Neal-Maes - Twins and Families

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 308

Methodology for Genetic

Studies of
Twins and Families
Michael C. Neale
Department of Psychiatry
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics
Virginia Commonwealth University
Hermine H. M. Maes
Department of Human Genetics
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics
Virginia Commonwealth University
Kluwer Academic Publishers B.V.
Dordrecht, The Netherlands
3
CONTRIBUTING AUTHORS
Lindon J. Eaves Department of Human Genetics
Medical College of Virginia
John K. Hewitt Department of Human Genetics
Medical College of Virginia
Joanne M. Meyer Department of Human Genetics
Medical College of Virginia
Michael C. Neale Department of Human Genetics
Medical College of Virginia
Hermine H. M. Maes Institute of Physical Education
Catholic University Leuven
Dorret I. Boomsma Department of Experimental Psychology
Free University, Amsterdam
Conor V. Dolan Department of Psychology
University of Amsterdam
Peter C. M. Molenaar Department of Psychology
University of Amsterdam
Karl G. J oreskog Department of Statistics
University of Uppsala
Nicholas G. Martin Queensland Institute of Medical
Research
Lon R. Cardon Institute for Behavioral Genetics
University of Colorado
David W. Fulker Institute for Behavioral Genetics
University of Colorado
Andrew C. Heath Department of Psychiatry
Washington University School of Medicine
Contents
1 The Scope of Genetic Analyses 1
1.1 Introduction and General Aims . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
1.2 Heredity and Variation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
1.2.1 Variation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
1.2.2 Familial Resemblance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
1.2.3 Within Family Dierences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
1.3 Building and Fitting Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
1.4 Elements of a Model: Causes of Variation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
1.4.1 Genetic Eects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12
1.4.2 Environmental Eects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
1.4.3 Genotype-Environment Eects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
1.5 Relationships between Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
1.5.1 Causes of Correlation between Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
1.5.2 Direction of Causation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
1.5.3 Developmental Change . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
1.6 The Context of our Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
1.6.1 Early History . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
1.6.2 19th Century Origins . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
1.6.3 Genetic, Factor, and Path Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
1.6.4 Integration of the Biometrical and Path-Analytic Approaches 30
1.6.5 Development of Statistical Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
2 Data Preparation 35
2.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
2.2 Continuous Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
2.2.1 Calculating Summary Statistics by Hand . . . . . . . . . . . 36
2.2.2 Using SAS to Summarize Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
2.2.3 Using PRELIS to Summarize Continuous Data . . . . . . . . 41
2.3 Ordinal Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
2.3.1 Univariate Normal Distribution of Liability . . . . . . . . . . 44
2.3.2 Bivariate Normal Distribution of Liability . . . . . . . . . . . 45
i
ii CONTENTS
2.3.3 Testing the Normal Distribution Assumption . . . . . . . . . 48
2.3.4 Terminology for Types of Correlation . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
2.3.5 Using PRELIS to Summarize Ordinal Data . . . . . . . . . . 50
2.4 Preparing Raw Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53
2.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54
3 Biometrical Genetics 55
3.1 Introduction and Description of Terminology . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
3.2 Breeding Experiments: Gametic Crosses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57
3.3 Derivation of Expected Twin Covariances . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60
3.3.1 Equal Gene Frequencies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60
3.3.2 Unequal Gene Frequencies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
3.4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
4 Matrix Algebra 71
4.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.2 Matrix Notation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.3 Matrix Algebra Operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
4.3.1 Binary Operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
4.3.2 Unary Operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
4.4 Equations in Matrix Algebra . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
4.5 Applications of Matrix Algebra . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
4.5.1 Calculation of Covariance Matrix from Data Matrix . . . . . 82
4.5.2 Transformations of Data Matrices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83
4.5.3 Further Operations and Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
4.6 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
4.6.1 Binary operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
4.6.2 Unary operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
5 Path Analysis and Structural Equations 87
5.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87
5.2 Conventions Used in Path Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
5.3 Assumptions of Path Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
5.4 Tracing Rules of Path Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
5.4.1 Tracing Rules for Standardized Variables . . . . . . . . . . . 92
5.4.2 Tracing Rules for Unstandardized Variables . . . . . . . . . . 92
5.5 Path Models for Linear Regression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
5.6 Path Models for the Classical Twin Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98
5.6.1 Path Coecients Model: Standardized Tracing Rules . . . . . 99
5.6.2 Variance Components Model: Unstandardized Tracing Rules 101
5.7 Identication of Models and Parameters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
5.8 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106
CONTENTS iii
6 Univariate Analysis 109
6.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
6.2 Fitting Genetic Models to Continuous Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
6.2.1 Basic Genetic Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
6.2.2 Body Mass Index in Twins . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112
6.2.3 Building a Path Coecients Model Mx Script . . . . . . . . . 115
6.2.4 Interpreting the Mx Output . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
6.2.5 Building a Variance Components Model Mx Script . . . . . . 123
6.2.6 Interpreting Univariate Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
6.2.7 Testing the Equality of Means . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127
6.2.8 Incorporating Data from Singleton Twins . . . . . . . . . . . 129
6.2.9 Conclusions: Genetic Analyses of BMI Data . . . . . . . . . . 131
6.3 Fitting Genetic Models to Binary Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132
6.3.1 Major Depressive Disorder in Twins . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132
6.4 Model for Age-Correction of Twin Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136
7 Power and Sample Size 141
7.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
7.2 Factors Contributing to Power . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
7.3 Steps in Power Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142
7.4 Power for the continuous case . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144
7.5 Loss of Power with Ordinal Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146
7.6 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148
8 Social Interaction 151
8.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
8.2 Basic Univariate Model without Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
8.3 Sibling Interaction Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153
8.3.1 Application to CBC Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
8.4 Consequences for Variation and Covariation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156
8.4.1 Derivation of Expected Covariances . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156
8.4.2 Numerical Illustration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159
9 Sex-limitation and G E Interaction 161
9.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 161
9.2 Sex-limitation Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162
9.2.1 General Model for Sex-limitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162
9.2.2 General Sex-limitation Model Mx Script . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
9.2.3 Restricted Models for Sex-limitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165
9.2.4 Application to Body Mass Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
9.3 Genotype Environment Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
9.3.1 Models for G E Interactions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170
9.3.2 Application to Marital Status and Depression . . . . . . . . . 172
iv CONTENTS
10 Multivariate Analysis 177
10.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
10.2 Phenotypic Factor Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 178
10.2.1 Exploratory and Conrmatory Factor Models . . . . . . . . . 178
10.2.2 Building a Phenotypic Factor Model Mx Script . . . . . . . . 179
10.2.3 Fitting a Phenotypic Factor Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180
10.3 Simple Genetic Factor Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
10.3.1 Multivariate Genetic Factor Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182
10.3.2 Alternate Representation of Genetic Factor Model . . . . . . 184
10.3.3 Fitting the Multivariate Genetic Model . . . . . . . . . . . . 185
10.3.4 Fitting a Second Genetic Factor . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188
10.4 Multiple Genetic Factor Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189
10.4.1 Genetic and Environmental Correlations . . . . . . . . . . . . 189
10.4.2 Cholesky Decomposition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190
10.4.3 Analyzing Genetic and Environmental Correlations . . . . . . 192
10.5 Common vs. Independent Pathway Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194
10.5.1 Independent Pathway Model for Atopy . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195
10.5.2 Common Pathway Model for Atopy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 198
11 Observer Ratings 201
11.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201
11.2 Models for Multiple Rating Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201
11.2.1 Rater Bias Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 203
11.2.2 Psychometric Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 205
11.2.3 Biometric Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 207
11.2.4 Comparison of Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 208
11.2.5 Application to CBC Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 210
11.2.6 Discussion of CBC Application . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 212
12 Repeated Measures 217
12.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 217
12.2 Phenotypic Simplex Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 218
12.2.1 Formulation of the Phenotypic Simplex Model in Mx . . . . . 219
12.3 Genetic Simplex Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 222
12.3.1 Mx Formulation of the Genetic Simplex Model . . . . . . . . 223
12.3.2 Application to Longitudinal Data on Weight . . . . . . . . . 224
12.3.3 Common Factor Model for Longitudinal Twin Data . . . . . 227
12.4 Problems with Repeated Measures Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 228
12.5 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 228
CONTENTS v
A Mx Scripts for Univariate Models 229
A.1 Path Coecients Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 229
A.2 Variance Components Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 230
A.3 Model for Means and Covariances . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 231
A.4 Univariate Genetic Model for Twin pairs and Singles . . . . . . . . . 233
A.5 Age-correction Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 235
B Mx Script for Power Calculation 237
B.1 ACE Model for Power Calculations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
C Mx Script for Sibling Interaction Model 241
C.1 Sibling Interaction Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 241
D Mx Scripts for Sex and GE Interaction 243
D.1 General Model for Scalar Sex-Limitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 243
D.2 Scalar Sex-Limitation Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 246
D.3 General Model for G E Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 248
D.4 Scalar G E interaction model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 251
E Mx Scripts for Multivariate Models 255
E.1 Phenotypic Factor Analysis of Four Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . 255
E.2 Genetic Factor Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 256
E.3 Bivariate Genetic Factor Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 257
E.4 Genetic Cholesky Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 258
E.5 Independent Pathway Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 259
E.6 Common Pathway Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 260
F Mx Script for Rater Bias Model 263
F.1 Rater Bias Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 263
F.2 CBC Items for Internalizing Scale Score . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 266
G Mx Script and Data for Simplex Model 269
G.1 Phenotypic Simplex Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 269
G.2 Genetic Simplex Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 271
Bibliography 272
Index 286
vi CONTENTS
List of Tables
2.1 Simulated measurements from MZ and DZ Twin Pairs. . . . . . . . . 37
2.2 Classication of correlations according to their observed distribution. 51
3.1 Punnett square for mating between two heterozygous parents. . . . . 58
3.2 Genetic covariance components for twins and siblings with equal
gene frequencies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61
3.3 Genetic covariance components for twins and siblings with unequal
gene frequencies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
6.1 Twin correlations and summary statistics for Body Mass Index in
the Australian Twin Register . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
6.2 Polynomial regression of absolute intra-pair dierence in BMI . . . . 114
6.3 Twin covariances for BMI . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115
6.4 Results of tting models to covariance matrices for BMI . . . . . . . 125
6.5 Standardized parameter estimates under best-tting model of BMI . 127
6.6 Model comparisons for BMI analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128
6.7 Means and variances for BMI of twins whose cotwin did not coop-
erate in the 1981 Australian survey. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130
6.8 Model tting results for BMI data from concordant-participant and
discordant-participant twin pairs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
6.9 Contingency tables of twin pair diagnosis of lifetime Major Depres-
sive Disorder in Virginia adult female twins. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133
6.10 Parameter estimates and goodness-of-t statistics for models of Ma-
jor Depressive Disorder . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134
6.11 Parameter estimates for Conservatism in Australian female twins . . 136
6.12 Age corrected parameter estimates for Conservatism . . . . . . . . . 139
7.1 Non-central value for power calculations of 1 df at = 0.05 . . . . 145
8.1 Preliminary results of model tting to externalizing behavior prob-
lems in Virginia boys from larger families. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
8.2 Parameter estimates and goodness of t statistics from tting models
of sibling interaction to CBC data. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156
vii
viii LIST OF TABLES
8.3 Eects of sibling interaction(s) on variance and covariance compo-
nents between pairs of relatives. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159
8.4 Eects of strong sibling interaction on the variance and covariance
between MZ, DZ, and unrelated individuals reared together . . . . . 160
9.1 Sample sizes and correlations for BMI data in Virginia and AARP
twins. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
9.2 Parameter estimates from tting genotype sex interaction models
to BMI. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
9.3 Sample sizes and correlations for depression data in Australian fe-
male twins. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173
9.4 Parameter estimates from tting genotype marriage interaction
models to depression scores. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
10.1 Observed correlations among arithmetic computation variables be-
fore and after doses of alcohol in Australian twins . . . . . . . . . . . 180
10.2 Parameter estimates and expected covariance matrix from the phe-
notypic factor model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182
10.3 Observed MZ and DZ twin correlations for arithmetic computation
variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
10.4 Parameter estimates from the full genetic common factor model . . . 186
10.5 Parameter estimates from the reduced genetic common factor model 187
10.6 Parameter estimates from the two genetic factors model . . . . . . . 189
10.7 Covariance matrices for skinfold measures in adolescent Virginian
male twins. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 193
10.8 Parameter estimates of the cholesky factors in the genetic and envi-
ronmental covariance matrices. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194
10.9 Maximum-likelihood estimates of genetic and environmental covari-
ance (above the diagnoals) and correlation (below the diagonals)
matrices for skinfold measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194
10.10Tetrachoric MZ and DZ correlations for asthma, hayfever, dust al-
lergy, and eczema in Australian twins . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195
10.11Parameter estimates from the independent pathway model for atopy 197
10.12Parameter estimates from the common pathway model for atopy . . 200
11.1 Observed variance-covariance and correlation matrices for parental
ratings of internalizing behavior problems in Virginia twins . . . . . 210
11.2 Model comparisons for internalizing problems analysis. . . . . . . . . 211
11.3 Parameter estimates from tting bias, psychometric, and biometric
models for parental ratings of internalizing behaviors. . . . . . . . . 212
11.4 Contributions to the phenotypic variances and covariance of moth-
ers and fathers ratings of young boys internalizing behavior. . . . . 213
LIST OF TABLES ix
12.1 Within-person correlations for weight measured at six-month inter-
vals on 66 females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 218
12.2 Parameter estimates and total variances using script 1 for weight . . 221
12.3 Parameter estimates and total variances using script 2 for weight . . 222
12.4 Correlations among latent factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 222
12.5 Genetic, environmental, and total phenotypic variances estimated
from the genetic simplex model applied to Fishbeins (1977) data on
weight . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 226
12.6 Estimated correlations among latent genetic [below diagonal] and
environmental [above diagonal] factors in a genetic simplex model
tted to weight data. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 227
x LIST OF TABLES
List of Figures
1.1 Variability in self reported weight in a sample of US twins. . . . . . 3
1.2 Two scatterplots of weight of DZ twins and unrelated individuals . . 6
1.3 Correlations for body mass index and conservatism between relatives 8
1.4 Scatterplot of weight in a large sample of MZ twins. . . . . . . . . . 9
1.5 Bar chart of weight dierences within twin pairs . . . . . . . . . . . 10
1.6 Diagram of the interrelationship between theory, model and empir-
ical observation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
1.7 Diagram of the intellectual traditions leading to modern mathemat-
ical genetic methodology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28
2.1 Univariate normal distribution with thresholds distinguishing or-
dered response categories. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
2.2 Contour and 3-D plots of the bivariate normal distribution . . . . . 46
2.3 Contour plots of bivariate normal distribution and a mixture of bi-
variate normal distributions showing one threshold in each dimension 47
2.4 Contour plots of bivariate normal distribution and a mixture of bi-
variate normal distributions showing two thresholds in each dimension 48
3.1 The d and h increments of the gene dierence A a . . . . . . . . . 57
3.2 Regression of genotypic eects on gene dosage . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
4.1 Graphical representation of the inner product of vector . . . . . . . . 75
4.2 Geometric representation of the determinant of a matrix . . . . . . . 76
5.1 Path diagram for three latent (A, B and C) and two observed (D
and E) variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89
5.2 Regression path models with manifest variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
5.3 Alternative representations of the basic genetic model . . . . . . . . 100
5.4 Regression path models with multiple indicators. . . . . . . . . . . . 107
6.1 Univariate genetic model for MZ or DZ twins reared together . . . . 116
6.2 Path model for age, additive genetic, shared environment, and spe-
cic environment eects on phenotypes of pairs of twins . . . . . . . 138
xi
xii LIST OF FIGURES
8.1 Basic path diagram for univariate twin data. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152
8.2 Path diagram for univariate twin data with sibling interaction. . . . 154
8.3 Path diagram showing inuence of arbitrary exogenous variable X
on phenotype P in a pair of relatives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157
9.1 General genotype sex interaction model for twin data . . . . . . . 163
9.2 Scalar genotype sex interaction model for twin data . . . . . . . . 166
9.3 General genotype environment interaction model for twin data . . 171
10.1 Multivariate Genetic Factor model for four variables . . . . . . . . . 183
10.2 Phenotypic Cholesky decomposition model for four variables . . . . . 191
10.3 Independent pathway model for four variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . 196
10.4 Common pathway model for four variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 199
11.1 Model for ratings of a pair of twins by their parents . . . . . . . . . 204
11.2 Psychometric or common pathway model for ratings of a pair of
twins by their parents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 206
11.3 Biometric or independent pathway model for ratings of a pair of
twins by their parents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 208
11.4 Diagram of nesting of biometric, psychometric, and rater bias models.209
12.1 Simplex model for a phenotypic time series. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 219
12.2 Simplex model of genetic and within-family environmental time series224
Chapter 1
The Scope of Genetic
Analyses
1.1 Introduction and General Aims
This book has its origin in a week-long intensive course on methods of twin data
analysis taught between 1987 and 1997 at the Katholieke Universiteit of Leuven
in Belgium, the University of Helsinki, Finland, and the Institute for Behavioral
Genetics, Boulder, in Colorado. Our principal aim here is to help those interested
in the genetic analysis of individual dierences to realize that there are more chal-
lenging questions than simply Is trait X genetic? or What is the heritability of
X? and that there are more exible and informative methods than those that have
been popular for more than half a century. We shall achieve this goal primarily
by considering those analyses of data on twins that can be conducted with the Mx
program. There are two main reasons for this restriction: 1) the basic structure
and logic of the twin design is simple and yet can illustrate many of the conceptual
and practical issues that need to be addressed in any genetic study of individual
dierences; 2) the Mx program is well-documented, freely available for personal
computers and Unix workstations, and can be used to apply all of the basic ideas
we shall discuss. We believe that the material to be presented will open many new
horizons to investigators in a wide range of disciplines and provide them with the
tools to begin to explore their own data more fruitfully.
The four main aims of this introductory chapter are:
1. to identify some of the scientic questions which have aroused the curiosity
of investigators and led them to develop the approaches we describe
2. to trace part of the intellectual tradition that led us to the approach we are
to present in this text
1
2 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
3. to outline the overall logical structure of the approach
4. to accomplish all of these with the minimum of statistics and mathematics.
Before starting on what we are going to do, however, it is important to point out
what we are not going to cover. There will be almost nothing in this book about
detecting the contribution of individual loci of large eect against the background
of other genetic and environmental eects (segregation analysis). In contrast to
the rst edition, there will be a chapter on linkage analysis concerning the location
on the genome of individual genes of major eect, if they exist. These issues have
been treated extensively elsewhere (see e.g., Ott, 1985, Sham, 1998, Lange, 1997,
Lynch & Walsh, 1998) often to the exclusion of issues that may still turn out to
be equally important, such as those outlined in this chapter. When the history of
genetic epidemiology is written, we believe that the approaches described here will
be credited with revealing the naivete of many of the simple assumptions about
the action of genes and environment that are usually made in the search for single
loci of large eect. Our work may thus be seen in the context of exploring those
parameters of the coaction of genes and environment which are frequently not
considered in conventional segregation and linkage analysis.
1.2 Heredity and Variation
Genetic epidemiology is impelled by three basic questions:
1. Why isnt everyone the same?
2. Why are children like their parents?
3. Why arent children from the same parents all alike?
These questions address variation within individuals and covariation between rela-
tives. As we shall show, covariation between relatives can provide useful informa-
tion about variation within individuals.
1.2.1 Variation
In this section we shall examine the ubiquity of variability, and its distinction from
mean levels in populations and sub-populations.
Variation is Everywhere
Almost everything that can be measured or counted in humans shows variation
around the mean value for the population. Figure 1.1 shows the pattern of varia-
tion for self-reported weight (lb.) in a U.S. sample. The observation that individ-
uals dier is almost universal and covers the entire spectrum of measurable traits,
1.2. HEREDITY AND VARIATION 3
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
1100
4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 5.5 5.6 5.7 5.8
Figure 1.1: Variability in self reported weight in a sample of US twins.
whether they be physical such as stature or weight, physiological such as heart rate
or blood pressure, or psychological such as personality, abilities, mental health, or
attitudes. The methods we shall describe are concerned with explaining why these
dierences occur.
Beyond the a priori Approach
As far as possible, the analyses we use are designed to be agnostic about the causes
of variation in a particular variable. Unfortunately, the same absence of a priori
bias is not always found among our scientic peers! A referee once wrote in a report
on a manuscript describing a twin study:
It is probably alright to use the twin study to estimate the genetic
contribution to variables which you know are genetic like stature and
weight, and its probably alright for things like blood pressure. But it
certainly cant be used for behavioral traits which we know are environ-
mental like social attitudes!
Such a crass remark nevertheless serves a useful purpose because it illustrates an
important principle which we should strive to satisfy, namely to nd methods that
are trait-independent; that is, they do not depend for their validity on investigators
a priori beliefs about what causes variation in a particular trait. Such considera-
4 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
tions may give weight to choosing one study design rather than another, but they
cannot be used to decide whether we should believe our results when we have them.
Biometrical Genetical and Epidemiological Approaches
Approaches that use genetic manipulation, natural or articial, to uncover latent
(i.e. unmeasured) genetic and environmental causes of variation are sometimes
called biometrical genetical (see e.g. Mather and Jinks, 1982). The methods may
be contrasted to the more conventional ones used in individual dierences, chiey in
the areas of psychology, sociology and epidemiology. The conventional approaches
try to explain variation in one set of measures (the dependent variables) by refer-
ences to dierences in another set of measures (independent variables). For exam-
ple, the risk for cardiovascular and lung diseases might be assumed to be dependent
variables, and cigarette smoking, alcohol use, and life stress independent variables.
A fundamental problem with this epidemiological approach is that its conclu-
sions about causality can be seriously misleading. Erroneous inferences would be
made if both the dependent and independent variables were caused by the same
latent genetic and environmental variables (see e.g., Chapters 6 and 10).
Not Much Can Be Said About Means
It is vital to remember that almost every result in this book, and every conclusion
that others obtain using these methods, relate to the causes of human dierences,
and may have almost nothing to do with the processes that account for the de-
velopment of the mean expression of a trait in a particular population. We are
necessarily concerned with what makes people vary around the mean of the pop-
ulation, race or species from which they are sampled. Suppose, for example, we
were to nd that dierences in social attitudes had a very large genetic component
of variation among U.S. citizens. What would that imply about the role of culture
in the determination of social attitudes? It could imply several things. First, it
might mean that culture is so uniform that only genetic eects are left to account
for dierences. Second, it might mean that cultural changes are adopted so rapidly
that environmental eects are not apparent. A trivial example may make this clear.
It is possible that understanding the genetic causes of variation in stature among
humans may identify the genes responsible for the dierence in stature between
humans and chimpanzees, but it is by no means certain. Neither would a demon-
stration that all human variation in stature was due to the environment lead us
to assume that the dierences between humans and chimpanzees were not genetic.
This point is stressed because, whatever subsequent genetic research on population
and species dierences might establish, there is no necessary connection between
what is true of the typical human and what causes variation around the central
tendency. For this reason, it is important to avoid such short-hand expressions
as height is genetic when really we mean individual dierences in height are
mainly genetic.
1.2. HEREDITY AND VARIATION 5
Variation and Modication
What has been said about means also extends to making claims about intervention.
The causes of variation that emerge from twin and family studies relate to a par-
ticular population of genotypes at a specic time in its evolutionary and cultural
history. Factors that change the gene frequencies, the expression of gene eects, or
the frequencies of the dierent kinds of environment may all aect the outcome of
our studies. Furthermore, if we show that genetic eects are important, the possi-
bility that a rare but highly potent environmental agent is present cannot entirely
be discounted. Similarly, a rare gene of major eect may hold the key to under-
standing cognitive development but, because of its rarity, accounts for relatively
little of the total variation in cognitive ability. In either case, it would be foolhardy
to claim too much for the power of genetic studies of human dierences. This does
not mean, however, that such studies are without value, as we shall show. Our
task is to make clear what conclusions are justied on the basis of the data and
what are not.
1.2.2 Familial Resemblance
Look at the two sets of data shown in Figure 1.2. The rst part of the gure is a
scatterplot of measurements of weight in a large sample of non-identical (fraternal,
dizygotic, DZ) twins. Each cross in the diagram represents a single twin pair. The
second part of the gure is a scatterplot of pairs of completely unrelated people
from the same population. Notice how the two parts of the gure dier. In the
unrelated pairs the pattern of crosses gives the general impression of being circular;
in general, if we pick a particular value on the X axis (rst persons weight), it makes
little dierence to how heavy the second person is on average. This is what it means
to say that measures are uncorrelated knowing the score of the rst member of
a pair makes it no easier to predict the score of the second and vice-versa. By
comparison, the scatterplot for the fraternal twins (who are related biologically to
the same degree as brothers and sisters) looks somewhat dierent. The pattern of
crosses is slightly elliptical and tilted upwards. This means that as we move from
lighter rst twins towards heavier rst twins (increasing values on the X axis),
there is also a general tendency for the average scores of the second twins (on the
Y axis) to increase. It appears that the weights of twins are somewhat correlated.
Of course, if we take any particular X value, the Y values are still very variable
so the correlation is not perfect. The correlation coecient (see Chapter 2) allows
us to quantify the degree of relationship between the two sets of measures. In the
unrelated individuals, the correlation turns out to be 0.02 which is well within the
range expected simply by chance alone if the measures were really independent.
For the fraternal twins, on the other hand, the correlation is 0.44 which is far
greater than we would expect in so large a sample if the pairs of measures were
truly independent.
The data on weight illustrate the important general point that relatives are
6 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
(a)
T
w
i
n
1
4.0
4.2
4.4
4.6
4.8
5.0
5.2
5.4
5.6
5.8
Twin 2
4.0 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 5.0 5.2 5.4 5.6 5.8
(b)
I
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l
1
4.0
4.2
4.4
4.6
4.8
5.0
5.2
5.4
5.6
5.8
Individual 2
4.0 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 5.0 5.2 5.4 5.6 5.8
Figure 1.2: Two scatterplots of weight in: a) a large sample of DZ twin pairs,
and b) pairs of individuals matched at random.
1.2. HEREDITY AND VARIATION 7
usually much more alike than unrelated individuals from the same population.
That is, although there are large individual dierences for almost every trait than
can be measured, we nd that the trait values of family members are often quite
similar. Figure 1.3 gives the correlations between relatives in large samples of
nuclear families for body mass index (BMI), and conservatism. One simple way
of interpreting the correlation coecient is to multiply it by 100 and treat it as a
percentage. The correlation ( 100) is the percentage of the total variation in a
trait which is caused by factors shared by members of a pair. Thus, for example,
our correlation of 0.44 for the weights of DZ twins implies that, of all the factors
which create variation in weight, 44% are factors which members of a DZ twin
pair have in common. We can oer a similar interpretation for the other kinds of
relationship. A problem becomes immediately apparent. Since the DZ twins, for
example, have spent most of their lives together, we cannot know whether the 44%
is due entirely to the fact that they shared the same environment in utero, lived
with the same parents after birth, or simply have half their genes in common
and we shall never know until we can nd another kind of relationship in which
the degree of genetic similarity, or shared environmental similarity, is dierent.
Figure 1.4 gives a scattergram for the weights of a large sample of identical
(monozygotic, MZ) twins. Whereas DZ twins, like siblings, on average share only
half their genes, MZ twins are genetically identical. The scatter of points is now
much more clearly elliptical, and the 45

tilt of the major axis is especially ob-


vious. The correlation in the weights in this sample of MZ twins is 0.77, almost
twice that found for DZs. The much greater resemblance of MZ twins, who are
expected to have completely identical genes establishes a strong prima facie case
for the contribution of genetic factors to dierences in weight. One of the tasks to
be addressed in this book is how to interpret such dierential patterns of family
resemblance in a more rigorous, quantitative, fashion.
1.2.3 Within Family Dierences
At a purely anecdotal level, when parents hear about the possibility that genes cre-
ate dierences between people, they will sometimes respond Well, thats pretty
obvious. Ive raised three sons the same way and theyve all turned out dierently.
At issue here is not whether their conclusions are soundly based on their data, so
much as to indicate that not all variation is due to factors that family members
share in common. No matter how much parents contribute genetically to their
children and, it seems, no matter how much eort they put into parenting, a large
part of the outcome appears beyond their immediate control. That is, there are
large dierences even within a family. Some of these dierences are doubtless due
to the environment since even identical twins are not perfectly alike. Figure 1.5 is
a bar chart of the (absolute) weight dierences within pairs of twins. The darker,
left-hand column of each pair gives the percentage of the DZ sample falling in the
indicated range of dierences, and the lighter, right-hand column shows the corre-
8 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
0 20 40 60 80 100 -20
0 20 40 60 80 100 -20
MZ Twin
Parent-Offspring
With MZ uncle/aunt
Sibling
DZ Twin
With uncle/aunt
With DZ uncle/aunt
Cousins thro MZ
Cousins
Husband-Wife
With spouse of MZ
With spouse of DZ
With spouse of Sib
Spouses of MZ
Spouses of DZ
With parent-in-law
MZ parent-in-law
DZ parent-in-law
Correlations for Body Mass Index (BMI)
Based on the "Virginia 30,000"
Correlation (%)
Correlation (%)
Relationship
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8
MZ Twin
Parent-Offspring
With MZ uncle/aunt
Sibling
DZ Twin
With uncle/aunt
With DZ uncle/aunt
Cousins thro MZ
Cousins
Husband-Wife
With spouse of MZ
With spouse of DZ
With spouse of Sib
Spouses of MZ
Spouses of DZ
With parent-in-law
MZ parent-in-law
DZ parent-in-law
Correlations for Conservatism
Based on the "Virginia 30,000"
Correlation
Correlation
Relationship
Figure 1.3: Correlations for body mass index (weight/height
2
) and conservatism
between relatives. Data were obtained from large samples of nuclear families as-
certained through twins.
1.2. HEREDITY AND VARIATION 9
T
w
i
n
1
4.0
4.2
4.4
4.6
4.8
5.0
5.2
5.4
5.6
5.8
Twin 2
4.0 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 5.0 5.2 5.4 5.6 5.8
Figure 1.4: Scatterplot of weight in a large sample of MZ twins.
sponding percentages for MZ pairs. For MZ twins, these dierences must be due
to factors in the environment that dier even within pairs of genetically identical
individuals raised in the same home. Obviously the dierences within DZ pairs are
much greater on average. The known mechanisms of Mendelian inheritance can
account for this nding since, unlike MZ twins, DZ twins are not genetically iden-
tical although they are genetically related. DZ twins represent a separate sample
from the genetic pool cornered by the parents. Thus, DZ twins will be correlated
because their particular parents have their own particular selection of genes from
the total gene pool in the population, but they will not be genetically identical
because each DZ twin, like every other sibling in the same family, represents the
result of separate, random, meioses
1
in the parental germlines.
1
meiosis is the process of gametogenesis in which either sperm or ova are formed
10 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
0
10
20
30
40
50
DZ twins MZ twins
0-5 6-10 11-15 16-20 21-25 26-30 >30
Percentage of sample
Absolute difference in Pounds Absolute difference in Pounds
Figure 1.5: Bar chart of absolute dierences in weight within MZ and DZ twin
pairs.
1.3 Building and Fitting Models
As long as we study random samples of unrelated individuals, our understanding of
what causes the dierences we see will be limited. The total population variation
is simply an aggregate of all the various components of variance. One practical ap-
proach to the analysis of variation is to obtain several measures of it, each known
to reect a dierent proportion of genetic and environmental components of the
dierences. Then, if we have a model for how the eects of genes and environment
contribute dierentially to each distinct measure of variation, we can solve to ob-
tain estimates of the separate components. Figure 1.6 shows the principal stages in
this process. There are two aspects: theory and data. The model is a formal, in our
case mathematical, statement which mediates between the logic of the theory and
the reality of the data. Once a model is formulated consistently, the predictions
implied for dierent sets of data can be derived by a series of elementary math-
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 11
Theory
Model Data
Elaboration/Decision Model-fitting
Model-building
Experimental-design
Figure 1.6: Diagram of the interrelationship between theory, model and empirical
observation.
ematical operations. Model building is the task of translating the ideas of theory
into mathematical form. A large part of this book is devoted to a discussion of
model building. Inspection of the model, sometimes aided by computer simulation
(see Chapters 7 and ??), may suggest appropriate study designs which can be used
to generate critical data to test some or all parts of a current model. The statistical
processes of model tting allow us to compare the predictions of a particular model
with the actual observations about a given population. If the model fails, then we
are forced to revise all or some of our theory. If, on the other hand, the model ts
then we cannot know it is right in some ultimate sense. However, we might now
wish to base new testable conjectures on the theory in order to enlarge the scope
of observations it can encompass.
1.4 Elements of a Model: Causes of Variation
No model is built in isolation. Rather it is built upon a foundation of what is either
already known or what might be a matter for fertile conjecture. Part of the di-
culty, but also the intrinsic appeal, of genetic epidemiology is the fact that it seeks
either to distinguish between major sets of theoretical propositions, or to integrate
them into an overall framework. From biology, and especially through knowledge
of genetics, we have a detailed understanding of the intricacies of gene expression.
From the behavioral and social sciences we have strong proposals about the impor-
tance of the environment, especially the social environment, for the development
of human dierences. One view of our task is that it gives a common conceptual
and mathematical framework to both genetic and environmental theories so that
we may decide which, if any, is more consistent with the facts in particular cases.
12 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
1.4.1 Genetic Eects
A complete understanding of genetic eects would need to answer a series of ques-
tions:
1. How important are genetic eects on human dierences?
2. What kinds of action and interaction occur between gene products in the
pathways between genotype and phenotype?
3. Are the genetic eects on a trait consistent across sexes?
4. Are there some genes that have particularly outstanding eects when com-
pared to others?
5. Whereabouts on the human gene map are these genes located?
Questions 4 and 5 are clearly very important, but are not the immediate concern
of this book. On the other hand, we shall have a lot to say about 1, 2, and 3. It is
arguable that we shall not be able to understand 4 and 5 adequately, if we do not
have a proper appreciation of these other issues.
The importance of genes is often expressed relative to all the causes of varia-
tion, both genetic and environmental. The proportion of variation associated with
genetic eects is termed the broad heritability. However, the complete analysis of
genetic factors does not end here because, as countless experiments in plant and
animal genetics have shown (well in advance of modern molecular genetics; see e.g.,
Mather and Jinks, 1982), genes can act and interact in a variety of ways before
their eects on the phenotype appear.
Geneticists typically distinguish between additive and non-additive genetic ef-
fects (these terms will be dened more explicitly in Chapter 3). These inuences
have been studied in detail in many non-human species using selective breeding
experiments, which directly alter the frequencies of particular genotypes. In such
experiments, the bulk of genetic variation is usually explained by additive genetic
eects. However, careful studies have shown two general types of non-additivity
that may be important, especially in traits that have been subject to strong di-
rectional selection in the wild. The two main types of genetic non-additivity are
dominance and epistasis.
The term dominance derives initially from Mendels classical experiments in
which it was shown that the progeny of a cross between two pure breeding lines
often resembled one parent more than the other. That is, an individual who car-
ries dierent alleles at a locus (the heterozygote) is not exactly intermediate in
expression between individuals who are pure breeding (homozygous) for the two al-
leles. While dominance describes the interaction between alleles at the same locus,
epistasis describes the interaction between alleles at dierent loci.
Epistasis is said to occur whenever the eects of one gene on individual dif-
ferences depend on which genotype is expressed at another locus. For example,
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 13
suppose that at locus A/a individuals may have genotype AA, Aa or aa, and at
locus B/b genotype BB, Bb or bb
2
. If the dierence between individuals with
genotype AA and those with genotype aa depends on whether they are BB or
bb, then there would be additive additive epistatic interactions. Experimental
studies have shown a rich variety of possible epistatic interactions depending on
the number and eects of the interacting loci. However, their detailed resolution
in humans is virtually impossible unless we are fortunate enough to be examining
a trait which is inuenced by a small number of known genetic loci. Therefore
we acknowledge their conceptual importance and model them if they are identi-
ed. Failure to take non-additive genetic eects into account may be one of the
main reasons studies of twins give dierent heritability estimates from studies of
adoptees and nuclear families (Eaves et al., 1992; Plomin et al., 1991).
As studies in genetic epidemiology become larger and better designed, it is
becoming increasingly clear that there are marked sex dierences in gene expres-
sion. An important factor in establishing this view has been the incorporation of
unlike-sex twin pairs in twin studies (Eaves et al., 1990). However, comparison of
statistics derived from any relationship of individuals of unlike sex with those of like
sex would yield a similar conclusion (see Chapter 9). We shall make an important
distinction between two types of sex-limited gene expression. In the simpler case,
the same genes aect both males and females, but their eects are consistent across
sexes and dier only by some constant multiple over all the loci involved. We shall
refer to this type of eect as scalar sex-limitation. In other cases, however, we shall
discover that genetic eects in one sex are not just a constant multiple of their
eects in the other. Indeed, even though a trait may be measured in exactly the
same way in males and females, it may turn out that quite dierent genes control
its expression in the two sexes. A classic example would be the case of chest-girth
since at least some of the variation expressed in females may be due to loci that,
while still present in males, are only expressed in females. In this case we shall
speak of non-scalar sex-limitation. None of us likes the term very much, but until
someone suggests something better we shall continue to use it!
1.4.2 Environmental Eects
Paradoxically, one of the greatest benets of studies that can detect and control
for genetic eects is the information they can provide about the sources of envi-
ronmental inuence. We make an important distinction between identifying which
are the best places to look for specic environmental agents and deciding what
those specic agents are. For example, it may be possible to show that variation
in diastolic blood pressure is inuenced by environmental eects shared by family
members long before it is possible to demonstrate that the salient environmental
factor is the amount of sodium in the diet. We make a similar distinction be-
tween estimating the overall contribution of genetic eects and identifying specic
2
This notation is described more fully in Chapter 3.
14 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
loci that account for a signicant fraction of the total genetic variation. Using
some of the methods we shall describe later in this book it may indeed be possible
to estimate the contribution of specic factors to the environmental component
of variation (see Chapter 10). However, using the biometrical genetical approach
which relies only on the complex patterns of family resemblance, it is possible to
make some very important statements about the structure of the environment in
advance of our ability to identify the specic features of the environment that are
most important. Although the full subtlety for analyzing the environment cannot
be achieved with data on twins alone, much less on twins reared together, it is nev-
ertheless possible to make some important preliminary statements about the major
sources of environmental inuence which can provide a basis for future studies.
We may conceive of the total environmental variation in a trait as arising from
a number of sources. The rst major distinction we make is between environmen-
tal factors that operate within families and those which create dierences between
families. Sometimes the environment within families is called the unique environ-
ment or the specic environment or the random environment. Dierent authors
may refer to it as V
E
, V
SE
, E
1
, E
W
or e
2
, but the important thing is to understand
the concept behind the symbols. The within-family environment refers to all those
environmental inuences which are so random in their origin, and idiosyncratic in
their eects, as to contribute to dierences between members of the same fam-
ily. They are captured by Hamlets words from the famous to be or not to be
soliloquy:
...the slings and arrows of outrageous fortune.
The within-family environment will even contribute to dierences between individ-
uals of the same genotype reared in the same family. Thus, the single most direct
measure of their impact is the variation within pairs of MZ twins reared together.
Obviously, if a large proportion of the total variation is due to environmental
dierences within families we might expect to look more closely at the dierent ex-
periences of family members such as MZ twins in the hope of identifying particular
environmental factors. However, we have to take account of a further important dis-
tinction, namely that between long-term and short-term environmental eects,
even within families. If we only make a single measurement on every individual in a
study of MZ twins, say, we cannot tell whether the observed phenotypic dierences
between members of an MZ twin pair are due to some lasting impact of an early
environmental trauma, or due to much more transient dierences that inuence
how the twins function on the particular occasion of measurement. Many of the
latter kinds of inuence are captured by the concept of unreliability variance
in measurement theory. There is, of course, no hard and fast distinction between
the two sources of variation because how far one investigator is prepared to treat
short-term uctuations as unreliability is largely a matter of his or her frame
of reference. In depression, which is inherently episodic, short term uctuations
in behavior may point to quite specic environmental factors that trigger specic
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 15
episodes (see, e.g., Kendler et al., 1986). The main thing to realize is that what a
single cross-sectional study assigns to the within-family environment may or may
not be resolved into specic non-trivial environmental causes. How far to proceed
with the analysis of within-family environment is a matter for the judgement and
ingenuity of the particular investigator, aided by such data on repeated measures
as he or she may gather.
The between-family environment would seem to be the place that many of the
conceptually important environmental eects might operate. Any environmental
factors that are shared by family members will create dierences between families
and make family members relatively more similar. The environment between fam-
ilies is sometimes called the shared environment, the common environment or just
the family environment. Sometimes it is represented by the symbols E
2
, EB, EC,
CE, c
2
or V
EC
. Again, all these symbols denote the same underlying processes.
In twin studies, the shared environment is expected to contribute to the cor-
relation of both MZ and DZ twins as long as they are reared together. Just as
we distinguish short-term and long-term eects of the within-family environment,
so it is conceptually important to note that the eects of the shared environment
may be more or less permanent and may persist even if family members are sepa-
rated later in life, or they may be relatively transient in that they are expressed as
long as individuals are living together, perhaps as children with their parents, but
are dissipated as soon as the source of shared environmental inuence is removed.
Such eects can be detected by comparing the analyses of dierent age groups in
a cross-sectional study, or by tracing changes in the contribution of the shared
environment in a longitudinal genetic study (see Chapter 12).
It is a popular misconception that studies of twins reared together can oer no
insight about the eects of the shared environment. As we shall see in the following
chapters, this is far from the case. Large samples of twins reared together can pro-
vide a strong prima facie case for the importance of between-family environmental
eects that account for a signicant proportion of the total variance. The weak-
ness of twin studies, however, is that the various sources of the shared environment
cannot be discriminated. It is nevertheless essential for our understanding of what
the twin study can achieve, to recognize some of the reasons why this design can
never be a one-shot, self-contained investigation and why investigators should be
open to the possibility of signicant extensions of the twin study (see Chapter ??).
The environmental similarity between twins may itself be due to several distinct
sources whose resolution would require more extensive studies. First, we may
identify the environmental impact of parents on their children. That is, part of
the common environment eect in twins, can be traced to the fact that children
learn from their parents. Formally, this implies that some aspect of variation in
the maternal or paternal phenotypes (or both) creates part of the environmental
variation between pairs of children. An excellent starting point for exploring some
of these eects is the extension of the classical twin study to include data on the
parents of twins (see Chapter ??). In principle, we might nd that parents do
16 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
not contribute equally to the shared family environment. The eect of mothers
on the environment of their ospring is usually called the maternal eect and
the impact of fathers is called the paternal eect. Although these eects can
be resolved by parent-ospring data, they cannot be separated from each other as
long as we only have twins in the sample.
Following the terms introduced by Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (1981), the
environmental eects of parent on child are often called vertical cultural transmis-
sion to reect the fact that non-genetic information is passed vertically down the
family tree from parents to children. However, the precise eects of the parental
environment on the pattern of family resemblance depend on which aspect of the
parental phenotype is aecting the osprings environment. The shared environ-
ment of the children may depend on the same trait in the parents that is being
measured in the ospring. For example, the environment that makes ospring
more or less conservative depends directly on the conservatism of their parents.
In this case we normally speak of phenotype-to-environment (P to E) trans-
mission. It is quite possible, however, that part of the shared environment of the
ospring is created by aspects of parental behavior that are dierent from those
measured in the children, although the two may be somewhat correlated. Thus, for
example, parental income may exercise a direct eect on ospring educational level
through its eect on duration and quality of schooling. Another example would
be the eect of parental warmth or protectiveness on the development of anxiety
or depression in their children. In this case we have a case of correlated variable
transmission. Haley, Jinks and Last (1981) make a similar distinction between the
one character and two character models for maternal eects. The additional
feature of the parental phenotype may or may not be measured in either parents
or children. When such additional traits are measured in addition to the trait of
primary interest we will require multivariate genetic models to perform the data
analysis properly. Some simple examples of these methods will be described in later
chapters. Two extreme examples of correlated variable transmission are where the
variable causing the shared environment is:
1. an index purely of the environmental determinants of the phenotype
environment-to-environment (E to E) transmission
2. purely genetic genotype-to-environment (G to E) transmission.
Although we can almost never claim to have a direct measure of the genotype for
any quantitative trait, the latter conception recognizes that there may be a genetic
environment (see e.g. Darlington, 1971), that is, genetic dierences between some
members of a population may be part of the environment of others. One conse-
quence of the genetic environment is the seemingly paradoxical notion that dierent
genetic relationships also can be used to tease out certain important aspects of the
environment. For example, the children of identical twins can be used to provide
a test of the environmental impact of the maternal genotype on the phenotypes of
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 17
their children (see e.g., Nance and Corey, 1976). A concrete example of this phe-
nomenon would be the demonstration that a mothers genes aect the birthweight
of her children.
Although researchers in the behavioral sciences almost instinctively identify the
parents as the most salient feature of the shared environment, we must recognize
that there are other environmental factors shared by family members that do not
depend directly on the parents. There are several factors that can create residual
(non-parental) shared environmental eects. First, there may be factors that are
shared between all types of ospring, twin or non-twin; these may be called sibling
shared environments. Second, twins may share a more similar pre-and postnatal
environment than siblings simply because they are conceived, born and develop at
the same time. This additional correlation between the environments of twins is
called the special twin environment and is expected to make both MZ and DZ twins
more alike than siblings even in the absence of genetic eects. It is important to
note that even twins separated at birth share the same pre-natal environment, so
a comparison of twins reared together and apart is only able to provide a simple
test of the post-natal shared environment
3
.
A further type of environmental partition, the special MZ twin environment
is sometimes postulated to explain the fact that MZ twins reared together are
more correlated than DZ twins. This is the most usual environmental explanation
oered as an alternative to genetic models for individual dierences because the
eects of the special MZ environment will tend to mimic those of genes in twins
reared together. It is because of concern that genetic eects may be partly con-
founded with any special MZ twin environments that we stress the importance
of thinking beyond the twin study to include other relationships. It becomes in-
creasingly dicult to favor a purely non-genetic explanation of MZ twin similarity
when the genetic model is able to predict the correlations for a rich variety of re-
lationships from a few fairly simple principles. Since the special twin environment,
however, would increase the correlation of MZ twins, its eects may often resemble
those of non-additive genetic eects (dominance and epistasis) in models for family
resemblance.
1.4.3 Genotype-Environment Eects
It has long been realized that the distinction we make for heuristic purposes between
genotype and environment is an approximation which ignores several processes
that might be important in human populations. Three factors defy the simple
separation of genetic and environmental eects, but are likely to be of potential
signicance from what we know of the way genes operate in other species, and from
the logical consequences of the grouping of humans into families of self-determining
individuals who share both genes and environment in common.
3
Twins born serially by embryo implantation are currently far too rare for the purposes of
statistical distinction between pre- and post-natal eects!
18 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
The factors we need to consider are:
1. assortative mating
2. genotype-environment covariance (CovGE, or genotype-environment correla-
tion, CorGE)
3. genotype environment interaction (GE).
Each of these will be discussed briey.
Assortative Mating
Any non-random pairing of mates on the basis of factors other than biological
relatedness is subsumed under the general category of assortative mating. Mating
based on relatedness is termed inbreeding, and will not be examined in this book.
We discuss assortative mating under the general heading of genotype-environmental
eects for two main reasons. First, when assortment is based on some aspect of
the phenotype, it may be inuenced by both genetic and environmental factors.
Second, assortative mating may aect the transmission, magnitude, and correlation
of both genetic and environmental eects.
In human populations, the rst indication of assortative mating is often a cor-
relation between the phenotypes of mates. Usually, such correlations are positive.
Positive assortment is most marked for traits in the domains of education, religion,
attitudes, and socioeconomic status. Somewhat smaller correlations are found in
the physical and cognitive domains. Mating is eectively random, or only very
slightly assortative, in the personality domain. We are not aware of any replicated
nding of a signicant negative husband-wife correlation, with the exception of
gender!
Assortative mating may not be the sole source of similarity between husband
and wife social interaction is another plausible cause. A priori, we might expect
social interaction to play a particularly important role in spousal resemblance for
habits such as cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption. Two approaches are
available for resolving spousal interaction from strict assortative mating. The rst
depends on tracing the change in spousal resemblance over time, and the second
requires analyzing the resemblance between the spouses of biologically related in-
dividuals (see Heath, 1987). Although the usual treatment of assortative mating
assumes that spouses choose one another on the basis of the trait being studied
(primary phenotypic assortment), we should understand that this is only one model
of a process that might be more complicated in reality. For example, mate selection
is unlikely to be based on an actual psychological test score. Instead it is probably
based on some related variable, which may or may not be measured directly. If
the variable on which selection is based is something that we have also measured,
we call it correlated variable assortment. If the correlated trait is not measured di-
rectly we have latent variable assortment. In the simplest case, the latent variable
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 19
may simply be the true value of trait of which the actual measure is just a more or
less unreliable index. We then speak of phenotypic assortment with error.
Once we begin to consider latent variable assortment, we recognize that the
latent variable may be more or less genetic. If the latent variable is due entirely
to the social environment we have one form of social homogamy (e.g., Rao et al.,
1974). We can conceive of a number of intriguing mechanisms of latent variable
assortment according to the presumed causes of the latent variable on which mate
selection is based. For example, mating may be based on one or more aspects of
the phenotypes of relatives, such as parents incomes, culinary skills, or siblings
reproductive history. In all these cases of correlated or latent variable assortment,
mate selection may be based on variables that are more reliable indices of the
genotype than the measured phenotype. This possibility was considered by Fisher
(1918) in what is still the classical treatment of assortative mating.
Clearly, the resolution of these various mechanisms of assortment is beyond the
scope of the conventional twin study, although multivariate studies that include
the spouses of twins, or the parents and parents-in-law of twins may be capable of
resolving some of these complex issues (see, e.g., Heath et al., 1985).
Even though the classical twin study cannot resolve the complexities of mate
selection, we have to keep the issue in mind all the time because of the eects
of assortment on the correlations between relatives, including twins. When mates
select partners like themselves phenotypically, they are also (indirectly) choosing
people who resemble themselves genetically and culturally. As a result, positive
phenotypic assortative mating increases the genetic and environmental correlations
between relatives. Translating this principle into the context of the twin study,
we will nd that assortative mating tends to increase the similarity of DZ twins
relative to MZ twins. As we shall see, in twins reared together, the genetic eects
of assortative mating will articially inate estimates of the family environmental
component. This means, in turn, that estimates of the genetic component based
primarily on the dierence between MZ correlations and DZ correlations will tend
to be biased downwards in the presence of assortative mating.
Genotype-Environment Correlation
Paradoxically, the factors that make humans dicult to study genetically are pre-
cisely those that make humans so interesting. The experimental geneticist can
control matings and randomize the uncontrolled environment. In many human so-
cieties, for better or for worse, consciously or unconsciously, people likely decide for
themselves on the genotype of the partner to whom they are prepared to commit
the future of their genes. Furthermore, humans are more or less free living organ-
isms who spend a lot of time with their relatives. If the problem of mate selection
gives rise to fascination with the complexities of assortative mating, it is the fact
that individuals create their own environment and spend so much time with their
relatives that generates the intriguing puzzle of genotype-environment correlation.
20 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
As the term suggests, genotype-environment correlation (CorGE) refers to the
fact that the environments that individuals experience may not be a random sample
of the whole range of environments but may be caused by, or correlated with, their
genes. Christopher Jencks (1972) spoke of the double advantage phenomenon in
the context of ability and education. Individuals who begin life with the advantage
of genes which increase their ability relative to the average may also be born into
homes that provide them with more enriched environments, having more money to
spend on books and education and being more committed to learning and teaching.
This is an example of positive CorGE. Cattell (1963) raised the possibility of nega-
tive CorGE by formulating a principle of cultural coercion to the biosocial norm.
According to this principle, which has much in common with the notion of stabiliz-
ing selection in population genetics, individuals whose genotype predisposes them
to extreme behavior in either direction will tend to evoke a social response which
will coerce them back towards the mean. For example, educational programs
that are designed specically for the average student may increase achievement in
below average students while attenuating it in talented pupils.
Many taxonomies have been proposed for CorGE. We prefer one that classi-
es CorGE according to specic detectable consequences for the pattern of varia-
tion in a population (see Eaves et al., 1977). The rst type of CorGE, genotype-
environment autocorrelation arises because the individual creates or evokes envi-
ronments which are functions of his or her genotype. This is the smorgasbord
model which views a given culture as having a wide variety of environments from
which the individual makes a selection on the basis of genetically determined prefer-
ences. Thus, an intellectually gifted individual would invest more time in mentally
stimulating activities. An example of possible CorGE from a dierent context is
provided by an ethological study of 32 month-old male twins published a number
of years ago (Lytton, 1977). The study demonstrated that parent-initiated interac-
tions with their twin children are more similar when the twins are MZ rather than
DZ. Of course, like every other increased correlation in the environment of MZ
twins, it may not be clear whether it is truly a result of a treatment being elicited
by genotype rather than simply a matter of identical individuals being treated more
similarly. That is, the direction of causation is not clear.
Insofar as the genotypes of individuals create or elicit environments, cross-
sectional twin studies will not be able to distinguish the ensuing CorGE from
any other eects of the genes. That is, positive CorGE will increase estimates of
all the genetic components of variance and negative CorGE will decrease them.
However, we will have no direct way of knowing which genetic eects act directly
on the phenotype and which result from the action of environmental variation
caused initially by genetic dierences. In this latter case, the environment may be
considered as part of the extended phenotype (see Dawkins, 1982). If the process
we describe were to accumulate during behavioral development, positive CorGE
would lead to an increase in the relative contribution of genetic factors with age,
but a constant genetic correlation across ages (see Chapter 12). However, nding
1.4. ELEMENTS OF A MODEL: CAUSES OF VARIATION 21
this pattern of developmental change would not necessarily imply that the actual
mechanism of the change is specically genotype-environment autocorrelation.
The second major type of CorGE is that which arises because the environment
in which individuals develop is provided by their biological relatives. Thus, one
individuals environment is provided by the phenotype of someone who is genet-
ically related. Typically, we think of the correlated genetic and environmental
eects of parents on their children. For example, a child who inherits the genes
that predispose to depression may also experience the pathogenic environment of
rejection because the tendency of parents to reject their children may be caused
by the same genes that increase risk to depression. As far as the ospring are
concerned, therefore, a high genetic predisposition to depression is correlated with
exposure to an adverse environment because both genes and environment derive
originally from the parents. We should note (i) that this type of CorGE can occur
only if parent-ospring transmission comprises both genetic factors and vertical
cultural inheritance, and (ii) that the CorGE is broken in randomly adopted in-
dividuals since the biological parents no longer provide the salient environment.
Adoption data thus provide one important test for the presence of this type of
genotype-environment correlation.
Although most empirical studies have focused on the parental environment as
that which is correlated with genotype, parents are not the only relatives who may
be inuential in the developmental process. Children are very often raised in the
presence of one or more siblings. Obviously, this is always the case for twin pairs.
In a world in which people did not interact socially, we would expect the presence
or absence of a sibling, and the unique characteristics of that sibling, to have no
impact on the outcome of development. However, if there is any kind of social
interaction, the idiosyncrasies of siblings become salient features of one anothers
environment. Insofar as the eect of one sibling or twin on another depends on
aspects of the phenotype that are under genetic control, we expect there to be
a special kind of genetic environment which can be classied under the general
category of sibling eects. When the trait being measured is partly genetic, and
also responsible for creating the sibling eects, we have the possibility for a specic
kind of CorGE. This CorGE arises because the genotype of one sibling, or twin,
is genetically correlated with the phenotype of the other sibling which is providing
part of the environment. When above average trait values in one twin tend to
increase trait expression in the other, we speak of cooperation eects (Eaves, 1976b)
or imitation eects (Carey, 1986b). An example of imitation eects would be any
tendency of deceptive behavior in one twin to reinforce deception in the other. The
alternative social interaction, in which a high trait value in one sibling tends to act
on the opposite direction in the other, produces competition or contrast eects We
might expect such eects to be especially marked in environments in which there
is competition for limited resources. It has sometimes been argued that contrast
eects are an important source of individual dierences in extraversion (see Eaves
et al., 1989) with the more extraverted twin tending to engender introversion in
22 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
his or her cotwin and vice-versa.
Sibling eects typically have two kinds of detectable consequence. First, they
produce dierences in trait mean and variance as a function of sibship size and
density. One of the rst indications of sibling eects may be dierences in variance
between twins and singletons. Second, the genotype-environment correlation cre-
ated by sibling eects depends on the biological relationship between the socially
interacting individuals. So, for example, the CorGE is greater in pairs of MZ twins
because each twin is reared with an cotwin of identical genotype. If there are co-
operation (imitation) eects we expect the CorGE to make the total variance of
MZ twins signicantly greater than that of DZs, which in turn would exceed that
of singletons (Eaves, 1976b). Competition (contrast) eects will tend to make the
MZ variance less than that of DZs. Other eects ensue for the covariances between
relatives, as discussed in Chapter 8. Sibling eects may conceivably be reciprocal,
if siblings inuence each other, or non-reciprocal, if an elder sibling, for example,
is a source of social inuence on a younger sibling.
Genotype Environment Interaction
The interaction of genotype and environment (G E) must always be distin-
guished carefully from CorGE. Genotype-environment correlation reects a non-
random distribution of environments among dierent genotypes. Good genotypes
get more or less than their fair share of good environments. By contrast, G E
interaction has nothing to do with the distribution of genetic and environmental
eects. Instead, it relates to the actual way genes and environment aect the
phenotype. G E refers to the genetic control of sensitivity to dierences in the
environment. The old adage sauce for the goose is sauce for the gander describes
a world in which G E is absent, because it implies that the same environmental
treatment has the same positive or negative eect regardless of the genotype of the
individual upon whom it is imposed.
An obvious example of G E interaction is that of inherited disease resistance.
Genetically susceptible individuals will be free of disease as long as the environment
does not contain the pathogen. Resistant individuals will be free of the disease even
in a pathogenic environment. That is, changing the environment by introducing
the pathogen will have quite a dierent impact on the phenotype of susceptible
individuals than on resistant ones. More subtle examples may be the genetic control
of sensitivity to the pathogenic eects of tobacco smoke or genetic dierences in
the eects of sodium intake on blood pressure.
The analysis of G E in humans is extremely dicult in practice because of
the diculty of securing large enough samples to detect eects that may be small
compared with the main eects of genes and environment. Studies of G E in
experimental organisms (see, e.g., Mather and Jinks, 1982) illustrate a number of
issues which are also conceptually important in thinking about GE in humans.
We consider these briey in turn.
1.5. RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN VARIABLES 23
The genes responsible for sensitivity to the environment are not always the
same as those that control average trait values. For example, one set of genes
may control overall liability to depression and a second set, quite distinct in their
location and mode of action, may control whether individuals respond more or less
to stressful environments. Another way of thinking about the issue is to consider
measurements made in dierent environments as dierent traits which may or may
not be controlled by the same genes. By analogy with our earlier discussion of sex-
limitation, we distinguish between scalar and non-scalar G E interaction.
When the same genes are expressed consistently at all levels of a salient envi-
ronmental variable so that only the amount of genetic variance changes between
environments, we have scalar genotype environment interaction. If, instead
of, or in addition to, changes in genetic variance, we also nd that dierent genes
are expressed in dierent environments we have non-scalar G E.
G E interaction may involve environments that can be measured directly or
whose eects can be inferred only from the correlations between relatives. Gener-
ally, our chances of detecting G E are much greater when we can measure the
relevant environments, such as diet, stress, or tobacco consumption. The simplest
situation, which we shall discuss in Chapter 9, arises when each individual in a
twin pair can be scored for the presence or absence of a particular environmental
variable such as exposure to severe psychological stress. In this case, twin pairs can
be divided into those who are concordant and those discordant for environmental
exposure and the data can be tested for dierent kinds of G E using relatively
simple methods.
One measurable feature of the environment may be the phenotype of an indi-
viduals parent. A problem frequently encountered, however, is the fact that many
measurable aspects of the environment, such as smoking and alcohol consumption,
themselves have a genetic component so that the problems of mathematical mod-
elling and statistical analysis become formidable. If we are unable to measure the
environment directly, our ability to detect and analyze G E will depend on the
same kinds of data that we use to analyze the main eects of genes and environ-
ment, namely the patterns of family resemblance and other, more complex, features
of the distribution of trait values in families. Generally, the detection of any in-
teraction between genetic eects and unmeasured aspects of the between-family
environment will require adoption data, particularly separated MZ twins. Interac-
tion between genes and the within-family environment will usually be detectable
only if the genes controlling sensitivity are correlated with those controlling average
expression of the trait (see, e.g., Jinks and Fulker, 1970).
1.5 Relationships between Variables
Many of the critics of the methods we are to describe argue that, for twin studies
at least, the so-called traditional methods such as taking the dierence between the
MZ and DZ correlations and doubling it as a heritability estimate give much the
24 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
same answer as the more sophisticated methods taught here. In the nal analysis,
it must be up to history and the consumer to decide, but in our experience there
are several reasons for choosing the methods presented here. First, as we have
already shown, the puzzle of human variation extends far beyond testing whether
genes play any role in variation. The subtleties of the environment and the varieties
of gene action call for methods that can integrate many more types of data and
test more complex hypotheses than were envisioned fty or a hundred years ago.
Only a model building/model tting strategy allows us to trace the implications
of a theory across all kinds of data and to test systematically for the consistency
of theory and observation. But even if the skeptic is left in doubt by the methods
proposed for the interpretation of variables considered individually, we believe that
the conventional approaches of fty years ago pale utterly once we want to analyze
the genetic and environmental causes of correlation between variables.
The genetic analysis of multiple variables will occupy many of the succeeding
chapters, so here it is sucient to preview the main issues. There are three kinds of
multivariate questions which are generic issues in genetic epidemiology, although
we shall address them in the context of the twin study. Each is outlined briey.
1.5.1 Causes of Correlation between Variables
The question of what causes variables to correlate is the usual entry point to mul-
tivariate genetic analysis. Students of genetics have long been familiar with the
concept of pleiotropy, i.e., that one genetic factor can aect several dierent phe-
notypes. Obviously, we can imagine environmental advantages and insults that
aect many traits in a similar way. Students of the psychology of individual dif-
ferences, and especially of factor analysis, will be aware that Spearman introduced
the concept of the general intelligence factor as a latent variable accounting for
the pattern of correlations observed between multiple abilities. He also introduced
an empirical test (the method of tetrad dierences) of the consistency between his
general factor theory and the empirical data on the correlations between abilities.
Such factor models however, only operate at the descriptive phenotypic level. They
aggregate into a single model genetic and environmental processes which might be
quite separate and heterogeneous if only the genetic and environmental causes
of inter-variable correlation could be analyzed separately. Cattell recognized this
when he put forward the notion of uid and crystallized intelligence. The
former was dependent primarily on genetic processes and would tend to increase
the correlation between measures that index genetic abilities. The latter was de-
termined more by the content of the environment (an environmental mold trait)
and would thus appear as loading more on traits that reect the cultural environ-
ment. An analysis of multiple symptoms of anxiety and depression by Kendler et
al. (1986) illustrates very nicely the point that the pattern of genetic and environ-
mental eects on multiple measures may dier very markedly. They showed that
twins responses to a checklist of symptoms reected a single underlying genetic
1.5. RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN VARIABLES 25
dimension which inuenced symptoms of both anxiety and depression. By con-
trast, the eects of the environment were organized along two dimensions (group
factors) one aecting only symptoms of anxiety and the other symptoms of
depression. More recently, this nding has been replicated with psychiatric diag-
noses (?; ?), which suggests that the liability to either disorder is due to a single
common set of genes, while the specic expression of that liability as either anx-
iety or depression is a function of what kind of environmental event triggers the
disorder in the vulnerable person. Such insights are impossible without methods
that can analyze the correlations between multiple measures into their genetic and
environmental components.
1.5.2 Direction of Causation
Students of elementary statistics have long been made to recite correlation does
not imply causation and rightly so, because a premature assignment of causality
to a mere statistical association could waste scientic resources and do actual harm
if treatment were to be based upon it. However, one of the goals of science is to
analyze complex systems into elementary processes which are thought to be causal
or more fundamental and, when actual experimental intervention is dicult, it
may be necessary to look to the nexus of intercorrelations among measures for
clues about causality.
The claim that correlation does not imply causality comes from a fundamental
indeterminacy of any general model for the correlation between a single pair of
variables. Put simply, if we observe a correlation between A and B, it can arise
from one or all of three processes: A causing B (denoted A B), B causing A, or
latent variable C causing A and B. A general model for the correlation between A
and B would need constants to account for the strength of the causal connections
between A and B, B and A, C and A, C and B. Clearly, a single correlation cannot
be used to determine four unknown parameters.
When we have more than two variables, however, matters may look a little dif-
ferent. It may now become possible to exclude some causal hypotheses as clearly
inconsistent with the data. Whether or not this can be done will depend on the
complexity of the causal nexus being analyzed. For example, a pattern of corre-
lations of the form r
AC
= r
AB
r
BC
would support one or other of the causal
sequences A B C or C B A in preference to orders that place A
or C in the middle.
The fact that causality implies temporal priority has been used in some appli-
cations to advocate a longitudinal strategy for its analysis. One approach is the
cross-lagged panel study in which the variables A and B are measured at two points
in time, t
0
and t
1
. If the correlation of A at t
0
with B at t
1
is greater than the
correlation of B at t
0
with A at t
1
, we might give some credence to the causal pri-
ority of A over B. Methods for the statistical assessment of such relative priorities
are known as cross-lagged panel analysis (?) and may assessed within structural
26 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
equation models (?).
The cross-lagged approach, though strongly suggestive of causality in some
circumstances, is not entirely foolproof. With this fact in view, researchers are
always on the look-out for other approaches that can be used to test hypotheses
about causality in correlational data. It has recently become clear that the cross-
sectional twin study, in which multiple measures are made only on one occasion,
may, under some circumstances, allow us to test hypotheses about direction of
causality without the necessity of longitudinal data. The potential contribution
of twin studies to resolving alternative models of causation will be discussed in
Chapter ??. At this stage, however, it is sucient to give a simple insight about
one set of circumstances which might lead us to prefer one causal hypothesis over
another.
Consider the ambiguous relationship between exercise and body weight. In
free-living populations, there is a signicant correlation between exercise and body
weight. How much of that association is due to the fact that people who exercise
use up more calories and how much to the fact that fat people dont like jogging? In
the simplest possible case, suppose that we found variation in exercise to be purely
environmental (i.e., having no genetic component) and variation in weight to be
partly genetic. Then there is no way that the direction of causation can go from
body weight to exercise because, if this were the case, some of the genetic eects
on body weight would create genetic variation in exercise. In practice, things are
seldom that simple. Data are nearly always more ambiguous and hypotheses more
complex. But this simple example illustrates that the genetic studies, notably the
twin study, may sometimes yield valuable insight about the causal relationships
between multiple variables.
1.5.3 Developmental Change
Any cross-sectional study is a slice at one time point across the continuing onto-
genetic dialogue between the organism and the environment. While such studies
help us understand outcomes, they may not tell us much about the process of be-
coming. For example, the longitudinal genetic study involving repeated measures
of twins may be thought of as a multivariate genetic study in which the multiple
occasions of measurement correspond to multiple traits in the conventional cross-
sectional study. In the conventional multivariate study we ask such questions as
How much do genes create the correlation between dierent variables?, so in the
longitudinal genetic study we ask How far do genes (or environment) account for
the developmental consistency of behavior? and To what extent are there specic
genetic and environmental eects expressed at each point in time?. These are but
two of a rich variety of questions which can be addressed with the methods we shall
describe. One indication of the insight that can ensue from such an approach to
longitudinal measures on twins comes from some of the data on cognitive growth
obtained in the ground-breaking Louisville Twin Study. In a reanalysis by model
1.6. THE CONTEXT OF OUR APPROACH 27
tting methods, Eaves et al. (1986) concluded that such data as had been pub-
lished strongly suggested the involvement of a single common set of genes which
were active from birth to adolescence and whose aects persisted and accumulated
through time. By contrast, the shared environment kept changing during devel-
opment. That is, parents who provided a better environment at one age did not
necessarily do so at another, even though whatever they did had fairly persistent
eects. The unique environment of the individual, however, was age-specic and
very ephemeral in its eect. Such a model, based as it was on only that part of
the data available in print, may not stand the test of more detailed scrutiny. Our
aim here is not so much to defend a particular model for cognitive development as
to indicate that a model tting approach to longitudinal kinship data can lead to
many important insights about the developmental process.
1.6 The Context of our Approach
Figure 1.7 summarizes the main streams of the intellectual tradition which converge
to yield the ideas and methods we shall be discussing here. The streams divide and
merge again at several places. The picture is not intended to be a comprehensive
history of statistical or behavioral genetics, so a number of people whose work is
extremely important to both disciplines are not mentioned. Rather, it tries to
capture the main lines of thought and the cast of characters who have been
especially inuential in our own intellectual development. Not all of us would give
the same weight to all the lines of descent.
1.6.1 Early History
To our knowledge, the rst use of twin resemblance as a means of resolving al-
ternative hypotheses about the causes of human individual dierences appears in
426 A.D. by Augustine of Hippo in Book V of the City of God. Augustine argued
that since twins, who were highly correlated in their times of birth, nevertheless
had such discrepant life histories, there was little empirical support for planetary
inuence on human destiny. For Augustines purpose, it was sucient that at least
some twin pairs showed markedly dierent life histories, despite being born at the
same time. To go beyond testing the astrological hypothesis and use twins to an-
swer the nature-nurture question required recognition of the fact that there are
two types of twins, identical and fraternal, and some way of distinguishing between
them.
1.6.2 19th Century Origins
Two geniuses of the last century provided the fundamental principles on which
much of what we do today still depends. Francis Galtons boundless curiosity,
ingenuity and passion for measurement were combined in seminal insights and
28 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
People and Ideas
Galton (1865-ish)
Correlation
Family Resemblance
Twins
Ancestral Heredity
Mendel (1865)
Particulate Inheritance
Genes: single in gamete
double in zygote
Segregation ratios
Darwin (1858,1871)
Natural Selection
Sexual Selection
Evolution
Fisher (1918)
Correlation & Mendel
Maximum Likelihood
ANOVA: partition of variance
Spearman (1904)
Common Factor Analysis
Wright (1921)
Path Analysis
Thurstone (1930's)
Multiple Factor Analysis
Mather (1949) &
Jinks (1971)
Biometrical Genetics
Model Fitting (plants)
Joreskog (1960)
Covariance
Structure Analysis
LISREL
Morton (1974)
Path Analysis &
Family Resemblance
Watson &
Crick (1953)
Jinks & Fulker (1970)
Model Fitting applied to humans
Martin & Eaves (1977)
Genetic Analysis of
Covariance Structure
Elston etc (19..)
Segregation
Linkage
Rao, Rice, Reich,
Cloninger (1970's)
Assortment
Cultural Inheritance
Neale (1990) Mx
Molecular
Genetics
Population
Genetics
2000
Figure 1.7: Diagram of the intellectual traditions leading to modern mathematical
genetic methodology.
1.6. THE CONTEXT OF OUR APPROACH 29
contributions which established the foundations of the scientic study of individ-
ual dierences. Karl Pearsons three-volume scientic biography of Galton is an
enthralling testimony to Galtons fascination and skill in bringing a rich variety
of intriguing problems under scientic scrutiny. His Inquiry into the Ecacy of
Prayer reveals Galton to be a true child of the Enlightenment to whom nothing
was sacred. To him we owe the rst systematic studies of individual dierences and
family resemblance, the recognition that the dierence between MZ and DZ twins
provided a valuable point of departure for resolving the eects of genes and culture,
the rst mathematical model (albeit inadequate) for the similarity between rela-
tives, and the development of the correlation coecient as a measure of association
between variables that did not depend on the units of measurement.
The specicity that Galtons theory of inheritance lacked was supplied by the
classical experiments of Gregor Mendel on plant hybridization. Mendels demon-
stration that the inheritance of model traits in carefully bred material agreed with
a simple theory of particulate inheritance still remains one of the stunning exam-
ples of how the alliance of quantitative thinking and painstaking experimentation
can predict, in advance of any observations of chromosome behavior or molecu-
lar science, the necessary properties of the elementary processes underlying such
complex phenomena as heredity and variation.
1.6.3 Genetic, Factor, and Path Analysis
The conict between those, like Karl Pearson, who followed a Galtonian model of
inheritance and those, like Bateson, who adopted a Mendelian model, is well known
to students of genetics. Although Pearson appeared to have some clues about
how Galtons data might be explained on Mendelian principles, it fell to Ronald
Fisher, in 1918, to provide the rst coherent and general account of how the cor-
relations between relatives could be explained on the supposition of Mendelian
inheritance. Fisher assumed what is now called the polygenic model, that is, he
assumed the variation observed for a trait such as stature was caused by a large
number of individual genes, each of which was inherited in strict conformity to
Mendels laws. By describing the eects of the environment, assortative mating,
and non-additive gene action mathematically, Fisher was able to show remarkable
consistency between Pearsons own correlations between relatives for stature and
a strictly Mendelian mechanism of inheritance. Some of the ideas rst expounded
by Fisher will be the basis of our treatment of biometrical genetics (Chapter 3).
In the same general era we witness the seeds of two other strands of thought
which continue to be inuential today. Charles Spearman, adopting Galtons idea
that a correlation between variables might reect a common underlying causal
factor, began to explore the pattern of correlations between multiple measures
of ability. So began the tradition of multivariate analysis which was, for much
of psychology at least, embodied chiey in the method of factor analysis which
sought the latent variables responsible for the observed patterns of correlation
30 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
between multiple variables. The notion of multiple factors, introduced through the
work of Thurstone, and the concept of factor rotation to simple structure, provided
much of the early conceptual and mathematical foundation for the treatment of
multivariate systems to be discussed in this book.
Sewall Wright, whose long and distinguished career spans all of the six decades
which have seen the explosion of genetics into the most inuential of the life sci-
ences, was the founding father of American population genetics. His seminal paper
on path analysis, published in 1921 established a parallel stream of thought to that
created by Fisher in 1918. The emphasis of Fishers work lay in the formulation
of a mathematical theory which could reconcile observations on the correlation
between relatives with a model of particulate inheritance. Wright, on the other
hand, was less concerned with providing a theory which could integrate two views
of genetic inheritance than he was with developing a method for exploring ways
in which dierent causal hypotheses could be expressed in a simple, yet testable,
form. It is not too gross an oversimplication to suggest that the contributions of
Fisher and Wright were each stronger where the other was weaker. Thus, Fishers
early paper established an explicit model for how the eects and interaction of
large numbers of individual genes could be resolved in the presence of a number of
dierent theories of mate selection. On the other hand, Fisher showed very little
interest in the environment, choosing rather to conceive of environmental eects as
a random variable uncorrelated between relatives. Fishers environment is what we
have called the within family environment, which seems appropriate for the kinds
of anthropometric variables that Fisher and his predecessors chose to illustrate the
rules of quantitative inheritance. However, it seems a little less defensible, on a
priori grounds, as a model for the eects of environment on what Pearson (1904)
called the mental and moral characteristics of man or those habits and lifestyles
that might have a signicant impact on risk for disease. By contrast, Wrights
approach virtually ignored the subtleties of gene action, considering only additive
genetic eects and treating them as a statistical aggregate which owed little to the
laws of Mendel beyond the fact that ospring received half their genes from their
mother and half from their father. On the other hand, Wrights strategy made it
much easier to specify familial environmental eects, especially those derived from
the social interaction of family members.
1.6.4 Integration of the Biometrical and Path-Analytic Ap-
proaches
These dierent strengths and weaknesses of the traditions derived from Fisher and
Wright persisted into the 1970s. The biometrical genetical approach, derived from
Fisher through the ground-breaking studies of Kenneth Mather and his student
John Jinks established what became known as the Birmingham School. The em-
phasis of this tradition was on the detailed analysis of gene action through carefully
designed and properly randomized breeding studies in experimental organisms. Ex-
1.6. THE CONTEXT OF OUR APPROACH 31
cept where the environment could be manipulated genetically (e.g., in the study
of the environmental eects of the maternal genotype), the biometrical genetical
approach treated the environment as a random variable. Even though the envi-
ronment might sometimes be correlated between families as a result of practical
limitations on randomization, it was independent of genotype. Thus, the Birm-
ingham Schools initial treatment of the environment in human studies allowed for
the partition of environmental components of variance into contributions within
families (EW) and between families (EB) but was very weak in its treatment of
genotype-environment correlation. Some attempt to remedy this deciency was of-
fered by Eaves (1976a; 1976b) in his treatment of vertical cultural transmission and
sibling interaction, but the value of these models was restricted by the assumption
of random mating.
The rediscovery of path analysis in a series of papers by Morton and his cowork-
ers in the early 70s showed how many of the more realistic notions of how envi-
ronmental eects were transmitted, such as those suggested by Cavalli-Sforza and
Feldman (1981), could be captured much better in path models than they could by
the biometrical approach. However, these early path models assumed that assorta-
tive mating to be based on homogamy for the social determinants of the phenotype.
Although the actual mechanism of assortment is a matter for empirical investiga-
tion, this strong assumption, being entirely dierent from the mechanisms proposed
by Fisher, precluded an adequate fusion of the Fisher and Wright traditions.
A crucial step was achieved in 1978 and 1979 in a series of publications describ-
ing a more general path model by Cloninger, Rice, and Reich which integrated the
path model for genetic and environmental eects with a Fisherian model for the
consequences of assortment based on phenotype. Since then, the approach of path
analysis has been accepted (even by the descendants of the Birmingham school)
as a rst strategy for analyzing family resemblance, and a number of dierent
nuances of genetic and environmental transmission and mate selection have now
been translated into path models. This does not mean that the method is without
limitations in capturing non-additive eects of genes and environment, but it is
virtually impossible today to conceive of a strategy for the analysis of a complex
human trait that does not include path analysis among the battery of techniques
to be considered.
1.6.5 Development of Statistical Methods
Underlying all of the later developments of the biometrical-genetical, path-analytic
and factor-analytic research programs has been a concern for the statistical prob-
lems of estimation and hypothesis-testing. It is one thing to develop models; to
attach the most ecient and reliable numerical values to the eects specied in
a model, and to decide whether a particular model gives an adequate account of
the empirical data, are completely dierent. All three traditions that we have
identied as being relevant to our work rely heavily on the statistical concept of
32 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
likelihood, introduced by Ronald Fisher as a basis for developing methods for pa-
rameter estimation and hypothesis testing. The approach of maximum likelihood
to estimation in human quantitative genetics was rst introduced in a landmark
paper by Jinks and Fulker (1970) in which they rst applied the theoretical and
statistical methods of biometrical genetics to human behavioral data. Essential
elements of their understanding were that:
1. complex models for human variation could be simplied under the assumption
of polygenic inheritance
2. the goodness-of-t of a model should be tested before waxing lyrical about
the substantive importance of parameter estimates
3. the most precise estimates of parameters should be obtained
4. possibilities exist for specifying and analyzing gene action and genotype
environment interaction
It was the conuence of these notions in a systematic series of models and methods
of data analysis which is mainly responsible for breaking the intellectual gridlock
into which human behavioral genetics had driven itself by the end of the 1960s.
Essentially the same statistical concern was found among those who had fol-
lowed the path analytic and factor analytic approaches. Rao, Morton, and Yee
(1974) used an approach close to maximum likelihood for estimation of parameters
in path models for the correlations between relatives, and earlier work on the analy-
sis of covariance structures by Karl J oreskog had provided some of the rst workable
computer algorithms for applying the method of maximum likelihood to param-
eter estimation and hypothesis-testing in factor analysis. Guided by J oreskogs
inuence, the specication and testing of specic hypotheses about factor rotation
became possible. Subsequently, with the collaboration of Dag S orbom, the analysis
of covariance structures became elaborated into the exible model for Linear Struc-
tural Relations (LISREL) and the associated computer algorithms which, over two
decades, have passed through a series of increasingly general versions.
The attempts to bring genetic methods to bear on psychological variables nat-
urally led to a concern for how the psychometricians interest in multiple variables
could be reconciled with the geneticists methods for separating genetic and envi-
ronmental eects. For example, several investigators (Vandenberg, 1965; Loehlin
and Vandenberg, 1968; Bock and Vandenberg, 1968) in the late 1960s began to
ask whether the genes or the environment was mainly responsible for the general
ability factor underlying correlated measures of cognitive ability. The approaches
that were suggested, however, were relatively crude generalizations of the classi-
cal methods of univariate twin data analysis which were being superseded by the
biometrical and path analytic methods. There was clearly a need to integrate the
model tting approach of biometrical genetics with the factor model which was still
1.6. THE CONTEXT OF OUR APPROACH 33
the conceptual framework of much multivariate analysis in psychology. In discus-
sion with the late Owen White, it became clear that J oreskogs analysis of covari-
ance structures provided the necessary statistical formulation. In 1977, Martin and
Eaves reanalyzed twin data on Thurstones Primary Mental Abilities using their
own FORTRAN program for a multi-group extension of J oreskogs model to twin
data and, for the rst time, used the model tting strategy of biometrical genetics
to test hypotheses, however simple, about the genetic and environmental causes
of covariation between multiple variables. The subsequent wide dissemination of a
multi-group version of LISREL (LISREL III) generated a rash of demonstrations
that what Martin and Eaves had achieved somewhat laboriously with their own
program could be done more easily with LISREL (Boomsma and Molenaar, 1986,
Cantor, 1983; Fulker et al., 1983; Martin et al., 1982; McArdle et al, 1980). After
teaching several workshops and applying LISREL to everyday research problems
in the analysis of twin and family data, we discovered that it too had its limita-
tions and was quite cumbersome to use in several applications. This led to the
development of Mx, which began in 1990 and which has continued throughout this
decade. Initially devised as a combination of a matrix algebra interpreter and a
numerical optimization package, it has simplied the specication of both simple
and complex genetic models tremendously (Neale et al., 2003.
In the 1980s there were many signicant new departures in the specication
of multivariate genetic models for family resemblance. The main emphasis was
on extending the path models, such as those of Cloninger et al., (1979a,b) to the
multivariate case (Neale & Fulker, 1984; Vogler, 1985). Much of this work is
described clearly and in detail by Fulker (1988) . Many of the models described
could not be implemented with the methods readily available at the time of writing
of the rst edition this book. Furthermore, several of the more dicult models were
not addressed in the rst edition because of the lack of suitable data. Since that
time many of the problems of specifying complex models have been solved using
Mx, and this edition presents some of these developments. In addition, several
research groups have now gathered data on samples large and diverse enough to
exploit most of the theoretical developments now in hand.
The collection of large volumes of data in a rich variety of twin studies from
around the world in the last ten years, coupled with the rocketing growth in the
power of micro-computers, oer an unprecedented opportunity. What were once
ground-breaking methods, available to those few who knew enough about statistics
and computers to write their own programs, can now be placed in the hands of
teachers and researchers alike.
34 CHAPTER 1. THE SCOPE OF GENETIC ANALYSES
Chapter 2
Data Preparation
2.1 Introduction
By denition, the primary focus of the study of human individual dierences is
on variation. As we have seen, the covariation between family members can be
especially informative about the causes of variation, so we nowturn to the statistical
techniques used to measure both variation within and covariation between family
members. We start by reviewing the calculation of variances and covariances by
hand, and then illustrate how one may use pograms such as SAS, SPSS and PRELIS
(SAS, 1988; SPSS, 1988; J oreskog and S orbom, 1988) to compute these summary
statistics in a convenient form for use with Mx. Our initial treatment assumes that
we have well-behaved, normally-distributed variables for analysis (see Section 2.2).
However, almost all studies involve some measures that are certainly not normal
because they consist of a few ordered categories, which we call ordinal scales. In
Section 2.3, we deal with the summary of these cruder forms of measurement, and
discuss the concepts of degrees of freedom and goodness-of-t that arise in this
context.
During this decade advances in computer software and hardware have made
the direct analysis of raw data quite practical. As we shall see, this method has
some advantages over the analysis of summary statistics, especially when there are
missing data. Section 2.4 describes the preparation of raw data for analysis with
Mx.
2.2 Continuous Data Analysis
Biometrical analyses of twin data often make use of summary statistics that re-
ect dierences, or variability, between and within members of twin pairs. Some
early studies used mean squares and products, derived from an analysis of variance
35
36 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
(Eaves et al., 1977; Martin and Eaves, 1977; Fulker et al., 1983; Boomsma and
Molenaar, 1987; Molenaar and Boomsma, 1987), but work over the past 15 years
has embraced variance-covariance matrices as the summary statistics of choice.
This approach, often called covariance structure analysis, provides greater exi-
bility in the treatment of some of the processes underlying individual dierences,
such as genotype sex or genotype environment interaction. In addition, vari-
ances and covariances are a more practical data summary for data that include
the relatives of twins, such as parents or spouses (Heath et al., 1985). Because
of the greater generality aorded by variances and covariances, we focus on these
quantities rather than mean squares.
2.2.1 Calculating Summary Statistics by Hand
The variances and covariances used in twin analyses often are computed using a
statistical package such as SPSS (SPSS, 1988) or SAS (SAS, 1988), or by PRELIS
(J oreskog and S orbom, 1988). Nevertheless, it is useful to examine how they are
calculated in order to ensure a comprehensive understanding of ones observed
data. In this section we describe the calculation of means, variances, covariances,
and correlations.
Some simulated measurements from 16 MZ and 16 DZ twin pairs are presented
in Table 2.1. The observed values in the columns labelled Twin 1 and Twin 2 have
been selected to illustrate some elementary principles of variation in twins
1
.
In order to obtain the summary statistics of variances and covariances for ge-
netic analysis, it is rst necessary to compute the average value for a set of measure-
ments, called the mean. The mean is typically denoted by a bar over the variable
name for a group of observations, for example X or Twin1 or Twin2. The formula
for calculation of the mean is:
X =
X
1
+X
2
+ +X
n
n
=
n

