Skinner 2007
Skinner 2007
Skinner 2007
1
Correspondence should be addressed to N. T. Feather, School of Psychology, Flinders
University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, Australia, 5001. E-mail: [email protected]
163
Deservingness Judgments
Responsibility
for
drug use
Values
heroin or consumed alcohol to excess. Heroin and alcohol were chosen for
this study because both drugs are known to be associated with significant
personal and social harms, and significant stigma is attached to heroin ad-
diction and heavy alcohol consumption. Therefore, we expect that issues
regarding access to and deservingness of medical care will be particularly
salient in this context.
The proposed theoretical model is presented in Figure 1 and will be dis-
cussed in further detail. As Figure 1 indicates, we expect that the patient’s
perceived deservingness of high- or low-quality care will be the most proximal
predictor of nurses’ satisfaction with each outcome. There are three possible
antecedents of deservingness judgments that are considered: affective reac-
tions to the drug user, attributions of responsibility for drug use, and values.
Findings from previous research are consistent with the assumption from
deservingness theory that deservingness judgments about another person’s
positive or negative outcome are moderated by how much that person is
positively or negatively evaluated (e.g., liked or disliked) by the person making
the judgments (e.g., Feather, 1999a, 1999b; Feather & Nairn, 2005; Feather &
Sherman, 2002). Consistent with these findings, we expect that deservingness
of high-quality care (a positive outcome) will increase with stronger positive
affect and weaker negative affect toward the drug user. The opposite pattern is
expected for deservingness of low-quality care (a negative outcome).
Responsibility Attributions
for the aggressive episode. Note that this study involved patients who were
expected to differ in their perceived responsibility for the aggressive incident,
and, indeed, the results showed that the patient with schizophrenia was
deemed to be less responsible when compared with the patient with per-
sonality disorder. In contrast to Feather and Johnstone’s study, the current
study involves patients who are similar in the sense that they are drug users
(alcohol, heroin), and responsibility judgments were targeted to their drug
use, rather than to a specific behavioral incident within the hospital.
It might be argued that the more these patients were perceived to be
responsible for their drug use, the more they would be perceived to deserve
low- rather than high-quality care, and the more angry and less sympathetic
the nurses would feel toward them. Consistent with this argument, Skitka
and Tetlock (1992, 1993) demonstrated that attributions of responsibility for
health care (e.g., AIDS patients, organ-donor recipients) resulted in less
generous allocation of (hypothetical) public resources. Research has also
shown that feelings of anger and lowered sympathy toward another person
are more evident when that person is perceived to be more responsible for a
negative event or outcome than when perceived responsibility is lower or
absent (e.g., Menec & Perry, 1998; Weiner, 1995; Weiner et al., 1988).
Complicating this argument as it relates to deservingness, however, is the
fact that people’s deservingness judgments are sometimes likely to reflect
beliefs about entitlement, especially in contexts in which entitlement norms
are salient. People sometimes confuse deservingness with entitlement in their
everyday language. Feather (1999b, 2002a, 2003) distinguished between de-
servingness and entitlement on conceptual grounds. He proposed that
deservingness refers to judgments about positive or negative outcomes that
are contingent on a person’s positive or negative behavior (e.g., a student
deserving a high grade on an exam because he or she studied hard). In
contrast, entitlement refers more to the external normative context of rules,
laws, or other quasilegal prescriptions (e.g., an employee becomes entitled to
a promotion on the basis of an affirmative-action policy). Keeping these
fairness principles apart has important implications. For example, in some
situations, deservingness and entitlement may be compatible; while in other
situations they may come into conflict (Feather, 2002a, 2003; Feather &
Johnstone, 2001; Feinberg, 1970).
We can add further to this analysis. While judgments of deservingness for
a contingent outcome presuppose personal causation for the action that
produced the outcome and some degree of perceived responsibility, there is
no reason to expect that a person’s perceived entitlement to an outcome
should depend in part on that person’s perceived responsibility. A judgment
that a patient who uses drugs is personally responsible for his or her
condition should not mean that he or she is not entitled to proper care, given
168 SKINNER ET AL.
the norms under which the hospital and its employees operate and the caring
roles that these employees are expected to perform. Note that norms of
entitlement had an important influence on nurses’ responses in Feather and
Johnstone’s (2001) study, and we expect them to do so in the current study
as well.
The implication of the preceding argument is that if the nurses responded
to questions about deservingness as if these questions concerned entitlement,
then their responses may be unrelated to the patient’s perceived responsi-
bility for drug abuse. Deservingness, then, would be equated with entitle-
ment, and all patients would be perceived to be entitled to good, quality
care, regardless of their perceived responsibility for their drug-related ail-
ments. Beliefs about entitlement would dictate the responses to be made,
especially when the context is one in which there are strong entitlement
norms about proper treatment directed toward the welfare of patients and
where, in contrast to Feather and Johnstone’s (2001) study, there is an
absence of a specific behavior (e.g., an aggressive episode) that would engage
deservingness judgments. Entitlement norms would dictate that nurses
should express sympathy (or positive affect), rather than anger (or negative
affect), toward patients who are under their care, regardless of patients’
perceived responsibility. Hence, the paths in Figure 1 between responsibility
and deservingness, and between responsibility and affect are presented with
considerable qualification, and, indeed, they may be absent.
