Article Thepsychologyofkink SRSPasaccepted
Article Thepsychologyofkink SRSPasaccepted
Article Thepsychologyofkink SRSPasaccepted
1
Collaborative Antwerp Psychiatric Research Institute, Faculty of Medicine
and Health Sciences, University of Antwerp, R3.22, Building R, Campus Drie Eiken,
Universiteitsplein 1, 2610, Antwerp, Belgium;
2
University Psychiatric Hospital Duffel, Stationsstraat 22c, 2570 Duffel, Belgium;
Abstract
and Discipline (BD), Dominance and Submission (DS), and Sadism and Masochism (SM). It
has been speculated that a substantial percentage of BDSM practitioners have experienced
(sexual) trauma in the past. Attachment style is an additional factor resulting from early life
dynamics that has been suggested to potentially influence BDSM interests. This study will
investigate to what extent BDSM interests are related to trauma and attachment style, while
general population completed a survey in 2017 assessing BDSM interests as well as the Brief
levels of physical abuse in adulthood but no significant differences emerged for other
Surprisingly, BDSM-practitioners had more secure and at the same time more anxious-
was associated with dominance, whereas the anxious-avoidant attachment style was
Discussion: Thus, our findings do not support the hypothesis of BDSM being a
Introduction
BDSM is a type of sexual expression that refers to Bondage and Discipline (BD),
Dominance and Submission (DS), and Sadism and Masochism (SM). With mutual consent,
sex or play partners typically make use of imposed physical restraints, assignments and (real
or played) punishments (BD) and have or implement fantasies about power relationships and
power roleplay (DS). They hereby enjoy causing or experiencing intense sensory stimuli like
pain (SM). More often than not, practitioners can have one of the 3 specific power exchange
roles during BDSM interactions: Dom – the person who is in control and assumes the
dominant role, Sub – the person who gives up control and assumes the submissive role or
Switch, where the practitioner alternates between dominant and submissive roles depending
Previously, the prevalence of BDSM in the general population has been reported to lie
between 2 and 65%; a broad range presumably due to use of differing definitions of BDSM
and other methodological aspects (Renaud & Byers, 1999; Richters et al 2008; Masters,
Johnson & Kolodny 1995; De Neef et al., 2019). Recently, our group demonstrated in a large
sample (n=1027) that 46.8% of the general population had ever performed at least one
BDSM-related activity, and an additional 22% indicated having (had) fantasies about it
(Holvoet et al., 2017). About 10% engaged in these activities on a regular basis. These high
prevalence rates show that at least certain levels of BDSM-interest are present within a
sadistic and masochistic interests and behaviors are still categorized as paraphilic disorders
within the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5 th edition (DSM-V).
With regard to the origination of these sexual interests, it has been speculated that a
substantial percentage of BDSM practitioners have experienced (sexual) trauma in the past
(e.g. Bekes, Perry, & Robertson, 2017). Indeed, some authors frame BDSM interests as
maladaptive coping mechanisms resulting from early life events. Finkelhor & Browne (1985)
argued that sexual abuse induced dysfunctional development of a child’s sexuality (sexual
feelings and attitudes), leading to the creation of unusual emotional and cognitive
pattern whereby people endlessly repeat patterns of behavior (like reenacting an event or
putting oneself in situations where the event is likely to happen) which were repressed and
traumatic in their childhood (Buckingham, 2002). This concept could explain the link
between early sexual abuse and “inappropriate” sexual behaviors later in life. Alternatively,
An association between trauma and masochism was indeed demonstrated by Frias and
colleagues (2017), who showed that masochistic women with borderline personality disorder
had higher childhood sexual abuse rates compared to their non-masochistic peers. This is also
in line with higher prevalence rates of self-reported sexual abuse in BDSM club members
(Nordling et al., 2000). Notwithstanding, only 9.6% of the SM club members in this study
actually reported childhood sexual abuse, which is a small minority. In addition, it has been
the only study that has found this difference between practitioners and the general population.
The Australian Study of Health and Relationships (ASHR) found contrary results in a national
representative sample wherein psychological distress and sexual functioning was examined.