i=1
X
i
n
, (2.1)
in which X
i
represents the i
th
observation and n is the total number of observations.
In the twin data of Table 2.1, the mean of the measurements on Twin 1 of the MZ
pairs is
Twin1 =
3 + 3 + 2 + + 2 + 2 + 1
16
= 32/16
= 2.0
1
These data are for illustration only; they would normally be treated as ordinal, not continuous,
and would be summarized dierently, as described in Section 2.3. Note also that we do not need
to have equal numbers of pairs in the two groups.
2.2. CONTINUOUS DATA ANALYSIS 37
Table 2.1: Simulated measurements from MZ and DZ Twin Pairs.
MZ DZ
Twin 1 Twin 2 Twin 1 Twin 2
3 2 0 1
3 3 2 3
2 1 1 2
1 2 4 3
0 0 3 1
2 2 2 2
2 2 2 2
3 2 1 3
3 3 3 4
2 3 1 0
1 1 1 1
1 1 2 1
4 4 3 3
2 3 3 2
2 1 2 2
1 2 2 2
The mean for the second MZ twin (Twin2) also is 2.0, as are the means for both
DZ twins.
The variance of the observations represents a measure of dispersion around the
mean; that is, how much, on average, observations dier from the mean. The
variance formula for a sample of measurements, often represented as s
2
or V
MZ
or
V
DZ
, is
s
2
=
(X
1
X)
2
+ (X
2
X)
2
+ + (X
n
X)
2
n 1
=
n

i=1
(X
i
X)
2
n 1
(2.2)
We note two things: rst, the dierence between each observation and the mean
is squared. In principle, absolute dierences from the mean could be used as a
measure of variation, but absolute dierences have a greater variance than squared
dierences (Fisher, 1920), and are therefore less ecient for use as a summary
statistic. Likewise, higher powers (e.g.

n
i=1
(X
i
X)
4
) also have greater variance.
In fact, Fisher showed that the square of the dierence is the most informative
measure of variance, i.e., it is a sucient statistic. Second, the sum of the squared
38 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
deviations is divided by n 1 rather than n. The denominator is n 1 in order to
compensate for an underestimate in the sample variance which would be obtained
if s
2
were divided by n. (This arises from the fact that we have already used one
parameter the mean to describe the data; see Mood & Graybill, 1963 for a
discussion of bias in sample variance). Again using the twin data in Table 2.1 as
an example, the variance of MZ Twin 1 is
V
MZT1
=
(3 2)
2
+ (3 2)
2
+ + (2 2)
2
+ (1 2)
2
15
=
1 + 1 + 0 + + 0 + 0 + 1
15
= 16/15
The variances of data from the second MZ twin, DZ Twin 1, and DZ Twin 2 also
equal 16/15.
Covariances are computationally similar to variances, but represent mean devia-
tions which are shared by two sets of observations. In the twin example, covariances
are useful because they indicate the extent to which deviations from the mean by
Twin 1 are similar to the second twins deviations from the mean. Thus, the co-
variance between observations of Twin 1 and Twin 2 represents a scale-dependent
measure of twin similarity. Covariances are often denoted by s
x,y
or Cov
MZ
or
Cov
DZ
, and are calculated as
s
x,y
=
(X
1
X)(Y
1
Y ) + (X
2
X)(Y
2
Y ) + + (X
n
X)(Y
n
Y )
n 1
=
n

i=1
(X
i
X)(Y
i
Y )
n 1
(2.3)
Note that the variance formula shown in Eq. 2.2 is just a special case of the covari-
ance when Y
i
= X
i
. In other words, the variance is simply the covariance between
a variable and itself.
For the twin data in Table 2.1, the covariance between MZ twins is
Cov
MZ
=
(3 2)(2 2) + (3 2)(3 2) + + (1 2)(2 2)
15
=
0 + 1 + 0 + 0 + + 4 + 0 + 0 + 0
15
= 12/15
The covariance between DZ pairs may be calculated similarly to give 8/15.
The correlation coecient is closely related to the covariance between two sets
of observations. Correlations may be interpreted in a similar manner as covari-
ances, but are rescaled to give a lower bound of -1.0 and an upper bound of 1.0.
2.2. CONTINUOUS DATA ANALYSIS 39
The correlation coecient, r, may be calculated using the covariance between two
measures and the square root of the variance (the standard deviation) of each mea-
sure:
r =
Cov
x,y
_
V
x
V
y
(2.4)
For the simulated MZ twin data, the correlation between twins is
r
MZ
=
12/15
_
(16/15)(16/15)
= 12/16 = .75,
and the DZ twin correlation is
r
DZ
=
8/15
_
(16/15)(16/15)
= 8/16 = .50
Although variances and covariances typically dene the observed information
for biometrical analyses of twin data, correlations are useful for comparing resem-
blances between twins as a function of genetic relatedness. In the simulated twin
data, the MZ twin correlation (r = .75) is greater than that of the DZ twins
(r = .50). This greater similarity of MZ twins may be due to several sources of
variation (discussed in subsequent chapters), but at the least is suggestive of a
heritable basis for the trait, as increased MZ similarity could result from the fact
that MZ twins are genetically identical, whereas DZ twins share only 1/2 of their
genes on average.
2.2.2 Using SAS to Summarize Data
The statistical packages SAS and SPSS are probably the most widely-used ways
to store data collected in twin studies. In some cases relational databases such as
Oracle, DB2, Paradox and Ingres may be used to store data collected from relatives
because these oer powerful ways to maintain data in a consistent fashion according
to normal form. Normal form is a way of storing data that avoids duplication of
information; this is very important to avoid inconsistencies in the data. The general
strategy may then be to use SAS or SPSS to extract the data from the database,
to do preliminary data cleaning, to compute scales scores and transform them as
necessary, and nally to dump the data in a format suitable for analysis with Mx.
Here we discuss the advantages and disadvantages of this approach, and illustrate
it with sample SAS scripts.
By creating intermediate les for Mx to read, we are violating an elementary
database principle to keep data in one place and one place only. This principle
arises from the observation that almost as soon as there are two copies of data
they become inconsistent and the updating chore requires more than double the
40 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
eort as both sets must be updated and inconsistencies must be resolved. For that
reason, it is best to consider the database as a master and to make updates to
that dataset and that dataset only. Data analysis then involves creation of the
intermediate data les using the same SAS or SPSS script. There are some very
important advantages to this procedure. First, we know that the intermediate,
le is not going to be updated by anyone else during our analysis especially
important in a multi-user environment. We want the comparison of models to be
conducted on the same data, not on data that have changed from one analysis
to the next! Second, the computation time taken to extract the data from the
database may be non-trivial and it does not have to be repeated for every analysis.
SAS scripts to compute covariance matrices
This is not the place to describe in detail the workings of SAS; the thousands of
pages in the manuals are quite adequate! All we aim to do here is to get the data in
and get the covariance matrix and means out. SAS has a useful procedure, PROC
CORR, which will print the required statistics, which can be cut and pasted into a
le for Mx use. However, as is commonly the case with computer tasks, investing
a little extra initial work on automation will save labor in the long run, and will
be more error-proof.
It often happens that data are stored at the individual subject level rather
than at the family level. Typically, each subject has a family number and an id
number to mark their position in the family (rst or second twin). A necessary
step to analyse the covariance between relatives is to glue the data from family
members together so that the family becomes the unit of measurement and co-
variances between family members may be computed. In SAS this is a relatively
simple operation although care must be taken to supply labels for the variables
that do not exceed the SAS maximum length of eight characters. The SAS script
in Appendix ?? shows the case for twin data, and goes beyond the initial require-
ment by taking the sex of the twins into account. Five groups are created, being
MZ male, DZ male, MZ female, DZ female and opposite DZ. The covariances
are computed and output to .dat les which contain the number of observations
(Nobservations), the number of input variables (NInput), labels (Labels), and
the covariance matrices (CMatrix). These .dat les may be used directly in Mx
in a diagram, or in a script using the #Include statement.
Note that the assignment of the twins as 1 or 2 is usually arbitrary for the same
sex groups, but in the opposite sex group the male (or female) twin is always rst,
and the female (or male) twin second. Strictly speaking, when there is no inherent
order to the observations the variance-covariance matrix is not the best summary
statistic to use. The intraclass correlation is the most appropriate summary for
observations that do not have any order; it uses a joint estimate of the variance
of twin 1 and twin 2, and partitions this into within pairs and between pairs com-
ponents. However, the intraclass correlation is more dicult to generalize to the
2.2. CONTINUOUS DATA ANALYSIS 41
multivariate and multiple classes of relatives situations so we stay with covariance
matrices here. Sometimes data on birth order or some other characteristic may be
used to distinguish more formally between twin 1 and twin 2 within a pair, thereby
giving some rationality to the ordering and use of covariance matrices. Should such
an approach be taken, it is necessary to split the DZ opposite sex twin group into
two groups according to whether the rst twin is female or male.
Appendix ?? shows a SAS macro for creating an Mx .dat le, which fully
describes the data: the number of variables, the sample size, the means and covari-
ances, and optionally labels for each of hte variables. Comments, beginning with !
indicate the date the le was created. The resulting .dat le might look like this:
!
! Mx dat file created by SAS on 03FEB1998
!
Data NInputvars=4 NObservations=844
CMatrix Full
1.0086 -0.0148 -0.0317 -0.0443
-0.0148 1.0169 -0.0062 0.0068
-0.0317 -0.0062 0.9342 0.0596
-0.0443 0.0068 0.0596 0.9697
Means
0.0139 -0.0729 0.0722 0.0159
Labels T1F1 T1F2 T2F1 T2F2
As will be seen in later chapters, this le is ready for immediate use for drawing
path diagrams in the Mx GUI or in an Mx script with the #include command.
2.2.3 Using PRELIS to Summarize Continuous Data
PRELIS was developped by Karl J oreskog and Dag S orbom as a preprocessor for
LISREL(J oreskog and S orbom, 1988). Here we apply PRELIS to the simulated
MZ twin data, and briey discuss some of the further features of the software. In
practice, data on MZ and DZ twins may be placed in separate les, often with one
or more lines of data per twin pair
2
. It is easy to use PRELIS to generate summary
statistics such as means and covariances for structural equation model tting.
Suppose that the MZ twin data in Table 2.1 are stored in a le called MZ.RAW
in the following way:
3 2
3 3
. .
. .
2
It is possible to use data les that contain both types of twins and some code to discriminate
between them, but it is less ecient.
42 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
. .
2 1
1 2
We can use free format to read these data. Free format means that there is
at least one space or end-of-line character between consecutive data items. These
data could be entered using any simple text editor. If a wordprocessor such as
Wordperfect or Microsoft Word were used, it would be necessary to save the le
as a DOS or ASCII text le. Next, we would prepare an ASCII le containing the
PRELIS commands to read these data and compute the means and covariances.
We refer to les containing program commands as scripts; the PRELIS script in
this case might look like this:
Simple prelis example to compute MZ covariances
DA NI=2 NO=0
LA
Twin1 Twin2
COntinuous Twin1 Twin2
RAw FIle=MZ.RAW
OU SM=MZ.COV MA=CM
The rst line is simply a title. PRELIS will treat all lines as part of the title until
a line beginning with DA is encountered. The DAta line is used to specify basic
features of the input (raw) data such as the number of input variables (NI) and the
number of observations (NO). Here we have specied the number of observations as
zero (NO=0), which asks PRELIS to count the number of cases for us. The next two
lines of the script supply labels (LA) for the variables; these are optional but highly
recommended when more than a few variables are to be read. Next, we dene
the variables Twin1 and Twin2 as continuous. By default, PRELIS 2 will treat
any variable with less than 15 categories as ordinal. Although this is a reasonable
statistical approach, it is not what we want for the purposes of this example. The
next line in the script (beginning RAw) tells PRELIS where to nd the data, and
the OUtput line signies the end of the script, and requests the covariance matrices
(MA=CM) to be saved in the le MZ.COV. This output le is created by PRELIS
it is also ASCII format and looks like this:
(6D13.6)
.106667D+01 .800000D+00 .106667D+01
The rst line of the le contains a FORTRAN format for reading the data. The
reader is referred to almost any text on FORTRAN, including Users Guides, for
a detailed description of formats. The format used here is D format, for double
precision. The 3 characters after the D give the power of 10 by which the printed
number should be multiplied, so our .106667D+01 is really .10666710
1
= 1.06667.
This number is part of the lower triangle of the covariance matrix. Since covariance
2.3. ORDINAL DATA ANALYSIS 43
matrices are always symmetric, only the lower triangle is needed. The le may in
turn be read by Mx for the purposes of structural equation model tting using
syntax such as
CMatrix File=MZ.COV
within an Mx script Mx by default expects only the lower triangle of covariance
matrices to be supplied.
Suppose that, instead of just two variables, we had a data le with 20 variables
per subject, with two lines for a twin pair. Also suppose that one of the variables
identies the zygosity of the pair, we wish to select only those pairs where zygosity
is 1, and we only want the covariance of four of the variables. We could read these
data into PRELIS using a FORTRAN format statement explicitly given in the
PRELIS script. The script might look like this:
PRELIS script to select MZs and compute covariances of 4 variables
DA NI=40 NO=0
LA
Zygosity Twin1P1 Twin1P2 Twin2P1 Twin2P2
RA FIle=MZ.RAW FO
(3X,F1.0,2x,F5.0,12X,F5.0/6X,F5.0,12X,F5.0)
SD Zygosity=1
OU SM=MZ.COV MA=CM
Note the FOrtran keyword at the end of the raw data line, indicating that the next
line contains a Fortran format statement. The SD command selects cases where
zygosity is 1, and deletes zygosity from the list of variables to be analyzed. Note
that the FORTRAN format implicitly skips all the irrelevant variables, retaining
only ve (as specied by the F1.0 and F5.0 elds). Although we could have started
with a more complete list of variables, read them in with an appropriate FORMAT,
and used the PRELIS command SD to delete those we did not want, it is more
ecient to save the program the trouble of reading these data by adjusting our
NI and format statement. On the other hand, if the data le is not large or if a
powerful computer is available, it may be better to use SD to save user time spent
modifying the script.
2.3 Ordinal Data Analysis
Suppose that instead of making measurements on a continuous scale, we are able
to discriminate only a few ordered categories with our measuring instrument. This
situation is commonly encountered when assessing the presence or absence of dis-
ease, or responses to a single item on a questionnaire. Although it is possible to
calculate a covariance matrix from these data, the correlations usually will be bi-
ased. The degree of bias depends on factors such as the number of categories and
44 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
f(x)
0.00
0.05
0.10
0.15
0.20
0.25
0.30
0.35
0.40
0.45
x
-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3
Figure 2.1: Univariate normal distribution with thresholds distinguishing ordered
response categories.
the number of observations in each category, and usually results in an underesti-
mate of the true liability correlation in the population. In this section we describe
methods for summarizing ordinal data.
2.3.1 Univariate Normal Distribution of Liability
One approach to the analysis of ordinal data is to assume that the ordered cate-
gories reect imprecise measurement of an underlying normal distribution of liabil-
ity. A second assumption is that the liability distribution has one or more threshold
values that discriminate between the categories (see Figure 2.1). This model has
been used widely in genetic applications (Falconer, 1960; Neale et al., 1986; Neale,
1988; Heath et al 1989a). As long as we consider one variable at a time, it is always
possible to place the thresholds so that the proportion of the distribution lying be-
tween adjacent thresholds exactly matches the observed proportion of the sample
that is found in each category. For example, suppose we had an item with four
possible responses: none, a little, quite a lot, and a great deal. In a sample of
200 subjects, 20 say none, 80 say a little, 98 say quite a lot and 2 say a great
deal. If our assumed underlying normal distribution has mean 0 and variance 1,
then placing thresholds at z-values of -1.282, 0.0 and 2.326 would partition the
normal distribution as required. In mathematical terms, if there are p categories,
p 1 thresholds are needed to divide the distribution. The expected proportion
2.3. ORDINAL DATA ANALYSIS 45
lying in category i is
_
ti
ti1
(x) dx
where t
0
= , t
p
= , and (x) is the unit variance normal probability density
function (pdf), given by
(x) =
e
.5x
2

2
This formulation is really a parametric model for the distribution of ordinal re-
sponses.
2.3.2 Bivariate Normal Distribution of Liability
When we have only one variable, there is no goodness-of-t test for the liability
model because it always gives a perfect t. However, this is not necessarily so when
we move to the multivariate case. Consider rst, the example where we have two
variables, each measured as a simple yes/no binary response. Data collected from
a sample of subjects could be summarized as a contingency table:
Item 1
Item 2 No Yes
Yes 13 55
No 32 15
It is at this point that we encounter the crucial statistical concept of degrees of
freedom (df). Fortunately, though important, calculating the number of df for
a model is usually very easy; it is simply the dierence between the number of
observed statistics and the number of parameters in the model. In the present case
we have a 2 2 contingency table in which there are four observed frequencies.
However, if we take the total sample size as given and work with the proportion
of the sample observed in each cell, we only need three proportions to describe
the table completely, because the total of the cell proportions is 1 and the last
cell proportion always can be obtained by subtraction. Thus in general for a table
with r rows and c columns we can describe the data as rc frequencies or as rc 1
proportions and the total sample size. The next question is, how many parameters
does our model contain?
The natural extension of the univariate normal liability model described above
is to assume that there is a continuous, bivariate normal distribution underlying the
distribution of our observations. Given this model, we can compute the expected
proportions for the four cells of the contingency table
3
The model is illustrated
graphically as contour and 3-D plots in Figure 2.2. The gures contrast the uncor-
related case (r = 0) with a high correlation in liability (r = .9) and are dramatically
3
Mathematically these expected proportions can be written as double integrals.
46 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
Z 0.008 0.032 0.057 0.081
0.105 0.129 0.154
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
Z 0.043 0.170 0.298 0.426
0.553 0.681 0.809
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 X
Y
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
3
Z
0.000
0.053
0.106
0.159
-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 X
Y
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
3
Z
0.000
0.279
0.558
0.838
Figure 2.2: Contour and 3-D plots of the bivariate normal distribution with
thresholds distinguishing two response categories. Contour plot in top left shows
zero correlation in liability and plot in bottom left shows correlation of .9; the
panels on the right shows the same data as 3-D plots.
2.3. ORDINAL DATA ANALYSIS 47
similar to the scatterplots of data from unrelated persons and from MZ twins, shown
in Figures 1.2 and 1.4 on pages 6 and 9. By adjusting the correlation in liability
and the two thresholds, the model can predict any combination of proportions in
the four cells. Because we use 3 parameters to predict the 3 observed proportions,
there are no degrees of freedom to test the goodness of t of the model. This can
be seen when we consider an arbitrary non-normal distribution created by mixing
two normal distributions, one with r = +.9 and the second with r = .9, as shown
in Figure 2.3. With thresholds imposed as shown, equal proportions are expected
(a)
Z 0.043 0.170 0.298 0.426
0.553 0.681 0.809
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
(b)
Z 0.08 0.34 0.59 0.84
1.09 1.34 1.59
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
Figure 2.3: Contour plots of a bivariate normal distribution with correlation .9
(top); and of a mixture of bivariate normal distributions (bottom), one with .9
correlation and the other with -.9 correlation. One threshold in each dimension is
shown.
in each cell, corresponding to a zero correlation and zero thresholds, not an un-
reasonable result but with just two categories we have no knowledge at all that our
distribution is such a bizarre non-normal example. The case of a 22 contingency
table is really a worst case scenario for no degrees of freedom associated with a
model, since absolutely any pattern of observed frequencies could be accounted for
with the liability model. Eectively, all the model does is to transform the data;
it cannot be falsied.
48 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
2.3.3 Testing the Normal Distribution Assumption
The problem of having no degrees of freedom to test the goodness of t of the
bivariate normal distribution to two binary variables is solved when we have at
least three categories in one variable and at least two in the other. To illustrate
this point, compare the contour plots shown in Figure 2.4 in which two thresholds
have been specied for the two variables. With the bivariate normal distribution,
(a)
Z 0.043 0.170 0.298 0.426
0.553 0.681 0.809
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
(b)
Z 0.08 0.34 0.59 0.84
1.09 1.34 1.59
Y
-3.0
-1.5
0.0
1.5
3.0
X
-3.0 -1.5 0.0 1.5 3.0
Figure 2.4: Contour plots of a bivariate normal distribution with correlation .9
(top) and a mixture of bivariate normal distributions, one with .9 correlation and
the other with -.9 correlation (bottom). Two thresholds in each dimension are
shown.
there is a very strong pattern imposed on the relative magnitudes of the cells on
the diagonal and elsewhere. There is a similar set of constraints with the mixture
of normals, but quite dierent predictions are made about the o-diagonal cells; all
four corner cells would have an appreciable frequency given a sucient sample size,
and probably in excess of that in each of the four cells in the middle of each side
[e.g., (1,2)]. The bivariate normal distribution could never be adjusted to perfectly
predict the cell proportions obtained from the mixture of distributions.
This intuitive idea of opportunities for failure translates directly into the concept
of degrees of freedom. When we use a bivariate normal liability model to predict
the proportions in a contingency table with r rows and c columns, we use r 1
thresholds for the rows, c 1 thresholds for the columns, and one parameter for
the correlation in liability, giving r +c 1 in total. The table itself contains rc 1
2.3. ORDINAL DATA ANALYSIS 49
proportions, neglecting the total sample size as above. Therefore we have degrees
of freedom equal to:
df = rc 1 (r +c 1) (2.5)
df = rc r c (2.6)
The discrepancy between the frequencies predicted by the model and those actually
observed in the data can be measured using the
2
statistic given by:

2
=
r

i=1
c

j=1
(O
ij
E
ij
)
2
E
ij
Given a large enough sample, the models failure to predict the observed data would
be reected in a signicant
2
for the goodness of t.
In principle, models could be tted by maximum likelihood directly to con-
tingency tables, employing the observed and expected cell proportions. This ap-
proach is general and exible, especially for the multigroup case the programs
LISCOMP (Muthen, 1987) and Mx (Neale, 1991) use the method but it is
currently limited by computational considerations. When we move from two vari-
ables to larger examples involving many variables, integration of the multivariate
normal distribution (which has to be done numerically) becomes extremely time-
consuming, perhaps increasing by a factor of ten or so for each additional variable.
An alternative approach to this problem is to use PRELIS 2 to compute each
correlation in a pairwise fashion, and to compute a weight matrix. The weight
matrix is an estimate of the variances and covariances of the correlations. The
variances of the correlations certainly have some intuitive appeal, being a measure
of how precisely each correlation is estimated. However, the idea of a correlation
correlating with another correlation may seem strange to a newcomer to the eld.
Yet this covariation between correlations is precisely what we need in order to
represent how much additional information the second correlation supplies over
and above that provided by the rst correlation. Armed with these two types of
summary statistics the correlation matrix and the covariances of the correlations,
we may t models using a structural equation modeling package such as Mx or
LISREL, and make statistical inferences from the goodness of t of the model.
It is also possible to use the bivariate normal liability distribution to infer the
patterns of statistics that would be observed if ordinal and continuous variables
were correlated. Essentially, there are specic predictions made about the expected
mean and variance of the continuous variable in each of the categories of the ordinal
variable. For example, the continuous variable means are predicted to increase
monotonically across the categories if there is a correlation between the liabilities.
An observed pattern of a high mean in category 1, low in category 2 and high again
in category 3 would not be consistent with the model. The number of parameters
used to describe this model for an ordinal variable with r categories is r + 2,
since we use r 1 for the thresholds, one each for the mean and variance of the
50 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
continuous variable, and one for the covariance between the two variables. The
observed statistics involved are the proportions in the cells (less one because the
nal proportion may be obtained by subtraction from 1) and the mean and variance
of the continuous variable in each category. Therefore we have:
df
oc
= (r 1) + 2r (r + 2) (2.7)
= 2r 3
So the number of degrees of freedom for such a test is 2r 3 where r is the number
of categories.
2.3.4 Terminology for Types of Correlation
One of the diculties encountered by the newcomer to statistics is the use of a wide
variety of terms for correlation coecients. There are many measures of association
between variables; here we conne ourselves to the parametric statistics computed
by normal theory. These statistics correspond most naturally to our genetic theory,
in which we assume that a large number of independent genetic and environmental
factors give rise to variation multifactorial inheritance
4
.
Table 2.2 shows the name given to the correlation coecient calculated under
normal distribution theory, according to whether each variable has: two categories
(dichotomous); several categories (polychotomous); or an innite number of cat-
egories (continuous). If both variables are dichotomous, then the correlation is
called a tetrachoric correlation as long as it is calculated using the bivariate nor-
mal integration approach described in Section 2.3 above. If we simply use the
Pearson product moment formula (described in Section 2.2.1 above) then we have
computed a phi-coecient which will probably underestimate the population cor-
relation in liability. Because the tetrachoric and polychoric are calculated with the
same method, some authors refer to the tetrachoric as a polychoric, and the same
is true of the use of polyserial instead of biserial. As we shall see, the theory behind
all these statistics is essentially the same.
2.3.5 Using PRELIS to Summarize Ordinal Data
Here we give a PRELIS script to read only two from a long list of psychiatric
diagnoses, coded as 1 or 0 in these data.
Diagnoses and age MZ twins: VARIABLES ARE:
DEPLN4 DEPLN2 DEPLN1 DEPLB4 DEPLB2 DEPLB1 GADLN6 GADLN1
GADLB6 GADLB1 GAD88B GAD88N PANN PANB PHON PHOB ETOHN
4
In fact quite a small number of genetic factors may give rise to a distribution which is for
almost all practical purposes indistinguishable from a normal distribution (Kendler and Kidd,
1986).
2.3. ORDINAL DATA ANALYSIS 51
Table 2.2: Classication of correlations according to their observed distribution.
Two Three or more
Measurement Categories Categories Continuous
Two Tetrachoric Polychoric Biserial
Three or more Polychoric Polychoric Polyserial
Continuous Biserial Polyserial Product Moment
ETOHB ANON ANOB BULN BULB DEPLN4T2 DEPLN2T2 DEPLN1T2
DEPLB4T2 DEPLB2T2 DEPLB1T2 GADLN6T2 GADLN1T2 GADLB6T2
GADLB1T2 GAD88BT2 GAD88NT2 PANNT2 PANBT2 PHONT2 PHOBT2
ETOHNT2 ETOHBT2 ANONT2 ANOBT2 BULNT2 BULBT2/
FORMAT IN FULL IS:
(2X, F8.2,F1.0, 43(1X,F1.0)
Diagnoses and age MZ twins
DA NI=3 NO=0
LA; DOB DEPLN4 DEPLN4T2
RA FI=DIAGMZ.DAT FO
(2X, F8.2,F1.0, 43x,F1.0)
OR DEPLN4-DEPLN4T2
OU MA=PM SM=DEPLN4MZ.COR SA=DEPLN4MZ.ASY PA
Diagnoses and age DZ twins
DA NI=3 NO=0
LA; DOB DEPLN4 DEPLN4T2
RA FI=DIAGDZ.DAT FO
(2X, F8.2,F1.0, 43x,F1.0)
OR DEPLN4-DEPLN4T2
OU MA=PM SM=DEPLN4DZ.COR SA=DEPLN4DZ.ASY PA
Note that again we have used the FORTRAN format to control which variables
are read. One key dierence from the continuous case is the use of MA=PM, which
requests calculation of a matrix of polychoric, polyserial and product moment cor-
relations. The program uses product moment correlations when both variables
are continuous, a polyserial (or biserial) when one is ordinal and the other con-
tinuous, and a polychoric (or tetrachoric) when both are ordinal. Running the
script produces four output les DEPLN4MZ.COR, DEPLN4MZ.ASY, DEPLN4DZ.COR and
DEPLN4DZ.ASY which may be read directly into Mx using PMatrix and ACovariance
commands. Notice that we have stacked two scripts in one le, one to read and
compute statistics from the MZ data le (FI=DIAGMZ.DAT) and a second to do the
52 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
same thing for the DZ data. Also notice that the SM command is used to output
the correlation matrix and SA is to save the asymptotic weight matrix. In fact,
PRELIS saves the weight matrix multiplied by the sample size which is what Mx
expects to receive when the ACov command is used. The PA command requests that
the asymptotic weight matrix itself be printed in the output. However, PRELIS
saves this le in a binary format which must be converted to ASCII for use with
Mx. The utility bin2asc, supplied with PRELIS, can be used for this purpose.
In the PRELIS output, there are a number of summary statistics for contin-
uous variables (means and standard deviations, and histograms) and frequency
distributions with bar graphs, for the ordinal variables. To provide the user with
some guide to the origin of statistics describing the covariance between variables,
PRELIS prints means and standard deviations of continuous variables separately
for each category of each pair of ordinal variables, and contingency tables between
each ordinal variables. Towards the end of the output there is a table printed with
the following format:
TEST OF MODEL
CORRELATION CHI-SQU. D.F. P-VALUE
___________ ________ ____ _______
DEPLN4 VS. DOB -.233 (PS) 5.067 1 .024
DEPLN4T2 VS. DOB .010 (PS) 6.703 1 .010
There are two quite dierent chi-squared tests printed on the output. The rst,
under TEST OF MODEL is a test of the goodness of t of the bivariate normal dis-
tribution model to the data. In the case of two ordinal variables with r and c
categories in each, there are rc r c df as described in expression 2.5 above.
Likewise there will be 2r 3 df for the continuous by ordinal statistics, as de-
scribed in expression 2.7. If the p-value reported by PRELIS is low (e.g. < .05),
then concern arises about whether the bivariate normal distribution model is ap-
propriate for these data. For a polyserial correlation (correlations between ordinal
and continuous variables), it may simply be that the continuous variable is not
normally distributed, or that the association between the variables does not follow
a bivariate normal distribution. For polychoric correlations, there is no univari-
ate test of normality involved, so failure of the model would imply that the latent
liability distributions do not follow a bivariate normal. Remember however that
signicance levels for these tests are not often the reported p-value, because we are
performing multiple tests. If the tests were independent, then with n such tests the
signicance level would not be the reported p-value but 1 (1 p)
n
. Therefore
concern would arise only if p was very small and a large number of tests had been
performed. In our case, the tests are not independent because, for example, the
correlation of A and B is not independent of the correlation of A and C, so the
attenuation of the level of signicance is not so extreme as the 1(1p)
n
formula
predicts. The amount of attenuation will be application specic, but would often
2.4. PREPARING RAW DATA 53
be closer to 1 (1 p)
n
than simply to p.
The second chi-squared statistic printed by PRELIS (not shown in the above
sample of output) tests whether the correlation is signicantly dierent from zero.
A similar result should be obtained if the summary statistics are supplied to Mx,
and a chi-squared dierence test is performed between a model which allows the
correlation to be a free parameter, and one in which the correlation is set to zero.
The use of weight matrices as input to Mx is described elsewhere in this book.
Here we have described the generation of a weight matrix for a correlation matrix,
but it is also possible to use weight matrices for covariance matrices
5
. Both meth-
ods are part of the asymptotically distribution free (ADF) methods pioneered by
Browne (1984). It is not yet clear whether maximum likelihood or ADF methods
are generally better for coping with data that are not multinormally distributed;
further simulation studies are required. The ADF methods require more numerical
eort and become cumbersome to use with large numbers of variables. This is so
because the size of the weight matrix rapidly increases with number of variables.
The number of elements on and below the diagonal of a matrix is a triangular
number given by k(k + 1)/2. The number of elements in the weight matrix is a
triangular number of a triangular number, or
k
4
+ 2k
3
+ 3k
2
+ 2k
8
In the case of correlation matrices, the number of elements is somewhat less, but
still increases as a quadratic function:
k
4
2k
3
+ 3k
2
2k
8
As a compromise when the number of variables is large, J oreskog and S orbom
suggest the use of diagonal weights, i.e. just the variances of the correlations and
not their covariances. However, tests of signicance are likely to be inaccurate with
this method and estimates of anything other than the full or true model would be
biased.
2.4 Preparing Raw Data
Almost by denition, raw data does not need to be prepared for analysis. However,
computer programs rarely communicate with each other without some form of
translation of data format, and getting data out of datasets maintained in popular
statistical packages such as SAS or SPSS and into Mx is no exception. In this
section we briey describe SAS scripts that output data into a le suitable for Mx
to read.
5
The number of elements in a weight matrix for a covariance matrix is greater than that for a
correlation matrix. For this reason, it is necessary to specify Matrix=PMatrix on the Data line of
a Mx job that is to read a weight matrix.
54 CHAPTER 2. DATA PREPARATION
Mx has two main ways to read individual scores. First, and most straightfor-
ward, is rectangular format, with one case per line, with variables separated by
one or more spaces. A case is a collection of possibly correlated observations, such
as several variables assessed on an individual, or on both members of a twin pair,
or on a whole family. Because family members correlate, it is necessary to consider
the whole family as a case. Separate cases are assumed to be uncorrelated, which
is important for statistical purposes. Certain new methods available in programs
such as Sudaan, SAS proc mixed, and Stata make it possible to account for some
correlation between dierent cases, usually when data are grouped, e.g., subjects in
the same school. These methods can prove useful for running standard statistical
analyses at the individual level (multiple regression, survival analysis) by taking
into account the covariation between family members. However, they do not help
with the preparation of data for modeling genetic and environmental factors which
is the primary objective here.
The default code that Mx recognises as indicating missing data is a dot .
which is the same as SAS. A sample SAS script to produce rectangular data is
shown in Appendix ??. Mxs missing command can be used to declare a dierent
string as the missing value, and it is important to note that this is a string and not
a numeric value, as 1.0 and 1.00 will be considered to be dierent.
The second main format for raw data that Mx accepts is variable length, or vl.
2.5 Summary
We have described in detail the statistical operations involved in, and the use of
SAS and PRELIS for, the measurement of variation and covariation. When we
have continuous measures, the calculations are quite simple and can be done by
hand, but for ordinal data the process is more complex. We obtain estimates of
polychoric and polyserial correlations by using software that numerically integrates
the bivariate normal distribution. In the process, we are eectively tting a model
of continuous multivariate normal liability with abrupt thresholds to the contin-
gency table. This model cannot be rejected when there are only two categories for
each measure, but may fail as the number of cells in the table increases.
While ordinal data are far more common than continuous measures in the be-
havioral sciences, we note that as the number of categories gets large (e.g., more
than 15) the dierence between the continuous and the ordinal treatments gets
small. In general, the researcher should try to obtain continuous measures if pos-
sible, since considerable statistical power can be lost when only a few response
categories are used, as we shall show in Chapter 7.
Chapter 3
Biometrical Genetics
3.1 Introduction and Description of Terminology
The principles of biometrical and quantitative genetics lie at the heart of virtually
all of the statistical models examined in this book. Thus, an understanding of
biometrical genetics is fundamental to our statistical approach to twin and fam-
ily data. Biometrical models relate the latent, or unobserved, variables of our
structural models to the functional eects of genes. It is these eects, based on the
principles of Mendelian genetics, that give our structural models a degree of valid-
ity quite unusual in the social sciences. The purpose of this chapter is to provide a
brief introduction to biometrical models. Extensive treatments of the subject have
been provided by Mather and Jinks (1982) and Falconer (1990). Here we employ
the notation of Mather and Jinks.
Before we begin our discussion of biometrical genetics, we must describe some
of the terms that are encountered frequently in biometrical and classical genetic
discourse. For the present purposes, we use the term gene in reference to a unit
factor of inheritance that inuences an observable trait or traits, following the ear-
lier usage by Fuller and Thompson (1978). Observable characteristics are referred
to as phenotypes. The site of a gene on a chromosome is known as the locus. Alleles
are alternative forms of a gene that occupy the same locus on a chromosome. They
often are symbolized as A and a or B and b or A
1
and A
2
. The simplest system
for a segregating locus involves only two alleles (A and a), but there also may be
a large number of alleles in a system. For example, the HLA locus on chromosome
6 is known to have 18 alleles at the A locus, 41 alleles at the B locus, 8 at C,
about 20 at DR, 3 at DQ, and 6 at DP (Bodmer 1987). Nevertheless, if one or
two alleles are much more frequent than the others, a two-allele system provides a
useful approximation and leads to an accurate account for the phenotypic variation
and covariation with which we are concerned. The genotype is the chromosomal
complement of alleles for an individual. At a single locus (with two alleles) the
55
56 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
genotype may be symbolized AA, Aa, or aa; if we consider multiple loci the geno-
type of an individual may be written as AABB, AABb, AAbb, AaBB, AaBb, Aabb,
aaBB, aaBb, or aabb, in the case of two loci, for example. Homozygosity refers to
a state of identical alleles at corresponding loci on homologous chromosomes; for
example, AA or aa for one locus, or AABB, aabb, AAbb, or aaBB for two loci. In
contrast, heterozygosity refers to a state of unlike alleles at corresponding loci, Aa
or AaBb, for example. When numeric or symbolic values are assigned to specic
genotypes they are called genotypic values. The additive value of a gene is the
sum of the average eects of the individual alleles. Dominance deviations refer to
the extent to which genotypes dier from the additive genetic value. A system in
which multiple loci are involved in the expression of a single trait is called polygenic
(many genes). A pleiotropic system (many growths) is one in which the same
gene or genes inuence more than one trait.
Biometrical models are based on the measurable eects of dierent genotypes
that arise at a segregating locus, which are summed across all of the loci that con-
tribute to a continuously varying trait. The number of loci generally is not known,
but it is usual to assume that a relatively large number of genes of equivalent ef-
fect are at work. In this way, the categories of Mendelian genetics that lead to
binomial distributions for traits in the population tend toward continuous distri-
butions such as the normal curve. Thus, the statistical parameters that describe
this model are those of continuous distributions, including the rst moment, or the
mean; second moments, or variances, covariances, and correlation coecients; and
higher moments such as measures of skewness where these are appropriate. This
polygenic model was originally developed by Sir Ronald Fisher in his classic paper
The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance
(Fisher, 1918), in which he reconciled Galtonian biometrics with Mendelian genet-
ics. One interesting feature of the polygenic biometrical model is that it predicts
normal distributions for traits when very many loci are involved and their eects
are combined with a multitude of environmental inuences. Since the vast majority
of biological and behavioral traits approximate the normal distribution, it is an in-
herently plausible model for the geneticist to adopt. We might note, however, that
although the normality expected for a polygenic system is statistically convenient
as well as empirically appropriate, none of the biometrical expectations with which
we shall be concerned depend on how many or how few genes are involved. The
expectations are equally valid if there are are only one or two genes, or indeed no
genes at all.
In the simplest twoallele system (A and a) there are two parameters that dene
the measurable eects of the three possible genotypes, AA, Aa, and aa. These
parameters are d, which is twice the measured dierence between the homozygotes
AA and aa, and h, which denes the measured eect of the heterozygote Aa, insofar
as it does not fall exactly between the homozygotes. The point between the two
homozygotes is m, the mean eect of homozygous genotypes. We refer to the
parameters d and h as genotypic eects. The scaling of the three genotypes is
3.2. BREEDING EXPERIMENTS: GAMETIC CROSSES 57
' E
aa AA
m
' E
h
Aa
' E ' E
d d
Figure 3.1: The d and h increments of the gene dierence A a. Aa may lie on
either side of m and the sign of h will vary accordingly; in the case illustrated h
would be negative. (Adapted from Mather and Jinks, 1977, p. 32).
shown in Figure 3.1.
To make the simple twoallele model concrete, let us imagine that we are talking
about genes that inuence adult stature. Let us assume that the normal range of
height for males is from 4 feet 10 inches to 6 feet 8 inches; that is, about 22
inches
1
. And let us assume that each somatic chromosome has one gene of roughly
equivalent eect. Then, roughly speaking, we are thinking in terms of loci for which
the homozygotes contribute
1
2
inch (from the midpoint), depending on whether
they are AA, the increasing homozygote, or aa, the decreasing homozygotes. In
reality, although some loci may contribute greater eects than this, others will
almost certainly contribute less; thus we are talking about the kind of model in
which any particular polygene is having an eect that would be dicult to detect by
the methods of classical genetics. Similarly, while the methods of linkage analysis
may be appropriate for a number of quantitative loci, it seems unlikely that the
majority of causes of genetic variation would be detectable by these means. The
biometrical approach, being founded upon an assumption that inheritance may be
polygenic, is designed to elucidate sources of genetic variation in these types of
systems.
3.2 Breeding Experiments: Gametic Crosses
The methods of biometrical genetics are best understood through controlled breed-
ing experiments with inbred strains, in which the results are simple and intuitively
1
Note: 1 inch = 2.54cm; 1 foot = 12 inches.
58 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
Table 3.1: Punnett square for mating between two heterozygous parents.
Male Gametes
1
2
A
1
2
a
Female Gametes
1
2
A
1
4
AA
1
4
Aa
1
2
a
1
4
Aa
1
4
aa
obvious. Of course, in the present context we are dealing with continuous variation
in humans, where inbred strains do not exist and controlled breeding experiments
are impossible. However, the simple results from inbred strains of animals apply
directly, albeit in more complex form, to those of free mating organisms such as
humans. We feel an appreciation of the simple results from controlled breeding
experiments provides insight and lends credibility to the application of the models
to human beings.
Let us consider a cross between two inbred parental strains, P
1
and P
2
, with
genotypes AA and aa, respectively. Since individuals in the P
1
strain can produce
gametes with only the A allele, and P
2
individuals can produce only a gametes, all
of the ospring of such a mating will be heterozygotes, Aa, forming what Gregor
Mendel referred to as the rst lial, or F
1
generation. A cross between two F
1
individuals generates what he referred to as the second lial generation, or F
2
,
and it may be shown that this generation comprises
1
4
individuals of genotype AA,
1
4
aa, and
1
2
Aa. Mendels rst law, the law of segregation, states that parents with
genotype Aa will produce the gametes A and a in equal proportions. The pioneer
Mendelian geneticist Reginald Punnett developed a device known as the Punnett
square, which he found useful in teaching Mendelian genetics to Cambridge un-
dergraduates, that gives the proportions of genotypes that will arise when these
gametes unite at random. (Random unions of gametes occur under the condition
of random mating among individuals). The result of other matings such as P
1