Values
The focus so far has been on relations between responsibility, affect, and
deservingness. In the current study, we also are interested in the role of
values as possible antecedents of responsibility attributions and affective
responses to the drug user. Values reflect general beliefs that people hold
about desirable or undesirable ways of behaving (e.g., being honest, being
loving) and about general goals or end states of existence (e.g., equality,
freedom; Feather, 1999b).
Values are assumed to be central components of the self-concept and to
be important influences on a person’s attitudes and behaviors. They are used
as criteria to guide the way people construe situations in terms of what
objects and events, behaviors and goals, or actions and outcomes are per-
ceived as desirable or undesirable, or as possessing positive or negative
valence (Feather, 1975, 1994, 1995; Rokeach, 1973), influencing the way
actions and their outcomes are evaluated (Feather, 1995). For example,
people with strong right-wing authoritarian values are more likely to view
crimes as serious (depending also on the context and the perpetrator of the
STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION IN HEALTH-CARE PROVISION 169
offense; Altemeyer, 1988; Christie, 1993; Feather, 1996, 1998). In the current
study, we explore the role of the higher order conservation and self-
transcendence value dimensions described by Schwartz (1992, 1994). These
values were chosen because they were most likely to relate to the issues of
drug use and provision of medical care that are examined in the current
study.
Conservation values encompass values that emphasize the importance of
conformity, tradition, and security values for self, as opposed to values that
emphasize the importance of openness to change (e.g., stimulation, self-
direction values). We expect that an emphasis on conservation values will be
associated with more punitive judgments, and will be reflected in perceptions
that drug users are largely responsible for their drug use and also in lower
positive affect and stronger negative affect toward the drug user. Evidence
from studies of right-wing authoritarians is consistent with this assumption.
Right-wing authoritarians tend to have stronger conservation values, to be
more punitive in their judgments, and to be more negative in their social
attitudes in situations in which these values are threatened (Feather, 1996,
1998, 1999b).
Self-transcendence values encompass universalism values that empha-
size the importance of general social goals (e.g., equality, social justice for
self), and benevolence values that emphasize the importance of prosocial
values that are more interpersonal in nature (e.g., being forgiving, being
helpful). They are opposed to values that emphasize self-enhancement
(e.g., power, achievement values). We expect that an emphasis on self-
transcendence values will be associated with more compassionate and
prosocial judgments, and will be reflected in perceptions that drug users are
less responsible for their drug use and also in higher positive affect and
lower negative affect toward the drug user. Evidence from values research
is consistent with this prediction. For example, people with stronger uni-
versalism values tend to perceive a crime committed by a member of the
public as less serious, depending on the context and nature of the crime
(Feather, 1996, 1998).
Summary
Method
Participants
Procedure
A 2 (Quality of Care: high vs. low) 2 (Drug Type: alcohol vs. heroin)
mixed within–between subjects design was used. The study was introduced
as an exploration of how nurses view individuals who use licit or illicit drugs.
The questionnaires were completed under anonymous conditions.
Participants were provided with a hypothetical scenario of a drug user
presenting to an Emergency Department (ED) nurse with a serious medical
condition associated with one type of drug use. Participants were randomly
assigned to a scenario addressing problematic alcohol use or injection of
heroin. The scenarios contained identical wording, except for the specific
type of drug and associated medical condition (heroin and a skin abscess;
alcohol and a stomach ulcer). The medical conditions were chosen carefully
to ensure that they represented common and realistic conditions associated
with each drug type, and were of equivalent seriousness with regard to
treatment priorities in an ED setting. The heroin scenario was as follows:
On a relatively quiet Sunday night, MJ presents at an Emer-
gency Department with a large and painful abscess on their
arm. On close questioning, MJ reveals to the triage nurse that
the abscess has developed from regular injection of heroin over
the past 5 years.
The alcohol scenario was as follows:
On a relatively quiet Sunday night, MJ presents at an Emer-
gency Department with a strong stomach pain. On close ques-
tioning, MJ reveals to the triage nurse that they have previously
been diagnosed with a large stomach ulcer that has developed
from regular excessive drinking sessions over the past 5 years.
STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION IN HEALTH-CARE PROVISION 171
After reading the first part of each scenario, participants were presented
with five questions concerning (a) perceived responsibility for drug use,
measured by the extent to which the patient was responsible for the drug use
(1 5 not at all responsible to 5 5 very responsible); and (b) positive (sym-
pathy, concern) and negative (anger, disappointment) affective responses to
the patient (e.g., 1 5 not at all angry to 5 5 very angry).
The affect items were chosen on the basis of previous research on af-
fective responses to stigmatized others (Feather & Johnstone, 2001; Menec
& Perry, 1998; Schmidt & Weiner, 1988). A principal-components factor
analysis with varimax rotation was conducted on the four scales. There were
two factors that emerged with eigenvalues greater than 1. Anger and dis-
appointment items loaded on a single factor with factor loadings of .82 and
.87, respectively. Scores on the two items were averaged to produce a mea-
sure of negative affect toward the drug user. Sympathy and concern items
loaded on a second factor with factor loadings of .83 and .85, respectively.
