To cite this article: Bin Li & Shimin Yin (2020): How perceived control affects advertising
avoidance intention in a skippable advertising context: a moderated mediation model, Chinese
Journal of Communication, DOI: 10.1080/17544750.2020.1776743
Article views: 4
Introduction
As the fastest growing form of media, online advertising has become a primary
revenue source for streaming media websites. According to MAGNA (2019),
global advertising revenue has continued to increase for several years, and it
reached about $600 billion in 2019. Advertising demand maintains strong in
countries with favorable economic conditions. The Asia-Pacific region is growing
rapidly, especially China, which has the second-largest advertising market in the
world. In China, the advertising market has grown exponentially, and video adver-
tising, which has been a mainstream advertising model, has exceeded $10 billion.
Compared with conventional users, online users are more willing to directly
access video content. When advertising appears, the online experience might be
disrupted, generating negative emotions in users, thus intensifying their avoidance
intention (Redondo & Aznar, 2018). Moreover, combined with the uneven quality
of ad content, the actual effectiveness of advertising does not meet expectations
(Rodgers & Thorson, 2000). About 30% of viewers use advertising blockers, and
ß 2020 The Centre for Chinese Media and Comparative Communication Research, The Chinese University of Hong Kong
2 B. Li and S. Yin
this phenomenon has become an urgent problem for advertisers and technology
platforms that try to attract users (SocialMediaToday, 2019). Skippable advertis-
ing, which is an innovative form, is catered to lowering avoidance intention in
online users. This new form has had a significant effect on enhancing perceived
control and making users feel that skippable advertising is less intrusive than non-
skippable advertising is (Belanche et al., 2017; Pashkevich et al., 2012). Similarly,
based on the premise of not harming the interests of advertisers or content
producers, offering skippable options can improve users’ viewing experience
(Pashkevich et al., 2012). Although this new advertising model is still in its infancy
in China, the attention to it continues to increase.
In skippable advertising, the “skip-ad” button appears during a short
mandatory clip, allowing users direct access to video content. Advertisers only
need to pay for users who watch for more than 30 s and do not pay for those who
skip (YouTube, 2016). Consequently, advertisers using this form often pay
attention to the overall quality of advertising content, especially the 5 s foreplay,
to increase advertising acceptance. This form, which has a high coverage rate in
foreign websites, has the characteristics of low risk and high return, thus providing
a win-win situation for users, advertisers, and platforms.
However, in China, there are essential differences in the business models that
video operators use compared with foreign ones. They mainly use two advertising
models: (1) users need to watch a large amount of nonskipping advertising to
access video content “for free”; (2) users open a membership to avoid advertising,
and only a few advertisers and video operators jointly offer skippable advertising.
In addition, video websites in China offer high-quality copyrighted video content
that is purchased at high prices, and its revenues depend on advertising and
membership fees. In contrast, YouTube is a video-sharing streaming media
platform without copyright costs, and advertising inventories are bid on based on
display effectiveness. Because of the differences in national conditions and business
models, it may not be feasible to directly graft the results of studies on YouTube,
for example, to Chinese platforms. Thus, it is urgently necessary to investigate the
responses of Chinese users. Their perspective could help in developing a richer
understanding of the phenomenon of skippable advertising avoidance.
Moreover, the gap between the increasing number and duration of advertising
and users’ autonomy has placed Chinese advertisers and video operators in the
dilemma that mandatory displays intensify the degree of exposure but induce
negative emotional responses. Moreover, offering the skippable advertising option
could result in the aggradation of advertising avoidance risk and decrease profit-
ability (Jeon et al., 2019; Pashkevich et al., 2012). Furthermore, users visit websites
to watch video content rather than advertising, so they have the final decision on
whether to skip advertising. Skippable advertising increases users’ feelings that
they are respected, so positive intention and acceptance may be enhanced (Youn
& Kim, 2019). Moreover, high-quality information induces pleasure and flow,
affecting users’ perceptions, attitudes, and intentions, thus lowering the probability
of advertising avoidance (Hwang & Jeong, 2019; Redondo & Aznar, 2018).