This study found that 2% of sexually active men and 1.4% of sexually active women had
4
engaged in BDSM activities within the past year, and found no difference in past sexual abuse
history, or levels of distress compared to other members of the general population (Richters et
al, 2008). Nevertheless, in a broader context, some research did demonstrate associations
between trauma and sexual behavior in general. For example, Meston, Herman & Trapnell
(1999) found a relationship between early sexual abuse and adult sexual behavior like
voyeurism. Later studies also found associations between trauma and health-risking sexual
behaviors in girls (Smith et al., 2006) and gay men (Kalichman et al., 2004). As a result, these
studies may suggest that (early) trauma can affect certain sexual behaviors later in life,
however the connection between trauma and BDSM practices asks for more research-based
elaboration. Furthermore, the actual relationship between BDSM interests and the adoption of
Attachment style is an additional factor resulting from early life dynamics that
potentially may influence BDSM interests, as it has been demonstrated that attachment style
could reliably predict some sexual attitudes and behaviors, with especially an anxious
attachment style seemingly predicting drug use during sexual contact, unsafe sex, and
negative attitudes towards condoms, in late adolescence (Feeney et al., 2000). Attachment is
defined as a lasting relationship between a person and the people around them with whom
they regularly interact. This emotional bond lays the foundations of identity formation, self-
attachment theory states that a good development of the emotional bond necessitates
sensitivity and reactiveness to the child (Bowlby, 1977). Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991)
defined four attachment styles for adults: 1. secure attachment style; 2. anxious-preoccupied
attachment style. Furthermore, Gentzler and Kerns (2004) found that an avoidant attachment
style was associated with practices of casual sex while an anxious attachment style was more
related to practices of unwanted but consensual sex. Similarly, Szielasko et al. (2013) found
that an avoidant attachment style was associated to a higher lifetime number of sexual
partners while ambivalence attachment predicted invasive and coercive sexual behaviors.
Only one study (Wismeijer & Van Assen, 2013) looked into the relationship between
attachment styles and BDSM preferences. They found no support for the persistent
noted that the few studies looking into the psychological dynamics of BDSM interests
typically include participants recruited from BDSM-themed internet forums or from BDSM-
clubs. Nevertheless, about 85% of the BDSM practitioners only engage in these sexual
activities in the privacy of their own homes (Holvoet et al., 2017). As such, research until now
has suffered from a strong selection bias. It may be rewarding to differentiate between
practitioners recruited from the BDSM community and private BDSM practitioners. The
present study will thus investigate to what extent BDSM interests are related to trauma and
METHODS
Study Design
Data were gathered between February 2017 and March 2017 by means of an online
experience with specific BDSM activities, 3) context of BDSM activities, 4) trauma history
and 5) attachment styles. For a detailed description of the survey’s structure and content, see
Holvoet et al. 2017. This survey was distributed amongst the general population via iVOX, a
market research and polling agency with access to a panel of 150,000 Belgian citizens
6
representative of the general population (www.ivox.be; n=1027), and within the online
BDSM community (through BDSM-specific forums; n=251) to which the survey was
the survey was about 15 minutes. The study was approved by the ethical committee board of
Participants
Survey completers (n=1289) were subdivided into 4 BDSM interest level groups: “No
interest” (NI, scoring ≤ 3 on all items; n=326); “Fantasy” (BDSM-F; score either 4 or 5 on
any item; n=192) and 2 BDSM Practice groups (score of ≥ 6 on any item; n=771). The BDSM
Practice group was further subdivided in a BDSM-Private Practice (BDSM-PP; n=559) and a
performed their activities solely at home (negative response on all BDSM community event
Participants in the NI and BDSM-F groups were all volunteers from a general
population study in Flanders. Participants from the 2 BDSM Practice groups were either
recruited from the general population or via Fetlife (an online BDSM community website),
four participating BDSM-associations (Kajira, VZW Steel Moon, Fetish Café, Club 78), as
Materials
organizations: Kajira Gent (student association Ugent for students with an interest in BDSM),
VZW Steel Moon (Antwerp), Fetish Café (Antwerp), and Club 78 (Oostham). Interests and
7
experiences with BDSM related activities, fetish-related activities and general socio-
economical factors like age, gender, study level, etc. were surveyed (see Holvoet, et al., 2017
The Brief Trauma Questionnaire (BTQ) (Schnurr et al., 2002) is a validated 10-item
self-report questionnaire derived from the Brief Trauma Interview (BTI; Schnurr, et al., 1995)
to scrutinize the presence and severity of past trauma. The first seven items, respectively
caretaker or teacher, 6) other physical violence and 7) unwanted sexual behavior towards the
participant were included in the analyses. It should be noted with item 6 that participants were
asked about if they were ever attacked, beaten, or robbed after the age of 18 by whoever,
including friends, family and strangers. No specification was made in the questionnaire
whether this was consensual or not. Severity of the specific traumas was measured by two
Questionnaire (RQ) was used (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). This self-report screening
instrument is designed to obtain continuous ratings of each of the following four attachment
patterns: (i) secure attachment style, (ii) anxious-avoidant attachment style, (iii) anxious-
Statistical analysis
8
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v22.0. Multiple group comparisons
with normally distributed data were done by the multivariate analysis of (co)variance method
(Pillai’s trace). For not normally distributed data the Kruskal-Wallis test was used. Analysis
of variance tests and Mann-Whitney tests were applied for single group comparisons
depending on whether the data was normally distributed or not. Contingency analyses (Chi-
square test) were used for group comparisons with nominal variables. Exploring associations
between variables was done by use of Pearson’s and Spearman Rho correlation methods,
depending on the normality of the data. Finally, a linear regression model (method=enter) was
applied to see which variables had the most predictive value for the overall outcome of
BDSM interests.
