ED586106
ED586106
ED586106
Autism: Distinct Reading Profiles and their Relation to Autism Symptom Severity
a
School of Education, One Shields Avenue, University of California, Davis, 95616, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Lowell, USA
Abstract
The goal of this study was to identify unique profiles of readers in a sample of 8-16-year olds
with higher functioning autism spectrum disorders (HFASD) and examine the profiles in relation
to ASD symptom severity. Eighty-one students were assessed utilizing a comprehensive reading
battery that included basic word reading, language, and comprehension. Using Latent Profile
Analysis, four empirically distinct profiles of readers emerged. Next, using the Autism
Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012), analyses were
with HFASD and significant differences between the reading profiles and ASD symptom
severity.
2
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Current science emphasizes the need to not only distinguish groups of children affected
by autism spectrum disorders (ASD) from comparison children but also to investigate the nature
Happe, Ronald & Plomin, 2006). The expectation and need to understand psychological
heterogeneity is especially true for older children because as cognitive, language, and emotional
processes develop and differentiate with age it is likely that there are greater degrees of freedom
for the expression of heterogeneity. There are many approaches to studying heterogeneity in
ASD including, for example, the study of differences in social engagement (Wing & Gould,
1979), differences in social attention (Rice, Moriuchi, Jones & Klin, 2012), differences in
executive function (Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2003; Geurts, Verte, Oosterlaan, Roeyers, &
Sergeant, 2004), sensory processing (Lane, Young, Baker, & Angley, 2010), and language
(Pickles, Anderson, & Lord, 2014; Rapin, Dunn, Allen, Stevens & Fein, 2009; Tager-Flusberg,
2006), as well as multidimensional approaches (Beglinger & Smith, 2001; Insel, 2014).
There are many reasons why understanding heterogeneity in ASD is of great interest.
One of these is that the identification of valid subgroups holds the promise of enabling a more
precise alignment of treatments and educational plans for affected individuals with ASD
(Beglinger & Smith, 2005; Miles et al. 2005). This is an especially important consideration for
school-aged children who begin to experience their kindergarten through 12th grade classrooms
as their primary venue for intervention. Currently we have very little information on the
heterogeneity of ASD that informs and advances contemporary educational practices for
elementary and secondary students with ASD (Dingfelder & Mandell, 2011; Kasari & Smith,
2013; Machalicek et al., 2008). This is particularly true for higher functioning children for
3
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
whom very little evidence-based information is available to guide optimal education in inclusive
regular education classrooms (Machalicek et al., 2008). This is unfortunate for two reasons.
First, current epidemiological data not only indicate that 1 in 66 children in second grade
throughout the nation are affected by ASD, but 47% of these children have average to above
average intellectual ability and 25% have borderline IQ. Only 28% of these second grade
children are affected by intellectual disabilities (Christensen et al., 2016). Many school-aged
children with ASD function in a range of intelligence that allows them to receive their education
in regular education classrooms (de Bruin, Deppeler, Moore, & Diamond, 2013; Fleury et al.,
2014), which would suggest that these students should gain benefit from general education
Second, although children with higher functioning ASD (HFASD) are capable of
receiving their education in regular education classrooms, they are at risk for academic
difficulties. One specific difficulty that has been empirically demonstrated in the literature is in
reading; these difficulties share similarities to students identified as reading disabled. Multiple
are able to fluently decode words, yet have difficulties understanding the meaning of written text,
in 33 - 65% of the samples (Estes, Rivera, Bryan, Cali, & Dawson, 2011; Jones et al., 2009;
Nation, Clarke, Wright, & Williams, 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts, Jones, Happé, &
Charman, 2013). Furthermore, poor reading abilities have been shown to be substantially
discrepant from IQ in many children, leading to the concern that many students are
underachieving (Jones et al., 2009). This provides evidence that current school-based reading
instruction does not sufficiently prevent negative reading comprehension outcomes for children
with HFASD. Less is known about the development of basic word recognition skills in students
4
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
with HFASD. Some studies have suggested that word decoding or word reading is not a specific
deficit that is more prevalent in this population of students (e.g. Brown, Oram-Cardy, & Johnson,
2013), however, it has not been thoroughly examined with a comprehensive word recognition
known about reading comprehension disability, and including the development of word
recognition, it may be possible to develop more targeted methods of instruction for this
population (Reutebuch, El Zein, Kim, Weinberg, & Vaughn, 2015). In this study, we extend the
current knowledge about the reading development and disability in students with HFASD in
three ways. First, we provide a deeper understanding of reading impairments in this sample,
strengths and weaknesses can be identified in a sample of students with HFASD. Third, we
investigate the relation between HFASD reading subgroups and ASD symptomatology.
Reading for meaning develops over time and builds upon two brain regions already
present in infancy: the visual object recognition and oral language systems (Dehaene, 2009). By
the time children are 5 or 6 years old, key visual recognition processes are well developed but
still maximally plastic. Children’s vocabulary grows 10-20 words per day by the end of their
second year, and by the time they are 6 years old, most have expert knowledge of phonology,
basic grammar rules, and a vocabulary of several thousand words. In the phonological stage of
reading (Frith, 1985), children develop letter-sound correspondence requiring proficient letter
5
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
recognition skills and phonological awareness, or the ability to discern individual speech sounds.
They learn to decode words, progressing from the simple to the complex. Morphemic awareness
develops as well, and children learn that prefixes, root words, and suffixes are associated with
pronunciation and meaning. In the orthographic stage (Frith, 1985), the lexical pathway used to
pathway. Oral language processing creates meaning from the words. These two processes may
develop relatively independently (Adlof, Catts, & Lee, 2010) and the relationship between these
factors and reading comprehension changes over time. Cain & Oakhill (2008) noted that for
younger children decoding is more important and the correlation between reading and listening
comprehension is low. By high school however, decoding differences are generally small and
the correlation between reading and listening comprehension is high. Therefore, they posited
that based on this model one should expect a person’s reading comprehension to develop to the
The Simple View of Reading describes successful reading comprehension as the result of
sufficient decoding and linguistic comprehension skills (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). These two
component skills are described as multiplicative in nature and therefore both sets of skills must
Reading (Joshi & Aaron, 2000), based upon the Simple View, incorporated processing speed, as
measured by speed of letter naming, as a third predictor of reading comprehension. Prior studies
have used the Component Model to investigate how poor readers may fall into subgroups that
differ across the components of word decoding and linguistic comprehension (Aaron, 1997;
Catts, Hogan & Fey, 2003; Catts & Kamhi, 1999; Gough & Tunmer, 1986). At least three
subgroups of poor readers are predicted by this model: (a) poor readers with word recognition
6
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
problems only (e.g. dyslexics), (b) poor readers with linguistic comprehension problems only
(e.g. poor comprehenders and/or hyperlexics), and (c) poor readers with difficulties in both
components [e.g. garden variety poor readers (Gough & Tunmer, 1986), mixed reading disabled
(Catts & Kamhi, 2005), or language-learning disabled (LLD; Berninger & May, 2011)].
