Financials
Financials
Financials
6; December 2015
Abstract
This study investigated the impact of successful grammatical error correction among EFL students in Saudi
Arabia, addressed the issues associated with correcting students’ mistakes, and grammar students’ preferences
regarding error correction. Descriptive analytical approach using quantitative research methods and a
questionnaire to collect data from 304 female students at the third secondary grade in Riyadh was employed. The
findings revealed that grammar correction helped students in language learning and in understanding and
remembering the correct answer. Problems such as teachers spending too much time on error correction during
class time, excessive us of Arabic language in English classes, and frustration among students when they make
too many errors. However, students preferred immediate correction of grammatical mistakes and preferred
written (coded) correction of their mistakes. Recommendations included the need for teachers to employ relevant
error correction methods of correction, taking into consideration students’ age, wishes, interests, and language
proficiency levels.
Keywords: Foreign Language, English, Grammar, Errors, Correction, Saudi Arabia
Introduction
Background
The interactions research in Second Language Acquisition (SLA) reports that focusing on linguistic form during
communication contribute to learners’ acquisition of the complex grammatical features along with particular L1-
L2 contrasts (Sheen, 2006). Studies that were conducted on an interaction in classrooms have shown that
feedback is essential in drawing learners' attention to form (Sheen, 2008). According to Long (2007), reactive
feedback that reacts a problem is effective in facilitating second language acquisition: it distinguishes a
grammatical from an ungrammatical one, adjusts the input to match the learners’ proficiency level, and it draws
attention to inter-language gaps, and motivates learners to modify the output. Moreover, preeminent interactions
researchers of second language acquisition report of empirical evidence that short-term second-language learning
is facilitated by corrective or interactional feedback, which includes verbal, error-and-form-focused, and
instructional linguistic evidence of deficient yet effective second language production). Thus, researchers should
work on examining how interaction aids second language development in more ascertainable ways (Mackey,
2007). According to Lee (1997), student can make errors because of the impact of L1, misinterpretation of a rule,
an attempt to speak at one’s best, distraction of attention, or because of all these and other reasons combined (p.
87).
The common cause of mistakes is that students often make an effort to communicate faster in order to develop
language fluency. Consequently, they lack concentration and forget about other aspects of their speech such
grammar, word choice and vocabulary. Fossilization is a practice in which learners “internalize an erroneous
linguistic pattern in their minds” (Leki, 1991, p. 46). These erroneous forms might not be permanent, but once the
learners concentrate on communication and not on form, they will deviate toward this incorrect inter language
pattern. There are also indications that many second language learners fail to obtain anticipated language
competence, being unable to reach the culmination of the inter language continuum. They stop studying as soon
as their inter language holds at least few rules dissimilar from those of the system of target language (Leeman,
2007).As soon as a student acquires a fossilized form, it is difficult to help him or her with further guidelines.
127
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
Ellis & Sheen (2006) suggest that this form can be treated as either a mistake or a correct target language (TL)
forms. For example, it can be assumed that a positive fossilized form would be the same the target language and
the inter language. A learner would acquire an incorrect fossilized form having reached a phase when a structure
has a different form in the TL. The effectiveness of certain types of feedback over others is also debated. For
instance, a number of researchers claim that recasts, defined as the reformulating the learners’ utterances that
include the correct form, are the most effective type of oral feedback that enhances second language learning. It is
considered an instrument for helping learners to cognitively perceive their errors immediately after having uttered
them (Doughty, 2003; Long, 2007). From another perspective, some investigators argue that verbal types of
feedback that require the correct form, such as elicitation and requesting clarifications, are more likely to
contribute to the SLA improvement by motivating learners to stretch their inter language (Ammar & Spada, 2006;
Lyster, 2004). EFL research has thoroughly tackled the important issue of correcting mistakes, and whether it
enhances learners’ grammatical accuracy. The reason for that is that mistakes are naturally occurring in any
learning context and are true evidence of the student’s developing competence in a L2. Therefore, feedbacks have
proven to be more effective in facilitating language acquisition because they present explicit information about
the target-language structure.
