Barry Richards - The Psychology of Politics-Routledge (2019)
Barry Richards - The Psychology of Politics-Routledge (2019)
Barry Richards - The Psychology of Politics-Routledge (2019)
BARRY RICHARDS
First published 2019
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2019 Barry Richards
The right of Barry Richards to be identified as author of this work
has been asserted by him in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of
the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or
reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical,
or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including
photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or
retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or
registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and
explanation without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
A catalog record has been requested for this book
ISBN: 978-1-138-55167-1 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-1-138-55170-1 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-1-315-14769-7 (ebk)
Typeset in Joanna
by Apex CoVantage, LLC
CONTENTS
2 Leaders 23
3 Nations 45
4 Ideologies 65
5 Prospects 87
Further reading 95
Notes97
References101
1
THE HEART OF POLITICS
But this does not take us entirely into a world of darkness, because
in looking deeply into the psyche we will also encounter the deep
roots of the capacities for empathy, concern and generosity. And those
positive capacities are as emotional and as ‘irrational’ (at least in a
narrow sense of ‘rationality’) as are the impulses towards destruction.
So, as the seventeenth-century French philosopher Blaise Pascal had
suggested in the now often-quoted line from his posthumous work
‘Thoughts’, we need to come out of the falsely dichotomised world of
early modern philosophy in which rationality, intellect and progress
stand on one side, with irrationality, emotion and backwardness on
the other. Psychoanalysis transcends that dichotomy, and it enables
us to see that the question is not whether reason or emotion is the
driver, since both are always involved, but what kinds of emotion are
involved, and therefore to which purposes reason is put.
THINKING PSYCHOSOCIALLY
The psychology of politics is basically the same as the psychology of
anything else. Our experiences and actions in the political domain are
shaped by the same fundamental emotional needs and resources as
we find at the centre of our personal lives, and in our work and leisure
activities. Political psychology is therefore the study of how these
basic forces play out in the context of politics. In different contexts,
different parts of our selves come more into play, which is why psy-
chological inquiry should always be linked with a study of the social
context – the fundamental principle of psychosocial analysis, which this
book will hopefully illustrate. In political contexts, whether we are
voters, activists, leaders, or ordinary people just trying to decide what
we think and feel about the news, it is our selves as members of wider
communities which will be in the foreground. And these ‘citizen’
types of self may differ in some ways from one’s self as friend, partner,
parent, sibling, colleague and so on. Still, all our selves are rooted in
the psychological fundamentals which we all share.
In the approach to be taken throughout this book, these funda-
mentals will be seen as needs and resources – emotional needs for certain
kinds of experience and certain kinds of relationship, and emotional
6 The heart of politics
does not exist. Much more could be said about this, but for our
purposes here the point is that a measure of trust in its environment
is necessary for the infant to develop psychologically in a good-
enough way. Because of its total dependency on others, it must be
able to trust that others will meet its needs. Without that trust, it
would be continually exposed to panic and terror at the awareness
of its own helplessness.
The psychoanalyst Joseph Sandler (1987) suggested that the primary
principle governing mental life was the ‘safety principle’, according to
which securing an experience of safety is our overriding objective.This
is not the same as a ‘survival instinct’ with the simple goal of physical
safety, though it may include that. We are psychological creatures, so
the safety we need is a mental one, a feeling safe. The most fundamental
type of this kind of safety is feeling safe in one’s own mind. That
may seem like an odd suggestion: how could you not be ‘safe’ just
in your own mind? The answer is to be found in the sufferings of
mental disorder: the fears and panics of neurotic anxiety, the feelings
of internal deadness in depression, and the terrifying experiences of
disintegration in psychotic states of mind, show that we cannot take
the stability or integrity of our own minds for granted.
Psychoanalytic investigation of early psychological development
has suggested that our earliest experience as babies, before our
selves have achieved a substantial degree of integration, can easily
be dominated by a fear of falling apart, of annihilation. The best
antidote to such fear is the experience of being ‘recognised’ by
another person, that is of having your fears acknowledged, and
your self affirmed and accepted, by another. The provision of such
experiences for the human infant naturally falls to the infant’s main
carer(s). A leading figure in post-Freudian psychoanalysis, Donald
Winnicott, wrote extensively in the mid-twentieth century about
the role of the ‘ordinary, good-enough’ mother in attuning to her
baby’s needs and fears, from the mild to the overwhelming, and so
facilitating the development in the infant’s mind of a stable sense
of self inhabiting a reasonably supportive environment (see, for
example, Winnicott, 1988). Thus a basis is laid for the experience and
The heart of politics 9
the expectation of safety. The work of the mother, and other primary
care-givers, in helping to create this vital sense of safety, was captured
in Winnicott’s concept of ‘holding’. In part this is a physical holding,
in an attentive and empathic way that enables an infant to feel safe
in the external world and in its own body, but it is more than that –
Winnicott talks of the ‘holding environment’.
