ED586106

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 47

A Comprehensive Examination of Reading Heterogeneity in Students with High Functioning

Autism: Distinct Reading Profiles and their Relation to Autism Symptom Severity

Nancy S. McIntyrea, Emily J. Solaria, Ryan P. Grimma, Lindsay E. Lerroa,

Joseph E. Gonzalesb and Peter C. Mundya

a
School of Education, One Shields Avenue, University of California, Davis, 95616, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Lowell, USA

Article Citation (Published April 2017):


McIntyre, N. S., Solari, E. J., Grimm, R. P., Lerro, L. E., Gonzales, J. E., & Mundy, P. C. (2017).
A comprehensive examination of reading heterogeneity in students with high functioning autism:
Distinct reading profiles and their relation to Autism Symptom Severity. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 47(4), 1086-1101.

Research Funded by: NIMH 1RO1MH085904, IES R324A120168


Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Abstract

The goal of this study was to identify unique profiles of readers in a sample of 8-16-year olds

with higher functioning autism spectrum disorders (HFASD) and examine the profiles in relation

to ASD symptom severity. Eighty-one students were assessed utilizing a comprehensive reading

battery that included basic word reading, language, and comprehension. Using Latent Profile

Analysis, four empirically distinct profiles of readers emerged. Next, using the Autism

Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012), analyses were

conducted to determine if significant differences existed between profiles as a result of ASD

symptomatology. Findings demonstrate the heterogeneous nature of reading profiles in students

with HFASD and significant differences between the reading profiles and ASD symptom

severity.

Keywords: Reading; Language; Reading Profiles; Higher Functioning Autism

2
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Current science emphasizes the need to not only distinguish groups of children affected

by autism spectrum disorders (ASD) from comparison children but also to investigate the nature

of heterogeneity in development among children affected by ASD (Georgiades et al., 2013;

Happe, Ronald & Plomin, 2006). The expectation and need to understand psychological

heterogeneity is especially true for older children because as cognitive, language, and emotional

processes develop and differentiate with age it is likely that there are greater degrees of freedom

for the expression of heterogeneity. There are many approaches to studying heterogeneity in

ASD including, for example, the study of differences in social engagement (Wing & Gould,

1979), differences in social attention (Rice, Moriuchi, Jones & Klin, 2012), differences in

executive function (Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2003; Geurts, Verte, Oosterlaan, Roeyers, &

Sergeant, 2004), sensory processing (Lane, Young, Baker, & Angley, 2010), and language

(Pickles, Anderson, & Lord, 2014; Rapin, Dunn, Allen, Stevens & Fein, 2009; Tager-Flusberg,

2006), as well as multidimensional approaches (Beglinger & Smith, 2001; Insel, 2014).

There are many reasons why understanding heterogeneity in ASD is of great interest.

One of these is that the identification of valid subgroups holds the promise of enabling a more

precise alignment of treatments and educational plans for affected individuals with ASD

(Beglinger & Smith, 2005; Miles et al. 2005). This is an especially important consideration for

school-aged children who begin to experience their kindergarten through 12th grade classrooms

as their primary venue for intervention. Currently we have very little information on the

heterogeneity of ASD that informs and advances contemporary educational practices for

elementary and secondary students with ASD (Dingfelder & Mandell, 2011; Kasari & Smith,

2013; Machalicek et al., 2008). This is particularly true for higher functioning children for

3
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

whom very little evidence-based information is available to guide optimal education in inclusive

regular education classrooms (Machalicek et al., 2008). This is unfortunate for two reasons.

First, current epidemiological data not only indicate that 1 in 66 children in second grade

throughout the nation are affected by ASD, but 47% of these children have average to above

average intellectual ability and 25% have borderline IQ. Only 28% of these second grade

children are affected by intellectual disabilities (Christensen et al., 2016). Many school-aged

children with ASD function in a range of intelligence that allows them to receive their education

in regular education classrooms (de Bruin, Deppeler, Moore, & Diamond, 2013; Fleury et al.,

2014), which would suggest that these students should gain benefit from general education

instruction, with individualized special education supports.

Second, although children with higher functioning ASD (HFASD) are capable of

receiving their education in regular education classrooms, they are at risk for academic

difficulties. One specific difficulty that has been empirically demonstrated in the literature is in

reading; these difficulties share similarities to students identified as reading disabled. Multiple

studies report symptomatology similar to reading comprehension disability, or individuals who

are able to fluently decode words, yet have difficulties understanding the meaning of written text,

in 33 - 65% of the samples (Estes, Rivera, Bryan, Cali, & Dawson, 2011; Jones et al., 2009;

Nation, Clarke, Wright, & Williams, 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts, Jones, Happé, &

Charman, 2013). Furthermore, poor reading abilities have been shown to be substantially

discrepant from IQ in many children, leading to the concern that many students are

underachieving (Jones et al., 2009). This provides evidence that current school-based reading

instruction does not sufficiently prevent negative reading comprehension outcomes for children

with HFASD. Less is known about the development of basic word recognition skills in students

4
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

with HFASD. Some studies have suggested that word decoding or word reading is not a specific

deficit that is more prevalent in this population of students (e.g. Brown, Oram-Cardy, & Johnson,

2013), however, it has not been thoroughly examined with a comprehensive word recognition

skills battery in large samples.

With a deeper, evidence-based understanding of the nature and variability of reading

difficulties, or disturbances, in school-aged children with HFASD, beyond what is already

known about reading comprehension disability, and including the development of word

recognition, it may be possible to develop more targeted methods of instruction for this

population (Reutebuch, El Zein, Kim, Weinberg, & Vaughn, 2015). In this study, we extend the

current knowledge about the reading development and disability in students with HFASD in

three ways. First, we provide a deeper understanding of reading impairments in this sample,

beyond reading comprehension difficulties, with a close examination of basic reading

development. Second, we attempt to determine if meaningful subgroups or profiles of reading

strengths and weaknesses can be identified in a sample of students with HFASD. Third, we

investigate the relation between HFASD reading subgroups and ASD symptomatology.

Development of Reading: Subcomponent Skills and Profiles of ASD Struggling Readers

Reading for meaning develops over time and builds upon two brain regions already

present in infancy: the visual object recognition and oral language systems (Dehaene, 2009). By

the time children are 5 or 6 years old, key visual recognition processes are well developed but

still maximally plastic. Children’s vocabulary grows 10-20 words per day by the end of their

second year, and by the time they are 6 years old, most have expert knowledge of phonology,

basic grammar rules, and a vocabulary of several thousand words. In the phonological stage of

reading (Frith, 1985), children develop letter-sound correspondence requiring proficient letter

5
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

recognition skills and phonological awareness, or the ability to discern individual speech sounds.

They learn to decode words, progressing from the simple to the complex. Morphemic awareness

develops as well, and children learn that prefixes, root words, and suffixes are associated with

pronunciation and meaning. In the orthographic stage (Frith, 1985), the lexical pathway used to

identify words by sight develops and progressively supplements the decoding/phonological

pathway. Oral language processing creates meaning from the words. These two processes may

develop relatively independently (Adlof, Catts, & Lee, 2010) and the relationship between these

factors and reading comprehension changes over time. Cain & Oakhill (2008) noted that for

younger children decoding is more important and the correlation between reading and listening

comprehension is low. By high school however, decoding differences are generally small and

the correlation between reading and listening comprehension is high. Therefore, they posited

that based on this model one should expect a person’s reading comprehension to develop to the

same level as their listening comprehension once word reading is fluent.

The Simple View of Reading describes successful reading comprehension as the result of

sufficient decoding and linguistic comprehension skills (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). These two

component skills are described as multiplicative in nature and therefore both sets of skills must

be operating sufficiently for successful reading comprehension. The Component Model of

Reading (Joshi & Aaron, 2000), based upon the Simple View, incorporated processing speed, as

measured by speed of letter naming, as a third predictor of reading comprehension. Prior studies

have used the Component Model to investigate how poor readers may fall into subgroups that

differ across the components of word decoding and linguistic comprehension (Aaron, 1997;

Catts, Hogan & Fey, 2003; Catts & Kamhi, 1999; Gough & Tunmer, 1986). At least three

subgroups of poor readers are predicted by this model: (a) poor readers with word recognition

6
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

problems only (e.g. dyslexics), (b) poor readers with linguistic comprehension problems only

(e.g. poor comprehenders and/or hyperlexics), and (c) poor readers with difficulties in both

components [e.g. garden variety poor readers (Gough & Tunmer, 1986), mixed reading disabled

(Catts & Kamhi, 2005), or language-learning disabled (LLD; Berninger & May, 2011)].