F
1
, the rst backcross, B
1
, and more complex combinations may be elucidated in a
similar manner. A simple usage of the Punnett square is shown in Table 3.1 for the
mating of two heterozygous parents in a twoallele system. The gamete frequen-
cies in Table 3.1 (shown outside the box) are known as gene or allelic frequencies,
and they give rise to the genotypic frequencies by a simple product of indepen-
dent probabilities. It is this assumption of independence based on random mating
that makes the biometrical model straightforward and tractable in more complex
situations, such as random mating in populations where the gene frequencies are
unequal. It also forms a simple basis for considering the more complex eects of
non-random mating, or assortative mating, which are known to be important in
human populations.
In the simple case of equal gene frequencies as we have in an F
2
population,
3.2. BREEDING EXPERIMENTS: GAMETIC CROSSES 59
it is easily shown that random mating over successive generations changes neither
the gene nor genotype frequencies of the population. Male and female gametes
of the type A and a from an F
2
population are produced in equal proportions so
that random mating may be represented by the same Punnett square as given in
Table 3.1, which simply reproduces a population with identical structure to the
F
2
from which we started. This remarkable result is known as HardyWeinberg
equilibrium and is the cornerstone of quantitative and population genetics. From
this result, the eects of non-random mating and other forces that change pop-
ulations, such as natural selection, migration, and mutation, may be deduced.
Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium is achieved in one generation and applies whether or
not the gene frequencies are equal and whether or not there are more than two
alleles. It also holds among polygenic loci, linked or unlinked, although in these
cases joint equilibrium depends on a number of generations of random mating.
For our purposes the genotypic frequencies from the Punnett square are im-
portant because they allow us to calculate the simple rst and second moments
of the phenotypic distribution that result from genetic eects; namely, the mean
and variance of the phenotypic trait. The genotypes, frequencies, and genotypic
eects of the biometrical model in Table 3.1 are shown below, and from these we
can calculate the mean and variance.
Genotype (i) AA Aa aa
Frequency (f)
1
4
1
2
1
4
Genotypic eect (x) d h d
The mean eect of the A locus is obtained by summing the products of the fre-
quencies and genotypic eects in the following manner:

A
=

f
i
x
i
=
1
4
d +
1
2
h
1
4
d
=
1
2
h (3.1)
The variance of the genetic eects is given by the sum of the products of the
genotypic frequencies and their squared deviations from the mean
2
:

2
A
=

f
i
(x
i

A
)
2
=
1
4
(d
1
2
h)
2
+
1
2
(h
1
2
h)
2
+
1
4
(d
1
2
h)
2
=
1
4
d
2

1
4
dh +
1
16
h
2
+
1
8
h
2
+
1
4
d
2
+
1
4
dh +
1
16
h
2
2
This is an application of the method described in Section 2.2.1. It looks a bit more intim-
idating here because of (a) the multiplication by the frequency, and (b) the use of letters not
numbers. To gain condence in this method, the reader may wish to choose values for d and h
and work through an example.
60 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
=
1
2
d
2
+
1
4
h
2
(3.2)
For this single locus with equal gene frequencies,
1
2
d
2
is known as the additive
genetic variance, or V
A
, and
1
4
h
2
is known as the dominance variance, V
D
. When
more than one locus is involved, perhaps many loci as we envisage in the polygenic
model, Mendels law of independent assortment permits the simple summation of
the individual eects of separate loci in both the mean and the variance. Thus, for
(k) multiple loci,
=
1
2
k

i=1
h
i
, (3.3)
and

2
=
1
2
k

i=1
d
2
i
+
1
4
k

i=1
h
2
i
= V
A
+V
D
. (3.4)
It is the parameters V
A
and V
D
that we estimate using the structural equations in
this book.
In order to see how this biometrical model and the equations estimate V
A
and
V
D
, we need to consider the joint eect of genes in related individuals. That is, we
need to derive expectations for MZ and DZ covariances in terms of the genotypic
frequencies and the eects of d and h.
3.3 Derivation of Expected Twin Covariances
3.3.1 Equal Gene Frequencies
Twin correlations may be derived in a number of dierent ways, but the most
direct method is to list all possible twin-pair genotypes (taken as deviations from
the population mean) and the frequency with which they arise in a random-mating
population. Then, the expected covariance may be obtained by multiplying the
genotypic eects for each pair, weighting them by the frequency of occurrence,
and summing across all possible pairs. By this method the covariance among
pairs is calculated directly. The overall mean for such pairs is, of course, simply
the population mean,
1
2
h, in the case of equal gene frequencies, as shown in the
previous section. There are shorter methods for obtaining the same result, but
these are less direct and less intuitively obvious.
The covariance calculations are laid out in Table 3.2 for MZ, DZ, and Unrelated
pairs of siblings, the latter being included in order to demonstrate the expected zero
covariance for genetically unrelated individuals. The nine possible combinations of
genotypes are shown in column 1, with their genotypic eects, x
1i
and x
2i
, in
3.3. DERIVATION OF EXPECTED TWIN COVARIANCES 61
Table 3.2: Genetic covariance components for MZ, DZ, and Unrelated siblings
with equal gene frequencies at a single locus (u = v =
1
2
).
Genotype Eect Frequency
Pair x1i x2i x1i 1 x2i 2 (x1i 1)(x2i 2) MZ DZ U
AA, AA d d d
1
2
h d
1
2
h d
2
dh +
1
4
h
2 1
4
9
64
1
16
AA, Aa d h d
1
2
h
1
2
h
1
2
dh
1
4
h
2
-
3
32
1
8
AA, aa d d d
1
2
h d
1
2
h d
2
+
1
4
h
2
-
1
64
1
16
Aa, AA h d
1
2
h d
1
2
h
1
2
dh
1
4
h
2
-
3
32
1
8
Aa, Aa h h
1
2
h
1
2
h
1
4
h
2 1
2
5
16
1
4
Aa, aa h d
1
2
h d
1
2
h
1
2
dh
1
4
h
2
-
3
32
1
8
aa, AA d d d
1
2
h d
1
2
h d
2
+
1
4
h
2
-
1
64
1
16
aa, Aa d h d
1
2
h
1
2
h
1
2
dh
1
4
h
2
-
3
32
1
8
aa, aa d d d
1
2
h d
1
2
h d
2
+dh +
1
4
h
2 1
4
9
64
1
16
x
1
= x
2
=
1
2
h in all cases; genetic covariance =

i
fi(x1i 1)(x2i 2)
columns 2 and 3. From these values the mean of all pairs,
1
2
h, is subtracted in
columns 4 and 5. Column 6 shows the products of these mean deviations. The
nal three columns show the frequency with which each of the genotype pairs occurs
for the three kinds of relationship. For MZ twins, the genotypes must be identical,
so there are only three possibilities and these occur with the population frequency
of each of the possible genotypes. For unrelated pairs, the population frequencies
of the three genotypes are simply multiplied within each pair of siblings since
genotypes are paired at random. The frequencies for DZ twins, which are the same
as for ordinary siblings, are more dicult to obtain. All possible parental types
and the proportion of paired genotypes they can produce must be enumerated, and
these categories collected up across all possible parental types. These frequencies
and the method by which they are obtained may be found in standard texts (e.g.,
Crow and Kimura, 1970, pp. 136-137; Falconer, 1960, pp. 152-157; Mather and
Jinks, 1971, pp. 214-215).
The products in column 6, weighted by the frequencies for the three sibling
types, yield the degree of genetic resemblance between siblings. In the case of MZ
62 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
twins, the covariance equals
Cov(MZ) = d
2
(
1
4
+
1
4
) +dh(
1
4
+
1
4
) +
1
4
h
2
(
1
4
+
2
4
+
1
4
)
=
1
2
d
2
+
1
4
h
2
, (3.5)
which is simply expression 3.2, the total genetic variance in the population. If
we sum over loci, as we did in expression 3.4, we obtain V
A
+ V
D
, the additive
and dominance variance, as we would intuitively expect since identical twins share
all genetic variance. The calculation for DZ twins, with terms in d
2
, dh, and h
2
initially separated for convenience, and collected together at the end, is
Cov(DZ) = d
2
(
9
64

1
64

1
64
+
9
64
)
+ dh(
9
64
+
3
64
+
3
64

3
64

3
64
+
9
64
)
+
1
4
h
2
(
9
64

6
64
+
1
64

6
64
+
20
64

6
64
+
1
64

6
64
+
9
64
)
=
1
4
d
2
+
1
16
h
2
(3.6)
When summed over all loci, this expression gives
1
2
V
A
+
1
4
V
D
. The calculation for
unrelated pairs of individuals yields a zero value as expected, since, on average,
unrelated siblings have no genetic variation in common at all:
Cov(U) = d
2
(
1
16

1
16

1
16
+
1
16
)
= dh(
1
16
+
1
16
+
1
16

1
16

1
16
+
1
16
)
=
1
4
h
2
(
1
16

2
16
+
1
16

2
16
+
4
16

2
16
+
1
16

2
16
+
1
16
)
= 0 (3.7)
It is the xed coecients in front of V
A
and V
D
, 1.0 and 1.0 in the case of MZ
twins and
1
2
and
1
4
, respectively, for DZ twins that allow us to specify the Mx
model and estimate V
A
and V
D
, as will be explained in subsequent chapters. These
coecients are the correlations between additive and dominance deviations for the
specied twin types. This may be seen easily in the case where we assume that
dominance is absent. Then, MZ and DZ genetic covariances are simply V
A
and
1
2
V
A
, respectively. The variance of twin 1 and twin 2 in each case, however, is the
population variance, V
A
. For example, the DZ genetic correlation is derived as
r
DZ
=
Cov(DZ)

V
T1
V
T2
=
1
2
V
A

V
A
V
A
=
1
2
3.3. DERIVATION OF EXPECTED TWIN COVARIANCES 63
3.3.2 Unequal Gene Frequencies
The simple results for equal gene frequencies described in the previous section
were appreciated by a number of biometricians shortly after the rediscovery of
Mendels work (Castle, 1903; Pearson, 1904; Yule, 1902). However, it was not until
Fishers remarkable 1918 paper that the full generality of the biometrical model
was elucidated. Gene frequencies do not have to be equal, nor do they have to be
the same for the various polygenic loci involved in the phenotype for the simple
fractions, 1,
1
2
,
1
4
, and 0 to hold, providing we dene V
A
and V
D
appropriately.
The algebra is considerably more complicated with unequal gene frequencies and
it is necessary to dene carefully what we mean by V
A
and V
D
. However, the end
result is extremely simple, which is perhaps somewhat surprising. We give the
avor of the approach in this section, and refer the interested reader to the classic
texts in this eld for further information (Crow and Kimura, 1970; Falconer, 1990;
Kempthorne, 1960; Mather and Jinks, 1982). We note that the elaboration of this
biometrical model and its power and elegance has been largely responsible for the
tremendous strides in inexpensive plant and animal food production throughout
the world, placing these activities on a rm scientic basis.
Consider the three genotypes, AA, Aa, and aa, with genotypic frequencies P,
Q, R:
Genotypes AA Aa aa
Frequency P Q R
The proportion of alleles, or gene frequency, is given by
gene frequency (A) = P +
Q
2
= u
(a) = R +
Q
2
= v . (3.8)
These expressions derive from the simple fact that the AA genotype contributes
only A alleles and the heterozygote, Aa, contributes
1
2
A and
1
2
a alleles. A Punnett
square showing the allelic form of gametes uniting at random gives the genotypic
frequencies in terms of the gene frequencies:
Male Gametes
u A v a
Female Gametes u A u
2
AA uvAa
v a uvAa v
2
aa
which yields an alternative representation of the genotypic frequencies
Genotypes AA Aa aa
Frequency u
2
2uv v
2
64 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
That these genotypic frequencies are in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium may be shown
by using them to calculate gene frequencies in the new generation, showing them
to be the same, and then reapplying the Punnett square. Using expression 3.8,
substituting u
2
, 2uv, and v
2
, for P, Q, and R, and noting that the sum of gene
frequencies is 1 (u+v = 1.0), we can see that the new gene frequencies are the same
as the old, and that genotypic frequencies will not change in subsequent generations
u
1
= u
2
+
1
2
2uv = u
2
+uv = u(u +v) = u
v
1
= v
2
+
1
2
2uv = v
2
+uv = v(u +v) = v . (3.9)
The biometrical model is developed in terms of these equilibrium frequencies and
genotypic eects as
Genotypes AA Aa aa
Frequency u
2
2uv v
2
Genotypic eect d h d
(3.10)
The mean and variance of a population with this composition is obtained in
analogous manner to that in 3.1. The mean is
= u
2
d + 2uvh v
2
d
= (u v)d + 2uvh (3.11)
Because the mean is a reasonably complex expression, it is not convenient to sum
weighted deviations to express the variance as in 3.2, instead, we rearrange the
variance formula

2
=

f
i
(x
i
)
2
=

f
i
(x
2
i
2x
i
+
2
)
=

f
i
x
2
i
2

f
i
x
i
+
2
=

f
i
x
2
i
2
2
+
2
=

f
i
x
2
i

2
(3.12)
Applying this formula to the genotypic eects and their frequencies given in 3.10
above, we obtain

2
= u
2
d
2
+ 2uvh
2
+v
2
d
2
[(u v)d + 2uvh]
2
= u
2
d
2
+ 2uvh
2
+v
2
d
2
[(u v)
2
d
2
+ 4uvdh(u v) + 4u
2
v
2
h
2
]
= u
2
d
2
+ 2uvh
2
+v
2
d
2
[(u
2
2uv v
2
)d
2
+ 4uvdh(u v) + 4u
2
v
2
h
2
]
= 2uv[d
2
+ 2(v u)dh + (1 2uv)h
2
]
= 2uv[d
2
+ 2(v u)dh + (v u)h
2
+ 2uvh
2
]
= 2uv[d + (v u)h]
2
+ 4u
2
v
2
h
2
. (3.13)
3.3. DERIVATION OF EXPECTED TWIN COVARIANCES 65
E
T
Genotypic
Eect
Gene Dose
0 1 2
d
m
h
+d
t
aa
t
Aa
t
AA

Figure 3.2: Regression of genotypic eects on gene dosage showing additive and
dominance eects under random mating. The gure is drawn to scale for u = v =
1
2
,
d = 1, and h =
1
2
.
When the variance is arranged in this form, the rst term (2uv[d+(vu)h]
2
) denes
the additive genetic variance, V
A
, and the second term (4u
2
v
2
h
2
) the dominance
variance, V
D
. Why this particular arrangement is used to dene V
A
and V
D
rather
than some other may be seen if we introduce the notion of gene dose and the
regression of genotypic eects on this variable, which essentially is how Fisher
proceeded to develop the concepts of V
A
and V
D
.
If A is the increasing allele, then we can consider the three genotypes, AA, Aa,
aa, as containing 2, 1, and 0 doses of the A allele, respectively. The regression of
genotypic eects on these gene doses is shown in Figure 3.2. The values that enter
into the calculation of the slope of this line are
Genotype AA Aa aa
Genotypic eect (y) d h d
Frequency (f) u
2
2uv v
2
Dose (x) 2 1 0
66 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
From these values the slope of the regression line of y on x in Figure 3.2 is given
by
y,x
=
x,y
/
2
x
. In order to calculate
2
x
we need
x
, which is

x
= 2u
2
+ 2uv
= 2u(u +v)
= 2u . (3.14)
Then,
2
x
is

2
x
= 2
2
u
2
+ 1
2
2uv 2
2
u
2
= 4u
2
+ 2uv 4u
2
= 2uv
using the variance formula in 3.12. In order to calculate
x,y
we need to employ
the covariance formula

x,y
=

f
i
x
i
y
i

y
, (3.15)
where
y
and
x
are dened as in 3.11 and 3.14, respectively. Then,

xy
= 2u
2
d + 2uvh 2u[(u v)d + 2uvh]
= 2u
2
d + 2uvh 2u
2
d + 2uvd 4u
2
vh
= 2uvd +h(2uv 4u
2
v)
= 2uvd + 2uvh(1 2u)
= 2uvd + 2uvh(1 u u)
= 2uvd + 2uvh(v u)
= 2uv[d + (v u)h] . (3.16)
Therefore, the slope is

y,x
=

xy

2
x
= 2uv[d + (v u)h]/2uv
= d + (v u)h . (3.17)
Following standard procedures in regression analysis, we can partition
2
y
into the
variance due to the regression and the variance due to residual. The former is
equivalent to the variance of the expected y; that is, the variance of the hypothetical
points on the line in Figure 3.2, and the latter is the variance of the dierence
between observed y and the expected values.
The variance due to regression is

xy
= 2uv[d + (v u)h][d + (v u)h]
= 2uv[d + (v u)h]
2
= V
A
(3.18)
3.3. DERIVATION OF EXPECTED TWIN COVARIANCES 67
and we may obtain the residual variance simply by subtracting the variance due to
regression from the total variance of y. The variance of genotypic eects (
2
y
) was
given in 3.13, and when we subtract the expression obtained for the variance due
to regression 3.18, we obtain the residual variances:

2
y

x,y
= 4u
2
v
2
h
2
= V
D
. (3.19)
In this representation, genotypic eects are dened in terms of the regression
line and are known as genotypic values. They are related to d and h, the genotypic
eects we dened in Figure 3.1, but now reect the population mean and gene
frequencies of our random mating population. Dened in this way, the genotypic
value (G) is G = A + D, the additive (A) and dominance (D) deviations of the
individual.
G = A + D frequency
G
AA
= 2v[d +h(v u)] 2v
2
h u
2
G
Aa
= (v u)[d +h(v u)] + 2uvh 2uv
G
aa
= 2u[d +h(v u)] 2u
2
h v
2
In the case of u = v =
1
2
, this table becomes
G = A + D frequency
G
AA
= d
1
2
h
1
4
G
Aa
=
1
2
h
1
2
G
aa
= d
1
2
h
1
4
from which it can be seen that the weighted sum of all Gs is zero (

f
i
G
i
= 0). In
this case the additive eect is the same as the genotypic eect as originally scaled,
and the dominance eect is measured around a mean of
1
2
h. This representation of
genotypic value accurately conveys the extreme nature of unusual genotypes. Let
d = h = 1, an example of complete dominance. In that case, G
AA
= G
Aa
=
1
2
and
G
aa
= 1
1
2
on our scale. Thus, aa genotypes, which form only
1
4
of the population,
fall far below the mean of 0, while the remaining
3
4
of the population genotypes
fall only slightly above the mean of 0. Thus, the bulk of the population appears
relatively normal, whereas aa genotypes appear abnormal or unusual. When dom-
inance is absent (h = 0), Aa genotypes, which form
1
2
of the population, have a
mean of 0 and the less frequent genotypes AA and aa appear deviant. This situa-
tion is accentuated as the gene frequencies depart from
1
2
. For example, with u =
3
4
,
v =
1
4
, and h = d = 1, then AA and Aa combined form
15
16
of the population with
a genotypic value of
1
8
, just slightly above the mean of 0, whereas the aa genotype
has a value of 1
7
8
. In the limiting case of a very rare allele, AA and Aa tend to 0,
the population mean, while only aa genotypes take an extreme value. These values
intuitively correspond to our notion of a rare disorder of extreme eect, such as
untreated phenylketonuria (PKU).
68 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
Table 3.3: Genetic covariance components for MZ, DZ, and Unrelated Siblings
with unequal gene frequencies at a single locus.
Genotype Eect Frequency
Pair x
1i
x
2i
MZ DZ U
AA, AA d d u
2
u
4
+u
3
v +
1
4
u
2
v
2
u
4
AA, Aa d h u
3
v +
1
2
u
2
v
2
2u
3
v
AA, aa d d
1
4
u
2
v
2
u
2
v
2
Aa, AA h d u
3
v +
1
2
u
2
v
2
2u
3
v
Aa, Aa h h 2uv u
3
v + 3u
2
v
2
+uv
3
4u
2
v
2
Aa, aa h d
1
2
u
2
v
2
+uv
3
2uv
3
aa, AA d d
1
4
u
2
v
2
u
2
v
2
aa, Aa d h
1
2
u
2
v
2
+uv
3
2uv
3
aa, aa d d u
4 1
4
u
2
v
2
+uv
3
+v
4
v
4
The genotypic values A and D that we employ in the Mx model have precisely
the expectations given above in 3.18 and 3.19, but are summed over all polygenic
loci contributing to the trait. Thus, the biometrical model gives a precise denition
to the latent variables employed in Mx for the analysis of twin data.
3.4 Summary
Table 3.3 replicates Table 3.2 employing genotypic frequencies appropriate to ran-
dom mating and unequal gene frequencies. Using the table to calculate covariances
among sibling pairs of the three types, MZ twins, DZ twins, and unrelated siblings,
gives
Cov(MZ) = 2uv[d + (v u)h]
2
+ 4u
2
v
2
h
2
= V
A
+V
D
Cov(DZ) = uv[d + (v u)h]
2
+u
2
v
2
h
2
=
1
2
V
A
+
1
4
V
D
Cov(U) = 0 = 0
By similar calculations, the expectations for half-siblings and for parents and
their ospring may be shown to be
1
4
V
A
and
1
2
V
A
, respectively. That is, these
relationships do not reect dominance eects. The MZ and DZ resemblances are
the primary focus of this text, but all ve relationships we have just discussed may
be analyzed in the extended Mx approaches we discuss in Chapter ??.
With more extensive genetical data, we can assess the eects of epistasis, or non-
allelic interaction, since the biometrical model may be extended easily to include
such genetic eects. Another important problem we have not considered is that
of assortative mating, which one might have thought would introduce insuperable
3.4. SUMMARY 69
problems for the model. However, once we are working with genotypic values such
as A and D, the eects of assortment can be readily accommodated in the model by
means of reverse path analysis (Wright, 1968) and the PearsonAitken treatment
of selected variables (Aitken, 1934). Fulker (1988) describes this approach in the
context of Fishers (1918) model of assortment.
In this chapter, we have given a brief introduction to the biometrical model that
underlies the model tting approach employed in this book, and we have indicated
how additional genetic complexities may be accommodated in the model. However,
in addition to genetic inuences, we must consider the eects of the environment
on any phenotype. These may be easily accommodated by dening environmental
inuences that are common to sib pairs and those that are unique to the individual.
If these environmental eects are unrelated to the genotype, then the variances
due to these inuences simply add to the genetic variances we have just described.
If they are not independent of genotype, as in the case of sibling interactions
and cultural transmission, both of which are likely to occur in some behavioral
phenotypes, then the Mx model may be suitably modied to account for these
complexities, as we describe in Chapters 8 and ??.
70 CHAPTER 3. BIOMETRICAL GENETICS
Chapter 4
Matrix Algebra
4.1 Introduction
Many people regard journal articles and books that contain matrix algebra as
prohibitively complicated and ignore them or shelve them indenitely. This is a
sad state of aairs because learning matrix algebra is not dicult and can reap
enormous benets. Science in general, and genetics in particular, is becoming
increasingly quantitative. Matrix algebra provides a very economical language to
describe our data and our models; it is essential for understanding Mx and other
data analysis packages. In common with most languages, the way to make it stick
is to use it. Those unfamiliar with, or out of practice at, using matrices will benet
from doing the worked examples in the text. Readers with a strong mathematics
background may skim this chapter, or skip it entirely, using it for reference only.
We do not give an exhaustive treatment of matrix algebra and operations but limit
ourselves to the bare essentials needed for structural equation modeling. There are
many excellent texts for those wishing to extend their knowledge; we recommend
Searle (1982) and Graybill (1969).
In this chapter, we will introduce matrix notation in Section 4.2 and matrix
operations in Section 4.3. The general use of matrix algebra is illustrated in Sec-
tion 4.4 on equations and Section 4.5 on other applications.
4.2 Matrix Notation
Although matrices and certain matrix operations were used as long ago as 2000 BC
in ancient China, it is only relatively recently that a comprehensive matrix algebra
has been developed. During the 1850s, Cayley worked on general algebraic systems
(Boyer, 1985 p. 627) and developed the basis of matrix algebra as it is used today.
The concept of a matrix is a very simple one, being just a table of numbers or
71
72 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
symbols laid out in rows and columns,
e.g.,
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_
or
_
_
a
11
a
12
a
13
a
21
a
22
a
23
a
31
a
32
a
33
_
_
In most texts, the table is enclosed in brackets, either: curved, (); square, [ ]; or
curly, {}.
It is conventional to specify the conguration of the matrix in terms of Rows
Columns and these are its dimensions or order. Thus the rst matrix above is of
order 3 by 2 and the second is a 3 3 matrix.
A common occurrence of matrices in behavioral sciences is the data matrix
where the rows are subjects and the columns are measures, e.g.,
Weight Height
S
1
50 20
S
2
100 40
S
3
150 60
S
4
200 80
It is convenient to let a single letter symbolize a matrix. This is written in
UPPERCASE boldface. Thus we might say that our data matrix is A, which in
handwriting we would underline with either a straight or a wavy line. Sometimes a
matrix is written
4
A
2
to specify its dimensions. The economy of using matrices is
immediately apparent: we can represent a whole table by a single symbol, whether
it contains just one row and one column, or a billion rows and a billion columns!
There are several special terms for matrices with one row or one column or both.
When a matrix consists of a single number, it is called a scalar; when it consists of
single column (row) of numbers it is called a column (row) vector. Scalars are usu-
ally represented as lower case, non-bold letters. Vectors are normally represented
as a bold lowercase letter. Thus, the weight measurements of our four subjects are
_

_
50
100
150
200
_

_
= a
We can refer to the specic elements of matrix A as a
ij
where i indicates the row
number and j indicates the column number.
Certain special forms of matrices exist. We have already dened scalars and
row and column vectors. A matrix full of zeroes is called a null matrix and a matrix
full of ones is called a unit matrix. Matrices in which the number of rows is equal
to the number of columns are called square matrices. Among square matrices,
diagonal matrices have at least one non-zero diagonal element, with every o-
diagonal element zero. By diagonal, we mean the leading diagonal from the top
4.3. MATRIX ALGEBRA OPERATIONS 73
left element of the matrix to the bottom right element. A special form of the
diagonal matrix is the identity matrix, I, which has every diagonal element one
and every non-diagonal element zero. The identity matrix functions much like the
number one in ordinary algebra.
4.3 Matrix Algebra Operations
Matrix algebra denes a set of operations that may be performed on matrices.
These operations include addition, subtraction, multiplication, inversion (multipli-
cation by the inverse is similar to division) and transposition. We may separate
the operations into two mutually exclusive categories: unary and binary. Unary
operations are performed on a single matrix, and binary operations combine two
matrices to obtain a single matrix result. Binary operations will be described rst.
4.3.1 Binary Operations
Addition and subtraction
Matrices may be added if and only if they have the same dimension. They are then
said to be conformable for addition. Each element in the rst matrix is added to
the corresponding element in the second matrix to form the same element in the
solution.
e.g.
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_
+
_
_
8 11
9 12
10 13
_
_
=
_
_
9 15
11 17
13 19
_
_
or symbolically,
A+B = C.
One cannot add
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_
+
_
8 10
9 11
_
because they have a dierent number of rows. Subtraction works in the same way
as addition, e.g.
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_

_
_
2 5
2 5
2 5
_
_
=
_
_
1 1
0 0
1 1
_
_
which is written
AB = C.
74 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
Matrix multiplication
Matrices are conformable for multiplication if and only if the number of columns in
the rst matrix equals the number of rows in the second matrix. This means that
adjacent columns and rows must be of the same order. For example, the matrix
product
3
A
2

2
B
1
may be calculated; the result is a 3 1 matrix. In general, if
we multiply two matrices
i
A
j

j
B
k
, the result will be of order i k.
Matrix multiplication involves calculating a sum of cross products among rows
of the rst matrix and columns of the second matrix in all possible combinations.
e.g.
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_
_
1 3
2 4
_
=
_
_
1 1 + 4 2 1 3 + 4 4
2 1 + 5 2 2 3 + 5 4
3 1 + 6 2 3 3 + 6 4
_
_
=
_
_
9 19
12 26
15 33
_
_
This is written
AB = C
The only exception to the above rule is multiplication by a single number called
a scalar. Thus, for example,
2
_
_
1 4
2 5
3 6
_
_
=
_
_
2 8
4 10
6 12
_
_
by convention this is often written as
2A = C.
Although convenient and often found in the literature, we do not recommend this
style of matrix formulation, but prefer use of the kronecker product. The kronecker
product of two matrices, symbolized AB is formed by multiplying each element
of A by the matrix B. If A is a scalar, every element of the matrix B is multiplied
by the scalar.
The simplest example of matrix multiplication is to multiply a vector by itself.
If we premultiply a column vector (n 1) by its transpose
1
, the result is a scalar
called the inner product. For example, if
a

=
_
1 2 3
_
then the inner product is
a

a =
_
1 2 3
_
_
_
1
2
3
_
_
= 1
2
+ 2
2
+ 3
2
= 14
which is the sum of the squares of the elements of the vector a. This has a simple
graphical representation when a is of dimension 2 1 (see Figure 4.1).
1
Transposition is dened in Section 4.3.2 below. Essentially the rows become columns and
vice versa.
4.3. MATRIX ALGEBRA OPERATIONS 75
y
x
V
0
Figure 4.1: Graphical representation of the inner product a

a of a (2 1) vector
a, with a

= (xy). By Pythagoras theorem, the distance of the point V from the


origin O is
_
x
2
+ y
2
, which is the square root of the inner product of the vector.
4.3.2 Unary Operations
Transposition
A matrix is transposed when the rows are written as columns and the columns are
written as rows. This operation is denoted by writing A

or A
T
. For our example
data matrix on page 72,
A

=
_
50 100 150 200
20 40 60 80
_
a row vector is usually written
a

=
_
50 100 150 200
_
Clearly, (A

= A.
Determinant of a matrix
For a square matrix A we may calculate a scalar called the determinant which we
write as |A|. In the case of a 2 2 matrix, this quantity is calculated as
|A| = a
11
a
22
a
12
a
21
.
76 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
We shall be giving numerical examples of calculating the determinant when we
address matrix inversion. The determinant has an interesting geometric represen-
tation. For example, consider two standardized variables that correlate r. This
situation may be represented graphically by drawing two vectors, each of length
1.0, having the same origin and an angle a, whose cosine is r, between them (see
Figure 4.2).
0
V
1
V
2
a
r = cos(a)
Figure 4.2: Geometric representation of the determinant of a matrix. The an-
gle between the vectors is the cosine of correlation between two variables, so the
determinant is given by twice the area of the triangle OV
1
V
2
.
It can be shown (the proof involves symmetric square root decomposition of
matrices) that the area of the triangle OV
1
V
2
is .5
_
|A|. Thus as the correlation
r increases, the angle between the lines decreases, the area decreases, and the
determinant decreases. For two variables that correlate perfectly, the determinant
of the correlation (or covariance) matrix is zero. Conversely, the determinant is at
a maximum when r = 0; the angle between the vectors is 90

, and we say that


the variables are orthogonal. For larger numbers of variables, the determinant is
a function of the hypervolume in n-space; if any single pair of variables correlates
perfectly then the determinant is zero. In addition, if one of the variables is a linear
combination of the others, the determinant will be zero. For a set of variables
with given variances, the determinant is maximized when all the variables are
orthogonal, i.e., all the o-diagonal elements are zero.
Many software packages [e.g., Mx; SAS, 1985] and numerical libraries (e.g.,
4.3. MATRIX ALGEBRA OPERATIONS 77
IMSL, 1987; NAG, 1990) have algorithms for nding the determinant and inverse
of a matrix. But it is useful to know how matrices can be inverted by hand, so we
present a method for use with paper and pencil. To calculate the determinant of
larger matrices, we employ the concept of a cofactor. If we delete row i and column
j from an nn matrix, then the determinant of the remaining matrix is called the
minor of element a
ij
. The cofactor, written A
ij
is simply:
A
ij
= (1)
i+j
minor a
ij
The determinant of the matrix A may be calculated as
|A| =
n

i=1
a
ij
A
ij
where n is the order of A.
The determinant of a matrix is related to the concept of deniteness of a matrix.
In general, for a null column vector x, the quadratic form x

Ax is always zero. For


some matrices, this quadratic is zero only if x is the null vector. If x

Ax > 0 for all


non-null vectors x then we say that the matrix is positive denite. Conversely, if
x

Ax < 0 for all non-null x, we say that the matrix is negative denite. However,
if we can nd some non-null x such that x

Ax = 0 then the matrix is said to be


singular, and its determinant is zero. As long as no two variables are perfectly
correlated, and there are more subjects than measures, a covariance matrix calcu-
lated from data on random variables will be positive denite. Mx will complain
(and rightly so!) if it is given a covariance matrix that is not positive denite. The
determinant of the covariance matrix can be helpful when there are problems with
model-tting that seem to originate with the data. However, it is possible to have
a matrix with a positive determinant yet which is negative denite (consider I
with an even number of rows), so the determinant is not an adequate diagnostic.
Instead we note that all the eigenvalues of a positive denite matrix are greater
than zero. Eigenvalues and eigenvectors may be obtained from software packages,
including Mx, and the numerical libraries listed above
2
.
Trace of a matrix
The trace of a matrix is simply the sum of its diagonal elements. Thus the trace
of the matrix
_
_
1 2 3
4 5 6
7 8 9
_
_
= 1 + 5 + 9 = 15
2
Those readers wishing to know more about the uses of eigenvalues and eigenvectors may
consult Searle (1982) or any general text on matrix algebra.
78 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
Inverse of a matrix
In ordinary algebra the division operation a b is equivalent to multiplication of
the reciprocal a
1
b
. Thus one binary operation, division, has been replaced by
two operations, one binary (multiplication) and one unary (forming
1
b
). In matrix
algebra we make an equivalent substitution of operations, and we call the unary
operation inversion. We write the inverse of the matrix A as A
1
, and calculate
it so that
AA
1
= I
and
A
1
A = I ,
where I is the identity matrix. In general the inverse of a matrix is not simply
formed by nding the reciprocal of each element (this holds only for scalars and
diagonal matrices
3
), but is a more complicated operation involving the determinant.
There are many computer programs available for inverting matrices. Some
routines are general, but there are often faster routines available if the program is
given some information about the matrix, for example, whether it is symmetric,
positive denite, triangular, or diagonal. Here we describe one general method that
is useful for matrix inversion; we recommend undertaking this hand calculation at
least once for at least a 3 3 matrix in order to fully understand the concept of a
matrix inverse.
Procedure: In order to invert a matrix, the following four steps can be used:
1. Find the determinant
2. Set up the matrix of cofactors
3. Transpose the matrix of cofactors
4. Divide by the determinant
For example, the matrix
A =
_
1 2
1 5
_
can be inverted by:
1.
|A| = (1 5) (2 1) = 3
2.
A
ij
=
_
(1)
2
5 (1)
3
1
(1)
3
2 (1)
4
1
_
=
_
5 1
2 1
_
3
N.B. For a diagonal matrix one takes the reciprocal of only the diagonal elements!
4.3. MATRIX ALGEBRA OPERATIONS 79
3.
A

ij
=
_
5 2
1 1
_
4.
A
1
=
1
3
_
5 2
1 1
_
=
_
5
3

2
3

1
3
1
3
_
To verify this, we can multiply AA
1
to obtain the identity matrix:
1
3
_
5 2
1 1
__
1 2
1 5
_
=
1
3
_
3 0
0 3
_
=
_
1 0
0 1
_
The result that AA
1
= I may be used to solve the pair of simultaneous
equations:
x
1
+ 2x
2
= 8
x
1
+ 5x
2
= 17
which may be written
_
1 2
1 5
__
x
1
x
2
_
=
_
8
17
_
i.e.,
Ax = y
premultiplying both sides by the inverse of A, we have
A
1
Ax = A
1
y
x = A
1
y
=
1
3
_
5 2
1 1
__
8
17
_
=
1
3
_
6
9
_
=
_
2
3
_
which may be veried by substitution.
80 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
For a larger matrix it is more tedious to compute the inverse. Let us consider
the matrix
A =
_
_
1 1 0
1 0 1
1 1 0
_
_
1. The determinant is
|A| = +1

0 1
1 0

1 1
1 0

+ 0

1 0
1 1

= +1 + 1 + 0 = 2
2. The matrix of cofactors is:
A
ij
=
_

_
+

0 1
1 0
1 0
1 0
1 0
0 1

1 1
1 0
1 0
1 0
1 0
1 1

1 0
1 1
1 1
1 1
1 1
1 0

_
=
_
_
1 1 1
0 0 2
1 1 1
_
_
3. The transpose is
A

ij
=
_
_
1 0 1
1 0 1
1 2 1
_
_
4. Dividing by the determinant, we have
A
1
=
1
2
_
_
1 0 1
1 0 1
1 2 1
_
_
=
_
_
.5 0 .5
.5 0 .5
.5 1 .5
_
_
which may be veried by multiplication with A to obtain the identity matrix.
4.4 Equations in Matrix Algebra
Matrix algebra provides a very convenient short hand for writing sets of equations.
For example, the pair of simultaneous equations
y
1
= 2x
1
+ 3x
2
y
2
= x
1
+x
2
may be written
y = Ax
4.4. EQUATIONS IN MATRIX ALGEBRA 81
i.e.,
_
y
1
y
2
_
=
_
2 3
1 1
__
x
1
x
2
_
Also if we have the following pair of equations:
y = Ax
x = Bz,
then
y = A(Bz)
= ABz
= Cz
where C = AB. This is very convenient notation compared with direct substitu-
tion. The Mx structural equations are written in this general form, i.e.,
Real variables (y) = Matrix Hypothetical variables.
To show the simplicity of the matrix notation, consider the following equations:
y
1
= 2x
1
+ 3x
2
y
2
= x
1
+x
2
x
1
= z
1
+z
2
x
2
= z
1
z
2
Then we have
y
1
= 2(z
1
+z
2
) + 3(z
1
z
2
)
= 5z
1
z
2
y
2
= (z
1
+z
2
) + (z
1
z
2
)
= 2z
1
+ 0
Similarly, in matrix notation, we have y = ABz, where
A =
_
2 3
1 1
_
, B =
_
1 1
1 1
_
and
AB =
_
5 1
2 0
_
,
or
y
1
= 5z
1
z
2
y
2
= 2z
2
82 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
4.5 Applications of Matrix Algebra
Matrix algebra is used extensively throughout multivariate statistics (see e.g., Gray-
bill, 1969; Mardia et al., 1979; Maxwell, 1977; Searle, 1982). Here we do not propose
to discuss statistical methods, but simply to show two examples of the utility of
matrices in expressing general formulae applicable to any number of variables or
subjects.
4.5.1 Calculation of Covariance Matrix from Data Matrix
Suppose we have a data matrix A with rows corresponding to subjects and columns
corresponding to variables. We can calculate a mean for each variable and replace
the data matrix with a matrix of deviations from the mean. That is, each element
a
ij
is replaced by a
ij

j
where
j
is the mean of the j
th
variable. Let us call the
new matrix Z. The covariance matrix is then simply calculated as
1
N 1
Z

Z
where N is the number of subjects.
For example, suppose we have the following data:
X Y X X Y Y
1 2 -2 -4
2 8 -1 2
3 6 0 0
4 4 1 -2
5 10 2 4
So the matrix of deviations from the mean is
Z =
_
_
_
_
_
_
2 4
1 2
0 0
1 2
2 4
_
_
_
_
_
_
and therefore the covariance matrix of the observations is
1
N 1
Z

Z =
1
4
_
2 1 0 1 2
4 2 0 2 4
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
2 4
1 2
0 0
1 2
2 4
_
_
_
_
_
_
4.5. APPLICATIONS OF MATRIX ALGEBRA 83
=
1
4
_
10 12
12 40
_
=
_
2.5 3.0
3.0 10.0
_
=
_
S
2
x
S
xy
S
xy
S
2
y
_
The diagonal elements of this matrix are the variances of the variables, and the
o-diagonal elements are the covariances between the variables. The standard
deviation is the square root of the variance (see Chapter 2).
The correlation is
S
xy
_
S
2
x
S
2
y
=
S
xy
S
x
S
y
In general, a correlation matrix may be calculated from a covariance matrix by
pre- and post-multiplying the covariance matrix by a diagonal matrix D in which
each diagonal element d
ii
is
1
Si
, i.e., the reciprocal of the standard deviation for
that variable. Thus, in our two variable example, we have:
_
1
Sx
0
0
1
Sy
_
_
S
2
x
S
xy
S
xy
S
2
y
_
_
1
Sx
0
0
1
Sy
_
=
_
1.0 R
xy
R
xy
1.0
_
4.5.2 Transformations of Data Matrices
Matrix algebra provides a natural notation for transformations. If we premultiply
the matrix
i
B
j
by another, say
k
T
i
, then the rows of Tdescribe linear combinations
of the rows of B. The resulting matrix will therefore consist of k rows corresponding
to the linear transformations of the rows of B described by the rows of T. A very
simple example of this is premultiplication by the identity matrix, I, which, as
noted earlier, merely has 1s on the leading diagonal and zeroes everywhere else.
Thus, the transformation described by the rst row may be written as multiply
the rst row by 1 and add zero times the other rows. In the second row, we
have multiply the second row by 1 and add zero times the other rows, and so the
identity matrix transforms the matrix B into the same matrix. For a less trivial
example, let our data matrix be X, then
X

=
_
2 1 0 1 2
4 2 0 2 4
_
and let
T =
_
1 1
1 1
_
then
Y

= TX

=
_
6 1 0 1 6
2 3 0 3 2
_
.
84 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
In this case, the transformation matrix species two transformations of the data:
the rst row denes the sum of the two variates, and the second row denes the
dierence (row 1 row 2). In the above, we have applied the transformation to the
raw data, but for these linear transformations it is easy to apply the transformation
to the covariance matrix. The covariance matrix of the transformed variates is
1
N 1
Y

Y =
1
N 1
(TX

)(TX

=
1
N 1
TX

XT

= T(V
x
)T

which is a useful result, meaning that linear transformations may be applied directly
to the covariance matrix, instead of going to the trouble of transforming all the
raw data and recalculating the covariance matrix.
4.5.3 Further Operations and Applications
There exists a great variety of matrix operations and functions with much broader
scope than the limited selection given in this chapter. For example, there are two
other forms of matrix multiplication in common use, direct or kronecker products,
and dot products. Similar extensions to addition and subtraction exist, and nu-
merous matrix functions beyond determinant and trace can be dened. One place
to study further operations is Searle (1982); applications and some denitions can
be found in Neale (2003). We hope that the outline provided here will make un-
derstanding structural equation modeling of twin data much easier, and provide a
starting point for those who wish to study the subject in more detail.
4.6 Exercises
If you nd these exercises insucient practice, more may be found in almost any
text on matrix algebra. Further practice may be obtained by computing the ex-
pected covariance matrix of almost any model in this book, selecting a set of trial
values for the parameters. The exercise can be extended by computing t func-
tions for the model and parameter values selected. For the purposes of general
introduction, however, the few given in this section should suce.
4.6.1 Binary operations
Let
A =
_
3 6
2 1
_
, B =
_
1 0 3 2
0 1 1 1
_
4.6. EXERCISES 85
1. Form AB.
2. Form BA. (Careful, this might be a trick question!)
Let
C =
_
3 6
2 1
_
, D =
_
1 2
3 4
_
1. Form CD.
2. Form DC.
3. In ordinary algebra, multiplication is commutative, i.e. xy = yx. In general,
is matrix multiplication commutative?
Let
E

=
_
1 0 3
1 2 1
_
1. Form E(C+D).
2. Form EC+ED.
3. In ordinary algebra, multiplication is distributive over addition, i.e. x(y+z) =
xy+xz. In general, is matrix multiplication distributive over matrix addition?
Is matrix multiplication distributive over matrix subtraction?
4.6.2 Unary operations
1. Show for two (preferably non-trivial) matrices conformable for multiplication
that
(AB)

= B

2. If C is
_
2 6
.5 4
_
,
nd the determinant of C.
3. What is the inverse of matrix C?
4. If D is
_
.2 .3
.4 .6
_
,
nd the determinant of D.
5. What is the inverse of D?
6. If tr(A) means the trace of A, what is tr(C) + tr(D)?
86 CHAPTER 4. MATRIX ALGEBRA
Chapter 5
Path Analysis and Structural
Equations
5.1 Introduction
Path analysis was invented by the geneticist Sewall Wright (1921a, 1934, 1960,
1968), and has been widely applied to problems in genetics and the behavioral
sciences. It is a technique which allows us to represent, in diagrammatic form, linear
structural models and hence derive predictions for the variances and covariances
(the covariance structure) of our variables under that model. The books by Kenny
(1979), Li (1975), or Wright (1968) supply good introductory treatments of path
analysis, and general descriptions of structural equation modeling can be found
in Bollen (1989) and Loehlin (1987). In this chapter we provide only the basic
background necessary to understand models used in the genetic analyses presented
in this text.
A path diagram is a useful heuristic tool to graphically display causal and
correlational relations or the paths between variables. Used correctly, it is one
of several mathematically complete descriptions of a linear model, which include
less visually immediate forms such as (i) structural equations and (ii) expected
covariances derived in terms of the parameters of the model. Since all three forms
are mathematically complete, it is possible to translate from one to another for such
purposes as applying it to data, increasing understanding of the model, verifying
its identication, or presenting results.
The advantage of the path method is that it goes beyond measuring the de-
gree of association by the correlation coecient or determining the best prediction
by the regression coecient. Instead, the user makes explicit hypotheses about
relationships between the variables which are quantied by path coecients. Bet-
ter still, the models predictions may be statistically compared with the observed
87
88 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
data. Path models are in fact extremely general, subsuming a large number of
multivariate methods, including (but not limited to) multiple regression, principle
component or factor analysis, canonical correlation, discriminant analysis and mul-
tivariate analysis of variance and covariance. Therefore those that take exception
to path analysis in its broadest sense, should be aware that they dismiss a vast
array of multivariate statistical methods.
We begin by considering the conventions used to draw and read a path di-
agram, and explain the dierence between correlational paths and causal paths
(Section 5.2). In Sections 5.3 and 5.4 we briey describe assumptions of the method
and tracing rules for path diagrams, respectively. Then, to illustrate their use, we
present simple linear regression models familiar to most readers (Section 5.5). We
dene these both as path diagrams and as structural equations some individ-
uals handle path diagrams more easily, others respond better to equations! We
also apply the method to two basic representations of a simple genetic model for
covariation in twins (Section 5.6), with special reference to the identity between
the matrix specication of a model and its graphical representation. Finally we
discuss identication of models and parameters in Section 5.7.
5.2 Conventions Used in Path Analysis
A path diagram usually consists of boxes and circles, which are connected by ar-
rows. Consider the diagram in Figure 5.1 for example.
Squares or rectangles are used to enclose observed (manifest or measured) vari-
ables, and circles or ellipses surround latent (unmeasured) variables.
Single-headed arrows (paths) are used to dene causal relationships in the
model, with the variable at the tail of the arrow causing the variable at the head.
Omission of a path from one variable to another implies that there is no direct causal
inuence of the former variable on the latter. In the path diagram in (Figure 5.1) D
is determined by A and B, while E is determined by B and C. When two variables
cause each other, we say that there is a feedback-loop, or reciprocal causation
between them. Such a feedback-loop is shown between variables D and E in our
example.
Double-headed arrows are used to represent a covariance between two variables,
which might arise through a common cause or their reciprocal causation or both.
In many treatments of path analysis, double-headed arrows may be placed only be-
tween variables that do not have causal arrows pointing at them. This convention
allows us to discriminate between dependent/endogenous variables and indepen-
dent/ultimate/exogenous variables.
Dependent variables are those variables we are trying to predict (in a regres-
sion model) or whose intercorrelations we are trying to explain (in a factor model).
Dependent variables may be determined or caused by either independent variables
or other dependent variables or both. In Figure 5.1, D and E are the dependent
variables. Independent variables are the variables that explain the intercorrelations
5.2. CONVENTIONS USED IN PATH ANALYSIS 89
A B C
D E
w x y z
r
p
q
s
Figure 5.1: Path diagram for three latent (A, B and C) and two observed (D and
E) variables, illustrating correlations (p and q) and path coecients (r, s, w, x, y
and z).
between the dependent variables or, in the case of the simplest regression mod-
els, predict the dependent variables. The causes of independent variables are not
represented in the model. A, B and C are the independent variables in Figure 5.1.
Omission of a double-headed arrow reects the hypothesis that two indepen-
dent variables are uncorrelated. In Figure 5.1 the independent variables B and
C correlate, C also correlates with A, but A does not correlate with B. This
illustrates (i) that two variables which correlate with a third do not necessarily
correlate with each other, and (ii) that when two factors cause the same dependent
variable, it does not imply that they correlate. In some treatments of path analysis,
a double-headed arrow from an independent variable to itself is used to represent
its variance, but this is often omitted if the variable is standardized to unit vari-
ance. However, for completeness and mathematical correctness, we do recommend
to always include the standardized variance arrows.
By convention, lower-case letters (or numeric values, if these can be specied)
are used to represent the values of paths or double-headed arrows, in contrast to
the use of upper-case for variables. We call the values corresponding to causal
paths path coecients, and those of the double-headed arrows simply correlation
coecients (see Figure 5.1 for examples). In some applications, subscripts identify
the origin and destination of a path. The rst subscript refers to the variable being
caused, and the second subscript tells which variable is doing the causing. In most
genetic applications we assume that the variables are scaled as deviations from the
90 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
means, in which case the constant intercept terms in equations will be zero and
can be omitted from the structural equations.
Each dependent variable usually has a residual, unless it is xed to zero ex-
hypothesi. The residual variable does not correlate with any other determinants
of its dependent variable, and will usually (but not always) be uncorrelated with
other independent variables.
In summary therefore, the conventions used in path analysis:
Observed variables are enclosed in squares or rectangles. Latent variables
are enclosed in circles or ellipses. Error variables are included in the path
diagram, and may be enclosed by circles or ellipses or (occasionally) not
enclosed at all.
Upper-case letters are used to denote observed or latent variables, and lower-
case letters or numeric values represent the values of paths or two-way arrows,
respectively called path coecients and correlation coecients.
A one-way arrow between two variables indicates a postulated direct inu-
ence of one variable on another. A two-way arrow between two variables
indicates that these variables may be correlated without any assumed direct
relationship.
There is a fundamental distinction between independent variables and depen-
dent variables. Independent variables are not caused by any other variables
in the system.
Coecients may have two subscripts, the rst indicating the variable to which
arrow points, the second showing its origin.
5.3 Assumptions of Path Analysis
Sewall Wright (Wright, 1968, p. 299) described path diagrams in the following
manner:
[In path analysis] every included variable, measured or hypothetical, is
represented by arrows as either completely determined by certain others
(the dependent variables), which may in turn be represented as simi-
larly determined, or as an ultimate variable (our independent variables).
Each ultimate factor in the diagram must be connected by lines with ar-
rowheads at both ends with each of the other ultimate factors, to indicate
possible correlations through still more remote, unrepresented factors,
except in cases in which it can safely be assumed that there is no corre-
lation .... the strict validity of the method depends on the properties of
formally complete linear systems of unitary variables.
5.4. TRACING RULES OF PATH ANALYSIS 91
Some assumptions of the method, implicit or explicit in Wrights description,
are:
Linearity: All relationships between variables are linear. The assumption of
a linear model seems valid as a wide variety of non-linear functions are well
approximated by linear ones particularly within a limited range. (Sometimes
non-linearity can be removed by appropriate transformation of the data prior
to statistical analysis; but some models are inherently non-linear).
Causal closure: All direct inuences of one variable on another must be in-
cluded in the path diagram. Hence the non-existence of an arrow between two
variables means that it is assumed that these two variables are not directly
related. The formal completeness of the diagram requires the introduction of
residual variables if they are not represented as one of the ultimate variables,
unless there is reason to assume complete additivity and determination by
the specied factors.
Unitary Variables: Variables may not be composed of components that be-
have in dierent ways with dierent variables in the system, but they should
vary as a whole. For example, if we have three variables, A, B, and C, but
A is really a composite of A1 and A2, and A1 is positively correlated with B
and C, but A2 is positively correlated with B but negatively correlated with
C, we have a potential for disaster!
5.4 Tracing Rules of Path Analysis
One of the greatest advantages of path diagrams is their foundation upon standard
rules for reading paths, called tracing rules, which yield the expected variances
and covariances among the variables in the diagram.
In this section we rst describe the tracing rules for standardized variables,
following Wrights (1934, 1968) development of the method, and then outline the
rules for unstandardized variables. Although nearly all path diagrams may be
traced using rules for unstandardized variables
1
, we present path derivations for
standardized and unstandardized variables separately because the former are much
easier to trace than the latter, and because rules for unstandardized variables are
fairly simple generalizations of the principles used in tracing paths between stan-
dardized variables. An excellent resource for learning tracing rules is the program
RAMPATH (McArdle and Boker, 1990), which has a draw bridges command that
illustrates the rules for any model.
1
Multivariate path diagrams, including delta path (van Eerdewegh, 1982), copath (Cloninger,
1980), and conditional path diagrams (Carey, 1986a) employ slightly dierent rules, but are
outside the scope of this book. See Vogler (1985) for a general description.
92 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
5.4.1 Tracing Rules for Standardized Variables
The basic principle of tracing rules is described by Sewall Wright (1934) with the
following words:
Any correlation between variables in a network of sequential relations
can be analyzed into contributions from all the paths (direct or through
common factors) by which the two variables are connected, such that the
value of each contribution is the product of the coecients pertaining to
the elementary paths. If residual correlations are present (represented by
bidirectional arrows) one (but never more than one) of the coecients
thus multiplied together to give the contribution of the connecting path,
may be a correlation coecient. The others are all path coecients.
In general, the expected correlation between two variables in a path diagram of
standardized variables may be derived by tracing all connecting routes (or chains)
between the variables, while adhering to the following conditions. One may:
1. Trace backwards along an arrow and then forward, or simply forwards from
one variable to the other but never forward and then back
2. Pass through each variable only once in each chain of paths
3. Trace through at most one two-way arrow in each chain of paths
A corollary of the rst rule is that one may never pass through adjacent arrowheads.
The contribution of each chain traced between two variables to their expected
correlation is the product of its standardized coecients. The expected correlation
between two variables is the sum of the contributions of all legitimate routes be-
tween those two variables. Note that these rules assume that there are no feedback
loops; i.e., that the model is recursive.
5.4.2 Tracing Rules for Unstandardized Variables
If we are working with unstandardized variables, the tracing rules of the previous
section are insucient to derive expected correlations. However, in the absence of
paths from dependent variables to other dependent variables, expected covariances,
rather than correlations, may be derived with only slight modications to the
tracing rules (see Heise, 1975):
1. At any change of direction in a tracing route which is not a two-way ar-
row connecting dierent variables in the chain, the expected variance of the
variable at the point of change is included in the product of path coecients;
thus, any path from an dependent variable to an independent variable will in-
clude the double-headed arrow from the independent variable to itself, unless
5.5. PATH MODELS FOR LINEAR REGRESSION 93
it also includes a double-headed arrow connecting that variable to another in-
dependent variable (since this would violate the rule against passing through
adjacent arrowheads)
2. In deriving variances, the path from a dependent variable to an independent
variable and back to itself is only counted once.
Perhaps a simpler approach to unstandardized path analysis is to make certain
that all residual variances are included explicitly in the diagram with double-headed
arrows pointing to the variable itself. Then the chains between two variables are
formed simply if we
1. Trace backwards, change direction at a two-headed arrow, then trace for-
wards.
As before, the expected covariance is computed by multiplying all the coecients in
a chain and summing over all possible chains. We consider chains to be dierent if
either a) they dont have the same coecients, or b) the coecients are in a dierent
order. For a clear and thorough mathematical treatment, see the RAMPATH
manual (McArdle and Boker, 1990).
5.5 Path Models for Linear Regression
In this Section we attempt to clarify the conventions, the assumptions and the
tracing rules of path analysis by applying them to regression models. The path
diagram in Figure 5.2a represents a linear regression model, such as might be
used, for example, to predict systolic blood pressure [SBP], Y
1
from sodium intake
X
1
. The model asserts that high sodium intake is a cause, or direct eect, of high
blood pressure (i.e., sodium intake blood pressure), but that blood pressure also
is inuenced by other, unmeasured (residual), factors. The regression equation
represented in Figure 5.2a is
Y
1
= a
1
+b
11
X
1
+E
1
, (5.1)
where a is a constant intercept term, b
11
the regression or structural coecient,
and E
1
the residual error term or disturbance term, which is uncorrelated with X
1
.
This is indicated by the absence of a double-headed arrow between X
1
and E
1
or
an indirect common cause between them [Cov(X
1
,E
1
) = 0]. The double-headed
arrow from X
1
to itself represents the variance of this variable: Var(X) = s
11
; the
variance of E
1
is Var(E) = z
11
. In this example SBP is the dependent variable and
sodium intake is the independent variable.
We can extend the model by adding more independent variables or more de-
pendent variables or both. The path diagram in Figure 5.2b represents a multiple
regression model, such as might be used if we were trying to predict SBP (Y
1
)
94 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
1
X
1
Y
2
X
1
Y
1
X
3
X
2
X
1
Y
1
X X3
2
Y
2
X
1
Y
1
X
3
X
2
Y
2
X
1
Y
1
X
3
X
2
Y
11
b
11
s
12
b
22
s
21
s
32
s
1
e
11
b
13
b
11
s
31
s
33
s
12
b
22
s
21
s
32
s
22
b
1
e
11
b
11
s
31
s
33
s
23
b
2
e
12
b
22
s
21
s
32
s
22
b
1
e
11
b
21
f
11
s
31
s
33
s
23
b
2
e
12
b
22
s
21
s
32
s
22
b
1
e
11
b
21
f
12
f
11
s
31
s
33
s
23
b
2
e
1
e
Figure 5.2: Regression path models with manifest variables. Univariate regres-
sion (top left). multiple regression (top middle), multivariate regression, case 1
(top right), multivariate regression, case 2 (bottom left), multivariate regression
(reciprocal feedback, bottom right).
from sodium intake (X
1
), exercise (X
2
), and body mass index [BMI] (X
3
), allow-
ing once again for the inuence of other residual factors (E
1
) on blood pressure.
The double-headed arrows between the three independent variables indicate that
correlations are allowed between sodium intake and exercise (s
21
), sodium intake
and BMI (s
31
), and BMI and exercise (s
32
). For example, a negative covariance
between exercise and sodium intake might arise if the health-conscious exercised
more and ingested less sodium; positive covariance between sodium intake and BMI
could occur if obese individuals ate more (and therefore ingested more sodium); and
a negative covariance between BMI and exercise could exist if overweight people
were less inclined to exercise. In this case the regression equation is
Y
1
= a
1
+b
11
X
1
+b
12
X
2
+b
13
X
3
+E
1
. (5.2)
Note that the estimated values for a
1
, b
11
and E
1
will not usually be the same
5.5. PATH MODELS FOR LINEAR REGRESSION 95
as in equation 5.1 due to the inclusion of additional independent variables in the
multiple regression equation 5.2. Similarly, the only dierence between Figures 5.2a
and 5.2b is that we have multiple independent or predictor variables in Figure 5.2b.
Figure 5.2c represents a multivariate regression model, where we now have two
dependent variables (blood pressure, Y
1
, and a measure of coronary artery disease
[CAD], Y
2
), as well as the same set of independent variables (case 1). The model
postulates that there are direct inuences of sodium intake and exercise on blood
pressure, and of exercise and BMI on CAD, but no direct inuence of sodium intake
on CAD, nor of BMI on blood pressure. Because the X
2
variable, exercise, causes
both blood pressure, Y
1
, and coronary artery disease, Y
2
, it is termed a common
cause of these dependent variables. The regression equations are
Y
1
= a
1
+b
11
X
1
+b
12
X
2
+E
1
and
Y
2
= a
2
+b
22
X
2
+b
23
X
3
+E
2
. (5.3)
Here a
1
and E
1
are the intercept term and error term, respectively, and b
11
and
b
12
the regression coecients for predicting blood pressure, and a
2
, E
2
, b
22
, and
b
23
the corresponding coecients for predicting coronary artery disease. We can
rewrite equation 5.3 using matrices (see Chapter 4 on matrix algebra),
_
Y
1
Y
2
_
=
_
a
1
a
2
_
+
_
b
11
b
12
0
0 b
22
b
23
_
_
_
X
1
X
2
X
3
_
_
+
_
1 0
0 1
__
E
1
E
2
_
or, using matrix notation,
y = a +Bx +Ie,
where y, a, x, and e are column vectors and B is a matrix of regression coef-
cients and I is an identity matrix. Note that each variable in the path diagram
which has an arrow pointing to it appears exactly one time on the left side of the
matrix expression.
Figure 5.2d diers from Figure 5.2c only by the addition of a causal path (f
12
)
from blood pressure to coronary artery disease, implying the hypothesis that high
blood pressure increases CAD (case 2). The presence of this path also provides
a link between Y
2
and X
1
(Y
2
Y
1
X
1
); this type of process with multiple
intervening variables is typically called an indirect eect (of X
1
on Y
2
). Thus we
see that dependent variables can be inuenced by other dependent variables, as
well as by independent variables. Figure 5.2e adds an additional causal path from
CAD to blood pressure (f
21
), thus creating a feedback-loop (hereafter designated
as ) between CAD and blood pressure. If both f parameters are positive, the
interpretation of the model would be that high SBP increases CAD and increased
CAD in turn increases SBP. Such reciprocal causation of variables requires special
96 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
treatment and is discussed further in Chapters 8 and ??. Figure 5.2e implies the
structural equations
Y
1
= a
1
+f
12
Y
2
+b
11
X
1
+b
12
X
2
+E
1
and
Y
2
= a
2
+f
21
Y
1
+b
22
X
2
+b
23
X
3
+E
2
(5.4)
In matrix form, we may write these equations as
_
Y
1
Y
2
_
=
_
a
1
a
2
_
+
_
0 f
12
f
21
0
__
Y
1
Y
2
_
+
_
b
11
b
12
0
0 b
22
b
23
_
_
_
X
1
X
2
X
3
_
_
+
_
1 0
0 1
__
E
1
E
2
_
i.e.,
y = a +Fy +Bx +Ie
Now that some examples of regression models have been described both in the
form of path diagrams and structural equations, we can apply the tracing rules of
path analysis to derive the expected variances and covariances under the models.
The regression models presented in this chapter are all examples of unstandardized
variables. We illustrate the derivation of the expected variance or covariance be-
tween some variables by applying the tracing rules for unstandardized variables in
Figures 5.2a, 5.2b and 5.2c. As an exercise, the reader may wish to trace some
of the other paths.
In the case of Figure 5.2a, to derive the expected covariance between X
1
and
Y
1
, we need trace only the path:
(i) X
1
s11
X
1
b11
Y
1
yielding an expected covariance of (s
11
b
11
). Two paths contribute to the ex-
pected variance of Y
1
,
(i) Y
1
b11
X
1
s11
X
1
b11
Y
1
,
(ii) Y
1
1
E
1
z11
E
1
1
Y
1
;
yielding an expected variance of Y
1
of (b
2
11
s
11
+z
11
).
In the case of Figure 5.2b, to derive the expected covariance of X
1
and Y
1
, we
can trace paths:
(i) Y
1
b11
X
1
s11
X
1
,
(ii) Y
1
b12
X
2
s21
X
1
,
(iii) Y
1
b13
X
3
s31
X
1
,
5.5. PATH MODELS FOR LINEAR REGRESSION 97
to obtain an expected covariance of (b
11
s
11
+ b
12
s
21
+ b
13
s
31
). To derive the
expected variance of Y
1
, we can trace paths:
(i) Y
1
b11
X
1
s11
X
1
b11
Y
1
,
(ii) Y
1
b12
X
2
s22
X
2
b12
Y
1
,
(iii) Y
1
b13
X
3
s33
X
3
b13
Y
1
,
(iv) Y
1
1
E
1
z11
E
1
1
Y
1
,
(v) Y
1
b11
X
1
s21
X
2
b12
Y
1
,
(vi) Y
1
b12
X
2
s21
X
1
b11
Y
1
,
(vii) Y
1
b11
X
1
s31
X
3
b13
Y
1
,
(viii) Y
1
b13
X
3
s31
X
1
b11
Y
1
,
(ix) Y
1
b12
X
2
s32
X
3
b13
Y
1
,
(x) Y
1
b13
X
3
s32
X
2
b12
Y
1
,
yielding a total expected variance of (b
2
11
s
11
+ b
2
12
s
22
+ b
2
13
s
33
+ 2b
11
b
12
s
21
+
2b
11
b
13
s
31
+ 2b
12
b
13
s
32
+z
11
).
In the case of Figure 5.2c, we may derive the expected covariance of Y
1
and Y
2
as the sum of
(i) Y
1
b11
X
1
s21
X
2
b22
Y
2
,
(ii) Y
1
b11
X
1
s31
X
3
b23
Y
2
,
(iii) Y
1
b12
X
2
s22
X
2
b22
Y
2
,
(iv) Y
1
b12
X
2
s32
X
3
b23
Y
2
,
giving [b
11
(s
21
b
22
+s
31
b
23
) + b
12
(s
22
b
22
+ s
32
b
23
)] for the expected covariance.
This expectation, and the preceding ones, can be derived equally (and arguably
more easily) by simple matrix algebra. For example, the expected covariance matrix
() for Y
1
and Y
2
under the model of Figure 5.2c is given as
= BSB