Statement of the Problem
Effective error correction has not received the much deserved attention in many EFL contexts despite its
usefulness in language learning. In the Saudi context, Faith (2012) found that error correction during English L2
learners’ oral communication in the United Kingdom and Saudi Arabia was beneficial. Furthermore, Mustafa
(2012) reported that the students in Saudi Arabia do not think positively of the correctional feedback and claim
that the feedback they wish for is very distinct from the one they receive. From a socio-cultural perspective, the
correctional feedback approaches are not the most suitable practices of the correction theory, which present major
difficulties to the learners’ development. Furthermore, Grami (2005) revealed that those Saudi ESL student
writers most assuredly desire and expect feedback from their teachers of writing. Thus, previously, error
correction studies dedicated little attention to grammar classes and aimed largely at verbal communication and
writing. As an altercation of this prevalent norm, this study attempts to investigate the successful error correction
in grammar classes from the learners’ point of view.
Research Questions
The main question of this study is as follows:
1. How does effective error correction in grammar classes influence second language acquisition from the
perspective of Saudi EFL students?
A set of sub-questions were formulated to address the main question and included:
1. To what extent does effective error correction affect Saudi EFL students' grammatical errors?
2. What are the problems associated with error correction in grammar classes?
3. Which preferences in error correction in grammar classes do Saudi EFL students express?
Research Significance
This research study’s significance anchors on addressing current educators’ requirements concerning the
identification of learner’s needs, especially those related to the problematic areas, such as providing error
correction effectively in grammar classrooms. In addition, it draws the attention of curriculum designers, teachers
and teacher trainers to students’ perspective on effective error correction as a way of maximizing the benefits of
error correction in EFL grammar classes.
Limitations of the Study
The limitations characterizing this study relate to place, time, and subject. Concerning place limitation, the study
is limited to the Saudi female ESL students in the third secondary stage in the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. With
respect to time, the study was conducted on the second semester of the 2013/2014 academic year. Finally, subject
limitations entail the fact that the study was limited to the students’ perception of successful error correction in
terms of grammar only. It addresses the questions of the impact of students’ preferences regarding error
correction, error correction in grammar classes, and issues associated with this process.
128
American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 5, No. 6; December 2015
129
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
When students go through the process of translation from their mother tongue to TL, they may use literal
translation of idioms and colloquial phrases. Sometimes students have to express their ideas and they might use a
glossary to help them cope with that assignment. During this process, other problems might come up associated
with choosing the suitable word for a particular context. Another possible reason is that when learners do not
know how to express what is on their minds in the target language they will retreat to the language system that is
more common for them, which is generally their mother tongue.
Overgeneralization
Overgeneralization occurs when a learner applies a rule to a context of the target language, using it incorrectly
(Krashen, 1985, p. 64). For example, the past tense indicator in English is “-ed.” Thus, students understand that
for a regular verb to be turned into a past one they just need to add "-ed." They might come up with sentences
such as: “I drived the car yesterday.” In this case the students are assuming that just by adding “ed” to the verb
“drive,” they are correctly expressing their thoughts. As Gass and Selinker (2008, p. 46) stated, overgeneralization
might occur due to ignorance of rule limitations which means that students might not understand when to apply
and when not to apply a given rule.
Grammatical Error Correction
Grammar is vital for EFL acquisition. Celce-Murcia (1994, 233) considers that the capability to communicate
one’s thoughts in written or oral form and to perform that with high precision and consistency is a great success in
SLA. As Olshtain (1994, p. 235) claims, “Within the communicative framework of language teaching, the skill of
writing enjoys special status.” It is obvious that any piece of writing or speech should have such features as proper
grammar so that the listener can understand what the speaker means.
Previous studies
An increasing amount of studies has also been studying whether specific types of CF are more efficient than
others in aiding LSA learners’ improvement of the accuracy in language classes. A significant number of
researchers divide feedback strategies into direct and indirect ones and investigate to what extent each of them
enhances linguistic accuracy (Ferris & Hedgcock, 1998; Lalande, 1982; Robb, Ross, & Shortreed, 1986). Direct
or explicit CF is when the educator indicates a mistake and provides the correct version, while indirect approaches
occur when the educator lets the students know that they have mistaken but are not intended on correcting them,
thus letting the student to find and amend the error on their own. In addition, researches that examine the impact
of indirect feedback approaches have inclined to offer a further distinction based on whether they sue a code.
Coded feedback indicates where exactly an error has occurred, and the type of a mistake is marked with a certain
code (for instance, PS refer to an error in the use or past simple tense forms). Un-coded feedback deals with
situations when the educator indicates to an error, circles it, or put an error tally on the side, but, in every case, lets
the learner to independently identify and correct the mistake.