RESULTS
Demographics
In total, 1440 individuals responded to the survey; of these, 1289 completed the survey with
251 completers recruited via online platforms and 1038 completers from the general
affinity with BDSM (no interest (NI) group), either on fantasy or on practice level and 15%
proclaimed having had BDSM related fantasies (n=192; BDSM-F group). Surprisingly,
almost 60% (n=771) of the completers have put at least 1 BDSM related activity into
practice, be it at home (BDSM private practice (BDSM-PP); 43% of our total sample) or
within a BDSM related community event (BDSM community practice (BDSM-CP); 16% of
our total sample). From the private practice group (BDSM-PP) 90,7% of the participants
(from the general population) completed the survey using iVox, and 9,3% completed the
survey with Survey Monkey. For the community practitioners group (BDSM-CP) 6,1% of its
participants made use of iVox, and 93,9% via Survey Monkey (See Table 1). For a detailed
9
components were created (see Holvoet et al., 2017): submissiveness (SUB; including items as
including items as ‘hitting a partner’, ‘let partner address you with title’, ‘blindfolding
‘watching people getting hit’ or ‘fire play’), and finally a component including items on the
use of attributes (ATT, including ‘use of medical attributes and ‘penetration using big
For each component and their corresponding items, summary scores were made in
order to create a total score for each underlying sub-domain of BDSM. By summing up the
summary scores of the 4 BDSM components (DOM, SUB, VOY and ATT) a total score for
effect for the 4 groups (V = .79, F (12, 3852) = 113.67, p < .001) was found for each of the
BDSM summary scores (TOT, SUB, DOM, VOY and ATT). Post-hoc Bonferroni analyses
showed that all group comparisons were significant (NI < BDSM-F < BDSM-PP
< BDSM-CP; all comparisons p ≤ .001) for each summary score (see figure
1).
BDSM Practice groups (PP & CP), 299 participants from the 771 BDSM practitioners
indicated to have an affiliation with a certain BDSM identity type. The 472 remaining
participants indicated experience with one or more BDSM related practices, but did not self-
identify as BDSM practitioner and did not indicate having a certain BDSM identity type.
dominant and switch identities more or less equally represented (28,8% and 31,1%
nature of this group as well as its small sample size, the BDSM identity type ‘other’ was not
Chi-square test results showed a significant effect for the distribution of gender among the
three BDSM identity types (χ2 (2) = 47.32, p < .001). Of the men, 42,9% indicated being
dominant, whereas 27,1% indicated being submissive and 29,9% as switches. As such, there
were significantly more dominant men and less submissive men (standardized residual > ±2).
Among the female participants, a majority of 59,3% were submissive, another 32,2% self-
identified as switches and only 8,5% were dominant. For women the finding was reversed,
hence there were significantly more submissive and less dominant women than expected
Occurrence of trauma for the four groups (NI, BDSM-F, BDSM-PP, BDSM-CP)
Subjects were questioned whether they had experienced trauma related to war/combat zones
(WAR), car accidents (CAR), natural/nuclear disasters (NAT), physical illness/disease (DIS),
11
physical beatings or attacks during childhood (PBC) and adulthood (PBA), or if they had
suffered unwanted sexual contact (USC) in their lifetime. See table 2 for the occurrence rates.