Furthermore, Aaron, Joshi, Gooden, and Bentum (2008) demonstrated the utility of the
Component Model in identifying the facet(s) of reading that is(are) the source of a child’s
A deeper examination of the two component skills, word recognition and linguistic
comprehension, reveals many essential sub skills. For example, poor readers who struggle with
processing, or the processing of speech sounds. There is empirical evidence that demonstrates a
correlational relation between facets of phonological processing and word recognition including:
phonological awareness (Bradley & Bryant. 1983; Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Share, 1995;
Swanson, Trainin, Necoechea, & Hammill, 2003; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994),
phonological decoding (Rastle & Coltheart, 1998; Rey, Ziegler, & Jacobs, 2000; Swanson et al,
2003), and rapid automatized naming (Kirby, Parrila, & Pfeiffer, 2003; Manis, Doi, & Bhada,
2000; Swanson et al, 2003). Vocabulary development has also been linked with word
recognition skill (Biemiller, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Chiappe, Chiappe & Gottardo,
2004; Nation, 2009; National Reading Panel, 2000; Ouellette & Beers, 2010; Perfetti, 2007).
Linguistic comprehension, or the oral language processing that creates meaning from
words, has a profound effect on the comprehension of written texts (e.g., Nation & Snowling,
2004; Roth, Speece, & Cooper, 2002). Empirical evidence suggests that higher-level linguistic
comprehension skills are underpinned by the depth and breadth of one’s vocabulary (Ouellette &
7
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Beers, 2010; Perfetti, 2007; Ricketts, Nation & Bishop, 2007; Roth, Speece & Cooper, 2002;
Senechal, Ouellette, & Rodney, 2006), syntax and grammar (Cain & Oakhill, 2006; Muter,
Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004; Nation, Clarke, Marshall, & Durand, 2004), verbal
(Hannon & Daneman, 2001; Long & Lea, 2005; McNamara, 2001), and narrative recall (Fuchs,
Fuchs, & Maxwell, 1988; Leslie & Caldwell, 2009). All of these facets of oral language support
the construction of a globally coherent situation model of a text; semantic, grammatical and
syntactic information provide the foundation of the text-based mental model, then continuous
connections between prior knowledge, inferences, and text ideas are made to create the situation
model required for proficient reading comprehension (Kintsch, 1988; Van Dijk & Kintsch,
1983).
Empirical evidence exists to demonstrate that both word recognition and linguistic
comprehension account for substantial unique variance in reading comprehension for children
with ASD, supporting the Simple View in this population (Jones et al., 2009; Lindgren, Folstein,
Tomblin, & Tager-Flusberg, 2009; Nation et al., 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al.,
2013). Similar to the subtypes described by Catts et al. (2003) with a typically developing
reading sample, many samples with ASD have displayed profiles comparable to poor
language and reading comprehension (e.g. Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009; Nation et al,
2006; Newman et al., 2007; Huemer & Mann, 2010; Wei, Christiano, Yu, Wagner & Spiker,
Other researchers have reported evidence that subgroups of children with poor
8
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
word decoding deficits (Asberg & Sandberg, 2012; Nation et al., 2006; White et al., 2006).
White et al. (2006) found that, similar to those with typical development, phonological skills
were a strong predictor of word recognition and spelling in 8-12-year-olds with ASD. However,
Gabig (2010) found that while phonological awareness was delayed in development for 5-7-year
olds with ASD in their sample, it was not significantly related to word reading or decoding, but it
was significantly correlated with receptive vocabulary. Similarly, in several other studies, word
recognition skills have been shown to correlate with language abilities, reporting subgroups of
children with poor word recognition associated with poor oral language skills (e.g. Brown et al.,
2013; Jacobs & Richdale, 2013; Lindgren et al., 2009; Nation et al., 2006; Norbury & Nation,
2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Three studies have reported that children with ASD who have age-
appropriate language skills scored significantly higher than those with language impairments on
al., 2009; Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). None of the studies reviewed
reported a subgroup of children displaying a dyslexic profile, or one in which impaired word
factors in more detail. For example, participants with ASD have been shown to have difficulty
integrating background knowledge and inferred knowledge explicitly with global text (Saldana
& Frith, 2007), using background knowledge to interpret and remember specific information or
resolve ambiguities in discourse (Wahlberg & Magliano, 2004), or responding to questions about
inferred emotions (Tirado & Saldana, 2016). Language impairment in adolescents with ASD
was associated with poorer performance on a passage-level inference measure (Norbury &
Nation, 2011), and in elementary school-aged children verbal ability was the strongest predictor
9
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Norbury & Nation (2011) suggested that difficulties integrating information from different
sources for global coherence and inference generation might be highly dependent on variance in
Reading for meaning is fundamental for accessing social, cultural, and political milieus
through written documents, and is a cognitively complex process. Current research suggests that
the risk for reading comprehension disability may be related to ASD symptomatology and be a
ASD; several studies have reported significant associations between individual differences in
reading development and diagnostic status, social functioning, or autistic symptom severity in
samples of school-aged children with ASD (Asberg, Kopp, Berg-Kelly, & Gillberg, 2010; Estes
et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2009; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Reading is a
written form of communication between the author and the reader, and as such, is likely to be
intentions or purpose for writing a text, which impedes learning from the text. Furthermore,
impeding the development of rich networks of semantic and episodic knowledge typically
norms may lead to difficulty developing skills that rely on social knowledge such as
elements.
10
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
The cognitive characteristics of many children with ASD include the tendency to focus
on details rather than global meaning (Booth & Happe, 2010), leading to particular problems
generating global coherence or processing at the gist level across a text (e.g., Pellicano, 2010),
which in turn leads to difficulty recalling, retelling, and comprehending stories (Diehl, Bennetto,
& Young, 2006; Williams, Goldstein, & Minshew, 2006). Furthermore, this local processing
bias, or weak central coherence (Happe & Frith, 2006), has been posited to lead to particular
difficulty integrating information both from the text and from background knowledge for
inference generation (Norbury & Nation, 2011) and global comprehension (Ricketts et al., 2013).
Another cognitive characteristic associated with ASD is the tendency to have restricted or fixated
interests, and this can limit exposure to situations where individuals learn about a wide variety of
topics and develop oral language skills across multiple contexts. This restricts vocabulary growth
except in fields of specific interests, and leads to more literal, less flexible understanding of
words and phrases. Combined, these difficulties constrain creation of a coherent mental model of
text that draws on a reader’s ability to combine text-based information with relevant background
knowledge to generate inferences about things not explicitly stated in the text (Kintsch, 1988;
McNamara, 2001). Overall, it may be that the severity of the social communication and ASD-
specific cognitive difficulties align with the severity of reading comprehension deficits for many
In summary, there have been several attempts to unpack the relation between ASD
symptomatology, language, and reading performance. The existing empirical literature suggests
individuals with ASD have particular difficulties with reading comprehension and those
difficulties may be associated with both language and symptom severity of individuals with
ASD. The extant data also presents some evidence that similar profiles of struggling readers exist
11
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
to those seen in typically developing populations, with perhaps the exception of a reading profile
that exhibits a dyslexic profile of poor word recognition alongside discrepantly proficient
linguistic comprehension. However, very few studies of reading in ASD have utilized reading
batteries that assess the Simple View of Reading and include the key sub skills supporting
linguistic comprehension and word recognition. Therefore, we still know very little about the
potential difficulties individuals with ASD have on tasks related to word recognition and how
these lower level reading variables interact with linguistic and reading comprehension.