Furthermore, Aaron, Joshi, Gooden, and Bentum (2008) demonstrated the utility of the

Component Model in identifying the facet(s) of reading that is(are) the source of a child’s

reading difficulty in order to better target intervention efforts.

A deeper examination of the two component skills, word recognition and linguistic

comprehension, reveals many essential sub skills. For example, poor readers who struggle with

accurate lower-level word recognition skills typically demonstrate deficits in phonological

processing, or the processing of speech sounds. There is empirical evidence that demonstrates a

correlational relation between facets of phonological processing and word recognition including:

phonological awareness (Bradley & Bryant. 1983; Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Share, 1995;

Swanson, Trainin, Necoechea, & Hammill, 2003; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994),

phonological decoding (Rastle & Coltheart, 1998; Rey, Ziegler, & Jacobs, 2000; Swanson et al,

2003), and rapid automatized naming (Kirby, Parrila, & Pfeiffer, 2003; Manis, Doi, & Bhada,

2000; Swanson et al, 2003). Vocabulary development has also been linked with word

recognition skill (Biemiller, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Chiappe, Chiappe & Gottardo,

2004; Nation, 2009; National Reading Panel, 2000; Ouellette & Beers, 2010; Perfetti, 2007).

Linguistic comprehension, or the oral language processing that creates meaning from

words, has a profound effect on the comprehension of written texts (e.g., Nation & Snowling,

2004; Roth, Speece, & Cooper, 2002). Empirical evidence suggests that higher-level linguistic

comprehension skills are underpinned by the depth and breadth of one’s vocabulary (Ouellette &

7
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Beers, 2010; Perfetti, 2007; Ricketts, Nation & Bishop, 2007; Roth, Speece & Cooper, 2002;

Senechal, Ouellette, & Rodney, 2006), syntax and grammar (Cain & Oakhill, 2006; Muter,

Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004; Nation, Clarke, Marshall, & Durand, 2004), verbal

reasoning and integration of background knowledge during reading to generate inferences

(Hannon & Daneman, 2001; Long & Lea, 2005; McNamara, 2001), and narrative recall (Fuchs,

Fuchs, & Maxwell, 1988; Leslie & Caldwell, 2009). All of these facets of oral language support

the construction of a globally coherent situation model of a text; semantic, grammatical and

syntactic information provide the foundation of the text-based mental model, then continuous

connections between prior knowledge, inferences, and text ideas are made to create the situation

model required for proficient reading comprehension (Kintsch, 1988; Van Dijk & Kintsch,

1983).

Empirical evidence exists to demonstrate that both word recognition and linguistic

comprehension account for substantial unique variance in reading comprehension for children

with ASD, supporting the Simple View in this population (Jones et al., 2009; Lindgren, Folstein,

Tomblin, & Tager-Flusberg, 2009; Nation et al., 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al.,

2013). Similar to the subtypes described by Catts et al. (2003) with a typically developing

reading sample, many samples with ASD have displayed profiles comparable to poor

comprehenders, or hyperlexics, who demonstrate adequate word decoding alongside poor

language and reading comprehension (e.g. Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009; Nation et al,

2006; Newman et al., 2007; Huemer & Mann, 2010; Wei, Christiano, Yu, Wagner & Spiker,

2015; Zuccarello et al., 2015).

Other researchers have reported evidence that subgroups of children with poor

comprehension have significant concomitant lower-level phonological, rapid naming, and/or

8
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

word decoding deficits (Asberg & Sandberg, 2012; Nation et al., 2006; White et al., 2006).

White et al. (2006) found that, similar to those with typical development, phonological skills

were a strong predictor of word recognition and spelling in 8-12-year-olds with ASD. However,

Gabig (2010) found that while phonological awareness was delayed in development for 5-7-year

olds with ASD in their sample, it was not significantly related to word reading or decoding, but it

was significantly correlated with receptive vocabulary. Similarly, in several other studies, word

recognition skills have been shown to correlate with language abilities, reporting subgroups of

children with poor word recognition associated with poor oral language skills (e.g. Brown et al.,

2013; Jacobs & Richdale, 2013; Lindgren et al., 2009; Nation et al., 2006; Norbury & Nation,

2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Three studies have reported that children with ASD who have age-

appropriate language skills scored significantly higher than those with language impairments on

standardized measures of reading comprehension, word recognition, and decoding (Lindgren et

al., 2009; Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). None of the studies reviewed

reported a subgroup of children displaying a dyslexic profile, or one in which impaired word

recognition is concomitant with proficient linguistic comprehension.

Several studies have placed emphasis on exploring higher-level linguistic comprehension

factors in more detail. For example, participants with ASD have been shown to have difficulty

integrating background knowledge and inferred knowledge explicitly with global text (Saldana

& Frith, 2007), using background knowledge to interpret and remember specific information or

resolve ambiguities in discourse (Wahlberg & Magliano, 2004), or responding to questions about

inferred emotions (Tirado & Saldana, 2016). Language impairment in adolescents with ASD

was associated with poorer performance on a passage-level inference measure (Norbury &

Nation, 2011), and in elementary school-aged children verbal ability was the strongest predictor

9
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

of performance on inferential reading comprehension questions (Lucas & Norbury, 2015).

Norbury & Nation (2011) suggested that difficulties integrating information from different

sources for global coherence and inference generation might be highly dependent on variance in

the language skills of students with ASD.

Reading Comprehension and ASD Symptomatology

Reading for meaning is fundamental for accessing social, cultural, and political milieus

through written documents, and is a cognitively complex process. Current research suggests that

the risk for reading comprehension disability may be related to ASD symptomatology and be a

component of the social-communicative and cognitive phenotype of school-aged children with

ASD; several studies have reported significant associations between individual differences in

reading development and diagnostic status, social functioning, or autistic symptom severity in

samples of school-aged children with ASD (Asberg, Kopp, Berg-Kelly, & Gillberg, 2010; Estes

et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2009; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Reading is a

written form of communication between the author and the reader, and as such, is likely to be

impacted by deficits in social communication abilities such as understanding an author’s

intentions or purpose for writing a text, which impedes learning from the text. Furthermore,

impairments in social communication skills may impact reading comprehension through

impeding the development of rich networks of semantic and episodic knowledge typically

developed through socially-mediated learning. Additionally, challenges in understanding social

norms may lead to difficulty developing skills that rely on social knowledge such as

understanding characters’ intentions, inference generation, and understanding of narrative

elements.

10
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

The cognitive characteristics of many children with ASD include the tendency to focus

on details rather than global meaning (Booth & Happe, 2010), leading to particular problems

generating global coherence or processing at the gist level across a text (e.g., Pellicano, 2010),

which in turn leads to difficulty recalling, retelling, and comprehending stories (Diehl, Bennetto,

& Young, 2006; Williams, Goldstein, & Minshew, 2006). Furthermore, this local processing

bias, or weak central coherence (Happe & Frith, 2006), has been posited to lead to particular

difficulty integrating information both from the text and from background knowledge for

inference generation (Norbury & Nation, 2011) and global comprehension (Ricketts et al., 2013).

Another cognitive characteristic associated with ASD is the tendency to have restricted or fixated

interests, and this can limit exposure to situations where individuals learn about a wide variety of

topics and develop oral language skills across multiple contexts. This restricts vocabulary growth

except in fields of specific interests, and leads to more literal, less flexible understanding of

words and phrases. Combined, these difficulties constrain creation of a coherent mental model of

text that draws on a reader’s ability to combine text-based information with relevant background

knowledge to generate inferences about things not explicitly stated in the text (Kintsch, 1988;

McNamara, 2001). Overall, it may be that the severity of the social communication and ASD-

specific cognitive difficulties align with the severity of reading comprehension deficits for many

children affected by ASD.