Comparing the overall trauma scores for each trauma item among all groups, Kruskal
Wallis tests showed significant differences for the trauma variables PBA (H(3) = 13.96, p
< .01), USC (H(3) = 8.67, p < .05) and the total trauma score (TOT trauma; H(3) = 13.15, p
< .01), but not for any of the other trauma scores (including PBC). Post-hoc analyses with
Mann-Whitney tests were done to follow-up this finding. A Bonferroni correction with a
critical value of .0083 was applied as well to report significant group differences. When
looking at the total trauma score (TOT trauma) post-hoc analyses revealed that community
practitioners of BDSM (BDSM-CP) had a higher total trauma score than participants in the NI
(U = 29678.50, r = -.13, p < .0083) and marginally significantly higher than BDSM-F group
had experienced less physical violence at a later age (18,8%) than practitioners of BDSM in
the trauma related to unwanted sexual contacts, no group differences could be found, although
club practitioners (BDSM-CP) tended to have experienced more sexual trauma (23,4%) than
non-practitioners with fantasies (BDSM-F; 12,4%; U = 18427.50, r = -.13, p = .011), but this
was not significant (given critical value of .0083). No other group comparisons for the trauma
variables PBA, USC, and TOT trauma were significant, see Figure 2.
Severity of trauma for the four groups (NI, BDSM-F, BDSM-PP, BDSM-CP)
For those individuals having reported presence of DIS and PBA trauma, additional
analyses by use of Kruskal-Wallis test were done on the severity of the trauma. A significant
group difference was only found for the DIS trauma (H(3) = 8.46, p < .05), but not for the
PBA trauma. Post-hoc analyses with Mann-Whitney tests were done to follow-up this finding.
A Bonferroni correction with a critical value of .0083 was applied as well to report significant
group differences. None of the separate group comparisons for the DIS trauma were
significant.
18,8%) and TOT trauma score than controls, but did not significantly differ on any of the
other trauma measures. Private practitioners only differed from controls on the trauma item
PBA (27,3% vs. 18,8%). Neither private nor community practitioners of BDSM did differ in
their degree of experienced trauma from the control group. Despite the significant results, a
couple of things should be taken into consideration. First, the effect sizes were small (-.30 > r
< .30). Secondly, because no specification was made whether the reported PBA trauma was
consensual or not, a possible confounding might be present in the significant results between
practitioners and controls: practitioners of BDSM could have answered the PBA trauma
question with ‘yes’ when actually thinking about beatings during BDSM play.
Associations between trauma and BDSM scores were calculated for the BDSM
practice groups (BDSM-PP & BDSM-CP). Within the BDSM-CP group, the TOT BDSM-
score was significantly related to the unwanted sexual contact trauma item (USC: rs = .25, p <
.001). Exploratory follow up analyses showed that specifically the BDSM component
13
submissiveness (SUB) was significantly but mildly associated with USC trauma (rs = .20, p
< .01). No other associations between BDSM- and trauma scores for the practitioners groups
were found.
Within the BDSM-CP group USC trauma was more reported by women (38.2%) than
men (11%) and this difference was significant (U = 3823.50, z = -4.61, p < .001). Other
findings were that bi- and pansexuals experienced more USC trauma than heterosexuals (U =
2946, z = -2.31, p < .05 and U = 618, z = -2.37, p < .05 respectively); bi- / pan- / asexuals
more than homosexuals (U = 157.50, z = -2.06, p < .05 / U = 31.50, z = -2.34, p < .05 / U =
4.50, z = -2.12, p < .05); people with the lowest education level more than averagely or highly
educated individuals (U = 236.50, z = -3.33, p = .001 and U = 531, z = -3.56, p < .001
respectively); and submissives and switches more than dominants (U = 1764.50, z = -2.47, p <
To investigate the link between BDSM interests/practices and attachment style for the
four groups, each subject’s most prominent attachment style was determined by means of the
overall attachment styles (V = .06, F(12, 3729) = 6.39, p < .001). Specifically, the test of
between-subjects effects showed that the four groups significantly differed from each other
for the secure (F(3) = 12.44, p < .001), anxious-avoidant (F(3) = 4.19, p < .01), and the
anxious-preoccupied attachment style (F(3) = 8.11, p < .001). No significant group effect was
found for the dismissive-avoidant attachment style. Post-hoc Bonferroni analyses were done
for comparing the groups on each of the four attachment styles, see Figure 3.