Furthermore, measurement of reading ability varies depending on the assessments used (Cutting
& Scarborough, 2006), and different findings from previous studies could be the result of using
different reading measures. Therefore, use of multiple measures of each component of reading
would be beneficial.
Current Study
with ASD. To our knowledge, no studies have analyzed subgroups, or distinct profiles, of
school-aged readers with ASD using an extensive reading battery that includes the sub skills of
lower-level word recognition abilities, including phonological processing and processing speed
measures, and higher-level linguistic and reading comprehension skills. In the current study, a
comprehensive reading and language assessment battery was collected with school-aged children
with HFASD to answer the following research questions: (a) Do individuals with HFASD
exhibit distinct reading profiles? (b) How do distinct reading subgroup profiles relate to ASD
symptom severity? Based on previous literature, we hypothesize that HFASD readers are
heterogeneous in nature, with relative strengths and weaknesses, therefore distinct profiles will
emerge. We also hypothesize that similar to previous research, individuals with HFASD who
12
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
demonstrate more severe reading discrepancies will also have more severe ASD
symptomatology.
Method
Participants
This research was conducted in compliance with the Institutional Review Board and
written parental consent and child assent was obtained prior to data collection. Participants were
81 (66 male) children, aged 8 to 16 years, who had a community diagnosis of ASD (see
descriptive statistics in Table 1). Enrolled subjects were recruited from the local community
through school districts, a university research subject tracking system, and word of mouth.
Individuals were included in the HFASD sample if they had a community diagnosis of ASD that
was confirmed by trained researchers using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule,
Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012), and if they had had a full-scale IQ (FIQ) estimate ≥
2011). A total of 93 individuals with ASD were recruited for this study; they all met criteria for
ASD on the ADOS, but 12 individuals were ineligible for the study due to FIQ < 75.
Exclusionary criteria included an identified syndrome other than ASD or ADHD (e.g. Fragile X),
significant sensory or motor impairment (e.g. visual impairments), a neurological disorder (e.g.
epilepsy, cerebral palsy), psychotic symptoms (e.g. hallucinations or delusions), or any major
medical disorder that could be associated with extended absences from school. Twenty-eight
percent of the children with HFASD also met criteria for ADHD according to parent report of a
community diagnosis. Most of the children in this sample spent much, or all, of their school day
in a general education classroom setting: 65% were in general education 81-100% of the day,
12% were in general education 41-80% of the day, 10% were in general education 1-40% of the
13
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
day, 10% were not in general education at all, and three percent did not report placement.
Eighty-four percent of the children attended public schools, and 91% had an IEP or 504 Plan.
Data reported are from assessment sessions that were conducted by members of trained
research group in a university-based child assessment laboratory over two 2.5-hour sessions.
Diagnostic Measures and Sample Description. The ADOS-2 (Lord et al., 2012) is a
semi-structured diagnostic assessment shown to have strong predictive validity compared to best
estimate clinical diagnoses (Charman & Gotham, 2013). Scores were utilized to both confirm
ASD diagnosis and as a distal measure in the second research question. Modules 3 and 4 were
administered, providing scores for Social Affect (SA) and Restricted and Repetitive Behavior
(RRB). The Module 3 yielded a raw subscore for SA and for RRB that combined to create the
Total Score. Intraclass correlations for interrater reliability for Module 3 were reported as 0.92
for SA, 0.91 for RRB and 0.94 for overall total raw score. Intraclass correlations for interrater
reliability for Module 4 were reported to be 0.93 for Social Interaction, 0.84 for Communication,
0.92 for Communication + Social Interaction, and 0.82 for Stereotyped Behaviors and Restricted
Interests (Lord et al., 2012). Module 4 scores were converted via the modified Module 4
IQ. The WASI-2 (Wechsler, 2011) provided an estimate of verbal and nonverbal
cognitive ability. Two verbal subtests, Vocabulary and Similarities, measured expressive
vocabulary and abstract semantic reasoning and formed the verbal composite (VIQ). Two
nonverbal subtests, Block Design and Matrix Reasoning, measured spatial perception, visual
abstract processing & problem solving with motor and non-motor involvement and formed the
performance composite (PIQ). Combined, the four subtests yielded an age-normed standard
14
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
score measurement of full-scale IQ (FIQ). The Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) index has established
internal consistency (0.96) and test-retest reliability for children ages 6-16, r = 0.94 (Wechsler,
2011). In this sample, internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .89 for
Vocabulary; .88 for Similarities; .87 for Block Design; and .92 for Matrix Reasoning.
Phonological processing and rapid automatized naming. The Elision and Nonword
Repetition (NWR) subtests were administered from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological
Processing, Second Version (CTOPP-2; Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1999) that yielded age-
normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3) measuring phonological awareness (PA) and expressive
coefficient from our sample for Elision (alpha = .93) was consistent with publisher reported
alphas (alphas = .81–.91; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). The internal consistency
Cronbach’s alpha coefficient from our sample for NWR (alpha = .78) was consistent with
publisher reported alphas (alphas = .73–.80).The speed at which participants were able to
connect orthographic and phonological representations was measured using two rapid
automatized naming (RAN) tasks from the CTOPP-2; Rapid Letter Naming and Rapid Digit
Naming subtests yielded separate age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3), and combined for
from our sample for Rapid Letter Naming (.89) and Rapid Digit Naming (.87) were consistent
Word Recognition. The Test of Word Reading Efficiency, Second Edition (TOWRE-2,
Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2012) provided an age-normed standard score (M = 100, SD
=15) measuring accuracy and fluency of sight word recognition (Sight Word Efficiency: SWE)
and phonemic decoding (Phonemic Decoding Efficiency: PDE). Participants read as many real
15
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
words (SWE) or decodable nonwords (PDE) as they were able to in 45 seconds per subtest.
Internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha coefficients from our sample for SWE (alpha = .97), and
PDE (alpha = .87) were generally consistent with publisher reported alphas for both subtests
(alphas > .90; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2012). Text-level reading accuracy was assessed
with age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3) from the Gray Oral Reading Tests – Fifth
Edition (GORT-5; Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012). Publisher (Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012) reported
Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for GORT-5 Accuracy scores ranged between .85 and .94 in the
Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig, Secord, 2003)
syntactic expressive language skills. In order to accurately recall increasingly longer and more
complex sentences, one must strategically utilize language structure (e.g., syntax) and meaning.