On the other hand, the studies conducted by Lee (1997) and Ferris and Roberts (2001) had control groups which
did not receive any CF. Lee’s work on error correction among Chinese EFL college students discovered a
substantial effect for the group whose mistakes were underlined, compared with the classes that were given no CF
or only a marginal proofreading. Ferris and Roberts (2001) studied the effects of three different approaches
(corrections indicated with codes; merely underlining mistakes, but not labeling them in any other way; and no
error CF) and discovered that both groups of students that were provided with CF showed better performance than
the control group with no comments on their mistakes. However, just as to Robb, Ross, and Shortreed (1986),
they concluded that there were only little difference between the first two groups. Additionally, it is worth noting
that Ferris and Roberts (2001) focused on revisions and not news papers. Ferris et al. (2000) have studied the
effects of various conditions of feedback on both revisions and news papers. In Discussion sections of the study,
Ferris (2002) stated that direct error CF resulted in a higher rate of successful amendments made to the papers
(88%) than indirect CF (77%). On the other hand, it has been discovered that over the course of study, those
students who were provided with indirect feedback showed the reduction of their error frequency rates
considerably more than those who were given direct feedback. Bitchener, Young and Cameron (2005) studied the
influence of different types of CF on the writing performance among ESL students. The study showed a
substantial effect for the mixture of written feedback and feedback sessions on the levels of accuracy in studying
the definite article and the past simple tense in news papers but no general effect on accuracy level progress for
feedback categories when the three error types were treated as one group.
130
American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 5, No. 6; December 2015
Noteworthy differences in accuracy among the four pieces of writing back up previous SLA findings that second
language learners, in the process of obtaining new language forms, may achieve high accuracy on one instance
but fail to do the same on other similar instances.
Error correction in the Saudi ESL context
Error correction is one of the matters that were hardly ever investigated in the Saudi ESL context at all
educational stages. Very few studies have tackled error correction effectiveness especially in grammar classes and
mainly focused on oral interaction and writing. This section provides an overview of the studies that addressed the
subject matter of correcting mistakes among EFL students in Saudi Arabia. Faqeih (2012) examined the
efficiency of error correction methods in oral communication among ESL & EFL students in the United Kingdom
of Great Britain and Saudi Arabia. The research aimed at examining the impacts that two types of oral CF, namely
met linguistic information and recasts, have on verbal assignment presentation on the subject of English modal
verbs (must, can and will). These methods were paralleled to interference with identical assignments but with no
correction feedback given.
The investigation also showed to which extent instructional context (EFL in Saudi Arabia; ESL in the UK) and
students’ attitudes towards correctional feedback mediated the influence of feedback on SLA. ESL adult students
of pre-intermediate level and EFL students in the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia were randomly assembled into one of
the three groups: recast, met linguistic information or task only (no feedback). The findings advocated that both
recasts and met linguistic information are advantageous for the comprehension of English modals, but efficiency
was affected by the result measures applied, the length of the period between interference and test, as well as the
setting (the UK and Saudi Arabia). Both types of feedback were reported to be very beneficial in the majority of
procedures regardless of the context. For most measures, the group with no CF in the UK showed no major gains
but in Saudi Arabia showed significant gains. The study showed that students had an equal preference for both
techniques in the EFL framework, but the ESL students preferred recasts. Mustafa (2012) held both informal and
semi-structured personal interviews in Saudi Arabian educational institutions in order to summarize the students’
attitudes towards the feedback provided to them, and about their insights on what they consider to be helpful
feedback. The researcher used socio-cultural theory as the methodological framework. The results state that the
Saudi students do not give a high rating to the feedback and that the feedback they receive is distinctly different
from what they expect. The students noted several weaknesses of feedback. From a socio-cultural perspective, the
feedback practices do not stick to the best practices of the system, resulting in major obstructions to the learners’
development.
Grami (2005) conducted a survey using structured questionnaires to examine a number of Saudi university level
ESL learners’ opinion on written feedback their teachers provide. The emphasis was mainly laid on linguistic
mistakes. The main aim of the research was to examine whether ESL learners would prefer to have their written
assignments corrected and all the mistakes commented, and if they do think that educators’ comments are
effectives. The most prominent finding is that the ESL student writers at KAAU in Saudi Arabia certainly wish
for and expect the teachers to provide them with feedback. The study also showed that learners also think that
they would benefit much from correctional feedback. This can be clearly seen through the high responses rates
that definitely display solid evidence that they would appreciate CF.