14
Surprisingly, participants in the BDSM-CP group had a more secure attachment style
than participants in the NI group (p < .001, SE: .14), BDSM-F group (p < .001, SE: .16) and
the BDSM-PP group (p < .001, SE: .13). Additionally, these community practitioners of
BDSM also had a less anxious-avoidant attachment style than participants from the groups,
BDSM-F (p < .05, SE: .18) and BDSM-PP (p < .05, SE: .14). For the anxious-preoccupied
attachment style findings were different. Here, participants from the BDSM-CP group had a
significantly more anxious-preoccupied attachment style than the participants from the NI
group (p < .001, SE: .14) and BDSM-PP group (p < .05, SE: .13). Also, participants from the
BDSM-F group were significantly more anxious-preoccupied than participants from the NI
group (p < .05, SE: .15). Private practitioners (BDSM-PP) did not differ in any of the four
Gender also seemed to have an overall significant effect on all four attachment styles
(MANCOVA - Pillai’s Trace: V = .03, F (4, 1237) = 10.88, p < .001). The test of between-
subjects showed that gender had independent significant effects for the secure (F(1, 1240) =
4.40, p < .05), anxious-avoidant (F(1, 1240) = 11.53, p = .001), anxious-preoccupied (F(1,
1240) = 9.63, p < .01), and dismissive-avoidant (F(1, 1240) = 8.77, p < .01) attachment style.
Within the total sample, male participants had a more secure (F(1, 1243) = 6.94, p < .01),
anxious-preoccupied (F(1, 1243) = 13.33, p < .001) and dismissive-avoidant (F(1, 1243) =
9.13, p < .01) attachment style, and a less anxious-avoidant (F(1, 1243) = 11.73, p = .001)
attachment style than female participants. On group level this difference between men and
women in attachment style was only found in the BDSM practice groups and not in the NI
15
and BDSM-F group. When controlling for gender the group effect on all four attachment
styles hardly changed and significant differences remained for the secure (p < .001), anxious-
avoidant (p < .01), and anxious-preoccupied (p < .001) attachment style. The gender “other”
Pearson’s correlation method, positive significant relations between secure attachment style
and the BDSM factors, DOM (r = .206, p < .05) and VOY (r = .211, p < .05) were found
within the BDSM-CP group. High scores on dominant practices and visual stimuli during
BDSM play tended to be associated with a more secure attachment style for participants who
practiced BDSM in clubs. Another positive association within the BDSM-CP group was
found for the anxious-avoidant attachment style and the SUB factor (r = .200, p < .01) of
Using Pillai’s Trace, the multivariate analysis of variance test showed an overall
significant group effect of BDSM identity for the attachment styles (V = .11, F(8, 576) = 4.36,
p < .001) within the subgroup of BDSM practitioners (BDSM-PP; BDSM-CP). The findings
were that participants significantly differed in terms of their secure attachment style (F(2,
290) = 4.66, p = .01) and anxious-avoidant attachment style (F(2, 290) = 11.86, p < .001).
Post-hoc Bonferroni analyses showed that Dom participants had a significantly more secure
attachment style than Sub participants (SE = .22, p = .01) and at the same time a significantly
less anxious-avoidant attachment style than Sub (SE = .24, p < .001), and Switch participants
16
(SE = .26, p < .001). No differences were found in attachment styles between Sub and Switch
When comparing controls (NI group) with BDSM community practitioners (BDSM-
CP group) who identified themselves with one of the three BDSM identity types
(Dom/Sub/Switch), differences were found for the secure (F(3, 492) = 11.34, p < .001),
anxious-avoidant (F(3, 492) = 3.82, p = .01), and anxious-preoccupied attachment style (F(3,
492) = 7.56, p < .001). Post-hoc Bonferroni analyses showed that controls had; a less secure
attachment style than Dom (p < .001), Sub (p < .01), and Switch (p < .05) practitioners; a more
anxious-avoidant attachment style than Dom participants (p < .01); and a less anxious-
preoccupied attachment style than Dom (p < .01), Sub (p < .05), and Switch (p < .01)
practitioners of BDSM. In conclusion, all BDSM identity types had a more secure attachment
style than controls, wherein dominance had the strongest association. Thereby, Dom
participants also had a less anxious-avoidant attachment style than other BDSM-identity types
or controls. No differences between all three BDSM identities and controls were found for the
made whether total BDSM-scores could be predicted by attachment style, trauma scores but
also by the variables gender, sexual preference, education levels and living area. A linear
17
(F=52.539; p < .001) with a R2 = .219. Sexual preference (p < .001), gender (p < .001), living
area (p <.001) and finally higher secure attachment style (p <.001) were significant predictors
for the intensity of the BDSM interest and practices. Non-heterosexual orientation, being male
and living in an urban region each predicted higher BDSM levels. Interestingly, none of the
DISCUSSION
This study investigated to what extent BDSM interests are related to early life
dynamics such as trauma and attachment style, in both BDSM private practitioners as well as
those practitioners that were part of the outdoor BDSM community. It was found that