Publisher (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003) reported Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients
ranged from .86 - .93 in the normative sample and .97 in an ASD subsample. Expressive
vocabulary was measured with the Vocabulary subtest from the WASI-II (Wechsler, 2011),
which yielded an age-normed T-score (M = 50, SD =10). This subtest was designed to measure
verbal concept formation and semantic knowledge by asking the participant to orally define
words of increasing complexity. The Auditory Reasoning subtest of the Test of Auditory
Processing Skills, Third Edition (TAPS-3; Martin & Brownwell, 2005) provided an age-normed
scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3) assessing higher order linguistic processing related to listening
Participants are read short vignettes (approximately 2-3 sentences each) and asked to respond to
16
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
one question for each vignette. In order for an answer to receive credit, a participant must either
make the correct inference, or correctly interpret an abstraction or idiom. Internal consistency
Cronbach’s alpha from our sample for Auditory Reasoning (alpha = .87) was generally
consistent with publisher reported alphas (alphas = .91-.96; Martin & Brownwell, 2005). The
Story Recall subtest of the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning, Second Edition
(WRAML2, Sheslow & Adams, 2003) tapped the ability to listen to and utilize narrative
structure to organize and retell gist and verbatim details of two orally presented narratives and
yielded an age-normed scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3). Internal consistency Cronbach’s alpha
from our sample for Story Recall (alpha = .95) was generally consistent with publisher reported
Reading Comprehension. The Gray Oral Reading Tests – Fifth Edition (GORT-5;
that yielded age-normed scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3). The individually administered test is
comprised of 16 progressively more difficult reading passages read aloud by the child, each
followed by 5 open-ended comprehension questions given orally by the tester with the passage
removed from view. Question types vary, from those asking for recall of details to those
requiring higher order processing such as synthesis of the main idea, understanding of causal
relations, or ability to make predictions. Publisher (Wiederholt & Bryant, 2012) reported
Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for Comprehension scores range between .90 and .96 in
Analytic Strategy
Differentiated Profiles of Reading Skills. All analyses were conducted using Mplus
7.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 - 2015). To answer the first research question, we began by
17
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
iteratively fitting a series of unconditional latent profile analyses beginning with a one-profile
model and increasing the number of profiles by one with each subsequent run. The twelve
reading-related measures (i.e. RAN, NWR, Elision, PDE, SWE, GORT Accuracy, CELF
Recalling Sentences, Expressive Vocabulary, TAPS Auditory Reasoning, WRAML Story Recall,
and GORT Comprehension) were used as latent profile indicators. See Figure 1 for a conceptual
diagram of the full model. As the twelve indicators represented the four broader constructs,
including the indicators simultaneously allows profiles to reflect differences across the four
constructs concomitantly. This analysis also provides an empirical method of deriving reading
profiles as opposed to using relatively arbitrary cutoff scores. Finally, examining the results in
light of the twelve indicators (and, consequently, the four broader constructs) simultaneously
enabled us to identify the greatest discrepancies across constructs among the emergent profiles.
Multiple fit indices were used to compare the models as no single fit index has been
shown to perfectly identify the optimal model (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007). First, we
utilized the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwartz, 1978) and adjusted BIC (ABIC)
with lower values indicating a preferred model. Additionally, we used two likelihood ratio based
indices, the Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) test and the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test (BLRT).
Both tests assess whether adding a profile significantly improves model fit such that a non-
significant p-value for a k-class model indicates the model with k - 1 classes is preferred. For
further information on these three fit indices, see Nylund et al. (2007). Finally, we employed two
information-heuristic indices, the Bayes Factor (BF) and correct model probability (cmP) that
have only recently been applied to mixture modeling (Masyn, 2013). The BF provides pairwise
comparisons of adjacent models that provides a ratio of the probability of a model with k classes
being preferred compared to a model with k + 1 classes. Values between 1 - 3 are weak evidence
18
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
for the k-class model, 3 - 10 are moderate evidence, and values greater than 10 indicate strong
evidence. The cmP provides a probability that each model is preferred compared to all of the
models under consideration. While not considered a fit index, we also examined entropy, which
provides a measure of the strength of classification, with values between .80 and 1.00 or greater
indicative of good classification (Ram & Grimm, 2009). While fit statistics aided us in
identifying a chosen model, we also considered the substantive interpretation of the latent
profiles in each model to ensure the chosen model was theoretically viable (Muthén, 2003).
membership to answer the second research question. This was accomplished by estimating
profile-specific means. This process has been shown to result in a shift in the latent profiles,
thereby altering the substantive interpretation of them (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014a; Nylund-
Gibson, Grimm, Quirk, & Furlong, 2014). Therefore, we implemented the BCH approach
(Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014b; Bakk, Tekle, & Vermunt, 2013; Bolck, Croon, & Hagenaars,
2004; Vermunt, 2010) in order to account for classification error and avoid profile shifts. This
membership. Finally, the BCH approach estimated profile-specific means of ADOS and
conducted all pairwise comparisons. For technical details of the BCH approach, see Asparouhov
& Muthén (2014b), Bakk, Tekle, & Vermunt (2013), and Bolck, Croon, & Hagenaars (2004).
Results
This section is divided into three subsections reflecting the model building steps. First,
we provide descriptive statistics to compare the present sample to national norms. Next, we
describe the latent profile enumeration process and label and interpret the emergent profiles.
19
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Finally, we present the results of the relation between the reading profiles and ASD
symptomatology severity.
Descriptive Statistics
Descriptive statistics, all reported as standard scores can be seen in Table 1. The
descriptive statistics demonstrate that the sample met criteria for ASD on the ADOS-2; measures
of IQ show normal range. A range of scores was seen on the reading related measures. On
average, the overall sample scored at least one standard deviation below the normed mean on all
reading measures. The one exception is the word decoding measure, PDE and SWE, where the
sample scored closer to the normed average of 100. In order to determine if the heterogeneity of
the samples reading abilities, next we conducted a series of latent profile analyses.
Fit statistics of the six latent profile models can be seen in Table 2. Values in boldface
indicate the preferred model for a particular fit index. The BIC reached a minimum value at the
4-profile model. However, the only statistically significant LMR value occurred with the 2-
profile model. The BLRT never became non-significant and, thus, was non-informative in
choosing a preferred model. Both the BF and cmP supported the 4-profile model. The entropy
While statistical evidence was clear for the 4-profile model, we also examined the profile
plot to ensure theoretical viability. Though the analysis was conducted using age-normed
standardized scores, these were rescaled to z-scores for the profile plot to foster interpretability.
The four profiles were characterized by their performance on the reading and language measures.
The profile plot for the 4-profile model can be seen in Figure 2. The profile demarcated by a
dashed line with square markers was labeled Readers with Comprehension Disturbance and
20
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
accounted for approximately 20% of this sample. These students were characterized by average
rapid automatized naming, phonological awareness, word decoding and word recognition, text
sentence-level syntactic expressive language skills, and story recall. Concomitant deficits in
auditory reasoning/inference, and reading comprehension typified this profile. The profile
demarcated by a dotted line with triangle markers accounted for about one-third of the sample
and was distinguished by poor performance (approximately 1 SD below average) across all
language and reading variables, so we termed this profile Readers with Global Disturbance. The
profile at the bottom of the plot depicted by a solid line with diamond markers accounted for
about 14% of the sample and was marked by very poor performance on all language and reading
variables, this subgroup was called Readers with Severe Global Disturbance. In particular,
retelling, and reading comprehension were very low with scores approximately 2 SD or more
below average. The final profile with a solid line with circular markers accounted for about 32%
of the sample and was delineated by scores in the average range on all language and reading
We also examined potential differences in age and gender among the emergent profiles
using the three-step method (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014a; Nylund-Gibson, Grimm, Quirk, &
Furlong, 2014). There were no effects of either age or gender. Readers of any age or either
The final step in this analysis was to relate ASD symptomatology (i.e. ADOS-2 total
score) to the heterogeneous reading profiles using the BCH approach. Results can be seen in
21
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Figure 3. The Readers with Severe Global Disturbance (M = 14.38) had the highest level of ASD
symptomatology and this was significantly higher than both the Readers with Global
Comprehension Disturbance (M = 11.31) did not significantly differ from any of the other three
Discussion
There is converging evidence that many individuals with ASD demonstrate difficulties
with reading; the majority of previous studies have concentrated specifically on reading
comprehension disturbance (Estes et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2009; Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright,
B., & Williams, C., 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts, Jones, Happé, & Charman, 2013).
