Cdev0101 Notes
Cdev0101 Notes
Cdev0101 Notes
Chapter 1
Development is the pattern of change that begins at conception and continues through
the life span. Most development involves growth, although it also includes decline.
Influences on Development
1. Health and Well-Being
o Physical Health: Questions about how physical health, such as nutrition
and exercise, affects development are critical. For instance, inadequate
nutrition can impact cognitive and emotional development.
o Mental Health: Increasing concerns about mental health, exacerbated by
factors like the COVID-19 pandemic, highlight the importance of addressing
psychological well-being from a young age.
2. Parenting
o Responsive Parenting: The quality of parenting, including emotional
responsiveness and support, plays a crucial role in healthy development.
Parenting practices can significantly influence a child's emotional and
behavioral outcomes.
3. Education
o Educational Impact: Educational experiences shape a child's cognitive and
socioemotional development. Effective education systems and relationships
with educators are vital for fostering a child’s growth.
4. Sociocultural Contexts (context- setting in which development occurs)
o Culture and Ethnicity: Cultural and ethnic backgrounds influence
developmental experiences and expectations. Cross-cultural studies examine
how these factors affect development across different societies.
o Socioeconomic Status (SES): SES impacts access to resources and
opportunities, which can affect developmental outcomes. Lower SES is often
associated with higher risks for developmental challenges.
o Gender: Gender identity and roles can influence various aspects of
development, including self-perception and social interactions.
Culture encompasses the behaviour patterns, beliefs, and all other products of a
specific group of people that are passed on from generation to generation. Culture
results from the interaction of people over many years Whatever its size, the group’s
culture influences the behaviour of its members.
Ethnicity (the word ethnic comes from the Greek word for “nation”) is rooted in cultural
heritage, nationality, race, religion, and language.
The factors that influence a child's growth and development are also related to the time
or era in which they are born. A cohort is a group of people who are born at a similar
point in history and share similar experiences as a result.
resilience is a capacity that can be developed and involves having access to resources
and the opportunities to practise coping skills in the presence of supportive relationships.
A child's resilience is determined by their experience with these individual, family, and
social factors. Children who experience adversity are more likely to thrive if they
experience even a few of these factors.
Page | 3
Biological, cognitive, and socioemotional processes interact and can influence each
other. These processes are deeply intertwined.
PERIODS OF DEVELOPMENT
The prenatal period is the time from conception to birth, roughly a nine-month period.
During this amazing time, a single cell grows into a fetus and then a baby, complete with
a brain and a vast range of capabilities.
Infancy is the developmental period that extends from birth to about 18 to 24 months of
age. Infancy is a time of extreme dependence on adults. Many psychological activities
are just beginning—the abilities to speak, to coordinate sensations and physical actions,
to think with symbols, and to imitate and learn from others.
Early childhood is the developmental period that extends from the end of infancy to
about 5 or 6 years of age; sometimes this period is called the preschool years. During
this time, young children learn to become more self-sufficient, and they spend many
hours in play and with peers. With support, children develop self-regulation in the early
childhood years.
Middle and late childhood is the developmental period between about 6 and 11 years
of age; sometimes this period is referred to as the elementary school years. Children
master the fundamental skills of reading, writing, and arithmetic, and they are formally
exposed to the larger world and its cultures. Achievement becomes a more central
theme of the child’s world, and self-regulation increases.
ISSUES IN DEVELOPMENT
Feral children are children who have grown up with limited human contact, either by
accident or because they have suffered severe neglect.
Continuity- development is the gradual growth or addition of skills and abilities that to
some degree were already there; it is a matter of quantitative change e.g. puberty
The early-later experience issue in human development revolves around the debate
about the relative importance of early versus later experiences in shaping an individual's
development. This issue explores how experiences at different stages of life impact
development and whether early experiences have a more significant influence than later
ones, or vice versa.
Page | 5
Psychoanalytic Theories
Primary motivation for human behaviour is social and reflects a desire to affiliate with
other people.
versus generation to develop and lead useful lives. The feeling of having done nothing to
stagnation, help the next generation is stagnation.
Industry 60s onward a person reflects on the past. If the person’s life review reveals a life well spent,
versus integrity will be achieved; if not, the retrospective glances likely will yield doubt or
inferiority gloom
Cognitive Theories
children actively construct their understanding of the world and go through four
stages of cognitive development.
Two processes, organization and adaptation, move us through the four stages of
development.
To make sense of our world, we organize our experiences. For example, we
separate important ideas from less important ideas, and we connect one idea to
another. In addition to organizing our observations and experiences, we adapt,
adjusting to new environmental demands.
the child’s cognition is qualitatively different in one stage compared with another.
Information-Processing Theory
Behaviourism: we can study scientifically only what can be directly observed and
measured.
This theory focuses on how a neutral stimulus can come to elicit a response after being
paired with a stimulus that naturally brings about that response.
essential behaviors. For instance, baby ducks imprint on their mother and follow
her around, ensuring their survival
Microsystem: This is the innermost layer of the ecological system and includes the
immediate environments that directly impact an individual. These include family, school,
peers, and neighborhood. Interactions within the microsystem are bidirectional; for
example, a child's behavior influences and is influenced by their family dynamics.
Mesosystem: The mesosystem involves the interactions between different
microsystems. It refers to the connections between the various settings in which an
individual is involved. For instance, the relationship between a child's family and their
school, or between their home life and their extracurricular activities, falls within the
mesosystem. Positive interactions across these settings can support development, while
negative interactions can create conflicts or stress.
Exosystem: The exosystem includes broader social systems that do not directly involve
the individual but still affect them. These can include the parent’s workplace, community
services, and social networks. For example, if a parent experiences job stress or
unemployment, it can indirectly impact the child’s development through changes in
family dynamics or economic stability.
Macrosystem: The macrosystem encompasses the larger cultural, economic, and
societal influences that shape an individual’s environment. This includes cultural values,
laws, and economic conditions. For example, societal attitudes towards education and
child-rearing practices are part of the macrosystem and can influence the overall
development of children within that society.
Chronosystem: The chronosystem refers to the dimension of time and how changes
over time impact development. It includes life transitions, historical events, and socio-
economic changes that influence an individual’s development. For example, the effects
of growing up during a time of economic recession or a global pandemic would be
considered within the chronosystem.
Page | 9
Chapter 2
o Hypothesized that traits like spatial thinking in men were advantageous in hunter-gatherer societies and
became more common over generations due to reproductive success.
3. Criticism of Evolutionary Psychology
o The scenarios described (e.g., spatial skills in hunters) might not have occurred exactly as proposed.
Critics argue that these are speculative and not always based on concrete evidence.
4. Current Hunter-Gatherer Societies
o Hadza Study: In hunter-gatherer societies like the Hadza, mothers and grandmothers' foraging
contributes more to family sustenance than hunting by males.
o Implications: This research highlights the importance of cooperative social behaviour and explains why
human females live past reproductive age, suggesting that grandmothers' roles were crucial for the
survival of grandchildren.
Evolutionary Developmental Psychology
1. Extended Childhood Period
o Humans take longer to reach reproductive maturity compared to other mammals, likely because of the
time needed to develop large brains and acquire complex societal skills.
o Human Development: The extended childhood period is thought to allow humans to learn and adapt to
the complexities of human society, which is key for survival and reproduction.
2. Domain-Specific Psychological Mechanisms
o Evolutionary psychology suggests that human minds are not general-purpose but consist of specialized
mechanisms to solve specific problems (e.g., spatial thinking, language, trading skills).
o These mechanisms evolved in response to recurring challenges faced by early humans.
3. Evolved Mechanisms and Modern Society
o Some evolved behaviours, while adaptive for early humans, may no longer be beneficial in contemporary
society.
Example: The tendency to crave high-calorie foods, which was advantageous in food-scarce
environments, now contributes to obesity in modern food-abundant societies.
Evaluating Evolutionary Psychology
1. Criticism
o One-sided Evolutionism: Critics like Albert Bandura argue that evolutionary psychology
overemphasizes the role of biology in behaviour, ignoring environmental influences.
o Bidirectional View: Instead of evolution dictating behaviour, it is suggested that biological and
environmental factors interact. Changes in the environment can create new evolutionary pressures,
leading to further biological changes (e.g., tool use led to the development of specific biological systems
for thought and language).
2. Cultural Flexibility
o Evolution provides humans with biological potentialities, but these do not strictly determine behaviour.
Human cultures vary widely (e.g., aggressive vs. peace-loving societies), showing that biology allows a
range of possibilities influenced by culture.
3. Challenges in Testing Evolutionary Psychology
o The long timescale of evolution makes it difficult to empirically study the development of human traits
through natural selection.
o Research on specific genes and their links to traits may offer the best method for testing evolutionary
psychology theories.
P a g e | 11
Gene expression is the process by which information from a gene is used to synthesize proteins.
Environmental factors such as hormones, light, nutrition, and behavior can turn genes "on" or "off."
External stressors like radiation or high levels of stress hormones (e.g., cortisol) can lead to DNA damage or
affect the rate of DNA repair.
Genetic Principles
1. Genotype and Phenotype
o Genotype refers to the genetic makeup of an individual, while phenotype is the observable
characteristics. The expression of a genotype to create a phenotype is complex and influenced by genetic
and environmental factors.
2. Dominant-Recessive Genes Principle
o Dominant gene: Always exerts its effects, overriding the recessive gene. For example, brown hair
dominates over blond hair.
o Recessive gene: Exerts its influence only if both paired genes are recessive.
o Example: Two brown-haired parents (with one dominant gene for brown hair and one recessive for
blond) can have a blond-haired child if both pass on their recessive blond hair genes.
3. Sex-Linked Genes
o X-linked inheritance occurs when a mutated gene is carried on the X chromosome. Since males have
only one X chromosome, they are more likely to express X-linked diseases (e.g., hemophilia, fragile X
syndrome). Females, with two X chromosomes, may carry the gene but are less likely to express the
disease unless both X chromosomes are affected.