Yet no caregivers, however loving and attuned, can provide perfect
and instant administration to the baby’s needs. No matter how devoted
the parent, there will be distracting demands on their attention, delays
in feeding or changing, interruptions and separations. There will be
pain and frustration. So there will be recurrent occasions for feeling
unsafe, for doubting that the world is trustworthy. One of the major
projects of psychoanalysis has been to explore this area of development
and its legacies in the adult mind. Insofar as a basic sense of safety
was established in infancy, this will be a resource that can contribute
to emotional well-being throughout life. But while the experience
of safety remains brittle or weak, or access to it is unreliable, the
individual may be vulnerable, or dysfunctional, perhaps at risk of
exploitation or manipulation by various promises of safety.
The connection here with politics is clear, especially in times when
even citizens of stable and lawful democracies have reason to feel
unsafe. So when political leaders strive to win the trust of the people,
they are entering territory where powerful and complex forces are at
work. Adult voters, trying to decide whom to trust, are judging what
they think they can see of the candidates, but are also unknowingly
comparing the candidates with their own buried memories or hopes
about what trustworthiness looks, sounds or feels like. We will explore
more of that later, but here we must stay for a little while longer in
this territory of difficult psychological theory, to meet some more of
the ideas which will be applied in subsequent chapters.
SPLIT WORLDS
There has been much psychoanalytic investigation of how infants
and young children deal with feeling unsafe, which has partly
10 T h e h e a r t o f p o l i t i c s
built on, and partly transformed, the foundations which the first
psychoanalyst Sigmund Freud laid for understanding the mind. This
work has produced a number of concepts describing the defences
which we employ in our minds to try to obliterate the unsafety, and
so avoid the anxiety associated with it. The most basic of these is
the process of splitting, in which all the objects of our experience (of
which people are of course the most important) are divided into
two categories, the absolutely ‘good’ and the absolutely ‘bad’. These
dramatically split objects of experience in the baby’s mind exemplify
what psychoanalysts call ‘unconscious phantasies’ – hidden internal
templates which shape our experience of the external world, and
determine its meaning for us. The rudimentary way of making sense
of the world by splitting it may to some extent be a spontaneous
feature of the infant mind – good = warm, satisfied, held, etc., while
bad = cold, hungry, alone, etc. Perhaps the polarity of good/bad is
its only way of ordering experience until it develops cognitively and
is able to make other and more complex discriminations.
But the lasting significance of splitting is in its power as an active
defence. It enables the infant – and later the adult – to find an object
of its experience which is entirely good, something in which it can
trust and in relation to which it therefore feels safe. Typically, this is an
image of the mother or primary caregiver, comprising only positive
elements of the infant’s experience of her. Those other sensations
and impressions of her which are associated with unsafety (via
negative experiences of pain, frustration or anxiety) are organised
into a separate object, the source of bad experience. In this way, our
inner world is structured by ambivalence. Our early experience of
the one reality outside us is split into two, so there is a good object,
which is loved, and a bad one, which is feared and hated. Both are
phantasies, though linked to elements of reality. Over time, this
duality of an idealised, perfect object and a denigrated, bad one will
be broken down by the cumulative effects of exposure to reality, by
experiences which demonstrate that there is actually just one person
out there who is the source of both types of experience. However, that
achievement depends on the infant having developed enough trust in
The heart of politics 11
the self – it is what he calls a core identity, which means that its stability
is of importance to the individual’s sense of well-being, even though
it is usually in the background of self-experience. The most important
large group identity for many people is national identity, though
active awareness of that is not a major feature of everyday life for
most people. Yet in his analyses of various national identities, Volkan
focusses more – inevitably perhaps given that his specialism has been
the understanding and resolution of conflict – on the regressive
potential of these large group identifications, and their role in the
creation and maintenance of conflicts. And generally, when social
scientists have turned to look at collective identities, they have often
found them to be at best mixed in their consequences. One of Volkan’s
many contributions has been to show how large group identities
become problematic when they are constructed or deployed defensively,
as a way of maintaining a sense of safety and/or dignity in response
to a crisis, loss or threat of some kind.