There is some evidence that beyond reading comprehension disturbance, there are different
profiles of readers in school-aged children with ASD (e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009;
Nation et al, 2006). In addition, research has delineated language subgroups in children and
adolescents with ASD (Rapin et al., 2009; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2003), and language
impairments have been linked to reading difficulties in this population (Lindgren et al., 2009;
Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). However, the relation between these
language and reading subgroups was previously unexamined using comprehensive reading and
language batteries. In the present study, the first research question probed the heterogeneity of
reading and language performance for individuals with HFASD based upon a comprehensive
battery of assessments of phonological processing, word recognition, and linguistic and reading
comprehension measures. The inclusion of both lower-level reading sub skills that are related to
22
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
simultaneous consideration of the relation between the two domains outlined by the Simple View
of Reading and their sub skills. Four distinct profiles emerged from the sample of students with
HFASD: Readers with Comprehension Disturbance, Readers with Global Disturbance, Readers
with Severe Global Disturbance and Average Readers. The second research question
investigated the relation between the subgroups of readers and ASD symptomatology in order to
further understand the relation between the social-communicative and cognitive phenotype of
ASD and reading related skills in sample of individuals diagnosed with HFASD.
hyperlexic reading disability profile predicted by the Component Model of Reading. This
subgroup has been frequently reported in prior studies of reading with individuals with ASD
(e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Jones et al, 2009; Nation et al, 2006; Newman et al., 2007; Huemer &
Mann, 2010; Wei et al., 2015; Zuccarello et al., 2015) and shares characteristics with a language
subgroup reported by Rapin et al. (2009) whose members demonstrated adequate phonology and
vocabulary alongside linguistic comprehension deficits. Grigorenko, Klin, and Volkmar (2003)
interest in letters and words, precocious and unprompted emergence of word decoding, and an
extreme degree of discrepancy between word recognition and other cognitive skills that emerges
between 3 and 5 years of age (Healy, 1982). Individuals in this profile demonstrated strong
phonological awareness, decoding, and word reading skills; it is possible that some of the
children in this group may have been considered hyperlexic earlier in their development. We do
not have data depicting the sample’s early reading development prior to age 8, but even if some
23
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
of the children demonstrated a precocious and circumscribed interest in word reading and
decoding when very young, they are now functioning in the average range, similar to findings
Single word expressive vocabulary for children in the Readers with Comprehension
Disturbance profile was in the average range. However, the two measures reported to be
(Condouris, Meyer, & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Rapin, Dunn, Allen,
Stevens, & Fein, 2009), posed a challenge for many of these children. Therefore, in this
subgroup, word recognition abilities did not necessarily align with structural language abilities as
reported in prior studies of reading and language in ASD (Lindgren et al., 2009; Lucas &
Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). Furthermore, children in this profile displayed higher-
deviations below average across the auditory reasoning/inference, story recall, and reading
word recognition skills and single word vocabulary and therefore may appear to be proficient
readers if other sub skills are not assessed, their moderate to profound structural language and
Readers with Global Disturbance corresponded with the garden variety poor reader
(Gough & Tunmer, 1986), or mixed reading disability subtype (Catts & Kamhi, 2005), and has
also been reported in prior studies of reading in ASD samples (Asberg & Sandberg, 2012;
Davidson & Weismer, 2014; Gabig, 2010; White et al., 2006; Nation et al., 2006). Children in
this subgroup shared characteristics with a language subgroup reported by Rapin et al. (2009)
who struggled with phonology, vocabulary, and linguistic comprehension. Similarly, Tager-
24
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Flusberg and Joseph (2003) identified an impaired language subtype of children with ASD who
tended to have phonological processing deficits and scores 1-2 SD below the mean on most
language tests. Unlike Readers with Comprehension Disturbance, Readers with Global
Disturbance demonstrated overall low word reading and decoding abilities commensurate with
their poor language skills. Similar to children in the Readers with Global Disturbance, the
Readers with Severe Global Disturbance resembled Rapin et al. (2009) and Tager-Flusberg &
Joseph’s (2003) language impaired subtypes previously described, but with far more severe
impairment. The distinction between the two latter profiles may be thought of as categorically
distinct areas of a continuum, such as the difference between the terms “below average” and “far
below average” that are sometimes used in diagnostic measures. This is consistent with
longitudinal evidence from Pickles et al. (2014) that oral language impairments in ASD present
in parallel patterns of development and proficiency levels after the age of seven. Together these
The individuals in the Average Readers subgroup did not struggle with the reading or
language measures, and in fact exhibited intact reading skills overall. Their performance across
the language measures was similar to that of Rapin et al.’s (2009) subgroup that demonstrated
average or above performance on all language and cognitive measures. Similarly, this subgroup
shared many characteristics with Tager-Flusberg and Joseph’s (2003) description of a group of
children with ASD with normal linguistic abilities who have intact phonological skills, fluency,
syntax and morphology, expressive language, and average to large lexicons. They noted
however, that comprehension may still be impaired at the discourse level, as well as for more
open-ended questions such as “why, when, and how”. Therefore, more complex measures of
reading and linguistic comprehension that require increased demands on cognitive resources,
25
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
inferential thinking, and social knowledge might still pose a challenge for those in the Average
group.
reasoning/inference measure. This is consistent with Tager-Flusberg and Joseph’s (2003) finding
regarding difficulty with open-ended questions, as well as research indicating that children with
ASD often have difficulties integrating information from background knowledge with that from
the text for global coherence and inference generation (Norbury & Nation, 2011; Wahlberg &
Magliano, 2004). However, some studies have demonstrated that there are aspects of inferencing
which may be preserved in children with ASD such as automatic inference generation between
sentences in very short passages (Saldana & Frith, 2007) and inferring emotions of main
characters in short texts (Tirado & Saldana, 2016). However, Tirado and Saldana (2016) also
found that their participants had difficulty responding to questions about those inferred emotions.