In summary, there have been several attempts to unpack the relation between ASD

symptomatology, language, and reading performance. The existing empirical literature suggests

individuals with ASD have particular difficulties with reading comprehension and those

difficulties may be associated with both language and symptom severity of individuals with

ASD. The extant data also presents some evidence that similar profiles of struggling readers exist

11
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

to those seen in typically developing populations, with perhaps the exception of a reading profile

that exhibits a dyslexic profile of poor word recognition alongside discrepantly proficient

linguistic comprehension. However, very few studies of reading in ASD have utilized reading

batteries that assess the Simple View of Reading and include the key sub skills supporting

linguistic comprehension and word recognition. Therefore, we still know very little about the

potential difficulties individuals with ASD have on tasks related to word recognition and how

these lower level reading variables interact with linguistic and reading comprehension.

Furthermore, measurement of reading ability varies depending on the assessments used (Cutting

& Scarborough, 2006), and different findings from previous studies could be the result of using

different reading measures. Therefore, use of multiple measures of each component of reading

would be beneficial.

Current Study

Previous research has started to investigate reading development in school-aged children

with ASD. To our knowledge, no studies have analyzed subgroups, or distinct profiles, of

school-aged readers with ASD using an extensive reading battery that includes the sub skills of

lower-level word recognition abilities, including phonological processing and processing speed

measures, and higher-level linguistic and reading comprehension skills. In the current study, a

comprehensive reading and language assessment battery was collected with school-aged children

with HFASD to answer the following research questions: (a) Do individuals with HFASD

exhibit distinct reading profiles? (b) How do distinct reading subgroup profiles relate to ASD

symptom severity? Based on previous literature, we hypothesize that HFASD readers are

heterogeneous in nature, with relative strengths and weaknesses, therefore distinct profiles will

emerge. We also hypothesize that similar to previous research, individuals with HFASD who

12
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

demonstrate more severe reading discrepancies will also have more severe ASD

symptomatology.

Method

Participants

This research was conducted in compliance with the Institutional Review Board and

written parental consent and child assent was obtained prior to data collection. Participants were

81 (66 male) children, aged 8 to 16 years, who had a community diagnosis of ASD (see

descriptive statistics in Table 1). Enrolled subjects were recruited from the local community

through school districts, a university research subject tracking system, and word of mouth.

Individuals were included in the HFASD sample if they had a community diagnosis of ASD that

was confirmed by trained researchers using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule,

Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012), and if they had had a full-scale IQ (FIQ) estimate ≥

75 as measured on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence-II (WASI-2, Wechsler,

2011). A total of 93 individuals with ASD were recruited for this study; they all met criteria for

ASD on the ADOS, but 12 individuals were ineligible for the study due to FIQ < 75.

Exclusionary criteria included an identified syndrome other than ASD or ADHD (e.g. Fragile X),

significant sensory or motor impairment (e.g. visual impairments), a neurological disorder (e.g.

epilepsy, cerebral palsy), psychotic symptoms (e.g. hallucinations or delusions), or any major

medical disorder that could be associated with extended absences from school. Twenty-eight

percent of the children with HFASD also met criteria for ADHD according to parent report of a

community diagnosis. Most of the children in this sample spent much, or all, of their school day

in a general education classroom setting: 65% were in general education 81-100% of the day,

12% were in general education 41-80% of the day, 10% were in general education 1-40% of the

13
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

day, 10% were not in general education at all, and three percent did not report placement.

Eighty-four percent of the children attended public schools, and 91% had an IEP or 504 Plan.

Measures and Procedures

Data reported are from assessment sessions that were conducted by members of trained

research group in a university-based child assessment laboratory over two 2.5-hour sessions.

Diagnostic Measures and Sample Description. The ADOS-2 (Lord et al., 2012) is a

semi-structured diagnostic assessment shown to have strong predictive validity compared to best

estimate clinical diagnoses (Charman & Gotham, 2013). Scores were utilized to both confirm

ASD diagnosis and as a distal measure in the second research question. Modules 3 and 4 were

administered, providing scores for Social Affect (SA) and Restricted and Repetitive Behavior

(RRB). The Module 3 yielded a raw subscore for SA and for RRB that combined to create the

Total Score. Intraclass correlations for interrater reliability for Module 3 were reported as 0.92

for SA, 0.91 for RRB and 0.94 for overall total raw score. Intraclass correlations for interrater

reliability for Module 4 were reported to be 0.93 for Social Interaction, 0.84 for Communication,

0.92 for Communication + Social Interaction, and 0.82 for Stereotyped Behaviors and Restricted

Interests (Lord et al., 2012). Module 4 scores were converted via the modified Module 4

algorithm per Hus & Lord (2014).

IQ. The WASI-2 (Wechsler, 2011) provided an estimate of verbal and nonverbal

cognitive ability. Two verbal subtests, Vocabulary and Similarities, measured expressive

vocabulary and abstract semantic reasoning and formed the verbal composite (VIQ). Two

nonverbal subtests, Block Design and Matrix Reasoning, measured spatial perception, visual

abstract processing & problem solving with motor and non-motor involvement and formed the

performance composite (PIQ). Combined, the four subtests yielded an age-normed standard

14
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

score measurement of full-scale IQ (FIQ). The Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) index has established

internal consistency (0.96) and test-retest reliability for children ages 6-16, r = 0.94 (Wechsler,

2011). In this sample, internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .89 for

Vocabulary; .88 for Similarities; .87 for Block Design; and .92 for Matrix Reasoning.

Phonological processing and rapid automatized naming. The Elision and Nonword

Repetition (NWR) subtests were administered from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological

Processing, Second Version (CTOPP-2; Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1999) that yielded age-

normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3) measuring phonological awareness (PA) and expressive

phonology/phonological memory respectively. The internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha

coefficient from our sample for Elision (alpha = .93) was consistent with publisher reported

alphas (alphas = .81–.91; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). The internal consistency

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient from our sample for NWR (alpha = .78) was consistent with

publisher reported alphas (alphas = .73–.80).The speed at which participants were able to

connect orthographic and phonological representations was measured using two rapid

automatized naming (RAN) tasks from the CTOPP-2; Rapid Letter Naming and Rapid Digit

Naming subtests yielded separate age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3), and combined for

an age-normed RAN index score (M = 100, SD = 15). Alternate-form reliability coefficients

from our sample for Rapid Letter Naming (.89) and Rapid Digit Naming (.87) were consistent

with publisher reported alternate-form reliability coefficients (.70–.93).

Word Recognition. The Test of Word Reading Efficiency, Second Edition (TOWRE-2,

Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2012) provided an age-normed standard score (M = 100, SD

=15) measuring accuracy and fluency of sight word recognition (Sight Word Efficiency: SWE)

and phonemic decoding (Phonemic Decoding Efficiency: PDE). Participants read as many real

15
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

words (SWE) or decodable nonwords (PDE) as they were able to in 45 seconds per subtest.

Internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha coefficients from our sample for SWE (alpha = .97), and

PDE (alpha = .87) were generally consistent with publisher reported alphas for both subtests

(alphas > .90; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2012). Text-level reading accuracy was assessed

with age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3) from the Gray Oral Reading Tests – Fifth

Edition (GORT-5; Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012). Publisher (Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012) reported

Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for GORT-5 Accuracy scores ranged between .85 and .94 in the

normative sample, and .93 in an ASD subgroup.

Linguistic Comprehension. The Recalling Sentences subtest from the Clinical

Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig, Secord, 2003)

provided an age-normed scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3) assessing sentence-level semantic and

syntactic expressive language skills. In order to accurately recall increasingly longer and more

complex sentences, one must strategically utilize language structure (e.g., syntax) and meaning.

Publisher (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003) reported Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients

ranged from .86 - .93 in the normative sample and .97 in an ASD subsample. Expressive

vocabulary was measured with the Vocabulary subtest from the WASI-II (Wechsler, 2011),

which yielded an age-normed T-score (M = 50, SD =10). This subtest was designed to measure

verbal concept formation and semantic knowledge by asking the participant to orally define

words of increasing complexity. The Auditory Reasoning subtest of the Test of Auditory

Processing Skills, Third Edition (TAPS-3; Martin & Brownwell, 2005) provided an age-normed

scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3) assessing higher order linguistic processing related to listening

comprehension, understanding implied meanings and idioms, and to making inferences.