+Z,
=
_
b
11
b
12
0
0 b
22
b
33
_
_
_
s
11
s
12
s
13
s
21
s
22
s
23
s
31
s
32
s
33
_
_
_
_
b
11
0
b
12
b
22
0 b
23
_
_
+
_
z
11
0
0 z
22
_
in which the elements of B are the paths from the X variables (columns) to the
Y variables (rows); the elements of S are the covariances between the independent
variables; and the elements of Z are the residual error variances.
98 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
5.6 Path Models for the Classical Twin Study
To introduce genetic models and to further illustrate the tracing rules both for
standardized variables and unstandardized variables, we examine some simple ge-
netic models of resemblance. The classical twin study, in which MZ twins and DZ
twins are reared together in the same home is one of the most powerful designs for
detecting genetic and shared environmental eects. Once we have collected such
data, they may be summarized as observed covariance matrices (Chapter 2), but
in order to test hypotheses we need to derive expected covariance matrices from
the model. We rst digress briey to review the biometrical principles outlined in
Chapter 3, in order to express the ideas in a pathanalytic context.
In contrast to the regression models considered in previous sections, many ge-
netic analyses of family data postulate independent variables (genotypes and envi-
ronments) as latent rather than manifest variables. In other words, the genotypes
and environments are not measured directly but their inuence is inferred through
their eects on the covariances of relatives. However, we can represent these mod-
els as path diagrams in just the same way as the regression models. The brief
introduction to path-analytic genetic models we give here will be treated in greater
detail in Chapter 6, and thereafter.
From quantitative genetic theory (see Chapter 3), we can write equations relat-
ing the phenotypes P
i
and P
j
of relatives i and j (e.g., systolic blood pressures of
rst and second members of a twin pair), to their underlying genotypes and envi-
ronments. We may decompose the total genetic eect on a phenotype into that due
to the additive eects of alleles at multiple loci, that due to the dominance eects
at multiple loci, and that due to the epistatic interactions between loci (Mather
and Jinks, 1982). Similarly, we may decompose the total environmental eect
into that due to environmental inuences shared by twins or sibling pairs reared
in the same family (shared, common, or between-family environmental eects),
and that due to environmental eects which make family members dier from one
another (random, specic, or within-family environmental eects). Thus, the
observed phenotypes, P
i
and P
j
, are assumed to be linear functions of the underly-
ing additive genetic variance (A
i
and A
j
), dominance variance (D
i
and D
j
), shared
environmental variance (C
i
and C
j
) and random environmental variance (E
i
and
E
j
). In quantitative genetic studies of human populations, epistatic genetic eects
are usually confounded with dominance genetic eects, and so will not be consid-
ered further here. Assuming all variables are scaled as deviations from zero, we
have
P
1
= e
1
E
1
+c
1
C
1
+a
1
A
1
+d
1
D
1
and
P
2
= e
2
E
2
+c
2
C
2
+a
2
A
2
+d
2
D
2
Particularly for pairs of twins, we do not expect the magnitude of genetic or
5.6. PATH MODELS FOR THE CLASSICAL TWIN STUDY 99
environmental eects to vary as a function of relationship
2
so we set e
1
= e
2
= e,
c
1
= c
2
= c, a
1
= a
2
= a, and d
1
= d
2
= d. In matrix form, we write
_
P
1
P
2
_
=
_
e c a d 0 0 0 0
0 0 0 0 e c a d
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
E
1
C
1
A
1
D
1
E
2
C
2
A
2
D
2
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
.
Unless two or more waves of measurement are used, or several observed variables
index the phenotype under study, residual eects are included in the random envi-
ronmental component, and are not separately specied in the model.
Figures 5.3a and 5.3b represent two alternative parameterizations of the ba-
sic genetic model, illustrated for the case of pairs of monozygotic twins (MZ) or
dizygotic twins (DZ), who may be reared together (MZT, DZT) or reared apart
(MZA, DZA). In Figure 5.3a, the traditional path coecients model, the vari-
ances of the latent variables A
1
, C
1
, E
1
, D
1
and A
2
, C
2
, E
2
, D
2
are standardized
(V
E
= V
C
= V
A
= V
D
= 1, and the path coecients e, c, a, or d quantifying the
paths from the latent variables to the observed variable, measured on both twins,
P
1
and P
2
are free parameters to be estimated. Figure 5.3b is called a variance
components model because it xes e = c = a = d = 1, and estimates separate ran-
dom environmental, shared environmental, additive genetic and dominance genetic
variances instead.
The traditional path model illustrates tracing rules for standardized variables,
and is straightforward to generalize to multivariate problems; the variance com-
ponents model illustrates an unstandardized path model. Provided all parameter
estimates are non-negative, tracing the paths in either parameterization will give
the same solution, with V
A
= a
2
, V
D
= d
2
, V
C
= c
2
and V
E
= e
2
.
5.6.1 Path Coecients Model
When applying the standardized tracing rules, it helps to draw out each tracing
route to ensure that they are neither forgotten nor traced twice. In the traditional
path model of Figure 5.3a, to derive the expected twin covariance for the case of
monozygotic twin pairs reared together, we can trace the following routes:
(i) P1
c
C1
1
C2
c
P2
(ii) P1
a
A1
1
A2
a
P2
2
i.e. we do not expect dierent heritabilities for twin 1 and twin 2; however for other rela-
tionships such as parents and children, the assumption may not be valid, as could be established
empirically if we had genetically informative data in both generations.
100 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
a)
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
1
D
2
D
1.0 1.0 1.0
a c e a c e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0
d
1.0
d
1.0 / 0.25
b)
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
1
D
2
D
A
V
C
V
E
V
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
A
V
C
V
E
V
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
D
V
1.0
D
V
1.0
1.0 / 0.25
Figure 5.3: Alternative representations of the basic genetic model: a) traditional
path coecients model, and b) variance components model.
5.6. PATH MODELS FOR THE CLASSICAL TWIN STUDY 101
(iii) P1
d
D1
1
D2
d
P2
so that the expected covariance between MZ twin pairs reared together will be
r
MZ
= c
2
+a
2
+d
2
. (5.5)
In the case of dizygotic twin pairs reared together, we can trace the following
routes:
(i) P1
c
C1
1
C2
c
P2
(ii) P1
a
A1
0.5
A2
a
P2
(iii) P1
d
D1
0.25
D2
d
P2
yielding an expected covariance between DZ twin pairs of
r
DZ
= c
2
+ 0.5a
2
+ 0.25d
2
. (5.6)
The expected variance of a variable again assuming we are working with
standardized variables is derived by tracing all possible routes from the variable
back to itself, without violating any of the tracing rules given in Section 5.4.1 above.
Thus, following paths from P1 to itself we have
(i) P1
e
E1
e
P1
(ii) P1
c
C1
c
P1
(iii) P1
a
A1
a
P1
(iv) P1
d
D1
d
P1
yielding the predicted variance for P1 or P2 in Figure 5.3a of
V
P
= e
2
+c
2
+a
2
+d
2
. (5.7)
An important assumption implicit in Figure 5.3 is that an individuals additive
genetic deviation is uncorrelated with his or her shared environmental or dominance
deviation (i.e., there are no arrows connecting the latent C and A variables of an
individual). In Chapter ?? we shall discuss how this assumption can be relaxed.
Also implicit in the coecient of 0.5 for the covariance of the additive genetic values
of DZ twins or siblings is the assumption of random mating, which we shall also
relax in Chapter ??.
5.6.2 Variance Components Model
Following the unstandardized tracing rules, the expected covariances of twin pairs
in the variance components model of Figure 5.3b, are also easily derived. For the
102 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
case of monozygotic twin pairs reared together (MZT), we can trace the following
routes:
(i) P1
1
C1
VC
C2
1
P2
(ii) P1
1
A1
VA
A2
1
P2
(iii) P1
1
D1
VD
D2
1
P2
so that the expected covariance between MZ twin pairs reared together will be
Cov(MZT) = V
C
+V
A
+ V
D
.
Only the latter two chains contribute to the expected covariance of MZ twin
pairs reared apart, as they do not share their environment. The expected covariance
of MZ twin pairs reared apart (MZA) is thus
Cov(MZA) = V
A
+V
D
.
In the case of dizygotic twin pairs reared together (DZT), we can trace the
following routes:
(i) P1
1
C1
VC
C2
1
P2
(ii) P1
1
A1
0.5VA
A2
1
P2
(iii) P1
1
D1
0.25VD
D2
1
P2
yielding an expected covariance between DZ twin reared together of
Cov(DZT) = V
C
+ 0.5V
A
+ 0.25V
D
.
Similarly, the expected covariance of DZ twin pairs reared apart (DZA) is
Cov(DZA) = 0.5V
A
+ 0.25V
D
.
In deriving expected variances of unstandardized variables, any chain from a
dependent variable to an independent variable will include the double-headed arrow
from the independent variable to itself (unless it also includes a double-headed
arrow connecting that variable to another independent variable) and each path
from an dependent variable to an independent variable and back to itself is only
counted once. In this example the expected phenotypic variance, for all groups of
relatives, is easily derived by tracing all the paths from P1 to itself:
(i) P1
1
E1
VE
E1
1
P1
(ii) P1
1
C1
VC
C1
1
P1
(iii) P1
1
A1
VA
A1
1
P1
(iv) P1
1
D1
VD
D1
1
P1
5.7. IDENTIFICATION OF MODELS AND PARAMETERS 103
yielding the predicted variance for P1 or P2 in Figure 5.3b of
V
P
= V
E
+V
C
+ V
A
+V
D
.
The equivalence between Figures 5.3a and 5.3b comes from the biometrical
principles outlined in Chapter 3: a
2
, c
2
, e
2
, and d
2
are dened as
VA
VP
,
VC
VP
,
VE
VP
, and
VD
VP
, respectively. Since correlations are calculated as covariances divided by the
product of the square roots of the variances (see Chapter 2), the twin correlations
in Figure 5.3a may be derived using the covariances and variances in Figure 5.3b.
Thus, in Figure 5.3b, the correlation for MZ pairs reared together is
r
MZT
=
V
C
+V
A
+V
D
_
(V
C
+V
A
+V
D
+V
E
)
_
(V
C
+V
A
+V
D
+V
E
)
=
V
C
+V
A
+V
D
V
P
=
V
C
V
P
+
V
A
V
P
+
V
D
V
P
= c
2
+a
2
+d
2
Similarly, the correlations for MZ twins reared apart, and for DZ twins together
and apart are
r
MZA
= a
2
+d
2
r
DZT
= c
2
+ 0.5a
2
+ 0.25d
2
r
DZA
= 0.5a
2
+ 0.25d
2
,
as in the case of Figure 5.3a.
5.7 Identication of Models and Parameters
One key issue with structural equation modeling is whether a model, or a parameter
within a model is identied. We say that the free parameters of a model are either
(i) overidentied; (ii) just identied; or (iii) underidentied. If all of the parameters
fall into the rst two classes, we say that the model as a whole is identied, but if
one or more parameters are in class (iii), we say that the model is not identied.
In this section, we briey address the identication of parameters in structural
equation models, and illustrate how data from additional types of relative may or
may not identify the parameters of a model.
When we applied the rules of standardized path analysis to the simple path
coecient model for twins (Figure 5.3a), we obtained expressions for MZ and DZ
covariances and the phenotypic variance:
Cov(MZ) = c
2
+a
2
+d
2
(5.8)
Cov(DZ) = c
2
+.5a
2
+.25d
2
(5.9)
V
P
= c
2
+a
2
+d
2
+e
2
(5.10)
104 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
These three equations have four unknown parameters c, a, d and e, and illustrate
the rst point about identication. A model is underidentied if the number of free
parameters is greater than the number of distinct statistics that it predicts. Here
there are four unknown parameters but only three distinct statistics, so the model
is underidentied.
One way of checking the identication of simple models is to represent the
expected variances and covariances as a system of equations in matrix algebra:
Ax = b
where x is the vector of parameters, b is the vector of observed statistics, and A
is the matrix of weights such that element A
ij
gives the coecient of parameter j
in equation i. Then, if the inverse of A exists, the model is identied. Thus in our
example we have:
_
_
1 1 1 0
1 .5 .25 0
1 1 1 1
_
_
_
_
_
_
c
2
a
2
d
2
e
2
_
_
_
_
=
_
_
b
1
b
2
b
3
_
_
. (5.11)
where b
1
is Cov(MZ), b
2
is Cov(DZ), and b
3
is V
P
. Now, what we would really
like to nd here is the left inverse, L, of A such that LA = I. However, it is easy
to show that left inverses may exist only when A has at least as many rows as it
does columns (for proof see, e.g., Searle, 1982, p. 147). Therefore, if we are limited
to data from a classical twin study, i.e. MZ and DZ twins reared together, it is
necessary to assume that one of the parameters a, c or d is zero to identify the
model. Let us suppose that we have reason to believe that c can be ignored, so
that the equations may be rewritten as:
_
_
1 1 0
.5 .25 0
1 1 1
_
_
_
_
a
2
d
2
e
2
_
_
=
_
_
b
1
b
2
b
3
_
_
and in this case, the inverse of A exists
3
. Another, generally superior, approach
to resolving the parameters of the model is to collect new data. For example, if we
collected data from separated MZ or DZ twins, then we could add a fourth row to
A in equation 5.11 to get (for MZ twins apart)
_
_
_
_
1 1 1 0
1 .5 .25 0
1 1 1 1
0 1 1 0
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
c
2
a
2
d
2
e
2
_
_
_
_
=
_
_
_
_
b
1
b
2
b
3
b
4
_
_
_
_
(5.12)
3
The reader may like to verify this by calculating the determinant according to the method
laid out in Section 4.3.2 or with the aid of a computer.
5.7. IDENTIFICATION OF MODELS AND PARAMETERS 105
where b
4
is Cov(MZA), and again the inverse of A exists. Now it is not neces-
sarily the case that adding another type of relative (or type of rearing environment)
will turn an underidentied model into one that is identied! Far from it, in fact,
as we show with reference to siblings reared together, and half-siblings and cousins
reared apart. Under our simple genetic model, the expected covariances of the
siblings and half-siblings are
Cov(Sibs) = c
2
+.5a
2
+.25d
2
(5.13)
Cov(Half sibs) = .25a
2
(5.14)
Cov(Cousins) = .125a
2
(5.15)
V
P
= c
2
+a
2
+d
2
+e
2
(5.16)
as could be shown by extending the methods outlined in Chapter 3. In matrix
form the equations are:
_
_
_
_
1 .5 .25 0
0 .25 0 0
0 .125 0 0
1 1 1 1
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
c
2
a
2
d
2
e
2
_
_
_
_
=
_
_
_
_
b
1
b
2
b
3
b
4
_
_
_
_
. (5.17)
where b
1
is Cov(Sibs), b
2
is Cov(Half-sibs), b
3
is Cov(Cousins), and b
4
is V
P
.
Now in this case, although we have as many types of relationship with dierent
expected covariance as there are unknown parameters in the model, we still cannot
identify all the parameters, because the matrix A is singular. The presence of
data collected from cousins does not add any information to the system, because
their expected covariance is exactly half that of the half-siblings. In general, if any
row (column) of a matrix can be expressed as a linear combination of the other
rows (columns) of a matrix, then the matrix is singular and cannot be inverted.
Note, however, that just because we cannot identify the model as a whole, it does
not mean that none of the parameters can be estimated. In this example, we can
obtain a valid estimate of additive genetic variance a
2
simply from, say, eight times
the dierence of the half-sib and cousin covariances. With this knowledge and the
observed full sibling covariance, we could estimate the combined eect of dominance
and the shared environment, but it is impossible to separate these two sources.
Throughout the above examples, we have taken advantage of their inherent
simplicity. The rst useful feature is that the parameters of the model only occur
in linear combinations, so that, e.g., terms of the form c
2
a are not present. While
true of a number of simple genetic models that we shall use in this book, it is
not the case for them all. Nevertheless, some insight may be gained by examining
the model in this way, since if we are able to identify both c and c
2
a then both
parameters may be estimated. Yet for complex systems this can prove a dicult
task, so we suggest an alternative, numerical approach. The idea is to simulate
expected covariances for certain values of the parameters, and then see whether a
program such as Mx can recover these values from a number of dierent starting
106 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
points. If we nd another set of parameter values that generates the same expected
variances and covariances, the model is not identied. We shall not go into this
procedure in detail here, but simply note that it is very similar to that described
for power calculations in Chapter 7.
5.8 Summary
In this chapter we have reviewed briey the use of path analysis to represent cer-
tain linear and genetic models. We have discussed the conventions of path analysis,
and shown how it may be used to derive the covariance matrices predicted under a
particular model. We emphasize that the systems described here have been chosen
as simple examples to illustrate elementary principles of path analysis. Although
these examples are somewhat simplistic in the sense that they do not elucidate
many of the characteristics of which structural equation models are capable, famil-
iarity with them should provide sucient skills for comprehension of other, more
advanced, genetic models described in this text and for development of ones own
path models.
However, one aspect of structural models which has not been discussed in this
chapter is that of multiple indicators. While not strictly a feature of path analysis,
multiple indicator models, those with more than one measure for each dependent
or independent variable warrant some attention because they are used often in
genetic analyses of twin data, and in analyses of behavioral data in general. Our
initial regression examples from Figure 5.2 assumed that we had only a single
measure for each variable (systolic blood pressure, sodium intake, etc), and could
ignore measurement error in these observed variables. Inclusion of multiple indica-
tors allows for explicit representation of assumptions about measurement error in
a model. In our regression example of Figures 5.2d and e, for example, we might
have several measures of our independent (x) variables, a number of measures of
sodium intake (e.g., diet diary and urinary sodium), multiple measures of exercise
(e.g., exercise diary and frequency checklist), and numerous measures of obesity
(e.g., self-report body mass index, measures of skinfold thickness). Likewise, we
might have many estimates of our dependent variables, such as repeated mea-
sures of blood pressure, and several tests for coronary artery disease. Figure 5.4
expands Figure 5.2a by illustrating the cases of (a) one variable per construct, (b)
two variables per construct, and (c) three or more observed variables per construct.
Covariance and variance expectations for multiple indicator models such as
those shown in Figure 5.4 follow without exception from the path tracing rules
outlined earlier in this chapter. However, the increase in number of variables in
these models often results in substantial increases in model complexity. One of the
important attractions of Mx is its exibility in specifying models using matrix alge-
bra. Various commands are available that allow changing the number of variables
with relative ease. It is to the Mx model specication that we now turn.
5.8. SUMMARY 107
1
X
1
Y
Y
L
X
L
2
X
2
Y
Y
L
X
L
1
X
1
Y
2
X
2
Y
Y
L
X
L
1
X
1
Y
3
X
3
Y
1
b
1.0
1.0
1
e
1
f
2
c
1
j
1
g
2
d
1
e
1.0
1
b
1
f
1.0
1
c
1
d
2
c
2
j
2
g
2
d
1
g
3
g
1
b
1
f
1
j
3
j
1
c
1
d
3
c
3
d
1
e
Figure 5.4: Regression path models with multiple indicators. Single indicator
variable model (left), two indicator variable model (middle), multiple indicator
variable model (right).
108 CHAPTER 5. PATH ANALYSIS AND STRUCTURAL EQUATIONS
Chapter 6
Univariate Analysis
6.1 Introduction
In this chapter we take the univariate model as described in Chapter 5, and apply
it to twin data. The main goals of this chapter are i) to enable the readers to apply
the models to their own data, and ii) deepen their understanding of both the scope
and the limitations of the method. In Section 6.2.1 a model of additive genetic
(A), dominance genetic (D), common environment (C), and random environment
(E) eects is presented although D and C are confounded when our data have
been obtained from pairs of twins reared together. The rst example concerns a
continuous variable: body mass index (BMI), a widely used measure of obesity,
and Section 6.2.2 describes how these data were obtained and summarized. In
Section 6.2.3 we t this model to authentic data, using Mx in a path coecients
approach, and discuss the output in Section 6.2.4. Section 6.2.5 illustrates the
univariate model tted with variance components, an alternative treatment which
may be skipped without loss of continuity. The results of initial model-tting to
BMI data appear in Section 6.2.6 and two extensions to the model, the use of means
(Section 6.2.7) and of unmatched twins (Section 6.2.8), are described before drawing
general conclusions about the BMI analyses in Section 6.2.9. In Section 6.3 the
basic model is applied to ordinal data. The second example (Section 6.3.1) describes
the collection and analysis of major depressive disorder in a sample of adult female
twins. This application serves to contrast the data summary and analysis required
for an ordinal variable against those appropriate for a continuous variable. In most
twin studies there is considerable heterogeneity of age between pairs. As shown in
Section 6.4, such heterogeneity can give rise to inated estimates of the eects of the
shared environment. We, therefore, provide a method of incorporating age into the
structural equation model to separate its eects from other shared environmental
inuences.
109
110 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
6.2 Fitting Genetic Models to Continuous Data
6.2.1 Basic Genetic Model
Derivations of the expected variances and covariances of relatives under a simple
univariate genetic model have been reviewed briey in the chapters on biometrical
genetics and path analysis (Chapters 3 and 5). In brief, from biometrical genetic
theory we can write structural equations relating the phenotypes, P, of relatives
i and j (e.g., BMI values of rst and second members of twin pairs) to their
underlying genotypes and environments which are latent variables whose inuence
we must infer. We may decompose the total genetic eect on a phenotype into
contributions of:
Additive eects of alleles at multiple loci (A),
Dominance eects at multiple loci (D),
Higher-order epistatic interactions between pairs of loci (additive additive,
additive dominance, dominance dominance: AA, AD, DD), and so on.
In practice even additive dominance and dominance dominance epistasis are
confounded with dominance in studies of humans, and the power of resolving ge-
netic dominance and additive additive epistasis is very low. We shall therefore
limit our consideration to additive and dominance genetic eects.
Similarly, we may decompose the total environmental eect into that due to
environmental inuences shared by twins or sibling pairs reared in the same family
(shared, common, or between-family environmental (C) eects), and that due to
environmental eects that make family members dier from one another (within-
family, specic, or random environmental (E) eects). Thus, the observed phe-
notypes, P
i
and P
j
, will be linear functions of the underlying additive genetic
deviations (A
i
and A
j
), dominance genetic deviations (D
i
and D
j
), shared envi-
ronmental deviations (C
i
and C
j
), and random environmental deviations (E
i
and
E
j
). Assuming all variables are scaled as deviations from zero, we have
P
1
= e
1
E
1
+c
1
C
1
+a
1
A
1
+d
1
D
1
P
2
= e
2
E
2
+c
2
C
2
+a
2
A
2
+d
2
D
2
(6.1)
In most models we do not expect the magnitude of genetic eects, or the environ-
mental eects, to dier between rst and second twins, so we set e
1
= e
2
= e, c
1
=
c
2
= c, a
1
= a
2
= a, d
1
= d
2
= d. Likewise, we do not expect the values of e, c, a,
and d to vary as a function of relationship. In other words, the eects of genotype
and environment on the phenotype are the same regardless of whether one is an
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 111
MZ twin, a DZ twin, or not a twin at all. In matrix form, we may write
_
P
1
P
2
_
=
_
a c e d 0 0 0 0
0 0 0 0 a c e d
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
A
1
C
1
E
1
D
1
A
2
C
2
E
2
D
2
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
As shown in Chapter 5, this model generates a predicted covariance matrix ()
which is equal to
_
a
2
+c
2
+ e
e
+d
2
a
2
+c
2
+d
2
a
2
+c
2
+d
2
a
2
+c
2
+e
e
+d
2
_
Unless two or more waves of measurement are used, or several variables index
the phenotype under study, residual eects (such as measurement error) will form
part of the random environmental component, and are not explicitly included in
the model.
To obtain estimates for the genetic and environmental eects in this model,
we must also specify the variances and covariances among the latent genetic and
environmental factors. Two alternative parameterizations are possible: 1) the vari-
ance components approach (Chapter 3), or 2) the path coecients model (Chap-
ter 5). The variance components approach becomes cumbersome for designs in-
volving more complex pedigree structures than pairs of relatives, but it does have
some numerical advantages.
In the variance components approach we estimate variances of the latent non-
shared and shared environmental and additive and dominance genetic variables,
V
E
, V
C
, V
A
, or V
D
, and x a = c = e = d = 1. Thus, the phenotypic variance is
simply the sum of the four variance components. In the path coecients approach
we standardize the variances of the latent variables to unity (V
E
= V
C
= V
A
=
V
D
=1) and estimate a combination of a, c, e, and d as free parameters. Thus, the
phenotypic variance is a weighted sum of standardized variables. In this volume
we will often refer to models that have particular combinations of free parameters
in the general path coecients model. Specically, we refer to an ACE model as
one having only additive genetic, common environment, and random environment
eects; an ADE model as one having additive genetic, dominance, and random
environment eects; an AE model as one having additive genetic and random
environment eects, and so on.
Figures 5.3a and 5.3b in Chapter 5 represent path diagrams for the two alter-
native parameterizations of the full basic genetic model, illustrated for the case of
pairs of monozygotic twins (MZ) or dizygotic twins (DZ), who may be reared to-
gether (MZT, DZT) or reared apart (MZA, DZA). For simplicity, we make certain
112 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
strong assumptions in this chapter, which are implied by the way we have drawn
the path diagrams in Figure 5.3:
1. No genotype-environment correlation, i.e., latent genetic variables A are un-
correlated with latent environmental variables C and E;
2. No genotype environment interaction, so that the observed phenotypes are
a linear function of the underlying genetic and environmental variables;
3. Random mating, i.e., no tendency for like to marry like, an assumption which
is implied by xing the covariance of the additive genetic deviations of DZ
twins or full sibs to 0.5V
A
;
4. Random placement of adoptees, so that the rearing environments of separated
twin pairs are uncorrelated.
We discuss ways in which these assumptions may be relaxed in subsequent chapters,
particularly Chapter 9 and Chapter ??.
6.2.2 Body Mass Index in Twins
Table 6.1 summarizes twin correlations and other summary statistics (see Chap-
ter 2) for untransformed BMI, dened as weight (in kilograms) divided by the
square of height (in meters). BMI is an index of obesity which has been widely
used in epidemiologic research (Bray, 1976; Jerey and Knauss, 1981), and has
recently been the subject of a number of genetic studies (Grilo and Pogue-Guile,
1991; Cardon and Fulker, 1992; Stunkard et al., 1986). Values between 2025
are considered to fall in the normal range for this population, with BMI < 20
taken to indicate underweight, BMI > 25 overweight, and BMI > 28 obesity (Aus-
tralian Bureau of Statistics, 1977) though standards vary across nations. The data
analyzed here come from a mailed questionnaire survey of volunteer twin pairs
from the Australian NH&MRC twin register conducted in 1981 (Martin and Jar-
dine, 1986; Jardine, 1985). Questionnaires were mailed to 5967 pairs age 18 years
and over, with completed questionnaires returned by both members of 3808 (64%)
pairs, and by one twin only from approximately 550 pairs, yielding an individual
response rate of 68%. The total sample has been subdivided into a young cohort,
aged 18-30 years, and an older cohort aged 31 and above. This allows us to exam-
ine the consistency of evidence for environmental or genetic determination of BMI
from early adulthood to maturity. For each cohort, twin pairs have been subdi-
vided into ve groups: monozygotic female pairs (MZF), monozygotic male pairs
(MZM), dizygotic female pairs (DZF), dizygotic male pairs (DZM) and opposite-
sex pairs (DZFM). We have avoided pooling MZ or like-sex DZ twin data across
sex before computing summary statistics. Pooling across sexes is inappropriate
unless it is known that there is no gender dierence in mean, variance, or twin pair
covariance, and no genotype sex interaction; it should almost always be avoided.
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 113
Table 6.1: Twin correlations and summary statistics for untransformed BMI in
twins concordant for participation in the Australian survey. BMI is calculated as
kg/m
2
. Notation used is N: sample size in pairs; r: correlation; x: mean;
2
:
variance; skew: skewness; kurt: kurtosis. Groups consist of monozygotic (MZ) or
dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs who are male (M), female (F) or opposite-sex (FM) and
young (Y) or older (O).
First Twin

Second Twin
N r x
2
skew kurt x
2
skew kurt
MZFY 534 .78 21.25 7.73 1.82 6.84 21.30 8.81 2.14 9.44
MZFO 637 .69 23.11 11.87 1.22 2.53 22.97 11.25 1.08 2.11
DZFY 328 .30 21.58 8.56 1.75 6.04 21.64 9.84 2.38 12.23
DZFO 380 .32 22.77 10.93 1.40 4.03 22.95 12.63 1.26 2.43
MZMY 251 .77 22.09 5.95 0.28 0.10 22.13 5.77 0.40 0.30
MZMO 281 .70 24.22 6.42 0.11 -0.05 24.30 7.85 0.43 0.63
DZMY 184 .32 22.71 8.16 1.00 1.71 22.61 9.63 1.55 6.24
DZMO 137 .37 24.18 8.28 0.41 0.70 24.08 7.42 0.72 0.43
DZFMY 464 .23 21.33 6.89 1.06 1.84 22.47 6.81 0.76 1.72
DZFMO 373 .24 23.07 12.63 1.23 2.24 24.65 8.52 0.88 1.49
Female twins are rst twin in opposite-sex pairs.
Among same-sex pairs, twins were assigned as rst or second members of a pair
at random. In the case of opposite-sex twin pairs, data were ordered so that the
female is always the rst member of the pair.
In both sexes and both cohorts, MZ twin correlations are substantially higher
than like-sex DZ correlations, suggesting that there may be a substantial genetic
contribution to variation in BMI. In the young cohort, the like-sex DZ correlations
are somewhat lower than one-half of the corresponding MZ correlations, but this
nding does not hold up in the older cohort. In terms of additive genetic (V
A
) and
dominance genetic (V
D
) variance components, the expected correlations between
MZ and DZ pairs are respectively r
MZ
= V
A
+ V
D
and r
DZ
= 0.5V
A
+ 0.25V
D
,
(see Chapters 3). Thus the fact that the like-sex DZ twin correlations are less than
one-half the size of the MZ correlations in the young cohort suggests a contribution
of genetic dominance, as well as additive genetic variance, to individual dierences
in BMI. Model-tting analyses (e.g., (Heath et al., 1989b) are needed to determine
whether the data:
1. Are consistent with simple additive genetic eects
2. Provide evidence for signicant dominance genetic eects
3. Enable us to reject a purely environmental model
114 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 6.2: Polynomial regression of absolute intra-pair dierence in BMI
(|BMI
T1
BMI
T2
|) on pair sum (BMI
T1
+ BMI
T2
), sum
2
, and sum
3
. The mul-
tiple regression on these three quantities is shown for raw and log-transformed
BMI scores.
Young Cohort Older Cohort
Sample Raw BMI R
2
Log BMI R
2
Raw BMI R
2
Log BMI R
2
MZF 0.11*** 0.04*** 0.16*** 0.06***
MZM 0.10*** 0.04* 0.09*** 0.03*
DZF 0.34*** 0.15*** 0.27*** 0.12***
DZM 0.15*** 0.06* 0.03 0.01
***p < .001; *p < .05.
4. Indicate signicant genotype age-cohort interaction.
Skewness and kurtosis measures in Table 6.1 indicate substantial non-normality
of the marginal distributions for raw BMI. We have also computed the polynomial
regression of absolute intra-pair dierence in BMI values on pair sum
1
separately
for each like-sex twin group. These are summarized in Table 6.2. If the joint
distribution of twin pairs for BMI is bivariate normal, these regressions should
be non-signicant. Here, however, we observe a highly signicant regression: on
average, pairs with high BMI values also exhibit larger intra-pair dierences in
BMI. This is likely to be an artefact of scale, since using a log-transformation sub-
stantially reduces the magnitude of the polynomial regression (as well as reducing
marginal measures of skewness and kurtosis).
In general, raw data or variance-covariance matrices, not correlations, should be
used for model-tting analyses with continuously distributed variables such as BMI.
The simple genetic models we t here predict no dierence in variance between
like-sex MZ and DZ twin pairs, but the presence of such variance dierences may
indicate that the assumptions of the genetic model are violated. This is an important
point which we must consider in some detail. To many researchers the opportunity
to expose an assumption as false may seem like something to be avoided if possible,
because it may mean i) more work or b) diculty publishing the results. But there
are better reasons not to use a technique that hides assumption failure. For sure,
if we tted models to correlation matrices, variance dierences would never be
observed, but to do so would be like, in physics, breaking the thermometer if a
temperature dierence did not agree with the theory. Rather, we should look
at failures of assumptions as opportunities in disguise. First, a novel eect may
have been discovered! Second, if the eect biases the parameters of interest, it
may be possible to contol for the eect statistically, and therefore obtain unbiased
1
i.e. the unsigned dierence between twin 1 and twin 2 of each pair, |BMI
twin 1
BMI
twin 2
|
with BMI
twin 1
+ BMI
twin 2
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 115
Table 6.3: Covariances of Twin Pairs for Body Mass Index: 1981 Australian
Survey. BMI = 7 ln(kg/(m
2
)).
w
Young Cohort (< 30) Older Cohort ( 30)
Covariance Matrix Means
a
Covariance Matrix Means
a
N Twin 1 Twin 2 x

N Twin 1 Twin 2 x

MZF T1 534 0.725 0.589 0.341 637 0.976 0.666 0.909


MZF T2 0.589 0.792 0.351 0.666 0.954 0.869
DZF T1 328 0.779 0.246 0.444 380 0.915 0.312 0.810
DZF T2 0.246 0.837 0.459 0.312 1.042 0.858
MZM T1 251 0.597 0.448 0.625 281 0.545 0.413 1.271
MZM T2 0.448 0.569 0.638 0.413 0.643 1.288
DZM T1 184 0.719 0.245 0.808 137 0.689 0.238 1.250
DZM T2 0.245 0.818 0.769 0.238 0.597 1.228
DZFM T1 464 0.683 0.153 0.372 373 1.036 0.196 0.892
DZFM T2 0.153 0.663 0.740 0.196 0.646 1.386
a
x

= x 21.
estimates. Third, we may have the opportunity to develop a new and useful method
of analysis.
To return to the task in hand, we present summary twin pair covariance matrices
in Table 6.3. These statistics have been computed for 7 ln (BMI), and means have
been computed as (7ln (BMI) 21), to yield summary statistics with magnitudes
of approximately unity. Rescaling the data in this way will often improve the
eciency of the optimization routines used in model-tting analyses (Gill et al.,
1981)
2
6.2.3 Building a Path Coecients Model Mx Script
With the introduction from the previous sections and chapters, we are now in a
position to set up a simple genetic model using Mx. The script in Appendix A.1
ts a simple univariate genetic model, estimating path coecients, to covariance
matrices for two like-sex twin groups: MZ twin pairs reared together, and DZ twin
pairs reared together. The script is written to ignore information on means. The
full path diagram is given in Figure 6.1 We have drawn this gure to correspond to
the variables in the model. The latent genetic and environmental variables A, C, E
and D cause the observed variables P
1
and P
2
. The script is written to t a model
with free parameters e, a, and d, and xing c to zero implying that there are
2
small observed variances (< .5) can be problematic as the predicted covariance matrix may
become non-positive denite.
116 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
1
D
2
D
1.0 1.0 1.0
a c e a c e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0
d
1.0
d
1.0 / 0.25
Figure 6.1: Univariate genetic model for data from monozygotic (MZ) or dizygotic
(DZ) twins reared together. Genetic and environmental latent variables cause the
phenotypes P
1
and P
2
. The correlation between A
1
and A
2
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5
for DZ twins. The correlation between D
1
and D
2
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.25 for DZ
twins.
no eects of shared environment on BMI. The script is extensively documented
using the comment facility in Mx: any line beginning with an exclamation mark is
interpreted as a comment. We shall consider this rst example Mx script in detail.
Please note that reading this section is not a substitute for reading in detail the
Mx manual (Neale et al., 2003), but merely a quick introduction to the essentials
of a Mx script for genetic applications.
Each new statement in a script begins on a new line. For each group, we will
have the following structure:
1. Title
2. Group type
3. Read and select any observed data, supply labels
4. Declare matrices to express the model
5. Specify parameters, (starting) values, equality constraints
6. Dene matrix formulae for the model or use matrix algebra
7. Request t functions, output and optimization options
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 117
8. End
We shall now examine the structure in greater detail, focusing on our BMI model.
We plan to test hypotheses about the contributions of genetic and environmental
factors to individual dierences in BMI using data collected from MZ and DZ twins
reared together. The Mx script therefore will have at least two groups. To simplify
the structure of the script, we have added a calculation group at the beginning.
We start the Mx script by indicating how many groups our job consists of with the
#NGroups 3 statement.
Title
Calculate genetic and environmental variance components
A new title must be given at the start of each group.
Set the Group Type
Calculation
Calculation groups allow the specication of matrix operations in an algebra
section which can greatly simplify the structure of the script. Here, we use the
calculation group to specify the free and xed parameters in the model, e, a, d,
and c, and calculate their squared quantities, to be used in the expectations of
the variances and the MZ and DZ covariances of the model (see Section ??).
Matrices Declaration
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! additive genetic path coefficient, a
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed ! common environmental path coefficient, c
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental path coefficient, e
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance genetic path coefficient, d
H Full 1 1 ! scalar, 0.5
Q Full 1 1 ! scalar, 0.25
End Matrices;
The matrices declaration section begins with a Begin Matrices; line and
ends with a End Matrices; line. Up to 26 matrices can be declared, each
starting on a new line. Matrix names are restricted to one letter, from A
to Z. The name is followed by the matrix type (see Mx manual for details
on available matrix types), the number of rows and the number of colums.
All matrix elements are xed by default. If the keyword Free appears, each
modiable element has a free parameter specied to be estimated. In this
118 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
example, four 1 1 matrices have been declared. Matrices X, Z and W rep-
resent free parameters a, e and d, respectively. The parameter c in matrix
Y is xed to zero (the word Fixed appears only for clarication). Two addi-
tional matrices, H and Q, are declared for xed scalars to be used in the model
specication.
Labels, Numbers and Parameters
Label Row X add_gen
Label Row Y comm_env
Label Row Z spec_env
Label Row W dom_gen
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .6 All
Labels can be given for the row or column (or both) of any matrix. Values can
be assigned to matrix elements using the Matrix command. If the matrix
element is modiable, the assigned value will be the starting value. The
Start command here is used to assign the same starting value to All the free
parameters in the model.
In genetic problems, we must assign starting values to parameters. In the
present case, the only parameters to be estimated are a, d and e. In choosing
starting values for twin data, a useful rule of thumb is to assume that the total
variance is divided equally between the parameters that are to be estimated.
In this case the predicted total variance is 3 .6
2
= 1.08 which is close to
the observed total variance in these data. For other data, other starting
values may be required. Good starting values can save a signicant amount
of computer time, whereas bad starting values may cause any optimizer to
fail to nd a global minimum, or to hit a maximum number of iterations
before converging.
Algebra Section
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X; ! additive genetic variance, a^2
C= Y*Y; ! common environmental variance, c^2
E= Z*Z; ! specific environmental variance, e^2
D= W*W; ! dominance genetic variance, d^2
End Algebra;
The algebra section begins with a Begin Algebra; statement and ends with
a End Algebra; statement. Each algebra operation starts on a new line and
ends with a semi-colon (it may run over several lines so a ; is essential to
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 119
mark the end of a formula). The matrix on the left side of the = sign is newly
dened as the result of the matrix operation on the right side of the = sign.
Matrices on the right have to be declared in the matrices declaration section
or dened in a previous algebra statement. In this example the quantities
a
2
, c
2
, e
2
and d
2
are calculated in matrices A, C, E and D, respectively, as the
model in the data groups is specied in these terms.
End
Every group ends with an End statement.
The structure for the data groups for MZ and DZ twins is very similar. We will
only discuss the rst data group in detail. The rst line gives the title for this
group.
Data Section
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=534
Labels bmi_t1 bmi_t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmimzf.cov
where:
1. NInputvars is number of input variables, i.e., 2n, if there are n variables
assessed for each member of a twin pair
2. NObservations is number of observations or sample size, i.e., number
of pairs used to compute the data matrix in this group.
Mx allows the user the option of reading a list of names for the observed
variables (Labels). This is very useful for clarication of the Mx output. Mx
will read a covariance matrix (CMatrix), a correlation matrix (KMatrix), or a
matrix of polychoric and polyserial correlations (PMatrix). The matrix may
be read as a lower triangle in free format (the default, the keyword Symmetric
is optional), or as a full matrix if the keyword Full is specied. It will also
read means (Means) when these are needed. Summary statistics can be read
from within the Mx script, for example,
CMatrix Symmetric
0.7247
0.5891 0.7915
Alternatively, the data matrices can be read from separate les, e.g.,
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmimzf.cov
120 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
The lines referring to the actual data, Data, Labels and CMatrix can be
saved in a dat le (e.g. ozbmimzf.dat which can then be included in the Mx
script with the following statement:
#include ozbmimzf.dat
Matrices declaration
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
The = Group 1; command includes all the declared and dened matrices
from the group 1 into the current group.
Model specication
Covariances A+C+D+E | A+C+D_
A+C+D | A+C+D+E;
The expected covariance matrix is specied using a matrix formulation with
the expected variances for twin 1 and twin 2 on the diagonal and the expected
covariance between twins, in this case for MZs, as the o-diagonal element.
The expectation for the variance, a
2
+c
2
+d
2
+e
2
, is translated into A+C+D+E;
that for the MZ covariance, a
2
+c
2
+d
2
, into A+C+D. The resulting four 1
1 matrices are concatenated using the horizontal bar | and the vertical bar
_ operators to form the 2 2 expected covariance matrix, corresponding to
the 2 2 observed covariance matrix. Note that the covariance statement
needs to end with a semicolon.
Options
Option RSiduals
Various options for statistical output and optimization can be specied. Usu-
ally, the choice of estimation procedure will be either maximum likelihood if
covariance matrices are being analyzed, or weighted least squares if matrices
of polychoric, polyserial, or product-moment correlations are being analyzed.
The RSiduals option is very useful as it results in the printing of the ob-
served, expected and residual matrices in the output.
The specication for the DZ group is very similar to that of the MZ group. Note
the dierent number of observations, the new lename containing the DZ observed
covariance matrix and the expected covariance matrix to match the expectation of
the DZ covariance, .5a
2
+ c
2
+ .25d
2
. A special form of matrix multiplication, the
Kronecker product, represented by the symbol @, is used to premultiply the matrix
A by the scalar .5 and the matrix D by the scalar .25. The specication extends
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 121
easily to the multivariate case (see Chapter 10).
After successfully running the Mx input script, by default, Mx generates an
output le which prints the
1. Users input script
2. Parameter Specications
3. Parameter Estimates
4. Measures of overall goodness-of-t.
Other useful output can be requested by additional options, including:
NDecimals=x set number of decimals in printed output (0 < x < 8, default:
x =4) useful for simulation work.
Iterations=xx set maximum number of iterations (default: 1000).
The Mx manual should be consulted for a full description of the options.
6.2.4 Interpreting the Mx Output
We can run the example of Appendix A.1 on a personal computer with Mx installed
by typing:
Mx univar.mx univar.mxo
where univar.mx is the name of the script le, and univar.mxo is the name of
the output le. We recommend mx and mxo as le extensions to make Mx input
and output distinct from input and output of other programs. This example ts
a model allowing for random environmental eects, additive genetic eects, and
dominance genetic eects, to the young female like-sex MZ and DZ covariance
matrices for log-transformed BMI. The Mx output includes:
1. Listing of the Mx script.
2. Parameter Specications for each group, indicating the parameters to be
estimated. Matrices are ordered alphabetically.
MATRIX W
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
1
1 3
MATRIX X
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
122 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
1
1 1
MATRIX Y
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
It has no free parameters specified
MATRIX Z
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
1
1 2
If no labels are specied in the input script, Mx will use consecutive numbers
for the rows and columns of each matrix. The matrix element 1 identies the
rst free parameter to be estimated (a), referring to the rst matrix element
that was declared free (Free) in the matrices declaration section. Similarly,
2 identies parameter e, and 3 identies parameter d. It is important to
check these to conrm that parameters have been correctly specied and
that the total number of estimated parameters corresponds to the number of
free parameters in the model to be tted.
3. Mx Parameter Estimates for each group, obtained at the solution. In the
case of Appendix A.1, for example, we obtain
MATRIX W
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
1
1 .5441
MATRIX X
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
1
1 .5621
MATRIX Y
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
It has no free parameters specified
MATRIX Z
This is a LOWER matrix of order 1 by 1
1
1 .4119
In other words, our maximum-likelihood parameter estimates are a = 0.56,
d = 0.54, and e = 0.41 for these data.
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 123
4. If we include the option RSiduals in a group, the observed, and expected
(tted) covariance matrix and residuals for that group are printed; Com-
parison of models should normally be based on likelihood-ratio chi-squared
tests, since signicance tests based on standard errors may be misleading for
this example (Neale et al., 1989b).
5. The goodness-of-t chi-squared is reported. In this example,
2
3
= 3.71, p =
0.29, indicating that the model gives a good t to the data. A small p value
(e.g. < .05) would indicate a lack of agreement between the data and the
predictions of the model.
6. Finally, standardized parameter estimates can be calculated for each group.
In this univariate case, we may standardize a
2
by computing a
2
/(a
2
+c
2
+e
2
+
d
2
) to give the proportion of the total variance in BMI which is accounted for
by additive genetic eects (40.4%). Similarly, we can calculate the proportion
of variance accounted for by random environmental eects (21.7%), and by
dominance genetic eects (37.9%). These analyses suggest that in young
women age 30 and under, additive and non-additive genetic factors account
for approximately 78% of the variance in BMI. These calculations can be
easily done in an algebra section.
Discussion of these results continues in Section 6.2.6.
6.2.5 Building a Variance Components Model Mx Script
We include the variance components parameterization of the basic structural equa-
tion model for completeness. It will not be developed and applied in as great detail
as the path coecients parameterization because (i) it is dicult to generalize to
more complex pedigree structures or multivariate problems, and (ii) doing so would
contribute much by weight but little by insight to this volume. Readers seeking
an easy introduction to twin models in Mx may skip this section and focus their
attention on Section 6.2.3, the path coecients parameterization.
For MZ and DZ twin pairs reared in the same family, the variance components
parameterization is presented in (Figure 5.3b). Under the simplifying assumptions
of the present chapter, the 2 2 expected covariance matrix of twin pairs () will
be, in terms of variance components,
_
V
E
+V
C
+V
A
+V
D

i
V
C
+
i
V
A
+
i
V
D

i
V
C
+
i
V
A
+
i
V
D
V
E
+V
C
+V
A
+V
D
_
where
i
is 1 for twins, full sibs or adoptees reared in the same household, but 0
for separated twins or other biological relatives reared apart;
i
is 1 for MZ twin
pairs, 0.5 for DZ pairs, full sibs, or parents and ospring, and 0 for genetically
unrelated individuals; and
i
is 1 for MZ pairs, 0.25 for DZ pairs or full sibs, and 0
for most other relationships. In terms of path coecients, we need only substitute
V
E
= e
2
, V
C
= c
2
, V
A
= h
2
, and V
D
= d
2
.
124 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
In data on twin pairs reared together the eects of shared environment and
genetic dominance are confounded. If both additive genetic eects and shared en-
vironmental eects contribute to variation in a trait, the covariance of DZ twin
pairs will be less than the MZ covariance, but greater than one-half the MZ covari-
ance. If both additive genetic eects and dominance genetic eects contribute to
variation in a trait, the covariance of DZ pairs will be less than one-half the MZ
covariance. In terms of variance components, therefore, a substantial dominance
genetic eect will lead to a negative estimate of the shared environmental vari-
ance component, if a model allowing for additive genetic and shared environmental
variance components is tted; while conversely a substantial shared environmental
eect will lead to a negative estimate of the dominance genetic variance compo-
nent, if a model allowing for additive and dominance genetic variance components
is tted (Martin et al., 1978). In terms of path coecients, however, since we are
estimating parameters c or d, c
2
or d
2
can never take negative values, and so we
will obtain an estimate of c = 0 in the presence of dominance, or d = 0 in the
presence of shared environmental eects. Additional data on separated twin pairs
(Jinks and Fulker, 1970) or on the parents or other relatives of twins (Fulker, 1982;
Heath, 1983) are needed to resolve the eects of shared environment and genetic
dominance when both are present.
Appendix A.2 illustrates an example script for tting a variance components
model to twin pair covariance matrices for two like-sex twin pair groups. We
estimate additive genetic, dominance genetic and random environmental variance
components in the matrices A, D and E. The covariance statement is the same as
for the path model example. The only change is in the calculation group, which
does not square the estimates to construct A, C, E and D.
For the young male like-sex pairs, the estimates are V
E
= 0.14, V
A
= 0.25,
and V
D
= 0.29. We can calculate standardized variance components by hand, as
V

E
= V
E
/V
P
, V

A
= V
A
/V
P
, and V

D
= V
D
/V
P
, where V
P
= V
E
+V
A
+V
D
= 0.6804
(which can be read directly from the variance in the expected covariance matrix).
In this example, random environmental eects account for 20.3% of the variance,
additive genetic eects for 36.4% of the variance, and dominance genetic eects for
43.3% of the variance of BMI in young adult males. By
2
test of goodness-of-t,
our model gives only a marginally acceptable t to the data (
2
3
= 7.28, p = 0.06).
6.2.6 Interpreting Univariate Results
In model-tting to univariate twin data, whether we use a variance components or
a path coecients model, we are essentially testing the following hypotheses:
1. No family resemblance (E model: e > 0: a = c = d = 0)
2. Family resemblance solely due to additive genetic eects (AE model: a >
0, e > 0, c = d = 0)
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 125
Table 6.4: Results of tting models to covariance matrices for Body Mass Index:
Two-group analyses, complete pairs only.
Young Older
Females Males Females Males
Model (df)
2
p
2
p
2
p
2
p
CE (4) 160.72 <.001 97.20 <.001 87.36 <.00v1 37.14 <.001
AE (4) 8.06 .09 10.88 .03 2.38 .67 5.03 .28
ACE (3) 8.06 <.05 10.88 .01 2.38 .50 5.03 .17
ADE (3) 3.71 .29 7.28 .06 1.97 .58 5.03 .17
3. Family resemblance solely due to shared environmental eects (CE model:
e > 0, c > 0, a = d = 0)
4. Family resemblance due to additive genetic plus dominance genetic eects
(ADE model: a > 0, d > 0, e > 0, c = 0)
5. Family resemblance due to additive genetic plus shared environmental eects
(ACE model: a > 0, c > 0, e > 0, d = 0).
Note that we never t a model that excludes random environmental eects, because
it predicts perfect MZ twin pair correlations, which in turn generate a singular
expected covariance matrix
3
. From inspection of the twin pair correlations for
BMI, we noted that they were most consistent with a model allowing for additive
genetic, dominance genetic, and random environmental eects. Model-tting gives
three important advantages at this stage:
1. An overall test of the goodness of t of the model
2. A test of the relative goodness of t of dierent models, as assessed by
likelihood-ratio
2
. For example, we can test whether the t is signicantly
worse if we omit genetic dominance for BMI
3. Maximum-likelihood parameter estimates under the best-tting model.
Table 6.4 tabulates goodness-of-t chi-squares obtained in four separate anal-
yses of the data from younger or older, female or male like-sex twin pairs. Let us
consider the results for young females rst. The non-genetic model (CE) yields
a chi-squared of 160.72 for 4 degrees of freedom
4
, which is highly signicant and
3
A singular matrix cannot be inverted (see Chapter 4) and, therefore, the maximum likelihood
t function cannot be computed.
4
The degrees of freedom associated with this test are calculated as the dierence between the
number of observed statistics (ns) and the number of estimated parameters (np) in the model.
Our data consist of two variances and a covariance for each of the MZ and DZ groups, giving
ns = 6 in total. The CE model has two parameters c and e, so ns np = 6 2 = 4df.
126 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
implies a very poor t to the data indeed. In stark contrast, the alternative model
of additive genes and random environment (AE) is not rejected by the data, but
ts moderately well (p = .09). Adding common environmental eects (the ACE
model) does not improve the t whatsoever, but the loss of a degree of freedom
makes the
2
signicant at the .05 level. Finally, the ADE model which substitutes
genetic dominance for common environmental eects, ts the best according to the
probability level. We can test whether the dominance variation is signicant by
using the likelihood ratio test. The dierence between the
2
of a general model
(
2
G
) and the that of a submodel (
2
S
) is itself a
2
and has df
S
df
G
degrees
of freedom (where subscripts S and G respectively refer to the submodel and gen-
eral model, in other words, the dierence in df between the general model and the
submodel). In this case, comparing the AE and the ADE model gives a likelihood
ratio
2
of 8.06 3.71 = 4.35 with 4 3 = 1df. This is signicant at the .05 level,
so we say that there is signicant deterioration in the t of the model when the
parameter d is xed to zero, or simply that the parameter d is signicant.
Now we are in a position to compare the results of model-tting in females and
males, and in young and older twins. In each case, a non-genetic (CE) model yields
a signicant chi-squared, implying a very poor t to the data: the deviations of
the observed covariance matrices from the expected covariance matrices under the
maximum-likelihood parameter estimates are highly signicant. In all groups, a
full model allowing for additive plus dominance genetic eects and random envi-
ronmental eects (ADE) gives an acceptable t to the data, although in the case
of young males the t is somewhat marginal. In the two older cohorts, however,
a model which allows for only additive genetic plus random environmental eects
(AE) does not give a signicantly worse t than the full (ADE) model, by likelihood-
ratio
2
test. In older females, for example, the likelihood-ratio chi-square is
2.38 1.97 = 0.41, with degrees of freedom equal to 4 3 = 1, i.e.,
2
1
= 0.41
with probability p = 0.52; while in older males we have
2
1
= 0.00, p = 1.00.
For the older cohorts, therefore, we nd no signicant evidence for genetic dom-
inance. In young adults, however, signicant dominance is observed in females
(as noted above) and the dominance genetic eect is almost signicant in males
(
2
1
= 3.6, p = 0.06).
Table 6.5 summarizes variance component estimates under the best-tting mod-
els. Random environment accounts for a relatively modest proportion of the total
variation in BMI, but appears to be having a larger eect in older than in younger
individuals (30-31% versus 20-22%). Although the estimate of the narrow heritabil-
ity (i.e., proportion of the total variance accounted for by additive genetic factors)
is higher in the older cohort (69-70% vs 36-40%), the broad heritability (additive
plus non-additive genetic variance) is higher in the young twins (78-80%).
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 127
Table 6.5: Standardized parameter estimates under best-tting model. Two-
group analyses, complete pairs only.
Parameter Estimates
a
2
c
2
e
2
d
2
Young females 0.40 0 0.22 0.38
Older females 0.69 0 0.31 0
Young males 0.36 0 0.20 0.44
Older males 0.70 0 0.30 0
6.2.7 Testing the Equality of Means
Applications of structural equation modeling to twin and other family data typi-
cally tend to ignore means. That is, observed measures are treated as deviations
from the phenotypic mean (and are thus termed deviation phenotypes)
5
, and like-
wise genetic and environmental latent variables are expressed as deviations from
their means, which usually are xed at 0. Most simple genetic models predict the
same mean for dierent groups of relatives, so, for example, MZ twins, DZ twins,
males from opposite-sex twin pairs, and males from like-sex twin pairs should have
(within sampling error) equal means. Where signicant mean dierences are found,
they may indicate sampling problems with respect to the variable under study or
other violations of the assumptions of the basic genetic model. Testing for mean
dierences also may be important in follow-up studies, where we are concerned
about the bias introduced by sample attrition, but we can compare mean scores
at baseline for those relatives who remain in a study with those who drop out.
Fortunately, Mx facilitates tests for mean dierences between groups.
For Mx to t a model to means and covariances, both observed means and a
model for them must be supplied. Appendix A.3 contains a Mx script for tting a
univariate genetic model which also estimates the means of rst and second twins
from MZ and DZ pairs. The rst change we make is to feed Mx the observed means
in our sample, which we do with the Means command:
Means 0.9087 0.8685
Second, we declare a matrix for the means, e.g. M Full 1 2 in the matrices decla-
ration section. Third, we can equate parameters for the rst and second twins by
using a Specify statement such as
Specify M 101 101
5
Except where explicitly noted, all models presented in this text treat observed variables as
deviation phenotypes.
128 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 6.6: Results of tting models to twin pair covariance matrices and twin
means for Body Mass Index: Two-group analyses, complete pairs only.
Young Older
Females Males Females Males
df
2
p
2
p df
2
p
2
p
Model I 6 7.84 .25 12.81 .05 7 5.74 .57 5.69 .58
Model II 5 3.93 .56 7.72 .17 6 4.75 .58 5.36 .50
Model III 3 3.71 .29 7.28 .06 4 2.38 .67 5.03 .17
Genetic Model ADE ADE AE AE
where 101 is a parameter number that has not been used elsewhere in the script.
By using the same number for the two means, they are constrained to be equal.
Fourth, we include a model for the means:
Means M;
In the DZ group we also supply the observed means, and adjust the model for the
means. We can then either (i) equate the mean for MZ twins to that for DZ twins
by using the same matrix M, copied from the MZ group or equated to that of the
MZ group as follows:
M Full 1 2 = M2
where M2 refers to matrix M in group 2; to t a no heterogeneity model (Model I); or
(ii) equate DZ twin 1 and DZ twin 2 means but allow them to dier from the MZ
means by declaring a new matrix (possibly called M too; matrices are specic to the
group in which they are dened, unless they are equated to a matrix or copied from
a previous group) to t a zygosity dependent means model (MZ = DZ, Model II);
or (iii) estimate four means, i.e., rst and second twins in each of the MZ and DZ
groups; to t the heterogeneity model (Model III). This third option gives a perfect
t to the data with regard to mean structure, so that the only contribution to the
t function comes from the covariance structure. Hence the four means model gives
the same goodness-of-t
2
as in the analyses ignoring means.
Table 6.6 reports the results of tting models incorporating means
to the like-sex twin pair data on BMI. In each analysis, we have considered only
the best-tting genetic model identied in the analyses ignoring means. Again we
subtract the
2
of a more general model from the
2
of a more restricted model to
get a likelihood ratio test of the dierence in t between the two. For the two older
cohorts we nd no evidence for mean dierences either between zygosity groups or
between rst and second twins. That is, the model that assumes no heterogeneity
of means (model 1) does not give a signicantly worse t than either (i) estimating
separate MZ and DZ means (model 2), or (ii) estimating 4 means. For older females,
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 129
likelihood-ratio chi-squares are
2
1
= 0.99, p = 0.32 and
2
3
= 3.36, p = 0.34;
and for older males,
2
1
= 0.36, p = 0.55 and
2
3
= 0.43, p = 0.33. Maximum-
likelihood estimates of mean log BMI in the older cohort are, respectively, 21.87
and 22.26 for females and males; estimates of genetic and environmental parameters
are unchanged from those obtained in the analyses ignoring means. In the younger
cohorts, however, we do nd signicant mean dierences between zygosity groups,
both in females (
2
1
= 3.91, p < 0.05) and in males (
2
1
= 5.09, p < 0.02). In both
sexes, mean log BMI values are lower in MZ pairs (21.35 for females, 21.63 for
males) than for DZ pairs (21.45 for females, 21.79 for males). As these data are
not age-corrected, it is possible that BMI values are still changing in this age-group,
and that the zygosity dierence reects a slight mean dierence in age. We shall
return to this question in Section 6.2.9.
6.2.8 Incorporating Data from Singleton Twins
In most twin studies, there are many twin pairs in which only one twin agrees
to cooperate. We call these pairs discordant-participant as opposed to concordant-
participant pairs, in which data are collected from both members of the pair. Sadly,
data from discordant-participant pairs are often just thrown away. This is unfortu-
nate not only because of the wasted eort on the part of the twins, researchers, and
data entry personnel, but also because they provide valuable information about the
representativeness of the sample for the variable under study. If sampling is satis-
factory, then we would expect to nd the same mean and variance in concordant-
participant pairs as in discordant-participant pairs. Thus, the presence of mean
dierences or variance dierences between these groups is an indication that bi-
ased sampling may have occurred with respect to the variable under investigation.
To take a concrete example, suppose that overweight twins are less likely to re-
spond to a mailed questionnaire survey. Given the strong twin pair resemblance
for BMI demonstrated in previous sections, we might expect to nd that individu-
als from discordant-participant pairs are on average heavier than individuals from
concordant-participant pairs. Such sampling biases will have dierential eects on
the covariances of MZ and DZ twin pairs, and thus may lead to biased estimates
of genetic and environmental parameters (Lykken et al., 1987; Neale et al., 1989b).
Table 6.7 reports means and variances for transformed BMI from
individuals from discordant-participant pairs in the 1981 Australian survey. Zy-
gosity assignment for MZ twins must be regarded as somewhat tentative, since most
algorithms for zygosity diagnosis based on questionnaire data require reports from
both members of a twin pair to conrm monozygosity (e.g., Eaves et al., 1989b).
In most groups, comparing Table 6.7 to Table 6.3, we observe both higher means
and higher variances in the discordant-participant pairs. It is clearly important to
test whether these dierences are statistically signicant.
To t a model simultaneously to the means, variances, and covariances of
concordant-participant pairs and the means and variances of discordant-participant
130 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 6.7: Means and variances for BMI of twins whose cotwin did not cooperate
in the 1981 Australian survey.
Young Cohort Older Cohort
Group N x