Learners’ Attitudes towards Error Correction
Educators and students do not have the same attitudes toward error correction. Teachers are more worried about
addressing errors than causes behind them. Some educators share a view expressed by Corder (1967), “if we were
to achieve a perfect teaching method the errors would never be committed in the first place, and that therefore the
occurrence of errors is merely a sign of the present inadequacy of our teaching techniques.” Thus, educators try
much technique to prevent the learners from making mistakes by corrections which they consider to be helpful in
making students aware of their errors in order not to repeat them again. Contrariwise, some educators argue that
insisting on correction and grammatical correctness may discourage students form studying foreign languages.
They also consider continuous correction to contribute to the rise of level of anxiety among students, which can
impede a learning process (Krashen, 1982). Like teachers, it is not surprising to see that some learners appreciate
being corrected periodically as they find it beneficial to the progress of language learning. According to Cathcart
and Olsen (1976), students support the practice of frequent correction of their mistakes in oral activities. In a
survey conducted on student writers, Leki (1991) finds that 100 per cent of these students want all their written
errors corrected.
131
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
Nevertheless, some foreign language learners can become very annoyed by continuous corrections, which they
find discouraging and distracting. They believe that they should be corrected if the mistake is dramatic yet they
still feel upset when they make one. Some report strong disapproval of corrections during speaking activities and
some would not even continue taking part in the classroom discussion just because they do not like to be
corrected. Because of so many dissimilar attitudes, both educators and students should develop a practical
technique of dealing with error-correction issues successfully. They should take into serious consideration and
adapt to each other’s preferences in learning and teaching.
The Research
The current study uses quantitative and qualitative methods that describe and analyze effective error correction in
grammar classes from students’ perspective. The study design constructed here is based on the study questions
formulated. These questions were raised from the researcher’s observation and the literature.
Participants and Questionnaire
This research comprised a sample of (304) female students at the 3rd secondary grade in the Saudi secondary
schools in Riyadh city. The selected students were regular students enrolled in the academic year 2013-2014. The
native language of the participants is Arabic and their second language is English. The language levels of the
participants range intermediate to upper intermediate as obtained from their responses to the research data
collection tools. The researcher prepared a questionnaire about error correction in grammar classes. The
questionnaire is composed of (42) statements. The researcher prepared the questionnaire based on the appropriate
literature and previous studies that tackled feedback in English language learning. The researcher issued (320)
questionnaire forms for both genders and got back (304) validly completed. Questionnaires. A 5-Likert point scale
was used for this study (1= “strongly disagree” through5= “strongly agree”)
Data Analysis
Several statistical tests including Pearson correlation tests, reliability tests, and generation of descriptive statistics
such as means, frequencies, and standard deviation were performed using SPSS. Validity is the extent to which a
study accurately reflects or assesses the specific concept that the researcher is attempting to measure (Robson,
1993). The researcher achieved the face questionnaire validity by submitting it to experts and requesting their
opinions on the suitability of the survey to the research objectives. More than half of the experts conveyed that the
questionnaire is suitable for what it is designed to measure as shown in Table 1 at the Appendix. Reliability is
defined by the extent to which study or any measuring activity produces the same result on recurring tests (APA,
1985). The researcher conducted reliability tests and realized a Cronbach’s Alpha coefficient of 0.772 (see Table
2), which was deemed satisfactory as shown in Table 2.Table 2 demonstrates that the values of the coefficient of
reliability of the segments using Cronbach’s Alpha were estimated between (0.849) and (0.418) which are
considered good to high reliability coefficients. In addition, the overall reliability coefficient for the Research
Instrument (i.e. for all the points of the questionnaire: 42 points) is (0.772), which is a high reliability coefficient.
This confirms the high levels of research instrument’s reliability and its suitability for application.
Results
This chapter presents the results of the study. It demonstrates the results of the field study in terms of the research
sample’ responses to the items of the questionnaire. The results of the field study are interpreted, according to the
following study questions:
1. To what extent does effective error correction affect Saudi EFL students' grammatical errors?
2. What are the problems associated with error correction in grammar classes?
3. What are Saudi EFL students' preferences for error correction in grammar classes?
Responses to the First Question
Table 3 shows that the research sample responses to the impact of correcting grammatical errors in English on
the linguistic errors were high, with an overall mean of (3.97). In other words, the students agreed that there is an
impact of correcting grammatical errors in English on linguistic errors in general. Below is a description of the
participants’ responses to each item in this section. The items are ranked from the highest mean to the lowest
mean as follows. The results concerning the impact of English grammatical errors correction on the rate of
linguistic errors made by students all over the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia.