practitioners from the BDSM community reported more physical abuse during adulthood but
not during childhood, as well as more diseases and, although not significantly, there was a
trend towards more unwanted sexual contacts in their lifetime. BDSM community
practitioners had higher total trauma scores compared to controls and equally reported more
adult physical abuse (PBA), whereas in private BDSM practitioners only higher levels of
PBA were found. No differences between groups were found for the severity of these
traumatic events. Also, there were no significant differences in traumatic events between
private and community practitioners. Although no significant group differences emerged for
the unwanted sexual traumatic events, mild significant associations between trauma and
BDSM were found in the community practice group (BDSM-CP), which seemed mostly
driven by the factor submissiveness (SUB). Such associations were not present in the private
practice group. Levels of intensity of BDSM interest and practices were predicted by a secure
community had an overall more secure and anxious-preoccupied attachment style than
controls and private practitioners of BDSM and a less anxious-avoidant attachment style than
non-practitioners having BDSM fantasies and private practitioners of BDSM. These findings
are in line with Wismeijer and Van Assen (2013) who demonstrated increases in both
Interestingly, in our sample, private practitioners of BDSM did not differ in any of the four
attachment styles from controls, suggesting that BDSM community members and private
practitioners have different attachment styles, and may represent different subgroups within
the general population. The contradictory result of the BDSM-CP group being more secure
and anxious-preoccupied at the same time leaves room for interpretation. Further exploration
within the BDSM community group showed no association between BDSM interests and the
anxious-preoccupied attachment style, which shows that factors other than intensity of BDSM
interests explain this difference between groups. Solely the secure attachment style was
associated with the intensity of BDSM interests: secure attachment was associated with
dominance and voyeurism. In addition, comparing the BDSM identities with controls, this
greater amount of secure attachment was found in all three subgroups of BDSM identities, but
It has been suggested (Freud, 1905) that BDSM activities are driven by an
unconscious negative reenactment pattern in which people seek out destructive situations and
relationships that have familiarity with their past trauma. Alternatively, BDSM-practices have
also been suggested (Califia, 1983) to be a coping mechanism that somehow facilitates trauma
unwanted sexual contacts among the community practitioners group which may be in line
with this notion, although it should also be noted that a vast majority of them (76,6%) did not
report any traumatic experiences of this type in their past. As such, simply framing BDSM-
19
practices as a coping style for sexual trauma would not be an adequate justification model.
behavior and the significance of a broad range of social influences on it, and argued that a
singular association between childhood abuse experiences and later sadomasochistic sexual
behavior would probably not be found. Nevertheless, 23,4% of the BDSM community group
had experienced sexual contacts they categorized as being unwanted, whereas this was only
the case for 16,2% of the controls. Women on one hand and the submissives and switches on
the other in the BDSM group seemed to drive this difference. The finding that community
practitioners (BDSM-CP) had a higher total trauma score than non-practitioners (NI &
BDSM-F), supports a possible link between trauma and BDSM. An alternative explanation
for the slight difference in prevalence of unwanted sexual contacts between the two samples
might have less to do with the actual sexually abusive event and more with its subjective
within BDSM-play (Faccio et al., 2014). Therefore, they might have a lower threshold in
from Klement and colleagues (2017) demonstrate that BDSM practitioners reported
significantly lower levels of sexism, rape myth acceptance, and victim blaming than non-
BDSM controls.
It should be noted that both private and community practitioners had experienced
more physical beatings during adulthood (PBA) than controls, hence not during childhood
(PBC). The link between childhood trauma and BDSM interests was therefore rather not
confirmed in this study. Besides, no association was found between PBA trauma and the
intensity of BDSM practices in our community sample, despite of the BDSM practice groups
(BDSM-PP & BDSM-CP) having experienced more PBA trauma than controls. These
findings postulate a small influence of physical beatings on BDSM intensity and practices.
20
Furthermore, it is unclear if those physical beatings were experienced within the context of
BDSM-play, which could then explain this difference between BDSM-practitioners and
controls. Elaborating on whether physical beatings happened inside or outside the BDSM
community is recommended for future studies. Again, the significant difference in PBA may
also be related to the emphasis on consent and personal boundaries as stated before.