2
N x


2
MZF 33 0.179 1.064 44 0.685 1.146
DZF 55 0.584 0.898 62 1.017 1.736
MZM 24 1.327 1.248 36 1.359 1.104
DZM 47 1.271 1.531 48 1.038 1.672
DZOS F 65 0.655 1.439 81 0.976 1.269
DZOS M 28 0.872 0.975 27 1.715 1.002
pairs, requires that we analyze data where there are dierent numbers of observed
variables per group, which is easily done in Mx.
Appendix A.4 presents a Mx script for testing for dierences in mean or vari-
ance. We constrain the means of the responding twin in groups four (MZ discordant-
participant) and ve (DZ discordant-participant) to equal those of twins from the
concordant-participant pairs. Our test for signicant dierences in means between
the concordant-participant and discordant-participant groups is the improvement
in goodness-of-t obtained when we allow these latter, discordant-participant pairs,
to take their own mean value.
Table 6.8 summarizes the results of model-tting. Model I is the no het-
erogeneity model of means and variances between concordant-participant versus
discordant-participant twins. Model II allows for heterogeneity of variances, Model
III for heterogeneity of means. Finally, Model IV tests both dierences in means
and variances. For these analyses, we considered only the best-tting genetic model
based on the results of the analyses ignoring means, and allowed for zygosity dif-
ferences in means only if these were found to be signicant in the analyses of the
previous section. In the younger cohorts, young female pairs are the only group in
which we nd no dierence between concordant-participant pairs and discordant-
participant pairs. In the two older cohorts a model allowing for heterogeneity of
means (Model III) gives a substantially better t than one that assumes no hetero-
geneity of means or variances (Model I: older females:
2
2
= 12.86, p < 0.001; older
males:
2
2
= 30.87, p < 0.001). Specifying heterogeneity of variances in addition
to heterogeneity of means does not produce a further improvement in t (older
females:
2
2
= 2.02, p = 0.36; older males:
2
2
= 1.99, p = 0.37). Such a result is
not atypical because the power to detect dierences in mean is much greater than
that to detect a dierence in variance.
When considering these results, we must bear in mind several possibilities.
Numbers of twins from the discordant-participant groups are small, and estimates
of mean and variance in these groups will be particularly vulnerable to outlier-
6.2. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO CONTINUOUS DATA 131
Table 6.8: Results of tting models to twin pair covariance matrices and twin
means for Body Mass Index: Two like-sex twin groups, plus data from twins from
incomplete pairs. Models test for heterogeneity of means or variances between
twins from pairs concordant vs discordant for cooperation in 1981 survey.
Young Older
Females Males Females Males
df
2
p
2
p df
2
p
2
p
Model I 11 8.16 .70 54.97 .001

13 20.62 .08 48.55 .001

Model II 9 6.03 .74 29.22 .001

11 17.84 .09 44.58 .001

Model III 9 5.70 .77 22.76 .01 11 7.76 .74 7.68 .74
Model IV 7 3.93 .79 7.72 .36 9 5.74 .77 5.69 .77
Genetic Model ADE ADE AE AE
Means Model MZ = DZ MZ = DZ MZ = DZ MZ = DZ

p < .001
eects; that is, to ination by one or two individuals of very high BMI. Further
outlier analyses (e.g., Bollen, 1989) would be needed to determine whether this is an
explanation of the variance dierence. In the young males, it is also possible that
age dierences between concordant-participant pairs and discordant-participant
pairs could generate the observed mean dierences.
6.2.9 Conclusions: Genetic Analyses of BMI Data
The analyses of Australian BMI data which we have presented indicate a signicant
and substantial contribution of genetic factors to variation in BMI, consistent with
other twin studies referred to at the beginning of Section 6.2.2. In the young cohort
like-sex pairs, we nd signicant evidence for genetic dominance (or other genetic
non-additivity), in addition to additive genetic eects, but in the older cohort
non-additive genetic eects are non-signicant. Further analyses are needed to
determine whether genetic and environmental parameters are signicantly dierent
across cohorts, or indeed between males and females (see Chapter 9).
We have discovered unexpected mean dierences between zygosity groups (in
the young cohort), and between twins whose cotwin refused to participate in the
1981 survey, and twins from concordant-participant pairs. It is possible that these
dierences reect only outlier eects caused by a handful of observations. In this
case, if we recode BMI as an ordinal variable, we might expect to nd no signicant
dierences in the proportions of twins falling into each category
6
. Alternatively,
6
Excessive contributions to the
2
by a small number of outliers could also be detected by
tting models directly to the raw data using Mx. Though a more powerful method of assessing
132 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
it is possible that there is an overall shift in the distribution of BMI, in which
case we must be concerned about the undersampling of obese individuals. If the
latter nding were conrmed, further work would be needed to explore the degree
to which genetic and environmental parameters might be biased (cf. Lykken et al.,
1987; Neale et al., 1989a; Neale and Eaves, 1992).
6.3 Fitting Genetic Models to Binary Data
It is very important to realize that binary or ordinal data do not preclude model-
tting. A large number of applications, from item analysis (e.g., Neale, et al, 1986;
Kendler et al., 1987) to psychiatric or physical illness (e.g., Kendler et al, 1992b,c)
do not have measures on a quantitative scale but are limited to discontinuous forms
of assessment. In Chapter 2 we discussed how ordinal data from twins could be
summarized as contingency tables from which polychoric correlations and their
asymptotic variances could be computed. Fitting models to this type of summary
statistic or directly to the contingency table data themselves involves a number of
additional considerations, which we illustrate here with data on major depressive
disorder. Although details of the sample and measures used have been provided
in several published articles (Kendler et al., 1991a,b; 1992a), we briey reiterate
the methods to emphasize some of the practical issues involved with an interview
study of twins.
6.3.1 Major Depressive Disorder in Twins
Data for this example come from a study of genetic and environmental risk factors
for common psychiatric disorders in Caucasian female same-sex twin pairs sampled
from the Virginia Twin Registry. The Virginia Twin Registry is a population-based
register formed from a systematic review of all birth certicates in the Common-
wealth of Virginia. Twins were eligible to participate in the study if they were born
between 1934 and 1971 and if both members of the pair had previously responded
to a mailed questionnaire, to which the individual response rate was approximately
64%. The cooperation rate was almost certainly higher than this, as an unknown
number of twins did not receive their questionnaire due to faulty addresses, im-
proper forwarding of mail, and so on. Of the total 1176 eligible pairs, neither
twin was interviewed in 46, one twin was interviewed and the other refused in 97,
and both twins were interviewed in 1033 pairs. Of the completed interviews, 89.3%
were completed face to face, nearly all in the twins home, and 10.7% (mostly twins
living outside Virginia) were interviewed by telephone. The mean age (SD) of
the sample at interview was 30.1 (7.6) and ranged from 17 to 55.
Zygosity determination was based on a combination of review of responses to
questions about physical similarity and frequency of confusion as children which
the impact of outliers, it is beyond the scope of this volume.
6.3. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO BINARY DATA 133
Table 6.9: Contingency tables of twin pair diagnosis of lifetime Major Depressive
Disorder in Virginia adult female twins.
MZ DZ
Twin 1 Normal Depressed Normal Depressed
Twin 2 Normal 329 83 201 94
Depressed 95 83 82 63
alone have proved capable of determining zygosity with over 95% accuracy (Eaves
et al., 1989b) and, in over 80% of cases, photographs of both twins. From this
information, twins were classied as either: denitely MZ, denitely DZ, probably
MZ, probably DZ, or uncertain. For 118 of the 186 pairs in the nal three cat-
egories, blood was taken and eight highly informative DNA polymorphisms were
used to resolve zygosity. If all probes are identical then there is a .9997 prob-
ability that the pair is MZ (Spence et al., 1988). Final zygosity determination,
using blood samples where available, yielded 590 MZ pairs, 440 DZ pairs and 3
pairs classied as uncertain. The DNA methods validated the questionnaire- and
photograph-based probable diagnoses in 84 out of 104 pairs; all 26 of 26 pairs in
the denite categories were conrmed as having an accurate diagnosis. The error
rate in zygosity assignment is probably well under 2%.
Lifetime psychiatric illness was diagnosed using an adapted version of the Struc-
tured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Diagnosis (Spitzer et al., 1987) an instru-
ment with demonstrable reliability in the diagnosis of depression (Riskind et al.,
1987). Interviewers were initially trained for 80 hours and received bimonthly re-
view sessions during the course of the study. Each member of a twin pair was
invariably interviewed by a dierent interviewer. DSM-III-R criteria were applied
by a blind review of the interview by K.S. Kendler, an experienced psychiatric diag-
nostician. Diagnosis of depression was not given when the symptoms were judged to
be the result of uncomplicated bereavement, medical illness, or medication. Inter-
rater reliability was assessed in 53 jointly conducted interviews. Chance corrected
agreement (kappa) was .96, though this is likely to be a substantial overestimate
of the value that would be obtained from independent assessments
7
.
Contingency tables of MZ and DZ twin pair diagnoses are shown in Table 6.9.
PRELIS estimates of the correlation in liability to depression are .435 for MZ
and .186 for DZ pairs. Details of using PRELIS to derive these statistics and
associated estimates of their asymptotic variances are given in Section 2.3. The
PMatrix command is used to read in the tetrachoric correlation matrix, and the
7
Such independent assessments would risk retest eects if they were close together in time.
Conversely, assessments separated by a long interval would risk actual phenotypic change from
one occasion to the next. For a methodological review of this area, see Helzer (1977)
134 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 6.10: Major depressive disorder in Virginia adult female twins. Parameter
estimates and goodness-of-t statistics for models and submodels including additive
genetic (A), common environment (C), random environment (E), and dominance
genetic (D) eects.
Parameter Estimates Fit statistics
Model a c e d
2
df p
E 1.00 56.40 2 .00
CE 0.58 0.81 6.40 1 .01
AE 0.65 0.76 .15 1 .70
ACE 0.65 0.76 .15 0
ADE 0.56 0.75 0.36 .00 0
ACov command reads the asymptotic weight matrices. In both cases we use the
File= keyword in order to read these data from les. Therefore our univariate Mx
input script is unchanged from that shown in Appendix A.1 on page 229, except
for the title and the dat le used.
Major depressive disorder in adult female MZ twins
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=590
#Include mzdepsum.dat
where the dat le reads
PMatrix File=MZdep.cov
ACov File=MZdep.asy
in the MZ group, with the same commands for the DZ group except for the number
of observations (NObs=440) and a global replacement of DZ for MZ. For clarity, the
comments at the beginning also should be changed.
Results of tting the ACE and ADE models and submodels are summarized in
Table 6.10. First, note that the degrees of freedom for tting to correlation matrices
are fewer than when tting to covariance matrices. Although we provide Mx with
two correlation matrices, each consisting of 1s on the diagonal and a correlation on
the o-diagonal, the 1s on the diagonal cannot be considered unique. In fact, only
one of them conveys information which eectively scales the covariance. There
is no information in the remaining three 1s on the diagonals of the MZ and DZ
correlation matrices, but Mx does not make this distinction. Therefore, we must
adjust the degrees of freedom by adding the option Option DFreedom=-3. Another
way of looking at this is that the diagonal 1s convey no information whatsoever,
but that we use one parameter to estimate the diagonal elements (e; it appears
only in the expected variances, not the expected covariances). Thus, there are 4
6.3. FITTING GENETIC MODELS TO BINARY DATA 135
imaginary variances and 1 parameter to estimate them giving 3 statistics too
many.
Second, the substantive interpretation of the results is that the model with just
random environment fails, indicating signicant familial aggregation for diagnoses
of major depressive disorder. The environmental explanation of familial covariance
also fails (
2
1
= 6.40) but a model of additive genetic and random environment
eects ts well (
2
1
= .15). There is no possible room for signicant improvement
with the addition of any other parameter, since there are only .15
2
units left.
Nevertheless, we tted both ACE and ADE models and found that dominance
genetic eects could account for the remaining variability whereas shared environ-
mental eects could not. This nding is in agreement with the observation that the
MZ correlation is slightly greater than twice the DZ correlation. The heritability
of liability to Major Depressive Disorder is moderate but signicant at 42%, with
the remaining variability associated with random environmental sources including
error of measurement. These results are not compatible with the view that shared
family experiences such as parental rearing, social class, or parental loss are key
factors in the etiology of major depression. More modest eects of these factors
may be detected by including them in multivariate model tting (Kendler et al.,
1992a; Neale et al., 1992).
Of course, every study has its limitations, and here the primary limitations are
that: (i) the results only apply to females; (ii) the twin population is not likely to be
perfectly representative of the general population, as it lacks twins who moved out
of or into the state, or failed to respond to initial questionnaire surveys; (iii) a small
number of the twins diagnosed as having major depression may have had bipolar
disorder (manic depression), which may be etiologically distinct; (iv) the reliance
on retrospective reporting of lifetime mental illness may be subject to bias by either
currently well or currently ill subjects or both; (v) MZ twins may be treated more
similarly as children than DZ twins; and (vi) not all twins were past the age at risk
of rst onset of major depression. Consideration of the rst ve of these factors
is given in Kendler et al. (1992c). Of particular note is that a test of limitation
(v), the equal environments assumption, was performed by logistic regression of
absolute pair dierence of diagnosis (scored 0 for normal and 1 for aected) on a
quasi-continuous measure of similarity of childhood treatment. Although MZ twins
were on average treated more similarly than DZ twins, this regression was found
to be non-signicant. General methods to handle the eects of zygosity dierences
in environmental treatment form part of the class of data-specic models to be
discussed in Section ??. Overall there was no marked regression of age on liability
to disease in these data, indicating that correction for the contribution of age to
the common environment is not necessary (see the next section). Variable age
at onset has been considered by Neale et al. (1989) but a full treatment of this
problem is beyond the scope of this volume. Such methods incorporate not only
censoring of the risk period, but also the genetic architecture of factors involved
in age at onset and their relationship to factors relevant in the etiology of liability
136 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 6.11: Conservatism in Australian females: standardized parameter esti-
mates for additive genotype (A), common environment (C), random environment
(E) and dominance genotype (D).
Parameter Estimates Fit statistics
Model a c e d
2
df p
E 1.000 823.76 5 .000
CE 0.804 0.595 19.41 4 .001
AE 0.836 0.549 56.87 4 .000
ACE 0.464 0.687 0.559 3.07 3 .380
ADE 0.836 0.549 0.000 56.87 3 .000
to disease. Note, however, that this problem, like the problem of measured shared
environmental eects, may also be considered as part of the class of data-specic
models.
6.4 Model for Age-Correction of Twin Data
We now turn to a slightly more elaborate example of univariate analysis, using data
from the Australian twin sample that were used in the BMI example earlier, but
in this case data on social attitudes. Factor analysis of the item responses revealed
a major dimension with low scores indicating radical attitudes and high scores
indicating attitudes commonly labelled as conservative. Our a priori expectation
is that variation in this dimension will be largely shaped by social environment
and that genetic factors will be of little or no importance. This expectation is
based on the dierences between the MZ and DZ correlations; r
MZ
= 0.68 and
r
DZ
= 0.59, indicating little, if any, genetic inuence on social attitudes. We also
might expect that conservatism scores are aected by age. We can use the Mx
script in Appendix A.5 to examine the age eects, reading in the age of each twin
pair and the conservatism scores for twin 1 (Cons_t1) and twin 2 (Cons_t2). Since
in this specication we have 3 indicator variables, we adjust NInput_vars=3. If we
initially ignore age, as an exploratory analysis, we can select only the conservatism
scores for analysis, using the Select command (note that the list of variables
selected must end with a semicolon ;).
The script ts the ACE model. The results of this model are presented in the
fourth line of the standardized results of Table 6.11, which shows that the squares
of parameters estimated from the model sum to one, because these correspond to
the proportions of variance associated with each source (A, C, and E).
The signicance of common environmental contributions to variance in conser-
vatism may be tested by dropping c (AE model) but this leads to a worsening of
6.4. MODEL FOR AGE-CORRECTION OF TWIN DATA 137

2
by 53.8 for 1 d.f., conrming its importance. Similarly, the poor t of the CE
model conrms that genetic factors also contribute to individual dierences (sig-
nicance of a is 19.41 3.07 = 16.34 for 1 df, which is highly signicant). The e
model, which hypothesizes that there is no family resemblance for conservatism, is
overwhelmingly rejected, illustrating of the great power of this data set to discrimi-
nate between competing hypotheses. For interest, we also present the results of the
ADE model. Since we have already noted that the DZ correlation is appreciably
greater than half the MZ correlation, it is clear that this model is inappropriate.
Symmetric with the results of tting an ACE model to the BMI data (where 2r
DZ
was still less than r
MZ
, indicating dominance), we now nd that the estimate of
d gets stuck on its lower bound of zero. The BMI and conservatism examples
illustrate in a practical way the perfect reciprocal dependence of c and d in the
classical twin design of which only one may be estimated. The issue of the recip-
rocal confounding of shared environment and genetic non-additivity (dominance
or epistasis) in the classical twin design has been discussed in detail in papers by
Martin et al., (1978), Grayson (1989), and Hewitt (1989).
It is clear from the results above that there are major inuences of the shared
environment on conservatism. One aspect of the environment that is shared with
perfect correlation by cotwins is their age. If a variable is strongly related to age
and if a twin sample is drawn from a broad age range, as opposed to a cohort
sample covering a narrow range of birth years, then dierences between twin pairs
in age will contribute to estimated common environmental variance. This is the
case for the twins in the Australian sample, who range from 18 to 88 years old.
It is clearly of interest to try to separate this variance due to age dierences from
genuine cultural dierences contributing to the estimate of c.
Fortunately, structural equation modeling, which is based on linear regression,
provides a very easy way of allowing for the eects of age regression while simulta-
neously estimating the genetic and environmental eects (Neale and Martin, 1989).
Figure 6.2 illustrates the method with a path diagram, in which the regression of
Cons
t1
and Cons
t2
on Age is s (for senescence), and this is specied in the script
excerpt below.
We now work with the full 3 3 covariance matrices (so the Select statement
is dropped from the previous job). We estimate simultaneously the contributions
of additive genetic, shared and unique environmental factors on conservatism, the
variance of age V*V, and the contribution of age to conservatism S*V.
Group 2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=3 NOberservations=941
Labels age cons_t1 cons_t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozconmzf.cov
Matrices= Group 1
Covariances V*V | V*S | V*S _
S*V | A+C+E+G | A+C+G _
S*V | A+C+G | A+C+E+G;
138 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
Age
Age
L
1.0 1.0 1.0
a c e a c e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0
v
s
s
Figure 6.2: Path model for additive genetic (A), shared environment (C) and
specic environment (E) eects on phenotypes P
1
and P
2
. The correlation between
A
1
and A
2
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins. The eects of age are modelled as a
standardized latent variable, L
A
ge, which is the sole cause of variance in observed
Age.
The matrix algebra here is more complex than usual, and for univariate analysis
it would be easier to draw the diagram with the MxGui. However, the algebraic
approach has the advantage that it is much easier to generalize to the multivariate
case.
Results of tting the ACE model with age correction are in the rst row of
Table 6.12. Standardized results are presented, from which we see that the stan-
dardized regression of conservatism on age (constrained equal in twins 1 and 2)
is 0.422. In the unstandardized solution, the rst loading on the age factor is the
standard deviation of the sample for age, in this case 13.2 years. The latter is
an estimated parameter, making ve free parameters in total. In each group we
have k(k+1)/2 statistics, where k is the number of observed variables, so there are
2 (k(k+1)/2-5 =7 degrees of freedom. Dropping either c or a still causes signif-
icant worsening of the t, and it also is very clear that one cannot omit the age
regression itself (nal ACE model;
2
8
= 370.17, p = .000).
It is interesting to compare the results of the ACE model in Table 6.11 with
those of the ACES model in Table 6.12. We see that the estimates of e and a are
identical in the two tables, accounting for 0.559
2
= 31% and 0.464
2
= 22% of the
total variance, respectively. However, in the rst table the estimate of c = 0.687,
accounting for 47% of the variance. In the analysis with age however, c = 0.534
6.4. MODEL FOR AGE-CORRECTION OF TWIN DATA 139
Table 6.12: Age correction of Conservatism in Australian females: standardized
parameter estimates for models of additive genetic (A), common environment (C),
random environment (E), and senescence or age (S).
Parameter Estimates Fit statistics
Model a c e s
2
df p
ACES 0.474 0.534 0.558 0.422 7.41 7 .388
AES 0.720 0.547 0.426 31.56 8 .000
CES 0.685 0.595 0.421 25.49 8 .001
ACE 0.464 0.687 0.559 370.17 8 .000
and accounts for 29% of variance, and age accounts for 0.422
2
= 18%. Thus, we
have partitioned our original estimate of 47% due to shared environment into 18%
due to age regression and the remaining 29% due to genuine cultural dierences.
If we choose, we may recalculate the proportions of variance due to a, c, and e, as
if we were estimating them from a sample of uniform age assuming of course
that the causes of variation do not vary with age (see Chapter 9). Thus, genetic
variance now accounts for 22/(100 18) = 27% and shared environment variance
is estimated to be 29/82 = 35%.
Our analysis suggests that cultural dierences are indeed important in deter-
mining individual dierences in social attitudes. However, before accepting this
result too readily, we should reect that estimates of shared environment may not
only be inated by age regression, but also by the eects of assortative mating
the tendency of like to marry like. Since there is known to be considerable assorta-
tive mating for conservatism (spouse correlations are typically greater than 0.6), it
is possible that a substantial part of our estimate of c
2
may arise from this source
(Martin et al., 1986). This issue will be discussed in greater detail in Chapter ??.
Age is a somewhat unusual variable since it is perfectly correlated in both
MZ and DZ twins (so long as we measure the members of a pair at the same
time). There are relatively few variables that can be handled in the same way,
partly because we have assumed a strong model that age causes variability in the
observed phenotype. Thus, for example, it would be inappropriate to model length
of time spent living together as a cause of cancer, even though cohabitation may
lead to greater similarity between twins. In this case a more suitable model would
be one in which the shared environment components are more highly correlated
the longer the twins have been living together. Such a model would predict greater
twin similarity, but would not predict correlation between cohabitation and cancer.
Some further discussion of this type of model is given in Section ?? in the context of
data-specic models. One group of variables that may be treated in a similar way
to the present treatment of age consists of maternal gestation factors. Vlietinck et
140 CHAPTER 6. UNIVARIATE ANALYSIS
al. (1989) tted a model in which both gestational age and maternal age predicted
birthweight in twins.
Finally we note that at a technical level, age and similar putative causal agents
might most appropriately be treated as x-variables in a multiple regression model.
Thus the observed covariance of the x-variables is incorporated directly into the
expected matrix, so that the analysis of the remaining y-variables is conditional
on the covariance of the x-variables. This type of approach is free of distributional
assumptions for the x-variables, and is analogous to the analysis of covariance.
However, when we t a model that estimates a single parameter for the variance
of age in each group, the estimated and observed variances are generally equal, so
the same results are obtained.
Chapter 7
Power and Sample Size
7.1 Introduction
In this chapter we discuss the power of the twin study to detect variance compo-
nents in behavioral characters. Our discussion is not in any way intended to be
an exhaustive description of the power of the twin study under all possible com-
binations of causal factors and model parameters. Such a description is in large
part available for the continuous case (Martin et al., 1978) and the ordinal case
(Neale et al., 1994), and there is an extensive comparison of the power of various
designs to detect cultural transmission (Heath et al., 1985). As we move out of
the framework of the univariate classical twin study to consider multivariate hy-
potheses and data from additional classes of relatives, a comprehensive treatment
rapidly becomes unmanageably large. Fortunately, it seems rather unnecessary
because the prospective researcher usually has certain specic aims of a study in
mind, and often has a reasonable idea about the values of some of the parameters
in the model. This information can be used to prune the prodigious tree of possible
scenarios to manageable proportions. All that is required is an understanding of
the factors contributing to power and the principles involved, which we aim to pro-
vide in Section 7.2 and Section 7.3 respectively. We illustrate these methods with a
limited range of examples for continuous (Section 7.4) and categorical (Section 7.5)
twin data.
7.2 Factors Contributing to Power
One of the greatest advantages of the model-tting approach is that it allows us to
conduct tests of signicance of alternative hypotheses. We can ask, for example,
whether a given data set really supports our assertion that shared environmental
eects contribute to variation in one trait or another (i.e., is c
2
> 0?).
141
142 CHAPTER 7. POWER AND SAMPLE SIZE
Our ability to show that a specic eect is important obviously depends on a
number of factors. These include:
1. The eect under consideration, for example, a
2
or c
2
;
2. The actual size of the eect in the population being studied larger values
are detected more easily than small values;
3. The probability level adopted as the conventional criterion for rejection of
the null-hypothesis that the eect is zero rejection at higher signicance
levels will be less likely to occur for a given size of eect;
4. The actual size of the sample chosen for study larger samples can detect
smaller eects;
5. The actual composition of the sample with respect to the relative frequencies
of the dierent biological and social relationships selected for study;
6. The level of measurement used categorical, ordinal, or continuous.
All of these considerations lead us to the important question of power. If we are
trying to get a sense of what we are likely to be able to infer from our own data set,
or if we are considering a new study, we must ask either What inferences can we
hope to be able to make with our data set? or What kind of data set and sample
sizes is it likely we will need to answer a particular set of questions? In the next
section we show how to answer these questions in relation to simple hypotheses
with twin studies and suggest briey how these issues may be explored for more
complex designs and hypotheses.
7.3 Steps in Power Analysis
The basic approach to power analysis is to imagine that we are doing an identical
study many times. For example, we pretend that we are trying to estimate a, c, and
e for a given population by taking samples of a given number of MZ and DZ twins.
Each sample would give somewhat dierent estimates of the parameters, depending
on how many twins we study, and how big a, c, and e are in the study population.
Suppose we did a very large number of studies and tabulated all the estimates of
the shared environmental component, c
2
. In some of the studies, even though there
was some shared environment in the population, we would nd estimates of c
2
that
were not signicant. In these cases we would commit type II errors. That is, we
would not nd a signicant eect of the shared environment even though the value
of c
2
in the population was truly greater than zero. Assuming we were using a
2
test for 1 df to test the signicance of the shared environment, and we had decided
to use the conventional 5% signicance level, the probability of Type II error would
be the expected proportion of samples in which we mistakenly decided in favor of
7.3. STEPS IN POWER ANALYSIS 143
the null hypothesis that c
2
= 0. These cases would be those in which the observed
value of
2
was less than 3.84, the 5% critical value for 1 df. The other samples in
which
2
was greater than 3.84 are those in which we would decide, correctly, that
there was a signicant shared environmental eect in the population. The expected
proportion of samples in which we decide correctly against the null hypothesis is
the power of the test.
Designing a genetic study boils down to deciding on the numbers and types of
relationships needed to achieve a given power for the test of potentially important
genetic and environmental factors. There is no general solution to the problem of
power. The answers will depend on the specic values we contemplate for all the
factors listed above. Before doing any power study, therefore, we have to decide
the following questions in each specic case:
1. What kinds of relationships are to be considered?
2. What signicance level is to be used in hypothesis testing?
3. What values are we assuming for the various eects of interest in the popu-
lation being studied?
4. What power do we want to strive for in designing the study?
When we have answered these questions exactly, then we can conduct a power
analysis for the specied set of conditions by following some basic steps:
1. Obtain expected covariance matrices for each set of relationships by substi-
tuting the assumed values of the population parameters in the model for each
relationship.
2. Assign some initial arbitrary sample sizes to each separate group of relatives.
3. Use Mx to analyze the expected covariance matrices just as we would to
analyze real data and obtain the
2
value for testing the specic hypothesis
of interest.
4. Find out (from statistical tables) how big that
2
has to be to guarantee the
power we need.
5. Use a simple formula (given below) to multiply our assumed sample size and
solve for the sample size we need.
It is essential to remember that the sample size we obtain in step ve only
applies to the particular eect, design, sample sizes, and even to the distribution of
sample sizes among the dierent types of relationship assumed in a specic power
calculation. To explore the question of power fully, it often will be necessary to
consider a number, sometimes a large number, of dierent designs and population
values for the relevant eects of genes and environment.
144 CHAPTER 7. POWER AND SAMPLE SIZE
7.4 Power for the continuous case
A common question in genetic research concerns the ability of a study of twins
reared together to detect the eects of the shared environment. Let us investigate
this issue using Mx. Following the steps outlined above, we start by stipulating
that we are going to explore the power of a classical twin study that is, one in
which we measure MZ and DZ twins reared together. We shall assume that 50%
of the variation in the population is due to the unique environmental experiences
of individuals (e
2
= 0.5). The expected MZ twin correlation is therefore 0.50. This
intermediate value is chosen to be typical of many of the less-familial traits. An-
thropometric traits, and many cognitive traits, tend to have higher MZ correlations
than this, so the power calculations should be conservative as far as such variables
are concerned. We assume further that the additive genetic component explains
30% of the total variation (a
2
= 0.30) and that the shared family environment
accounts for the remaining 20% (c
2
= 0.20). We now substitute these parameter
values into the algebraic expectations for the variances and covariances of MZ and
DZ twins:
Total variance = a
2
+c
2
+e
2
= 0.30 + 0.2 + 0.5 = 1.00
MZ covariance = a
2
+c
2
= 0.30 + 0.2 = 0.50
DZ covariance = .5a
2
+c
2
= 0.15 + 0.2 = 0.35
In Appendix B.1 we show a version of the Mx code for tting the ACE model
to the simulated covariance matrices. In addition to the expected covariances we
must assign an arbitrary sample size and structure. Initially, we shall assume the
study involves equal numbers, 1000 each, of MZ and DZ pairs. In order to conduct
the power calculations for the c
2
component, we can run the job for the full (ACE)
model rst and then the AE model, obtaining the expected dierence in
2
under
the full and reduced models just as we did earlier for testing the signicance of the
shared environment in real data.
Notice that tting the full ACE model yields a goodness-of-t
2
of zero. This
should always be the case when we use Mx to solve for all the parameters of the
model we used to generate the expected covariance matrices because, since there is
no sampling error attached to the simulated covariance matrices, there is perfect
agreement between the matrices supplied as data and the expected values under
the model. In addition, the parameter estimates obtained should agree precisely
with those used to simulate the data; if they are not, but the t is still perfect, it
suggests that the model is not identied (see Section 5.7). w Therefore, as long as
we are condent that we have specied the structural model correctly and that the
full model is identied, there is really no need to t the full model to the simulated
covariances matrices since we know in advance that the
2
is expected to be
zero. In practice it is often helpful to recover this known result to increase our
condence that both we and the software are doing the right thing.
For our specic case, with samples of 1000 MZ and DZ pairs, we obtain a
7.4. POWER FOR THE CONTINUOUS CASE 145
Table 7.1: Non-centrality parameter, , of non-central
2
distribution for 1 df
required to give selected values of the power of the test at the 5% signicance level
(selected from Pearson and Hartley, 1972).
Desired Power
0.25 1.65
0.50 3.84
0.75 6.94
0.80 7.85
0.90 10.51
0.95 13.00
goodness-of-t
2
4
of 11.35 for the AE model. Since the full model yields a perfect
t (
2
3
= 0), the expected dierence in
2
for 1 df testing for the eect of the
shared environment is 11.35. Such a value is well in excess of the 3.84 necessary
to conclude that c
2
is signicant at the 5% level. However, this is only the value
expected in the ideal situation. With real data, individual
2
values will vary
greatly as a function of sampling variance. We need to choose the sample sizes to
give an expected value of
2
such that observed values exceed 3.84 in a specied
proportion of cases corresponding to the desired power of the test.
It turns out that such problems are very familiar to statisticians and that the
expected values of
2
needed to give dierent values of the power at specied
signicance levels for a given df have been tabulated extensively (see Pearson and
Hartley, 1972). The expected
2
is known as the centrality parameter () of the
non-central
2
distribution (i.e., when the null-hypothesis is false). Selected values
of the non-centrality parameter are given in Table 7.1 for a
2
test with 1 df and
a signicance level of 0.05.
With 1000 pairs of MZ and DZ twins, we nd a non-centrality parameter of
11.35 when we use the
2
test to detect c
2
which explains 20% of the variation
in our hypothetical population. This corresponds to a power somewhere between
90% ( = 10.51) and 95% ( = 13.00). That is, 1000 pairs each of MZ and DZ
twins would allow us to detect, at the 5% signicance level, a signicant shared
environmental eect when the true value of c
2
was 0.20 in about 90-95% of all
possible samples of this size and composition. Conversely, we would only fail to
detect this much shared environment in about 5-10% of all possible studies.
Suppose now that we want to gure out the sample size needed to give a power
of 80%. Let this sample size be N

. Let N
0
be the sample size assumed in the initial
power analysis (2000 pairs, in our case). Let the expected
2
for the particular
test being explored with this sample size be
2
E
(11.35, in this example). From
Table 7.1, we see that the non-centrality parameter, , needs to be 7.85 to give a
power of 0.80. Since the value of
2
is expected to increase linearly as a function of
146 CHAPTER 7. POWER AND SAMPLE SIZE
sample size we can obtain the sample size necessary to give 80% power by solving:
N

2
E
N
0
(7.1)
=
7.85
11.35
2000
= 1383
That is, in a sample comprising 50% MZ and 50% DZ pairs reared together, we
would require 1,383 pairs in total, or approximately 692 pairs of each type to be
80% certain of detecting a shared environmental eect explaining 20% of the total
variance, when a further 30% is due to additive genetic factors.
It must be emphasized again that this particular sample size is specic to the
study design, sample structure, parameter values and signicance level assumed in
the simulation. Smaller samples will be needed to detect larger eects. Greater
power requires larger samples. Larger studies can detect smaller eects, and nally,
some parameters of the model may be easier to detect than others.
7.5 Loss of Power with Ordinal Data
An important factor which aects power but is often overlooked is the form of
measurement used. So far we have considered only continuous, normally distributed
variables, but of course, these are not always available in the biosocial sciences. An
exhaustive treatment of the power of the ordinal classical twin study is beyond the
scope of this text, but we shall simply illustrate the loss of power incurred when we
use more crude scales of measurement (Neale et al., 1994). Consider the example
above, but suppose this time that we wish to detect the presence of additive genetic
eects, a
2
, in the data. For the continuous case this is a trivial modication of the
input le to t a model with just c and e parameters. The chi-squared from running
this program is 19.91, and following the algebra above (equation 7.1) we see that
we would require 2000 7.85/19.91 = 788 pairs in total to be 80% certain of
rejecting the hypothesis that additive genes do not aect variation when in the
true world they account for 30%, with shared environment accounting for a further
20%. Suppose now that rather than measuring on a continuous scale, we have a
dichotomous scale which bisects the population; for example, an item on which
50% say yes and 50% say no. The data for this case may be summarized as a
contingency table, and we wish to generate tables that: (i) have a total sample size
of 1000; (ii) reect a correlation in liability of .5 for MZ and .35 for DZ twins; and
(iii) reect our threshold value of 0 to give 50% either side of the threshold. Any
routine that will compute the bivariate normal integral for given thresholds and
correlation is suitable to generate the expected proportions in each cell. In this
case we use a short Mx script (Neale, 1991) to generate the data for PRELIS. We
can use the weight option in PRELIS to indicate the cell counts for our contingency
tables. Thus, the PRELIS script might be:
7.5. LOSS OF POWER WITH ORDINAL DATA 147
Power calculation MZ twins
DA NI=3 NO=0
LA; SIM1 SIM2 FREQ
RA FI=expectmz.frq
WE FREQ
OR sim1 sim2
OU MA=PM SM=SIMMZ.COV SA=SIMMZ.ASY PA
with the le expectmz.frq looking like this:
0 0 333.333
0 1 166.667
1 0 166.667
1 1 333.333
A similar approach with the DZ correlation and thresholds gives expected fre-
quencies which can be used to compute the asymptotic variance of the tetrachoric
correlation for this second group. The simulated DZ frequency data might appear
as
0 0 306.9092
0 1 193.0908
1 0 193.0908
1 1 306.9092
The cells display considerable symmetry there are as many concordant no
pairs as there are concordant yes pairs because the threshold is at zero. Running
PRELIS generates output les, and we can see immediately that the correlations
for MZ and DZ twins remain the desired .5 and .35 assumed in the population.
The next step is to feed the correlation matrix and the weight matrix (which only
contains one element, the asymptotic variance of the correlation between twins)
into Mx, in place of the covariance matrix that we supplied for the continuous
case. This can be achieved by changing just three lines in each group of our Mx
power script:
#NGroups 2
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1000
PMatrix File=SIMMZ.COV
ACov File=SIMMZ.ASY
with corresponding lenames for the DZ group, of course. When we t the model
to these summary statistics we observe a much smaller
2
than we did for the
continuous case; the
2
is only 6.08, which corresponds to a requirement of 2,582
pairs in total for 80% power at the .05 level. That is, we need more than three
times as many pairs to get the same information about a binary item than we
need for a continuous variable. The situation further deteriorates as we move the
148 CHAPTER 7. POWER AND SAMPLE SIZE
threshold to one side of the distribution. Simulating contingency tables, computing
tetrachorics and weight matrices, and tting the false model when the threshold is
one standard deviation (SD) to the right (giving 15.9% in one category and 84.1%
in the other), the
2
is a mere 3.29, corresponding a total sample size of 4,772 total
pairs. More extreme thresholds further reduce power, so that for an item (or a
disease) with a 95:5% split we would require 13,534 total pairs. Only in the largest
studies could such sample sizes be attained, and they are quite unrealistic for data
that could be collected by personal interview or laboratory measurement. On the
positive side, it seems unlikely that given the advantages of the clinical interview or
laboratory setting, our only measure could be made at the crude yes or no binary
response level. If we are able to order our data into more than two categories, some
of the lost power can be regained. Following the procedure outlined above, and
assuming that there are two thresholds, one at 1 SD and one at +1 SD, then the

2
obtained is 8.16, corresponding to only 1,924 pairs for 80% chance of nding
additive variance signicant at the .05 level. If one threshold is 0 and the other at
1 SD then the
2
rises slightly to 9.07, or 1,730 pairs. Further improvement can be
made if we increase the measurements to comprise four categories. For example,
with thresholds at 1, 0, and 1 SD the
2
is 12.46, corresponding to a sample size
of 1,240 twin pairs.
While estimating tetrachoric correlations from a random sample of the popula-
tion has considerable advantages, it is not always the method of choice for studies
focused on a single outcome, such as schizophrenia. In cases where the base rates
are so low (e.g., 1%) then it becomes inecient to sample randomly, and an ascer-
tainment scheme in which we select cases and examine their relatives is a practical
and powerful alternative, if we have good information on the base rate in the pop-
ulation studied. The necessary power calculations can be performed using the
computer packages LISCOMP (Muthen, 1987) or Mx (Neale, 1997).
7.6 Exercises
1. Change the example program to obtain the expected
2
for the test for addi-
tive genetic eects. Find out how many pairs are needed to obtain signicant
estimates of a
2
in 80% of all possible samples.
2. Explore the eect of power of a particular test of altering the proportion of
MZ and DZ twins in the sample.
3. Show that the change in expected
2
is proportional to the change in sample
size.
4. Obtain and tabulate the sample sizes necessary to detect a signicant a
2
when the population parameter values are as follows:
7.6. EXERCISES 149
a
2
c
2
0.10 0.00
0.30 0.00
0.60 0.00
0.90 0.00
In what way do these values change if there are shared environmental eects?
5. Show that with small sample sizes for the number of pairs in each group,
some bias in the chi-squared is introduced. Consider whether or not this may
be due to the n 1 part of the maximum likelihood loss function.
150 CHAPTER 7. POWER AND SAMPLE SIZE
Chapter 8
Social Interaction
8.1 Introduction
This chapter introduces a technique for specifying and estimating paths between
dependent variables, so called non-recursive models. Uses of this technique include:
modeling social interactions, for example, sibling competition and cooperation;
testing for direction of causation in bivariate data, e.g., whether life events cause
depression or vice versa; and developmental models for longitudinal or repeated
measurements.
Models for sibling interaction have been popular in genetics for some time
(Eaves, 1976b), and the reader should see Carey (1986b) for a thorough treatment
of the problem in the context of variable family size. Here we provide an introduc-
tory outline and application for the restricted case of pairs of twins, and we assume
no eects of other siblings in the family. We further conne our treatment to sib-
ling interactions within variables. Although multivariate sibling interactions (such
as aggression in one twin causing depression in the cotwin) may in the long run
prove to be more important than those within variables, they are beyond the scope
of this introductory text. Section 8.2 provides a summary of the basic univariate
genetic model without interaction. The extension to include sibling interaction is
described in Section 8.3. Details on the consequences of sibling interaction on the
variation and covariation are discussed in Section 8.4
8.2 Basic Univariate Model without Interaction
Up to this point, we have been concerned primarily with decomposing observed
phenotypic variation into its genetic and environmental components. This has
been accomplished by estimating the paths from latent or independent variables to
dependent variables. A basic univariate path diagram is set out in Figure 8.1. This
151
152 CHAPTER 8. SOCIAL INTERACTION
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
1.0 1.0 1.0
a c e a c e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
Figure 8.1: Basic path diagram for univariate twin data for P
1
and P
2
. The
correlation between A
1
and A
2
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins.
path diagram shows the deviation phenotypes P
1
and P
2
, of a pair of twins. Here
we refer to the phenotypes as deviation phenotypes to emphasize the point that the
model assumes variables to be measured as deviations from the means, which is
the case whenever we t models to covariance matrices and do not include means.
The deviation phenotypes P
1
and P
2
result from their respective additive genetic
deviations, A
1
and A
2
, their shared environment deviations, C
1
and C
2
, and their
non-shared environmental deviations, E
1
and E
2
. The linear model corresponding
to the path diagram is:
P
1
= aA
1
+cC
1
+eE
1
P
2
= aA
2
+cC
2
+eE
2
In matrix form we can write:
_
P
1
P
2
_
=
_
a c e 0 0 0
0 0 0 a c e
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
A
1
C
1
E
1
A
2
C
2
E
2
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
or as a matrix expression
y = Gx
8.3. SIBLING INTERACTION MODEL 153
Details of specifying and estimating this basic univariate model are given in
Chapter 6. One of the interesting assumptions of this basic ACE model is that the
siblings or twins phenotypes have no inuence on each other. This assumption
may well be true of height or nger print ridge count, but is it necessarily true for a
behavior like smoking, a psychiatric condition like depression, delinquent behavior
in children or even an anthropometric measure like the body mass index? We should
not, in general, assume a priori that a source of variation is absent, especially
when an empirical test of the assumption may be readily performed. However,
we may as well recognize from the onset that evidence for social interactions or
sibling eects is pretty scarce. The fact is that usually one form or another of
the basic univariate model adequately describes a twin or family data set, within
the power of the study. This tells us that there will not be evidence of signicant
social interactions since, were such eects substantial, they would lead to failure
of basic univariate models. Nevertheless, this extension of the basic models is
of considerable theoretical interest and studying its outcome on the expectations
derived from the models can provide insight into the nature and results of social
inuences. The applications to bivariate and multivariate causal modeling are
perhaps even more intriguing and will be taken up in Chapter ??.
8.3 Sibling Interaction Model
Suppose that we are considering a phenotype like number of cigarettes smoked.
For the sake of exposition we will set aside questions about the appropriate scale of
measurement, what to do about non-smokers and so on, and assume that there is
a well-behaved quantitative variable, which we can call smoking for short. What
we want to specify is the inuence of one siblings (twins) smoking on the other
siblings (cotwins) smoking. Figure 8.2 shows a path diagram which extends the
basic univariate model for twins to include a path of magnitude s from each twins
smoking to the cotwin. If the path s is positive then the sibling interaction is essen-
tially cooperative, i.e., the more (less) one twin smokes the more (less) the cotwin
will smoke as a consequence of this direct inuence. We can easily conceive of a
highly plausible mechanism for this kind of inuence when twins are cohabiting; as
a twin lights up she oers her cotwin a cigarette. If the path s is negative then the
sibling interaction is essentially competitive. The more (less) one twin smokes the
less (more) the cotwin smokes. Although such competition contributes negatively
to the covariance between twins, it may well not override the positive covariance
resulting from shared familial factors. Thus, even in the presence of competition
the observed phenotypic covariation may still be positive. If interactions are coop-
erative in some situations and competitive in others, our analyses will reveal the
predominant mode. But before considering the detail of our expectations, let us
look more closely at how the model is specied. The linear model is now:
P
1
= sP
2
+aA
1
+cC
1
+eE
1
(8.1)
154 CHAPTER 8. SOCIAL INTERACTION
1
P
1
A
1
C
1
E
2
P
2
A
2
C
2
E
1.0 1.0 1.0
a c e a c e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
s
s
Figure 8.2: Path diagram for univariate twin data with sibling interaction for P
1
and P
2
. The correlation between A
1
and A
2
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins.
P
2
= sP
1
+aA
2
+cC
2
+eE
2
(8.2)
In matrix form we have
_
P
1
P
2
_
=
_
0 s
s 0
__
P
1
P
2
_
+
_
a c e 0 0 0
0 0 0 a c e
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
A
1
C
1
E
1
A
2
C
2
E
2
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
or
y = By +Gx
In this form the B matrix is a square matrix with the number of rows and
columns equal to the number of dependent variables. The leading diagonal of the
B matrix contains zeros. The element in row i and column j represents the path
from the j
th
dependent variable to the i
th
dependent variable. From this equation
we can deduce, as shown in more detail below, that:
y(I B) = Gx
y = (I B)
1
Gx
8.3. SIBLING INTERACTION MODEL 155
8.3.1 Application to CBC Data
By way of illustration we shall analyze data collected using the Achenbach Child
Behavior Checklist (CBC; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) on juvenile twins aged
8 through 16 years living in Virginia. Mothers were asked the extent to which a
series of problem behaviors were characteristic of each of their twin children over
the last six months. The 118 problem behaviors that were rated can be categorized,
on the basis of empirical clustering, into two broad dimensions of internalizing and
externalizing problems. The former are typied by fears, psychosomatic complaints,
and symptoms of anxiety and depression. Externalizing behaviors are characterized
by acting out delinquent and aggressive behaviors. The factor patterns vary
somewhat with the age and sex of the child but there are core items which load on
the broad factors in both boys and girls at younger (6-11 years) and older (12-16
year) ages. The 24 core items for the externalizing dimension analyzed by Silberg
et al. (1992) and Hewitt et al. (1992) include among other things: arguing a lot,
destructive behavior, disobedience, ghting, hanging around with children who
get into trouble, running away from home, stealing, and bad language. For such
behaviors we might suspect that siblings will inuence each other in a cooperative
manner through imitation or mutual reinforcement. The Mx script in Appendix C.1
species the model for sibling interactions shown in Figure 8.2.
By varying the script, the standard E, AE, CE, and ACE models may be tted
to the data to obtain the results shown in Table 8.1. Clearly the variation and
Table 8.1: Preliminary results of model tting to externalizing behavior problems
in Virginia boys from larger families.
Fit statistics Parameter Estimates
Model df
2
AIC a c e
AE 4 32.57 24.6 0.78 0.33
CE 4 29.80 21.8 0.78 0.43
AC E 3 4.95 -1.0 0.50 0.64 0.34
co-aggregation of boys behaviors problems cannot be explained either by a model
which allows only for additive genetic eects (along with non-shared environmental
inuences), nor by a model which excludes genetic inuences altogether. The ACE
model ts very well (p = .18) and suggests a heritability of 33% with shared
environmental factors accounting for 52% of the variance
1
. But is the ACE model
the best in this case? We observe that the pooled individual phenotypic variances of
the MZ twins (0.915) are greater than those of the DZ twins (0.689) and, although
1
The reader might like to consider what the components of this shared variance might include
in these data obtained from the mothers of the twins and think forward to our treatment of rating
data in Chapter 11.
156 CHAPTER 8. SOCIAL INTERACTION
this discrepancy is apparently not statistically signicant with our sample sizes (171
MZ pairs and 194 DZ pairs), we might be motivated to consider sibling interactions.
Fitting the model shown in Figure 8.2 yields results given in Table 8.2. Our gen-
Table 8.2: Parameter estimates and goodness of t statistics from tting models
of sibling interaction to CBC data.
Fit statistics Parameter estimates
Model df
2
AIC a c e s
E+s 4 29.80 21.8 * *
AE+s 3 1.80 -4.2 0.61 0.42 0.23
CE+s 3 29.80 21.8 0.88 0.28 -0.10
ACE+s 2 1.80 -2.2 0.61 .000
1
0.42 0.23
* Indicates parameters out of bounds.
1
This parameter is xed on the lower bound (0.0) by Mx
eral conclusion is that while the evidence for social interactions is not unequivocal,
a model including additive genetic eects, non-shared environments, and reciprocal
sibling cooperation provides the best account of these data.
8.4 Consequences for Variation and Covariation
In this section we will work through the matrix algebra to derive expected variance
and covariance components for a simplied model of sibling interaction. We then
show how this model can be adapted to handle the specic cases of additive and
dominant genetic, and shared and non-shared environmental eects. Numerical
examples of strong competition and cooperation will be used to illustrate their
eects on the variances and covariances of twins and unrelated individuals reared
in the same home.
8.4.1 Derivation of Expected Covariances
To understand what it is about the observed statistics that suggests sibling inter-
actions in our twin data we must follow through a little algebra. We shall try to
keep this as simple as possible by considering the path model in Figure 8.3, which
depicts the inuence of an arbitrary latent variable, X, on the phenotype P. As
long as our latent variables A, C, E, etc. are independent of each other, their
eects can be considered one at a time and then summed, even in the presence of
social interactions. The linear model corresponding to this path diagram is
P
1
= sP
2
+xX
1
(8.3)
8.4. CONSEQUENCES FOR VARIATION AND COVARIATION 157

X
1

X
2
x x
e
e
e
e
e
ee


P
1
P
2
E
'
s
s
Figure 8.3: Path diagram showing inuence of arbitrary exogenous variable X on
phenotype P in a pair of relatives (for univariate twin data, incorporating sibling
interaction).
P
2
= sP
1
+xX
2
(8.4)
Or, in matrices:
_
P
1
P
2
_
=
_
0 s
s 0
__
P
1
P
2
_
+
_
x 0
0 x
__
X
1
X
2
_
which in turn we can write more economically as
y = By +Gx
Following the rules for matrix algebra set out in Chapters 4 and ??, we can rear-
range this equation, as before:
y By = Gx (8.5)
Iy By = Gx (8.6)
(I B)y = Gx , (8.7)
and then, multiplying both sides of this equation by the inverse of (I - B), we have
y = (I B)
1
Gx . (8.8)
In this case, the matrix (I - B) is simply
_
1 s
s 1
_
,
158 CHAPTER 8. SOCIAL INTERACTION
which has determinant 1 s
2
, so (I B)
1
is
1
1 s
2

_
1 s
s 1
_
.
The symbol is used to represent the Kronecker product, which in this case simply
means that each element in the matrix is to be multiplied by the constant
1
1s
2
.
We have a vector of phenotypes on the left hand side of equation 8.8. In the
chapter on matrix algebra (p. 82) we showed how the covariance matrix could be
computed from the raw data matrix Tby expressing the observed data as deviations
from the mean to form matrix U, and computing the matrix product UU

. The
same principle is applied here to the vector of phenotypes, which has an expected
mean of 0 and is thus already expressed in mean deviate form. So to nd the
expected variance-covariance matrix of the phenotypes P
1
and P
2
, we multiply by
the transpose:
E {yy

} =
_
(I B)
1
Gx
__
(I B)
1
Gx
_

(8.9)
= (I B)
1
GE {xx

} G

(I B)
1

. (8.10)
Now in the middle of this equation we have the matrix product E {xx

}. This is
the covariance matrix of the x variables. For our particular example, we want two
standardized variables, X
1
and X
2
to have unit variance and correlation r so the
matrix is:
_
1 r
r 1
_
.
We now have all the pieces required to compute the covariance matrix, recalling
that for this case,
G =
_
x 0
0 x
_
(8.11)
(I B)
1
=
1
1 s
2

_
1 s
s 1
_
(8.12)
E {xx

} =
_
1 r
r 1
_
. (8.13)
The reader may wish to show as an exercise that by substituting the right hand sides
of equations 8.11 to 8.13 into equation 8.10, and carrying out the multiplication,
we obtain:
E {yy