It is possible that for individuals with ASD, there is particular difficulty with a deep
understanding of inferences in situations that are more abstract such as in the context of reading
unknown text, and that these difficulties may be exacerbated in longer texts. This is an important
This study found both similarities and differences compared to the subgroups of neuro-
typical readers reported by Catts et al. (2003). The most prominent difference was that no
dyslexic profile emerged in our study whereas this subgroup made up 35.5% of the poor readers
in their sample. This finding is consistent with previous studies of reading in samples with ASD
(Lindgren et al., 2009; Lucas & Norbury, 2014; Norbury & Nation, 2011). Similar to these prior
studies, poor word reading and decoding in our sample was generally associated with structural
26
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
language difficulties as well as language and reading comprehension impairments, not as a stand-
However, both the Readers with Global Disturbance and the Readers with Severe Global
Disturbance profiles resembled the Catts et al. (2003) language-learning disabilities subgroup.
Combining our two Global Disturbance profiles would account for 47.3% of our sample
compared to 35.7% in the Catts et al. sample. Thus, while we identified a similar subgroup, the
prevalence rates differed between the two studies. The differences might be a result of the
younger age (i.e., second grade) used in the Catts et al. (2003) study. However, language delay
and impairment is common in children with ASD (Pickles et al., 2014) and it is probable that
children with ASD who struggle with reading are more typified by impairments in either
difficulties. This could explain the lack of a dyslexic profile along with a greater prevalence of
children who resembled the language-learning disabilities subgroup in Catts et al. (2003).
associated with ASD diagnosis and symptom severity (Asberg et al., 2010; Estes et al., 2011;
Jones et al., 2009; Norbury & Nation, 2011; Ricketts et al., 2013). Results of this study are
consistent with these previous findings: reading comprehension scores were highest when ASD
symptomatology as measured by the ADOS-2 was lowest. Readers with Severe Global
Disturbance, who demonstrated the poorest linguistic and reading comprehension abilities, had
significantly higher levels of ASD symptomatology than children in the Average Reader and
Global Disturbance subgroups. They also struggled the most with sight word recognition and
text reading accuracy, consistent with individuals referred to in the typically developing
27
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
literature as having a language-learning disability (LLD; Berninger & May, 2011). The current
study provides additional evidence that the social communicative and cognitive phenotype of
ASD impacts both linguistic and reading comprehension for many students with ASD throughout
The majority of students with HFASD are educated in general education classrooms.
Extant data suggests that these students are being underserved in these settings in the area of
reading development, with many of them scoring at least one grade level below their typically
ASD, 65% of the students were in general education classes 81-100% of the day, and an
additional 12% were in these classes 41-80% of the day, yet almost 68% of the students
Furthermore, these profiles were related to the severity of social communication and cognitive
characteristics associated with ASD. This has important implications for educating students with
difficult to expect general education teachers to know how to meet the reading instructional
needs of individuals with HFASD when very little is known about the development of the sub
In order to address the unique instructional needs of students with ASD, and to be able to
develop the most effective reading intervention protocols, a more in-depth investigation into
reading profiles in this population was necessary. The results of this study demonstrate that
assessment and intervention methods must be tailored to meet the specific reading needs of
individual students, and the specific skill deficits depicted in these profiles can be addressed.
28
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Thorough assessment of both word recognition and linguistic comprehension sub skills is
important; for example, average single word expressive vocabulary was higher for all subgroups
than was auditory reasoning ability and an overreliance on vocabulary skill level could lead to
overlooking a key domain for intervention. When planning intervention, students in the Readers
recognition skills and comprehension, would benefit from explicit structural language
intervention and linguistic comprehension instruction. However, students in the Severe Global
and Global Disturbance subgroups would benefit from explicit phonological processing, word
subgroup would likely benefit from a much more intense intervention in these areas and may
require additional behavioral scaffolding to sufficiently engage with the intervention. These
types of targeted interventions could be implemented through collaborations with various school
professionals including reading specialists, speech and language pathologists, and special
Conclusions
The proportion of individuals with reading disturbance has been shown repeatedly to be
greater in samples of individuals with ASD than in the general population. The data in the
present study concurred with previous literature that a large percentage of individuals with ASD
demonstrate reading disturbance and that this disturbance is associated with language
impairments. Furthermore, this study provided additional evidence that phonological awareness
is associated with word decoding for school-aged children with ASD, as is seen in typically
developing samples, and word recognition deficits were concomitant with language deficits. It
has also been argued and shown empirically that there is a significant relation between the
29
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
social-communicative and cognitive phenotype of ASD and reading performance. The present
study demonstrates support for this finding in a much more specific way, by showing that ASD
developmental span across elementary and secondary school years was extensive. Future studies
would benefit from even larger samples at each age and grade level to more fully understand
reading profiles in students with ASD. Future studies would also benefit from data collection
with students with a broader range of ASD severity, as this study only included individuals with
HFASD. We would also suggest that text reading fluency be collected as a part of future reading
batteries as it is possible that while this sample showed relative strength on word reading, it may
not translate to fluent reading of connected text. In addition to text reading, we also note
limitations related to the language measures used in this study. In the future, we would suggest
collecting more robust measures of language development in order to gain a better understanding
of the role language plays in reading comprehension. Another limitation of this study is that the
standardized measures used may not have been robust enough to adequately capture the extent of
higher-level linguistic and reading comprehension challenges. A future study would benefit
from the inclusion of additional reading and language comprehension measures that are more
complex and would demand more cognitive resources, inferential thinking, narrative retelling,
and social knowledge. Longer texts, both fiction and nonfiction, that are similar to those used in
classrooms, as well as other genres such as persuasive essays or satire might also uncover
additional targets for intervention even for those in the Average Reader group. Longitudinal
studies would also contribute further to our understanding of patterns of subgroup membership
30
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
and how they may change with intervention and maturation. Finally, future investigations are
needed to further probe the specific aspects of ASD symptomatology that are associated with
classrooms in order to develop effective interventions for school-aged children with ASD.
This study was funded by NIMH 1RO1MH085904, IES R324A110174, and the UC Davis
Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in
accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and
with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in
the study.
31
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
References
Aaron, P. G. (1997). The impending demise of the discrepancy formula. Review of Educational
Research, 67(4), 461-502.
Aaron, P. G., Joshi, R. M., Gooden, R., & Bentum, K. E. (2008). Diagnosis and treatment of
reading disabilities based on the component model of reading an alternative to the discrepancy
model of LD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(1), 67-84.
Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second versus eighth
grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 332-345.
Åsberg, J., Kopp, S., Berg-Kelly, K., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Reading comprehension, word
decoding and spelling in girls with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) or attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD): Performance and predictors. International Journal of
Language & Communication Disorders, 45(1), 61-71.
Åsberg, J., & Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2012). Dyslexic, delayed, precocious or just normal?
Word reading skills of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Research in Reading,
35(1), 20-31.
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014a). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step
approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3),
329-341. doi:10/1080/10705511.2014.915181
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014b). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Using the
BCH method in Mplus to estimate a distal outcome model and an arbitrary secondary model.
Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/examples/webnotes/webnote21.pdf
Bakk, Z., Tekle, F.B., & Vermunt, J.K. (2013). Estimating the association between latent class
membership and external variables using bias-adjusted three-step approaches. Sociological
Methodology, 43, 272-311. doi: 10.1177/0081175012470644
Beglinger, L. J., & Smith, T. H. (2001). A review of subtyping in autism and proposed
dimensional classification model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 411-
422.