As well as its role in violent conflicts, the idea and the experience
of collective identity has functioned to support divisive and fragment-
ing political trends in recent years. While this is a complex area, and
benefits have come from ‘identity politics’ in the basic voice and dig-
nities it has brought to social groups which were hitherto effectively
excluded from the public sphere, a constant focus on sectional identi-
ties and interests can have a corrosive effect on democratic culture. If
a self-selected ‘identity’ is enough to merit a platform and proclaim
an agenda, then anyone can pile in and make demands. There is also
a telling irony in the way that the ‘identitarian’ movement of various
‘alt-right’ and anti-Islam groups in present-day Europe and the US
appears to have named itself after one of the signature concepts of
the 1968 generation of radicals, whose legacy of liberalism it sees as
deeply damaging and seeks to overturn.
The key point psychologically, however, is that defensive
mobilisations of large group identity (in which aggression is often
the chosen means of defence) do not arise spontaneously. They are
the result of threats to that identity, whether very real at political and
material levels, or very real in the shared phantasy life of the group.
The heart of politics 17
be faced and contained must then be dealt with using one of the
defence mechanisms which psychoanalysis has studied, two of which
have been mentioned above (splitting and projection), and others of
which will be described later. Defences are maladaptive and generate
problems both in an individual’s personal life and, when we deploy
them collectively, at organisational and societal levels.
These psychological resources of safety, dignity and containment
can be seen as the bases of the ‘emotional capital’ available in any
given society or community. The depth of these resources in a soci-
ety will depend on how much the adult individuals in that society
are in possession of them, which in turn will depend primarily on
what kind of growing up they experienced, how much it left them
able to feel safe, self-respecting and strong. A considerable burden in
trying to sustain emotional capital falls on the caring and therapeutic
professions who are charged with dealing with and trying to remedy
the consequences of damaged psychological development.5 Still, the
main factor will be how well the psycho-cultural make-up of a soci-
ety facilitates good emotional development, i.e. how the patterns of
family life, social relationships and everyday behaviours impact upon
the child’s development. In particular there is what we might call a
‘parental matrix’, comprising the cluster of primary caregivers (the
one, two or more people providing the main childcare, whether or
not they are actual parents) and their personalities, how they relate
to the baby/child, the situations of care, and the child’s experience
of it all. This matrix shapes what kind of people we are, how we
experience the world and feel about it, and therefore what levels of
emotional capital we can contribute to our society. Of course, societ-
ies are not homogeneous in this regard; there are huge variations in
emotional capital between families, let alone between different social
and cultural groups. But the overall levels of societal provision for
well-being, and cultural traditions, will affect every individual for
better or worse.
A society’s emotional resources will be especially called upon
when it is under stress or threat, as when a nation is in conflict with
another nation, or internally. Our emotional capital will influence
The heart of politics 19
unsafe, and by a yearning for the dignity which some people felt they
had lost as Britain became a post-industrial and less cohesive society.
This link in populist rhetorics between the principles of safety
and dignity on the one hand, and culturally conservative and divisive
politics on the other, may lead some people to be suspicious of a psy-
chological approach which argues for the importance of those two
principles. Surely we don’t want politicians to be encouraging us to
be fearful and preoccupied with safety. Nor do we want them pump-
ing us up with unexamined pride. This is an important point, and
sometimes a complex one, and we will encounter it again in his book.
But the main point here is that politics has to address psychological
realities as much as it does military and economic ones. Just a few
decades ago, it would not have seemed likely that some ‘inconvenient
truths’ about environmental change would be contesting for priority
in political agendas. And in this psychological age, politics has to cope
with a further major expansion of its core agendas. The global spread
of democracy, the proliferation of media, and the rise of ‘soft power’,
have added the inconvenience and intransigence of the human psyche
to the list of political ineluctables.
Understanding what people feel, and why they feel it, is a core task
of politics. If the experiences of safety and dignity are basic needs,
then feelings of unsafety and of indignity will not melt away if they
are ignored, not even if there are attempts to distract those suffering
from them by pointing to other things which they should be glad of.