4. Polygenic Inheritance
o Most traits are determined by the interaction of multiple genes (polygenic inheritance), not just by a
single gene or gene pair. For example, traits like height and intelligence involve the interaction of several
genes and environmental factors.
o Gene-gene interaction: Studies focus on how multiple genes work together to influence traits, behaviors,
and diseases (e.g., immune system functioning, asthma, cancer).
Chromosomal and Gene-Linked Abnormalities
1. Chromosomal Abnormalities
o These occur when there are issues with chromosome separation during meiosis, leading to conditions like
Down syndrome (extra chromosome 21), Klinefelter syndrome (XXY), Turner syndrome (missing or
altered X chromosome in females), and XYY syndrome (extra Y chromosome in males).
Common Chromosomal Abnormalities:
o Down syndrome: Causes intellectual disabilities, distinct facial features, and physical abnormalities.
Occurs more frequently with advanced maternal age.
o Klinefelter syndrome: Affects males (XXY), leading to physical abnormalities like undeveloped testes
and enlarged breasts. Hormone therapy can be helpful.
o Turner syndrome: Females with one X chromosome missing or altered. Causes short stature, webbed
neck, and fertility issues. Hormone therapy is a common treatment.
o XYY syndrome: Males with an extra Y chromosome are often taller than average, but no special
treatment is required.
2. Gene-Linked Abnormalities
o Gene-linked abnormalities arise from harmful genes rather than chromosomal issues. Over 7,000 such
disorders exist, though most are rare. Some well-known examples include:
Gene-Linked Disorders:
o Phenylketonuria (PKU): Individuals can't metabolize phenylalanine, leading to intellectual disability if
untreated. A special diet can prevent the disorder from manifesting.
o Sickle-cell anemia: Affects red blood cells, limiting oxygen delivery and causing pain and anemia. It is
more common in African Americans.
o Cystic fibrosis: A glandular dysfunction hampers breathing and digestion, shortening the lifespan.
Treatment includes physical therapy and medication.
o Tay-Sachs disease: Causes progressive mental and physical deterioration due to lipid accumulation in the
nervous system, leading to early death.
o Hemophilia: Causes delayed blood clotting, leading to internal and external bleeding.
P a g e | 13
o Spina bifida: A neural tube defect leading to spine and brain abnormalities, often treated with surgery
and therapy.
Heredity-Environment Interaction
Genes interact with the environment, and environmental manipulation can prevent certain genetic disorders from
developing. For example, the effects of PKU can be mitigated by avoiding phenylalanine in the diet.
This interaction illustrates that genes are not destiny. The right environmental conditions can prevent certain
genetic risks from becoming phenotypic disorders.
Considerations of Genetic Abnormalities
Every individual carries DNA variations that might predispose them to diseases. However, not all individuals
develop disorders. The development of a genetic disorder often depends on environmental factors or the presence
of compensatory genes.
Genetic counseling helps families understand genetic risks and make informed decisions about managing these
risks. Genetic counselors analyze inheritance patterns and suggest strategies for offsetting potential effects.
Key Takeaways:
Dominant-recessive genes determine many traits but can vary in expression depending on the combination of
genes inherited from both parents.
Sex-linked genes are especially impactful on males, as they lack a second X chromosome to counter harmful X-
linked mutations.
Polygenic traits reflect the complexity of genetic inheritance, often involving multiple genes and environmental
influences.
Chromosomal and gene-linked abnormalities present in various forms and degrees of severity, but treatments
can improve quality of life for affected individuals.
Environmental interactions can mitigate the effects of genetic disorders, emphasizing the importance of both
heredity and environment in development.
P a g e | 14
Behaviour Genetics
1. Introduction to Behaviour Genetics
P a g e | 15
Definition: Behaviour genetics is the field that examines the influence of heredity and environment on individual
differences in human traits and development.
Purpose: Rather than focusing on how much heredity or environment affects a person, the goal is to understand
why people differ—whether due to genetic differences, environmental factors, or their interaction.
2. Key Methods Used in Behaviour Genetics
Twin Studies:
o Compare behavioural similarities between identical twins (genetically identical) and fraternal twins (no
more genetically similar than regular siblings).
o Helps researchers examine the role of heredity in traits, such as conduct problems.
o Example: Identical twins show greater similarity in conduct problems compared to fraternal twins,
suggesting a genetic influence.
o Caveat: Identical twins may also have more similar environments, which could exaggerate genetic
influences.
o
Adoption Studies:
o Compare adopted children with their biological parents (heredity) and adoptive parents (environment).
o Seek to determine whether children's behaviour is more similar to their biological or adoptive parents.
o Another form of adoption study compares biological siblings with adoptive siblings.
3. Heredity-Environment Correlations
The interaction between genetics and environment is complex. Heredity-environment correlations describe how
an individual’s genes influence the types of environments they are exposed to.
Three Types of Heredity-Environment Correlations (Sandra Scarr):
1. Passive Correlation:
Parents provide both genes and a rearing environment.
Example: Musically inclined parents pass on musical genes and create a music-rich environment
for their children.
2. Evocative Correlation:
A child’s genetically influenced traits elicit specific responses from the environment.
Example: A cheerful, outgoing child elicits positive social interactions, reinforcing social traits.
3. Active Correlation (Niche-Picking):
Children seek out environments that match their genetic propensities.
Example: A child with a genetic interest in sports may spend more time in playgrounds or sports
fields, engaging with others who share similar interests.
Developmental Changes:
o As children grow older, passive correlations diminish and active correlations become more prominent,
especially during adolescence when children gain more control over their environments.
4. Shared and Nonshared Environmental Experiences
Shared Environment:
o Experiences common to siblings, such as parents’ socioeconomic status, parenting styles, or the
neighbourhood.
o Example: Children in the same family share the same home environment and parental values.
Nonshared Environment:
o Unique experiences for each child, both within and outside the family.
o Includes differences in peer groups, different school experiences, and individual interactions with parents.
o Plomin’s Research: Found that shared environments contribute very little to differences in personality
and interests among siblings.
5. Research Findings in Heredity-Environment Interaction
Robert Plomin:
o Shared environments have limited influence on personality.
o Nonshared environments are more important in explaining why siblings differ, even when raised in the
same family.
o Example: One sibling might pursue sports, while another might focus on music, due to inherited
tendencies and the environments they seek out.
Influence of Intelligence:
P a g e | 16
o Early in childhood, parenting (e.g., parental intelligence) plays a strong role in creating enriching
environments (e.g., providing books, museum trips).
o As children age, their own intelligence becomes more predictive of the quality of the environments they
seek (e.g., academic clubs, advanced classes).
Prenatal Development
1. Conception
Conception occurs when a sperm unites with an egg in the female's fallopian tube, resulting in a fertilized egg, or
zygote.
Fertilization leads to a genetically unique zygote, directing developmental changes.
Identical twins (monozygotic) come from a single zygote that splits, while fraternal twins (dizygotic) develop
from two eggs fertilized by different sperm.
2. Course of Prenatal Development
Prenatal development lasts around 266 to 280 days (38–40 weeks) and is divided into three periods:
1. Germinal Period (0–2 weeks)
o Begins with conception, involving zygote creation, cell division, and attachment to the uterine wall.
o By the end of the first week, the cells differentiate into the blastocyst (inner cells become the embryo)
and the trophoblast (provides support/nutrition).
o Implantation occurs 11–15 days after conception.
2. Embryonic Period (2–8 weeks)
o Cell differentiation intensifies, and life-support systems develop. Major organs begin to form
(organogenesis).
o The embryo forms three layers:
P a g e | 17
Teratogen Definition: A teratogen is any agent that can potentially cause birth defects or negatively alter
cognitive and behavioral outcomes in a developing fetus. Given the prevalence of teratogens, nearly every fetus is
exposed to some level of these agents, making it challenging to pinpoint the exact causes of specific
developmental issues.
Field of Study: Teratology is the study of the causes of birth defects. Relatedly, behavioral teratology focuses on
how certain exposures might affect cognitive and behavioral functioning without causing physical defects.
Influencing Factors:
1. Dose: Increased exposure to a teratogen typically results in greater effects.
2. Genetic Susceptibility: The genetic makeup of both the pregnant individual and the fetus can influence
the impact of teratogens. For instance, male fetuses are often more vulnerable to teratogens than females.
3. Timing of Exposure: The timing of exposure to a teratogen is crucial; the embryonic period is generally
more sensitive than the fetal period. Critical periods for organ development are vital, as damage during
these times can lead to significant issues.
Categories of Teratogens
1. Substance Use in Pregnancy
o Prescription Drugs: Some medications can have teratogenic effects. Women are advised to consult
healthcare providers about the safety of any medication during pregnancy.
o Psychoactive Drugs: Substances such as caffeine, alcohol, nicotine, and illicit drugs like cocaine and
methamphetamine can harm fetal development.
Alcohol: Heavy drinking can lead to fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD), which result in
physical, cognitive, and behavioral deficits.
Nicotine: Associated with low birth weight, preterm births, and long-term developmental issues.
Cocaine and Methamphetamines: Linked to various adverse outcomes, including reduced birth
weight and developmental challenges.
Marijuana: Although findings are mixed, marijuana use during pregnancy may lead to negative
outcomes such as low birth weight and premature birth.
2. Environmental Hazards
o Exposure to environmental toxins (e.g., radiation, heavy metals, chemical pollutants) can pose risks to
fetal development. X-ray radiation, for instance, can be particularly harmful in early pregnancy.
3. Maternal Factors
o Maternal Diseases: Conditions such as rubella and sexually transmitted infections can cross the placental
barrier and cause harm.
HIV/AIDS: Can be transmitted during pregnancy, birth, or breastfeeding.
Diabetes: Associated with higher rates of congenital malformations and other complications.
o Maternal Nutrition: A well-balanced diet is essential for healthy fetal development. Deficiencies,
particularly in folic acid, can lead to neural tube defects and other issues.
4. Incompatible Blood Types: Rh incompatibility can lead to severe complications in fetal development, including
miscarriage and brain damage.