At present, although there are several theories and practices of skippable
advertising in foreign countries, relatively few exist in China, which is the largest
developing country in the world. Previous studies have described the factors that
influence advertising avoidance (Belanche et al., 2017; Jeon et al., 2019) but have
not considered different backgrounds and business models. Advertising avoidance
Chinese Journal of Communication 3
Ad avoidance intention
The behaviors of ignoring, skipping, and eliminating advertising are caused by
psychological reactance, which is induced by the increasing duration or quantity
of advertising. Users are overwhelmed, which results in advertising avoidance:
users reduce the related behavior of receiving advertisements at different levels
(Speck & Elliott, 1997). Advertising avoidance reduces the possibility of exposure
and hinders regular contact between advertisements and users, which is considered
a major challenge to advertisers (Fransen et al., 2015). Most video operators in
China currently use cost per mille (CPM) billing rules, which forces users to watch
advertisements, thus ensuring the effectiveness of the display. However, users have
become disgusted with long-time playback and low-quality advertising, so they
have adopted different strategies to cope with the forced exposure to such adver-
tising (Jeon et al., 2019; Redondo & Aznar, 2018).
According to the theory of planned behavior (TPB), behavioral intention,
which is defined as the psychological process that an individual undergoes before
4 B. Li and S. Yin
Perceived control
Perceived control, which is a psychological need, refers to the cognitive condition
of the individual’s ability to control a specific environment (Quick & Stephenson,
2007). For example, the skip-ad option offers online video users active control
rather than passive control provided by advertising blockers. The decision, which
users choose to avoid, is more likely to occur when individuals realize that the
choice will have a favorable effect on their interests (Gerbasi & Prentice, 2013).
The self-selection option is intended to motivate users to take the initiative (Jung,
2011). When goal-oriented users are forced to watch advertising, they tend to
experience a psychological state that enables them to restore lost control
(Redondo & Aznar, 2018). In contrast, the skip-ad option allows users to choose
to watch advertising, which induces perceived control.
Perceived control is a variable that affects individual attitudes and behavioral
intentions. When the degree of control is higher, individuals tend to have positive
attitudes and behaviors (Hui & Toffoli, 2002). Behavioral intentions are con-
strained by controlled conditions, such as ability, opportunity, and resources.
Perceived control drives behavioral intention: the more resources and opportuni-
ties an individual has or the more power in performing tasks, the higher the level
of perceived control and the stronger the behavioral intention (Ajzen, 1991).
Moreover, psychological reactance theory is based on the assumption that individ-
uals believe they have the power to make autonomous decisions (Brehm & Brehm,
2013). Compared with forced exposure, providing the skip-ad option probably
increases the endogenous motivation to produce positive intentions when individu-
als’ decisions are not disturbed by other factors (Moller et al., 2006).
In the context of online advertising, perceived control can also affect individu-
als’ responses, and the ability to choose freely increases the enthusiasm for proc-
essing information (Langer, 1975). Video users who lose control intend to resist
Chinese Journal of Communication 5
rather than obey, which leads to avoiding advertising, thus hindering communica-
tion between advertisements and users and defeating the purpose of advertising
(Edwards et al., 2002; Moller et al., 2006). Hence, this study is conducted to reveal
the importance of perceived control in skippable ad avoidance intention based on
Chinese video websites. Previous studies in the literature showed that returning
users’ control restored the freedom lost by ad intrusiveness and reduced negative
emotional responses (Jeon et al., 2019; Okazaki et al., 2012; Redondo & Aznar,
2018). Furthermore, the skip-ad option made users feel respect and freedom,
which lowered their avoidance intention (Youn & Kim, 2019). Therefore, we pro-
pose the following hypotheses:
Perceived intrusiveness
The term perceived intrusiveness refers to the psychological state caused by disrup-
tion to the individuals’ continuous cognitive process. The term is used to explain
audiences’ negative perceptions of advertising in a goal-oriented state (Hairong
et al., 2002). Previous studies showed that perceived intrusiveness was induced by
perceived goal impediment, advertising skepticism, and negative experience, mainly
based on unidirectional and forced advertising (Cho & Cheon, 2004; Edwards et al.,
2002; Kelly et al., 2010). Some researchers found that perceived intrusiveness was
induced by an interrupted target, information overload, or low relevance (Ha &
McCann, 2008; Jung, 2017). In the context of online video websites, advertising
intrusiveness was found to disrupt viewing goals, which could be aggravated when
exposure was forced. It has been confirmed that online advertising is more intrusive
than conventional advertising is (Hairong et al., 2002). Indeed, users were found to
be annoyed by video advertising because it interfered with their viewing experience,
which increased their perceptions of high intrusiveness (Cho & Cheon, 2004).