The finding that BDSM practitioners were overall more secure in their attachment
styles than controls, resonates with sociological theories about BDSM which rather view
these practices as a recreational leisure rather than as a pathological practice. This could also
be more in line with the studies of Cowan (1982) and Stekel (1953) that showed that BDSM-
practitioners were found to be more successful and well-rounded persons. Maybe a more
secure attachment style is needed in order to participate in BDSM-play and in particular for
the dominant role. Given the kind of activities that are put into practice, a lot of trust is needed
with each participant, regardless of the role they have. This exchange of trust is generally
easier for securely attached individuals than for insecurely attached people.
It could also be that the relationship between secure attachment and dominance is
bidirectional in nature. Even though the basis of attachment is formed in the early stages of
someone’s life, it may be subject to changes due to new experiences. Therefore, it is plausible
for people with the dominant role to get more confidence and a more secure attachment style
within relationships because of the trust they are receiving from their submissive BDSM-
partners. Adopting the dominant role and performing the activities that come with it might
enhance or facilitate more confidence and secure feelings about oneself in the same way as an
assertiveness training could enhance relationships between people. But these hypotheses are
To summarize, both associations between BDSM interest and, albeit weakly, trauma
on one hand and attachment style on the other were found. However, despite the demonstrated
21
link between trauma and BDSM, the final model showed that none of the trauma items were a
good enough predictor for the intensity of BDSM interests, which should be taken into
consideration. Rather, sexual preference, living area, gender and secure attachment entered as
Moreover, this is a first study that makes a distinction between private and community
out of 4 attachment styles. Hence, they had a more secure and anxious-preoccupied
attachment style and a less anxious-avoidant attachment style than private practitioners. This
could be due to the different contexts in which BDSM-practices are played out. Being able to
practice it more often and being part of a community could enhance the secure attachment
style practitioners experience with others. Since each BDSM identity group had a more secure
attachment style than controls, the conclusion that BDSM interests and practices come from
insecure attachment styles can’t be drawn. Rather, indications for the inverse relationship
were shown. Further, no differences were found between private and community
practitioners concerning their degree of experienced trauma, which again shows little
From the perspective from social policy, our findings argue against framing BDSM-
medicalization and even criminalization, our data, together with a large body of scientific
evidence argue much more for acceptance of BDSM practices within the realm of normal
sexual interests and behavior. Active interventions aimed at tackling the stigma surrounding
these sexual interests are needed, including the distribution of adequate information.
There may be some limitations in this study. The fact that the survey in this study was
22
a self-report measure could lead to biased results. It is possible that there was a tendency
within the BDSM-population to portray themselves differently with the aim of changing the
fixed beliefs about people who practice BDSM. BDSM practitioners had higher education
levels and lived in more populated regions may also be a source of bias. Another limitation
concerns the different way the people in the two groups are approached for this study. We
made use of a social media approach for the BDSM group, whereas the control group were
contacted by a research bureau. Moreover, only those BDSM participants were included that
were active in online communities, and as such, do not necessarily reflect all BDSM
practitioners, thus hampering generalizability towards all subjects with BDSM interests. In
addition, the BDSM-group may also have included participants from the Netherlands, as the
invitation and the survey were presented in Dutch, a language spoken both in Belgium and
The Netherlands. As such, cultural differences between countries and different policies in the
psychical and sexual unwanted contacts, but did not investigate other emotional and
To conclude, this study found an association between BDSM interest and experienced
practiced BDSM had a more secure attachment style than non-practitioners. The intensity of
BDSM interest was predicted by a secure attachment style, sexual preferences other than
heterosexuality, gender (i.e. higher scores in males), and urban living area but not by
traumatic experiences. Taken together, these findings argue against the hypothesis of BDSM
being a maladaptive coping mechanism in response to early life dynamics. Future research is
References
test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-
244.
Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: I. Aetiology and
130, 201-210.
Coppens, V., Ten Brink, S., Huys, W., Fransen, E., Morrens, M. (2020). A Survey on
Interest Profile, and Role Identity. Journal of Sex Research 57, 129-136.
Faccio, E., Casini, C., & Cipolletta, S. (2014) Forbidden games: the construction of
sexuality and sexual pleasure by BDSM ‘players’. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 16,
752-764.
Feeney, J. A., Peterson, C., Gallois, C., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Attachment as a
predictor of sexual attitudes and behavior in late adolescence. Psychology & Health,
14, 1105-1122.
25
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse: A
Freud, S. (1905). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. Standard edition, 7 (ed. and
Frías, Á, González, L., Palma, C., & Farriols, L. (2017). Is there a relationship
Gentzler, A. L., & Kerns, K. A. (2004). Associations between insecure attachment and
Grant, J., & Crawley, J. (2002). Transference and projection : mirrors to the self (p.