5. Maternal Age: Both younger mothers (adolescents) and older mothers (35+) face increased risks for various
complications, including higher rates of miscarriage and congenital anomalies.
6. Emotional States and Stress: High levels of maternal stress and anxiety can lead to negative outcomes, including
preterm birth and developmental issues in offspring.
Paternal Factors
The father's health and behaviors also influence prenatal development. Exposure to environmental toxins, paternal
smoking, and genetic abnormalities in sperm can all lead to adverse outcomes in offspring.
P a g e | 20
2. Second Stage:
o Onset: Begins when the baby’s head starts moving through the cervix and birth canal.
o Duration: Typically lasts 45 minutes to 1 hour.
o Process: The mother pushes with each contraction, which occur almost every minute and last for about a
minute.
o Completion: Ends when the baby is completely born.
3. Third Stage (Afterbirth):
o Duration: Shortest stage, lasting only a few minutes.
o Process: The placenta, umbilical cord, and other membranes are detached and expelled.
Childbirth Setting and Attendants
Common Settings:
o Majority of births occur in hospitals (97-99% in Canada), with only 1-3% taking place at home (Public
Health Agency of Canada, 2018).
Cultural Variations in Attendants:
o North America: Fathers or birth coaches typically accompany mothers.
o East African Nigoni Culture: Male partners are excluded; female relatives support the mother, while the
husband departs until after birth.
o Pukapukan Culture (Pacific Islands): Childbirth is a community event, with women giving birth in
shelters open for public observation.
Midwives
Global Practice: Midwifery is practiced widely; in Holland, over 40% of deliveries are by midwives.
Increasing Popularity in Canada: Midwifery is becoming more integrated into provincial healthcare systems
(Mattison et al., 2020).
Benefits: Midwife-led care for low-risk women shows fewer procedures during labor, greater satisfaction, and
fewer adverse outcomes compared to physician-attended births (Raipuria et al., 2018).
Doulas
Role: A doula provides continuous physical, emotional, and educational support to mothers before, during, and
after childbirth.
Research Findings: Presence of a doula has positive effects on the childbirth experience (McLeish & Redshaw,
2018).
Methods of Childbirth
Family-Centered Approach: Canadian hospitals offer a variety of options regarding delivery methods, focusing
on the safety and comfort of both mother and baby.
Types of Medication Used:
1. Analgesia:
o Used to relieve pain (e.g., narcotics like Demerol).
2. Anesthesia:
o Blocks sensation or consciousness, typically used during late first-stage labor and delivery.
3. Pitocin (Synthetic Oxytocin):
o Stimulates uterine contractions to induce labor.
Natural and Prepared Childbirth:
Natural Childbirth: Focuses on reducing pain through education, breathing techniques, and relaxation methods.
Prepared Childbirth (Lamaze Method): Emphasizes controlled breathing and detailed education about
childbirth anatomy and physiology, typically involving a partner as a coach.
Alternative Methods:
Techniques: Water birth, massage, yoga, acupuncture, hypnosis, and music therapy promote relaxation and
wellness during labor.
Caesarean Delivery
Definition: A surgical procedure where the baby is delivered through an incision in the abdomen, typically
performed when the baby is in a breech position or in other complicated scenarios.
Risks and Benefits: Ongoing debate about the advantages and disadvantages of Caesarean sections, which have
increased significantly; in Canada, 26% of births are Caesarean deliveries, a 50% rise over the past 20 years
P a g e | 24
Additional Assessments
Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS): For typical infants.
Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Network Neurobehavioral Scale (NNNS): For at-risk infants
(premature or substance-exposed).
1. Cephalocaudal Pattern: This "top-down" growth pattern indicates that the most rapid growth occurs at
the head. For instance, the brain and eyes develop faster than the jaw. In sensory and motor
development, infants can see before they gain control over their torso, and they can use their hands
before they can crawl or walk.
2. Proximodistal Pattern: This "center-out" growth sequence begins at the center of the body and extends
toward the extremities. Muscle control of the trunk and arms develops before that of the hands and
fingers. Infants typically use their whole hand before mastering finger control.
Infancy and Childhood
Infancy:
Newborns average about 50 cm (20 inches) in length and weigh around 3,500 grams (7½ pounds). After losing
5-7% of their body weight in the first days of life, they gain approximately 140-170 grams (5-6 ounces) weekly
during the first month. By 4 months, they double their birth weight and nearly triple it by their first birthday.
Their height increases by about 2.5 cm (1 inch) per month, reaching 1.5 times their birth length by the end of
the first year.
By 2 years, infants typically weigh 12-14 kg (26-32 pounds) and are about 89-90 cm (32-35 inches) tall.
Early Childhood:
As children grow, the percentage increase in height and weight decreases. By preschool age, girls and boys are
nearly equal in size. During these years, children begin to slim down as their trunks lengthen. Though they still
have larger heads relative to their bodies, this proportion changes significantly as they approach 5 years of age.
Environmental factors, alongside heredity, impact growth patterns. Nutrition plays a crucial role; unfortunately,
malnutrition remains a significant issue worldwide, with UNICEF reporting that approximately 3 million
children die annually from related causes.
Middle and Late Childhood
During middle and late childhood (ages 6-11), growth slows. Children gain about 5-8 cm (2-3 inches) per year
and approximately 2-3 kg (5-7 pounds). Muscle mass and strength increase as baby fat decreases, with children
doubling their strength capacity during these years. Growth patterns in this stage are marked by improvements
in muscle tone and physical coordination.
Adolescence
Following childhood, puberty triggers rapid physical changes. Puberty, marked by hormonal shifts, occurs
earlier in girls (ages 10-14) than boys (ages 12-16). The average age of menarche has decreased over the
decades, often attributed to improved health and nutrition, although factors such as obesity and decreased
physical activity also contribute.
Hormonal Changes:
During puberty, androgens (mainly testosterone) dominate in males, while estrogens (mainly estradiol)
dominate in females. These hormones are responsible for various physical changes, including the development
of secondary sexual characteristics.
Growth Spurt:
The most significant growth spurts occur during puberty, with girls experiencing peak height gain around 11.5
years and boys around 13.5 years. Girls grow about 9 cm (3.5 inches) per year, while boys grow about 10 cm (4
inches).
Sexual Maturation:
In boys, the sequence of changes includes increases in penis and testicle size, pubic hair development, and voice
changes. For girls, breast development and widening of hips occur before menstruation begins, which may
happen between ages 9-15.
Understanding these growth patterns and changes helps recognize the complexities of physical development
throughout childhood and adolescence.
P a g e | 29
Parietal Lobes: Assist with spatial location, sensation, and motor control.
Deep Structures:
o Hypothalamus and Pituitary Gland: Regulate various bodily functions.
o Amygdala: Involved in emotional processing.
o Hippocampus: Critical for memory and emotion.
Neuron Functionality
Basic Operation:
o Axons: Send electrical signals away from the neuron’s central part.
o Dendrites: Receive signals from other neurons.
o Synapses: Tiny gaps where neurotransmitters facilitate communication between neurons.
Myelin Sheath:
o A layer of fat cells that encases axons, speeding up electrical impulses.
o Developed over time, analogous to the evolution of highways for efficient transport of information.
Neural Circuits:
o Groups of neurons that work together to process specific types of information.
o For example, circuits in the prefrontal cortex and midbrain are crucial for attention and working
memory.
Hemispheric Specialization:
o Left hemisphere: More involved in speech and grammar.
o Right hemisphere: Associated with humor and metaphor use.
o Complex functions often require both hemispheres to work together.
Hemisphere Specialization:
o Newborns show more electrical activity in the left hemisphere for speech sounds.
Developmental Trajectory:
o Primary motor areas develop earlier than sensory areas.
o Frontal lobes remain immature at birth, gradually developing the ability for physiological regulation and
cognitive skills as the child matures (Bell & Cuevas, 2013).
o Amygdala: Matures earlier than the prefrontal cortex and is linked to emotional responses, such as
aggression (Romeo, 2017).
o Figure 3.22 illustrates key brain areas: corpus callosum, prefrontal cortex, and amygdala.
3. Developmental Social Neuroscience
o Examines interactions between development, brain changes, and socioemotional processes (Blakemore &
Mills, 2014; Salley et al., 2013).
o Charles Nelson’s Perspective: Adolescents experience strong emotions but lack the prefrontal cortex
maturity to manage them effectively.
o Highlights a disparity between emotional intensity and cognitive control during adolescence.
4. Influences on Brain Development
o Ongoing debate about the sequence of brain changes and experiences (Lerner et al., 2009).
o A study showed that resisting peer pressure can enhance prefrontal cortex development (Paus et al.,
2007).
o Factors influencing adjustment in adolescents include:
Biological Factors: Smaller hippocampal volume correlates with academic achievement.
Psychosocial Factors: Parental cultural socialization plays a role (Qu et al., 2018).
IMAGEN study: Suggests a strong relationship between biological and social/environmental
factors affecting brain development (Modabbernia et al., 2020).
5. Nature vs. Nurture Debate
o The question remains whether biological changes precede experiences or vice versa, highlighting the
nature/nurture issue in development (Giedd et al., 2012).
Childhood
Recommended Sleep Duration:
o Canadian Paediatric Society suggests 10 to 12 hours of sleep for school-age children .
Sleep Structure:
o Most young children experience one daytime nap and typically sleep through the night .
o Quality of sleep is critical, with interruptions leading to adverse outcomes .
Challenges in Sleep:
o Over 40% of children encounter sleep problems, which can lead to long-term developmental issues .
o Sleep deprivation at age 12 predicts higher risk of substance use in adolescence .
Factors Influencing Sleep:
o Emotional security in family relationships promotes better sleep quality .
o Strategies for improving sleep include maintaining a cool, dark bedroom, consistent sleep schedules, and
calming pre-bedtime activities .
Adolescence
Recommended Sleep Duration:
o Teens are advised to get 8 to 10 hours of sleep, but averages are closer to 6.5-7.5 hours .