Scores on these two items were averaged to produce a measure of positive
affect toward the drug user.
Internal Reliabilities
Values Measure
Values were measured using the Schwartz Value Survey (SVS; Schwartz,
1992). Participants indicated the extent to which 57 values are a ‘‘guiding
principle in your life’’ using the standard SVS 9-point response scale ranging
from - 1 (opposed to my values) to 0 (not important) to 7 (of supreme im-
portance). The scale taps 10 different value types that reflect four higher
order value dimensions. The higher order value dimensions of conservation
and self-transcendence were considered in the current model.2
Self-transcendence value types were tapped by two scales: universalism
(i.e., understanding, appreciation, tolerance, protection for the welfare of all
people and for nature; a 5 .83) and benevolence (i.e., preservation and en-
hancement of the welfare of people with whom one is in frequent personal
contact; a 5 .77; Schwartz, 1994). Conservation value types were tapped by
2
Examination of correlation matrices indicates that the values that were excluded from the
current analyses did not demonstrate large or consistent relationships with the dependent
variables.
STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION IN HEALTH-CARE PROVISION 173
Results
Table 1
Variable M SD M SD t
Responsibility 4.08 0.94 4.03 0.98 0.41
Positive affect 3.88 0.82 3.84 0.78 0.47
Negative affect 2.43 1.04 2.57 1.08 - 1.08
Note. df 5 275; None of the t tests were significant (p>.05).
Means for Deservingness of High Versus Low Quality of Care and Satisfaction With Outcome According to Drug Type
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Self-transcendence F - .19 - .16 .27 - .18 .20 - .32 .10 - .28
2. Conservation - .13 F .11 - .03 .23 - .16 .12 .01 .07
3. Responsibility for drug use - .01 .23 F .01 .14 - .07 .06 .02 .04
4. Sympathy/concern .24 - .02 - .04 F - .11 .37 - .38 .31 - .42
5. Anger/disappointment - .21 .32 .08 - .12 F - .28 .37 - .16 .28
6. Deservingness of high-quality care .22 - .02 .01 .31 - .23 F - .64 .62 - .50
7. Deservingness of low-quality care - .28 .07 - .13 - .28 .32 - .43 F - .40 .62
8. Satisfaction with high-quality care .21 .12 .15 .35 - .05 .55 - .37 F - .49
9. Satisfaction with low-quality care - .20 .01 - .02 - .47 .06 - .37 .59 - .38 F
Note. Alcohol correlations are presented above the diagonal, while heroin correlations are presented below the diagonal.
p o .05. p o .01. p o .001.
STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION IN HEALTH-CARE PROVISION 177
Regression Analyses
The proposed model was tested using path analysis. The analysis used
multiple regression with pairwise deletion of missing cases to generate
standardized regression coefficients. Path coefficients were obtained by
regressing each variable on those that directly impinged on it. Figure 2
summarizes the results of the regression analysis in a path diagram (non-
significant paths for both drugs are not shown, but will be discussed in
further detail).
Key:
Responsibility Alcohol
for Heroin
drug use
.09
.23* 2
R = .01
R2 = .05
-.09
-.18* .34***
.29**
Figure 2. Theoretical model of affective responses to the drug user and deservingness and
satisfaction (standardized coefficients). Path coefficients for negative affect on deservingness of
low-quality care and for positive affect on deservingness of high-quality care were identical
when rounded to two decimal places. p o .05. p o .01. p o .001.
178 SKINNER ET AL.
user, nor was the self-transcendence value dimension associated with re-
duced negative affect (with the exception of a weak effect in the heroin
scenario). In addition, conservation and self-transcendence value dimen-
sions did not predict perceived responsibility for drug use in the alcohol
scenario, and only the conservation value dimension predicted perceived
responsibility in the heroin scenario.
As expected, affective responses to the patient predicted the perceived
deservingness of high- and low-quality care. Low-quality care was perceived
to be more deserved when negative affect was stronger and positive affect
toward the patient was weaker. The opposite pattern was evident for per-
ceived deservingness of high-quality care. However, perceived responsibility
for drug use did not predict positive or negative affect toward the drug user
or deservingness judgments. As expected, perceived deservingness of each
outcome (i.e., high- or low-quality care) strongly predicted satisfaction with
each respective outcome.
Discussion
Deservingness or Entitlement?
Values
associated with this threat. In contrast, individuals who placed a high value
on self-transcendence value types would be expected to be more sensitive to
aspects of the scenario that related to the provision of care and assistance.
Therefore, in this context, the activation of self-transcendence value types
would be related to the potential for fulfilling important values and to
positive affect (sympathy, concern) associated with this potential fulfilment.
Methodological Limitations
References
Aalto, M., Pekuri, P., & Seppa, K. (2001). Primary health care nurses’ and
physicians’ attitudes, knowledge, and beliefs regarding brief intervention
for heavy drinkers. Addiction, 96, 305-311.
STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION IN HEALTH-CARE PROVISION 183