} =
x
2
(1 s
2
)
2

_
1 + 2sr +s
2
r + 2s +rs
2
r + 2s +rs
2
1 + 2sr +s
2
_
(8.14)
We can use this result to derive the eects of sibling interaction on the variance
and covariance due to a variety of sources of individual dierences. For example,
when considering:
8.4. CONSEQUENCES FOR VARIATION AND COVARIATION 159
1. additive genetic inuences, x
2
= a
2
and r = , where is 1.0 for MZ twins
and 0.5 for DZ twins;
2. shared environment inuences, x
2
= c
2
and r = 1;
3. non-shared environmental inuences, x
2
= e
2
and r = 0;
4. genetic dominance, x
2
= d
2
and r = , where = 1.0 for MZ twins and
= 0.25 for DZ twins.
These results are summarized in Table 8.3.
Table 8.3: Eects of sibling interaction(s) on variance and covariance components
between pairs of relatives.
Source Variance Covariance
Additive genetic (1 + 2s +s
2
)a
2
( + 2s +s
2
)a
2
Dominance genetic (1 + 2s +s
2
)d
2
( + 2s +s
2
)d
2
Shared environment (1 + 2s +s
2
)c
2
(1 + 2s +s
2
)c
2
Non-shared environment (1 +s
2
)e
2
2se
2
represents the scalar
1
(1s
2
)
2
obtained from equation 8.14.
8.4.2 Numerical Illustration
To illustrate these eects numerically, let us consider a simplied situation in which
a
2
= .5, d
2
= 0, c
2
= 0, e
2
= .5 in the absence of social interaction (i.e., s = 0);
in the presence of strong cooperation, s = .5; and in the presence of strong com-
petition, s = .5. Table 8.4 gives the numerical values for MZ and DZ twins and
unrelated pairs of individuals reared together (e.g., adoptive siblings). In terms
of correlations, phenotypic cooperation mimics the eects of shared environment
while phenotypic competition may mimic the eects of non-additive genetic vari-
ance. However, the eects can be distinguished because social interactions result in
dierent total phenotypic variances for dierently related pairs of individuals. All
of the other kinds of models we have considered predict that the population vari-
ance of individuals is not aected by the presence or absence of relatives. However,
cooperative interactions increase the variance of more closely related individuals
the most, while competitive interactions increase them the least and under some
circumstances may decrease them. Thus, in twin data, cooperation is distinguished
from shared environmental eects because cooperation results in greater total phe-
notypic variance in MZ than in DZ twins. Competition is distinguished from non-
additive genetic eects because it results in lower total phenotypic variance in MZ
160 CHAPTER 8. SOCIAL INTERACTION
Table 8.4: Eects of strong sibling interaction on the variance and covariance
between MZ, DZ, and unrelated individuals reared together. The interaction pa-
rameter s takes the values 0, .5, and .5 for no sibling interaction, cooperation,
and competition, respectively.
MZ twins DZ twins Unrelated
Interaction Var Cov r Var Cov r Var Cov r
None 1.00 .50 .50 1.00 .25 .25 1.00 .00 .00
Cooperation 3.11 2.89 .93 2.67 2.33 .88 2.22 1.78 .80
Competition 1.33 .44 .33 1.78 -.67 -.38 2.22 -1.78 -.80
than in DZ twins. This is the bottom line: social interactions cause the variance
of a phenotype to depend on the degree of relationship of the social actors.
There are three observations we should make about this result. First, a test
of the contrary assumption, i.e., that the total observed variance is independent
of zygosity in twins, was set out by Jinks and Fulker (1970) as a preliminary re-
quirement of their analyses and, as has been noted, is implicitly provided whenever
we t models without social interactions to covariance matrices. For I.Q., educa-
tional attainment, psychometric assessments of personality, social attitudes, body
mass index, heart rate reactivity, and so on, the behavior genetic literature is re-
plete with evidence for the absence of the eects of social interaction. Second,
analyses of family correlations (rather than variances and covariances) eectively
standardize the variances of dierent groups of individuals and throw away the
very information we need to distinguish social interactions from other inuences.
Third, if we are working with categorical data and adopting a threshold model (see
Chapter 2), we can make predictions about the standardized thresholds in dierent
groups. Higher quantitative variances lead to smaller (i.e., less deviant) thresholds
and therefore higher prevalence for the extreme categories. Thus, for example, if
abstinence vs. drinking status is inuenced by sibling cooperation on a latent un-
derlying phenotype, and abstinence has a frequency of 10% in DZ twins, we should
expect a higher frequency of abstinence in MZ twins. These models are relatively
simple to implement in Mx (Neale, 1997).
Chapter 9
Sex-limitation and G E
Interaction
9.1 Introduction
As described in Chapter 6, the basic univariate ACE model allows us to estimate
genetic and environmental components of phenotypic variance from like-sex MZ
and DZ twin data. When data are available from both male and female twin pairs,
an investigator may be interested in asking whether the variance prole of a trait
is similar across the sexes or whether the magnitude of genetic and environmental
inuences are sex-dependent. To address this issue, the ACE model may be tted
independently to data from male and female twins, and the parameter estimates
compared by inspection. This approach, however, has three severe limitations: (1)
it does not test whether the heterogeneity observed across the sexes is signicant;
(2) it does not attempt to explain the sex dierences by tting a particular sex-
limitation model; and (3) it discards potentially useful information by excluding
dizygotic opposite-sex twin pairs from the analysis. In the rst part of this chapter
(Section 9.2), we outline three models for exploring sex dierences in genetic and
environmental eects (i.e., models for sex-limitation) and provide an example of
each by analyzing twin data on body mass index (BMI) (Section 9.2.4).
Just as the magnitude of genetic and environmental inuences may dier ac-
cording to sex, they also may vary under disparate environmental conditions. If
dierences in genetic variance across environmental exposure groups result in dier-
ential heritability estimates for these groups, a genotype environment interaction
is said to exist. Historically, genotype environment (G E) interactions have
been noted in plant and animal species (Mather and Jinks, 1982); however, there
is increasing evidence that they play an important role in human variability as well
(Heath and Martin, 1986; Heath et al., 1989b). A simple method for detecting
161
162 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
G E interactions is to estimate components of phenotypic variance conditional
on environmental exposure (Eaves, 1982). In the second part of this chapter (Sec-
tion 9.3), we illustrate how this method may be employed by suitably modifying
models for sex-limitation. We then apply the models to depression scores of female
twins and estimate components of variance conditional on a putative buering
environment, marital status (Section 9.3.2).
9.2 Sex-limitation Models
9.2.1 General Model for Sex-limitation
The general sex-limitation model allows us to (1) estimate the magnitude of ge-
netic and environmental eects on male and female phenotypes and (2) determine
whether or not it is the same set of genes or shared environmental experiences that
inuence a trait in males and females. Although the rst task may be achieved
with data from like-sex twin pairs only, the second task requires that we have
data from opposite-sex pairs (Eaves et al., 1978). Thus, the Mx script we describe
will include model specications for all 5 zygosity groups (MZmale, MZfemale,
DZmale, DZfemale, DZopposite-sex).
To introduce the general sex-limitation model, we consider a path diagram for
opposite-sex pairs, shown in Figure 9.1. Included among the ultimate variables in
the diagram are female and male additive genetic (A
f
and A
m
), dominant genetic
(D
f
and D
m
), and unique environmental (E
f
and E
m
) eects, which inuence
the latent phenotype of the female (P
f
) or male (P
m
) twin. The additive and
dominant genetic eects are correlated within twin pairs ( = 0.50 for additive
eects, and = 0.25 for dominant eects) as they are for DZ like-sex pairs in
the simple univariate ACE model. This correlational structure implies that the
genetic eects represent common sets of genes which inuence the trait in both
males and females; however, since a
m
and a
f
or d
m
and d
f
are not constrained to
be equal, the common eects need not have the same magnitude across the sexes.
Figure 9.1 also includes ultimate variables for the male (or female) member of the
opposite-sex twin pair (A

m
and D

m
) which do not correlate with genetic eects
on the female phenotype. For this reason, we refer to A

m
and D

m
as sex-specic
variables. Signicant estimates of their eects indicate that the set of genes which
inuences a trait in males is not identical to that which inuences a trait in females.
To determine the extent of male-female genetic similarity, one can calculate the
male-female genetic correlation (r
g
). As usual (see Chapter 2) the correlation is
computed as the covariance of the two variables divided by the product of their
respective standard deviations. Thus, for additive genetic eects we have
r
g
=
a
m
a
f
_
a
2
f
(a
2
m
+a
2
m
)
9.2. SEX-LIMITATION MODELS 163
f
P
f
A
f
D
f
E
m
P
m
A
m
D
m
E
m
A'
m
D'
1.0 1.0 1.0
f
a
f
d
f
e
m
a
m
d
m
e
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.25
1.0 1.0
m
d'
m
a'
Figure 9.1: The general genotype sex interaction model for twin data. Path
diagram is shown for DZ opposite-sex twins P
f
and P
m
. The correlation between
A
f
and A
m
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins. The correlation between D
f
and
D
m
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.25 for DZ twins.
Alternatively, a similar estimate may be obtained for dominant genetic eects.
However, the information available from twin pairs reared together precludes the
estimation of both sex-specic parameters, a

m
and d

m
and, consequently, both
additive and dominance genetic correlations. Instead, models including A

m
or D

m
may be t to the data, and their ts compared using appropriate goodness-of-t
indices, such as Akaikes Information Criteria (AIC; Akaike, 1987; see Section ??).
This criterion may be used to compare the t of an ACE model to the t of an
ADE model. AIC is one member of a class of indices that reect both the goodness
of t of a model and its parsimony, or ability to account for the observed data with
few parameters.
To generalize the model specied in Figure 9.1 to other zygosity groups, the
parameters associated with the female phenotype are equated to similar eects on
the phenotypes of female same-sex MZ and DZ twin pairs. In the same manner,
all parameters associated with the male phenotype (reecting eects which are
common to both sexes as well as those specic to males) are equated to eects on
both members of male same-sex MZ and DZ pairs. As a result, the model predicts
that variances will be equal for all female twins, and all male twins, regardless
of zygosity group or twin status (i.e., twin 1 vs. twin 2). The model does not
necessarily predict equality of variances across the sexes.
164 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
9.2.2 General Sex-limitation Model Mx Script
The full Mx specication for the general sex-limitation model is provided in Ap-
pendix D.1. In theory, the same approach that was used to specify the simple uni-
variate ACE model (Chapter 6) in Mx could be used for the general sex-limitation
model. That is, genetic and environmental parameters can be specied in calcu-
lation groups and the matrices can be included in the data groups to specify the
expected covariance matrices. The female and male parameters are declared in
separate groups which simplies the data groups. The only dierences between
the male and female data groups are the details about the data and the number of
the group from which matrices are being imported. Note that for the general sex
limitation model, one extra matrix (N) is declared in the male group to account for
the male-specic additive genetic eects.
While the specication of the same-sex groups is a straightforward extension of
the univariate model, the opposite-sex group requires some special attention. First,
the matrices for both male and female variance components are read in to formulate
the expected variance for females (twin 1) and males (twin 2). Second, the expected
covariance between male and female twins can be specied by multiplying the male
and female path coecient matrices. Although not a problem in the univariate
analysis, note that the male-female expected covariance matrix is not necessarily
symmetric.
Without boundary constraints on the parameters, this specication may lead
to negative parameter estimates for one sex, especially when the DZ opposite-sex
correlation is low, as compared to DZ like-sex correlations. Such negative param-
eter estimates result in a negative genetic (or common environmental) covariation
between the sexes. Although a negative covariation is plausible, it seems quite un-
likely that the same genes or common environmental inuences would have opposite
eects across the sexes. With the availability of linear and non-linear constraints in
Mx, we can parameterize the general sex-limitation model so that the male-female
covariance components are constrained to be non-negative by using a boundary
statement:
Bound .000 10 X 1 1 1 Z 1 1 1 W 1 1 1
Bound .000 10 X 2 1 1 Z 2 1 1 W 2 1 1 N 2 1 1
where .000 is the lower boundary, 10 is the upper boundary followed by matrix
elements.
In this example, we estimate sex-specic additive genetic eects (and x the sex-
specic dominance eects to zero). The data are log-transformed indices of body
mass index (BMI) obtained from twins belonging to the Virginia and American
Association of Retired Persons twin registries. A detailed description of these data
will be provided in section 9.2.4, in the discussion of the model-tting results.
9.2. SEX-LIMITATION MODELS 165
9.2.3 Restricted Models for Sex-limitation
In this section, we describe two restricted models for sex-limitation. The rst
we refer to as the common eects sex-limitation model, and the second, the scalar
sex-limitation model. Both are sub-models of the general sex-limitation model and
therefore can be compared to the more general model using likelihood-ratio
2
dierence tests.
Common Eects Sex-limitation Model
The common eects sex-limitation model is simply one in which the sex-specic
pathways in Figures 9.1 (a

m
or d

m
) are xed to zero or the additive (or dominant)
genetic correlation between males and females is xed to .50 (or .25). As a result,
only the genetic eects which are common to both males and females account for
phenotypic variance and covariance. Although the genes may be the same, the
magnitude of their eect is still allowed to dier across the sexes. This restricted
model may be compared to the general sex-limitation model using a
2
dierence
test with a single degree of freedom.
Information to discern between the general sex-limitation model and the com-
mon eects model comes from the covariance of DZ opposite-sex twin pairs. Specif-
ically, if this covariance is signicantly less than that predicted from genetic eects
which are common to both sexes (i.e., less than [(a
m
a
f
) +(d
m
d
f
)]), then there
is evidence for sex-specic eects. Otherwise, the restricted model without these
eects should not t signicantly worse than the general model. Mere inspection
of the correlations from DZ like-sex and opposite-sex pairs may alert one to the
fact that sex-specic eects are playing a role in trait variation, if it is found that
the opposite sex-correlation is markedly less than the like-sex DZ correlations.
Scalar Eects Sex-limitation Model
The scalar sex-limitation model is a sub-model of both the general model and the
common eects model. In the scalar model, not only are the sex-specic eects
removed, but the variance components for females are all constrained to be equal
to a scalar multiple (k
2
) of the male variance components, such that a
2
f
= k
2
a
2
m
,
d
2
f
= k
2
d
2
m
, and e
2
f
= k
2
e
2
m
. As a result, the standardized variance components
(e.g., heritability estimates) are equal across sexes, even though the unstandardized
components dier.
Figure 9.2 shows a path diagram for DZ opposite-sex under the scalar sex-
limitation model, and Appendix D.2 provides the Mx specication. Unlike the
model in Figure 9.1, the scalar model does not include separate parameters for
genetic and environmental eects on males and females instead, these eects are
equated across the sexes. Because of this equality, negative estimates of male-female
genetic covariance cannot result. To introduce a scaling factor for the male (or
166 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
female) variance components, we can pre and postmultiply the expected variances
by a scalar.
f
A
f
D
f
E
m
A
m
D
m
E
f
P
m
P
f
L
m
L
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.25
f
a
f
d
f
e
m
a
m
d
m
e
1.0 k
Figure 9.2: The scalar genotype sex interaction model for twin data. Path
diagram is shown for DZ opposite-sex twins P
f
and P
m
. The correlation between
A
f
and A
m
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins. The correlation between D
f
and
D
m
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.25 for DZ twins.
The full scalar sex-limitation model may be compared to the full common eects
model using a
2
dierence test with 2 degrees of freedom. Similarly, the scalar
sex-limitation model may be compared to the model with no sex dierences (that is,
one which xes k to 1.0) using a
2
dierence test with a single degree of freedom.
The restricted sex-limitation models described in this section are not an ex-
haustive list of the sub-models of the general sex-limitation model. Within either
of these restricted models (as within the general model), one can test hypothe-
ses regarding the signicance of genetic or environmental eects. Also, within the
common eects sex-limitation model, one may test whether specic components of
variance are equal across the sexes (e.g., a
m
may be equated to a
f
, or e
m
to e
f
).
9.2. SEX-LIMITATION MODELS 167
Again, sub-models may be compared to more saturated ones through
2
dierence
tests, or to models with the same number of parameters with Akaikes Information
Criteria.
9.2.4 Application to Body Mass Index
In this section, we apply sex-limitation models to data on body mass index col-
lected from twins in the Virginia Twin Registry and twins ascertained through
the American Association of Retired Persons (AARP). Details of the membership
of these two twin cohorts are provided in Eaves et al. (1991), in their analysis of
BMI in extended twin-family pedigrees. In brief, the Virginia twins are members
of a population based registry comprised of 7,458 individuals (Corey et al., 1986),
while the AARP twins are members of a volunteer registry of 12,118 individuals
responding to advertisements in publications of the AARP. The Virginia twins
mean age is 39.7 years (SD = 14.3), compared to 54.5 years (SD = 16.8) for the
AARP twins. Between 1985 and 1987, Health and Lifestyle questionnaires were
mailed to twins from both of these cohorts. Among the items on the questionnaire
were those pertaining to physical similarity and confusion in recognition by others
(used to diagnose zygosity) and those asking about current height and weight (used
to compute body mass index). Questionnaires with no missing values for any of
these items were returned by 5,465 Virginia and AARP twin pairs.
From height and weight data, body mass index (BMI) was calculated for the
twins, using the formula:
BMI = wt(kg)/ht(m)
2
The natural logarithm of BMI was then taken to normalize the data. Before calcu-
lating covariance matrices of log BMI, the data from the two cohorts were combined,
and the eects of age, age squared, sample (AARP vs. Virginia), sex, and their in-
teractions were removed. The resulting covariance matrices are provided in the Mx
scripts in Appendices D.1 and D.2, while the correlations and sample sizes appear
in Table 9.1 below.
Table 9.1: Sample sizes and correlations for BMI data in Virginia and AARP
twins.
Zygosity Group N r
MZF 1802 0.744
DZF 1142 0.352
MZM 750 0.700
DZM 553 0.309
DZO 1341 0.251
168 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
We note that both like-sex MZ correlations are greater than twice the respective
DZ correlations; thus, models with dominant genetic eects, rather than common
environmental eects, were t to the data.
In Table 9.2, we provide selected results from tting the following models:
general sex-limitation (I); common eects sex-limitation (II-IV); and scalar sex-
limitation (V). We rst note that the general sex-limitation model provides a good
t to the data, with p = 0.32. The estimate of a

m
under this model is fairly small,
and when set to zero in model II, found to be non-signicant (
2
1
= 2.54, p > 0.05).
Thus, there is no evidence for sex-specic additive genetic eects, and the common
eects sex-limitation model (model II) is favored over the general model. As an
exercise, the reader may wish to verify that the same conclusion is reached if the
general sex-limitation model with sex-specic dominant genetic eects is compared
to the common eects model with d

m
removed.
Note that under model II the dominant genetic parameter for females is quite
small; thus, when this parameter is xed to zero in model III, there is not a signi-
cant worsening of t, and model III becomes the most favored model. In model IV,
we consider whether the dominant genetic eect for males can also be xed to zero.
The goodness-of-t statistics indicate that this model ts the data poorly (p < 0.01)
and provides a signicantly worse t than model III (
2
1
= 26.73, p < 0.01). Model
IV is therefore rejected and model III remains the favored one.
Finally, we consider the scalar sex-limitation model. Since there is evidence for
dominant genetic eects in males and not in females, it seems unlikely that this
model, which constrains the variance components of females to be scalar multiples
of the male variance components, will provide a good t to the data, unless the
additive genetic variance in females is also much smaller than the male additive
genetic variance. The model-tting results support this contention: the model
provides a marginal t to the data (p = 0.05), and is signicantly worse than
model II (
2
2
= 7.82, p < 0.05 ). We thus conclude from Table 9.2 that model III
is the best tting model. This conclusion would also be reached if AIC was used
to assess goodness-of-t.
Using the parameter estimates under model III, the expected variance of log
BMI (residuals) in males and females can be calculated. A little arithmetic reveals
that the phenotypic variance of males is markedly lower than that of females (0.17
vs. 0.28). Inspection of the parameter estimates indicates that the sex dierence
in phenotypic variance is due to increased genetic and environmental variance in
females. However, the increase in genetic variance in females is proportionately
greater than the increase in environmental variance, and this dierence results in a
somewhat larger broad sense (i.e., a
2
+d
2
) heritability estimate for females (75%)
than for males (69%).
The detection of sex dierences in environmental and genetic eects on BMI
leads to questions regarding the nature of these dierences. Speculation might
suggest that the somewhat lower male heritability estimate may be due to the
fact that males are less accurate in their self-report of height and weight than
9.3. GENOTYPE ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION 169
Table 9.2: Parameter estimates from tting genotype sex interaction models to
BMI.
MODEL
Parameter I II III IV V
a
f
0.449 0.454 0.454 0.454 0.346
d
f
0.172 0.000 0.288
e
f
0.264 0.265 0.265 0.267 0.267
a
m
0.210 0.240 0.240 0.342
d
m
0.184 0.245 0.245
e
m
0.213 0.213 0.213 0.220
a

m
0.198
k 0.778

2
9.26 11.80 11.80 38.53 19.62
d.f. 8 9 10 11 11
p 0.32 0.23 0.30 0.00 0.05
AIC -6.74 -6.20 -8.20 16.53 -2.38
are females. With additional information, such as test-retest data, this hypothesis
could be rigorously tested. The sex dependency of genetic dominance is similarly
curious. It may be that the common environment in females exerts a greater
inuence on BMI than in males, and, consequently, masks a genetic dominance
eect. Alternatively, the genetic architecture may indeed be dierent across the
sexes, resulting from sex dierences in selective pressures during human evolution.
Again, additional data, such as that from reared together adopted siblings, could
be used to explore these alternative hypotheses.
One sex-limitation model that we have not considered, but which is biologically
reasonable, is that the across-sex correlation between additive genetic eects is the
same as the across-sex correlation between the dominance genetic eects
1
. Fitting
a model of this type involves a non-linear constraint which can easily be specied
in Mx.
9.3 Genotype Environment Interaction
As stated in the introduction of this chapter, genotype environment (G E)
interactions can be detected by estimating components of phenotypic variance con-
1
The reasoning goes like this: (e.g.) males have a elevated level of a chemical that prevents any
gene expression from certain loci, at random with respect to the phenotype under study. Thus,
both additive and dominant genetic eects would be reduced in males vs females, and hence the
same genetic correlation between the sexes would apply to both.
170 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
ditional on environmental exposures. To do so, MZ and DZ covariance matrices
are computed for twins concordant for exposure, concordant for non-exposure , and
discordant for exposure, and structural equation models are tted to the result-
ing six zygosity groups. The Mx specications for alternative G E interaction
models are quite similar to those used in a sex-limitation analysis; however, there
are important dierences between the two. In a G E interaction analysis, the
presence of a sixth group provides the information for an additional parameter
to be estimated. Further, the nature of alternative hypotheses used to explain
heterogeneity across groups diers from those invoked in a sex-limitation analysis.
In section 9.3.1 we detail these dierences, and in section 9.3.2 we illustrate the
method with an application to data on marital status and depression.
9.3.1 Models for G E Interactions
The models described in this section are appropriate for analyzing G E interac-
tion when genes and environment are acting independently. However, if there is
genotype environment correlation, then more sophisticated statistical procedures
are necessary for the analysis. One way of detecting a G E correlation is to com-
pute the cross-correlations between one twins environment and the trait of interest
in the cotwin (Heath et al., 1989b). If the cross-correlation is not signicant, there
is no evidence for a G E correlation, and the G E analysis may proceed using
the methods described below.
General G E Interaction Model
First we consider the general G E interaction model, similar to the general sex-
limitation model discussed in section 9.2.3. This model not only allows the magni-
tude of genetic and environmental eects to vary across environmental conditions,
but also, by using information from twin pairs discordant for environmental expo-
sure, enables us to determine whether it is the same set of genes or environmental
features that are expressed in the two environments. Just as we used twins who
were discordant for sex (i.e., DZO pairs) to illustrate the sex-limitation model,
we use twins discordant for environmental exposure to portray the general G E
interaction model. Before modeling genetic and environmental eects on these indi-
viduals, one must order the twins so that the rst of the pair has not been exposed
to the putative modifying environment, while the second has (or vice versa, as long
as the order is consistent across families and across groups). The path model for
the discordant DZ pairs is then identical to that used for the dizygotic opposite-sex
pairs in the sex-limitation model; for the discordant MZ pairs, it diers only from
the DZ model in the correlation structure of the ultimate genetic variables (see
Figure 9.3).
Among the ultimate variables in Figure 9.3 are genetic eects that are cor-
related between the unexposed and exposed twins and those that inuence only
the latter (i.e., environment-specic eects). For the concordant unexposed and
9.3. GENOTYPE ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION 171
u
P
u
A
u
D
u
E
e
P
e
A
e
D
e
E
1.0 1.0 1.0
u
a
u
c
u
e
e
a
e
c
e
e
1.0 1.0 1.0
rg rd
Figure 9.3: The general genotype environment interaction model for twin data.
Path diagram is for MZ and DZ twins discordant for environmental exposure, P
u
and P
e
. The correlation between A
u
and A
e
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins.
The correlation between D
u
and D
e
is 1.0 for MZ and 0.25 for DZ twins. The
subscripts u and e identify variables and parameters and unexposed and exposed
twins, respectively.
concordant exposed MZ and DZ pairs, path models are comparable to those used
for female-female and male-male MZ and DZ pairs in the sex-limitation analysis,
with environment-specic eects (instead of sex-specic eects) operating on the
exposed twins (instead of the male twins). As a result, the model predicts equal
variances within an exposure class, across zygosity groups.
In specifying the general G E interaction model in Mx, one must again use
boundary constraints, in order to avoid negative covariance estimates for the pairs
discordant for exposure (Appendix D.3).
Unlike the general sex-limitation analysis, there is enough information in a
G E analysis to estimate two environment-specic eects. Thus, the magni-
tude of environment-specic additive and dominant genetic or additive genetic and
common environmental eects can be determined. It still is not possible to simul-
taneously estimate the magnitude of common environmental and dominant genetic
eects.
172 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
Common Eects G E Interaction Model
A common eects G E model can also be tted to covariance matrices computed
conditionally on environmental exposure by simply xing the environment-specic
eects of the general model to zero, and comparing the two using a
2
dierence
test. The information from pairs discordant for environmental exposure allows for
this comparison.
A critical sub-model of the common eects G E model is one which tests the
hypothesis that exposure group heterogeneity is solely due to heteroscedasticity, or
group dierences in random environmental variance, rather than group dierences
in genetic variance. To t this model, the genetic parameters are simply equated
across groups, while allowing the random environmental eects to take on dierent
values. If this model does not t worse than the full common eects model, then
there is evidence for heteroscedasticity.
A second sub-model of the common eects G E interaction model is one which
constrains the environmental parameters to be equal across exposure groups, while
allowing the genetic variance components to dier. If this model is not signicantly
worse than the full common eects model, then there is evidence to suggest that
the environmental interaction only involves a dierential expression of genetic, but
not environmental, inuences.
Scalar Eects G E Interaction Model
As with the scalar sex-limitation model, the scalar G E interaction model equates
genetic and environmental eects on exposed twins to be a scalar multiple of sim-
ilar eects on twins who have not been exposed to a modifying environment. As
a consequence, the heritability of a trait remains constant across exposure groups,
and there is no evidence for a genotype environment interaction. This situa-
tion may arise if there is a mean-variance relationship, and an increase in trait
mean under a particular environmental condition is accompanied by an increase
in phenotypic variation. When this is the case, the ratio of the genetic variance
component and environmental variance component is expected to remain the same
in dierent environments.
The Mx specication for the scalar G E interaction model is identical to that
used for the scalar sex-limitation model, except for the addition of MZ discordant
pairs. The Mx script in Appendix D.4 illustrates how these pairs may be included.
9.3.2 Application to Marital Status and Depression
In this section, we determine whether the heritability of self-report depression scores
varies according to the marital status of female twins. Our hypothesis is that mar-
riage, or a marriage-type relationship, serves as a buer to decrease an individuals
inherited liability to depression, consequently decreasing the heritability of the
trait.
9.3. GENOTYPE ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION 173
The data were collected from twins enrolled in the Australian National Health
and Medical Research Council Twin register. In this sample, mailed questionnaires
were sent to the 5,967 pairs of twins on the register between November 1980 and
March 1982 (see also Chapter 10). Among the items on the questionnaire were
those from the state depression scale of the Delusions-Symptoms States Inventory
(DSSI; Bedford et al., 1976) and a single item regarding marital status. The anal-
yses performed here focus on the like-sex MZ and DZ female pairs who returned
completed questionnaires. The ages of the respondents ranged from 18 to 88 years;
however, due to possible dierences in variance components across age cohorts, we
have limited our analysis to those twins who were age 30 or less at the time of their
response. There were 570 female MZ pairs in this young cohort, with mean age
23.77 years (SD=3.65); and 349 DZ pairs, with mean age 23.66 years (SD=3.93).
Using responses to the marital status item, pairs were subdivided into those
who were concordant for being married (or living in a marriage-type relationship);
those who were concordant for being unmarried; and those who were discordant
for marital status. In the discordant pairs, the data were reordered so that the rst
twin was always unmarried. Depression scores were derived by summing the 7 DSSI
item scores, and then taking a log-transformation of the data [x

= log
10
(x+1)] to
reduce heteroscedasticity. Covariance matrices of depression scores were computed
for the six zygosity groups after linear and quadratic eects of age were removed.
The matrices are provided in the Mx scripts in Appendices D.3 and D.4, while the
correlations and sample sizes are shown in Table 9.3. We note (i) that in all cases,
MZ correlations are greater than the corresponding DZ correlations; and (ii) that
for concordant married and discordant pairs, the MZ:DZ ratio is greater than 2:1,
suggesting the presence of genetic dominance.
Table 9.3: Sample sizes and correlations for depression data in Australian female
twins.
Zygosity Group N r
MZ - Concordant single 254 0.409
DZ - Concordant single 155 0.221
MZ - Concordant married 177 0.382
DZ - Concordant married 107 0.098
MZ - Discordant 139 0.324
DZ - Discordant 87 0.059
Before proceeding with the G E interaction analyses, we tested whether there
was a G E correlation involving marital status and depression. To do so, cross-
correlations between twins marital status and cotwins depression score were com-
puted. In all but one case (DZ twin 1s depression with cotwins marital status;
174 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
r = 0.156, p < 0.01), the correlations were not signicant. This near absence of
signicant correlations implies that a genetic predisposition to depression does not
lead to an increased probability of remaining single, and indicates that a G E
correlation need not be modeled.
Table 9.4 shows the results of tting several models: general GE (I); full
common-eects G E (II); three common-eects sub-models (III-V); scalar G x E
(VI); and no G E interaction (VII). Parameter estimates subscripted s and m re-
fer respectively to single (unexposed) and married twins. Models including genetic
dominance parameters, rather than common environmental eects, were tted to
the data. The reader may wish to show that the overall conclusions concerning
G E interaction do not dier if shared environment parameters are substituted
for genetic dominance.
Table 9.4: Parameter estimates from tting genotype marriage interaction
models to depression scores.
MODEL
Parameter I II III IV V VI VII
a
s
0.187 0.187 0.207 0.209 0.186 0.206 0.188
d
s
0.106 0.105
e
s
0.240 0.240 0.246 0.245 0.257 0.247 0.246
a
m
0.048 0.048 0.163 0.162 0.186 0.206 0.188
d
m
0.171 0.173
e
m
0.232 0.232 0.243 0.245 0.232 0.247 0.246
a

m
0.008
k 0.916

2
15.44 15.48 18.88 18.91 22.32 20.08 27.19
d.f. 11 12 14 15 15 15 16
p 0.16 0.22 0.17 0.22 0.10 0.17 0.04
AIC -6.56 -9.52 -9.12 -11.09 -7.68 -9.92 -4.81
Model I is a general G E model with environment-specic additive genetic
eects. It provides a reasonable t to the data (p = 0.16), with all parameters of
moderate size, except a

m
. Under model II, the parameter a

m
is set to zero, and
the t is not signicantly worse than model I (
2
1
= 0.04, p = 0.84). Thus, there
is no evidence for environment-specic additive genetic eects. As an exercise, the
reader may verify that the same conclusion can be made for environment-specic
dominant genetic eects.
Under model III, we test whether the dominance eects on single and married
individuals are signicant. A
2
dierence of 3.40 (p = 0.183, 2 df.) between
models III and II indicates that they are not. Consequently, model III, which
9.3. GENOTYPE ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION 175
excludes common dominance eects while retaining common additive genetic and
specic environmental eects, is favored.
Models IV - VII are all sub-models of III: the rst species no dierences in
environmental variance components across exposure groups; the second species
no dierences in genetic variance components across groups; the third constrains
the genetic and environmental variance components of single twins to be scalar
multiples of those of married twins; and the fourth species no genetic or environ-
mental dierences between the groups. When each of these is compared to model
III using a
2
dierence test, only model VII (specifying complete homogeneity
across groups) is signicantly worse than the fuller model (
2
2
= 8.28, p = 0.004).
In order to select the best sub-models from IV, V and VI, Akaikes Information
Criteria were used. These criteria indicate that model IV which allows for group
dierences in genetic, but not environmental, eects gives the most parsimo-
nious explanation for the data. Under model IV, the heritability of depression is
42% for single, and 30% for married twins. This nding supports our hypothesis
that marriage or marriage-type relationships act as a buer against the expression
of inherited liability to depression.
176 CHAPTER 9. SEX-LIMITATION AND G E INTERACTION
Chapter 10
Multivariate Analysis
10.1 Introduction
Until this point we have been concerned primarily with methods for analyzing single
variables obtained from twin pairs; that is, with estimation of the relevant sources
of genetic and environmental variation in each variable separately. Most studies,
however, are not designed to consider single variables, but are trying to understand
what factors make sets of variables correlate, or co-vary, to a greater or lesser extent.
Just as we can partition variation into its genetic and environmental components,
so too we can try to determine how far the covariation between multiple measures
is due to genetic and environmental factors. This partitioning of covariation is
one of the rst tasks of multivariate genetic analysis, and it is one for which the
classical twin study, with its simple and regular structure, is especially well-suited.
In Chapter 1 we described three of the main issues in the genetic analysis of
multiple variables. These issues include
1. contribution of genes and environment to the correlation between variables
2. direction of causation between observed variables
3. genetic and environmental contributions to developmental change.
Each of these questions presumes either a dierent data collection strategy or a
dierent model or both; for example, analysis of measurements of correlated traits
taken at the same time (question 1) requires somewhat dierent methods than
assessments of the same trait taken longitudinally (question 3). However, all of
the multivariate issues share the requirement of multiple measurements from the
same subjects. In this chapter we direct our attention to the rst issue: genetic and
environmental contributions to observed correlations among variables. We describe
twin methods for the other two questions in Chapters ?? ??.
177
178 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
The treatment of multivariate models presented here is intended to be intro-
ductory. There are many specic topics within the broad domain of multivariate
genetic analysis, some of which we address in subsequent chapters. Here we exclude
treatment of observed and latent variable means and analysis of singleton twins.
10.2 Phenotypic Factor Analysis
Factor analysis is one of the most widely used multivariate methods. The gen-
eral idea is to explain variation within and covariation between a large number of
observed variables with a smaller number of latent factors. Here we give a brief
outline of the method those seeking more thorough treatments are referred to
e.g., Gorsuch (1983), Harman (1976), Lawley and Maxwell (1971). Typically the
free parameters of primary interest in factor models are the factor loadings and
factor correlations. Factor loadings indicate the degree of relationship between a
latent factor and an observed variable, while factor correlations represent the re-
lationships between the hypothesized latent factors. An observed variable that is
a good indicator of a latent factor is said to load highly on that factor. For
example, in intelligence research, where factor theory has its origins (Spearman,
1904), it may be noted that a vocabulary test loads highly on a hypothesized (la-
tent) verbal ability factor, but loads to a much lesser extent on a latent spatial
ability factor; i.e., the vocabulary test relates strongly to verbal ability, but less so
to spatial ability. Normally a factor loading is identical to a path coecient of the
type described in Chapter 5.
In this section we describe factor analytic models and present some illustrative
applications to observed measurements without reference to genetic and environ-
mental causality. We turn to genetic factor models in Section 10.3.
10.2.1 Exploratory and Conrmatory Factor Models
There are two general classes of factor models: exploratory and conrmatory. In
exploratory factor analysis one does not postulate an a priori factor structure;
that is, the number of latent factors, correlations among them, and the factor
loading pattern (the pattern of relative weights of the observed variables on the
latent factors) is calculated from the data in some manner which maximizes the
amount of variance/covariance explained by the latent factors. More formally, in
exploratory factor analysis:
1. There are no hypotheses about factor loadings (all variables load on all fac-
tors, and factor loadings cannot be constrained to be equal to other loadings)
2. There are no hypotheses about interfactor correlations (either all correlations
are zero orthogonal factors, or all may correlate oblique factors)
3. Only one group is analyzed
10.2. PHENOTYPIC FACTOR ANALYSIS 179
4. Unique factors (those that relate only to one variable) are uncorrelated,
5. All observed variables need to have specic variances.
These models often are tted using a statistical package such as SPSS or SAS,
in which one may explore the relationships among observed variables in a latent
variable framework.
In contrast, conrmatory factor analysis requires one to formulate a hypothesis
about the number of latent factors, the relationships between the observed and
latent factors (the factor pattern), and the correlations among the factors. Thus,
a possible model of the data is formulated in advance as a factor structure, and
the factor loadings and correlations are estimated from the data
1
. As usual, this
model-tting process allows one to test the ability of the hypothesized factor struc-
ture to account for the observed covariances by examining the overall t of the
model. Typically the model involves certain constraints, such as equalities among
certain factor loadings or equalities of some of the factor correlations. If the model
fails then we may relax certain constraints or add more factors, test for signicant
improvement in t using the chi-squared dierence test, and examine the overall
goodness of t to see if the new model adequately accounts for the observed covari-
ation. Likewise, some or all of the correlations between latent factors may be set
to zero or estimated. Then we can test if these constraints are consistent with the
data. Conrmatory factor models are the type we are concerned with using Mx.
10.2.2 Building a Phenotypic Factor Model Mx Script
The factor model may be written as
Y
ij
= b
i
X
j
+E
ij
with
i = 1, , p (variables)
j = 1, , n (subjects)
and where the measured variables Y are a function of a subjects value on the
underlying factor X (henceforth the j subscript indicating subjects in Y will be
omitted). These subject values are called factor scores. Although the use of factor
scores is always implicit in the application of factor analysis, they cannot be deter-
mined precisely but must be estimated, since the number of common and unique
factors always exceeds the number of observed variables. In addition, there is a
specic part (E) to each variable. The bs are the pvariate factor loadings of mea-
sured variables on the latent factors. To estimate these loadings we do not need
1
In exploratory factor analysis the term factor structure is used to describe the correlations
between variables and factors, but in conrmatory analysis, as described here, the term often
describes the characteristics of a hypothesized factor model.
180 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
to know the individual factor scores, as the expectation for the p p covariance
matrix (
Y,Y
) consists only of a p m matrix of factor loadings B (m equals the
number of latent factors), a m m correlation matrix of factor scores P, and a
p p diagonal matrix of specic variances E :

Y,Y
= BPB

+ E. (10.1)
In problems with uncorrelated latent factors, P is an identity matrix, so equa-
tion 10.1 reduces to

Y,Y
= BB

+E. (10.2)
Thus, the parameters in the model consist of factor loadings and specic variances
(sometimes also referred to as error variances).
10.2.3 Fitting a Phenotypic Factor Model
Martin et al. (1985) obtained data on arithmetic computation from male and female
twins who were measured once before and three times after drinking a standard
dose of alcohol. To illustrate the use of a conrmatory factor analysis model in Mx,
we analyze data from MZ females (rst born twin only). The observed variances
and correlations are shown in Table 10.1. The conrmatory model is one in which
a single latent factor is hypothesized to account for all the covariances among the
four variables. The Mx script in Appendix E.1 shows the model specications and
the 4 4 input matrix.
Table 10.1: Observed correlations (with variances on the diagonal) for arithmetic
computation variables from female MZ twins before (time 0) and after (times 1
3) standard doses of alcohol.
Time 0 Time 1 Time 2 Time 3
Time 0 259.66
Time 1 .81 259.94
Time 2 .83 .87 245.24
Time 3 .87 .87 .90 249.30
The parameters in the group type statement indicate that we have NObservations=42
subjects) and NInput_vars=4 input variables. The loadings of the four variables
on the single common factor are estimated in matrix B and their specic variances
are estimated on the diagonal of matrix E. In this phenotypic factor model, we
have sucient information to estimate factor loadings and specic variances for
the four variables, but we cannot simultaneously estimate the variance of the com-
mon factor because the model would then be underidentied. We therefore x the
10.3. SIMPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 181
variance of the latent factor to an arbitrary non-zero constant, which we choose to
be unity in order to keep the factor loadings and specic variances in the original
scale of measurement (Value 1 P 1 1).
The Mx output (after editing) from this common factor model is shown below.
The PARAMETER SPECIFICATIONS section illustrates the assignment of parameter
numbers to matrices declared Free in the matrices declaration section. Consecutive
parameter numbers are given to free elements in matrices in the order in which they
appear. It is always advisable to check the parameter specications for the correct
assignment of free and constrained parameters. The output depicts the single
common factor structure of the model: there are free factor loadings for each of
the four variables on the common factor, and specic variance parameters for each
of the observed variables. Thus, the model has a total of 8 parameters to explain
the 4(4 + 1)/2 = 10 free statistics.
The results - from the MX PARAMETER ESTIMATES section of the Mx output -
are summarized in Table 10.2.3. The chi-squared goodness-of-t value of 1.46 for
2 degrees of freedom suggests that this single factor model adequately explains
the observed covariances (p = .483). This also may be seen by comparing the
elements of the tted covariance matrix and the observed covariance matrix, which
are seen to be very similar. The tted covariance matrix is printed by Mx when
the RSiduals option is added. The tted covariance matrix is calculated by Mx
using expression 10.2 with the nal estimated parameter values.
Mx Output from Phenotypic Factor Model
----------------------------------------------------------
PARAMETER SPECIFICATIONS
MATRIX B
1
TIME1 1
TIME2 2
TIME3 3
TIME4 4
MATRIX E
TIME1 TIME2 TIME3 TIME4
1 5 6 7 8
10.3 Simple Genetic Factor Models
The factor analytic approach outlined above can be readily applied to multivari-
ate genetic problems. This was rst suggested by Martin and Eaves (1977) for
the analysis of twin data (although in their original publication they use matrices
182 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 10.2: Parameter estimates and expected covariance matrix from the phe-
notypic factor model
B E Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 4
Time 1 14.431 51.422 Time 1 259.670
Time 2 14.745 42.509 Time 2 212.784 259.927
Time 3 14.699 29.174 Time 3 212.115 216.736 245.229
Time 4 15.119 20.709 Time 4 218.181 222.933 222.233 249.297

2
= 0.46, 2 df, p=.483
of mean squares and cross-products between and within twin pairs). As in the
phenotypic example above, a single common factor is proposed to account for cor-
relations among the variables, but now one such factor is hypothesized for each of
the components of variation, genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared envi-
ronmental. Data from genetically related individuals are used to estimate loadings
of variables on common genetic and environmental factors, so that variances and
covariances may be explained in terms of these factors.
10.3.1 Multivariate Genetic Factor Model
Using genetic notation, the genetic factor model can be represented as
P
ij
= a
i
A
j
+c
i
C
j
+e
i
E
j
+U
ij
with
i = 1, , p (variables)
j = 1, , n (subjects)
The measured phenotype (P) (again, omitting the j subscript) consists of multiple
variables that are a function of a subjects underlying additive genetic deviate (A),
common (between-families) environment (C), and non-shared (within-families) en-
vironment (E). In addition, each variable P
j
has a specic component U
j
that
itself may consist of a genetic and a non-genetic part. In this initial application, we
assume that U
j
is entirely random environmental in origin, an assumption we relax
later. Parameters a, c, and e are the pvariate factor loadings of measured variables
on the latent factors. A path diagram of this model is shown in Figure 10.3.1.
In Mx, there are a number of alternative ways to specify the model. One
approach is to specify the factor structure for the genetic, shared and specic envi-
ronmental factors in one matrix, e.g. Bwith twice the number of variables (for both
twins) as rows and the number of factors for each twin as columns. If we assume one
10.3. SIMPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 183
T1
P2
C
A
C
C
C
E
C
A
C
C
C
E
T1
P1
T1
P3
T1
P4
T2
P2
T2
P1
T2
P3
T2
P4
1
R
2
R
3
R
4
R
1
R
2
R
3
R
4
R
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Figure 10.1: Multivariate Genetic Factor model for four variables. All labels for
path-coecients have been omitted.
genetic, one shared environmental and one specic environmental common factor
per twin (A
1
, A
2
, C
1
, C
2
, E
1
, E
2
) for our four-variate arithmetic computation ex-
ample (shown as T0 T3 to represent administration times 03 before and after
standard doses of alcohol for twin 1 (Tw1) and twin 2 (Tw2) respectively), the B
matrix would look like
A
1
C
1
E
1
A
2
C
2
E
2
Tw1-T0 1 5 9 0 0 0
Tw1-T1 2 6 10 0 0 0
Tw1-T2 3 7 11 0 0 0
Tw1-T3 4 8 12 0 0 0
Tw2-T0 0 0 0 1 5 9
Tw2-T1 0 0 0 2 6 10
Tw2-T2 0 0 0 3 7 11
Tw2-T3 0 0 0 4 8 12
In this case with m = 6 factors and four observed variables for each twin (p = 8),
B would be a p m (8 6) matrix of the factor loadings, P the mm correlation
matrix of factor scores, and E a p p diagonal matrix of unique variances. The
184 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
expected covariance may then be calculated as in equation 10.1:

Y,Y
= BPB

+ E. (10.3)
In a multivariate analysis of twin data according to this factor model, is
a 2p 2p predicted covariance matrix of observations on twin 1 and twin 2 and
B is a 2p 2m matrix of loadings of these observations on latent genotypes and
non-shared and common environments of twin 1 and twin 2. The factor loadings
between A
1
and A
2
, E
1
and E
2
, and C
1
and C
2
are constrained to be equal for twin
1 and twin 2, similar to the path coecients of the univariate models discussed in
previous chapters. The equality constraints on the parameters are obtained in Mx
by using the same non-zero parameter number in a Specification statement for
the free parameters. The unique variances also are equal for both members of a
twin pair. These may be estimated on the diagonal of the 2p 2p E matrix (e.g.,
Heath et al., 1989c). To t this model, B and E are estimated from the data and P
(2m2m) must be xed a priori (for example, the correlation between A
1
for twin
1 and A
2
for twin 2 is 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ twins; the correlation between
the C variables of twin 1 and twin 2 is 1.0).
One alternative specication of this model is to include the unique variances
in matrix B and x E to zero. The factor patterns for A and E of twin 1 and
twin 2 are identical to that in Section 10.2.3. The main dierence lies in the
treatment of the unique variances. In the earlier example these were estimated as
variances on the diagonal of E, but now they are modeled as the square roots of the
variances. These quantities are now square roots because the unique variances are
calculated as the product BPB

in the expected covariance expression whereas in


the previous example the quantities were estimated as the unproducted quantity
E. One might expect that this subtle change would have no eect on the model
(as indeed it does not in this example), but on occasion these alternative residual
specications may produce dierent outcomes. The situation of residual variances
< 0.0 makes little sense in genetic analyses because it implies an impossible negative
variance component. Consequently, although it may be possible to make alternative
representations like this in Mx, we recommend this model, as it constrains unique
variances to be 0.0. Nevertheless, both methods give identical solutions when
tted to the data used in these examples.
10.3.2 Alternate Representation of Genetic Factor Model
One of the features of Mx is its exibility for specifying the same or very similar
models in dierent ways. Frequently the choice of model specication is simply
a matter of individual preference, convenience, or familiarity with Mx notation,
particularly when a model can be written in several dierent ways with no change in
the substantive or numerical outcome. However, at other times very subtle changes
in the Mx formulation of a model translate into a completely dierent substantive
10.3. SIMPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 185
question. While it may be true that exibility imparts confusion, it is important
to recognize and distinguish alternative representations of genetic models in Mx.
While the approach discussed above may be fairly intuitive, the B matrix may
become relatively big, therefore increasing the chance of errors in editing. An alter-
native approach is to specify the common factors and residual variances for genetic,
shared and specic environmental factors in separate matrices. One advantage of
this approach is that the model can be easily adapted for a dierent number of
common factors or observed variables. For example, if we use a 4 1 matrix X for
the genetic common factor, a 41 matrix Y for the shared environmental common
factor, a 4 1 matrix Z for the specic environmental common factor and a 4 4
diagonal matrix F for the unique variances, the matrices section in Mx would be
X Full 4 1 Free ! genetic common factor
Y Full 4 1 Free ! shared environmental common factor
Z Full 4 1 Free ! specific environmental common factor
F Diag 4 4 Free ! specific environmental unique variances
We can then pre-calculate the genetic, shared and specic environmental variance
components in the algebra section:
A= X*X;
C= Y*Y;
E= Z*Z +F*F;
and these matrices can be used to specify the expected covariance matrices for MZ
and DZ twins in a similar fashion as the univariate models. Note that by using
a Kronecker product for the genetic variance component in DZ twins (H@A) every
element of the A matrix is multiplied by one half. One additional feature in Mx
that allows for exible model specication is the #define statement. One possible
use is to dene the number of variables up front, e.g.
#define nvar 4
and use the dened variables in the matrices section:
X Full nvar 1 Free ! genetic common factor
Y Full nvar 1 Free ! shared environmental common factor
Z Full nvar 1 Free ! specific environmental common factor
F Diag nvar nvar Free ! specific environmental unique variances
If we wanted to do an analysis with just three variables, the only change to be
made, besides the NInput_vars and Select statements, is the #define statement.
10.3.3 Fitting the Multivariate Genetic Model
To illustrate the genetic common factor model we t it to the arithmetic compu-
tation data, but now using both members of the female twin pairs and specifying
186 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
two groups for the MZ and DZ twins. The observed variances and correlations
examined in this analysis are presented in Table 10.3. Appendix E.2 shows the full
Mx script for this model.
Table 10.3: Observed female MZ (above diagonal) and DZ (below diagonal) cor-
relations and variances for arithmetic computation variables.
Twin 1 Twin 2
T0 T1 T2 T3 T0 T1 T2 T3
T1 T0 1.0 .81 .83 .87 .78 .65 .71 .68
T1 .89 1.0 .87 .87 .74 .74 .74 .71
T2 .85 .90 1.0 .90 .73 .66 .72 .70
T3 .83 .86 .86 1.0 .74 .71 .74 .75
T2 T0 .23 .31 .36 .34 1.0 .73 .78 .79
T1 .22 .32 .34 .38 .81 1.0 .86 .87
T2 .16 .23 .27 .35 .79 .86 1.0 .87
T3 .23 .31 .34 .37 .81 .86 .87 1.0
MZ 297.9 229.4 247.4 274.9 281.9 359.7 326.9 281.1
DZ 259.7 259.9 245.2 249.3 283.8 249.5 262.1 270.9
The results from this common factor model are shown in Table 10.3.3. The
parameter estimates indicate a substantial genetic basis for the observed arithmetic
covariances, as the genetic loadings are much higher than either the shared and non-
shared environmental eects. The unique variances in F also appear substantial but
these do not contribute to covariances among the measures, only to the variance
of each observed variable. The
2
56
value of 46.77 suggests that this single factor
model provides a reasonable explanation of the data. (Note that the 56 degrees
of freedom are obtained from 2 8(8 + 1)/2 free statistics minus 16 estimated
parameters).
Table 10.4: Parameter estimates from the full genetic common factor model
A
C
C
C
E
C
E
S
Time 1 15.088 1.189 4.142 46.208
Time 2 13.416 5.119 6.250 39.171
Time 3 13.293 4.546 7.146 31.522
Time 4 13.553 5.230 5.765 34.684

2
= 46.77, 56 df, p=.806
Earlier in this chapter we alluded to the fact that conrmatory factor models
10.3. SIMPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 187
allow one to statistically test the signicance of model parameters. We can perform
such a test on the present multivariate genetic model. The Mx output above
shows that the shared environment factor loadings are much smaller than either
the genetic or non-shared environment loadings. We can test whether these loadings
are signicantly dierent from zero by modifying slightly the Mx script to x these
parameters and then re-estimating the other model parameters. There are several
possible ways in which one might modify the script to accomplish this task, but
one of the easiest methods is simply to change the Y to have no free elements.
Performing this modication in the rst group eectively drops all C loadings
from all groups because the Begin Matrices= Group 1; statement in the second
and third group equates its loadings to those in the rst. Thus, the modied
script represents a model in which common factors are hypothesized for genetic and
non-shared environmental eects to account for covariances among the observed
variables, and unique eects are allowed to contribute to measurement variances.
All shared environmental eects are omitted from the model.
Since the modied multivariate model is a sub- or nested model of the full
common factor specication, comparison of the goodness-of-t chi-squared values
provides a test of the signicance of the deleted C factor loadings. The full model
has 56 degrees of freedom and the reduced one: 2 8(8 + 1)/2 12 = 60 d.f.
Thus, the dierence chi-squared statistic for the test of C loadings has 60 56 = 4
degrees of freedom. As may be seen in the table below, the
2
60
of the reduced
model is 51.08, and, therefore, the dierence
2
4
is 51.08 46.77 = 4.31, which
is non-signicant at the .05 level. This non-signicant chi-squared indicates that
the shared environmental loadings can be dropped from the multivariate genetic
model without signicant loss of t; that is, the arithmetic data are not inuenced
by environmental eects shared by twins. Parameter estimates from this reduced
model are given below in Table 10.3.3.
Table 10.5: Parameter estimates from the reduced genetic common factor model
A
C
C
C
E
C
E
S
Time 1 14.756 3.559 59.502
Time 2 14.274 6.331 39.433
Time 3 14.081 7.047 30.843
Time 4 14.405 5.845 36.057

2
= 51.08, 60 df, p=.787
The estimates for the genetic and non-shared environment parameters dier
somewhat between the reduced model and those estimated in the full common
factor model. Such dierences often appear when tting nested models, and are
not necessarily indicative of misspecication (of course, one would not expect the
188 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
estimates to change in the case where parameters to be omitted are estimated
as 0.0 in the full model). The tting functions used in Mx (see Chapter ??) are
designed to produce parameter estimates that yield the closest match between the
observed and estimated covariance matrices. Omission of selected parameters, for
example, the C loadings in the present model, generates a dierent model and
thus may be expected to yield slightly dierent parameter estimates in order to
best approximate the observed matrix.
10.3.4 Fitting a Second Genetic Factor
The genetic common factor model we introduced in Sections 10.3.3 and 10.3.2 may
be extended to address more specic questions about the data. In the arithmetic
computation measures, for example, it is reasonable to hypothesize two genetic
factors: one general factor contributing to all measurements of arithmetic compu-
tation, and a second alcohol factor which inuences the measures taken after
the challenge dose of alcohol. The most parsimonious extension of our common
factor model may involve the addition of only 1 free parameter which represents
each of the factor loadings on the alcohol factor (that is, the alcohol loadings may
be equated for all alcohol measurements).
The Mx script corresponds very closely to that used in section 10.3.2, using the
X for the genetic common factors We add the latent alcohol factors for twins 1 and
2 as a second column with the following specication statement:
Specify X
1 0
2 100
3 100
4 100
We use a high number for the loading on the second factor to avoid overlap with
pre-assigned parameter numbers by using the Free keyword. The addition of the
single parameter for all alcohol loadings reects a model having 13 parameters and
28(8+1)/213 = 59 degrees of freedom. We can, therefore, test the signicance
of the alcohol factor by comparing the goodness-of-t chi-squared value for this
model with that obtained from the model of Section 10.3.2 for a 60 59 = 1 d.f.
test. Table 10.3.4 shows the results of the two-factor multivariate genetic model.
The estimated genetic factor loading for the alcohol variables (4.27) is reason-
ably large, but much smaller than the loadings on the general genetic factor. This
dierence is more apparent when we consider proportions of genetic variance ac-
counted for by these two factors, being 4.27
2
/(13.70
2
+4.27) or 9% for the alcohol
factor, and 100 9 = 91% for the general genetic factor. The model yields a

2
59
= 47.52 (p = .86), indicating a good t to the data. The chi-squared test for
the signicance of the alcohol factor loadings is 51.08 47.52 = 3.56, which is not
quite signicant at the .05 level. Thus, while the hypothesis of there being genetic
10.4. MULTIPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 189
Table 10.6: Parameter estimates from the two genetic factors model
A
C
1 A
C
2 E
C
E
S
Time 1 15.067 0.000 4.408 6.674
Time 2 13.701 4.270 6.091 6.277
Time 3 13.518 4.270 6.800 5.644
Time 4 13.832 4.270 5.695 5.928

2
= 47.52, 59 df, p=.858
eects on the alcohol measures additional to those inuencing arithmetic skills ts
the observed data better, the increase in t obtained by adding the alcohol factor
does not reach statistical signicance.
10.4 Multiple Genetic Factor Models
10.4.1 Genetic and Environmental Correlations
We now turn from the one- and two-factor multivariate genetic models described
above and consider more general multivariate formulations which may encompass
many genetic and environmental factors. These more general approaches subsume
the simpler techniques described above.
Consider a simple extension of the one- and two-factor AE models for multiple
variables (Sections 10.3.210.3.4). The total phenotypic covariance matrix in a
population, C
p
, can be decomposed into an additive genetic component, A, and a
random environmental component, E:
C
p
= A+E , (10.4)
We are leaving out the shared environment in this example just for simplicity. More
complex expectations for 10.4 may be written without aecting the basic idea. A
is called the additive genetic covariance matrix and E the random environmental
covariance matrix. If A is diagonal, then the traits comprising A are genetically
independent; that is, there is no additive genetic covariance between them. One
interpretation of this is that dierent genes aect each of the traits. Similarly, if
the environmental covariance matrix, E, is diagonal, we would conclude that each
trait is aected by quite dierent environmental factors.
On the other hand, suppose A were to have signicant o-diagonal elements.
What would that mean? Although there are many reasons why this might happen,
one possibility is that at least some genes are having eects on more than one vari-
able. This is known as pleiotropy in the classical genetic literature (see Chapter 3).
190 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Similarly, signicant o-diagonal elements in E (or C, if it were included in the
model) would indicate that some environmental factors inuence more than one
trait at a time.
The extent to which the same genes or environmental factors contribute to the
observed phenotypic correlation between two variables is often measured by the
genetic or environmental correlation between the variables. If we have estimates of
the genetic and environmental covariance matrices, Aand E, the genetic correlation
(r
g
) between variables i and j is
r
gij
=
a
ij
_
(a
ii
a
jj
)
(10.5)
and the environmental correlation, similarly, is
r
eij
=
e
ij
_
(e
ii
e
jj
)
. (10.6)
The analogy with the familiar formula for the correlation coecient is clear.
The genetic covariance between two phenotypes is quite distinct from the genetic
correlation. It is possible for two traits to have a very high genetic correlation yet
have little genetic covariance. Low genetic covariance could arise if either trait had
low genetic variance. Vogler (1982) and Carey (1988) discuss these issues in greater
depth.
10.4.2 Cholesky Decomposition
Clearly, we cannot resolve the genetic and environmental components of covariance
without genetically informative data such as those from twins. Under our simple
AE model we can write, for MZ and DZ pairs, the expected covariances between
the multiple measures of rst and second members very simply:
C
MZ
= A
C
DZ
= A
with the total phenotypic covariance matrix being dened as in expression 10.4.
The coecient in DZ twins is the familiar additive genetic correlation between
siblings in randomly mating populations (i.e., 0.5).
The method of maximum likelihood, implemented in Mx, can be used to es-
timate A and E. However, there is an important restriction on the form of these
matrices which follows from the fact that they are covariance matrices: they must
be positive denite. It turns out that if we try to estimate A and E without im-
posing this constraint they will very often not be positive denite and thus give
nonsense values (greater than or less than unity) for the genetic and environmental
correlations. It is very simple to impose this constraint in Mx by recognizing that
10.4. MULTIPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 191
any positive denite matrix, F, can be decomposed into the product of a triangular
matrix and its transpose:
F = TT

, (10.7)
where T is a triangular matrix (i.e., one having xed zeros in all elements above the
diagonal and free parameters on the diagonal and below). This is sometimes known
as a triangular decomposition or a Cholesky factorization of F. Figure 10.2 shows
this type of model as a path diagram for four variables. In our case, we represent the
T1
P2
1
A
4
A
1
A
4
A
T1
P1
T1
P3
T1
P4
T2
P2
T2
P1
T2
P3
T2
P4
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
2
A
3
A
2
A
3
A
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5
Figure 10.2: Phenotypic Cholesky decomposition model for four variables. All
labels for path-coecients have been omitted.
genetic and environmental covariance matrices in Mx by their respective Cholesky
factorizations:
A = XX

(10.8)
and
E = ZZ

, (10.9)
where X and Z are triangular matrices of additive genetic and within-family envi-
ronment factor loadings.
A triangular matrix such as T, X, or Z is square, having the same number of
rows and columns as there are variables. The rst column has non-zero entries in
192 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
every element; the second has a zero in the rst element and free, non-zero elements
everywhere else, and so on. Thus, the Cholesky factors of F, when F is a 3 3
matrix of the product TT

, will have the form:


T =
_
_
b
11
0 0
b
21
b
22
0
b
31
b
32
b
33
_
_
.
It is important to recognize that common factor models such as the one described
in Section 10.3 are simply reduced Cholesky models with the rst column of pa-
rameters estimated and all others xed at zero.
10.4.3 Analyzing Genetic and Environmental Correlations
We illustrate the estimation of the genetic and environmental covariance matri-
ces for a simple case of skinfold measures made on 11 year-old male twins from
the Medical College of Virginia Twin Study (Schieken et al., 1989)
2
. Our skinfold
assessments include four dierent measures which were obtained using standard
anthropometric techniques. The measures were obtained for biceps (BIC), sub-
scapular (SSC), suprailiac (SUP), and triceps (TRI) skinfolds. The raw data were
averaged for the left and right sides and subjected to a logarithmic transformation
prior to analysis in order to remove the correlation between error variance and
skinfold measure. The 8 8 covariance matrices for the male MZ and DZ twins
are given in Table 10.7.
An example Mx program for estimating the Cholesky factors of the addi-
tive genetic and within-family environmental covariance matrices is given in Ap-
pendix E.4. The matrices X and Z are now declared as free lower triangular
matrices.
When this program is run with the data from male twins, we obtain a goodness-
of-t chi-squared of 68.92 for 52 d.f. (p = .058) suggesting that the AE model gives a
reasonable t to these data. Setting the o-diagonal elements of the genetic factors
to zero yields a chi-squared that may be compared using the dierence test to see
whether the measures can be regarded as genetically independent. This chi-squared
turns out to be 110.96 for 6 d.f. which is highly signicant. Therefore, the genetic
correlations between these skinfold measures cannot be ignored. Similarly, setting
the environmental covariances to zero yields a signicant increase in chi-squared
of 356.98, also for 6 d.f. Clearly, there are also highly signicant environmental
covariances among the four variables.
Table 10.8 gives the estimates of the Cholesky factors of the genetic and en-
vironmental covariance matrices produced by Mx. Carrying out the pre- and
post-multiplication of the Cholesky factors (see equations 10.8 and 10.9) gives the
2
We are grateful to Dr. Richard Schieken for making these data, gathered as part of a project
supported by NHLBI award HL-31010, available prior to publication.
10.4. MULTIPLE GENETIC FACTOR MODELS 193
Table 10.7: Covariance matrices for skinfold measures in adolescent Virginian
male twins.
Dizygotic Male Pairs (N=33)
BIC1 SSC1 SUP1 TRI1 BIC2 SSC2 SUP2 TRI2
BIC1 .154
SSC1 .199 .301
SUP1 .227 .330 .380
TRI1 .129 .174 .201 .127
BIC2 .044 .034 .035 .038 .178
SSC2 .065 .082 .074 .054 .210 .308
SUP2 .081 .090 .097 .067 .233 .324 .390
TRI2 .043 .039 .038 .037 .144 .184 .211 .142
Monozygotic Male Pairs (N=84)
BIC1 SSC1 SUP1 TRI1 BIC2 SSC2 SUP2 TRI2
BIC1 .129
SSC1 .127 .176
SUP1 .170 .216 .303
TRI1 .104 .110 .147 .104
BIC2 .098 .107 .149 .082 .123
SSC2 .010 .141 .185 .088 .130 .189
SUP2 .126 .165 .242 .110 .162 .219 .284
TRI2 .084 .091 .134 .084 .101 .113 .144 .107
Variable Labels: BIC=Biceps; SSC=Subscapular; SUP=Suprailiac;
TRI=Triceps. 1 and 2 refer to measures on rst and second twins
maximum-likelihood estimates of the genetic and environmental covariance matri-
ces, which we present in the upper part of Table 10.9. The lower part of Table 10.9
gives the matrices of genetic and environmental correlations derived from these
covariances (see 10.5 and 10.6).
We see that the genetic correlations between the four skinfold measures are
indeed very large, suggesting that the amount of fat at dierent sites of the body
is almost entirely under the control of the same genetic factors. However, in this
example, the environmental correlations also are quite large, suggesting that envi-
ronmental factors which aect the amount of fat at one site also have a generalized
eect over all sites.
194 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 10.8: Parameter estimates of the cholesky factors in the genetic and envi-
ronmental covariance matrices.
Genetic Factor Environmental Factor
Variable A1 A2 A3 A4 E1 E2 E3 E4
BIC 0.340 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.170 0.000 0.000 0.000
SSC 0.396 0.182 0.000 0.000 0.160 0.138 0.000 0.000
SUP 0.487 0.159 0.148 0.000 0.180 0.117 0.093 0.000
TRI 0.288 0.016 0.036 0.110 0.117 0.039 -0.004 0.085
Table 10.9: Maximum-likelihood estimates of genetic and environmental covari-
ance (above the diagnoals) and correlation (below the diagonals) matrices for skin-
fold measures.
Genetic Environmental
Variable BIC SSC SUP TRI BIC SSC SUP TRI
BIC 0.116 0.135 0.166 0.098 0.029 0.027 0.030 0.020
SSC 0.909 0.190 0.222 0.117 0.759 0.044 0.045 0.024
SUP 0.914 0.955 0.284 0.148 0.769 0.908 0.054 0.025
TRI 0.927 0.863 0.894 0.097 0.778 0.757 0.716 0.023
Note: The variances are given on the diagonals of the two matrices
10.5 Common vs. Independent Pathway Models
As another example of multivariate analysis we consider four atopic symptoms
reported by female twins in a mailed questionnaire study (Duy et al., 1990;
1992). Twins reported whether they had ever (versus never) suered from asthma,
hayfever, dust allergy and eczema. Tetrachoric correlation matrices were calculated
with PRELIS and are shown in the Mx script in Appendix E.5 and in Table 10.10.
Tetrachoric or polychoric matrices and their corresponding asymptotic covariance
matrices are read in with the PMatrix and ACov statements. The script shows
that asymptotic covariance matrices are stored in les named ahdemzf.acv and
ahdedzf.acv respectively for MZ and DZ twins. Reading polychoric matrices ags
Mx that the weighted least squares (WLS) t function is required, rather than max-
imum likelihood. Maximum-likelihood estimation is not appropriate when there are
glaring departures from normality; the dichotomous items used in this example are
inevitably non-normal.
10.5. COMMON VS. INDEPENDENT PATHWAY MODELS 195
Table 10.10: Tetrachoric correlations for female MZ (above diagonal) and DZ
(below diagonal) twins for asthma (A), hayfever (H), dust allergy (D), and eczema
(E).
Twin 1 Twin 2
A H D E A H D E
Twin 1 Asthma .56 .57 .27 .59 .41 .43 .09
Hayfever .52 .76 .26 .37 .59 .42 .20
Dust Allergy .59 .75 .31 .40 .45 .52 .19
Eczema .29 .31 .28 .23 .15 .19 .59
Twin 2 Asthma .26 .17 .04 .14 .55 .64 .15
Hayfever .13 .32 .26 .09 .40 .77 .12
Dust Allergy .08 .17 .21 .02 .68 .72 .22
Eczema .22 .11 .09 .31 .25 .22 .28
10.5.1 Independent Pathway Model for Atopy
Inspection of the correlation matrices in Table 10.10 reveals that the presence of
any one of the symptoms is associated with an increased risk of the others within
an individual (hence the concept of atopy). All four symptoms show higher MZ
correlations (0.592, 0.593, 0.518, 0.589) than DZ correlations in liability (0.262,
0.318, 0.214, 0.313) and there is a hint of genetic dominance (or epistasis) for
asthma and dust allergy (DZ correlations less than half their MZ counterparts).
Preliminary multivariate analysis suggests that dominance is acting at the level
of a common factor inuencing all symptoms, rather than as specic dominance
contributions to individual symptoms. Our rst model for covariation of these
symptoms is shown in the path diagram of Figure 10.3
Because each of the three common factors (A, D, E) has its own paths to each
of the four variables, this has been called the independent pathway model (Kendler
et al., 1987) or the biometric factors model (McArdle and Goldsmith, 1990). This
is translated into Mx in the Appendix E.5 script. The specication of this example
is very similar to the multivariate genetic factor model described earlier in this
chapter. The three common factors are specied in nvar1 matrices X, W and Z,
where nvar is dened as 4, representing the four atopy measures. The genetic and
environmental specics are estimated in nvarnvar matrices G and F. The genetic,
dominance and specic environmental covariance matrices are then calculated in
the algebra section. The rest of the script is virtually identical to that for the
univariate model.
One important new feature of the model shown in Figure 10.3 is the treatment of
variance specic to each variable. Such residual variance does not generally receive
196 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
T1
P2
C
A
C
D
C
E
C
A
C
D
C
E
T1
P1
T1
P3
T1
P4
T2
P2
T2
P1
T2
P3
T2
P4
1
A
2
A
3
A
4
A
1
A
2
A
3
A
4
A
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.25
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5
Figure 10.3: Independent pathway model for four variables. All labels for path-
coecients have been omitted.
much attention in regular non-genetic factor analysis, for at least two reasons.
First, the primary goal of factor analysis (and of many multivariate methods) is to
understand the covariance between variables in terms of reduced number of factors.
Thus the residual, variable specic, components are not the focus. A second reason
is that with phenotypic factor analysis, there is simply no information to further
decompose the variable specic variance. However, in the case of data on groups of
relatives, we have two parallel goals of understanding not only the within-person
covariance for dierent variables, but also the across-relatives covariance structure
both within and across variables. The genetic and environmental factor structure
at the top of Figure 10.2 addresses the genetic and environmental components of
variance common to the dierent variables. However, there remains information
to discriminate between genetic and environmental components of the residuals,
10.5. COMMON VS. INDEPENDENT PATHWAY MODELS 197
which in essence answers the question of whether family members correlate for the
variable specic portions of variance.
A second important dierence in this example using correlation matrices in
which diagonal variance elements are standardized to one is that the degrees
of freedom available for model testing are dierent from the case of tting to
covariance matrices in which all k(k + 1)/2 elements are available, where k is
the number of input variables. We encountered this dierence in the univariate
case in Section 6.3.1, but it is slightly more complex in multivariate analysis. For
correlation matrices, since the k diagonal elements are xed to one, we apparently
have gk fewer degrees of freedom than if we were tting to covariances, where g is
the number of data groups. However, since for a given variable the sum of squared
estimates always equals unity (within rounding error), it is apparent that not all
the parameters are free, and we may conceptualize the unique environment specic
standard deviations (i.e., the e
i
s) as being obtained as the square roots of one minus
the sum of squares of all the other estimates. Since there are v (number of variables)
such constrained estimates, we actually have v more degrees of freedom than the
above discussion indicates, the correct adjustment to the degrees of freedom when
tting multivariate genetic models to correlation matrices is (g k v). Since in
most applications k = 2v, the adjustment is usually 3v. In our example v = 4 and
the adjustment is indicated by the option DFreedom=-12. (Note that the DFreedom
adjustment applies for the goodness-of-t chi-squared for the whole problem, not
just the adjustment for that group).
Edited highlights of the Mx output are shown below and the goodness-of-t
chi-squared indicates an acceptable t to the data. The adjustment of 12 to
the degrees of freedom which would be available were we working with covariance
matrices (72) leaves 60 statistics. We have to estimate 3 4 factor loadings and
2 4 specic loadings (20 parameters in all), so there are 60 20 = 40 d.f. It is a
wise precaution always to go through this calculation of degrees of freedom not
because Mx is likely to get them wrong, but as a further check that the model has
been specied correctly.
Table 10.11: Parameter estimates from the independent pathway model for atopy
E
C
A
C
D
C
H
S
E
S
Asthma .320 .431 .466 .441 .548
Hayfever .494 .772 .095 .000 .388
Dust Allergy .660 .516 .431 .297 -.159
Eczema .092 .221 .260 .712 .606