Sleep Deprivation Consequences:
o Lack of sleep is associated with poor academic performance, risk-taking behaviors, and increased
substance use .
o A significant percentage of teens report falling asleep in school or arriving late due to oversleeping .
Biological Changes:
o Adolescents undergo a hormonal phase shift that delays melatonin secretion, resulting in later sleep onset
compared to younger adolescents .
o Screen time and social media usage are linked to decreased sleep duration .
Research Insights:
o Mary Carskadon found that older adolescents tend to be sleepier during the day due to this biological
clock shift .
o Increased sleep is correlated with improved mental health and fewer behavioral problems .
o Exercise contributes to physical development and enhances cognitive skills, including attention and
creativity (Han et al., 2017; Best, 2010).
Vigorous Activity Advantages:
o Vigorous exercise yields more benefits than moderate activity (Owens et al., 2017).
Metabolic Health:
o Higher physical activity levels are associated with lower risks of metabolic diseases (Nyström et al.,
2017).
Sedentary Interruptions:
o Brief interruptions of sedentary behavior (e.g., three minutes of walking every 30 minutes) can improve
metabolic health in overweight children (Broadney et al., 2018).
Adolescence
Decline in Exercise:
o Physical activity declines from early to late adolescence, particularly in American teens (Allison et al.,
2007).
Gender Differences:
o Male adolescents tend to be more active than females, with 40% of females and 57% of males meeting
activity guidelines (Butcher et al., 2008).
Health Outcomes:
o Regular exercise is linked to better weight regulation, lower blood pressure, and reduced risk of type 2
diabetes (Goldfield et al., 2012; So et al., 2013).
Positive Effects of Exercise on Adolescents:
Substance Use: Higher levels of exercise correlate with lower rates of alcohol, cigarette, and drug use (Teery-
McElrath et al., 2012).
Mood and Sleep: A daily running program improves sleep quality, mood, and concentration (Kalak et al., 2012).
Depression: Exercise interventions can reduce depression among adolescents (Dopp et al., 2012).
Cognition: Regular physical activity improves cognitive functions, including memory and creativity (Misuraca et
al., 2017).
4. Influences on Children’s and Adolescents’ Exercise Habits
Role of Parents
Positive Modeling: Children with exercising parents are more likely to adopt active lifestyles (Lindsay et al.,
2018).
Role of Peers
Support and Influence: Peer support and the quality of friendships influence physical activity levels in
adolescents (Mollborn & Lawrence, 2018).
Screen-Based Activity
Negative Correlation with Fitness: Increased screen time is associated with lower physical fitness and higher
rates of overweight (Potter et al., 2018; Sisson et al., 2010).
Health Problems: High screen time correlates with various health issues, including obesity and depression
(Costigan et al., 2013; Twenge et al., 2018).
5. Strategies to Increase Exercise in Children and Adolescents
Enhance School Fitness Classes: Improve the quality of physical education programs.
Volunteer Programs: Offer more community-run physical activity programs in schools.
Increase Community Engagement: Foster community initiatives that promote physical activity.
Screen Time Regulations: Implement regulations at home and school to limit screen time.
Child-Led Activities: Encourage children to plan their own physical activities.
Family Involvement: Challenge families to engage in more physical activities together.
Chapter 4
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Processes of Cognitive Development
1. Schemes
P a g e | 38
Definition: Mental representations or frameworks that organize knowledge and facilitate understanding
of the world.
Types of Schemes:
o Behavioral Schemes: Characterize infancy, involving physical activities (e.g., sucking, looking,
grasping).
o Mental Schemes: Develop in childhood, encompassing strategies and cognitive activities (e.g.,
classifying objects by size, shape, or color).
Evolution of Schemes: As individuals grow, they construct a vast array of schemes for various
activities, such as driving or managing finances.
2. Assimilation and Accommodation
Assimilation:
o Definition: Incorporating new experiences into existing schemes.
o Example: A toddler learning the word "car" may initially label all vehicles as cars, using
existing knowledge to understand new experiences.
Accommodation:
o Definition: Modifying existing schemes or creating new ones to incorporate new information.
o Example: Upon realizing that motorcycles and trucks are not cars, the toddler adjusts their
understanding and creates new categories.
Process Interaction: Assimilation and accommodation work together, continuously refining schemes
through experiences.
3. Organization
Definition: The process of connecting schemes to create a higher-order cognitive system.
Function: As children encounter new information, they organize their understanding, which evolves
with their experiences.
4. Equilibration
Definition: The process of balancing assimilation and accommodation to achieve a stable understanding
of the world.
Equilibrium: A comfortable cognitive state when most experiences fit existing schemes.
Disequilibrium: A state of cognitive conflict when new information does not fit existing schemes,
prompting a need to adjust understanding.
Example: A child who believes pouring liquid into a different shaped container changes its amount may
initially be confused but will resolve this through exploration and experimentation.
o Definition: Understanding that objects continue to exist even when out of sight.
o Importance: Marks a significant cognitive achievement in the sensorimotor stage.
Critiques of Piaget's Sensorimotor Stage
1. Transition Processes:
o Research has questioned Piaget's claims about the necessity of certain processes for transitioning
between substages, particularly the A-not-B error, which may be influenced by memory rather
than cognitive understanding.
2. Earlier Cognitive Abilities:
o Studies suggest infants possess a more advanced understanding of the world than Piaget
proposed. The violation of expectations method demonstrates that infants are surprised by
unexpected outcomes, indicating prior knowledge.
3. Role of Nature:
o The core knowledge approach argues that children are born with innate learning abilities tailored
for specific domains (e.g., space, number sense, object permanence). This perspective implies
that Piaget underestimated innate cognitive capabilities.
4. Number Sense:
o Research indicates that infants can distinguish quantities, challenging the notion that numerical
understanding is absent in young infants.
o Symbolic Representation: Children use symbols (words, drawings) to represent objects and ideas. For
example, a child might use a broom as a horse.
o Pretend Play: Engaging in imaginative play, enhancing creativity and understanding of roles.
Limitations of Symbolic Function:
Egocentrism:
o Definition: Difficulty in seeing the world from perspectives other than their own.
o Example: A child assumes everyone sees what they see; during a phone call, they might nod instead of
verbally responding, assuming the parent can see them.
Animism:
o Definition: The belief that inanimate objects have lifelike qualities.
o Example: A child might say, "The tree pushed the leaf off," attributing human actions to non-human
entities.
Illustrations of Creativity:
Drawings during this substage can be fanciful and imaginative, like blue suns and yellow skies, reflecting their
unique perception of the world.
2. Intuitive Thought Substage (Ages 4 to 7)
Definition: The second substage characterized by the beginning of primitive reasoning and a desire to know the
answers to questions.
Key Developments:
o Primitive Reasoning: Children start asking “why” questions, indicating a growing interest in
understanding their environment.
o Questions: A 5-year-old might ask an average of 76 questions per hour, seeking to learn about their
world.
Limitations of Intuitive Thought:
Centration:
o Definition: Focusing on one characteristic of an object to the exclusion of others.
o Example: In a conservation task, a child might focus only on the height of liquid in a container rather than
its volume.
Lack of Conservation:
o Definition: Failing to understand that altering an object’s appearance does not change its fundamental
properties.
o Example: In Piaget's conservation task, children may believe that pouring liquid into a taller, thinner glass
increases the amount of liquid because they focus on height rather than volume.
Key Concepts in Preoperational Thought
1. Egocentrism
Inability to differentiate between one’s own perspective and that of others.
Illustrated by Piaget's three-mountains task, where children select photos from their perspective rather than the
doll's perspective.
2. Animism
The belief that inanimate objects possess human-like qualities or feelings.
3. Centration
The tendency to focus on one aspect of a situation while neglecting others.
4. Lack of Conservation
The inability to understand that quantity remains the same despite changes in shape or appearance.
Cognitive Limitations and Variability
Children may perform differently on various conservation tasks, showing that cognitive development is not
uniform across all areas.
Research indicates that attention and brain development, particularly in the prefrontal cortex, play significant
roles in performance on conservation tasks.
P a g e | 41
o Example: Two identical balls of clay; one is shaped into a long piece. Children learn that the amount of
clay remains the same despite the change in shape by ages 7-8.
o Conservation involves recognizing that transformations alter appearance but not the properties (length,
number, mass, quantity, area, weight, volume).
o Mastery order: number → length → liquid quantity → mass → weight → volume.
o Horizontal Décalage: Similar abilities don’t emerge simultaneously; some conservation tasks are
mastered before others.
Development of Classification
Key Skills:
o Classification: Understanding relationships among sets and subsets.
o Seriation: Ordering objects along a quantitative dimension (e.g., length).
Example: Ordering sticks by length rather than categorizing them as “big” or “little.”
o Transitivity: Understanding relational logic (e.g., if A > B and B > C, then A > C).
Formal Operational Stage
Age Range: Between 11 and 15 years.
Characteristics:
o Ability to think abstractly and logically beyond concrete experiences.
o Development of idealistic and hypothetical thinking (e.g., imagining ideal parents, future possibilities).
o More systematic problem-solving through hypothetical-deductive reasoning.
o Increased capacity to reflect on one’s own thoughts (metacognition).
Adolescent Egocentrism
Concepts:
o Imaginary Audience: Adolescents feel as though they are the focus of attention (e.g., worrying about
their appearance).
o Personal Fable: Sense of uniqueness and invincibility, leading to a belief that others cannot understand
their feelings.
o Not all children exhibit “preoperational” thinking uniformly; some may demonstrate advanced
understanding earlier.
2. Stages of Development:
o Piaget’s notion of stages as unitary structures is challenged; not all concepts appear simultaneously.
o Development is not strictly stage-like; children may display different competencies at varying times.
3. Effects of Training:
o Children can be trained to think at higher levels than suggested by Piaget, indicating potential for
cognitive development through targeted instruction.