132
American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 5, No. 6; December 2015
Table 4 shows that the responses to the impact of English grammatical errors correction on the rate of linguistic
errors made the students all over the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia were high, with an overall mean of (3.99). In other
words, the students agreed that there is an impact of English grammatical errors correction on the rate of linguistic
errors made by students all over the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia.
Results of the Second Question
As shown in Table 5, the research sample responses to the items of these sections are low with a mean score
(2.76). In other words, the students neither agreed nor disagreed in their response to whether there are problems
related to grammatical errors corrections in the classroom in general or not. Below is a description of the
participants’ responses to each item in this section. The constituent items are ranked from the highest mean to the
lowest mean for ease of reference.
Responses to the Third Question
As illustrated in Table 6, the research sample responses to the items of these sections are not high with a mean
score (3.32). This means that the students expressed varying opinions with regards to their preferences to error
correction in grammar classes. Below is a description of the participants’ responses to each item in this section.
The items are ranked from the highest mean to the lowest mean.
Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations
This chapter presents a discussion of the research results mentioned in the above-mentioned chapter. In addition,
this chapter presents a conclusion of the results of the study and provided recommendations for better handling of
the research issue.
Discussion of Results
The first question in this study was about identifying the students’ perception of the impact of English
grammatical error correction on their rate of linguistic errors. The findings of this question revealed that the
students see that the grammatical errors help them in language learning, the students benefit when the teacher
corrects their English grammatical errors, learning grammar helps them in language learning, they listen carefully
to their teacher when she corrects their grammatical error, and that they benefit from repeating the sentences after
correcting them. In addition, the students expressed that the teachers are not very interested in correcting their
grammatical errors. This is because the teachers point out their errors, do not repeat the errors after correction, and
they make grammatical errors because of their ignorance of the grammatical rules. On the other hand, students
were of the opinion that English grammatical errors have an impact on the rate of their linguistic errors. They
expressed that error correction helps them remember the correct answer in the following times, error correction
helps them, error correction helps them understand the correct answer, they care for the errors corrected by the
teacher so as not to repeat them, and that error correction encourages them to search for the correct answer and
compare the right and wrong answer. However, students did not highly estimate that the teacher helps them to
understand their grammatical errors after correction and that the teacher points to their mistakes immediately.
These findings were supported by scholars such as Mustafa (2012) and Mackey (2007). They confirmed that
correction of grammatical errors helps students in language learning. Also, Lyster (2004) and Long (2007)
support the findings of these study that students make a grammatical error due to poor knowledge of grammatical
rules and that the teacher plays a key role in correcting grammatical errors. From a pedagogical standpoint, error
correction is a vital factor of form-focused instruction and it is recommended for successful L2 teaching (Long,
2007) and that feedback delivered through verbal communication can help L2 acquisition by linking form and
meaning. When given in response to mistakes during conversational interaction, error correction offers a
possibility for learners to focus their attention on form as it is important to the intended meaning (Grami, 2005).
The results of the second question revealed the most common challenges that learners experience with regard to
error correction practices. The top ranking challenges included the perception that teachers corrects the
grammatical errors during the exercises more than the activities, excessive use of the Arabic language too much in
the English class, and teachers paying more attention to the word usage, spelling, and articulation problems than
to the grammatical mistakes. In addition, the students ranked several other challenges lowly. These included
teachers correcting grammatical errors without helping students understand the errors, the frustration when
teachers correct their grammatical errors, embarrassment when the instructor points out an error, and resistance
against error correction.
133
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
These findings confirm previous studies’ findings that reported that EFL students have linguistic and emotional
problems with regards to their errors in the class. For example, Leeman (2007) and Grami (2005) revealed that
students feel reluctant to participate in the class as they fear being embarrassed by their teachers. Also, this is
supported by Ammar and Spada (2006) who confirmed that the key reason beyond EFL students’ grammatical
errors is the overlap between their target language and their mother tongue. The third research question was about
student preferences for correcting mistakes in grammar classes among EFL students in Saudi Arabia. The findings
from this section showed that students prefer when educators correct all their inaccuracies, provide written
feedback especially with special code marking, and correction during group activities. In addition, students prefer
when teachers point learners to their inaccuracies as they speak and such correction should be conveyed orally. A
significant result, therefore, was that most students find error correction to be a positive emotional experience
once applied using relevant procedures. For example, when the students were asked if they preferred to be
corrected every time they made a mistake or only when the mistake was important, most students did not prefer
correction of every error. This revelation matches the findings by Lyster (2002) and Révész (2002).