Regarding users who patronize platforms to watch video content, long-time and
low-quality unauthorized advertising might be perceived as an invasion of their pri-
vate space, which leads to the loss of perceived control, resulting in perceived intru-
siveness (Bailey et al., 2001; Kelly et al., 2010). Moreover, McCoy et al. (2008)
indicated that users who lacked the ability to remove control perceived higher intru-
siveness when advertising obscured the viewing content, leading to negative attitudes
and intentions. Youn and Kim (2019) used Facebook as an example, showing that
users felt a higher sense of autonomy and control if they could freely choose
whether to watch advertising, thereby lowering the perceived intrusiveness associated
with negative emotions and behaviors, such as irritation and avoidance.
According to the flow theory, when individuals exert all their mental power
into an activity, they are in a state of high concentration and excitement
(Csikszentmihalyi & LeFevre, 1989). However, the purpose of patch advertising is
to divert the attention from video watching to advertising, which breaks the nor-
mal flow. Moreover, being forced to watch advertising for a long time was found
to potentially increase internal anxiety (Belanche et al., 2017; Lee, 2011).
Moreover, in response to loud advertising noise, users allocated limited attention
selectively, thus inducing negative emotions (Ha & McCann, 2008). Indeed, as an
6 B. Li and S. Yin
H3: Perceived ad content quality moderates the strength of the mediated relationship
between perceived control and advertising avoidance intention.
H3a: The relationship might be relatively strong when perceived advertising content
quality is low.
H3b: The relationship might be relatively weak when perceived advertising content
quality is high.
Method
Data collection method and participants
Considering that Internet video users are generally young and familiar with skip-
pable advertising, we applied convenience sampling to select college students as
the participants in the study. The survey was divided into three parts. The first
part investigated the contact condition of video patch advertising to eliminate col-
lege students who had not experienced it. In the second and main part, 13 items
were developed according to the four variables in the model. The third part was
designed to elicit personal information. The data collection was conducted accord-
ing to standard requirements (Kline, 2015). We employed the structural equation
model (SEM) in the data analysis.
The final sample consisted of 302 subjects (117 males and 185 females); 79.14%
were aged between 18 and 22 years; 85.76% were educated undergraduates; and
25.50% spent more than two hours online each day.
According to the usage habits of most Chinese netizens, we listed the existing
video website platforms (e.g. Youku, iQiyi, and Tencent Video) to satisfy the plat-
form usage requirements of the subjects to the greatest extent. Then we applied a
one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) to determine whether different types of
video website platforms affected these three factors. The results showed no
8 B. Li and S. Yin
Measures
The variable scales were adopted from previous studies to ensure reliability and
validity. Before applying the scale, we used standard methods of translation and
back translation (Brislin, 1980). First, professional translators were invited to
translate the English scale into Chinese and then back into English. After the com-
parison, the scale was modified to ensure measurement equivalence. We also
altered the semantic structure of many items to adapt to skippable advertising in
the Chinese context.
According to the research purpose, the four latent variables were measured.
All items were rated on a seven-point Likert scale (from 1 ¼ completely disagree to
7 ¼ completely agree).
Perceived control. A four-item scale was adopted from previous studies (Jewell
& Kidwell, 2005; Youn & Kim, 2019), including “I feel I have the ability to skip
advertising,” “I feel I have a lot of control over skipping advertising.” Cronbach’s
a was 0.913, indicating that the reliability of perceived control performs well.