Participants are read short vignettes (approximately 2-3 sentences each) and asked to respond to

16
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

one question for each vignette. In order for an answer to receive credit, a participant must either

make the correct inference, or correctly interpret an abstraction or idiom. Internal consistency

Cronbach’s alpha from our sample for Auditory Reasoning (alpha = .87) was generally

consistent with publisher reported alphas (alphas = .91-.96; Martin & Brownwell, 2005). The

Story Recall subtest of the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning, Second Edition

(WRAML2, Sheslow & Adams, 2003) tapped the ability to listen to and utilize narrative

structure to organize and retell gist and verbatim details of two orally presented narratives and

yielded an age-normed scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3). Internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha

from our sample for Story Recall (alpha = .95) was generally consistent with publisher reported

alphas (alphas = .91-.92; Sheslow & Adams, 2003).

Reading Comprehension. The Gray Oral Reading Tests – Fifth Edition (GORT-5;

Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012) provided a standardized measurement of reading comprehension

that yielded age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3). The individually administered test is

comprised of 16 progressively more difficult reading passages read aloud by the child, each

followed by 5 open-ended comprehension questions given orally by the tester with the passage

removed from view. Question types vary, from those asking for recall of details to those

requiring higher order processing such as synthesis of the main idea, understanding of causal

relations, or ability to make predictions. Publisher (Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012) reported

Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for Comprehension scores range between .90 and .96 in

the normative sample, and .97 in an ASD subsample.

Analytic Strategy

Differentiated Profiles of Reading Skills. All analyses were conducted using Mplus

7.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 - 2015). To answer the first research question, we began by

17
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

iteratively fitting a series of unconditional latent profile analyses beginning with a one-profile

model and increasing the number of profiles by one with each subsequent run. The twelve

reading-related measures (i.e. RAN, NWR, Elision, PDE, SWE, GORT Accuracy, CELF

Recalling Sentences, Expressive Vocabulary, TAPS Auditory Reasoning, WRAML Story Recall,

and GORT Comprehension) were used as latent profile indicators. See Figure 1 for a conceptual

diagram of the full model. As the twelve indicators represented the four broader constructs,

including the indicators simultaneously allows profiles to reflect differences across the four

constructs concomitantly. This analysis also provides an empirical method of deriving reading

profiles as opposed to using relatively arbitrary cutoff scores. Finally, examining the results in

light of the twelve indicators (and, consequently, the four broader constructs) simultaneously

enabled us to identify the greatest discrepancies across constructs among the emergent profiles.

Multiple fit indices were used to compare the models as no single fit index has been

shown to perfectly identify the optimal model (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007). First, we

utilized the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwartz, 1978) and adjusted BIC (ABIC)

with lower values indicating a preferred model. Additionally, we used two likelihood ratio based

indices, the Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) test and the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test (BLRT).

Both tests assess whether adding a profile significantly improves model fit such that a non-

significant p-value for a k-class model indicates the model with k - 1 classes is preferred. For

further information on these three fit indices, see Nylund et al. (2007). Finally, we employed two

information-heuristic indices, the Bayes Factor (BF) and correct model probability (cmP) that

have only recently been applied to mixture modeling (Masyn, 2013). The BF provides pairwise

comparisons of adjacent models that provides a ratio of the probability of a model with k classes

being preferred compared to a model with k + 1 classes. Values between 1 - 3 are weak evidence

18
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

for the k-class model, 3 - 10 are moderate evidence, and values greater than 10 indicate strong

evidence. The cmP provides a probability that each model is preferred compared to all of the

models under consideration. While not considered a fit index, we also examined entropy, which

provides a measure of the strength of classification, with values between .80 and 1.00 or greater

indicative of good classification (Ram & Grimm, 2009). While fit statistics aided us in

identifying a chosen model, we also considered the substantive interpretation of the latent

profiles in each model to ensure the chosen model was theoretically viable (Muthén, 2003).

Linking Reading Profiles to ASD Symptomatology. After choosing the preferred

unconditional model, we examined differences in ASD symptomatology based on latent profile

membership to answer the second research question. This was accomplished by estimating

profile-specific means. This process has been shown to result in a shift in the latent profiles,

thereby altering the substantive interpretation of them (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014a; Nylund-

Gibson, Grimm, Quirk, & Furlong, 2014). Therefore, we implemented the BCH approach

(Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014b; Bakk, Tekle, & Vermunt, 2013; Bolck, Croon, & Hagenaars,

2004; Vermunt, 2010) in order to account for classification error and avoid profile shifts. This

method does so by applying weights to individuals based on posterior probabilities of profile

membership. Finally, the BCH approach estimated profile-specific means of ADOS and

conducted all pairwise comparisons. For technical details of the BCH approach, see Asparouhov

& Muthén (2014b), Bakk, Tekle, & Vermunt (2013), and Bolck, Croon, & Hagenaars (2004).

Results

This section is divided into three subsections reflecting the model building steps. First,

we provide descriptive statistics to compare the present sample to national norms. Next, we

describe the latent profile enumeration process and label and interpret the emergent profiles.

19
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Finally, we present the results of the relation between the reading profiles and ASD

symptomatology severity.

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics, all reported as standard scores can be seen in Table 1. The

descriptive statistics demonstrate that the sample met criteria for ASD on the ADOS-2; measures

of IQ show normal range. A range of scores was seen on the reading related measures. On

average, the overall sample scored at least one standard deviation below the normed mean on all

reading measures. The one exception is the word decoding measure, PDE and SWE, where the

sample scored closer to the normed average of 100. In order to determine if the heterogeneity of

the samples reading abilities, next we conducted a series of latent profile analyses.

Identifying Differentiated Reading Profiles

Fit statistics of the six latent profile models can be seen in Table 2. Values in boldface

indicate the preferred model for a particular fit index. The BIC reached a minimum value at the

4-profile model. However, the only statistically significant LMR value occurred with the 2-

profile model. The BLRT never became non-significant and, thus, was non-informative in

choosing a preferred model. Both the BF and cmP supported the 4-profile model. The entropy

value for the 4-profile model was .90.

While statistical evidence was clear for the 4-profile model, we also examined the profile

plot to ensure theoretical viability. Though the analysis was conducted using age-normed

standardized scores, these were rescaled to z-scores for the profile plot to foster interpretability.

The four profiles were characterized by their performance on the reading and language measures.

The profile plot for the 4-profile model can be seen in Figure 2. The profile demarcated by a

dashed line with square markers was labeled Readers with Comprehension Disturbance and

20
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

accounted for approximately 20% of this sample. These students were characterized by average

rapid automatized naming, phonological awareness, word decoding and word recognition, text

reading accuracy, and expressive vocabulary, alongside low-average phonological memory,

sentence-level syntactic expressive language skills, and story recall. Concomitant deficits in

auditory reasoning/inference, and reading comprehension typified this profile. The profile

demarcated by a dotted line with triangle markers accounted for about one-third of the sample

and was distinguished by poor performance (approximately 1 SD below average) across all

language and reading variables, so we termed this profile Readers with Global Disturbance. The

profile at the bottom of the plot depicted by a solid line with diamond markers accounted for

about 14% of the sample and was marked by very poor performance on all language and reading

variables, this subgroup was called Readers with Severe Global Disturbance. In particular,

RAN, sentence-level syntactic expressive language skills, auditory reasoning/inference, narrative

retelling, and reading comprehension were very low with scores approximately 2 SD or more

below average. The final profile with a solid line with circular markers accounted for about 32%

of the sample and was delineated by scores in the average range on all language and reading

variables; this subgroup is called Average Readers.

We also examined potential differences in age and gender among the emergent profiles

using the three-step method (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014a; Nylund-Gibson, Grimm, Quirk, &

Furlong, 2014). There were no effects of either age or gender. Readers of any age or either

gender were equally likely to be assigned to any of the latent profiles.