Other questions inquired whether the learners were more comfortable if the teachers corrected their inaccuracies
in front of the group or in private. Most students preferred to be corrected in as a group and not individually. This
result receives support from Truscott (1999) and Han (2002) who affirm that students may feel embarrassed
during oral error correction. A related item asked if the students preferred to be corrected immediately or after
class. Many students reported a preference for delayed correction, which again strengthens the students' responses
concerning the affective impact of error correction. The data collected from the questionnaire illustrated that some
correction techniques were more or less helpful than the others. The results correlate with Sheen’s (2004) findings
that point to the direct correlation between the context in which an error occurs and the efficiency of a given type
of error correction. While this variability has not been clearly visible in terms of student perception, this option
should not be ignored. Type of activity, classroom changing aspects (Morris & Atone, 2003), and the time when
the error occurs could be significant in shaping learners’ preferences. An error at the end of a sentence, for
example, may leave students more exposed to a met linguistic explanation that would stimulate self-correction. A
mistake that occurs mid-utterance might result in learners preferring to have a recast, which would let them finish
their thought without going into reverse.
Conclusions
Based on the significant findings from this study and the data obtained from previous studies, four conclusions
were made. First, there is an indication that effective error correction can be achieved through striking a balance
between making errors clear to students and correcting them in a relevant manner that does not cause
embarrassment, fear, or feeling of discomfort. Second, there is evidence of the effectiveness of error correction,
involving a variety of explicit ad implicit technique combinations that promote noticing of errors and learning of
structures. Thus, it is entirely possible and necessary to combine error correction techniques depending upon the
particular learning circumstances. Third, timely explicit and implicit error correction is beneficial in helping
students to enhance their language skills. Precisely, error correction is of a great significance in increasing
students’ understanding of their linguistic limitations and thus affects their inter-language structures.
Recommendations
1. Teachers are recommended to adopt and employ relevant error correction techniques in correcting
student's errors at the secondary stage and other stages taking into consideration students’ age, needs, interests and
linguistic proficiency levels.
2. Implementation of error correction should be integrated within an overall plan of the whole curriculum
and its results should be evaluated within this overall perspective as well.
3. Curriculum designers, teacher-trainers and textbook writers should make use of error correction
methodology as means of developing students’ learning in grammar. Teachers' books should include instructions
on procedures of applying relevant error correction techniques and procedures.
Suggestions for Further Research
The current study investigated Saudi EFL students’ perspective on effective error correction in grammar
classrooms. However, further research support is needed to explore Saudi EFL students' perspective on effective
error correction in other areas such as teaching vocabulary, reading, listening, writing and speaking.
134
American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 5, No. 6; December 2015
Secondly, further research is required to examine Saudi EFL students’ perspective on effective error correction
with other student populations at different levels of education and in different locations. Thirdly, research
investigating Saudi EFL students' perspective on effective error correction, different techniques and strategies,
and the relative effectiveness of each technique and strategy would be beneficial in giving educators insights to
the most effective error correction approaches from a student perspective.
References
Ammar, A., & Spada, N. (2006). One size fits all? Recasts, prompts, and L2 learning. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 28(4), 543-574.
Bitchener, J., Young, S., & Cameron, D. (2005). The effect of different types of corrective feedback on ESL
student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14(3), 191-205.
Brown, H. D. (1980). Principles of language learning and teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Cathcart, R., & Olsen, J. (1976). Teachers’ and students’ preference for correction of classroom
conversation errors. TESOL, 76(2), 22-40.
Corder, S.P. (1967). The significance of learners’ errors. IRAL-International Review of Applied Linguistics in
Language Teaching, 5(1-4),161-170.
Corder, S.P. (1973). Introducing applied linguistics. Harmonds worth, UK: Penguin Education.
Doughty, C.J. (2003). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C. J. Doughty & M. H.
Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256-310). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Ellis, R., & Sheen, Y. (2006). Re-examining the role of recasts in L2 acquisition. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 28, 575-600.
Faqeih, H. (2012). The effectiveness of error correction during oral interaction: Experimental studies with
English L2 learners in the United Kingdom and Saudi Arabia. (Doctoral dissertation). New York, NY:
University of York.