Beglinger, L., & Smith, T. (2005). Concurrent validity of social subtype and IQ after early
intensive behavioral intervention in children with autism: A preliminary investigation. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 295-303.
Berninger, V. W., & May, M., (2011). Evidence-based diagnosis and treatment for specific
learning disabilities involving impairments in written and/or oral language. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 44(2), 167-183.
32
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44.
Bolck, A., Croon, M., & Hagenaars, J. (2004). Estimating latent structure models with
categorical variables: One-step versus three-step estimators. Political Analysis, 12, 3-27.
doi: 10.1093/pan/mph001
Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and… fork”: Examining central coherence
in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development with a linguistic task.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 377-393.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal
connection. Nature.
Brown, H. M., Oram-Cardy, J., & Johnson, A. (2013). A meta-analysis of the reading
comprehension skills of individuals on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 932-955.
Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Bindman, M. (1998). Awareness of language in children who have
reading difficulties: Historical comparisons in a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 39(4), 501-510.
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension
difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683-696.
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (Eds.). (2008). Children's comprehension problems in oral and written
language: A cognitive perspective. New York: Guilford Press.
Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2003). The effects of morphological structure on children’s
reading of derived words in English. In Reading Complex Words (pp. 27-52). Springer US.
Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. E. (2003). Subgrouping poor readers on the basis of
individual differences in reading-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(2), 151-
164.
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (1999). Causes of reading disabilities. Language and Reading
Disabilities, 95-127.
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (2005). Classification of reading disabilities. Language and
Reading Disabilities, 2, 72-93.
33
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters
in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13(2), 195-212. doi:10.1007/BF01246098
Charman, T., & Gotham, K. (2013). Measurement issues: Screening and diagnostic instruments
for autism spectrum disorders–lessons from research and practise. Child and Adolescent Mental
Health, 18(1), 52-63.
Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Gottardo, A. (2004). Vocabulary, context, and speech perception
among good and poor readers. Educational Psychology, 24(6), 825-843.
Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized
measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349-358.
Constantino, J. & Gruber, C. (2005). Social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western
Psychological Services.
de Bruin, C. L., Deppeler, J. M., Moore, D. W., & Diamond, N. T. (2013). Public school-based
interventions for adolescents and young adults with an autism spectrum disorder: A meta-
analysis. Review of Educational Research, 83(4), 521-550.
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Penguin.
Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-
functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of abnormal child psychology,
34(1), 83-98.
Dingfelder, H. E., & Mandell, D. S. (2011). Bridging the research-to-practice gap in autism
intervention: An application of diffusion of innovation theory. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 597-609.
Ehlers, S., Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). A screening questionnaire for Asperger syndrome
and other high functioning autism spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 29, 129–140.
34
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Estes, A., Rivera, V., Bryan, M., Cali, P., & Dawson, G. (2011). Discrepancies between
academic achievement and intellectual ability in higher-functioning school-aged children with
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1044-1052.
Fleury, V. P., Hedges, S., Hume, K., Browder, D. M., Thompson, J. L., Fallin, K., ... & Vaughn,
S. (2014). Addressing the academic needs of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in
secondary education. Remedial and Special Education, 35(2), 68-79.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. Surface Dyslexia, 32, 301-330.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal reading comprehension
measures. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2), 20-28.
Gabig, C. S. (2010). Phonological awareness and word recognition in reading by children with
autism. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(2), 67-85.
Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Boyle, M., Hanna, S., Duku, E., Zwaigenbaum, L., ... & Smith, I.
(2013). Investigating phenotypic heterogeneity in children with autism spectrum disorder: a
factor mixture modeling approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 206-215.
Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are
executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 836-854.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and
Special Education, 7(1), 6-10.
Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Annotation: Hyperlexia: Disability or
superability? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8), 1079-1091.
Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for measuring and understanding individual
differences in the component processes of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93(1), 103.
Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism.
Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218-1220.
Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: Revised
algorithm and standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
44(8), 1996-2012.
35
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Huemer, S. V., & Mann, V. (2010). A comprehensive profile of decoding and comprehension in
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 485-493.
Insel, T. R. (2014). The NIMH research domain criteria (RDoC) project: precision medicine for
psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry.
Jacobs, D. W., & Richdale, A. L. (2013). Predicting literacy in children with a high-functioning
autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(8), 2379-2390.
Jones, C. R., Happé, F., Golden, H., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Simonoff, E., ... & Charman, T.
(2009). Reading and arithmetic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Peaks and dips in
attainment. Neuropsychology, 23(6), 718.
Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of reading
made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21(2), 85-97.
Kasari, C., & Smith, T. (2013). Interventions in schools for children with autism spectrum
disorder: Methods and recommendations. Autism, 17(3), 254-267.
Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness
as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 453.
Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E., & Angley, M. T. (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in
autism: Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
40(1), 112-122.
Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (2009). 19 Formal and Informal Measures of Reading Comprehension.
Handbook of research on reading comprehension, 403.
Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and
reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment
and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2(1), 22-38.
Long, D. & Lea, R. (2005). Have we been searching for meaning in all the wrong places?
Defining the “search after meaning” principle in comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39(2&3),
279-298.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic
observation schedule, second edition. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
36
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Lucas, R. & Norbury, C. (2014). Levels of text comprehension in children with autism spectrum
disorders (ASD): The influence of language phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 44(11), 2756-2768.
Lucas, R. & Norbury, C (2015). Making inferences from text: It’s vocabulary that matters.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1224-1232.
Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M. F., Beretvas, N., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Sorrells, A., ... &
Rispoli, M. (2008). A review of school-based instructional interventions for students with autism
spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 395-416.
Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and
orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325-333.
Martin, N. & Brownwell, R. (2005). Test of auditory processing skills - third edition. Novato,
CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
Masyn, K. (2013). Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. The Oxford handbook of
quantitative methods in psychology, 2, 551-611.
McNamara, D. S. (2001). Reading both high-coherence and low-coherence texts: Effects of text
sequence and prior knowledge. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue
canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 55(1), 51.
Miles, J. H., Takahashi, T. N., Bagby, S., Sahota, P. K., Vaslow, D. F., Wang, C. H., ... &
Farmer, J. E. (2005). Essential versus complex autism: definition of fundamental prognostic
subtypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 135(2), 171-180.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary,
and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: evidence from a
longitudinal study. Developmental psychology, 40(5), 665.
Muthén, B. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth mixture modeling: comment
on Bauer and Curran (2003).
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén
& Muthén.
Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in
children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment?
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 199-211.
37
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright, B., & Williams, C. (2006). Patterns of reading ability in children
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(7), 911-919.
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills
contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342-356.
National Reading Panel (US), National Institute of Child Health, & Human Development (US).
(2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction: Reports of the subgroups. National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, National Institutes of Health.
Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Naples, A. J., Babitz, T., Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L.
(2007). Hyperlexia in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 760-774.
Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in
latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural
Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535-569.
Nylund-Gibson, K., Grimm, R., Quirk, M., & Furlong, M. (2014). A latent transition mixture
model using the three-step specification. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary
Journal, 21(3), 439-454. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915375
Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2010). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and
visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23(2), 189-208.
Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict
developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530.
Pickles, A., Anderson, D. K., & Lord, C. (2014). Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development
of language: a 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(12), 1354-1362.
Ram, N. & Grimm, K.J. (2009). Growth mixture modeling: A method for identifying differences
in longitudinal change among unobserved groups. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 33, 565-576. doi: 10.1177/0165025409343765
38
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Rapin, I., Dunn, M.A., Allen, D. A., Stevens, M. C., & Fein, D. (2009). Subtypes of language
disorders in school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 66-84.
Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on
nonword reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(2), 277-282.
Reutebuch, C. K., El Zein, F., Kim, M. K., Weinberg, A.N., & Vaughn, S. (2015). Investigating
a reading comprehension intervention for high school students with autism spectrum disorder: a
pilot study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 9, 96-111.
Rey, A., Ziegler, J. C., & Jacobs, A. M. (2000). Graphemes are perceptual reading units.
Cognition, 75(1), B1-B12.
Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism
spectrum disorders: visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238-248.
Ricketts, J., Jones, C. R., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2013). Reading comprehension in autism
spectrum disorders: The role of oral language and social functioning. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 807-816.
Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all
reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 235-257.
Roth, L., Lai, S., White, B., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Orthographic and morphological processing
as predictors of reading achievement. In annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of
Reading, Vancouver, BC.
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection
between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95(5), 259-272.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire. Los
Angeles, CA : Western Psychological Services.
Saldaña, D. & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make bridging inferences from world
knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 310-319.
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, fourth
edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, a Harcourt Assessment Company.
Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive
role of early vocabulary to future reading. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 2, 173-182.
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading
acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151-218.
39
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Sheslow, D. & Adams, W. (2003). Wide range assessment of memory and learning, second
edition: Administration and technical manual. Wilmington, Del: Wide Range, Inc.
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming,
phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of
Educational Research, 73(4), 407-440.
Tirado, M. J., & Saldaña, D. (2016). Readers with autism can produce inferences, but they
cannot answer inferential questions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3),
1025-1037.
Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (2012, 1999). Test of word reading efficiency, second
edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed Inc.
Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension (pp. 11-12). New
York: Academic Press.
Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent Class Modeling with Covariates: Two Improved+ Three-Step
Approaches. Political analysis, mpq025.
Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological
processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed Inc.
Wahlberg, T., & Magliano, J. P. (2004). The ability of high function individuals with autism to
comprehend written discourse. Discourse Processes, 38(1), 119-144.
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, second edition. San Antonio,
TX: NCS Pearson.
Wiederholt, J. & Bryant, B. (2012). Gray oral reading tests – fifth edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
40
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Wei, X., Christiano, E. R., Jennifer, W. Y., Wagner, M., & Spiker, D. (2014). Reading and math
achievement profiles and longitudinal growth trajectories of children with an autism spectrum
disorder. Autism, 1362361313516549.
White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., & Ramus, F. (2006). A double
dissociation between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability: A comparison of autistic
and dyslexic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(5), 748-761.
Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile of memory function in
children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 21.
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated
abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 11-29.
Zuccarello, R., Di Blasi, F. D., Zingale, M., Panerai, S., Finocchiaro, M., Trubia, G., ... &
Zoccolotti, P. (2015). Reading decoding and comprehension in children with autism spectrum
disorders: Evidence from a language with regular orthography. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 17, 126-134.
41
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Table 1
Measure M SD Range
PP and RAN
RANc 85.68 21.92 1-145
NWRa 7.50 2.15 1-13
Elisiona 9.94 3.08 1-15
Word Recognition
PDEc 94.89 14.81 58-127
SWEc 93.29 14.75 57-136
GORTAcca 8.03 2.69 2-16
Linguistic Comprehension
CELFrsa 7.36 3.15
Evocabb 46.96 9.89 24-69
AudReasa 6.04 2.77 1-11
StryReca 7.94 3.31 1-15
Reading Comprehension
GORTCompa 7.37 2.61 1-13
ASD Symptomatology
ADOS-2 Total 10.94 3.65 7-24
IQ
FIQc 100.00 14.00 76-132
VIQc 96.00 15.00 60-136
PIQc 105.00 16.00 71-150
Age 11.24 2.19 8-16
Note. PP = Phonological Processing; RAN = Rapid Automatized
Naming; NWR = Nonword Repetition; PDE = Phonemic Decoding
Efficiency; SWE = Sight Word Efficiency; GORTAcc = GORT text
accuracy; CELFrs = CELF Recalling Sentences; EVocab =
Expressive Vocabulary; AudReas = Auditory Reasoning; StryRec =
Story Recall; GORTComp = GORT reading comprehension.
a
Scaled score, M=10, SD = 3. bT-score, M = 50, SD = 10.
c
Standard score, M = 100, SD = 15.
42
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Table 2
LMR BLRT
Profile LL BIC ABIC p-value p-value BF cmP Min n
1 -2687.81 5472.31 5402.93 - - <.001 <.001 -
2 -2580.87 5311.15 5203.92 0.006 <.001 1.56 0.17 34
3 -2554.94 5312.03 5166.96 0.683 <.001 0.16 0.11 13
4 -2526.75 5308.38 5125.46 0.181 <.001 34.81 0.69 11
5 -2503.93 5315.48 5094.72 0.37 <.001 - 0.02 10
6 Did not converge
Note. LL = Log-likelihood; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; ABIC = Adjusted BIC; LMR = Lo-
Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test; BLRT = Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test; BF = Bayes Factor;
cmP = Correct Model Probability.
43
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Table 3
44
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
Reading
Profiles ADOS
Figure 1. Conceptual diagram of heterogeneous reading profiles and relation to ASD symptomatology.
45
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
0.5
-0.5
-1
-1.5
-2
-2.5
-3
c
E
cc
ab
R
rs
N
on
as
p
Re
m
PD
SW
W
LF
RA
Re
A
oc
isi
Co
ry
N
RT
EV
CE
El
ud
St
RT
O
A
G
O
G
Severe Global Disturbance (14.1%) Comprehension Disturbance (20.6%)
Global Disturbance (33.2%) Average Readers (32.1%)
Figure 2. Profile plot based on reading measures. PP = Phonological Processing; RAN = Rapid Automatized Naming; NWR = Nonword Repetition; PDE =
Phonemic Decoding Efficiency; SWE = Sight Word Efficiency; GORTAcc = GORT text accuracy; CELFrs = CELF Recalling Sentences; EVocab = Expressive
Vocabulary; AudReas = Auditory Reasoning; StryRec = Story Recall; GORTComp = GORT reading comprehension.
46
Running Head: READING SUBGROUPS HFASD
*
16
*
14
12 14.38
(5.54)
10
ADOS Mean
11.31
8 (2.99)
10.15 9.98
6 (3.32) (2.69)
0
Severe Global Comprehension Global Disturbance Average Readers
Disturbance Disturbance
Reading Profile
Figure 3. Means (standard deviations) of ADOS-2 scores by reading profile. An asterisk indicates significantly different (p < .05)
ADOS-2 means between a given pair of reading profiles.
47