Relating Reading Profiles to ASD Symptomatology

The final step in this analysis was to relate ASD symptomatology (i.e. ADOS-2 total

score) to the heterogeneous reading profiles using the BCH approach. Results can be seen in

21
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Figure 3. The Readers with Severe Global Disturbance (M = 14.38) had the highest level of ASD

symptomatology and this was significantly higher than both the Readers with Global

Disturbance (M = 10.15) and Average Readers (M = 9.98) profiles. Readers with

Comprehension Disturbance (M = 11.31) did not significantly differ from any of the other three

profiles and there were no other significant differences across profiles.

Discussion

There is converging evidence that many individuals with ASD demonstrate difficulties

with reading; the majority of previous studies have concentrated specifically on reading

comprehension disturbance (Estes et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2009; Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright,

B., & Williams, C., 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts, Jones, Happé, & Charman, 2013).

There is some evidence that beyond reading comprehension disturbance, there are different

profiles of readers in school-aged children with ASD (e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009;

Nation et al, 2006). In addition, research has delineated language subgroups in children and

adolescents with ASD (Rapin et al., 2009; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2003), and language

impairments have been linked to reading difficulties in this population (Lindgren et al., 2009;

Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). However, the relation between these

language and reading subgroups was previously unexamined using comprehensive reading and

language batteries. In the present study, the first research question probed the heterogeneity of

reading and language performance for individuals with HFASD based upon a comprehensive

battery of assessments of phonological processing, word recognition, and linguistic and reading

comprehension measures. The inclusion of both lower-level reading sub skills that are related to

word recognition and variables related to higher-level linguistic comprehension allowed

22
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

simultaneous consideration of the relation between the two domains outlined by the Simple View

of Reading and their sub skills. Four distinct profiles emerged from the sample of students with

HFASD: Readers with Comprehension Disturbance, Readers with Global Disturbance, Readers

with Severe Global Disturbance and Average Readers. The second research question

investigated the relation between the subgroups of readers and ASD symptomatology in order to

further understand the relation between the social-communicative and cognitive phenotype of

ASD and reading related skills in sample of individuals diagnosed with HFASD.

HFASD Reading Subgroups

The Readers with Comprehension Disturbance typified the poor comprehender or

hyperlexic reading disability profile predicted by the Component Model of Reading. This

subgroup has been frequently reported in prior studies of reading with individuals with ASD

(e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009; Nation et al, 2006; Newman et al., 2007; Huemer &

Mann, 2010; Wei et al., 2015; Zuccarello et al., 2015) and shares characteristics with a language

subgroup reported by Rapin et al. (2009) whose members demonstrated adequate phonology and

vocabulary alongside linguistic comprehension deficits. Grigorenko, Klin, and Volkmar (2003)

noted disagreement in the literature as to whether hyperlexia is synonymous with a reading

comprehension disorder, or whether it is a unique condition characterized by an almost obsessive

interest in letters and words, precocious and unprompted emergence of word decoding, and an

extreme degree of discrepancy between word recognition and other cognitive skills that emerges

between 3 and 5 years of age (Healy, 1982). Individuals in this profile demonstrated strong

phonological awareness, decoding, and word reading skills; it is possible that some of the

children in this group may have been considered hyperlexic earlier in their development. We do

not have data depicting the sample’s early reading development prior to age 8, but even if some

23
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

of the children demonstrated a precocious and circumscribed interest in word reading and

decoding when very young, they are now functioning in the average range, similar to findings

reported by Newman et al. (2007).

Single word expressive vocabulary for children in the Readers with Comprehension

Disturbance profile was in the average range. However, the two measures reported to be

sensitive markers of language impairment, Nonword Repetition and Recalling Sentences

(Condouris, Meyer, & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Rapin, Dunn, Allen,

Stevens, & Fein, 2009), posed a challenge for many of these children. Therefore, in this

subgroup, word recognition abilities did not necessarily align with structural language abilities as

reported in prior studies of reading and language in ASD (Lindgren et al., 2009; Lucas &

Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). Furthermore, children in this profile displayed higher-

level linguistic comprehension deficits ranging from approximately 1 to nearly 2 standard

deviations below average across the auditory reasoning/inference, story recall, and reading

comprehension measures. In summary, while children in this subgroup demonstrated adequate

word recognition skills and single word vocabulary and therefore may appear to be proficient

readers if other sub skills are not assessed, their moderate to profound structural language and

linguistic comprehension difficulties significantly impaired reading for meaning.

Readers with Global Disturbance corresponded with the garden variety poor reader

(Gough & Tunmer, 1986), or mixed reading disability subtype (Catts & Kamhi, 2005), and has

also been reported in prior studies of reading in ASD samples (Asberg & Sandberg, 2012;

Davidson & Weismer, 2014; Gabig, 2010; White et al., 2006; Nation et al., 2006). Children in

this subgroup shared characteristics with a language subgroup reported by Rapin et al. (2009)

who struggled with phonology, vocabulary, and linguistic comprehension. Similarly, Tager-

24
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Flusberg and Joseph (2003) identified an impaired language subtype of children with ASD who

tended to have phonological processing deficits and scores 1-2 SD below the mean on most

language tests. Unlike Readers with Comprehension Disturbance, Readers with Global

Disturbance demonstrated overall low word reading and decoding abilities commensurate with

their poor language skills. Similar to children in the Readers with Global Disturbance, the

Readers with Severe Global Disturbance resembled Rapin et al. (2009) and Tager-Flusberg &

Joseph’s (2003) language impaired subtypes previously described, but with far more severe

impairment. The distinction between the two latter profiles may be thought of as categorically

distinct areas of a continuum, such as the difference between the terms “below average” and “far

below average” that are sometimes used in diagnostic measures. This is consistent with

longitudinal evidence from Pickles et al. (2014) that oral language impairments in ASD present

in parallel patterns of development and proficiency levels after the age of seven. Together these

two subtypes comprised about 47% of the sample.

The individuals in the Average Readers subgroup did not struggle with the reading or

language measures, and in fact exhibited intact reading skills overall. Their performance across

the language measures was similar to that of Rapin et al.’s (2009) subgroup that demonstrated

average or above performance on all language and cognitive measures. Similarly, this subgroup

shared many characteristics with Tager-Flusberg and Joseph’s (2003) description of a group of

children with ASD with normal linguistic abilities who have intact phonological skills, fluency,

syntax and morphology, expressive language, and average to large lexicons. They noted

however, that comprehension may still be impaired at the discourse level, as well as for more

open-ended questions such as “why, when, and how”. Therefore, more complex measures of

reading and linguistic comprehension that require increased demands on cognitive resources,

25
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

inferential thinking, and social knowledge might still pose a challenge for those in the Average

group.

Many of the children in all subgroups performed poorly on the auditory

reasoning/inference measure. This is consistent with Tager-Flusberg and Joseph’s (2003) finding

regarding difficulty with open-ended questions, as well as research indicating that children with

ASD often have difficulties integrating information from background knowledge with that from

the text for global coherence and inference generation (Norbury & Nation, 2011; Wahlberg &

Magliano, 2004). However, some studies have demonstrated that there are aspects of inferencing

which may be preserved in children with ASD such as automatic inference generation between

sentences in very short passages (Saldana & Frith, 2007) and inferring emotions of main

characters in short texts (Tirado & Saldana, 2016). However, Tirado and Saldana (2016) also

found that their participants had difficulty responding to questions about those inferred emotions.

It is possible that for individuals with ASD, there is particular difficulty with a deep

understanding of inferences in situations that are more abstract such as in the context of reading

unknown text, and that these difficulties may be exacerbated in longer texts. This is an important

area to target for explicit instruction.

This study found both similarities and differences compared to the subgroups of neuro-

typical readers reported by Catts et al. (2003). The most prominent difference was that no

dyslexic profile emerged in our study whereas this subgroup made up 35.5% of the poor readers

in their sample. This finding is consistent with previous studies of reading in samples with ASD

(Lindgren et al., 2009; Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). Similar to these prior

studies, poor word reading and decoding in our sample was generally associated with structural

26
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

language difficulties as well as language and reading comprehension impairments, not as a stand-

alone dyslexic profile.

However, both the Readers with Global Disturbance and the Readers with Severe Global

Disturbance profiles resembled the Catts et al. (2003) language-learning disabilities subgroup.