Holvoet, L., Huys, W., Coppens, V., Seeuws, J., Goethals, K., & Morrens, M. (2017).
Fifty shades of Belgian gray: The prevalence of BDSM-related fantasies and activities
Kalichman, S. C., Gore-Felton, C., Benotsch, E., Cage, M., & Rompa, D. (2004).
sexual abuse and HIV risks among men who have sex with men. Journal of Child
consent may be associated with lower rape-supportive beliefs. The Journal of Sex
Masters, W. H., Johnson, V. E., & Kolodny, R. C. (1995). Heterosexuality. New York:
Perennial.
Meston, C. M., Heiman, J. R., Trapnell, P. D. (1999). The relation between early
abuse and adult sexuality. The Journal of Sex Research, 36, 385-395.
26
Nordling, N., Sandnabba, N. K., & Santtila, P. (2000). The prevalence and effects of
Renaud, C. A., & Byers, E. S. (1999). Exploring the frequency, diversity, and content
of university students’ positive and negative sexual cognitions. The Canadian Journal
Richters, J., de Visser, R. O., Rissel, C. E., Grulich, A. E., & Smith, A. M. A. (2008).
Roush, J. F., Brown, S. L., Mitchell, S. M., & Cukrowicz, K. C. (2017). Shame, guilt,
and suicide ideation among bondage and discipline, dominance and submission, and
Schnurr, P. P., Spiro, A., Vielhauer, M. J., Findler, M. N., & Hamblen, J. L. (2002).
Trauma in the lives of older men: Findings from the normative aging study. Journal of
Schnurr, P. P., Vielhauer, M. J., & Weathers, F. W. (1995). Brief Trauma Interview.
Unpublished interview.
Smith, D. K., Leve, L. D., & Chamberlain, P. (2006). Adolescent girls' offending and
health-risking sexual behavior: The predictive role of trauma. Child Maltreatment, 11,
346-353.
Stekel, W. (1953). Sadism and masochism: The psychology of hatred and cruelty (tr.
E. Gutheil). Volume 1 and 2. New York: Liveright. (Original work published 1929)
361-370.
Private Communit
No Interest Fantasy
practice y practice p
Number of completers 326 (25%) 192 (15%) 559 (43%) 212 (16%)
44.55 ± 37.91 ± 37.19 ± 40.83 ±
Age 13.14 13.78 11.73 13.64 <.001*
Male 39.9% 54.7% 44.7% 56.6% <.001#
Gender Female 59.8% 45.3% 54.7% 42.5%
Other 0.3% 0.0% 0.5% 0.9%
Urban 16.0% 13.5% 19.3% 31.6% <.001#
Living
Suburban 26.1% 28.6% 24.5% 59.9%
Area
Rural 58.0% 57.8% 56.2% 40.1%
Heterosexual 93.6% 90.1% 87.1% 63.2% <.001#
Homosexual 3.1% 6.3% 6.4% 4.7%
Sexual
Bisexual 2.1% 2.6% 5.4% 25.0%
Preference
Asexual 0.9% 1.0% 0.2% 0.9%
Other 0.3% 0.0% 0.9% 6.1%
Table 2. Occurrence rates for the seven types of traumatic events for each of the four BDSM
interest level groups (NI: neither fantasies nor practices; BDSM-F: fantasies without
Figure 1. Mean scores of each BDSM summary score for all BDSM intensity level groups
(NI: neither fantasies nor practices; BDSM-F: fantasies without practices; BDSM-PP:
setting).
31
Figure 2. Mean rank scores of the Kruskal-Wallis test and Mann-Whitney test results of all
group comparisons for the variables PBA, USC and the total trauma score. A Bonferroni
correction with a critical value of p = .0083 as significance level was applied. (NI: neither
fantasies nor practices; BDSM-F: fantasies without practices; BDSM-PP: practices of BDSM
Figure 3. Mean attachment styles scores and MANOVA test results for all group comparisons
using post-hoc Bonferroni analyses. (NI: neither fantasies nor practices; BDSM-F: fantasies
Figure 4. Mean attachment style scores and post-hoc Bonferroni test results (MANOVA) of
comparing the BDSM identities within the BDSM practice groups (BDSM-PP & BDSM-CP).
34
Figure 5. Mean attachment style scores and post-hoc Bonferroni test results (MANOVA) of
the comparisons between controls and all BDSM identities from the community practice
group (BDSM-CP).