2
= 38.44, 40 df, p=.540
We can test variations of the above model by dropping the common factors one
198 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
at a time, or by setting additive genetic specics to zero. This is easily done by
dropping the appropriate elements. Note that xing E specics to zero usually
results in model failure since it generates singular expected covariance matrices
()
3
. Neither does it make biological sense since it is tantamount to saying that a
variable can be measured without error; it is hard to think of a single example of
this in nature! We could also elaborate the model by specifying a third source of
specic variance components, or by substituting shared environment for dominance,
either as a general factor or as specic variance components.
10.5.2 Common Pathway Model for Atopy
In this section we focus on a much more stringent model which hypothesizes that the
covariation between symptoms is determined by a single phenotypic latent variable
called atopy. Atopy itself is determined by additive, dominance and individual
environmental sources of variance. As in the independent pathway model, there are
still specic genetic and environmental eects on each symptom. The path diagram
for this model is shown in Figure 10.4. Because there is now a latent variable
ATOPY which has direct phenotypic paths to each of the symptoms, this has been
called the common pathway model (Kendler et al., 1987) or the psychometric factors
model (McArdle and Goldsmith, 1990).
The Mx script corresponding to this path diagram, given in Appendix E.6,
contains several new features. Again, there are a number of alternative ways to
specify this model in Mx. We use the same approach as in previous models and
specify the genetic and environmental covariance matrices in a calculation group
up front. In this example, matrices X, W and Z represent the single additive and
dominance genetic and specic environmental loadings on the latent phenotype.
The factor loadings on the observed variables are estimated in 4 1 matrix S. The
residual variances are decomposed in genetic and environmental diagonal matrices
G and F. The data groups are identical to those of the independent pathway model.
One nal feature of the model is that since ATOPY is a latent variable whose
scale (and hence variance) is not indexed to any measured variable, we must x
its residual variance term (EATOPY) to unity to make the model identied. This
inevitably means that the estimates for the loadings contributing to ATOPY are
arbitrary and hence so are the paths leading from ATOPY to the symptoms. It is
thus particularly important to standardize the solution so that the total variance
explained for each symptom is unity. The xing of the loading on EATOPY clearly
has implications for the calculation of degrees of freedom, as we shall see below.
The condensed output for this model is presented below, showing the completely
standardized estimates which give unit variance for each variable.
Note that here NInput_vars=8 so there are 56 (2 NI(NI 1)/2) unique corre-
lations. From the above table it appears that 15 parameters have been estimated,
3
This problem is extreme when maximum likelihood is the t function, because the inverse of
is required.
10.5. COMMON VS. INDEPENDENT PATHWAY MODELS 199
T1
P2
C
A
C
D
C
E
C
A
C
D
C
E
T1
P1
T1
P3
T1
P4
T2
P2
T2
P1
T2
P3
T2
P4
1
A
2
A
3
A
4
A
1
A
2
A
3
A
4
A
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
L L
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.25
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5 1.0 / 0.5
1.0 1.0
Figure 10.4: Common pathway model for four variables. All labels for path-
coecients have been omitted.
but in fact EATOPY was xed and the four E specics are obtained by dierence,
so there are only 10 free parameters in the model, hence 46 degrees of freedom.
The latent variable ATOPY has a broad heritability of over 0.6 (1 .610
2
=
.686
2
+.397
2
) of which approximately a quarter is due to dominance, and this factor
has an important phenotypic inuence on all symptoms, particularly dust allergy
(0.941) and hayfever (0.814). There are still sizeable specic genetic inuences not
accounted for by the ATOPY factor on all symptoms except dust allergy (.059
2
).
However, despite the appeal of this model, it does not t as well as the independent
pathway model and the imposition of constraints that covariation between symp-
toms arises purely from their phenotypic relation with the latent variable ATOPY
has worsened t by
2
= 12.93 for 6 degrees of freedom, which is signicant at the
5% level.
200 CHAPTER 10. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS
Table 10.12: Parameter estimates from the common pathway model for atopy
Atopy E
C
A
C
D
C
A
S
E
S
Asthma .671 .531 .517
Hayfever .814 .456 .358
Dust Allergy .941 -.059 .334
Eczema .301 .735 .608
Atopy .686 .397 .610

2
= 51.37, 46 df, p=.238
We conclude that while there are unique environmental, additive, and non-
additive genetic factors which inuence all four symptoms of atopy, these have
dierential eects on the symptoms; the additive and non-additive factors, for ex-
ample, having respectively greater and lesser proportional inuence on hayfever
than the other symptoms. While it is tempting to interpret this as evidence for at
least two genes, or sets of genes, being responsible for the aggregation of symptoms
we call atopy, this is simplistic as in fact such patterns could be consistent with
the action of a single gene or indeed with polygenic eects. For a full discussion
of this important point see Mather and Jinks (1982) and Carey (1988).
Chapter 11
Observer Ratings
11.1 Introduction
Rather than measuring an individuals phenotype directly, we often have to rely on
ratings of the individual made by an observer. An important example is the assess-
ment of children via ratings from parents and teachers. In this chapter we consider
in some detail the assessment of children by their parents. Since the ratings ob-
tained in this case are a function of both parent and child, disentangling the childs
phenotype from that of the rater becomes an important methodological problem.
For the analysis of genetic and environmental contributions to childrens behavior,
solutions to this are available when multiple raters, e.g., two parents, rate multiple
children, e.g., twins. This chapter describes and illustrates simple Mx models for
the analysis of parental ratings of childrens behavior (Section 11.2). We show how
the assumption that mothers and fathers are rating the same behavior in children
can be contrasted with the weaker alternative that parents are rating correlated
behaviors. Given the stronger assumption, which appears adequate for ratings of
some childrens behavior problems, the contribution of rater bias and unreliability
may be separated from the shared and non-shared environmental components of
variation of the true phenotype of the child. The models are illustrated with an
application to CBC data (Section 11.2.5).
11.2 Models for Multiple Rating Data
A primary source of information about a childs behavior is the description of that
behavior by his or her parents. In the study of child and adolescent psychopathology
for example, parental reports are fundamental to the widely used assessment system
developed by Achenbach and Edelbrock (1981). However, dierent informants do
not generally agree in detail about a given childs behavior (Achenbach et al., 1987;
201
202 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
Loeber et al., 1989) and, of course, there are very good reasons why this should
be so (Cox and Rutter, 1985). Dierent informants, such as the child, parents,
teachers or peers, have dierent situational exposure, dierent degrees of insight,
and dierent perceptions, evaluations and normative standards that may create
rater dierences of various kinds in reporting problem behaviors. How we analyze
parental ratings of childrens behavior, and the models we employ in the course of
our analyses, will depend on the assumptions we make. In this chapter we discuss
the application of three classes of models biometric, psychometric, and bias
models.
First, suppose we took an agnostic view of the relationship between the ratings
by dierent informants by thinking of them as assessing dierent phenotypes of
the child. The phenotypes may be correlated but for unspecied reasons. This
view may be appropriate if mothers and fathers reported on behaviors observed
in distinct situations, or if they did not share a common understanding of the
behavioral descriptions. In such a case it would be appropriate to treat the analysis
of mothers and fathers ratings as a standard bivariate genetic and environmental
analysis where the two variables are the mothers ratings and fathers ratings. We
shall refer to the class of standard bivariate factor model as biometric models (see
Chapter 10 for examples).
Second, suppose we made the more restrictive assumption that there is (i)
a common phenotype of the children which is assessed both by mothers and by
fathers, and (ii) a component of each parents ratings which results from an assess-
ment of an independent aspect of the child. Mothers ratings and fathers ratings
would correlate because they are indeed making assessments based on shared ob-
servations and have a shared understanding of the behavioral descriptions used in
the assessments. In this case, we approach the analysis of parental ratings through
a special form of model for bivariate data which we will refer to as psychometric
models (see Chapter 10 for examples).
Third, we consider a model of rater bias. Bias in this context is considered
to be the tendency of an individual rater to overestimate or underestimate scores
consistently. This tendency is a deviation from the mean of all possible raters
in the rater group; no reference is made here to any external criterion such as a
clinicians judgement. Neale and Stevenson (1989) considered the general problem
of rater bias and the particular issues of parental biases in ratings of children. They
presented a model in which the rating of a childs phenotype is considered to be a
function both of the childs phenotype and of the bias introduced by the rater. In
this way it is possible, when two parents rate each of their twin children, to conduct
a behavior genetic analysis of the variation in the latent phenotype while allowing
for variation due to rating biases. If the rater bias model adopted by Neale and
Stevenson (1989) provides an adequate account of the ratings of children by their
parents, it becomes possible to partition the variance in these parental ratings
into their components due to reliable trait variance, due to parental bias , and
due to unreliability or error in the particular rating of a particular child. The
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 203
reliable trait variance can then be decomposed into its components due to genetic
inuences, shared environments, and individual environments. Since rater bias
models represent restricted special cases for the parental ratings of more general
biometric and psychometric models of the kind discussed by Heath et al., (1989) and
McArdle and Goldsmith (1990) and in Chapter 10 of this volume, it is possible to
compare the adequacy of bias models with the alternative bivariate psychometric
and biometric models. Further, comparison of the biometric and psychometric
models indicates how reasonable it is to assume that two raters are assessing the
same phenotype in a child. As we move from the biometric to the psychometric to
the bias models, our assumptions become more restrictive but, if appropriate, our
analyses become more directly informative psychologically. Here we outline how
an analysis of parental ratings using the bias model can be implemented simply
using Mx. We discuss the properties of the alternative models and illustrate their
application with data from a twin study of child and adolescent behavior problems.
11.2.1 Rater Bias Model
Figure 11.1 shows a path model for the ratings of twins by their parents, in which
the phenotypes of a pair of twins (PT
1
and PT
2
) are functions of additive genetic
inuence (A), shared environments (C) and non-shared environments (E). The
ratings by the mother (MoT) and father (FaT) are functions of the twins phe-
notype, the maternal (B
M
) or paternal (B
F
) rater bias, and residual errors (R
M
,
etc).
If this model is correct, the following discriminations may be made:
1. the structural analysis of the latent phenotypes of the children can be con-
sidered independently of the rater biases and unreliability of the ratings;
2. the extent of rater biases and unreliability of ratings can be estimated;
3. the relative accuracy of maternal and paternal ratings can be assessed.
A simple implementation of the model in Mx is achieved by dening the model by
the following matrix equations:
_
_
_
_
MoT
1
MoT
2
FaT
1
FaT
2
_
_
_
_
=
_
_
_
_
b
m
0
b
m
0
0 b
f
0 b
f
_
_
_
_
_
B
M
B
F
_
+
_
_
_
_
1 0
0 1
0
0
_
_
_
_
_
PT
1
PT
2
_
+
_
_
_
_
r
m
0 0 0
0 r
m
0 0
0 0 r
f
0
0 0 0 r
f
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
R
M
R
M
R
F
R
F
_
_
_
_
(11.1)
204 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
A C E A C E
T1
Mo
T1
Fa
T2
Mo
T2
Fa
M
R
F
R
T1
P
T2
P
F
R
M
R
M
B
F
B
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Figure 11.1: Model for ratings of a pair of twins (1 and 2) by their parents.
Maternal and paternal observed ratings (MoT and FaT) are linear functions of
the true phenotypes of the twins (PT), maternal and paternal rater bias (B
M
and
B
F
), and residual error (R
M
and R
F
).
or
y = Bb + Ll +Rr
and
_
PT
1
PT
2
_
=
_
a c e 0 0 0
0 0 0 a c e
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
A
1
C
1
E
1
A
2
C
2
E
2
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
_
(11.2)
(11.3)
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 205
or
l = Gx
Thus
y = Bb +LGx +Rr
Then, the covariance matrix of the ratings is given by
E{yy

} = E{Bb +LGx +Rr}{Bb +LGx +Rr}

(11.4)
= BB

+RR

+LGE{xx

}G

(11.5)
The termGE{xx

}G

generates the usual expectations for the ACE model. The


expectations are ltered to the observed ratings through the factor structure L and
are augmented by the contributions from rater bias (B) and residual inuences (R).
An Mx script for this model is listed in Appendix F.1. In considering the rater
bias model, and the other models discussed below, we should note that parameters
need not be constrained to be equal when rating boys and girls and, as Neale and
Stevenson (1988) pointed out, we need not necessarily assume that parental biases
are equal for MZ and DZ twins ratings. This latter relaxation of the parameter
constraints allows us to consider the possibility that twin correlations dier across
zygosities for reasons related to dierential parental biases based on beliefs about
their twins zygosity.
11.2.2 Psychometric Model
Figure 11.2 shows a bivariate psychometric or common pathway model. Imple-
mentation of this model in Mx can be achieved by the approaches illustrated in
Chapter 10. The psychometric model estimates, for each source of inuence (A, C,
and E) the variance for mothers ratings, the variance for fathers ratings and the
covariance between these ratings. These estimates are subject to the constraints
that the covariances are positive and neither individual rating variance can be less
than the covariance between the ratings. The psychological implication of this
psychometric model is that the mothers and fathers ratings are composed of con-
sistent assessments of reliable trait variance, together with assessments of specic
phenotypes uncorrelated between the parents.
There are some technical points to note with this model. First, bivariate data
for MZ and DZ twins (of a given sex) yield 20 observed variances and covariances.
However, only 9 of these have unique expectations under the classes of model we are
considering, the remaining 11 being replicate estimates of particular expectations
(e.g., the variance of maternal ratings of MZ twin 1, of MZ twin 2, of DZ twin 1
and of DZ twin 2 are four replicate estimates of the variance of maternal ratings in
the population). Given this, we might expect our 9 parameter psychometric model
to t as well as any other 9 parameter model for bivariate twin data. However,
there are some implicit constraints in our psychometric model. For example, the
phenotypic covariance of mothers and fathers ratings cannot be greater than the
206 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
A C E A C E
T1
Mo
T1
Fa
T2
Mo
T2
Fa
M1
A
T1
P
T2
P
F1
C
M1
C
M1
E
F1
A
F1
E
M2
A
F2
C
M2
C
M2
E
F2
A
F2
E
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 / 0.5 1.0
1.0
1.00 1.00
1.0 1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0
Figure 11.2: Psychometric or common pathway model for ratings of a pair of
twins (1 and 2) by their parents. Maternal and paternal observed ratings (MoT
and FaT) are linear functions of the latent phenotypes of the twins (PT), and
rater specic variance (e.g., A
M
, C
M
and E
M
).
variance of either type of rating. Such constraints may cause the model to fail
in some circumstances even though the 9 parameter biometric model discussed
below (Figure 11.3) may t adequately
1
. The second technical point is that if we
do not constrain the loadings of the common factor to be equal on the mothers
ratings and on the fathers ratings, and assume that there is no specic genetic
variance for either mothers ratings or for fathers ratings, then this variant of
the psychometric model is formally equivalent to our version in the Neale and
Stevenson bias model described above. In this case the shared environmental
1
There are in fact some other special cases such as scalar sex-limitation where identical
genetic or environmental factors may have dierent factor loadings for males and females
when the psychometric model may t as well or better than the biometric model.
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 207
specic variances for the mothers and fathers ratings are formally equivalent to the
maternal and paternal biases in the earlier model, while the non-shared specic
variances are equal to the unreliability variance of the earlier parameterization.
Thus, although the 9 parameter psychometric model and the bias model do not form
a nested pair (Mulaik et al., 1989), they represent alternative sets of constraints
on a more general 10 parameter model (which is not identied with two-rater twin
data) and these constrained models may be compared in terms of parsimony and
goodness of t. Furthermore, we may consider a restricted bias model in which
the scaling factor in Figure 11.1 is set to unity and which, therefore, has 7 free
parameters and is nested within both the psychometric model and the unrestricted
bias model. This restricted bias model may therefore be tested directly against
either the psychometric or the unrestricted bias models by a likelihood ratio chi-
square.
11.2.3 Biometric Model
The nal model to be considered is the biometric model shown in Figure 11.3, and
again may be readily implemented using the procedure described in Chapter 10.
In this model there are two factors for each source of variance (A, C, and E).
One factor is subscripted M, e.g., A
M
, and loads on the maternal rating (MoT)
and on the paternal rating (FaT). The other factor subscripted F, e.g., A
F
, loads
only on the paternal rating. Thus, for each source of inuence we estimate three
factor loadings which enable us to reconstruct estimates of the contribution of this
inuence to the variance of maternal ratings, the variance of paternal ratings and
the covariance between them. Which factor loads on both types of rating and
which on only one is arbitrary. This type of model is referred to as a Cholesky
model or decomposition or a triangular model and provides a standard general
approach to multivariate biometrical analysis (see Chapter 10). This biometric
model is a saturated unconstrained model for the nine unique expected variances
and covariances (in the absence of sibling interactions or other inuences giving
rise to heterogeneity of variances across zygosities, cf. Heath et al., 1989) and pro-
vides the most general approach to estimating the genetic, shared environmental
and non-shared environmental components of variance and covariance. However,
the absence of theoretically motivated constraints lessens the psychological infor-
mativeness of the model for the analysis of parental ratings. In this context, we
may use the biometric model rst to test the adequacy of the assumption that of
the 20 observed variances and covariances for bivariate twin data of a given sex, 11
represent replicate estimates of the 9 unique structural expectations. Once again,
sex dierences in factor loadings (scalar sex limitation) may in principle lead to
model failure for opposite sex data even though the biometric model is adequate
for a given sex. In this case the non-scalar sex limitation model described in Heath
et al. (1989) and Chapter 9 would be required. The bivariate biometric model
provides a baseline for comparison of the adequacy of the psychometric and bias
208 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
T1
Mo
T1
Fa
T2
Mo
T2
Fa
M1
A
F1
C
M1
C
M1
E
F1
A
F1
E
M2
A
F2
C
M2
C
M2
E
F2
A
F2
E
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0
1.0
1.0
Figure 11.3: Biometric or independent pathway model for ratings of a pair of
twins (1 and 2) by their parents. Maternal and paternal observed ratings (MoT
and FaT) are linear functions of general (subscript M) and restricted (subscript
F) genetic and environmental factors.
models. This comparison alerts us to the important possibility that mothers and
fathers are assessing dierent (but possibly correlated) phenotypes as, for example,
they might be if mothers and fathers were reporting on behaviors observed in dif-
ferent situations or without a common understanding of the behavioral descriptions
used in the assessment protocol.
11.2.4 Comparison of Models
We have considered four alternative models for parental ratings of childrens behav-
ior. Each model is for bivariate twin data where the two variables are the special
case of mothers ratings and fathers ratings of the childrens behavior. The least
restrictive model, the biometric model, provides a baseline for comparison with
the psychologically more informative psychometric and bias models. The most re-
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 209
stricted bias model may be formally tested by likelihood ratio chi-square against
either the psychometric or the unrestricted bias models. However, these latter two
are not themselves nested. The relationships between these models, without taking
into account sex limitations, are summarized in Figure 11.4. In this gure the solid
arrows represent the process of constraining a more general model to yield a more
Biometric model
(9 parameters)
Constrained
rotation
Psychometric model;
identified
(9 parameters)
Restricted bias model
(7 parameters)
Bias model
(8 parameters)
General psychometric
model; not identified
(10 parameters)
Constrained
rotation
=1.0 a = a = 0
m f
a = a = 0
m f
=1.0
Figure 11.4: Diagram of nesting of biometric, psychometric, and rater bias mod-
els.
restrictive model; the model at the arrow head is nested within the model at the
tail of the arrow and may be tested against it by a likelihood ratio chi square. The
dashed arrows represent rotational constraints on the biometric model. The nine
parameter psychometric model requires, for example, that the covariance between
maternal and paternal ratings be no greater than the variance of either type of
rating; in factor analytic terms this would require a constrained rotation of the
biometric model solution. The ten parameter psychometric model, allowing not
equal to unity, still imposes the constraints that the contributions of the common
inuences to the variance of maternal ratings, the variance of paternal ratings, and
the covariance between them be in the ratio 1 :
2
: for each source of inuence.
Thus, even though this model has 10 parameters (and hence is not identied for
bivariate twin data) any of its solutions, arrived at by xing one of the parameters
to an arbitrary value, will again represent in factor analytic terms a constrained
rotation of the biometric model.
210 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
11.2.5 Application to Data from Child Behavior Checklist
To illustrate the application of these models we consider an updated set of data
rst presented by Hewitt, et al., (1990) and now based on 983 families where
both parents rated each of their twin children using Achenbachs Child Behavior
Checklist (CBC; Achenbach and Edelbrock, 1983). For the full analysis, published
in Hewitt et al. (1992), data from a population-based sample of 500 MZ twin pairs
and 483 DZ twin pairs were considered and ratings were included irrespective of
the biological or social relationship of the parent to the child. The children were
Caucasian and ranged in age from 8 to 16 years. Ratings on 23 core items assessing
childrens internalizing behavior in both younger and older children and in either
boys or girls were totalled to obtain an internalizing scale score for each child. The
items contributing to this scale are listed in Appendix F.2.
For illustrative purposes in this chapter we just consider the prepubertal
subsample of younger children aged 8-11 years. More detailed analyses, including
older children, may be found in Hewitt et al. (1992). The scale scores were log-
transformed to approximate normality and adjusted for linear regression on age
and sex within age cohorts. The observed variances, covariances, and correlations
of the resulting scores are given in Table 11.1 by zygosity and sex group.
Table 11.1: Observed variance-covariance matrices (lower triangle) and twin cor-
relations (above the diagonal) for parental ratings (mother (Mo); father (Fa)) of
internalizing behavior problems in ve zygosity-sex groups (MZ female, N=96; MZ
male, N=102; DZ female, N=102; DZ male, N=97; DZ male-female, N=103). All
twins were between 8 and 11 years at assessment.
Zygosity/sex Male Female
Twin 1 Twin 2 Twin 1 Twin 2
Mo Fa Mo Fa Mo Fa Mo Fa
MZ MoT1 .675 .40 .74 .43 MoT1 .694 .47 .84 .46
FaT1 .265 .652 .35 .77 FaT1 .312 .638 .37 .72
MoT2 .513 .237 .714 .51 MoT2 .569 .238 .666 .45
FaT2 .292 .513 .354 .676 FaT2 .308 .461 .293 .647
DZ MoT1 .621 .47 .70 .34 MoT1 .565 .41 .55 .29
FaT1 .315 .719 .35 .73 FaT1 .241 .604 .25 .57
MoT2 .434 .236 .623 .37 MoT2 .291 .137 .488 .52
FaT2 .233 .531 .251 .743 FaT2 .171 .347 .285 .604
DZMF MoT1 .538 .26 .49 .18
FaT1 .162 .730 .17 .56
MoT2 .243 .102 .465 .37
FaT2 .103 .372 .191 .574
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 211
A summary of the adequacy of the models tted to these data on younger
childrens internalizing problems is shown in Table 11.2. The illustrative program
in Appendix F.1 runs the analysis for the bias model with 34 degrees of freedom.
As can be seen from Table 11.2, all three types of model give excellent ts
to the data for younger children, with the psychometric model being preferred
Table 11.2: Model comparisons for internalizing problems analysis.
Fit statistics
Model