Combining our two Global Disturbance profiles would account for 47.3% of our sample

compared to 35.7% in the Catts et al. sample. Thus, while we identified a similar subgroup, the

prevalence rates differed between the two studies. The differences might be a result of the

younger age (i.e., second grade) used in the Catts et al. (2003) study. However, language delay

and impairment is common in children with ASD (Pickles et al., 2014) and it is probable that

children with ASD who struggle with reading are more typified by impairments in either

language comprehension alone or language comprehension coupled with word reading

difficulties. This could explain the lack of a dyslexic profile along with a greater prevalence of

children who resembled the language-learning disabilities subgroup in Catts et al. (2003).

Relation of Reading Profiles to ASD Symptom Severity


Previous research has provided evidence that reading comprehension is negatively

associated with ASD diagnosis and symptom severity (Asberg et al., 2010; Estes et al., 2011;

Jones et al., 2009; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Results of this study are

consistent with these previous findings: reading comprehension scores were highest when ASD

symptomatology as measured by the ADOS-2 was lowest. Readers with Severe Global

Disturbance, who demonstrated the poorest linguistic and reading comprehension abilities, had

significantly higher levels of ASD symptomatology than children in the Average Reader and

Global Disturbance subgroups. They also struggled the most with sight word recognition and

text reading accuracy, consistent with individuals referred to in the typically developing

27
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

literature as having a language-learning disability (LLD; Berninger & May, 2011). The current

study provides additional evidence that the social communicative and cognitive phenotype of

ASD impacts both linguistic and reading comprehension for many students with ASD throughout

the school-age years.

Potential Implications for Treatment of Reading Disturbance

The majority of students with HFASD are educated in general education classrooms.

Extant data suggests that these students are being underserved in these settings in the area of

reading development, with many of them scoring at least one grade level below their typically

developing peers on reading assessments. In this sample of higher-functioning children with

ASD, 65% of the students were in general education classes 81-100% of the day, and an

additional 12% were in these classes 41-80% of the day, yet almost 68% of the students

demonstrated various profiles of moderate to severe language and reading difficulties.

Furthermore, these profiles were related to the severity of social communication and cognitive

characteristics associated with ASD. This has important implications for educating students with

an ASD diagnosis, particularly in socially-mediated, language-based learning contexts. It is

difficult to expect general education teachers to know how to meet the reading instructional

needs of individuals with HFASD when very little is known about the development of the sub

skills necessary for successful comprehension in this population of students.

In order to address the unique instructional needs of students with ASD, and to be able to

develop the most effective reading intervention protocols, a more in-depth investigation into

reading profiles in this population was necessary. The results of this study demonstrate that

assessment and intervention methods must be tailored to meet the specific reading needs of

individual students, and the specific skill deficits depicted in these profiles can be addressed.

28
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Thorough assessment of both word recognition and linguistic comprehension sub skills is

important; for example, average single word expressive vocabulary was higher for all subgroups

than was auditory reasoning ability and an overreliance on vocabulary skill level could lead to

overlooking a key domain for intervention. When planning intervention, students in the Readers

with Comprehension Disturbance subgroup, who demonstrate dissociation between word

recognition skills and comprehension, would benefit from explicit structural language

intervention and linguistic comprehension instruction. However, students in the Severe Global

and Global Disturbance subgroups would benefit from explicit phonological processing, word

recognition, and linguistic comprehension intervention. The Severe Global Disturbance

subgroup would likely benefit from a much more intense intervention in these areas and may

require additional behavioral scaffolding to sufficiently engage with the intervention. These

types of targeted interventions could be implemented through collaborations with various school

professionals including reading specialists, speech and language pathologists, and special

education or general education teachers.

Conclusions

The proportion of individuals with reading disturbance has been shown repeatedly to be

greater in samples of individuals with ASD than in the general population. The data in the

present study concurred with previous literature that a large percentage of individuals with ASD

demonstrate reading disturbance and that this disturbance is associated with language

impairments. Furthermore, this study provided additional evidence that phonological awareness

is associated with word decoding for school-aged children with ASD, as is seen in typically

developing samples, and word recognition deficits were concomitant with language deficits. It

has also been argued and shown empirically that there is a significant relation between the

29
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

social-communicative and cognitive phenotype of ASD and reading performance. The present

study demonstrates support for this finding in a much more specific way, by showing that ASD

symptom severity is related differentially to specific profiles of readers.

Limitations and Future Directions


A limitation of the current study is that while the sample size was relatively large, the

developmental span across elementary and secondary school years was extensive. Future studies

would benefit from even larger samples at each age and grade level to more fully understand

reading profiles in students with ASD. Future studies would also benefit from data collection

with students with a broader range of ASD severity, as this study only included individuals with

HFASD. We would also suggest that text reading fluency be collected as a part of future reading

batteries as it is possible that while this sample showed relative strength on word reading, it may

not translate to fluent reading of connected text. In addition to text reading, we also note

limitations related to the language measures used in this study. In the future, we would suggest

collecting more robust measures of language development in order to gain a better understanding

of the role language plays in reading comprehension. Another limitation of this study is that the

standardized measures used may not have been robust enough to adequately capture the extent of

higher-level linguistic and reading comprehension challenges. A future study would benefit

from the inclusion of additional reading and language comprehension measures that are more

complex and would demand more cognitive resources, inferential thinking, narrative retelling,

and social knowledge. Longer texts, both fiction and nonfiction, that are similar to those used in

classrooms, as well as other genres such as persuasive essays or satire might also uncover

additional targets for intervention even for those in the Average Reader group. Longitudinal

studies would also contribute further to our understanding of patterns of subgroup membership

30
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

and how they may change with intervention and maturation. Finally, future investigations are

needed to further probe the specific aspects of ASD symptomatology that are associated with

reading and language-based learning in structured, multifaceted social contexts such as

classrooms in order to develop effective interventions for school-aged children with ASD.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

This study was funded by NIMH 1RO1MH085904, IES R324A110174, and the UC Davis

Department of Psychiatry, Lisa Capps Endowment for Research on Neurodevelopmental

Disorders (P. Mundy, PI).

Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in

accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and

with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in

the study.

31
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

References

Aaron, P. G. (1997). The impending demise of the discrepancy formula. Review of Educational
Research, 67(4), 461-502.

Aaron, P. G., Joshi, R. M., Gooden, R., & Bentum, K. E. (2008). Diagnosis and treatment of
reading disabilities based on the component model of reading an alternative to the discrepancy
model of LD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(1), 67-84.

Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second versus eighth
grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 332-345.

Åsberg, J., Kopp, S., Berg-Kelly, K., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Reading comprehension, word
decoding and spelling in girls with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) or attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD): Performance and predictors. International Journal of
Language & Communication Disorders, 45(1), 61-71.

Åsberg, J., & Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2012). Dyslexic, delayed, precocious or just normal?
Word reading skills of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Research in Reading,
35(1), 20-31.

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014a). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step
approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3),
329-341. doi:10/1080/10705511.2014.915181

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014b). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Using the
BCH method in Mplus to estimate a distal outcome model and an arbitrary secondary model.
Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/examples/webnotes/webnote21.pdf

Bakk, Z., Tekle, F.B., & Vermunt, J.K. (2013). Estimating the association between latent class
membership and external variables using bias-adjusted three-step approaches. Sociological
Methodology, 43, 272-311. doi: 10.1177/0081175012470644

Beglinger, L. J., & Smith, T. H. (2001). A review of subtyping in autism and proposed
dimensional classification model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 411-
422.

Beglinger, L., & Smith, T. (2005). Concurrent validity of social subtype and IQ after early
intensive behavioral intervention in children with autism: A preliminary investigation. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 295-303.

Berninger, V. W., & May, M., (2011). Evidence-based diagnosis and treatment for specific
learning disabilities involving impairments in written and/or oral language. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 44(2), 167-183.

32
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Biemiller, A. (2007). The influence of vocabulary on reading acquisition. Encyclopedia of


Language and Literacy Development, 1-10.

Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44.

Bolck, A., Croon, M., & Hagenaars, J. (2004). Estimating latent structure models with
categorical variables: One-step versus three-step estimators. Political Analysis, 12, 3-27.
doi: 10.1093/pan/mph001

Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and… fork”: Examining central coherence
in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development with a linguistic task.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 377-393.

Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal
connection. Nature.

Brown, H. M., Oram-Cardy, J., & Johnson, A. (2013). A meta-analysis of the reading
comprehension skills of individuals on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 932-955.

Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Bindman, M. (1998). Awareness of language in children who have
reading difficulties: Historical comparisons in a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 39(4), 501-510.

Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension
difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683-696.

Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (Eds.). (2008). Children's comprehension problems in oral and written
language: A cognitive perspective. New York: Guilford Press.

Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2003). The effects of morphological structure on children’s
reading of derived words in English. In Reading Complex Words (pp. 27-52). Springer US.

Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. E. (2003). Subgrouping poor readers on the basis of
individual differences in reading-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(2), 151-
164.

Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (1999). Causes of reading disabilities. Language and Reading
Disabilities, 95-127.

Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (2005). Classification of reading disabilities. Language and
Reading Disabilities, 2, 72-93.

33
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters
in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13(2), 195-212. doi:10.1007/BF01246098

Charman, T., & Gotham, K. (2013). Measurement issues: Screening and diagnostic instruments
for autism spectrum disorders–lessons from research and practise. Child and Adolescent Mental
Health, 18(1), 52-63.

Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Gottardo, A. (2004). Vocabulary, context, and speech perception
among good and poor readers. Educational Psychology, 24(6), 825-843.

Christensen, D. L. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among


children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites,
United States, 2012. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries, 65.

Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized
measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349-358.

Constantino, J. & Gruber, C. (2005). Social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western
Psychological Services.

Cutting, L., & Scarborough, H. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative


contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on
how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 277-299.

de Bruin, C. L., Deppeler, J. M., Moore, D. W., & Diamond, N. T. (2013). Public school-based
interventions for adolescents and young adults with an autism spectrum disorder: A meta-
analysis. Review of Educational Research, 83(4), 521-550.

Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Penguin.

Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-
functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of abnormal child psychology,
34(1), 83-98.

Dingfelder, H. E., & Mandell, D. S. (2011). Bridging the research-to-practice gap in autism
intervention: An application of diffusion of innovation theory. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 597-609.

Ehlers, S., Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). A screening questionnaire for Asperger syndrome
and other high functioning autism spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 29, 129–140.

34
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Estes, A., Rivera, V., Bryan, M., Cali, P., & Dawson, G. (2011). Discrepancies between
academic achievement and intellectual ability in higher-functioning school-aged children with
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1044-1052.

Fleury, V. P., Hedges, S., Hume, K., Browder, D. M., Thompson, J. L., Fallin, K., ... & Vaughn,
S. (2014). Addressing the academic needs of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in
secondary education. Remedial and Special Education, 35(2), 68-79.

Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. Surface Dyslexia, 32, 301-330.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal reading comprehension
measures. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2), 20-28.

Gabig, C. S. (2010). Phonological awareness and word recognition in reading by children with
autism. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(2), 67-85.

Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Boyle, M., Hanna, S., Duku, E., Zwaigenbaum, L., ... & Smith, I.
(2013). Investigating phenotypic heterogeneity in children with autism spectrum disorder: a
factor mixture modeling approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 206-215.

Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are
executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 836-854.

Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Lawrence
Erlbaum.

Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and
Special Education, 7(1), 6-10.

Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Annotation: Hyperlexia: Disability or
superability? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8), 1079-1091.

Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for measuring and understanding individual
differences in the component processes of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93(1), 103.

Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism.
Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218-1220.

Healy, J. M. (1982). The enigma of hyperlexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 319-338.

Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: Revised
algorithm and standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
44(8), 1996-2012.

35
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Huemer, S. V., & Mann, V. (2010). A comprehensive profile of decoding and comprehension in
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 485-493.

Insel, T. R. (2014). The NIMH research domain criteria (RDoC) project: precision medicine for
psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry.

Jacobs, D. W., & Richdale, A. L. (2013). Predicting literacy in children with a high-functioning
autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(8), 2379-2390.

Jones, C. R., Happé, F., Golden, H., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Simonoff, E., ... & Charman, T.
(2009). Reading and arithmetic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Peaks and dips in
attainment. Neuropsychology, 23(6), 718.

Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of reading
made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21(2), 85-97.

Kasari, C., & Smith, T. (2013). Interventions in schools for children with autism spectrum
disorder: Methods and recommendations. Autism, 17(3), 254-267.

Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-


integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163.

Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness
as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 453.

Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E., & Angley, M. T. (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in
autism: Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
40(1), 112-122.

Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (2009). 19 Formal and Informal Measures of Reading Comprehension.
Handbook of research on reading comprehension, 403.

Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and
reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment
and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2(1), 22-38.

Long, D. & Lea, R. (2005). Have we been searching for meaning in all the wrong places?
Defining the “search after meaning” principle in comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39(2&3),
279-298.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic
observation schedule, second edition. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

36
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Lucas, R. & Norbury, C. (2014). Levels of text comprehension in children with autism spectrum
disorders (ASD): The influence of language phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 44(11), 2756-2768.

Lucas, R. & Norbury, C (2015). Making inferences from text: It’s vocabulary that matters.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1224-1232.

Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M. F., Beretvas, N., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Sorrells, A., ... &
Rispoli, M. (2008). A review of school-based instructional interventions for students with autism
spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 395-416.

Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and
orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325-333.

Martin, N. & Brownwell, R. (2005). Test of auditory processing skills - third edition. Novato,
CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

Masyn, K. (2013). Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. The Oxford handbook of
quantitative methods in psychology, 2, 551-611.

McNamara, D. S. (2001). Reading both high-coherence and low-coherence texts: Effects of text
sequence and prior knowledge. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue
canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 55(1), 51.

Miles, J. H., Takahashi, T. N., Bagby, S., Sahota, P. K., Vaslow, D. F., Wang, C. H., ... &
Farmer, J. E. (2005). Essential versus complex autism: definition of fundamental prognostic
subtypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 135(2), 171-180.

Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary,
and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: evidence from a
longitudinal study. Developmental psychology, 40(5), 665.

Muthén, B. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth mixture modeling: comment
on Bauer and Curran (2003).

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén
& Muthén.

Nation, K. (2009). Reading comprehension and vocabulary. Beyond decoding, 176-194.

Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in
children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment?
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 199-211.

37
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright, B., & Williams, C. (2006). Patterns of reading ability in children
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(7), 911-919.

Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills
contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342-356.

National Reading Panel (US), National Institute of Child Health, & Human Development (US).
(2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction: Reports of the subgroups. National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, National Institutes of Health.

Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Naples, A. J., Babitz, T., Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L.
(2007). Hyperlexia in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 760-774.

Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in


adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding skill.
Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 191-210.

Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in
latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural
Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535-569.

Nylund-Gibson, K., Grimm, R., Quirk, M., & Furlong, M. (2014). A latent transition mixture
model using the three-step specification. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary
Journal, 21(3), 439-454. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915375

Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2010). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and
visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23(2), 189-208.

Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict
developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530.

Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of


Reading, 11(4), 357-383.

Pickles, A., Anderson, D. K., & Lord, C. (2014). Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development
of language: a 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(12), 1354-1362.

Ram, N. & Grimm, K.J. (2009). Growth mixture modeling: A method for identifying differences
in longitudinal change among unobserved groups. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 33, 565-576. doi: 10.1177/0165025409343765

38
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Rapin, I., Dunn, M.A., Allen, D. A., Stevens, M. C., & Fein, D. (2009). Subtypes of language
disorders in school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 66-84.

Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on
nonword reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(2), 277-282.

Reutebuch, C. K., El Zein, F., Kim, M. K., Weinberg, A.N., & Vaughn, S. (2015). Investigating
a reading comprehension intervention for high school students with autism spectrum disorder: a
pilot study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 9, 96-111.

Rey, A., Ziegler, J. C., & Jacobs, A. M. (2000). Graphemes are perceptual reading units.
Cognition, 75(1), B1-B12.

Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism
spectrum disorders: visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238-248.

Ricketts, J., Jones, C. R., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2013). Reading comprehension in autism
spectrum disorders: The role of oral language and social functioning. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 807-816.

Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all
reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 235-257.