In the British referendum of 2016, the Remain campaign, concen-
trating solely on economic arguments, completely failed to address
those feelings. There was perhaps a failure to respond to the fact that
many people could not see how EU membership brought them any
economic benefit, but in any case this strategy could be described
as emotionally illiterate: it failed to read the emotions of the public,
which were such that for many people the economic arguments were
not the most important.
In contrast, a key feature of populist politics is its tendency to
home in precisely on feelings of unsafety and humiliation in sections
of the public. Although in itself this falls far short of what we might
22 T h e h e a r t o f p o l i t i c s
In the rest of this chapter we will however focus more on the ‘fol-
lowership’ side of leadership. We will consider how broad changes in
society have affected leadership styles, by modifying what we want
and need to see in leaders. These changes bring the psychological
dimensions of leader-follower relations more into focus. We will dis-
cuss the question of whether leaders are made in the image of their
followers, or vice versa. Then we will look at some examples of how
leaders respond to their public’s needs for safety and dignity, and of
what containment they can offer of the anxieties around those needs.
LEADING BY EXAMPLE
Let’s continue to test the view that neither the media nor leaders
actually create their publics, but simply reflect them. Is this true at
moments of crisis, when there may be clear choices available to lead-
ers about how to lead? There are always different structures of feel-
ing8 present in the pool of public emotion, and at times of acute
disturbance and uncertainty when leaders are looked to for guidance
they may be able to choose which of these to express and support.
After a major terrorist attack, for example, there is grief, fear, rage and
resolve, and leaders’ choice of language in the aftermath shapes and
modulates some of these feelings more than others, with differing
consequences on a number of fronts, especially in relation to social
cohesion and to support for counter-terrorism policies. Two days after
77 terrorist murders in Oslo and on the island of Utoya in Norway
in July 2011, the then Norwegian Prime Minister Jens Stoltenberg in
a national memorial address spoke powerfully about the victims, and
then struck a remarkably positive note of optimistic resolve.
We can compare this with the quotations at the start of this chapter,
a few of the many statements made by the Rodrigo Duterte about
the ongoing crisis of massive drug use in his country, the Philip-
pines. The contrast between these two leaders demonstrates the fun-
damental role of public emotion in shaping leadership. Neither set
Leaders 35
rather than border security is often the guarantee of safety, via state
provision for health and welfare, while dignity may also be materi-
ally defined in terms of better pay, secure employment and housing,
rather than being seen culturally.
One feature which all varieties of ‘populism’, including the M5S,
have in common, and which therefore is often taken in academic
discussions of the term to be its defining characteristic, is a deep
disaffection with the political ‘establishment’. The populist leader’s
appeal is that of the outsider, someone untainted by the complacency
or corruption of the elite, someone who can be in touch with the
‘people’, and so really offers something different. Psychologically, this
is the wish for purity, a narcissistic impulse of reaching for the perfect
world, in response to a collapse of trust in politics in the real world.
As such, the enthusiasm for the new order, the ‘Golden Dawn’ prom-
ised by the extremist party of that name in Greece, is condemned to
eventual collapse, as the promise of purity cannot be delivered.
The extent to which contemporary populism is a new develop-
ment in leader-follower relations is debatable. Has there not for a long
time been an us vs. them at the heart of human society, ready to be
stirred up by leaders who believe in themselves as forces of renewal,
and now more easily stirred via the direct and fevered channels of
social media? Or are we seeing a new kind of leadership, facilitated
by modern communications but with a new emotional dynamic?
At the psychological level, there is nothing new about today’s pop-
ulisms. Their basic dynamic appears to be one in which the parental
figures (the incumbent political class, the ‘elite’) are felt to have com-
prehensively failed, even to have betrayed those for whom they are
responsible. A deep, amorphous anger fills the political air and people
feel various combinations of abandoned, deceived and exploited. As
we saw in Chapter 1, feelings of distrust and rage against parental fig-
ures of early life are universal. So how can we understand the rise of
cynicism and anger in popular attitudes towards politics, to the point
where elections are won by parties or leaders whose key promise is to
overthrow the establishment, to replace the actual, bad parents with
an ideal, good one?