Ferris, D. R., & Hedgcock, J. S. (1998). Teaching ESL composition: Purpose, process, and practice. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ferris, D. R., Chaney, S. J., Komura, K., Roberts, B. J., & McKee, S. (2000). Perspectives, problems, and
practices in treating written error (pp. 14-18), Colloquium presented at international TESOL Convention
(March 14-18), Vancouver, BC.
Ferris, D., & Roberts, B. (2001). Error feedback in L2 writing classes: How explicit does it need to be? Journal of
Second Language Writing, 10(3), 161-184.
Ferris, D.R. (2002) Treatment of error in second language student writing. Ann Arbor, MI: University of
Michigan Press.
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second language acquisition. An introductory course (3rd ed.).New York, NY:
Routledge.
Grami, G.M. (2005). The effect of teachers’ written feedback on ESL students’ perception: A study in a Saudi
ESL university-level context. Annual Review of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, 2.
Retrieved from http://research.ncl.ac.uk/ARECLS/vol2_documents/Grami/grami.htm
Krashen, S. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London, England: Longman.
Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford, England: Pergamon.
Lalande, J. F. (1982). Reducing composition errors: An experiment. The Modern Language Journal, 66(2), 140-
149.
Leeman, J. (2007). Feedback in L2 learning: Responding to errors during practice. In R. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practice
in a second language (pp. 111-137). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Leki, I. (1991). The preferences of ESL students for error correction in college‐level writing classes. Foreign
language annals, 24(3), 203-218.
Loewen, S. (2005). Incidental focus on form and second language learning. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 27(3), 361-386.
Long, M. H. (2007). Recasts in SLA: The story so far. In M. H. Long (Ed.), Problems in SLA (pp. 75 – 116).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 26, 399-432.
135
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
Lyster, R., Lightbown, P. M., & Spada, N. (1999). A response to Truscott’s “What's wrong with oral grammar
correction.” Canadian Modern Language Review 55(4), 456-467.
Lyster, R., & Rasta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
20, 37-66.
Mackey, A. (Ed.). (2007). Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical
studies. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Mustafa, R. (2012). Feedback on the feedback: Socio-cultural interpretation of Saudi ESL learners’ opinions
about writing feedback. English Language Teaching, 5(3), 3-15.
Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, I. (1986). Salience of feedback on error and its effect on EFL writing quality.
TESOL Quarterly, 20(1), 83–96.
Sheen, Y. (2006). Exploring the relationship between characteristics of recasts and learner uptake. Language
Teaching Research, 10(4), 361-392.
Sheen, Y. (2008). Recasts, language anxiety, modified output and L2 learning. Language Learning, 58(4), 835-
874.
Appendix
Part 1 Part 2 Part 3 Part 4
S Pearson S Pearson S Pearson S Pearson
1 .182** 1 .617** 1 .455** 1 .396**
2 .632** 2 .588** 2 .613** 2 .312**
3 .331** 3 .678** 3 .647** 3 .253**
4 .629** 4 .639** 4 .632** 4 .415**
5 .607** 5 .597** 5 .660** 5 .518**
6 .522** 6 .729** 6 .423** 6 .550**
7 .639** 7 .623** 7 .598** 7 .442**
8 .569** 8 .704** 8 .618** 8 .404**
9 .527** - - 9 .657** 9 .369**
10 .548** - - 10 .663** 10 .316**
- - - - 11 .690** - -
- - - - 12 .535** - -
- - - - 13 .501** - -
- - - - 14 .351** - -
** Correlation is important at the level of 0.01.
*Correlation is important at 0.05.
S Statements SA A DK D SD
St
d.
D
R
n
F 40 91 81 75 17
1 My grammatical errors in the English class are too many. 3.20 1.12 10
% 13.1 29.7 26.5 24.5 5.6
F 156 115 15 13 7
2 I benefit when the teacher corrects my English grammatical errors. 4.31 0.92 2
% 51.0 37.6 4.9 4.2 2.3
My grammatical errors are due to my ignorance of the grammatical F 68 98 64 55 19
3 3.46 1.20 9
rules usage. % 22.2 32.0 20.9 18.0 6.2
F 165 102 27 10 1
4 The correction of grammatical errors helps me in language learning. 4.38 0.81 1
% 53.9 33.3 8.8 3.3 .3
F 173 86 21 19 6
5 Learning the grammar helps me in writing the sentences correctly. 4.31 0.98 3
% 56.5 28.1 6.9 6.2 2.0
F 71 91 97 37 10
6 I do not repeat the errors that my teacher corrects for me. 3.58 1.07 8
% 23.2 29.7 31.7 12.1 3.3
7 I listen carefully to my teacher when she corrects my grammatical F 143 124 23 14 2 4.28 0.84 4
136
American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 5, No. 6; December 2015
an
ea
M
S Statements SA A DK D SD
St
d.
