
1392
1393
1394
1395
1396
1397
5 Traps: Intractable Conflict as a Dynamical System
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Tice, D. M., & Nelson, N. M.
(2008). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898.
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach.
Psychological Science, 11, 249–254.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.
Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood
and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1093–1104.
Wegner, D. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.). (1993). Handbook of mental control. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought
suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.
Weiner, N. (1948). Cybernetics, or control and communication in the animal and the machine.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wessells, M. G. (2007). Post-conflict healing and reconstructing for peace: The power of social
mobilization. In J. D. White & A. J. Marsella (Eds.), Fear of persecution: Global human rights,
international law and human well being (pp. 258–278). Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.
Zartman, I. W. (1986). Ripening conflict, ripe moment, formula and mediation. In D. BenDahmane
& J. McDonald (Eds.), Perspectives on negotiation. Washington, DC: GPO.
Zartman, I. W. (1989). Ripe for resolution: Conflict and intervention in Africa (2nd ed.). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Zartman, I. W. (2000). Ripeness: The hurting stalemate and beyond. In P. Stem & D. Druckman
(Eds.), International conflict resolution after the cold war. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Zartman, I. W. (2003). Negotiating with terrorists. International Negotiation, 8, 443–450.
Zartman, W. A. (2005). Rethinking the economics of war. In C. J. Arnson (Ed.), The intersection of
need, creed, and greed. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Author Queries
Chapter No.: 5
Query Refs.
Details Required
AU1
The citation “Greenberg, Simon,
Porteus, Pyszczynski, & Solomon,
1995” (original) has been changed
to “Greenberg, Porteus, Simon,
Pyszczynski & Solomon 1995”.
Please check if appropriate.
AU2
The citation “Gottman et al. (2002) ”
(original) has been changed to “Gottman (2002)”. Please check if appropriate.
AU3
The citation “Coleman, Kugler, &
Fuchs, 2011” (original) has been
changed to “Kugler, Coleman &
Fuchs 2011”. Please check if appropriate.
AU4
The citation “Johnson et al., 2006”
(original) has been changed to “Johnson and Johnson 2006”. Please check
if appropriate.
AU5
The citation “Gottman et al., 1999”
(original) has been changed to “Gottman and Silver 1999”. Please check
if appropriate.
AU6
Please note that reference "Gottman,
Swanson, & Swanson, 2002b" has
been changed to "Gottman, Swanson,
& Swanson, 2002".
AU7
Please note that reference "Vallacher
et al. (2010b)" is cited in the text but
not provided in the list
AU8
In this sentence “When conflicts are
total]”opening parenthesis is missing
here. Please check.
AU9
The citation “Crocker, Hampson, &
Aall, 2004” (original) has been
changed to “Crocker & Hampson
2004”. Please check if appropriate.
AU10
Please note that reference "Morris
(2004)" is cited in the text but not
provided in the list
AU11
Please note that Bar-Tal (1999) is
cited in the text but not provided in
the list
Author’s response
AU12
Note that the second occurrence of
“Figs. 5.1a–d, have been renumbered
as Fig. 5.2a–d and Fig. 5.2a,b and
Fig. 5.3 have been renumbered as
Fig. 5.3a,b and Fig. 5.4 both in the
text and in the legend. Please check.
AU13
Please provide caption for “Fig. 5.2.”
AU14
Please note that reference “Latané,
Nowak, and Liu (1994)” is not cited
in the text but provided in the list.