Perceived intrusiveness. Based on Hairong et al.’s (2002) original scale,
Redondo and Aznar’s (2018) was applied. Four items were designed, including
“Video patch advertising disturb my viewing experience,” “Video patch advertising
distracts my attention.” Cronbach’s a was 0.950, indicating that the reliability of
perceived intrusiveness performs well.
Perceived ad content quality. A two-item scale adapted from Hwang and Jeong
(2019) was applied, including “Video patch advertising provide sufficient information,”
“Video patch advertising contains necessary information.” Cronbach’s a was 0.844,
indicating that the reliability of perceived ad content quality performs well.
Ad avoidance intention. We measured using three items on Van den Broeck
et al.’s (2018) scale, including “I would immediately click the skip-ad button,” “I
would avoid watching video patch advertising.” Cronbach’s a was 0.733, indicat-
ing that the reliability of ad avoidance intention performs well.
Control variables. Previous research showed that gender influenced anxiety per-
ception and avoidance intention (Rojas-Mendez et al., 2009). Moreover, age, edu-
cation level, income, and online duration were also shown to be influential factors.
Therefore, we treated them as control variables.
Results
The hypotheses were tested using Amos 25.0 and SPSS PROCESS Macro (v3.3)
(Hayes, 2017). SEM should be used in a two-step procedure (Anderson &
Gerbing, 1988). First, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was employed to
Chinese Journal of Communication 9
process the model comparison and determine whether the variables were independ-
ent of each other. When the various fitting indicators achieved the standard
requirements, a path analysis was performed.
Measurement model
The maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method was employed to measure
internal consistency, discriminant validity, and convergent validity. The compari-
son of different factor models (Table 1) showed that the four-factor model was the
most suitable (v2/df ¼ 2.741, GFI ¼ 0.926, RMSEA ¼ 0.076, CFI ¼ 0.968,
SRMR ¼ 0.077) and achieved the standard values (Kline, 2015). First, the min-
imum CR value of internal consistency was 0.765, which was greater than the crit-
ical value of 0.6 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). Second, the minimum AVE value of
discriminant validity was 0.530, which exceeded the critical value of 0.5 (Fornell &
Larcker, 1981). All AVEs were larger than the MSVs and ASVs (Table 2). The
correlations were from –0.694 to 0.400, below the square root of AVE, and signifi-
cantly less than 1.0 (Bagozzi et al., 1991). Finally, the standardized factor loading
of each latent variable was between 0.61 and 0.98, thus reaching the recommended
cutoff values (Hair et al., 2013).
Mediation analysis
The mediating effect was tested using the SPSS PROCESS Macro (Model 4).
Table 3 shows the results of the regression analysis of the mediating effect.
Perceived control negatively affected advertising avoidance intention (H1; b ¼
0.323, p < 0.001). Moreover, the negative effect of perceived control on perceived
10 B. Li and S. Yin
Bootstrap 95%CI
Effect b Boot SE t-value Lower Upper
PC!PI –0.583 0.063 –10.283 –0.696 –0.450
PI!AAI 0.135 0.052 3.120 0.035 0.241
PC!AAI –0.323 0.053 –7.539 –0.423 –0.215
Note: All estimated values shown are nonstandardized coefficients. Because of limited space, the
regression effects of the control variables were not reported; 5,000 (resamples), 95% (confidence level),
and significance at 0.001.
intrusiveness was the most significant (b ¼ 0.583, p < 0.001), and perceived intru-
siveness positively affected advertising avoidance intention (b ¼ 0.135, p < 0.001).
To test the mediation effect, we conducted a bootstrapping analysis, which is
the mainstream method currently used, to determine the effects of perceived intru-
siveness on the relationship between perceived control and advertising avoidance
intention. The results were as follows (see Table 4). The mediating effect was
–0.079 (CI ¼ [–0.143, 0.021]), which indicates that the mediating effect of
“perceived control ! perceived intrusiveness ! ad avoidance intention” was sig-
nificant. Moreover, the confidence interval of direct effect was from –0.341 to
–0.147, indicating that the direct effect was significant. These results indicated that
perceived intrusiveness played a partial mediating role in the indirect path. Thus,
H2 is supported.