4. Cultural and Educational Influences:
o Cultural context plays a significant role in cognitive development, which Piaget underestimated. Practices
in education can accelerate or inhibit skill acquisition.
Neo-Piagetian Approach
Revisions Needed: Argues that Piaget's theories require significant updates.
Emphasis on Processing: Focus on children's attention, memory, and strategies in problem-solving.
Task Division: Attention to specific tasks and dividing problems into manageable steps provides a more accurate
portrayal of children's thinking.
Summary
Piaget's theory has provided valuable insights into cognitive development, influencing educational practices. However,
contemporary research suggests the need for revisions, acknowledging cultural, individual, and contextual factors in
children's learning. The Neo-Piagetian perspective offers a more nuanced understanding, focusing on cognitive strategies
and the variability in children's cognitive abilities.
o Begin teaching at the upper limit of the ZPD, providing sufficient support for children to reach higher
levels of skill and knowledge.
4. Peer Learning
o Utilize more-skilled peers to facilitate learning, creating collaborative learning environments.
5. Encouraging Private Speech
o Foster children’s use of private speech, promoting self-regulation and problem-solving abilities.
6. Meaningful Contexts for Learning
o Contextualize instruction in real-world applications to enhance engagement and understanding.
7. Transformative Classroom Environments
o Implement flipped classrooms and integrated grade levels to promote student-centered learning and peer
collaboration.
Evaluating Vygotsky’s Theory
Contributions:
o Emphasized the role of social interaction and cultural context in cognitive development.
o Introduced practical concepts such as ZPD and scaffolding, which have influenced educational practices.
o Highlighted the importance of language in thought processes.
Criticisms:
o May downplay the role of individual cognitive processes in favor of social interactions.
o Lack of precise mechanisms on how ZPD and scaffolding work in practice.
o Some researchers argue that cognitive development can occur without significant social interaction,
challenging the centrality of social factors in Vygotsky's theory.
Comparison with Piaget’s Theory
Similarities:
o Both view children as active learners who construct knowledge.
o Emphasize developmental stages, though their views on the nature and sequence differ.
Differences:
o Piaget: Focuses on individual exploration and cognitive structures; children learn through interacting with
the physical world.
o Vygotsky: Highlights the social context and cultural tools in learning; cognitive development is heavily
influenced by social interactions.
Evaluating Vygotsky’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Overview
Vygotsky’s theory, although contemporaneous with Piaget's, gained recognition later and has not been evaluated
as thoroughly.
Emphasizes sociocultural influences on children's development and the contextual factors in learning.
Key Distinctions from Piaget’s Theory
Inner Speech:
o Vygotsky highlights the importance of inner speech in cognitive development, while Piaget views it as a
sign of immaturity.
Constructivist Approaches:
o Both theories are constructivist, but Vygotsky’s is specifically a social constructivist approach,
focusing on knowledge construction through social interaction.
Focus Shift:
o Moving from Piaget to Vygotsky represents a shift from individual learning to collaboration, social
interaction, and sociocultural activity.
Endpoint of Cognitive Development
Piaget: The endpoint is formal operational thought, representing a stage where logical and abstract thinking
becomes possible.
Vygotsky: The endpoint varies based on cultural values and the skills deemed important within that culture.
Knowledge Construction
Piaget: Knowledge is constructed through transforming, organizing, and reorganizing previous knowledge.
Vygotsky: Knowledge is constructed through social interactions with peers and teachers.
P a g e | 46
Understanding Cognitive Development: Developmental cognitive neuroscience (DCN) examines how cognitive
development is supported by changes in brain structure and function (Johnson & de Haan, 2015).
Neurobiological Framework: DCN frames cognitive development as a neurobiological process, which enhances
our understanding of how learning occurs.
Historical Context
Piaget and Vygotsky: Both theorists emphasized interaction and social context in cognitive development but did
not address the brain's role. Their ideas are foundational but limited in light of modern neuroscience (Ward,
2019).
Key Concept: Curiosity
Neural Activation: Studies using fMRI show that different regions of the brain activate when individuals are
curious, suggesting that curiosity can vary among children and can influence learning outcomes (Kidd & Hayden,
2015).
Stress, Emotions, and Cognition
Executive Function Skills
Prefrontal Cortex: Adele Diamond's research focuses on the prefrontal cortex, crucial for executive functions
such as:
o Reasoning
o Working Memory
o Cognitive Flexibility
o Inhibitory Control
o Attention
Training and Improvement: Evidence suggests that executive function skills can be improved through targeted
training programs for all ages (Diamond & Ling, 2016).
Impact of Stress on Cognition
Cortisol and Executive Functions: High levels of cortisol, a stress hormone, negatively affect prefrontal cortex
functioning and specific cognitive capacities, including:
o Visual and verbal memory
o Working memory for numbers, words, and mathematical concepts (Heffelfinger & Newcomer, 2001;
Lupien et al., 2005; MacKinnon-McQuarrie et al., 2014).
Link Between Learning and Emotion: There is a strong connection between emotional well-being and cognitive
performance. Stress must be addressed to optimize learning conditions.
Math Anxiety and Performance
Neuroscience Findings: Research shows that math anxiety disrupts functioning in brain regions associated with
working memory, affecting math performance (Ramirez et al., 2016).
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience and Education
Collaboration Between Neuroscience and Education
International Mind, Brain and Education Society: Promotes the merging of neuroscience research with
educational practices.
Caution Against Misleading Products: Educators must be wary of commercial brain-based learning products
lacking scientific backing (Ansari, Coch, & De Smedt, 2011).
Applications in Education
1. Training Executive Function Skills:
o Programs aimed at improving skills like reasoning, working memory, and cognitive flexibility can
enhance academic performance (Cortés Pascual et al., 2019).
o Computer-based training programs have shown success in strengthening these skills (Diamond, 2012).
2. Addressing Stress and Emotions:
o Recognizing the impact of neurophysiological stress on learning emphasizes the need for a calm and safe
environment for children.
o This understanding informs how educators support students’ emotional and cognitive needs.
3. Incorporating Neuroscience in Educator Training:
o Basic knowledge of brain structure and function should be part of teacher training to better support
learning.
P a g e | 48
o Educators must discern between valuable research-based programs and those lacking scientific evidence
(Ansari, 2011).
4. Informing Educational Priorities:
o Recent findings on stress and emotional needs are shaping educational practices and policies.
o Greater emphasis is being placed on addressing socio-emotional needs to enhance cognitive potential.
Conclusion
Developmental cognitive neuroscience provides critical insights into cognitive processes, emphasizing the relationship
between brain function, emotional health, and learning. Its integration into educational practices has the potential to create
more effective and supportive learning environments for children.
Chapter 5
MOTOR, SENSORY, AND PERCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT
Dynamic Systems View and Reflexes in Motor Development
1. Arnold Gesell’s Maturation Theory (1934)
Gesell believed motor skills develop through a genetic plan or maturation.
He observed that infants develop motor skills like rolling over, sitting, and standing in a fixed order and
within specific time frames.
His work emphasized the hereditary nature of motor development.
2. Dynamic Systems Theory
Proposed by Esther Thelen (1941–2004), dynamic systems theory suggests motor development is an
interactive process involving multiple factors.
Infants develop motor skills by perceiving the environment and acting on it. Perception and action are
coupled.
Key Points:
o Motor skills are assembled as solutions to infant goals.
o Infants are motivated to act (e.g., reaching a toy), leading them to develop a new motor
behavior.
o A motor skill results from the interaction of several factors:
Development of the nervous system.
The body’s physical properties (e.g., leg growth, muscle control).
Motivation to achieve a goal.
Environmental support (e.g., a parent encouraging a child to walk).
Motor Skill Development Process:
o Infants explore different movement patterns to solve motor challenges.
P a g e | 49
o Motor development involves a process of adaptation through repeated cycles of action and
perception.
o Even universal milestones like crawling or walking are learned through fine-tuning movements
to fit new tasks.
Example of Motor Skill Development:
o Gabriel (from Thelen’s study) must adapt his body and movements to grasp a toy from a sitting
position, adjusting in real-time.
3. Reflexes in Infants
Reflexes are automatic responses to stimuli and serve as survival mechanisms for infants.
Some reflexes are important for survival (e.g., rooting and sucking reflexes) as they help newborns
obtain nourishment.
Examples of Reflexes:
o Rooting Reflex: When the infant’s cheek is stroked, they turn toward the touch and begin to
suck.
o Sucking Reflex: Newborns automatically suck objects placed in their mouth, a crucial reflex for
feeding.
o Moro Reflex: In response to sudden movement or noise, newborns arch their back and fling
their arms and legs out, then close them. This reflex was a survival mechanism for primate
ancestors.
Reflex Persistence and Disappearance:
o Some reflexes, like coughing, sneezing, and blinking, persist throughout life.
o Other reflexes, like rooting and Moro reflex, disappear by 3 to 4 months as the infant gains
voluntary control over movements.
o The persistence or reappearance of reflexes can indicate neurological issues.
4. Grasping Reflex and Transition to Voluntary Action
The grasping reflex is triggered when an object touches the infant’s palm.
By 3 months, infants begin to develop voluntary control over this reflex, transitioning into more
refined and purposeful grasping.
5. Differences in Reflexive Behaviors
Variability exists in reflexive behaviors among newborns. For instance, newborns' sucking abilities
differ, with some being more efficient than others.
The process of mastering feeding reflects changes in the nervous system and coordination with
environmental factors (e.g., how the infant is held, the flow of milk).
6. Reflexes in the Context of Dynamic Systems Theory
Earlier views treated reflexes as entirely genetic and automatic.
The modern view aligns with dynamic systems theory, suggesting that reflexes can be modulated by
infants to achieve goals. For instance, they can adjust their movements to make a mobile move or
change their sucking rate to hear a recording.
7. Studies on Infant Reflexes and Sucking Behavior
T. Berry Brazelton’s Study (1956): Observed changes in infant sucking behavior over time.
o 85% of infants exhibited non-feeding-related sucking (fingers, pacifiers).
o By age 1, most had stopped, but 40% continued thumb-sucking into school age (Kessen, Haith,
& Salapatek, 1970).