df
2
AIC
Restricted bias 36 30.07 -41.9
Bias 34 25.78 -42.2
Psychometric 32 20.71 -43.3
Biometric 32 20.95 -43.1
by Akaikes Information Criterion. Thus, our rst conclusion would be that to
a very good approximation, mothers and fathers can be assumed to be rating
the same phenotype in their children when using the Child Behavior Checklist,
at least as far as these internalizing behaviors are concerned. This may not be
so for other behaviors or assessment instruments and in each particular case the
assumption ought to be tested by a comparison of models of the kind we have
described. Although there are numerous submodels or alternative models that
may be considered, (for example: no sex limitation; non-scalar sex-limitation; and
setting non-signicant parameters to zero), only a subset will be presented here for
illustration.
Table 11.3 shows the parameter estimates for the full bias and psychometric
models allowing for scalar sex limitation and, in the case of the biometric model, we
have allowed for non-scalar sex-limitation
2
of the shared environmental inuences
specic to fathers ratings (
2
31
= 20.76 for the model presented with the correlation
between boys and girls eects of this kind estimated at 0.86 rather than unity).
To show the relationship between the more parsimonious bias model and the full
parameterization of the biometric model, in Table 11.4 we present the expected
contributions of A, C, and E to the variance of mothers ratings, fathers ratings,
and the covariances between mothers and fathers ratings. What Table 11.4 shows
is that, providing the rater bias model is adequate, we can partition the environ-
mental variance of mothers and fathers ratings into variance attributable to those
eects consistently rated by both parents and those eects which either represent
rater bias or residual unreliable environmental variance. In this particular case,
while a univariate consideration of maternal ratings would suggest a heritability of
47% [= .263/(.263 + .194 + .108)], a shared environmental inuence of 34%, and
2
This is to avoid estimated loadings of opposite sign in boys and girls see Chapter 9.
212 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
Table 11.3: Parameter estimates from tting bias, psychometric, and biometric
models for parental ratings of internalizing behaviors.
Bias model Psychometric model Biometric model
Path Boys Girls Path Boys Girls Path Boys Girls
a .519 .163 a .370 .145 a
m
.513 .134
c .277 .363 a
m
.338 -.027 a
fm
.261 .132
e .189 .156 a
f
-.069 .281 a
f
.265 .286
a .671 1.416
b
m
.320 .545 c .308 .449 c
m
.440 .659
b
f
.509 .473 c
m
.332 .479 c
fm
.225 .308
r
2
m
.074 .154 c
f
.437 .507 c
f
.490 .603
r
2
f
.175 .115
e .176 .200 e
m
.328 .423
e
m
.278 .372 e
fm
.096 .097
e
f
.386 .333 e
f
.414 .377
a non-shared environmental inuence of 19%, it is clear that more than half of
the shared environmental inuence can be attributed to rater bias, and the major
portion of the non-shared environmental inuence to unreliability or inconsistency
between ratings. The heritability of internalizing behaviors in young boys rated
consistently by both parents may be as high as 70% [= .269/(.269 +.077 +.036)].
11.2.6 Discussion of CBC Application
The data we have analyzed are restricted to parental checklist reports of their
twin childrens behavior problems, without the benet of self reports, teachers re-
ports or clinical interviews. As such, they are limited by the ability of parents to
provide reliable and valid integrative assessments of their children, using cursorily
dened concepts like Sulks a lot, Worrying, or Fears going to school. It is clear
from meta-analyses of intercorrelations of ratings of children by dierent types in-
formants that while the level of agreement between mothers and fathers is often
moderate (e.g., yielding correlations around .5 to .6), the level of agreement be-
tween parents and other informants (e.g., parent with child or parent with teacher)
is modest and generally yields a correlation around 0.2 to 0.3 (Achenbach et al.,
1987). Thus, parental consistency in evaluating their children does not guarantee
cross situational validity, although it does provide evidence that ratings of behavior
observable by parents are not simply reecting individual rater biases. In assessing
the importance of the home environment on childrens behavior this becomes a
critical issue since studies of childrens behavior based on ratings by a single indi-
vidual in each family, e.g., the mother, confound the rater bias with the inuence of
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 213
Table 11.4: Contributions to the phenotypic variances and covariance of mothers
and fathers ratings of young boys internalizing behavior.
Biometric model Bias model
Ratings Cov (r) Ratings Cov (r)
Source Mother Father M-F Mother Father M-F
A .268 .138 .134 (.70) .269 .121 .181 (1.0)
C .194 .291 .099 (.42) .077 .035 .051 (1.0)
Bias .102 .259 .000 (.00)
C + Bias .194 .291 .099 (.42) .179 .294 .051 (.22)
E .108 .181 .031 (.22) .036 .016 .024 (1.0)
Residual .074 .175 .000 (.00)
E + Residual .108 .181 .031 (.22) .110 .191 .024 (.17)
Phenotypic
Total .564 .609 .264 (.45) .558 .606 .256 (.44)
Italicized numbers indicate parameters are xed ex hypothesi in the rater bias
model.
the home environment. This may have the dual eect of inating global estimates
of the home environments inuence while at the same time either attenuating the
relationship between objective indices of the environment and childrens behavior
(which is being assessed by a biased observer) or spuriously augmenting apparent
relationships which are in fact relationships between environmental indices and
maternal or paternal rating biases.
An issue distinguishable from that of bias is that of behavior sampling or situ-
ational specicity. Thus maternal and paternal ratings of children may dier not
because of the tendency of individual parents to rate children in general as more
or less problematic (bias), but because they are exposed to dierent samples of
behavior. If this is so, then treating informants ratings as if they were assessing a
common phenotype, albeit in a biased or unreliable way, will be misleading. It is
of considerable psychological importance to know whether dierent observers are
being presented with dierent behaviors. The approach outlined in this chapter
rst enables us to examine the adequacy of the assumption that dierent infor-
mants are assessing the same behaviors and then, if that assumption is deemed
adequate, to separate the contributions of rater bias and unreliability from the
genetic and environmental contribution to the common behavioral phenotype. For
our particular example, all the models t our data adequately and the bias model,
even in its restricted version, does not t signicantly worse than the psychometric
or biometric models.
Although not presented here, there is some evidence that for externalizing be-
havior mothers and fathers cannot be assumed to be simply assessing the same
214 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
phenotype with bias. In this context it is worth noting, however, that the adequacy
of the assumption that parents are assessing the same phenotype in their children
does not imply a high parental correlation (which may be lowered by bias and
unreliability) and, conversely, even though parents may be shown to be assessing
dierent phenotypes in their children to a signicant degree the parental correlation
in assessments may predominate over variance specic to a given parent. Our com-
parison of the bias with the psychometric and biometric models provides important
evidence of the equivalence of the internalizing behaviors assessed by mothers and
fathers using this instrument. This equivalence does not preclude bias or unrelia-
bility and the evidence presented in Table 11.4 provides a striking illustration of
the impact of these sources of variation on maternal or paternal assessments. A
shared environmental component which might be estimated to account for 34% of
variance if mothers ratings alone were considered, may correspond to only 20%
of the variance when maternal biases have been removed. Similarly, a non-shared
environmental variance component of 19% of variance may correspond to 9% of
variance in individual dierences between children that can be consistently rated
by both parents. Finally, once allowance has been made for bias and inconsistency
or unreliability, the estimated heritability rises from 47% to 70% in this case.
We have not been concerned here to seek the most parsimonious submodel
within each of the model types. We should be aware that although we have, for
the younger children, presented the full models with sex limitation, dierences
between boys and girls are not necessarily signicant (for example, although the
biometric model without sex limitation t our data signicantly worse than the
corresponding model allowing for sex limitation (
2
9
= 21.31, p < .05), the overall
t without sex limitation is still adequate,
2
41
= 42.26). Furthermore, individual
parameter estimates reported for our full models may not depart signicantly from
zero. Other limitations of the method are that it does not allow for interaction
eects between parents and children
3
and, in our application, assumed the inde-
pendence of maternal and paternal biases. The analysis of parental bias under
this model requires that both parents rate each of two children. Distinguishing be-
tween correlated parental biases and shared environmental inuences would require
a third, independent, rater (e.g., a teacher); thus we cannot rule out a contribution
of correlated biases to our estimates of the remaining shared family environmental
inuence.
The nal caveat against overinterpretation of particular parameter estimates is
that we have reported analyses for families in which both parents have returned
a questionnaire and we have made no distinction between dierent biological or
social parental statuses. Clearly, we anticipate that the inclusion and exclusion
criteria are not neutral with respect to childrens behavior problems and their
perception by parents. However, we have illustrated that behavior genetic analyses
3
However, if these eects were substantial and if MZ twins correlated more highly than DZ
twins in their interactional style, the variance of parents ratings should dier (Neale et al., 1992).
Given sucient sample size, these eects would lead to failure of these models.
11.2. MODELS FOR MULTIPLE RATING DATA 215
are possible even when we have to rely on ratings by observers, providing that we
have at least two degrees of relatedness among those being rated (e.g., MZ and DZ
twins). Without an approach of this sort we have no way of establishing whether
parents are assessing the same behaviors in their children and whether analyses will
spuriously inate estimates of the shared environment as much as parental biases
inate the correlations for pairs of twins independent of zygosity. Extension of the
model to include other raters, for example, teachers, is straightforward.
216 CHAPTER 11. OBSERVER RATINGS
Chapter 12
Repeated Measures
12.1 Introduction
This chapter deals with the genetic analysis of repeated measures. Examples of
data that are collected in repeated measures designs include: dietary intake mea-
sured over several days or weeks; blood pressure taken under dierent conditions
of rest and stress; psychophysiological data such as EEG that may be sampled
with frequencies of 100 Hz or more (i.e., 100 times per second); performance mea-
sured during learning experiments; IQ measures taken at several dierent ages; or
behavioral indices of development collected over several years of childhood. Two
fundamental questions are important for analysis of these data:
1. Are there changes in the magnitude of the genetic and environmental eects
over time? For example, are there changes in heritability?
2. Do the same genetic and environmental inuences operate throughout time?
For example, are the genes that inuence behavior early in life dierent from
the genes that inuence the same trait later in life?
If there are no cohort eects, the rst question can be addressed in a cross-sectional
study that measures subjects of dierent times, but the second question can only
be answered in a longitudinal setting. Data collected in this way are essentially
multivariate, if we consider the multi to refer to the multiple occasions of mea-
surement. However, the direct application of the multivariate methods described
in Chapters 10 and ?? would not take full advantage of our a priori knowledge of
the data structure. By denition, causation is unidirectional through time; earlier
causes can have only later eects. This constraint gives added power to the study
of genetic and environmental variability we may assess whether new genes
or new environmental factors start to operate at specic points in time. Given
sucient occasions of measurement, we may be able to discriminate between: (i)
217
218 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
Table 12.1: Within-person correlations for weight measured at six-month intervals
on 66 females (Fischbein, 1977).
Weight 1 Weight 2 Weight 3 Weight 4 Weight 5 Weight 6
Weight 1 1.000
Weight 2 0.985 1.000
Weight 3 0.968 0.981 1.000
Weight 4 0.957 0.970 0.985 1.000
Weight 5 0.932 0.940 0.964 0.975 1.000
Weight 6 0.890 0.897 0.927 0.949 0.973 1.000
completely transient factors including, but not restricted to, measurement error;
(ii) the long-term consequences of experience at one point in time; and (iii) the
continuous presence and inuence of a causal factor.
We shall start our treatment of longitudinal genetic analysis with a simplex
model for phenotypic correlations (see Section ??). Phenotypic simplex models
are relatively easy to implement in Mx and elucidate some important features of
longitudinal measurements. Given this basic understanding of the potential of
time series data, the reader should have no diculty understanding the extension
to genetically informative data (see Section ??).
12.2 Phenotypic Simplex Model
Data that are measured repeatedly in time on the same subjects are often charac-
terized by a specic correlation structure among the measures at the dierent time
points. More specically, it can be seen quite often that correlations are highest
among adjoining occasions and that they fall away systematically as the distance
between time points increases. Such a pattern is called a simplex structure af-
ter Guttman (1954; see also Wohlwill, 1973). The simplex structure of repeated
observations is illustrated in Table 12.2
with correlations for repeated assessments of weight (in kilograms) in 66 fe-
males from a sample of opposite sex DZ twins in a longitudinal study by Fischbein
(1977). In this example, the data were taken at 6 month intervals starting when
subjects were on average 11.5 years of age. Although all correlations are high, it is
clear that they decrease systematically as the time between measurements increases
(i.e., as one moves further down the columns away from the principal diagonal).
This correlation pattern can be explained well by a simplex model, such as that
illustrated graphically in the path diagram in Figure 12.1 (see also J oreskog, 1970).
In this gure, the observed measurements (i.e., weight) are shown as Y variables
which serve as indicators of the latent X variables, weighted by the factor loadings
12.2. PHENOTYPIC SIMPLEX MODEL 219
l
i
. Measurement errors are shown as R variables, and factor residuals as Zs. The
regression coecients of X on X
i1
, the b weights, are solely responsible for the
covariation of measurements over time.
1
Y
2
Y
3
Y
4
Y
5
Y
6
Y
1
X
2
X
3
X
4
X
5
X
6
X
1
Z
2
Z
3
Z
4
Z
5
Z
6
Z
1
R
2
R
3
R
4
R
5
R
6
R
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1
z
2
z
3
z
4
z
5
z
6
z
1
l
2
l
3
l
4
l
5
l
6
l
1
r
2
r
3
r
4
r
5
r
6
r
2
b
3
b
4
b
5
b
6
b
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Figure 12.1: Simplex model for a phenotypic time series.
We will rst illustrate the use of the simplex model in Mx for a phenotypic
analysis with the weight data in Table 12.2.
12.2.1 Formulation of the Phenotypic Simplex Model in Mx
Two types of model can be distinguished: the measurement model and the struc-
tural equation model. The measurement model describes how latent variables are
related to observed variables and can be thought of as a conrmatory factor anal-
ysis model. For an observed variable Y with latent variable X and measurement
error E, we can write the measurement model at time each point as follows:
Y
i
= l
i
X
i
+R
i
(12.1)
var(Y
i
) = l
i
2
var(X
i
) + var(R
i
) (12.2)
To dene the units of measurement in the latent X variables, the factor loadings
(l
i
) can be xed at unity so that the measurement scale of the latent variables is
the same as in the observed variables. This implies that the variance of the latent
factors is to be estimated. Alternatively, the latent factors can be standardized to
have unit variance and the factor loadings can be estimated. As we have noted
220 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
elsewhere, it is not possible to estimate both the variance of and the regression on
a latent variable (see Chapter ??).
The structural equation model causally relates latent variables to other latent
variables. We have already encountered examples of structural models in the con-
text of sibling interactions and direction of causation (see Chapters 8 and ??).
Another example is the simplex model, in which latent variables at time i are in-
uenced by the latent variables at time i 1. Such relationships amongst latent
variables are often termed autoregressive and may be described by the following
equation:
X
i
= b
i
X
i1
+Z
i
(12.3)
var(X
i
) = b
i
2
var(X
i1
) + var(Z
i
) (12.4)
where X
i
is the latent variable at time i (i > 0), b
i
is the regression of the latent
factor on the previous latent factor, and Z represents a random input term (innova-
tion) that is uncorrelated with X
i1
. There is an important conceptual distinction
between innovations of latent factors and measurement errors of observed variables.
The innovations are that part of the latent factor at time i that is not caused by
the latent factor at time i 1, but are part of every subsequent time point i + 1.
On the other hand, the R terms are random errors of measurement that do not
inuence subsequent observed variables.
The parameters of this model are:
1. l
i
: the factor loadings of the observed on the latent factors;
2. z
0
= var(X
0
): the standard deviation of the latent factor at time t = 0;
3. z
i
= var(X
i
): the standard deviations of the innovations at times t > 0;
4. b
i
: the regression of the latent factor at time i on time i 1;
5. r
i
: the variances of the measurement errors.
The weight data that were introduced above can be analyzed according to a
simplex model using two alternative Mx scripts in Appendix G.1.
Mx Script 1 for Phenotypic Simplex Model
In the rst script the factor loadings in L are xed at 1 - by using an identity
matrix - so that the latent variables have the same measurement scale as the ob-
served data (kilograms in this case). At each time point we estimate the standard
deviations of the innovations on the diagonal of the Z matrix. Of course, at the
rst measurement occasion the rst latent factor cannot be explained by factors
associated with an earlier point in time and, therefore, this rst factor is itself
regarded as an innovation (i.e., X
1
= Z
1
). In B we estimate the autoregression
12.2. PHENOTYPIC SIMPLEX MODEL 221
Table 12.2: Parameter estimates and total variances using script 1 for weight.
Time var(b
t
) var(X
t1
) var(Z
t
) var(X
t
) var(R
t
) total variance
t = 1 7.165
2
= 51.337 0.365
2
51.470
t = 2 1.049
2
51.337 +1.225
2
= 57.992 0.365
2
58.125
t = 3 1.029
2
57.992 +1.440
2
= 63.478 0.365
2
63.611
t = 4 1.056
2
63.478 +1.363
2
= 72.644 0.365
2
72.777
t = 5 0.969
2
72.644 +1.810
2
= 71.486 0.365
2
71.619
t = 6 0.942
2
71.486 +1.809
2
= 66.707 0.365
2
66.840
Chi=12.995, df=9, p=0.163
coecients that tell us how much of the variance in the latent factors at each oc-
casion is accounted for by the previous factor. Parameters on the diagonal of the
R matrix estimate the residual, non-transmissible standard deviations that include
measurement error. To identify the error variances at the rst and the last mea-
surement occasions additional constraints are needed, because error variances at
these occasions cannot be distinguished from innovation variance. In the examples
below we have constrained all error variances to be equal.
Mx Script 2 for Phenotypic Simplex Model
In this second script the innovations are standardized to have unit variance and at
each time point the factor loadings are estimated in the L matrix. The rst latent
factor in this specication may be thought of as an innovation because it cannot be
explained by factors associated with an earlier point in time. Again we use the B
matrix to specify the occasion to occasion transmission but here the transmission
is of the standardized X variables.
In Table 12.2.1 it can be seen that the bs are relatively high and the variances
of the innovations (Z
t
) small. Variances associated with measurement error (R
t
)
also are small and likely not signicant. The total variances at each measurement
occasion can be obtained according to equations 12.2 and 12.4:
At the second time point the total variance of the latent factor is 57.992. This
is (within rounding error) equal to the second diagonal element of the covari-
ance matrix of X. Only a small proportion of this variance is due to innovation:
1.500/57.968 = 0.026, the vast majority comes from amplication of existing vari-
ance at the rst time point. At t = 6 we see that the variance of the latent factor is
decreasing and that the contribution of new inuences becomes somewhat larger.
The output for the second Mx setup gives the estimates of the factor loadings
of the observed variables on latent variables in the L matrix. As may be seen, these
loadings correspond to those of the Z matrix in the rst script. The estimates in
B, however are quite dierent. In this case they can be conceived of as scaled
222 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
Table 12.3: Parameter estimates and total variances using script 2 for weight.
Time var(b
t
) var(X
t1
) var(Z
t
) var(X
t
) var(R
t
) total variance
t = 1 7.165
2
= 51.341 0.365
2
51.470
t = 2 6.139
2
1.225
2
= 58.022 0.365
2
58.150
t = 3 0.875
2
38.691 1.440
2
= 63.530 0.365
2
63.663
t = 4 1.116
2
30.655 1.363
2
= 72.693 0.365
2
72.826
t = 5 0.730
2
39.150 1.809
2
= 71.507 0.365
2
71.640
t = 6 0.942
2
21.839 1.809
2
= 66.727 0.365
2
66.860
Chi=12.995, df=9, p=0.163
Table 12.4: Correlations among latent factors
X
1
X
2
X
3
X
4
X
5
X
6
X
1
1.000
X
2
0.987 1.000
X
3
0.971 0.984 1.000
X
4
0.958 0.971 0.987 1.000
X
5
0.936 0.948 0.964 0.977 1.000
X
6
0.913 0.925 0.940 0.953 0.975 1.000
regression coecients and their absolute values have to be interpreted with care.
As the
2
and degrees of freedom for both Mx specications are the same, it is clear
that these dierences in parameter estimates do not aect the goodness-of-t of
the model. As is easily veried (how?) that the standardized solutions for both Mx
setups are identical. The standardized solution also gives the correlation matrix
among latent factors.
The correlations on the rst subdiagonal are the standardized bs. This corre-
lation matrix of the latent factors clearly reects the simplex structure we observe
in the data.
12.3 Genetic Simplex Model
In a behavior genetics context we usually want to analyze more than one latent
construct, for example, genetic and environmental components of variance. To
estimate such eects we can divide each of the X factors into a genetic and a non-
genetic part. In the context of simplex models we want to t a genetic and an
12.3. GENETIC SIMPLEX MODEL 223
environmental time series
1
, and we can specify both of these time series using Mx.
12.3.1 Mx Formulation of the Genetic Simplex Model
Letting A
i
and E
i
represent the additive genetic and within-family environmental
factors at each occasion (the X variables), the measurement model at each occasion
becomes:
Y
i
= a
i
A
i
+e
i
E
i
+r
i
R
i
+s
i
S
i
.
And for the structural part of the model we can write:
A
i
= b
i
A
i1
+X
i
(12.5)
E
i
= d
i
E
i1
+Z
i
(12.6)
For t = 3 the covariance matrix of these latent processes then equals:
_
_
_
_
_
_
var(A
1
) + var(E
1
)
b
2
var(A
1
) +d
2
var(E
1
) var(A
2
) + var(E
2
)
b
2
b
3
var(A
1
) +d
2
d
3
var(E
1
) b
3
var(A
2
) +d
3
var(E
2
) var(A
3
) + var(E
3
)
_
_
_
_
_
_
where
var(A
i
) = b
i
2
var(A
i1
) + var(X
i
)
var(E
i
) = d
i
2
var(E
i1
) + var(Z
i
) .
This genetic simplex model is shown diagrammatically in Figure 12.2 for the more
complete case of 6 variables, as in the Fischbein weight data.
Similar to the phenotypic simplex model, the genetic simplex model can be
specied in a variety of ways in Mx. We opt for the rst approach and estimate
the innovations and the transmission paths while xing the factors loadings to one.
This implies pre- and post-multiplying the expected covariances by an identity ma-
trix, we have simplied the script by ommitting the L matrix. Consistent with the
notation in the equations, matrices X and B are used respectively for the genetic
innovations and transmissions and matrices Z and D for the environmental innova-
tions and transmissions. It is possible to estimate both genetic and environmental
residuals (measurement error) in diagonal matrices (R and S). Figure 12.2 is drawn
for the complete model.
1
Here we t the simplex only to additive genetic and within-family environmental eects.
See Section 12.5 for a discussion of extended simplex formulations involving other variance
components.
224 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
1
Y
2
Y
3
Y
4
Y
5
Y
6
Y
1
A
2
A
3
A
4
A
5
A
6
A
1
X
2
X
3
X
4
X
5
X
6
X
1
E
2
E
3
E
4
E
5
E
6
E
1
R
2
R
3
R
4
R
5
R
6
R
1
S
2
S
3
S
4
S
5
S
6
S
1
Z
2
Z
3
Z
4
Z
5
Z
6
Z
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1
x
2
x
3
x
4
x
5
x
6
x
1
a
2
a
3
a
4
a
5
a
6
a
1
e
2
e
3
e
4
e
5
e
6
e
2
b
3
b
4
b
5
b
6
b
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
6
z
1
z
2
z
3
z
4
z
5
z
6
f
5
f
4
f
2
f
3
f
1
s
2
s
3
s
4
s
5
s
6
s
6
r
5
r
4
r
3
r
2
r
1
r
Figure 12.2: Simplex model of genetic and within-family environmental time
series. The gure is drawn for one twin.
12.3.2 Application to Longitudinal Data on Weight
From the same study by Fischbein (1977) we have weight data for 32 MZ and 51
DZ female twin pairs. Appendix G.2 gives the Mx setup for the genetic analysis of
these data. The matrices for the genetic and environmental structure are set up in
the rst calculation group.
#Define nvar 6
#Define nvarm1 5
G1: Genetic and Environmental structure
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Diag nvar nvar Free ! genetic innovation paths
B Diag nvarm1 nvarm1 Free ! genetic transmission paths
Z Diag nvar nvar Free ! specific env innovation paths
D Diag nvarm1 nvarm1 Free ! specific env transmission paths
I Iden nvar nvar
J Zero 1 nvarm1
12.3. GENETIC SIMPLEX MODEL 225
L Zero nvar 1
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
G= (J_B)|L ;
T= (I-G)~ ;
A= T*(X*X)*T ;
F= (J_D)|L ;
U= (I-F)~ ;
E= U*(Z*Z)*U ;
End Algebra;
End
The rst few lines dene the number of groups and dene two variables, nvar
for the number of variables and nvarm1 for the number of variables minus 1. We de-
ne the matrices X for the additive genetic factors at each occasion of measurement
(X
1
, ..., X
6
) and Z for the environmental innovations (Z
1
, ..., Z
6
) as diagonal and
free. Note that we do not specify any measurement errors since these were found to
be non-signicant in the previous analysis. The transmission matrices require free
subdiagonal elements which correspond to the regression of each A
i
on A
i1
and
E
i
on E
i1
. In Mx, we could specify this using a 6 6 lower triangular matrices
in which the subdiagonal elements are declared free with a Specification state-
ment. We choose a more general formulation here by declaring two 5 5 diagonal
matrices (B for the genetic and D for the environmental transmission paths). We
then stick a 1 5 row vector of zeros J on top of this diagonal matrix using the
horizontal concatenator, and a 6 1 column vector of zeros L to the right of this
block using the vertical concatenator. The resulting G and F matrices are then of
the following form:
0 0 0 0 0 0
? 0 0 0 0 0
0 ? 0 0 0 0
0 0 ? 0 0 0
0 0 0 ? 0 0
0 0 0 0 ? 0
where ? indicates a free parameter. The same (I-G)~ formulation is used as in
the phenotypic simplex model to correctly specify the autoregressive paths. Fi-
nally the matrices with the innovations are pre- and post-multiplied by the matrix
formulation for the transmission paths to obtain the genetic A and the specic
environmental E covariance matrices. Group 2 and 3 are then used to t the
model to the MZ and DZ data respectively. The formulation for the expected
covariance matrices is similar to that of the univariate and multivariate models
described in previous chapters. Note that this model has 22 free parameters and,
thus, 2[12(12 + 1)]/2 22 = 156 22 = 134 degrees of freedom.
226 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
Table 12.5: Genetic, environmental, and total phenotypic variances estimated
from the genetic simplex model applied to Fishbeins (1977) data on weight
Variance
Time Genetic Environmental Total
var(b
t
) var(A
t1
) +var(X
t
) var(d
t
) var(E
t1
) +var(Z
t
) var(Y
t
)
t = 1 4.79
2
= 22.94 1.82
2
= 3.31 26.25
t = 2
_
1.05
2
22.94 + 1.12
2
_
= 26.55
_
0.92
2
3.31 + 0.56
2
_
= 3.12 29.67
t = 3
_
1.04
2
26.55 + 1.50
2
_
= 30.97
_
1.05
2
3.12 + 0.98
2
_
= 4.40 35.37
t = 4
_
1.02
2
30.97 + 1.23
2
_
= 33.73
_
0.85
2
4.40 + 0.95
2
_
= 4.08 37.81
t = 5
_
1.02
2
33.73 + 1.39
2
_
= 37.02
_
0.84
2
4.08 + 0.81
2
_
= 3.53 40.55
t = 6
_
0.97
2
37.02 + 1.39
2
_
= 36.76
_
1.01
2
3.53 + 0.99
2
_
= 4.58 41.34
With the parameter estimates from the output we can calculate the genetic and
environmental variances at each time point and then compute the heritabilities.
The total genetic variance at time point i, i > 0, is computed as:
var(A
i
) = b
2
i
var(A
i1
) + var(X
i
)
The environmental variances are computed in the same way. The genetic, environ-
mental, and total (phenotypic) variances estimated from this model are shown in
Table 12.5.
As in the phenotypic analysis, we see that there is an increase in total variance
over time (26.25 to 41.34), and here we see that this is caused by an increase
in the genetic part of the variance, whereas the environmental variances do not
change very much. We can now address the questions that were raised in the
introduction to this chapter, i.e., i) are there changes in heritabilities over time?
and ii) do the same genetic and environmental inuences operate throughout time?
For the rst question we can calculate the heritabilities by dividing the genetic
variance at each time point by the total variance, which yields the heritability
estimates 0.87, 0.89, 0.88, 0.89, 0.91 and 0.89. Clearly there is little change in
the magnitude of genetic inuence on weight over time. For the second question,
we can partition the heritable variation at each time into (i) genetic inuences
novel to the occasion, and (ii) genetic eects persisting from the previous occasion.
That is, a portion of each heritability is due to new genetic inuences impacting
weight assessments at each point in time, and a portion is due to the genetic eects
which were already operating at the previous measurement point. The former can
be calculated from the parameter estimates as var(X
i
)/var(A
i
) and the latter as
b
2
i
var(A
i1
)/var(A
i
). For example, on the second occasion 1.25/29.67 = 4% of
the total variance consists of new genetic variance and 25.29/29.67 = 85% of the
total variance consists of amplication of existing variance at the rst time point.
12.3. GENETIC SIMPLEX MODEL 227
Table 12.6: Estimated correlations among latent genetic [below diagonal] and
environmental [above diagonal] factors in a genetic simplex model tted to weight
data.
A
1
/E
1
A
2
/E
2
A
3
/E
3
A
4
/E
4
A
5
/E
5
A
6
/E
6
A
1
/E
1
1.000 .947 .837 .738 .665 .589
A
2
/E
2
.976 1.000 .884 .779 .702 .622
A
3
/E
3
.941 .964 1.000 .881 .794 .704
A
4
/E
4
.919 .942 .977 1.000 .902 .799
A
5
/E
5
.896 .917 .952 .974 1.000 .886
A
6
/E
6
.872 .893 .927 .949 .974 1.000
At the other time points also (i = 3 . . . 6), only a small part of the total variance
is due to genetic innovation terms (6%, 4%, 5%, and 5%). Thus, we see that the
genetic eects do not change dramatically during this period of development; i.e.,
the same genetic inuences are operating over time.
As in the phenotypic example, we can get the correlations among the latent
factors. In this case, the latent variables are genetic and non-shared environmental
factors; thus, the standardized solution gives the genetic and environmental corre-
lations for the weight data. We present these correlations in Table 12.3.2. It may
be seen that both of these matrices conform to a simplex structure and from these
matrices it also is clear that both genetic and environmental stabilities are high.
Evidence for genetic simplex patterns in morphological traits also has been found
in studies of animal development (Arnold, 1990).
12.3.3 Common Factor Model for Longitudinal Twin Data
Since the above analyses indicate that the genetic and environmental correlations
are high, we may ask if a factor model also would give a good t to the data [see,
e.g., Boomsma and Molenaar (1987), Cardon and Fulker (1991) and Cardon et al.,
(1992) for empirical assessments of this question]. To address this question we t a
model that includes a common genetic and a common within-family environmental
factor and measurement-specic environmental factors to the weight covariances
(genetic specic factors were not dierent from zero). The Mx specication for
this model is described in detail in Section 10.3.2 of Chapter 10 and an example
script is given in Appendix ?? for the case of 4 measures. The extension to 6
variables in the present case is fairly trivial and is left to the reader. Although the
factor model represents a more parsimonious account of the data (18 vs. 22 free
parameters), the goodness-of-t chi-squared for this model is much higher than for
the one obtained from the simplex model (common factor
2
138
= 359.29, p = .000
vs. simplex
2
134
= 160.99, p = .056). Thus, the simplex model appears to provide
228 CHAPTER 12. REPEATED MEASURES
a better explanation of the weight observations.
12.4 Problems with Repeated Measures Data
Analysis of repeatedly measured variables may create some specic numeric prob-
lems. Covariance matrices associated with highly intercorrelated repeated mea-
sures can become nearly singular. As a consequence, the chi-squared goodness-of-
t statistic may be positively biased (in contrast to parameter estimates, which
are generally unbiased). Secondly, even if there are no singularity problems, the
large number of variables in a covariance analysis of repeated measures may lead
to indeterminacies during computation. This situation resembles the occurrence
of collinearity in regression analysis, which usually can be counteracted by the
invocation of ridge regression. A similar approach can be used in Mx by
1. the addition of a small positive constant to the diagonal of the observed
covariance matrix, and
2. correcting the model for this perturbation by adding a matrix for residuals[?]
and xing the diagonal at the same positive constant (see Boomsma et al.,
1989a,b)
12.5 Discussion
The genetic analysis of development and age-related changes in human behavior
involves more issues, such as modeling age of onset for example, than the questions
that have been addressed here. An overview of some of these other issues is given
by Eaves et al., (1990a). The genetic analysis of repeated measures as outlined in
this chapter is a very exible approach to the analysis of change and continuity.
The simplex model allows for both dierential heritabilities and environmental
variances at dierent time points, as well as dierent genetic and environmental
correlations between time points. This model can be extended in several ways.
An additional latent simplex structure may be specied to test the inuence of
common-environmental components (see, e.g., Eaves et al., 1986; Hewitt et al.,
1988; Phillips and Fulker, 1989; Cardon et al., 1992) The model also can be used
for the analysis of multivariate time series. In this case, each single time series
may conform to a simplex structure, while the relationship between dierent types
of variables can be analyzed with a conrmatory factor analysis model (see e.g.,
Cardon, 1992). The simplex model used here in the analysis of the weight data
species a so called rst-order autoregressive model, where a measure is inuenced
only by the previous one. It is also possible to specify higher-order autoregressive
models, where variables at t = i are inuenced for example by variables at time
i 1 as well as at time i 2. All of these analyses can be carried out with Mx.
Chapter A
Mx Scripts for Univariate
Models
A.1 Path Coecients Model
The following Mx script represents a univariate genetic model tted to covariance
matrices for two twin groups: 1) MZ pairs reared together, and 2) DZ pairs reared
together.
! Path Coefficients Model
! BMI data in Australian twins
#NGroups 3
G1: Model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed
Z Lower 1 1 Free
W Lower 1 1 Free
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra:
End
229
230 CHAPTER A. MX SCRIPTS FOR UNIVARIATE MODELS
G2: young female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=534
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmimzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+C+D+E | A+C+D _
A+C+D | A+C+D+E ;
Options RSidual
End
G3: young female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=328
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmidzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .6 All
Covariances A+C+D+E | H@A+C+Q@D _
H@A+C+Q@D | A+C+D+E ;
Options RSidual NDecimals=4
End
A.2 Variance Components Model
This Mx script ts a variance components model to twin covariances on BMI. It is
a two group problem, for MZ and DZ pairs reared together.
! Variance Components Model
! BMI data in Australian female twins
#NGroups 2
G1: young female MZ twin pairs
Data NGroups=2 NInput_vars=2 NObservations=534
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmimzf.cov
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
Begin Matrices;
A Lower 1 1 Free
C Lower 1 1 Fixed
E Lower 1 1 Free
A.3. MODEL FOR MEANS AND COVARIANCES 231
D Lower 1 1 Free
End Matrices;
Covariances A+C+D+E | A+C+D _
A+C+D | A+C+D+E ;
Options RSidual
End
G2: young female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=328
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmidzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .6 All
Covariances A+C+D+E | H@A+C+Q@D _
H@A+C+Q@D | A+C+D+E ;
Options RSidual NDecimals=4
End
A.3 Model for Means and Covariances
The following Mx script estimates path coecients for like-sex twins under the
univariate genetic model, incorporating estimation of means.
! Model for Means and Covariances
! BMI data in Australian female twins
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed ! common environmental structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Fixed ! dominance structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
232 CHAPTER A. MX SCRIPTS FOR UNIVARIATE MODELS
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: older female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput-vars=2 NObservations=637
Means File=ozbmiomz.mea
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmiomz.cov
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
M Full 2 1 Free
Means M ; ! model for means
Covariances ! model for covariances
A+C+D+E | A+C+D _
A+C+D | A+C+D+E ;
Options RSidual
End
G3: older female dz twin pairs
Data NInput-vars=2 NObservations=380
Means File=ozbmiodz.mea
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmiodz.cov
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
M Full 2 1 Free
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .6 All
Start .3 M 2 1 1 M 2 2 1 M 3 1 1 M 3 2 1
Means M ;
Covariances A+C+D+E | H@A+C+Q@D _
H@A+C+Q@D | A+C+D+E ;
Options Multiple RSidual
End
!no heterogeneity of means for birthorder
Specify 2 M 3 3 ! equate pt1=pt2 for mz twins
Specify 3 M 5 5 ! equate pt1=pt2 for dz twins
End
A.4. UNIVARIATE GENETIC MODEL FOR TWIN PAIRS AND SINGLES233
!no heterogeneity of means for zygosity
Specify 3 M 3 3 ! equate means in group 2 to group 1
End
A.4 Univariate Genetic Model for Twin pairs and
Singles
This Mx script ts the simple univariate genetic model (incorporating means) to
BMI data from like-sex female twins in which (a) both twins in the pair responded
to the survey; (b) the cotwin did not cooperate.
! Univariate Genetic Model for Twin pairs and Singles
! BMI data in Australian twins
#NGroups 5
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed ! common environmental structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Fixed ! dominance structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: older female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput-vars=2 NObservations=637
Means File=ozbmiomz.mea
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmiomz.cov
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
M Full 2 1
End Matrices;
Specify M 3 3
Means M ; ! model for means
234 CHAPTER A. MX SCRIPTS FOR UNIVARIATE MODELS
Covariances ! model for covariances
A+C+D+E | A+C+D _
A+C+D | A+C+D+E ;
Option RSidual
End
G3: older female dz twin pairs
Data NInput-vars=2 NObservations=380
Means File=ozbmiodz.mea
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozbmiodz.cov
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
M Full 2 1
End Matrices;
Specify M 3 3
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Means M ;
Covariances A+C+D+E | H@A+C+Q@D _
H@A+C+Q@D | A+C+D+E ;
Option RSidual
End
G4: older female mz twins whose cotwins did not respond
Data NInput-vars=1 NObservations=44
Mean File=ozbmismz.mea
CMatrix File=ozbmismz.cov
Labels bmi
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
M Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Specify M 3
Means M ;
Covariances A+C+D+E ;
Option RSidual
End
G5: older female dz twins whose cotwins did not respond
Data NInput-vars=1 NObservations=62
Mean File=ozbmisdz.mea
CMatrix File=ozbmisdz.cov
A.5. AGE-CORRECTION MODEL 235
Labels bmi
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
M Full 1 1
End Matrices
Specify M 3
Start .6 All
Start .3 M 2 1 1 M 2 2 1 M 3 1 1 M 3 2 1 M 4 1 1 M 5 1 1
Means M ;
Covariances A+C+D+E ;
Options NDecimals=4
Option RSidual
End
A.5 Age-correction Model
This Mx script is the basic model without age eects described in Section 6.4.
Modications to the script to incorporate age eects are described in Section 6.4
on page 137. The model is t to conservatism data from wAustralian female twins.
! Age Correction Model
! Conservatism data from Australian female twins
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Y Lower 1 1 Free ! common environmental structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
S Lower 1 1 Free ! effect of age on phenotype
V Lower 1 1 Free ! variance of age
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
G= S*S ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=3 NObservations=941
236 CHAPTER A. MX SCRIPTS FOR UNIVARIATE MODELS
Labels age cons_t1 cons-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozconmzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances V*V | S*V | S*V _
S*V | A+C+E+G | A+C+G _
S*V | A+C+G | A+C+E+G ;
Option RSidual
End
G3: female dz twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=3 NObservations=548
Labels age cons_t1 cons-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozcondzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start 5 All
Start 15 V 1 1 1
Covariances V*V | S*V | S*V _
S*V | A+C+E+G | H@A+C+G _
S*V | H@A+C+G | A+C+E+G ;
Options NDecimals=4
Option RSidual
End
Chapter B
Mx Script for Power
Calculation
B.1 ACE Model for Power Calculations
The Mx script below ts the univariate ACE model to simulated twin covariances.
The application is described in Section 7.3.
! ACE Model for Power Calculations
! Simulated data
! Simulate the data
! 30% additive genetic (.5477=.3)
! 20% common environment (.4472=.2)
! 50% random environment (.7071=.5)
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Fixed ! genetic structure
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed ! common environmental structure
Z Lower 1 1 Fixed ! specific environmental structure
End Matrices;
Matrix X .5477
Matrix Y .4472
Matrix Z .7071
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
237
238 CHAPTER B. MX SCRIPT FOR POWER CALCULATION
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: MZ twin pairs
Calculation
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E ;
Options MX%E=mzsim.cov
End
G3: DZ twin pairs
Calculation
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Covariances A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E ;
Options MX%E=dzsim.cov
End
! Fit the wrong model to the simulated data
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Y Lower 1 1 Fixed ! common environmental structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: MZ twin pairs
B.1. ACE MODEL FOR POWER CALCULATIONS 239
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1000
CMatrix Full File=mzsim.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1000
CMatrix Full File=dzsim.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start .5 All
Covariances A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E ;
Options RSiduals Power= .05,1 ! .05 sig level & 1 df
End
240 CHAPTER B. MX SCRIPT FOR POWER CALCULATION
Chapter C
Mx Script for Sibling
Interaction Model
C.1 Sibling Interaction Model
The following Mx script represents a univariate genetic model incorporating sibling
interaction tted to covariance matrices for MZ and DZ pairs reared together.
! Sibling Interaction Model
! CBC data in US twins
#NGroups 3
G1: genetic structure
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
B Symm 2 2 ! sibling interaction parameters
I Iden 2 2
End Matrices;
Specify B 0 3 0
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
P= (I-B)~ ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: Male MZ twins in larger families
241
242 CHAPTER C. MX SCRIPT FOR SIBLING INTERACTION MODEL
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=171
Labels exter1 exter2
CMatrix Symmetric File=uscbcmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances P* (A+E | A _
A | A+E) * P ;
Options RSidual
End
G3: Male DZ twins in larger families
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=194
Labels exter1 exter2
CMatrix Symmetric File=uscbcdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start .3 All
Start .0 B 1 2 1
Covariances P* (A+E | H@A _
H@A | A+E) * P ;
Options NDecimals=4
Options RSidual
End
Chapter D
Mx Scripts for Sex and GE
Interaction
D.1 General Model for Scalar Sex-Limitation
This Mx script estimates sex-dependent additive genetic eects and xes the sex-
dependent dominance eects to zero. Four same-sex groups are used: MZ female,
DZ female, MZ male, and DZ male.
! General Model for Sex-Limitation
! BMI data in US twins
#NGroups 7
G1: female model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: male model parameters
Calculation
243
244 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance structure
N Lower 1 1 Free ! male set of genes
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
M= N*N ;
End Algebra;
End
G3: Female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1802
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmimzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G4: Female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1142
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .5 All
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G5: Male MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=750
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
D.1. GENERAL MODEL FOR SCALAR SEX-LIMITATION 245
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmimzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
Covariances A+D+E+M | A+D+M _
A+D+M | A+D+E+M ;
Option RSiduals
End
G6: Male DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=553
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
H Full 1 1 =H4
Q Full 1 1 =Q4
Covariances A+D+E+M | H@A+Q@D+H@M _
H@A+Q@D+H@M | A+D+E+M ;
Option RSiduals
End
G7: Female-Male DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1341
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzo.cov
Begin Matrices;
A Symm 1 1 =A1
D Symm 1 1 =D1
E Symm 1 1 =E1
J Symm 1 1 =A2
K Symm 1 1 =D2
L Symm 1 1 =E2
M Symm 1 1 =M2
X Lower 1 1 =X1
W Lower 1 1 =W1
Z Lower 1 1 =Z1
O Lower 1 1 =X2
P Lower 1 1 =W2
R Lower 1 1 =Z2
N Lower 1 1 =N2
H Full 1 1 =H4
Q Full 1 1 =Q4
End Matrices;
Start .5 All
Covariances
246 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
A+D+E | H@(X*O)+Q@(W*P) _
H@(O*X)+Q@(P*W) | J+K+L+M /
Option RSiduals
End
D.2 Scalar Sex-Limitation Model
This Mx script ts a model in which genetic and environmental factors are propor-
tional across the sexes, so that a
m
= ka
f
; d
m
= kd
f
; and e
m
= ke
f
.
! Scalar Sex-Limitation Model
! BMI data in US twins
#NGroups 6
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance structure
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: Female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1802
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmimzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: Female DZ twin pairs
D.2. SCALAR SEX-LIMITATION MODEL 247
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1142
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G4: Male MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=750
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmimzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
K Diag 2 2 ! common multiplier
End Matrices;
Specify K 4 4
Covariances K *(A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E) *K ;
Option RSiduals
End
G5: Male DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=553
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
K Diag 2 2 = K4
Covariances K *(A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E) *K ;
Option RSiduals
End
G6: Female-Male DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=1341
Labels bmi_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=usbmidzo.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
K Diag 2 2
End Matrices;
Specify K 0 4
Matrix K 1 0
Bound 0 1 K 4 1 1 K 4 2 2 K 5 1 1 K 5 2 2 K 6 2 2
248 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
Start .7 All
Covariances K *(A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E) *K ;
Option RSiduals
Options NDecimals=4
End
D.3 General Model for G E Interaction
This Mx script ts a G E interaction model in which the environmental agent is
dichotomous. Thus we discriminate between concordant exposed, discordant, and
concordant non-exposed pairs of (i) MZ and (ii) DZ twins, giving six groups in
total.
! General Model for GxE Interaction
! Depression in Australian twins
#NGroups 8
G1: singles model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: married model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Free ! dominance structure
N Lower 1 1 Free ! set of genes for married couples
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D.3. GENERAL MODEL FOR G E INTERACTION 249
D= W*W ;
M= N&N ;
End Algebra;
End
G3: Concordant single MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=254
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepsmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G4: Concordant single DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=155
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepsdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G5: Concordant married MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=177
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepmmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
Covariances A+D+E+M | A+D+M _
A+D+M | A+D+E+M ;
Option RSiduals
End
G6: Concordant married DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=107
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
250 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepmdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
H Full 1 1 = H4
Q Full 1 1 = Q4
Covariances A+D+E+M | H@A+Q@D+H@M _
H@A+Q@D+H@M | A+D+E+M ;
Option RSiduals
End
G7: Discordant MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=139
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepdmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
J Symm 1 1 =A2
K Symm 1 1 =D2
L Symm 1 1 =E2
M Symm 1 1 =M2
O Lower 1 1 =X2
P Lower 1 1 =W2
R Lower 1 1 =Z2
N Lower 1 1 =N2
End Matrices;
Covariances A+D+E | (X*O)+(W*P) _
(O*X)+(P*W) | J+K+L+M ;
Option RSiduals
End
G8: Discordant DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=87
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepddz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 7;
H Full 1 1 = H4
Q Full 1 1 = Q4
End Matrices;
Start .7 All
Covariances A+D+E | H@(X*O)+Q@(W*P) _
H@(O*X)+Q@(P*W) | J+K+L+M ;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
End
D.4. SCALAR G E INTERACTION MODEL 251
D.4 Scalar G E interaction model
The following Mx script ts a model in which there is a proportionate change of the
multifactorial genetic and environmental eect between exposed and non-exposed
individuals.
! Scalar GxE Interaction Model
! Depression data in Australian female twins
#NGroups 7
G1: singles model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic structure
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific environmental structure
W Lower 1 1 Fixed ! dominance structure
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
D= W*W ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: Concordant single MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=254
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepsmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: Concordant single DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=155
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepsdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
252 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G4: Concordant married MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=177
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepmmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
K Diag 2 2 ! scalar
End Matrices;
Specify K 4 4
Covariances K *(A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E) *K ;
Option RSiduals
End
G5: Concordant married DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=107
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepmdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1 = H3
Q Full 1 1 = Q3
K Diag 2 2 = K4
Covariances K *(A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ) *K ;
Option RSiduals
End
G6: Discordant MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=139
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepdmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
K Diag 2 2
End Matrices;
Specify K 0 4
Matrix K 1 .7
Covariances K *(A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ) *K ;
Option RSiduals
D.4. SCALAR G E INTERACTION MODEL 253
End
G7: Discordant DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=2 NObservations=87
Labels dep_t1 bmi-t2
CMatrix Symmetric File=ozdepddz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1 = H3
Q Full 1 1 = Q3
K Diag 2 2 = K6
End Matrices;
Start .5 All
Covariances K *(A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ) *K ;
Option RSiduals
Options NDecimals=4
End
254 CHAPTER D. MX SCRIPTS FOR SEX AND GE INTERACTION
Chapter E
Mx Scripts for Multivariate
Models
E.1 Phenotypic Factor Analysis of Four Variables
This Mx script performs a phenotypic factor analysis to arithmetic computation
data from Australian female twins. The data comprise assessments taken once
before (T0) and three times after (T1 T3) a standard dose of alcohol.
#NGroups 1
Phenotypic Factor Analysis of Four Variables
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=42
CMatrix
259.664
209.325 259.939
209.532 220.755 245.235
221.610 221.491 221.317 249.298
Labels Time1 Time2 Time3 Time4
Begin Matrices;
B Full 1 4 Free
P Symm 1 1
E Diag 4 4 Free
End Matrices;
Value 1 P 1 1
Start 9 All
Covariances B*P*B+E;
Option RSiduals
End
255
256 CHAPTER E. MX SCRIPTS FOR MULTIVARIATE MODELS
E.2 Genetic Factor Model
The following Mx script ts the genetic common factor model as described in Chap-
ter 10 for additive (A), common environmental (C), and non-shared environment
(E) eects to arithmetic computation data from Australian female twins.
! Genetic Factor Model
! Arithmetic Computation after Alcohol Administration in Australian female twins
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Full 4 1 Free ! genetic common factor
Y Full 4 1 Free ! shared environmental common factor
Z Full 4 1 Free ! specific environmental common factor
F Diag 4 4 Free ! specific environmental specifics
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z+ F*F ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=43
Labels Tw1-T0 Tw1-T1 Tw1-T2 Tw1-T3 Tw2-T0 Tw2-T1 Tw2-T2 Tw2-T3
CMatrix File=ozarimz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=44
Labels Tw1-T0 Tw1-T1 Tw1-T2 Tw1-T3 Tw2-T0 Tw2-T1 Tw2-T2 Tw2-T3
Cmatrix File=ozaridz.cov
Begin Matrices = Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start .5 All
E.3. BIVARIATE GENETIC FACTOR MODEL 257
Covariances A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E ;
Option RSiduals
Options Multiple NDecimals=4
End
! no shared environmental common factor
Drop 5 to 8
End
E.3 Bivariate Genetic Factor Model
The Mx script below adds a genetic alcohol factor to the common factors for
A and E in Appendix ??. Genetic eects on the three arithmetic computation
measurements taken after alcohol administration load on the alcohol factor.
! Bivariate Genetic Factor Model
! Arithmetic Computation after Alcohol Administration in Australian female twins
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Full 4 2 Free ! genetic common factor
Y Full 4 1 Fixed ! shared environmental common factor
Z Full 4 1 Free ! specific environmental common factor
F Diag 4 4 Free ! specific environmental specifics
End Matrices;
Specify X
1 0
2 100 ! 100: second genetic common factor
3 100
4 100
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z+ F*F ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=43
Labels Tw1-T0 Tw1-T1 Tw1-T2 Tw1-T3 Tw2-T0 Tw2-T1 Tw2-T2 Tw2-T3
258 CHAPTER E. MX SCRIPTS FOR MULTIVARIATE MODELS
CMatrix File=ozarimz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=44
Labels Tw1-T0 Tw1-T1 Tw1-T2 Tw1-T3 Tw2-T0 Tw2-T1 Tw2-T2 Tw2-T3
Cmatrix File=ozaridz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start .5 All
Covariances A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E ;
Option RSidual NDecimals=4
End
E.4 Genetic Cholesky Model
The Mx script below ts a cholesky decomposition model to four skinfold measures.
Triangular cholesky matrices are t only for additive genetic, A, and within-family
environment, E, eects.
! Genetic Cholesky Model
! Skinfold Measures in US (Virginia) male twins
#NGroups 3
G1: genetic structure
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 4 4 Free ! genetic cholesky (lower triangular)
Z Lower 4 4 Free ! specific environmental cholesky
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
E= Z*Z ;
End Algebra;
End
E.5. INDEPENDENT PATHWAY MODEL 259
G2: MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=84
Labels Bicep1 Subsca1 Supra1 Tricep1 Bicep2 Subsca2 Supra2 Tricep2
Cmatrix File=usskfmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+E | A _
A | A+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=33
Labels Bicep1 Subsca1 Supra1 Tricep1 Bicep2 Subsca2 Supra2 Tricep2
Cmatrix File=usskfdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start .5 All
Covariances A+E | H@A _
H@A | A+E ;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
End
E.5 Independent Pathway Model
This Mx script ts the multivariate independent pathway model to Australian
twin data on asthma, hayfever, dust allergy, and eczema. The data are ordinal,
thus, polychoric correlations are modeled, using asymptotic weight matrices pro-
vided by PRELIS.
! Independent Pathway Model
! Asthma, Hayfever, Dust allergy, Eczema in Australian female twins
#NGroups 3
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Full 4 1 Free ! genetic common factor
W Full 4 1 Free ! dominance common factor
Z Full 4 1 Free ! specific environmental common factor
G Diag 4 4 Free ! genetic specifics
F Diag 4 4 Free ! specific environmental specifics
260 CHAPTER E. MX SCRIPTS FOR MULTIVARIATE MODELS
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X+ G*G ;
D= W*W ;
E= Z*Z+ F*F ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=1232
Labels asthma1 hayfvr1 dustal1 eczema1 asthma2 hayfvr2 dustal2 eczema2
PMatrix File=ozastmz.cov
ACov File=ahdemzf.acv
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=751
Labels asthma1 hayfvr1 dustal1 eczema1 asthma2 hayfvr2 dustal2 eczema2
Pmatrix File=ozastdz.cov
ACov File=ahdedzf.acv
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .4 All
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
Option DFreedom=-12
End
E.6 Common Pathway Model
The following Mx script ts the common pathway model to ordinal data on
asthma, hayfever, dust allergy, and eczema from the Australian twin sample.
Asymptotic weight matrices and polychoric correlations were obtained from PRELIS.
E.6. COMMON PATHWAY MODEL 261
! Common Pathway Model
! Asthma, Hayfever, Dust allergy, Eczema in Australian female twins
#NGroups 4
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic factor on latent phenotype
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific env factor on latent phenotype
W Lower 1 1 Fixed ! dominance factor on latent phenotype
B Diag 4 4 Free ! genetic specifics
F Diag 4 4 Free ! specific environmental specifics
S Full 4 1 Free ! factor structure
I Iden 2 2
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
A= S&(X*X) + B*B ;
E= S&(Z*Z) + F*F ;
D= S&(W*W) ;
L= X*X + Y*Y + Z*Z;
End Algebra;
End
G2: female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=1232
Labels asthma1 hayfvr1 dustal1 eczema1 asthma2 hayfvr2 dustal2 eczema2
PMatrix File=ozastmz.cov
ACov File=ahdemzf.acv
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances A+D+E | A+D _
A+D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=8 NObservations=751
Labels asthma1 hayfvr1 dustal1 eczema1 asthma2 hayfvr2 dustal2 eczema2
Pmatrix File=ozastdz.cov
ACov File=ahdedzf.acv
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Q Full 1 1
End Matrices;
262 CHAPTER E. MX SCRIPTS FOR MULTIVARIATE MODELS
Matrix H .5
Matrix Q .25
Start .3 All
Covariances A+D+E | H@A+Q@D _
H@A+Q@D | A+D+E ;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
Option DFreedom=-12
End
G4: Constrain variance of latent factor to 1
Constraint
Begin Matrices;
L computed =L1
I unit 1 1
End Matrices;
Constraint L = I ;
End
Chapter F
Mx Script for Rater Bias
Model
F.1 Rater Bias Model
The Mx script below ts a rater bias model to parental ratings of young childrens
Child Behavior Checklist internalizing behavior problems. The script represents
a ve group problem, for MZ-male, MZ-female, DZ-male, DZ-female, and DZ-
opposite sex twins.
! Rater Bias Model
! CBC data in US twins
#NGroups 7
G1: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic factor on latent phenotype
Y Lower 1 1 Free ! common env factor on latent phenotype
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific env factor on latent phenotype
B Full 4 2 ! rater bias factors
F Diag 4 4 ! specific environmental specifics
S Diag 2 2 ! factor structure
I Iden 2 2
End Matrices;
Specify B 4 0 4 0 0 5 0 5
Specify F 6 6 7 7
Specify S 15 15
Begin Algebra;
263
264 CHAPTER F. MX SCRIPT FOR RATER BIAS MODEL
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
R= B*B;
J= F*F ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: model parameters
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Lower 1 1 Free ! genetic factor on latent phenotype
Y Lower 1 1 Free ! common env factor on latent phenotype
Z Lower 1 1 Free ! specific env factor on latent phenotype
B Full 4 2 ! rater bias factors
F Diag 4 4 ! specific environmental specifics
S Diag 2 2 ! factor structure
I Iden 2 2
End Matrices;
Specify B 11 0 11 0 0 12 0 12
Specify F 13 13 14 14
Specify S 19 19
Begin Algebra;
A= X*X ;
C= Y*Y ;
E= Z*Z ;
R= B*B;
J= F*F ;
End Algebra;
End
G3: MZ boys twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=96
Labels morg_t1 morg_t2 farg_t1 farg_t2
CMatrix File=uscbcmzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
Covariances R+ J+ ((I_S)*(A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E )*(I_S)) ;
Option RSiduals
End
G4: DZ boys twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=102
F.1. RATER BIAS MODEL 265
Labels morg_t1 morg_t2 farg_t1 farg_t2
CMatrix File=uscbcdzm.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
Covariances R+ J+ ((I_S)*(A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E )*(I_S)) ;
Matrix H .5
Option RSiduals
End
G5: MZ girls twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=102
Labels morg_t1 morg_t2 farg_t1 farg_t2
CMatrix File=uscbcmzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
Covariances R+ J+ ((I_S)*(A+C+E | A+C _
A+C | A+C+E )*(I_S)) ;
Option RSiduals
End
G6: DZ girls twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=97
Labels morg_t1 morg_t2 farg_t1 farg_t2
CMatrix File=uscbcdzf.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 2;
H Full 1 1 = H4
Covariances R+ J+ ((I_S)*(A+C+E | H@A+C _
H@A+C | A+C+E )*(I_S)) ;
Option RSiduals
End
G7: DZ girl-boy twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=4 NObservations=103
Labels morg_t1 morg_t2 farg_t1 farg_t2
CMatrix File=uscbcdzo.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1
M Symm 1 1 = A2
N Symm 1 1 = C2
O Symm 1 1 = E2
S Diag 2 2
R Full 4 2
J Diag 4 4
H Full 1 1 = H4
266 CHAPTER F. MX SCRIPT FOR RATER BIAS MODEL
Covariances
R*R+ J*J+ ((I_S)* (M+N+O | H@(\sqrt(A*M))+\sqrt(C*N) _
H@(\sqrt(A*M))+\sqrt(C*N) | A+C+E)* (I_S)) /
Specify S 19 15
Specify R 11 0 4 0 0 12 0 5
Specify J 13 6 14 7
Start .2 All
Start .5 X 1 1 1 Y 1 1 1 Z 1 1 1 X 2 1 1 Y 2 1 1 Z 2 1 1
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
End
F.2 CBC Items for Internalizing Scale Score
Below are the core items of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBC: Achenbach, 1988)
assessing childrens internalizing behaviors.
1. Cant get his/her mind o certain thoughts, obsessions (describe):
2. Fears going to school
3. Fears he/she might do something bad
4. Feels he/she has to be perfect
5. Hears sounds or voices that arent there (describe):
6. Too fearful or anxious
7. Feels dizzy
8. Feels too guilty
9. Overtired
10. Aches or pains
11. Headaches
12. Nausea, feels sick
13. Stomach-aches or cramps
14. Vomiting, throwing up
15. Refuses to talk
16. Secretive, keeps things to self
17. Self-conscious or easily embarassed
F.2. CBC ITEMS FOR INTERNALIZING SCALE SCORE 267
18. Stares blankly
19. Strange behavior (describe):
20. Strange ideas (describe):
21. Sulks a lot
22. Unhappy, sad or depressed
23. Worrying
268 CHAPTER F. MX SCRIPT FOR RATER BIAS MODEL
Chapter G
Mx Script and Data for
Simplex Model
G.1 Phenotypic Simplex Model
The Mx script below shows two alternative parameterizations of the phenotypic
simplex model applied to the within-person covariances among weight measure-
ments at successive six-month intervals from 66 females obtained from Fischbeins
(1977) sample of opposite-sex DZ twin pairs.
! Phenotypic Simplex Model (estimate innovations)
! Weight data in Swedisch twins
#NGroups 1
#Define nvar 6
G1: Longitudinal Data
Data NInputvars=6 NObservations=66
Labels wt_1 wt_2 wt_3 wt_4 wt_5 wt_6
CMatrix Symmetric File=swwt.cov
Begin Matrices;
S Lower nvar nvar
I Iden nvar nvar
D Diag nvar nvar Free
E Diag nvar nvar
L Iden nvar nvar
End Matrices;
Specify S
0
7 0
269
270 CHAPTER G. MX SCRIPT AND DATA FOR SIMPLEX MODEL
0 8 0
0 0 9 0
0 0 0 10 0
0 0 0 0 11 0
Specify E
12 12 12 12 12 12
Matrix D 7 1 1 1 1 1
Start 1 E 1 1 to E nvar nvar
Begin Algebra;
T= \stnd((I-S)~ *(D*D) *((I-S)~));
End Algebra;
Covariances E*E +L *((I-S)~ *(D*D) *((I-S)~)) *L;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=3
End
! Phenotypic Simplex Model (estimate factor loadings)
! Weight data in Swedisch twins
#NGroups 1
#Define nvar 6
G1: Longitudinal Data
Data NInputvars=6 NObservations=66
Labels wt_1 wt_2 wt_3 wt_4 wt_5 wt_6
CMatrix Symmetric File=swwt.cov
Begin Matrices;
S Lower nvar nvar
I Iden nvar nvar
D Iden nvar nvar
E Diag nvar nvar
L Diag nvar nvar Free
End Matrices;
Specify S
0
7 0
0 8 0
0 0 9 0
0 0 0 10 0
0 0 0 0 11 0
Specify E
12 12 12 12 12 12
Matrix L 7 1 1 1 1 1
Start 1 E 1 1 to E nvar nvar
Begin Algebra;
T= \stnd((I-S)~ *(D*D) *((I-S)~));
G.2. GENETIC SIMPLEX MODEL 271
U= (L*(I-S)~ *((I-S)~)*L);
B= (I-S)~ *((I-S)~);
End Algebra;
Covariances E*E +L *((I-S)~ *(D*D) *((I-S)~)) *L;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=3
End
G.2 Genetic Simplex Model
The following Mx script ts a simplex model to additive genetic (G) and non-shared
environmental (E) eects over 5 successive six-month intervals.
! Genetic Simplex Model
! Weight data in Swedisch twins
#NGroups 3
#Define nvar 6
#Define nvarm1 5
G1: genetic and environmental structure
Calculation
Begin Matrices;
X Diag nvar nvar Free ! genetic innovation paths
B Diag nvarm1 nvarm1 Free ! genetic transmission paths
Z Diag nvar nvar Free ! specific env innovation paths
D Diag nvarm1 nvarm1 Free ! specific env transmission paths
I Iden nvar nvar
J Zero 1 nvarm1
L Zero nvar 1
End Matrices;
Begin Algebra;
G= (J_B)|L ;
T= (I-G)~ ;
A= T*(X*X)*T ;
F= (J_D)|L ;
U= (I-F)~ ;
E= U*(Z*Z)*U ;
End Algebra;
End
G2: young female MZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=12 NObservations=32
CMatrix Symmetric File=swwtmz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
272 CHAPTER G. MX SCRIPT AND DATA FOR SIMPLEX MODEL
Covariances A+E | A _
A | A+E ;
Option RSiduals
End
G3: young female DZ twin pairs
Data NInput_vars=12 NObservations=51
CMatrix Symmetric File=swwtdz.cov
Begin Matrices= Group 1;
H Full 1 1
End Matrices;
Matrix H .5
Start 4 All
Start .4 B 1 1 1 - B 1 5 5 D 1 1 1 - D 1 5 5
Covariances A+E | H@A _
H@A | A+E ;
Option RSiduals NDecimals=4
End
Bibliography
Achenbach, T. M. & Edelbrock, C. S. (1981). Behavior problems and competencies
reported by parents of normal and disturbed children age four through sixteen.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Number 188
in 46.
Achenbach, T. M. & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child Gehavior
Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Prole. Burlington, VT: University of
Vermont Dept. of Psychiatry.
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent
behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correla-
tions for situational specicity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213232.
Aitken, A. C. (1934). Note on selection from a multivariate normal population.
Proceedings of the Edinburgh Mathematical Society B, 4, 106110.
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and aic. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.
Arnold, S. J. (1990). Inheritance and the evolution of behavioral ontogenies. In M.
Hahn, J. Hewitt, N. Henderson, & R. Benno (Eds.), Developmental Behavior
Genetics. Neural, Biometrical, and Evolutionary Approaches, (pp. 167189).
Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (1977). Alcohol and tobacco consumption patterns:
February 1977 (catalogue no. 4312.0. ed.). Australian Bureau of Statistics.
Bedford, A., Foulds, G. A., & Sheeld, B. F. (1976). A new personal disturbance
scale (DSSI/SAD). British Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, 15, 387394.
Bock, R. D. & Vandenberg, S. G. (1968). Components of heritable variation in
mental test scores. In S. G. Vandenberg (Ed.), Progress in human behavior
genetics, (pp. 233260). The Johns Hopkins Press: Baltimore.
Bodmer, W. F. (1987). HLA, immune response, and disease. In F. Vogel & K. Sper-
ling (Eds.), Human Genetics: Proceedings of the 7th international congress,
(pp. 107113). Springer-Verlag: New York.
273
274 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York: John
Wiley.
Boomsma, D. I., Martin, N. G., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1989a). Factor and simplex
models for repeated measures: Application to two psychomotor measures of
alcohol sensitivity in twins. Behavior Genetics, 19, 7996.
Boomsma, D. I. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1986). Using lisrel to analyze genetic and
environmental covariance structure. Behavior Genetics, 16, 237250.
Boomsma, D. I. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1987). The genetic analysis of repeated
measures. Behavior Genetics, 17, 111123.
Boomsma, D. I., Van Den Bree, M. B., Orlebeke, J. F., & Molenaar, P. C. M.
(1989b). Resemblances of parents and twins in sports participation and heart
rate. Behavior Genetics, 19, 123141.
Boyer, C. B. (1985). A history of mathematics. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton
University Press.
Bray, J. A. (1976). The Obese Patient. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Cantor, R. M. (1983). A multivariate genetic analysis of ridge count data from the
ospring of monozygotic twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 32,
161208.
Cardon, L. R. (1992). Multivariate Path Analysis of Specic Cognitive Abilities in
the Colorado Adoption Project. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado.
Cardon, L. R. & Fulker, D. W. (1991). Sources of continuity in infant predictors
of adult IQ. Intelligence, 15, 279293.
Cardon, L. R. & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Genetic inuences on body fat from birth
to age 9. Genetic Epidemiology. (in press).
Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W., DeFries, J. C., & Plomin, R. (1992). Continuity
and change in general cognitive ability from 1 to 7 years. Developmental
Psychology. (in press).
Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W., & J oreskog, K. G. (1991). A LISREL model with
constrained parameters for twin and adoptive families. Behavior Genetics, 21,
327350.
Carey, G. (1986a). A general multivariate approach to linear modeling in human
genetics. American Journal of Human Genetics, 39, 775786.
Carey, G. (1986b). Sibling imitation and contrast eects. Behavior Genetics, (pp.
319341).
BIBLIOGRAPHY 275
Carey, G. (1988). Inference about genetic correlations. Behavior Genetics, 18,
329338.
Castle, W. E. (1903). The law of heredity of Galton and Mendel and some laws gov-
erning race improvement by selection. Proceedings of the American Academy
of Sciences, 39, 233242.
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of uid and crystallized intelligence: A critical
experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 122.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution:
A Quantitative Approach. Princeton: Princeton University.
Cloninger, C. R. (1980). Interpretation of intrinsic and extrinsic structural rela-
tions by path analysis: Theory and application to assortative mating. Genetic
Research, 36, 133145.
Cloninger, C. R., Rice, J., & Reich, T. (1979a). Multifactorial inheritance with
cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. A general model of combined
polygenic and cultural inheritance. American Journal of Human Genetics, 31,
176198.
Cloninger, C. R., Rice, J., & Reich, T. (1979b). Multifactorial inheritance with
cultural transmission and assortative mating. III. Family structure and the
analysis of separation experiments. American Journal of Human Genetics, 31,
366388.
Corey, L. A., Eaves, L. J., Mellen, B. G., & Nance, W. E. (1986). Testing for de-
velopmental changes in gene expression on resemblance for quantitative traits
in kinships of monozygotic twins. Genetic Epidemiology, 3, 7383.
Cox, A. & Rutter, M. (1985). Diagnostic appraisal and interviewing. In M. Rutter
& L. Hersor (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry. Blackwell: Oxford, (2nd
ed.).
Crow, J. F. & Kimura, M. (1970). Introduction to Population Genetics Theory.
New York: Harper and Row.
Darlington, C. D. (1971). Axiom and process in genetics. Nature, 234, 131133.
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype: The Gene as the Unit of Selection.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Duy, D. L. & Martin, N. G. (1992). Inferring the direction of causation in cross-
sectional twin data: theoretical and empirical considerations. Genetic Epi-
demiology, In press.
276 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Duy, D. L., Martin, N. G., Battistutta, D., Hopper, J. L., & Mathews, J. D.
(1990). Genetics of asthma and hayfever in australian twins. American Review
of Respiratory Disease, 142, 13511358.
Eaves, L. J. (1976a). The eect of cultural transmission on continuous variation.
Heredity, 37, 4157.
Eaves, L. J. (1976b). A model for sibling eects in man. Heredity, 36, 205214.
Eaves, L. J. (1982). The utility of twins. In V. Anderson, et al (Ed.), Genetic
Bases of the Epilepsies. New York: Raven Press.
Eaves, L. J., Fulker, D. W., & Heath, A. C. (1989). The eects of social homogamy
and cultural inheritance on the covariances of twins and their parents. Behavior
Genetics, 19, 113122.
Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., Hewitt, J. K., & Martin, N. G. (1992).
Sex dierences and non-additivity in the eects of genes on personality. Psy-
chological Science. (in press).
Eaves, L. J., Hewitt, J. K., Meyer, J. M., & Neale, M. C. (1990). Approaches
to quantitative genetic modeling of development and age-related changes. In
M. E. Hahn, J. K. Hewitt, N. D. Henderson, & R. Benno (Eds.), Developmental
Behavior Genetics. Neural, Biometrical and Evolutionary Approaches, (pp.
266277). Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Eaves, L. J., Last, K. A., Martin, N. G., & Jinks, J. L. (1977). A progressive ap-
proach to non-additivity and genotype-environmental covariance in the anal-
ysis of human dierences. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
Psychology, 30, 142.
Eaves, L. J., Last, K. A., Young, P. A., & Martin, N. G. (1978). Model-tting
approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Heredity, 41, 249320.
Eaves, L. J., Long, J., & Heath, A. C. (1986). A theory of developmental change in
quantitative phenotypes applied to cognitive development. Behavior Genetics,
16, 143162.
Eaves, L. J., Neale, M. C., & Meyer, J. M. (1991). A model for comparative ratings
in studies of within-family dierences. Behavior Genetics, 21, 531536.
Falconer, D. S. (1960). Quantitative Genetics. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.
Falconer, D. S. (1990). Introduction to Quantitative Genetics (3rd ed.). New York:
Longman Group Ltd.
Fischbein, S. (1977). Intra-pair similarity in physical growth of monozygotic and
of dizygotic twins during puberty. Annals of Human Biology, 4, 417430.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 277
Fisher, R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the supposition of
Mendelian inheritance. Translations of the Royal Society, Edinburgh, 52, 399
433.
Fisher, R. A. (1920). A mathematical examination of the methods of determining
the accuracy of an observation by the mean error, and by the mean square
error. Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society, 80, 758770.
Fulker, D. W. (1982). Extensions of the classical twin method. In Human genetics,
part A: The unfolding genome (pp. 395406). New York: Alan R. Liss.
Fulker, D. W. (1988). Genetic and cultural transmission in human behavior. In
B. S. Weir, E. J. Eisen, M. M. Goodman, & G. Namkoong (Eds.), Proceedings
of the second international conference on quantitative genetics (pp. 318340).
Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Fulker, D. W., Baker, L. A., & Bock, R. D. (1983). Estimating components of
covariance using LISREL. Data Analyst, 1, 58.
Fuller, J. L. & Thompson, W. R. (1978). Foundations of Behavior Genetics. St.
Louis: C. V. Mosby.
Gill, P. E., Murray, W., , & Wright, M. H. (1981). Practical Optimization. New
York: Academic Press.
Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor Analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Graybill, F. A. (1969). Introduction to Matrices with Applications in Statistics.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.
Grayson, D. A. (1989). Twins reared together: Minimizing shared environmental
eects. Behavior Genetics, 19, 593603.
Grilo, C. M. & Pogue-Guile, M. F. (1991). The nature of environmental inuences
on weight and obesity: A behavior genetic analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
110, 520537.
Guttman, L. (1954). A new approach to factor analysis: The radex. In P. F.
Lazarsfeld (Ed.), Mathematical thinking in the social sciences, (pp. 258349).
Free Press: Glencoe, Ill.
Haley, C. S., Jinks, J. L., & Last, K. (1981). The monozygotic twin half-sib method
for analyzing maternal eects and sex-linkage in humans. Heredity, 46, 227
238.
Harman, H. H. (1976). Modern Factor Analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
278 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Heath, A. C. (1983). Human Quantitative Genetics: Some Issues and Applications.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oxford, Oxford, England.
Heath, A. C. (1987). The analysis of marital interaction in cross-sectional twin
data. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 36, 4149.
Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., & Martin, N. G. (1989a). Interactive eects of genotype
and social environment on alcohol consumption in female twins. Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 50, 3848.
Heath, A. C., Kendler, K. S., Eaves, L. J., & Markell, D. (1985). The resolution of
cultural and biological inheritance: Informativeness of dierent relationships.
Behavior Genetics, 15, 439465.
Heath, A. C. & Martin, N. G. (1986). Detecting the eects of genotype envi-
ronment interaction on personality and symptoms of anxiety and depression.
Behavior Genetics, 16, 622.
Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., Hewitt, J. K., Eaves, L. J., & Fulker, D. W. (1989b).
Testing structural equation models for twin data using LISREL. Behavior
Genetics, 19, 936.
Heise, D. R. (1975). Causal Analysis. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Helzer, J. E., Robins, L. N., Taibleson, M., Woodru, R. A., Reich, T., & Wish,
E. D. (1977). Reliability of psychiatric diagnosis. Archives of General Psychi-
atry, 34, 129133.
Hewitt, J. K. (1989). Of biases and more in the study of twins reared together: A
reply to Grayson. Behavior Genetics, 19, 605610.
Hewitt, J. K., Eaves, L. J., Neale, M. C., & Meyer, J. M. (1988). Resolving causes
of developmental continuity or tracking. I. Longitudinal twin studies during
growth. Behavior Genetics, 18, 133151.
Hewitt, J. K., Silberg, J. L., & Erickson, M. (1990). Genetic and environmental
inuences on internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in childhood
and adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 20, 725. (abstract).
Hewitt, J. K., Silberg, J. L., Neale, M. C., Eaves, L. J., & Erickson, M. (1992).
The analysis of parental ratings of childrens behavior using LISREL. Behavior
Genetics. (in press).
IMSL (1987). IMSL Users Manual. Version 1.0. Houston, Texas: IMSL, Inc.
Jardine, R. (1985). A twin study of personality, social attitudes, and drinking
behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Australian National University,
Australia.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 279
Jerey, D. B. & Knauss, M. R. (1981). The etiologies, treatments, and assessments
of obesity. In S. N. Haynes & L. Gannon (Eds.), Psychosomatic disorders: A
psychophysiological apprach to etiology and treatment. New York: Praeger.
Jencks, C. (1972). Inequality: A Reassessment of the Eect of Family and Schooling
in America. New York: Basic Books.
Jinks, J. L. & Fulker, D. W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical genetical,
MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 73, 311349.
J oreskog, K. G. (1970). Estimation and testing of simplex models. British Journal
of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 23, 121145.
J oreskog, K. G. & S orbom, D. (1988). PRELIS - A Program for Multivariate Data
Screening and Data Summarization. A Preprocessor for LISREL (second ed.).
Mooresville, Indiana: Scientic Software, Inc.
J oreskog, K. G. & S orbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A Guide to the Program and
Applications (2nd ed.). Chicago: SPSS, Inc.
Kempthorne, O. (1960). Biometrical Genetics. New York: Pergammon Press.
Kendler, K. S., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., & Eaves, L. J. (1986). Symptoms of
anxiety and depression in a volunteer twin population: The etiologic role of
genetic and environmental factors. Archives General Psychiatry, 43, 213221.
Kendler, K. S., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., & Eaves, L. J. (1987). Symptoms
of anxiety and symptoms of depression: Same genes, dierent environments?
Archives General Psychiatry, 44, 451457.
Kendler, K. S. & Kidd, K. K. (1986). Recurrence risks in an oligogenic threshold
model: The eect of alterations in allele frequency. Annals Human Genetics,
50, 8391.
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., Kessler, R. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1991).
Life events and depressive symptoms: A twin study perspective. In P. McGuf-
n & R. Murray (Eds.), The New Genetics of Mental Illness (pp. 144162).
London: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1992a).
Childhood parental loss and adult psychopathology in women: A twin study
perspective. Archives General Psychiatry, 49, 109116.
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J.
(1992b). Generalized anxiety disorder in women: A population based twin
study. Archives General Psychiatry. (in press).
280 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1992c).
Major depression and generalized anxiety disorder: Same genes, (partly) dif-
ferent environments? Archives General Psychiatry. (in press).
Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and Causality. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Lawley, D. N. & Maxwell, A. E. (1971). Factor Analysis as a Statistical Method.
London: Butterworths.
Li, C. C. (1975). Path Analysis: A Primer. Pacic Grove, CA: Boxwood Press.
Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1989). Optimal
informants on childhood disruptive behaviors. Developmental Psychopathology,
1, 317337.
Loehlin, J. C. (1987). Latent Variable Models. Baltimore: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Loehlin, J. C. & Vandenberg, S. G. (1968). Genetic and environmental compo-
nents in the covariation of cognitive abilities: An additive model. In S. G.
Vandenberg (Ed.), Progress in human behavior genetics, (pp. 261285). Johns
Hopkins University Press: Baltimore.
Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., & Tellegen, A. (1987). Recruitment bias in twin
research: the rule of two-thirds reconsidered. Behavior Genetics, 17, 343362.
Lytton, H. (1977). Do parents create, or respond to dierences in twins? Develop-
mental Psychology, 13, 456459.
Mardia, K. V., Kent, J. T., & Bibby, J. M. (1979). Multivariate Analysis. New
York: Academic Press.
Martin, N. G. & Eaves, L. J. (1977). The genetical analysis of covariance structure.
Heredity, 38, 7995.
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., Feindgold, L. M., & Eysenck,
H. J. (1986). Transmission of social attitudes. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science, 83, 43644368.
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Kearsey, M. J., & Davies, P. (1978). The power of the
classical twin study. Heredity, 40, 97116.
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., & Loesch, D. Z. (1982). A genetical analysis of
covariation between nger ridge counts. Annals Human Biology, 9, 539552.
Martin, N. G. & Jardine, R. (1986). Eysencks contribution to behavior genetics.
In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), Hans Eysenck: Consensus and Controversy.
Falmer Press: Lewes, Sussex.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 281
Martin, N. G., Oakeshott, J. G., Gibson, J. B., Starmer, G. A., Perl, J., & Wilks,
A. V. (1985). A twin study of psychomotor and physiological responses to an
acute dose of alcohol. Behavior Genetics, 15, 305347.
Mather, K. & Jinks, J. L. (1971). Biometrical Genetics. London: Chapman and
Hall.
Mather, K. & Jinks, J. L. (1977). Introduction to Biometrical Genetics. Ithaca,
New York: Cornell University Press.
Mather, K. & Jinks, J. L. (1982). Biometrical genetics: The Study of Continuous
Variation (3rd ed.). London: Chapman and Hall.
Maxwell, A. E. (1977). Multivariate Analysis in Behavioral Research. New York:
John Wiley.
McArdle, J. J., Connell, J. P., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1980). Structural modeling
of stability and genetic inuences: Some results from a longitudinal study of
behavioral style. Behavior Genetics, 10, 487.
McArdle, J. J. & Goldsmith, H. H. (1990). Alternative common-factor models for
multivariate biometric analyses. Behavior Genetics, 20, 569608.
Molenaar, P. C. M. & Boomsma, D. I. (1987). Application of nonlinear factor
analysis to genotype-environment interaction. Behavior Genetics, 17, 7180.
Mood, A. M. & Graybill, F. A. (1963). Introduction to the Theory of Statistics
(2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Mulaik, S. A., James, L. R., VanAlstine, J., Bennett, N. Lind, S., & Stilwell, C. D.
(1989). Evaluation of goodness-of-t indices for structural equations models.
Psychological Bulletin, 105, 430445.
Muthen, B. O. (1987). LISCOMP: Analysis of Linear Structural Equations with a
Comprehensive Measurement Model. Mooresville, IN: Scientic Software, Inc.
NAG (1990). The NAG Fortran Library Manual, Mark 14. Oxford: Numerical
Algorithms Group.
Nance, W. E. & Corey, L. A. (1976). Genetic models for the analysis of data from
the families of identical twins. Genetics, (pp. 811825).
Neale, M. C. (1988). Handedness in a sample of volunteer twins. Behavior Genetics,
18, 6979.
Neale, M. C. (1991). Mx: Statistical Modeling. Box 3 MCV, Richmond, VA 23298:
Department of Human Genetics.
282 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & H., M. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical Modeling.
VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry.
Neale, M. C. & Eaves, L. J. (1992). Estimating and controlling for the eects of
volunteer bias with pairs of relatives. Behavior Genetics, in press.
Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., Hewitt, J. K., Eaves, L. J., & Fulker, D. W. (1989).
Fitting genetic models with LISREL: Hypothesis testing. Behavior Genetics,
19, 3769.
Neale, M. C. & Martin, N. G. (1989). The eects of age, sex and genotype on self-
report drunkenness following a challenge dose of alcohol. Behavior Genetics,
19, 6378.
Neale, M. C., Rushton, J. P., & Fulker, D. W. (1986). The heritability of items from
the eysenck personality questionnaire. Personality and Individual Dierences,
7, 771779.
Neale, M. C. & Stevenson, J. (1989). Rater bias in the EASI temperament scales:
A twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (pp. 446455).
Ott, J. (1985). Analysis of Human Genetic Linkage. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Pearson, E. S. & Hartley, H. O. (1972). Biometrika Tables for Statisticians, vol-
ume 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pearson, K. (1904). On a generalized theory of alternative inheritance, with special
references to Mendels laws. Phil. Trans. Royal Society A, 203, 5386.
Phillips, K. & Fulker, D. W. (1989). Quantitative genetic analysis of longitudinal
trends in adoption designs with application to IQ in the Colorado Adoption
Project. Behavior Genetics, 19, 621658.
Plomin, R. & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic inuence
on environmental measures. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 14, 373397.
Rao, D. C., Morton, N. E., & Yee, S. (1974). Analysis of family resemblance II. A
linear model for familial correlation. American Journal of Human Genetics,
26, 331359.
Rice, J., Cloninger, C. R., & Reich, T. (1978). Multifactorial inheritance with cul-
tural transmission and assortative mating. I. Description and basic properties
of the unitary models. American Journal of Human Genetics, 30, 618643.
Riskind, J. H., Beck, A. T., Berchick, R. J., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1987).
Reliability of DSM-III diagnoses for major depression and generalized anxiety
disorder using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III. Archives General
Psychiatry, 44, 817820.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 283
SAS (1985). SAS/IML Users Guide, Version 5 edition. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.
SAS (1988). SAS/STAT Users guide: Release 6.03. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.
Schieken, R. M., Eaves, L. J., Hewitt, J. K., Mosteller, M., Bodurtha, J. M.,
Moskowitz, W. B., & Nance, W. E. (1989). Univariate genetic analysis of blood
pressure in children: the mcv twin study. American Journal of Cardiology, 64,
13331337.
Searle, S. R. (1982). Matrix Algebra Useful for Statistics. New York: John Wiley.
Silberg, J. L., Erickson, M. T., Eaves, L. J., & Hewitt, J. K. (1992). The contribu-
tion of environmental factors to maternal ratings of behavioral and emotional
problems in children and adolescents. (manuscript in preparation).
Spence, J. E., Corey, L. A., Nance, W. E., Marazita, M. L., Kendler, K. S., &
Schieken, R. M. (1988). Molecular analysis of twin zygosity using VNTR
DNA probes. American Journal of Human Genetics, 43(3), A159 (Abstract).
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Structured Clinical Interview
for DSM-III-R. New York: Biometrics Research Dept. and New York State
Psychiatric Institute.
SPSS (1988). SPSS-X Users Guide (3rd ed.). Chicago: SPSS Inc.
Stunkard, A. J., Foch, T. T., & Hrubec, Z. (1986). The body-mass index of twins
who have been reared apart. New England Journal of Medicine, 314, 193198.
van Eerdewegh, P. (1982). Statistical selection in multivariate systems with applica-
tions in quantitative genetics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Washington
University, St. Louis.
Vandenberg, S. G. (1965). Multivariate analysis of twin dierences. In S. G.
Vandenberg (Ed.), Methods and goals in human behavior genetics, (pp. 29
43). Academic Press: New York.
Vlietinck, R., Derom, R., Neale, M. C., Maes, H., Van Loon, H., Van Maele, G.,
Derom, C., & Thiery, M. (1989). Genetic and environmental variation in the
birthweight of twins. Behavior Genetics, 19, 151161.
Vogler, G. P. (1982). Multivariate behavior genetic analyses of correlations vs.
phenotypically standardized covariances. Behavior Genetics, 12, 473478.
Vogler, G. P. (1985). Multivariate path analysis of familial resemblance. Genetic
Epidemiology, 2, 3553.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1973). The Study of Behavioral Development. New-York: Academic
Press.
284 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Wright, S. (1921). Correlation and causation. Journal of Agricultural Research,
20, 557585.
Wright, S. (1934). The method of path coecients. Annals of Mathematical Statis-
tics, 5, 161215.
Wright, S. (1960). The treatment of reciprocal interaction, with or without lag, in
path analysis. Biometrics, 16, 189202.
Wright, S. (1968). Evolution and the Genetics of Populations. Volume 1. Genetic
and Biometric Foundations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Yule, G. U. (1902). Mendels laws and their probable relation to intra-racial hered-
ity. New Phytology, 1, 192207.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 285
Index
ACE model, 125
ACE model, 111, 134, 136, 137, 144,
153, 155, 161, 163
additive genetic
variance, 98
additive genetic
covariance matrix, 189
deviations, 110
value, 56
variance, 60, 65
ADE model, 111, 124, 134, 163
AE model, 111
AE model, 124, 136, 144, 155
age eects, 136
age of onset, 228
age-correction, 135
age-correction model, 135
AIC, see Akaike Information
Criterion
Akaike Information
Criterion, 175
Akaike Information
Criterion, 167
alcohol, 180
allele, 55
American Association of Retired
Persons twin registries, 167
ascertainment, 148
assortative mating, 18, 58, 68, 139
asymptotically distribution free
methods, 53
atopic symptoms, 193
Australian NH&MRC twin register,
173
Australian NH&MRC twin register,
112
autoregressive time series, 220
basic genetic model, 110
BETA matrix, 151
between-family environment, see
shared environment
binary data, see ordinal data
biometric factors model, see
independent pathway model
biometric model, 202
biometrical genetical approach, 4, 31
biometrical genetics, 30, 55
biserial correlation, 50
bivariate genetic model, 208
bivariate normal distribution, 45
BMI, see body mass index
body mass index, 112, 167
Boker, Steven M, 91, 93
Boomsma, Dorret I, 33, 227, 228
breeding experiments, 57
broad heritability, 12
Browne, Michael W, 53
Cardon, Lon R, 112, 227, 228
Carey, Gregory, 21, 151, 190, 200
Cattell, Raymond, 24
causal closure, 91
Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi, 16, 31
CBC, see Child Behavior Checklist
CE model, 155
CE model, 124, 125
centrality parameter, 145
Child Behavior Checklist, 155, 210
286
INDEX 287
cholesky decomposition, 191, 207
cholesky factorization, 191
classical twin study, 177
classical twin study, 98, 144
Cloninger, Robert C, 31, 33
coecient
correlation, 88, 90
path, 88, 90
regression, 88
common eects
sex-limitation model, 165
common eects G E
interaction model, 172
common environment, see
shared environment
common factor model, see
genetic factor model
common pathway model, 198, 205
competition eects, 153
competition eects, 21, 159
contingency table, 132
contingency table, 45
continuous data, 35, 114
contrast eects, see
competition eects
cooperation eects, 21, 153, 159
correlated variable
assortment, 18
transmission, 16
correlation, 5
coecient, 38
biserial, 50
polychoric, 50
polyserial, 50
product moment, 50
tetrachoric, 50
covariance, 38, 88
structure, 87
structure analysis, 36
covariance matrix
expected, 122
observed, 122
covariation, 35, 156, 177
cross-lagged panel design, 25
data matrix, 72
degrees of freedom, 45, 196
Delusions-Symptoms States
Inventory, 173
dependent variables, 4, 88
depression, 172
developmental change and continuity,
26, 228
deviation phenotypes, 127, 152
direction of causation, 25
disturbance terms, see
error variances
dizygotic twins
reared apart, 99
reared together, 101, 102
dizygotic twins, 5, 99
reared apart, 102
reared together, 99
dominance, 12
deviations, 56, 110
variance, 60, 65, 98
double-headed arrow, 88
DZ, see dizygotic twins
E model, 124, 155
Eaves, Lindon, 13, 2022, 27, 31, 33,
129, 131, 132, 151, 162, 167,
181, 228
endogenous variables, see
dependent variables
environmental exposure
discordant for exposure, 170
environmental correlation, 190
environmental eects
between families, 13
within families, 13
environmental exposure, 170
concordant for non-exposure, 170
concordant for exposure, 170
epidemiological approach, 4
epistasis, 12, 68, 98, 110
equal environments assumption, 135
288 INDEX
equality of means, 127
error variances, 180
exogenous variable, see
independent variables
expected covariance, 87, 91, 99, 101
expected twin covariances, 98
expected twin covariances, 60, 156
equal gene frequencies, 60
numerical illustration, 159
unequal gene frequencies, 63
expected variance, 91, 101, 102
factor analysis
structure, 179
factor analysis, 32
correlations, 178
error variances, 180
loadings, 178, 180
pattern, 178
scores, 179
factor-analytic approach, 31
Falconer, D S, 44, 55, 61, 63
family environment, see
shared environment
feedback loop, 88, 92, 95
Feldman, Marcus, 16, 31
Fisher, Ronald, 19, 29
Fisher, Ronald A, 3032, 37, 56, 63,
65, 69
FORTRAN format, 42
Fulker, David W, 23, 32, 33, 69, 112,
124, 160, 227, 228
Galton, Francis, 27
gamete, 58
gametic crosses, 57
gene, 55
gene frequencies, 58, 63
general G E interaction model, 170
general sex-limitation model, 162
genetic cholesky model, 190
genetic correlation, 190
genetic eects
additive, 12
non-additive, 12
genetic environment, 16
genetic factor model
multiple, 189
environmental correlations, 189
genetic correlations, 189
simple, 181
second common factor, 188
single common factor, 181
genetic simplex model, 222
genotype, 55
genotype age-cohort
interaction, 113
genotype environment interaction,
112
genotype environment interaction,
18, 22, 161
non-scalar, 23
scalar, 23
genotype environment interaction
model, 169
common eects, 172
general, 170
scalar eects, 172
genotype-environment
autocorrelation, 20
correlation, 18, 19, 112
covariance, 18
genotype-environment eects
assortative mating, 18
correlated variable
assortment, 18
transmission, 16
G E interaction, 22
genotype-environment
autocorrelation, 20
correlation, 19
latent variable assortment, 18
phenotypic assortment, 18
sibling eects, 21
social homogamy, 19
stabilizing selection, 20
genotypic
INDEX 289
eects, 56, 64
mean, 59, 64
variance, 59, 64
frequencies, 58, 63
values, 56, 67
Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, 59
Heath, Andrew C, 18, 19, 44, 113,
124, 141, 161, 170, 184, 203,
207
heritability
broad, 127
narrow, 127
heterogeneity
of means, 130
of means, 127
of variances, 130
heteroscedasticity, 172
heterozygote, 12, 56
Hewitt, John K, 137, 155, 210, 228
homozygote, 12, 56
hypothesis testing, 32
identication, 103
combined parameters, 105
model, 103
numerical approach, 105
parameter, 103
imitation eects, see
cooperation eects
IMSL, 76
inbreeding, 18
independent pathway model, 195
independent variables, 4, 88
Jinks, John L, 55
Jinks, John L, 4, 12, 16, 22, 23, 30,
32, 57, 61, 63, 98, 124, 161,
200
J oreskog, Karl G, 32, 33, 53, 218
Kendler, Kenneth S, 15, 24, 132, 133,
135, 195, 198
latent variable assortment, 18
latent variables, 88, 98
law of independent assortment, 60
law of segregation, 58
liability, see multivariate normal,
liability distribution
likelihood, 32
likelihood ratio test, 126, 209
linear regression
common cause, 95
linear regression, 93
cause, 93
direct eect, 93
indirect eects, 95
linear structural model, 55
linear structural model, 87
linearity, 91
linkage analysis, 2
LISCOMP, 148
LISREL, 32
locus, 55
longitudinal data, 224
major depressive disorder, 132
manifest variables, see
observed variables
marital status, 172
Martin, Nicholas G, 33
Martin, Nicholas G, 33, 112, 124, 137,
139, 141, 161, 180, 181
Mather, Kenneth, 98
Mather, Kenneth, 4, 12, 22, 30, 55,
57, 61, 63, 161, 200
matrix
addition, 73
cofactor, 77
correlation, 82
covariance, 82
data, 72, 82
deniteness
negative, 77
positive, 77
determinant, 75
diagonal, 72
290 INDEX
dimensions, 72
identity, 73
inner product, 74
inverse, 78, 104
multiplication, 74
null, 72
order, 72
scalar, 72
singular, 77
square, 72
subtraction, 73
trace, 77
transformation, 83
transpose, 75
unit, 72
vector, 72
matrix algebra, 71
applications, 82
equations, 80
operations, 73
binary, 73
unary, 75
Maximum Likelihood, 32
McArdle, J Jack, 33, 91, 93, 195, 198,
203
mean, 4, 37
mean squares, 35
measured variables, see
observed variables
measurement error, 106
measurement error, 111
Medical College of Virginia
Twin Study, 192
Mendel, Gregor, 29, 58
Mendelian genetics, 55
model building, 11
model tting, 11
Molenaar, Peter C, 33, 227
monozygotic twins
reared apart, 102
reared together, 102
monozygotic twins, 7, 99
reared together, 99
reared apart, 99
reared together, 99
Morton, Newton E, 31, 32
multifactorial inheritance, 50
multiple indicators, 106
multiple genetic factor model, 189
multiple indicators, 111
multiple occasions of
measurement, 217
multiple rating data, 201
multivariate genetic analysis, 24, 177
multivariate genetic factor model, 182
common pathway model, 193
genetic cholesky model, 190
independent pathway model, 193
multivariate genetic model, 16, 184,
185
multivariate normal
liability distribution
bivariate, 45
univariate, 44
Muthen, Bengt O, 148
Muthen, Bengt O, 49
Mx, 76, 115, 123, 146, 148, 160
Mx output, 121
goodness-of-t statistics, 123
parameter estimates, 122
parameter specications, 121
standardized parameter estimates,
123
Mx script, 115
algebra section, 118
calculation group, 117
data section, 119
matrices declaration, 117
model specication, 120
title, 117
mx scripts
options, 120
MZ, see monozygotic twins
NAG, 76
Nance, Walter E, 17
INDEX 291
Neale, Michael C, 206
Neale, Michael C, 33, 44, 49, 84, 122,
129, 131, 132, 135, 137, 141,
146, 148, 160, 202, 205, 214
nested model, 187
non-central chi-squared distribution,
145
non-scalar
G E interaction, 23
sex-limitation, 13
normal distribution assumption, 48
observed statistics, 45
observed variables, 88
observer ratings
maternal, 203
observer ratings, 201
paternal, 203
one-way arrow, see
single-headed arrow
ordinal data, 35, 43
liability, 44
thresholds, 44
parameter estimation, 32
parameters, 45
parsimony, 163
path coecients model, 99
path analysis, 31, 87
assumptions, 90
causal closure, 91
linearity, 91
unitary variables, 91
conventions, 88
tracing rules, 91
standardized variables, 92, 99
unstandardized variables, 92,
101
path coecients model, 99, 111, 115
path coeents, 90
path diagram, 87, 91, 93, 98
dependent variables, 88
double-headed arrow, 88
independent variables, 88
latent variables, 88
observed variables, 88
paths, 88
causal, 88
correlational, 88
single-headed arrow, 88
path-analytic approach, 31
Pearson, Karl, 29
Pearson, Karl, 29
phenotype, 55
phenotypic factor model, 178
conrmatory factor model, 178
exploratory factor model, 178
phenotypic simplex model, 218
pleiotropy, 24, 56, 189
polychoric correlation, 50, 52
polygenic model, 29
polygenic system, 56
polyserial correlation, 50, 52
power, 141
contributing factors, 141
power analysis, 142
continuous data, 144
ordinal data, 146
power calculation, 142
PRELIS, 36, 41, 50, 146
primary phenotypic assortment, 18
product moment correlation, 50
psychometric factors model, see
common pathway model
psychometric model, 202
Punnett square, 58
Punnett, Reginald, 58
quantitative genetics, 55
RAMPATH, 91
random environmental
covariance matrix, 189
deviations, 110
random environment, 14
random environmental
variance, 98
random mating, 58, 101, 112
292 INDEX
Rao, D C, 19
Rao, DC, 32
rater bias, 202
parental, 202
rater bias model, 203
rating data model
biometric, 207
psychometric, 205
rater bias, 203
raw data, 35
reciprocal causation, 88
recursive model, 92, 151
regression models, 93
Reich, Ted, 31
repeated measures, 217
cross-sectional data, 217
longitudinal data, 217
multiple occasions, 217
causal factors, 218
long-term factors, 218
transient factors, 218
problems
collinearity, 228
singularity, 228
residual variables, 90
Rice, John, 31
sample size, 141
SAS, 36, 40, 76
scalar
G E interaction, 23
sex-limitation, 13
scalar eects
sex-limitation model, 165
scalar eects G E
interaction model, 172
segregation analysis, 2
sex-limitation, 13, 161
non-scalar, 13
scalar, 13
sex-limitation model, 162
common eects, 165
general, 162
scalar eects, 165
shared environmental
variance, 98
shared environment, 15
shared environmental
deviations, 110
sibling eects
competition, 159
cooperation, 153, 159
sibling interaction, 158
sibling eects, 21
competition, 21, 153
cooperation, 21
sibling interaction, 151
sibling interaction model, 153
sibling shared environment, 17
signicance test, 126, 141, 187
simple genetic factor model, 181
simple genetic model, 98
simplex model, 218
autoregressive model, 220
rst-order, 228
genetic, 222
measurement model, 219
phenotypic, 218
structural equation model, 219
simplex structure
factor loadings, 218, 220
innovations, 220
measurement errors, 219, 220
regression coecients, 219, 220
transmission, 221
single-headed arrow, 88
singleton twins
concordant-participant pairs, 129
discordant-participant twins, 129
singleton twins, 129
skinfold measures, 192
social attitudes, 135
social homogamy, 19
social interaction, 18, 151
S orbom, Dag, 32, 53
Spearman, Charles, 24, 29
INDEX 293
special MZ twin environment, 17
special twin environment, 17
specic environment, see
random environment
specic variances, see
error variances
SPSS, 36
stabilizing selection, 20
standard deviation, 39
standardized variables, 92
structural equations, 87
Structured Clinical Interview, 133
sucient statistic, 37
summary statistics, 35
correlation coecient, 38
covariance, 38
mean, 37
mean squares, 35
standard deviation, 39
variance, 37
weight matrix, 53
tetrachoric correlation, 50, 147
threshold, 44
Thurstone, LL, 33
tracing rules, 91
transformation, 114
triangular decomposition, see
cholesky decomposition
twin design, 109
twin design, 1, 98, 177
twin pairs
dizygotic, 5, 112
like-sex, 161
opposite-sex, 112, 161, 165
female, 161
male, 161
monozygotic, 7, 112
like-sex, 161
twoallele system, 56
two-way arrow, see
double-headed arrow
type II error, 142
ultimate variables, see
independent variables
unique environment, see
random environment
unique variances, see
error variances
unitary variables, 91
univariate genetic model, 110
univariate genetic analysis, 109
univariate normal distribution, 44
unmeasured variables, see
latent variables
unreliability, 14, 201
unstandardized variables, 92
variables
continuous, 50
dichotomous, 50
polychotomous, 50
standardized, 92
unstandardized, 92
variance, 37, 89
variance components model, 111
variance components model, 99, 101,
123
variation, 2, 35, 109, 156, 177
vertical cultural transmission, see
cultural transmission
Virginia Twin Registry, 132, 167
Vogler, G.P, 190
weight, 218
weight matrix, 53
within-family environment, see
random environment
Wright, Sewall, 30, 31, 69, 87, 9092
Yee, S, 32
zygosity diagnosis, 132

You might also like