This document discusses emotional development in children from infancy through age 10. It notes that infants begin demonstrating discrete emotions like smiling and laughter between 2-4 months, and can recognize emotions in others by 3 months. Emotional and cognitive development occur simultaneously. By age 1, children use social referencing to gauge reactions from caregivers. By age 4, children begin learning ways to regulate their own emotions. Temperament, empathy, attachment styles, and school/friendships also influence a child's emotional development.
This document discusses emotional development in children from infancy through age 10. It notes that infants begin demonstrating discrete emotions like smiling and laughter between 2-4 months, and can recognize emotions in others by 3 months. Emotional and cognitive development occur simultaneously. By age 1, children use social referencing to gauge reactions from caregivers. By age 4, children begin learning ways to regulate their own emotions. Temperament, empathy, attachment styles, and school/friendships also influence a child's emotional development.
This document discusses emotional development in children from infancy through age 10. It notes that infants begin demonstrating discrete emotions like smiling and laughter between 2-4 months, and can recognize emotions in others by 3 months. Emotional and cognitive development occur simultaneously. By age 1, children use social referencing to gauge reactions from caregivers. By age 4, children begin learning ways to regulate their own emotions. Temperament, empathy, attachment styles, and school/friendships also influence a child's emotional development.
This document discusses emotional development in children from infancy through age 10. It notes that infants begin demonstrating discrete emotions like smiling and laughter between 2-4 months, and can recognize emotions in others by 3 months. Emotional and cognitive development occur simultaneously. By age 1, children use social referencing to gauge reactions from caregivers. By age 4, children begin learning ways to regulate their own emotions. Temperament, empathy, attachment styles, and school/friendships also influence a child's emotional development.
Download as PPTX, PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as pptx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 14
Emotional Development
Dr. Sasmita Mishra
At what age do infants experience and demonstrate discrete emotions? As young as 2months old demonstrate smiling Laughter at 3-4 months old Then anger, sadness and surprise Read other’s emotion at the age of 3months. Emotion and cognitive development happen simultaneously Social referencing
One year old will look at their care givers and
depending on their reactions, will cry or laugh. Emotional regulation
Four year old cover his/her ears or eyes while
watching frightening television show. By the age of 10 years they learn different ways to regulate and express emotions and draw sympathy and support of others. Progress in both these tasks-regulating and expressing emotions-play a key role in children’s ability to form increasingly complex social relationships. Temperament-emotional style
Temperament: stable individual differences in
the quality or intensity of emotional reactions. Dimensions of temperament Positive emotionality Distress anger Fear Activity level Categories of children based on temperament Easy children (40%) Difficult children (10%) Slow-to-warm up children (15%) These temperaments are relatively stable. Difficult children experience behavioural problems in later part of life. They face difficulty in adjusting in schools, making friendship and get along with others. Empathy
Our ability to recognize the emotion of others, to
understand these feelings, and to experience them ourselves, at least to a degree. During first two years of development, our cognitive development permits us to distinguish ourselves clearly from others-to form a concept of self. Then even more sophisticated theory of mind. This allows us to understand that others have feelings that may differ from our own. Empathy Children of eighteen moths old try to do something comforting when another child is distressed-for instance, touching or patting that child. At two years they may offer an object or go to seek adult help. At 4 years, most children can understand why others are upset and have a grasp of the kinds of situations that can cause people emotionally distress. Ultimately, empathy develops to the point where individuals experience guilt if they view themselves as the cause of another’s distress. Attachment
A long affectional bond between infants and
their caregivers. Infants differ in the quality or style of their attachment to the caregivers. Secure attachment Insecure/avoidant attachment Insecure/ambivalent attachment Disorganized attachment Children of different culture differ in the attachment style Factors influencing attachment style
Maternal sensitivity Infant temperament Effect of attachment in later life
Secure attachment: more sociable, better in
solving certain kind of problem, more tolerant of frustration, and more flexible and persistent in many situations. Avoidant attached infant worry constantly about loosing their romantic partners. Ambivalent attachment type show ambivalent reaction to romantic relationships. Contact, comfort and attachment
Harry Harlow-direct contact with soft objects
Experiment on monkeys School and friendships
Healthy friendship enhances social skills and
helps in healthy emotional development Children who have been victims of bullies, develop anxiety, depression, loneliness and low self-esteem. Close friends protect children from bullies.