Reflexes serve as building blocks for more complex motor behaviors, transitioning from automatic to
voluntary control as the infant matures.
The dynamic systems theory emphasizes the active role of the infant in motor development, showing
how infants use their perceptions to continuously adjust their actions to reach their goals.
Cultural influence: Cultural beliefs and practices regarding caregiving can impact how motor skills are
stimulated and supported.
Learning to Walk
Locomotion and posture are closely linked, especially in walking, which involves balancing on one leg
while swinging the other forward. Early leg alternation is present even before birth due to established
neural pathways.
Challenges in walking:
o Infants initially take small steps due to limited balance control and strength.
o Occasionally, infants take large steps, indicating improved balance and strength.
Learning surfaces: Infants learn through trial and error about safe and risky surfaces for walking, as
seen in Karen Adolph’s steep slope experiments. Experienced crawlers and walkers learn to avoid
steep slopes and assess their skills against environmental challenges.
Practice and learning: Infants accumulate immense experience with balance and locomotion. Each step
varies slightly due to different terrains and bodily constraints, which aids in refining walking skills.
First Year Motor Milestones and Variations
Infants go through a standard sequence of gross motor milestones, but timing can vary due to
environmental factors like the introduction of sleep positions (e.g., sleeping on the back delayed
crawling).
Cultural variations: Some infants may never crawl or may discover alternative locomotion methods,
such as rolling.
Individual differences: Not all infants follow the standard motor milestones, with some skipping
crawling or walking later than expected.
Development in the Second Year
Motor accomplishments in the first year lead to increased independence in the second year. Gross motor
skills by 18-24 months include:
o Pulling a toy on a string
o Walking quickly or running
o Walking backward without losing balance
o Kicking and throwing a ball while standing
o Jumping in place
Structured exercise classes are generally not recommended for infants, though vigorous handling (in
certain cultures) may advance motor development.
Motor Development in Early Childhood (3-5 years)
3 years old: Children enjoy simple movements like hopping, jumping, and running.
4 years old: Children become more adventurous, climbing low jungle gyms and learning to go down
stairs using one foot per step.
5 years old: Children become more confident and perform more complex motor activities, such as
running races and attempting stunts.
Organized sports are widely available and offer children opportunities to improve motor skills, learn
competition, persistence, and develop peer relationships.
Positive impacts of sports:
o Improved physical fitness
o Enhanced motor skills
o Socialization and teamwork
o Reduced likelihood of obesity
Negative impacts:
o Pressure to achieve and win
o Risk of injury or burnout
o Stress related to performance expectations
Conclusion
Active participation: Encouraging active, rather than passive, activities benefits children’s gross motor
development, cognitive functioning, and overall health. Structured play and sports are essential in
promoting these skills throughout early childhood and beyond.
o Touch is one of the earliest senses to develop, with newborns responding to physical touch,
which plays a key role in emotional bonding.
Intermodal Perception
Intermodal Perception: The ability to integrate information from two or more senses (e.g., sight and
sound).
o Example: Infants can associate the sound of a voice with the sight of a moving mouth.
Nature, Nurture, and Perceptual Development
Gene-Environment Interplay: Both genetics and experiences shape sensory and perceptual
development.
o Cochlear Implants: An example of how medical technology (nurture) can alter sensory
development in children with hearing loss.
Techniques for Studying Infant Sensory and Perceptual Development
Eye-Tracking:
o Tracks where infants are looking to study their visual focus and attention.
o Helps in understanding cognitive development and social interaction.
o Used to study at-risk infants, such as those born prematurely or those at risk for autism.
High-Amplitude Sucking:
o Measures auditory perception by tracking sucking responses to sounds.
Habituation and Dishabituation:
o Used to assess infants' ability to recognize and differentiate between stimuli.
The Orienting Response:
o Measures the infant's ability to hear or see by observing whether they turn their head towards the
stimulus.
Key Studies and Methods
1. Fantz’s Visual Preference Method: Demonstrates infants' preference for patterns over plain stimuli.
2. Habituation and Dishabituation: Shows that infants can distinguish between familiar and new stimuli.
3. High-Amplitude Sucking: Tracks how infants respond to auditory changes.
4. Eye-Tracking Technology: Advances the study of infant perception, such as understanding gaze and
focus during social interactions or media exposure (e.g., Sesame Street study).
Conclusion
Sensory and perceptual development begins at birth and progresses rapidly as infants interact with their
environment.
Both nature (genetics) and nurture (experience) play crucial roles in shaping how infants sense and
perceive the world.
Visual Perception
Infancy:
1. Visual Acuity:
o Newborn vision is very limited: estimated at 20/240 on the Snellen chart. Objects far away are
unclear.
o By 6 months of age, vision improves to 20/40.
o This gradual improvement is a typical part of visual sensory development.
2. Face Perception:
P a g e | 56
Prenatal Hearing:
o By the last two months of pregnancy, fetuses can hear sounds from outside the womb,
including music and their mother’s voice.
o Recognition and Learning:
DeCasper & Spence (1986) study: Fetuses that heard The Cat in the Hat preferred it
after birth over a different story, indicating prenatal learning of rhythm and sound.
Mother’s Voice Preference: Fetuses and newborns prefer their mother's voice over the
father's. Studies (Lee & Kisilevsky, 2014) showed that fetuses increased their heart rate
when hearing their mother's voice.
Postnatal Changes in Hearing:
o Loudness: Newborns cannot hear soft sounds as well as adults. They need louder sounds for
perception, making it difficult to diagnose hearing impairments early on.
o Pitch: Infants are more sensitive to high-pitched sounds but less to low-pitched ones. By 7
months, they can process multiple pitches and by 2 years, they can distinguish different pitches
effectively.
o Localization: Even newborns can detect where a sound originates, but this skill improves
significantly by 7 months.
Hearing Interventions:
o Cochlear Implants: Used for children born deaf, as early as 12 months. Early intervention with
cochlear implants or hearing aids, along with supportive language exposure, improves speech
and language outcomes.
o Otitis Media: A middle-ear infection common in infancy that can affect hearing and language
development. Chronic otitis media may require tubes to drain fluid from the ear.
o Sensitivity to taste begins before birth, with fetuses learning about different flavors through
amniotic fluid and later through breast milk.
o Preferences:
Newborns show a preference for sweet tastes.
2-hour-old infants react differently to sweet, sour, and bitter tastes, displaying facial
expressions that reflect their preferences.
o By a few months of age, infants also develop a preference for salty tastes, which is nutritionally
adaptive.
Intermodal Perception
Definition: Intermodal perception involves integrating information from two or more sensory modalities
(e.g., vision and hearing).
o Example: When watching a basketball game, individuals combine the visual input of a ball
bouncing with the sound of it hitting the ground.
Development in Infancy:
o Early Reflexes: Newborns exhibit primitive intermodal abilities, such as turning their head
toward a sound.
o Visual-Auditory Integration: By 3 months, infants look longer at their mother when they hear
her voice, showing coordination between visual and auditory cues.
o Visual-Touch Integration: By 2-3 months, infants can coordinate visual and touch information,
and this ability develops rapidly across the first year.
Maturation:
o Early forms of intermodal perception become sharper through experience. During the first six
months, infants have difficulty connecting sensory inputs, but by the second half of the first
year, their ability to mentally integrate these inputs improves.
Nature, Nurture, and Perceptual Development
Nature vs. Nurture:
o Nativists believe that perception is largely innate (inborn), while empiricists argue that
perception is learned through experience.
o The Gibsons’ ecological view leans toward nativism, suggesting that infants are born with the
ability to detect size and shape constancy and other perceptual skills, but also acknowledge that
environmental interaction plays a role.
o Piaget’s Constructivist View: Perception develops through cognitive stages, implying that
infants learn complex perceptual tasks through experience rather than being born with them.
Research on Cataracts:
o Maurer’s research on infants born with cataracts highlights the role of early visual input in
perceptual development.
o Infants whose cataracts are removed in the first few months show normal visual development,
while delayed surgery leads to impaired visual perception. This emphasizes the importance of
early sensory experience for normal perceptual development.
Summary
P a g e | 59
Infants are born with innate perceptual abilities, but their skills improve dramatically through
experience and interaction with their environment.
The development of perception involves a dynamic interaction between nature and nurture, with
environmental experiences shaping and refining sensory abilities.
Early intervention in cases of sensory impairments (e.g., hearing loss, cataracts) is critical to ensure
normal perceptual and cognitive development.
Perceptual-Motor Coupling
Definition and Overview
Perceptual-motor coupling refers to the interconnectedness of perception and motor action.
Traditionally, psychology has distinguished between perceiving and doing, but contemporary research
emphasizes that perception and action are tightly linked.
Key researchers:
o Esther Thelen’s dynamic systems approach examines how individuals organize motor behaviors
to coordinate perception and action.
o Eleanor and James J. Gibson's ecological approach focuses on how perception guides action and
vice versa.
Main Concepts
1. Dynamic Systems Approach (Thelen)
o People organize motor behaviors to engage in both perceiving and acting. Babies, for example,
must coordinate their movements with perceptual information to balance, reach, and move
through space.
o Infants are motivated to move by what they perceive, such as reaching for a toy. Their
movements may initially be awkward, but they learn patterns that help them achieve their goals.
2. Ecological Approach (Gibsons)
o Perception and action are not separate processes; they work together. Action can guide
perception (e.g., moving introduces new views of objects and people), and perception can guide
action (e.g., reaching for a toy based on seeing it).
o Through active movement, individuals learn how to adapt to their environment by perceiving
and acting in response to new situations.
Perceptual-Motor Development in Infants
Example of infant action-perception coupling:
o Infants see an attractive toy and must figure out how to move their limbs to reach it. Initially,
their attempts are uncoordinated, but over time, they learn to adjust their actions to reach their
desired object effectively.
o Locomotion (e.g., crawling or walking) helps babies understand different visual perspectives
and how surfaces support their weight.
o Mouthing objects introduces object properties, such as texture and hardness, further educating
perception.