We also employed Amos 25.0 to analyze the path of the structural model to
further verify the hypotheses. The results showed that perceived control and
Chinese Journal of Communication 11
moderator variable clearly and directly (Preacher et al., 2007). In Figure 2, the
straight line represents the moderated mediation effect, and the dotted line repre-
sents the 95% confidence band. When the value of perceived ad content quality
ranged from 1.626 to 5.331—the full seven points—the indirect effect of perceived
intrusiveness on the path from perceived control to advertising avoidance intention
was significant.
Implications
From an economic perspective, China has become the second largest advertising
market compared with the USA. Moreover, video patch advertising is the main-
stream delivery model in China at present. Hence, in this study, we explored the
potential influence of users’ avoidance intention on skippable video patch advertis-
ing. It was beneficial to advertisers and operators to collect data on advertising
14 B. Li and S. Yin
clicks and dwelling time through background management functions, filter user
portraits interested in advertising, and formulate accurate advertising strategies sci-
entifically and effectively. The results of our study have implications for adver-
tisers to improve the quality of advertising content, eliminate poor-quality
advertising, and transform marketing effectiveness.
This study has contributed to the literature on user behavioral intention in both
theory and practice, especially regarding avoidance intention in online video adver-
tising. The results of this empirical research are consistent with previous studies,
thus narrowing the research gap and enriching the theoretical knowledge about
skippable video patch advertising. Moreover, our study is valuable because there
was little previous research on how skippable video advertising, designed as an
interactive media tool, might be recreated in the personal spaces of college students
(Kelly et al., 2010), who are representatives of the net generation, thereby providing
a new perspective and extending the research on online advertising avoidance inten-
tion. Indeed, the original purpose of advertising was to create and distribute persua-
sive information to enable better communication with audiences (Schultz, 2016). In
the context of online video websites, the form of mandatory advertising exposure is
not valid because users want to access video content. In addition, the appearance of
an “unexpected guest” may induce psychological reactance, which is not conducive
to brand preference and the continuance usage intention of platforms. It may even
cause users to avoid advertising, shut down web pages, or use ad blockers.
China’s culture of high-power distance, users might be accustomed to the sta-
tus quo of advertising on video websites, and they rarely voice their opinions.
However, with the awakening of individual consciousness, users are also striving
to initiate power distribution. They intend to have the power to decide rather than
sacrifice their freedom to obtain video content. Hence, it is necessary to give users
a free hand in choosing the number and duration of advertising they watch
according to their needs and preferences. Instead of allowing users to avoid adver-
tising entirely by using ad blockers, video operators should use skippable advertis-
ing with high-quality content. The new model developed in the present study may
arouse the curiosity and favorability of users (Pihlstr€ om & Brush, 2008). High-
quality content also helps to increase pleasure, improve viewing experiences, and
promote acceptance willingness (Chandrasekaran et al., 2018). Our results sup-
ported that perceived control is induced when the skip-ad option is provided, thus
lessening perceived intrusiveness. In addition, combined with high-quality advertis-
ing content, advertising avoidance intention may be reduced.
Users who have experienced skippable advertising in China could be regarded
as innovators although their innovative behavior may be caused by personal char-
acteristics such as curiosity (Pihlstr€
om & Brush, 2008). It is necessary to investi-
gate the early adopters’ intention to avoid advertising because based on their
perception of skippable video patch advertising, we could not only predict the
avoidance intention and behavior of future users but also provide suggestions and
references for Chinese advertisers and video operators to follow the skippable
advertising model or maintain their present model.