Roth, L., Lai, S., White, B., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Orthographic and morphological processing
as predictors of reading achievement. In annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of
Reading, Vancouver, BC.

Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection
between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95(5), 259-272.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire. Los
Angeles, CA : Western Psychological Services.

Saldaña, D. & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make bridging inferences from world
knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 310-319.

Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, fourth
edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, a Harcourt Assessment Company.

Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive
role of early vocabulary to future reading. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 2, 173-182.

Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading
acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151-218.

39
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Sheslow, D. & Adams, W. (2003). Wide range assessment of memory and learning, second
edition: Administration and technical manual. Wilmington, Del: Wide Range, Inc.

Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming,
phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of
Educational Research, 73(4), 407-440.

Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience


Research, 6(3), 219-224.

Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism.


Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430),
303-314.

Tirado, M. J., & Saldaña, D. (2016). Readers with autism can produce inferences, but they
cannot answer inferential questions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3),
1025-1037.

Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (2012, 1999). Test of word reading efficiency, second
edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed Inc.

Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension (pp. 11-12). New
York: Academic Press.

Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent Class Modeling with Covariates: Two Improved+ Three-Step
Approaches. Political analysis, mpq025.

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related


phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 73.

Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological
processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed Inc.

Wahlberg, T., & Magliano, J. P. (2004). The ability of high function individuals with autism to
comprehend written discourse. Discourse Processes, 38(1), 119-144.

Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, second edition. San Antonio,
TX: NCS Pearson.

Wiederholt, J. & Bryant, B. (2012). Gray oral reading tests – fifth edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

40
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Wei, X., Christiano, E. R., Jennifer, W. Y., Wagner, M., & Spiker, D. (2014). Reading and math
achievement profiles and longitudinal growth trajectories of children with an autism spectrum
disorder. Autism, 1362361313516549.

White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., & Ramus, F. (2006). A double
dissociation between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability: A comparison of autistic
and dyslexic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(5), 748-761.

Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile of memory function in
children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 21.

Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated
abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 11-29.

Zuccarello, R., Di Blasi, F. D., Zingale, M., Panerai, S., Finocchiaro, M., Trubia, G., ... &
Zoccolotti, P. (2015). Reading decoding and comprehension in children with autism spectrum
disorders: Evidence from a language with regular orthography. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 17, 126-134.

41
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Tables & Figures

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics for the Full Sample

Measure M SD Range
PP and RAN
RANc 85.68 21.92 1-145
NWRa 7.50 2.15 1-13
Elisiona 9.94 3.08 1-15
Word Recognition
PDEc 94.89 14.81 58-127
SWEc 93.29 14.75 57-136
GORTAcca 8.03 2.69 2-16
Linguistic Comprehension
CELFrsa 7.36 3.15
Evocabb 46.96 9.89 24-69
AudReasa 6.04 2.77 1-11
StryReca 7.94 3.31 1-15
Reading Comprehension
GORTCompa 7.37 2.61 1-13
ASD Symptomatology
ADOS-2 Total 10.94 3.65 7-24
IQ
FIQc 100.00 14.00 76-132
VIQc 96.00 15.00 60-136
PIQc 105.00 16.00 71-150
Age 11.24 2.19 8-16
Note. PP = Phonological Processing; RAN = Rapid Automatized
Naming; NWR = Nonword Repetition; PDE = Phonemic Decoding
Efficiency; SWE = Sight Word Efficiency; GORTAcc = GORT text
accuracy; CELFrs = CELF Recalling Sentences; EVocab =
Expressive Vocabulary; AudReas = Auditory Reasoning; StryRec =
Story Recall; GORTComp = GORT reading comprehension.
a
Scaled score, M=10, SD = 3. bT-score, M = 50, SD = 10.
c
Standard score, M = 100, SD = 15.

42
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Table 2

Fit Statistics of the Six LPA Models

LMR BLRT
Profile LL BIC ABIC p-value p-value BF cmP Min n
1 -2687.81 5472.31 5402.93 - - <.001 <.001 -
2 -2580.87 5311.15 5203.92 0.006 <.001 1.56 0.17 34
3 -2554.94 5312.03 5166.96 0.683 <.001 0.16 0.11 13
4 -2526.75 5308.38 5125.46 0.181 <.001 34.81 0.69 11
5 -2503.93 5315.48 5094.72 0.37 <.001 - 0.02 10
6 Did not converge
Note. LL = Log-likelihood; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; ABIC = Adjusted BIC; LMR = Lo-
Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test; BLRT = Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test; BF = Bayes Factor;
cmP = Correct Model Probability.

43
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

Table 3

Profile-specific Means (Standard Errors) of All Indicator Variables

Severe Global Comprehension Global


Measure Disturbance Disturbance Disturbance Average Readers
PP and RAN
RANc 71.70 (8.12) 96.79 (4.39) 80.24 (4.50) 90.31 (4.51)
NWRa 6.25 (0.54) 7.60 (0.50) 7.22 (0.44) 8.27 (0.45)
Elisiona 7.58 (1.24) 11.93 (0.78) 8.34 (0.79) 11.35 (0.38)
Word Recognition
PDEc 86.20 (4.64) 105.81 (3.99) 82.33 (3.32) 104.89 (2.09)
SWEc 76.70 (3.37) 101.22 (3.95) 86.64 (2.98) 102.66 (2.60)
GORTAcca 5.61 (0.70) 8.56 (0.66) 6.63 (0.30) 10.26 (0.58)
Linguistic
Comprehension
CELFrsa 2.67 (0.75) 7.18 (0.53) 6.74 (0.54) 10.28 (0.41)
EVocabb 35.36 (2.56) 47.22 (2.48) 42.94 (1.39) 55.74 (1.53)
AudReasa 2.75 (0.53) 4.66 (0.97) 6.71 (0.67) 7.67 (0.47)
StryReca 3.78 (0.76) 7.40 (1.05) 8.00 (0.63) 9.86 (0.58)
Reading
Comprehension
GORTCompa 3.50 (0.45) 5.85 (0.64) 7.42 (0.26) 10.00 (0.40)
Note. PP = Phonological Processing; RAN = Rapid Automatized Naming; NWR = Nonword Repetition; PDE = Phonemic Decoding
Efficiency; SWE = Sight Word Efficiency; GORTAcc = GORT text accuracy; CELFrs = CELF Recalling Sentences; EVocab =
Expressive Vocabulary; AudReas = Auditory Reasoning; StryRec = Story Recall; GORTComp = GORT reading comprehension.
a
Scaled score, M=10, SD = 3. bT-score, M = 50, SD = 10. cStandard score, M = 100, SD = 15.

44
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

PP and RAN Word/Text Linguistic Reading


Recognition Comprehension Comprehension

Reading
Profiles ADOS

Figure 1. Conceptual diagram of heterogeneous reading profiles and relation to ASD symptomatology.

45
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

0.5

-0.5

-1

-1.5

-2

-2.5

-3

c
E

cc

ab
R

rs
N

on

as

p
Re

m
PD

SW
W

LF
RA

Re
A

oc
isi

Co
ry
N

RT

EV
CE
El

ud

St

RT
O

A
G

O
G
Severe Global Disturbance (14.1%) Comprehension Disturbance (20.6%)
Global Disturbance (33.2%) Average Readers (32.1%)

Figure 2. Profile plot based on reading measures. PP = Phonological Processing; RAN = Rapid Automatized Naming; NWR = Nonword Repetition; PDE =
Phonemic Decoding Efficiency; SWE = Sight Word Efficiency; GORTAcc = GORT text accuracy; CELFrs = CELF Recalling Sentences; EVocab = Expressive
Vocabulary; AudReas = Auditory Reasoning; StryRec = Story Recall; GORTComp = GORT reading comprehension.

46
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD

*
16
*
14

12 14.38
(5.54)
10
ADOS Mean

11.31
8 (2.99)
10.15 9.98
6 (3.32) (2.69)

0
Severe Global Comprehension Global Disturbance Average Readers
Disturbance Disturbance
Reading Profile

Figure 3. Means (standard deviations) of ADOS-2 scores by reading profile. An asterisk indicates significantly different (p < .05)
ADOS-2 means between a given pair of reading profiles.

47

You might also like