Importance of Interaction Between Perception and Action:
o Action educates perception: Exploring objects through touch, vision, and movement helps
infants learn about the world. For example, when infants manipulate an object, they learn about
its texture, size, and weight.
o Perception motivates action: Infants are driven to move by what they see. For instance, if they
see a toy they want to play with, they must figure out how to use their motor skills to reach it.
P a g e | 60
o Children apply what they have learned in previous situations to new ones, showing adaptability
in cognitive tasks. This process is supported by metacognition—thinking about thinking.
o Metacognition example: Children learn to remember better by relating information to personal
experiences, showing a deeper level of understanding and processing.
Comparison with Piaget’s Theory
Similarities:
o Both approaches emphasize that children actively construct knowledge and understanding.
o Both recognize cognitive limitations at different stages of development and seek to explain how
these limitations are overcome.
o The information-processing approach, like Piaget’s, can be seen as constructivist, where
children actively direct their own cognitive development.
Differences:
o Piaget's theory involves distinct, qualitative stages of development (e.g., sensorimotor,
preoperational, etc.), while the information-processing approach sees cognitive development as
gradual and continuous.
o The information-processing approach places greater emphasis on the precise analysis of
cognitive processes, focusing on mechanisms such as encoding, automaticity, and strategy
development to explain change.
o Piaget's theory highlights major transitions between stages, while the information-processing
approach suggests that cognitive skills develop continuously without abrupt shifts.
Key Takeaways
The information-processing approach views children as active participants in their cognitive
development.
Cognitive resources like capacity and processing speed are essential for memory and problem-
solving, and both improve as children grow.
Mechanisms of change, such as encoding, automaticity, and strategy construction, drive cognitive
development.
Continuous and gradual development is a hallmark of the information-processing approach, in
contrast to Piaget’s stage-based theory.
Practical Implications
Understanding how children process information helps educators develop effective learning strategies
and interventions, especially for children from disadvantaged backgrounds.
Metacognitive strategies (e.g., thinking about how to remember information) are especially useful in
improving learning outcomes.
4. Executive Attention: Managing goal-directed tasks, detecting errors, and monitoring progress (e.g.,
planning and adapting strategies in a complex game).
Attention in Infancy
Newborn Attention: Even newborns can detect contours and fixate on them. Over time, infants become
better at scanning objects.
By 4 months, infants can selectively attend to specific objects.
Orienting/Investigative Process:
Involves directing attention to important locations and recognizing features of objects.
Between 3 and 9 months, infants rapidly improve their ability to flexibly and quickly deploy their
attention.
Sustained Attention in Infants:
New stimuli prompt an initial orienting response, followed by sustained attention.
At 3 months, infants can maintain focus for 5–10 seconds, increasing through the second year.
Memory
What is Memory?
Definition: Memory is the retention of information over time, allowing individuals to connect past
experiences with present situations. It is essential for performing daily activities and functions.
Key Processes of Memory
1. Encoding: The initial process of transforming sensory input into a format that can be stored.
2. Storage: The maintenance of encoded information over time.
3. Retrieval: The process of recalling stored information when needed.
Types of Memory
Short-term Memory:
o Capacity: Limited (7±2 items).
o Duration: Typically retained for 15 to 30 seconds.
o Role: Acts as a temporary storage for information before it is either discarded or moved to long-
term memory.
Long-term Memory:
o Duration: More permanent storage of information.
o Role: Stores more extensive information, such as personal experiences and knowledge (e.g.,
childhood games, first loves).
Working Memory
Definition: A type of short-term memory that involves active manipulation and assembly of
information.
Components (Alan Baddeley’s Model):
o Central Executive: Manages and regulates attention and cognitive resources.
o Phonological Loop: Deals with verbal and auditory information.
o Visuospatial Sketchpad: Handles visual and spatial information.
Importance of Working Memory in Development
Better working memory is linked to enhanced:
o Reading comprehension.
o Math skills.
P a g e | 65
o Problem-solving abilities.
Recent Studies:
o Working memory predicts emergent literacy in low-income children.
o It influences foreign language learning in fourth graders.
o Computerized interventions improve reading performance in children.
o Assessment in kindergarten helps predict future academic achievement.
Constructing Memories
Memory construction is not a simple recording process. Instead, memories are formed and reconstructed
through several cognitive processes.
Schema Theory
Definition: Schemas are mental frameworks that help organize and interpret information. They
influence memory encoding, interpretation, and retrieval.
Example: A basketball fan and a visitor without basketball knowledge may interpret the same
conversation differently due to their existing schemas.
Fuzzy Trace Theory
Definition: Proposes two types of memory representations:
1. Verbatim Trace: Detailed and precise information.
2. Fuzzy Trace: General ideas or the gist of the information.
Developmental Trend: Preschoolers remember verbatim better, while older children and adults favor
gist, leading to improved memory with age.
Content Knowledge and Expertise
The ability to remember new information is affected by prior knowledge. Expertise enhances memory
performance.
Expert vs. Novice:
o Experts retain more information relevant to their expertise.
o Example Study: Experienced chess players remember chess-related information better than
novices, but novices outperform in unrelated stimuli.
Developmental Changes
Memory improves with age due to increased expertise and knowledge across various subjects.
Sociocultural factors also play a role in shaping memory development but are often overlooked in
research.
Summary
Memory is a complex cognitive function that evolves throughout life stages, influenced by developmental
factors, knowledge acquisition, and social contexts. Understanding how memory works and changes can help in
fields such as education, psychology, and cognitive development.
Infancy
Memory Development
Early Memory: Contrary to past beliefs (Leach, 2010), research indicates that infants as young as 3
months demonstrate early stages of memory development (Howe, Courage, & Rooksby, 2009).
First Memories
Rovee-Collier's Research: Infants can remember perceptual-motor information.
o Experiment: Babies were placed in cribs under mobile toys, with a ribbon tied to their ankle.
Kicking made the mobile move.
o Findings: Babies would kick again weeks later if the mobile looked the same as when they were
originally tied to it, demonstrating memory retention.
Implicit vs. Explicit Memory:
P a g e | 66
o Implicit Memory: Exists without conscious recollection; involves procedural memories (e.g.,
riding a bicycle).
Infants display implicit memory from 2 to 6 months, capable of retaining experiences
until 1.5 to 2 years (Rovee-Collier & Barr, 2010).
o Explicit Memory: Involves conscious recall of facts/experiences; develops in the second half of
the first year (Bauer & Fivush, 2013).
Long-term Memory
Maturation of Brain Structures: Between 6 to 12 months, maturation of the hippocampus and frontal
lobes facilitates explicit memory (Bauer & Fivush, 2013).
Developmental Timeline:
o 6 months: Memory retention for 24 hours.
o 20 months: Memory for experiences from 12 months prior.
Infantile Amnesia
Definition: Most adults recall little from the first 3 years of life (Riggins et al., 2016).
Causes:
o Immature prefrontal lobes and lack of connections to the hippocampus hinder long-term
memory formation (Jabès & Nelson, 2015).
Childhood
Memory Improvements
Memory significantly improves after infancy (Bauer & Fivush, 2013; Bjorklund & Causey, 2017).
Young children can recall more information with appropriate cues and prompts.
Factors Influencing Memory
Expertise: Children's growing knowledge in various areas helps improve memory recall.
Fuzzy Trace Theory:
o Young children encode and store verbatim traces.
o Older children start using gist memory, leading to more enduring memory traces.
Memory Span
Development of Short-term Memory:
o Classic studies show memory span increases with age (e.g., from 2 digits at 2 years to 5 digits at
7 years; Dempster, 1981).
Factors Influencing Memory Span:
o Processing Speed: Faster identification of memory items correlates with larger memory span.
o Rehearsal: Older children are more likely to use rehearsal strategies.
Memory Strategies
1. Organization:
o Organizing information enhances memory (e.g., remembering months in chronological order is
easier than alphabetically).
o By middle childhood, children often use organization to aid memory (Schneider & Ornstein,
2015).
2. Elaboration:
o Involves deeper processing of information, such as making personal associations or examples.
o Older children and adolescents are more likely to use elaboration strategies spontaneously.
3. Imagery:
o Using mental images aids memory retention, more effective for older children but beneficial for
young children in recalling visual information.
Teaching Strategies to Improve Memory
P a g e | 67
Repeat with Variation: Reinforcing lessons with variations enhances memory consolidation and
expands retrieval routes.
Memory-relevant Language: Teachers should incorporate language that supports memory in their
instruction, improving student performance (Ornstein et al., 2010).
Reconstructive Memory in Children
Schema Influence: Children’s memories are influenced by schemas, affecting how they encode, store,
and retrieve information.
Susceptibility to Suggestion:
o Research shows preschoolers are the most suggestible age group (Ceci, Hritz, & Royer, 2016).
o Distortions can arise from suggestive interviewing techniques.
Adolescence
Memory Changes
Memory Span and Working Memory:
o Memory span continues to increase during adolescence.
o Working memory also improves, as evidenced by cross-sectional studies showing substantial
increases from ages 8 to 24 (Swanson, 1999).
Neural Functioning
Brain imaging studies suggest improvements in working memory during adolescence are linked to shifts
in neural functioning (Simmonds, Hallquist, & Luna, 2017).
Continuing Development
Working memory capabilities continue to improve into adulthood.
Summary
Memory development is a complex, dynamic process that evolves from infancy through adolescence.
Infants show early implicit memory, childhood brings improvements in both explicit memory and the
use of strategies, and adolescence marks continued enhancement of working memory. Understanding
these stages is crucial for educators and parents to support memory retention in children and adolescents.
Thinking: An Overview
Definition of Thinking:
o Involves the manipulation and transformation of information stored in memory.
o Central executive role in Baddeley’s model of working memory.
o Functions include reasoning, reflecting, evaluating ideas, solving problems, and making
decisions.