Considering different social contexts and business models and exploring the
behavioral preferences for skippable advertising are beneficial for the overall opti-
mization of Chinese video operators as well as the promotion of user growth and
website traffic. Moreover, the findings of the present study have valuable implica-
tions for international video operators that plan to expand their business into
Chinese Journal of Communication 15
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the editors and anonymous reviewers for their helpful
suggestions and comments on the article.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Notes on contributors
Bin Li takes a successive postgraduate and doctoral program in the Department of Business
Administration, School of Management, Huazhong University of Science and Technology,
China. His current research interests include consumers’ cognitive and emotional processing
of advertising, online social support, marketing, and persuasion communications in the
interactive media environment.
Shimin Yin is Assistant Professor in the Department of Marketing, Business School of
Anhui University, China. He holds a Ph.D. in Business Management and an M.A. in
Public Administration from Sichuan University, China. Yin’s teaching and research
interests are focused on basic and applied research in new media marketing, tourism
marketing, and integrated marketing communication.
16 B. Li and S. Yin
ORCID
Bin Li http://orcid.org/0000-0003-3303-7304
References
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A
review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), 411–423.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411
Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. W. (1991). Assessing construct validity in
organizational research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36(3), 421–458. https://doi.
org/10.2307/2393203
Bailey, B. P., Konstan, J. A., Carlis, J. V. (2001). The effects of interruptions on task
performance, annoyance, and anxiety in the user interface. Paper presented at the
Interact. https://www.cse.unr.edu/sushil/class/ps/papers/EffectInterruptions-interact-
2001.pdf
Bang, H., Kim, J., & Choi, D. (2018). Exploring the effects of ad-task relevance and ad
salience on ad avoidance: The moderating role of internet use motivation. Computers in
Human Behavior, 89, 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.07.020
Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1993). A simple theory of advertising as a good or bad.
The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(4), 941–964. https://doi.org/10.2307/2118455
Becker-Olsen, K. L. (2003). And now, a word from our sponsor: A look at the effects of
sponsored content and banner advertising. Journal of Advertising, 32(2), 17–32. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2003.10639130
Belanche, D., Flavian, C., & Perez-Rueda, A. (2017). Understanding interactive online
advertising: Congruence and product involvement in highly and lowly arousing,
skippable video advertising. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 37, 75–88. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.intmar.2016.06.004
Bhatnagar, N., & Wan, F. (2011). Is self-character similarity always beneficial? Journal of
Advertising, 40(2), 39–50. https://doi.org/10.2753/JOA0091-3367400203
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (2013). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and
control. Academic Press.
Brislin, R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis of oral and written materials.
Methodology, 2, 389–444.
Carroll, A., Barnes, S. J., Scornavacca, E., & Fletcher, K. (2007). Consumer perceptions
and attitudes towards SMS advertising: Recent evidence from New Zealand.
International Journal of Advertising, 26(1), 79–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2007.
11072997
Chandrasekaran, D., Srinivasan, R., & Sihi, D. (2018). Effects of offline ad content on
online brand search: Insights from super bowl advertising. Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science, 46(3), 403–430. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-017-0551-8
Cho, C.-H., & Cheon, H. J. (2004). Why do people avoid advertising on the Internet?
Journal of Advertising, 33(4), 89–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2004.10639175
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 815–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.56.5.815
Edwards, S. M., Li, H. R., & Lee, J. H. (2002). Forced exposure and psychological
reactance: Antecedents and consequences of the perceived intrusiveness of pop-up
advertising. Journal of Advertising, 31(3), 83–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2002.
10673678
Elpers, J. L. W., Wedel, M., & Pieters, R. G. (2003). Why do consumers stop viewing
television commercials? Two experiments on the influence of moment-to-moment
Chinese Journal of Communication 17
Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
32(2), 311–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311
Lee, C. (2011). What happens if online video advertising is skippable? Testing a hierarchical
model of advertising avoidance. University of Florida.