D
R
n
k
errors. % 46.7 40.5 7.5 4.6 .7
F 132 99 48 14 13
8 I feel that the teacher is interested in correcting my grammatical errors. 4.06 1.07 6
% 43.1 32.4 15.7 4.6 4.2
F 112 120 26 32 15
9 The teacher explains to me the error I made in the grammar. 3.92 1.15 7
% 36.6 39.2 8.5 10.5 4.9
Repeating the sentences after correcting them helps me in learning F 150 100 32 20 4
10 4.22 0.96 5
English grammar. % 49.0 32.7 10.5 6.5 1.3
an
ea
M
S Statements SA A DK D SD
St
d.
D
R
n
k
Pay attention to errors that the teacher corrected so as not F 108 139 43 11 4
1 4.10 0.87 3
repeat them in the next times. % 35.3 45.4 14.1 3.6 1.3
F 82 105 66 42 11
2 The teacher corrects my grammatical errors immediately. 3.67 1.12 8
% 26.8 34.3 21.6 13.7 3.6
The teacher helps me understand my grammatical errors after F 94 126 38 35 12
3 3.84 1.11 6
correction. % 30.7 41.2 12.4 11.4 3.9
F 157 110 23 13 1
4 Error correction helps in understanding the correct answer. 4.35 0.82 2
% 51.3 35.9 7.5 4.2 0.3
Error correction helps in remembering the correct answer in F 162 108 26 8 2
5 4.37 0.80 1
the following times. % 52.9 35.3 8.5 2.6 .7
F 112 99 48 37 9
6 Error correction encourages seeking for the right answer. 3.88 1.12 4
% 36.6 32.4 15.7 12.1 2.9
Error correcting lead to make a comparison between the right F 94 122 48 33 8
7 3.86 1.06 5
and wrong answer. % 30.7 39.9 15.7 10.8 2.6
Correction by the teacher helps me to correct my errors F 104 94 69 29 10
8 3.83 1.10 7
myself. % 34.0 30.7 22.5 9.5 3.3
Total Mean 3.99
Table 4. Impact of English grammatical error correction on linguistic error rate
Std. D
Mean
Rank
S Statements SA A DK D SD
137
ISSN 2162-139X (Print), 2162-142X (Online) © Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.aijcrnet.com
Std. D
Mean
Rank
S Statements SA A DK D SD
Std. D
S Statements SA A DK D SD
Mean
Rank
F 65 101 38 79 23
1 I prefer oral correction for my errors. 3.35 1.27 6
% 21.2 33.0 12.4 25.8 7.5
F 107 100 49 38 11
2 I prefer written correction for my errors. 3.83 1.14 2
% 35.0 32.7 16.0 12.4 3.6
I prefer that the teacher corrects my errors F 96 113 30 46 18
3 3.74 1.22 3
immediately when I speak. % 31.4 36.9 9.8 15.0 5.9
I prefer that the teacher corrects my errors F 75 77 40 84 30
4 3.27 1.35 7
after I finish speaking. % 24.5 25.2 13.1 27.5 9.8
I prefer that the teacher corrects my errors F 47 70 59 93 34
5 3.01 1.27 8
after finishing the activities. % 15.4 22.9 19.3 30.4 11.1
I prefer that the teacher corrects my errors F 34 53 33 115 70
6 2.56 1.31 9
after the end of the lesson. % 11.1 17.3 10.8 37.6 22.9
It is preferred that errors are corrected during F 81 119 60 27 18
7 3.71 1.13 4
group work. % 26.5 38.9 19.6 8.8 5.9
I prefer that my teacher and my colleagues F 55 113 49 57 30
8 3.35 1.25 5
correct my errors. % 18.0 36.9 16.0 18.6 9.8
I like that the teacher corrects all my F 155 84 35 25 6
9 4.17 1.05 1
grammatical errors. % 50.7 27.5 11.4 8.2 2.0
I do not like that the teacher corrects all my F 19 36 36 110 105
10 2.20 1.21 10
grammatical errors.
% 6.2 11.8 11.8 35.9 34.3
Table 6. Saudi EFL students' preferences for error correction in grammar classes
138