Infancy: Concept Formation and Categorization
Focus of Research:
P a g e | 68
o Investigates concept formation and categorization (Gelman, 2013; Rakison & Lawson, 2013).
o Concepts are cognitive groupings of similar objects, events, people, or ideas.
o Essential for generalization; without concepts, each object/event would appear unique.
Do Infants Have Concepts?
o Yes, infants possess concepts, although the exact onset of concept formation is not fully
understood (Ferguson & Waxman, 2017).
Research Findings:
o Habituation Experiments:
Infants (3 to 4 months) can group objects by similar appearances (e.g., animals) (Quinn,
2016).
Infants look longer at novel objects than at familiar ones.
o Jean Mandler's Contributions:
Early categorizations are classified as perceptual categorization, based on perceptual
features (e.g., size, color, movement) (Mandler, 2012).
Conceptual categories (e.g., distinguishing birds from airplanes) begin forming around 7
to 9 months.
Example study: Infants (9 to 11 months) classify birds as animals and airplanes as
vehicles despite perceptual similarities (Mandler & McDonough, 1993).
o Second Year of Life:
Continued advancement in categorization (Poulin-Dubois & Pauen, 2017).
Early concepts remain broad (e.g., "food," "animal").
As cognitive development progresses, categories become more distinct (e.g., "fruit" →
"apple," "flying animal" → "bird").
Infants begin categorizing based on shape, a strategy that evolves in early childhood
(Ware, 2017).
Importance of Categorization:
o Learning to categorize correctly is crucial for understanding the world.
o Gopnik (2010) emphasized that sorting the world into the correct categories represents a
significant cognitive advancement.
Key Takeaways:
Thinking is a complex process that evolves throughout development.
Infants are capable of concept formation and categorization, beginning in early infancy.
Perceptual categorizations lay the foundation for more complex conceptual understandings as infants
grow.
P a g e | 69
Intense interests and gender differences in categorization preferences emerge in late infancy and early
childhood.
Childhood Thinking
1. Executive Function
o Definition: A collection of higher-level cognitive processes linked to the prefrontal cortex,
essential for managing thoughts to engage in goal-directed behavior and self-control (Carlson,
Zelazo, & Faja, 2013; Miller & Marcovitch, 2015).
o Components:
Cognitive Inhibition: The ability to suppress a strong tendency or response.
Cognitive Flexibility: The capacity to shift attention between different tasks or topics.
Goal-Setting: The ability to establish objectives (e.g., sharing toys).
Delay of Gratification: Forgoing immediate rewards for greater future rewards
(McDermott & Fox, 2018).
o Development:
Transitions from a stimulus-driven toddler to a child capable of flexible, goal-directed
problem solving (Zelazo, 2015).
Linked with school readiness (Ursache, Blair, & Raver, 2012).
o Role of Parents and Teachers: Good parenting models executive function skills, influencing
children's self-regulation and success in school (Masten, 2012).
o Changes in Middle and Late Childhood:
Self-Control/Inhibition: Essential for concentration and resisting impulsive responses.
Working Memory: Crucial for processing information encountered in school.
Flexibility: Important for considering different strategies and perspectives.
o Research Findings:
Executive function may predict school readiness better than IQ (Duckworth et al., 2019).
Activities like aerobic exercise, mindfulness, and certain curricula (e.g., Montessori) can
enhance executive function (Blair & Raver, 2014; Diamond, 2013).
Inhibitory control in children correlates with positive long-term outcomes (Moffitt et al.,
2011).
o Critiques: The broad concept of executive function may lack consensus on components and
connections; further research is needed (Friedman & Miyake, 2017).
2. Critical Thinking
o Definition: Reflective and productive thinking that evaluates evidence and seeks to understand
how and why things happen.
o Key Aspects:
Questioning facts and looking for supporting evidence.
Arguing logically rather than emotionally.
Recognizing multiple good answers and evaluating them.
Speculating to create new ideas.
o Educational Deficiency: Many schools focus on single correct answers instead of encouraging
deep, meaningful thinking (Grigg & Lewis, 2019).
o Improving Critical Thinking:
Presenting controversial topics and encouraging debates (Litman & Greenleaf, 2018).
Implementing mindfulness to enhance awareness and cognitive flexibility (Farrar &
Tapper, 2018; Langer, 2005).
Mindfulness training improves executive function, attention, and empathy (Roeser &
Zelazo, 2012).
Techniques like yoga and meditation may benefit cognitive development.
3. Scientific Thinking
o Children as Natural Scientists: Children ask fundamental questions and seek answers, showing
curiosity and experimentation.
P a g e | 70
o Speed and Capacity: Increased processing speed and capacity allow for better allocation of
cognitive resources.
o Knowledge Base: Adolescents possess a broader knowledge base across various domains.
o Knowledge Construction: Enhanced ability to combine knowledge in novel ways.
o Strategy Use: Adolescents show greater spontaneity and variety in strategies for obtaining and
applying knowledge (e.g., planning, evaluating alternatives).
Foundation: A solid basis in literacy and math from childhood is essential for the adequate
development of critical thinking skills in adolescence, though some may catch up later.
5. Decision Making
Increased Decision-Making Opportunities: Adolescents face more decisions regarding friendships,
dating, sexual activity, education, etc.
Competence:
o Older adolescents exhibit more competent decision-making than younger adolescents and
children.
o Compared to children, young adolescents are better at generating options, considering
perspectives, anticipating consequences, and evaluating credibility.
Imperfect Skills: Despite improvements, decision-making skills are not flawless, and practical
experience can heavily influence outcomes.
Example: While adolescents may excel in driver training, they still have high accident rates, although
graduated driver licensing (GDR) has shown to reduce these rates.
Both analytical and experiential systems can be beneficial, especially with ongoing improvements in
executive function.
Metacognition
Definition:
Metacognition refers to "cognition about cognition," or the awareness and understanding of one's own
thought processes. It involves knowing when and how to use various strategies for learning and
problem-solving.
Forms of Metacognition:
1. Planning: Deciding how much time to allocate for a task.
2. Evaluation: Monitoring progress toward task completion.
3. Self-regulation: Modifying strategies based on task demands.
Importance of Metacognition:
Enhances the effectiveness of cognitive tasks, leading to improved learning outcomes.
Example: Training low-achieving students in metacognitive skills for math problems improved their
performance and attitudes toward math (Cardelle-Elawar, 1992).
Metamemory:
A specific form of metacognition that involves understanding memory processes.
Includes:
o General knowledge about memory (e.g., recognition vs. recall).
o Personal awareness of one’s own memory capabilities.
Developmental Changes in Metacognition
Childhood:
Children begin to develop metacognitive skills, improving their ability to assess and monitor their own
learning and problem-solving strategies.
Adolescence:
Metacognitive abilities continue to evolve, with adolescents becoming more adept at reflecting on their
thinking and applying strategies effectively across various contexts.
Metacognition in Childhood
Developmental Milestones (Age 5-6):
Awareness of Memory:
o Children understand that familiar items are easier to learn than unfamiliar ones.
o Short lists are perceived as easier to remember compared to longer lists.
o Recognition tasks (e.g., multiple-choice) are understood to be easier than recall tasks (e.g., essay
questions).
o Awareness of forgetting increases with time; they know that memory is less reliable as time
passes (Lyon & Flavell, 1993).
Limitations of Metamemory:
Understanding of Relatedness:
o Young children struggle to grasp that related items (e.g., apple, orange) are easier to remember
than unrelated items (e.g., apple, car).
o They do not yet recognize that remembering the gist of a story is easier than recalling verbatim
details.
o By fifth grade, students begin to comprehend that gist recall is easier than verbatim recall
(Kreutzer & Flavell, 1975).
Inflated Self-Assessment:
Overestimation of Memory Abilities:
P a g e | 74
Preschool children often have an inflated view of their memory capabilities. For instance, many
o
predicted they could recall all 10 items from a list of 10, but none succeeded in doing so (Flavell,
Friedrichs, & Hoyt, 1970).
o As children progress through elementary school, they start to evaluate their memory skills more
realistically.
Cues and Memory:
Appreciation for Memory Cues:
o Preschoolers have limited understanding of the importance of memory cues (e.g., using examples
to remember concepts).
o By ages 7-8, children begin to appreciate how cues can aid memory recall.
General Improvement:
o Overall, children’s understanding of their memory abilities and their evaluation skills improve
significantly from early elementary years to around 11-12 years of age (Bjorklund & Causey,
2017).
Metacognition in Adolescence
Key Changes:
Increased Metacognitive Capacity:
o Adolescents develop a greater ability to monitor and manage their cognitive resources,
enhancing their ability to tackle learning tasks effectively (Kuhn, 2009).
o Their metacognitive skills lead to more effective cognitive functioning and improved learning
outcomes.
Research Findings:
1. Study on Young Adolescents (Age 12-14):
o As adolescents age, they increasingly utilize metacognitive skills more effectively in subjects
like math and history.
o For example, 14-year-olds show improved monitoring of their text comprehension compared to
younger peers (van der Stel & Veenman, 2010).
2. Importance of Metacognitive Skills in College:
o Metacognitive skills (e.g., planning, strategy selection, monitoring) significantly contribute to
college students' critical thinking abilities (Magno, 2010).
Attention Allocation:
Effective Resource Management:
o Adolescents demonstrate a better understanding of how to allocate their attention to various
aspects of a task compared to children.
o They develop a meta-level understanding of strategies, knowing when and which strategies to
apply in different learning contexts.
Individual Variation:
Diversity in Metacognitive Abilities:
o There is considerable individual variation in metacognitive skills among adolescents.
o Some adolescents effectively utilize metacognitive strategies to enhance their learning, while
others may struggle significantly (Kuhn, 2009).
Conclusion
P a g e | 75
Metacognition develops significantly from childhood through adolescence, with children gaining
foundational skills and adolescents refining and applying these skills more effectively.
Understanding one's own cognitive processes is crucial for effective learning and problem-solving, with
variations in development influenced by individual experiences and environmental factors.