Lee, C. S. & Ma, L. (2012). News sharing in social media: The effect of gratifications and
prior experience. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(2), 331–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.chb.2011.10.002
Li, W., & Huang, Z. (2016). The research of influence factors of online behavioral
advertising avoidance. American Journal of Industrial and Business Management, 6(9),
947–957. https://doi.org/10.4236/ajibm.2016.69092
Lynch, J., John, G., & Srull, T. K. (1982). Memory and attentional factors in consumer
choice: Concepts and research methods. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 18–37.
https://doi.org/10.1086/208893
MAGNA. (2019). Global advertising forecast. https://magnaglobal.com/magna-advertising-
forecasts-summer-2019-update/
McCoy, S., Everard, A., Polak, P., & Galletta, D. F. (2008). An experimental study
of antecedents and consequences of online ad intrusiveness. International Journal of
Human-Computer Interaction, 24(7), 672–699. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447310802335664
Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The
moderating role of autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8),
1024–1036. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288008
Okazaki, S., Molina, F. J., & Hirose, M. (2012). Mobile advertising avoidance: Exploring
the role of ubiquity. Electronic Markets, 22(3), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12525-
012-0087-1
Pashkevich, M., Dorai-Raj, S., Kellar, M., & Zigmond, D. (2012). Empowering online
advertisements by empowering viewers with the right to choose: The relative
effectiveness of skippable video advertisements on YouTube. Journal of Advertising
Research, 52(4), 451–457. https://doi.org/10.2501/JAR-52-4-451-457
Pihlstr€
om, M., & Brush, G. J. (2008). Comparing the perceived value of information and
entertainment mobile services. Psychology and Marketing, 25(8), 732–755. https://doi.
org/10.1002/mar.20236
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation
hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
42(1), 185–227. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170701341316
Quick, B. L., & Stephenson, M. T. (2007). Further evidence that psychological reactance
can be modeled as a combination of anger and negative cognitions. Communication
Research, 34(3), 255–276. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650207300427
Redondo, I., & Aznar, G. (2018). To use or not to use ad blockers? The roles of knowledge
of ad blockers and attitude toward online advertising. Telematics and Informatics,
35(6), 1607–1616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2018.04.008
Rodgers, S., & Thorson, E. (2000). The interactive advertising model: How users perceive
and process online advertising. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 1(1), 41–60. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2000.10722043
Rojas-Mendez, J. I., Davies, G., & Madran, C. (2009). Universal differences in advertising
avoidance behavior: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Business Research, 62(10),
947–954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2008.08.008
Schultz, D. (2016). The future of advertising or whatever we’re going to call it. Journal of
Advertising, 45(3), 276–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2016.1185061
SocialMediaToday. (2019). Global ad blocking behavior. https://www.socialmediatoday.com/
news/global-ad-blocking-behavior-2019-infographic/
Speck, P. S., & Elliott, M. T. (1997). Predictors of advertising avoidance in print and
broadcast media. Journal of Advertising, 26(3), 61–76. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.
1997.10673529
Van den Broeck, E., Poels, K., & Walrave, M. (2018). An experimental study
on the effect of ad placement, product involvement and motives on Facebook
ad avoidance. Telematics and Informatics, 35(2), 470–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.tele.2018.01.006
Chinese Journal of Communication 19
Wang, Y., & Sun, S. J. (2010). Assessing beliefs, attitudes, and behavioral responses toward
online advertising in three countries. International Business Review, 19(4), 333–344.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibusrev.2010.01.004
Wilbur, K. C. (2016). Advertising content and television advertising avoidance. Journal of
Media Economics, 29(2), 51–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/08997764.2016.1170022
Woller, K. M., Buboltz, W. C., & Loveland, J. M. (2007). Psychological reactance:
Examination across age, ethnicity, and gender. The American Journal of Psychology,
120(1), 15–24. doi:10.2307/20445379
Youn, S., & Kim, S. (2019). Understanding ad avoidance on Facebook: Antecedents and
outcomes of psychological reactance. Computers in Human Behavior, 98, 232–244.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.04.025
YouTube. (2016). How it works. https://www.google.es/advertising/video/advertisers/benefits.
html
Zhang, J., & Mao, E. (2016). From online motivations to ad clicks and to behavioral
intentions: An empirical study of consumer response to social media advertising.
Psychology & Marketing, 33(3), 155–164. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20862