Leaders 41
Essentialist theories of the nation and its ancient roots were rejected
later in the twentieth century by a second wave of theories, called
‘modernist’ by Smith (2009) because they see nations as the product
of the ‘modern’ period of history, to have come into being from
the seventeenth or eighteenth century on, without older historical
roots. Various accounts have been given of this process, for example,
those emphasising the role of capitalist economic development and
competition, or those which focus on the role of nationalisms as
reactions to global imperialism. In recent decades perhaps the most
influential have been what we can call ‘constructionist’ accounts,
according to which nations have emerged as a consequence of particular
socio-cultural changes. Foremost amongst these are the theories
of political scientist Benedict Anderson (1983) and philosopher/
anthropologist Ernest Gellner (1983), who considered the emerging
forces of, respectively, print media in the eighteenth century and
public education systems in the nineteenth century as major origins
of national consciousness in the first modern nations. While a people
may have lived under the same jurisdiction, they did not know they
were a ‘nation’ until their schools or their newspapers told them who
they were. In analyses of this sort, a sense of national identity and unity
is to some extent an artefact. It is an imaginary construction which has
been engineered in people’s minds, whether as a by-product of other
developments or as part of a deliberate strategy intended somehow to
serve the interests of political or economic elites.1
Illuminating and important though modernist theories may be,
from a psychological point of view we have to agree with Smith’s
judgment that they are limited by their neglect of the powerful affective
dimensions of nations and nationalism, a power seen abundantly in
international contests in sport, in diverse forms of political rhetoric,
and in many wars. There is a crucial imaginative component in all our
social experiences – a principle at the heart of the approach taken in
this book – but it is hard to explain the depth and intensity of national
feeling if we see the nation as an artifice, which can be de-constructed
and de-mystified. While there are many determinedly cosmopolitan
people who opt out of national identities, viewed globally it seems
48 N at i o n s
would be to turn away altogether from what may be seen as the whole
fraudulent business of nationhood and nationalism).
ANTIPATHIES TOWARDS
THE NATION-STATE
So except in those cases where a self-identified nation is not a state,
our attachments to nations are closely linked to our dependencies
on states. However, the state is the institutionalisation of leadership,
and so we have as deep an ambivalence towards the state as we do
towards leaders. Like many ambivalences, this is typically managed
by a division of emotional labour. Some people in a society spring
to the attack, while others leap to the defence. The most full-frontal
attack on the state has come from ‘neo-liberalism’, which we will
look at more closely in the next chapter, and focusses on a critique
of the state as the antithesis of freedom, especially the freedom for
markets to operate without restraint. However, libertarianisms of the
Left and the centre have also contributed substantially to spreading
suspicion of the state, for example, in opposition to what they see
as a sinisterly encroaching ‘security state’ which wants to establish
regimes of surveillance and control over all its citizens. Proposals for
N at i o n s 63
identity cards, where these don’t already exist, and the introduction
of counter-terrorism intelligence gathering, are foci for libertarian
protest, while security leaks and ‘whistle-blowing’ are applauded. In
the UK, the government’s counter-terrorism programme ‘Prevent’
has been the object of particular hostility as an intrusive ‘snooper’s
charter’. It is ironic that for some people ‘security’ now connotes the
opposite of its literal meaning, in that ‘securitisation’, for example,
refers to extensions of the state’s reach which are believed to put the
freedom and privacy of citizens at risk.
As well as fears of intrusion, the spectre of corruption is an
important component in many people’s perceptions of the state,
and one that is becoming more common in the established liberal
democracies where until recently the lack of trust in politics focussed
mainly on charges of incompetence, infighting and duplicity. For
example, in 2009 there was an outbreak across the UK of huge
opprobrium directed towards the chief political representatives of
the UK state, Members of Parliament, when some were found to have
used their expense accounts to pay for personal purchases which
should not have been covered by public funds. The amounts involved
were trivial compared to the bonuses which many people in the
private sector have been receiving for years, but the dishonesty of
the payments led to sustained moral outrage.
So the nation-state, and the ideas of ‘the government’ or
‘politicians’ or ‘the establishment’ with which it may be conflated,
are somewhat short of friends at the moment, and under attack from
various directions, at least in the liberal democracies. It could even
be suggested that a hatred of the state has become normative across
the West. There certainly is an ambivalence towards the nation-state,
which is the source of much tension and conflict within societies.
Psychologically, however, we find it easier to line ourselves up on
one side or the other, for or against the nation or the nation-state,
than to experience the ambivalence within ourselves, since that
brings all the discomforts of uncertainty and inner conflict. Turning
our internal ambivalences into external conflicts is often more
comfortable.
64 N at i o n s
NEO-LIBERALISM
At the centre of most ideologies is a statement of something which
is good, and a complementary identification of something which is
bad. An ideology consists, essentially, of a set of ideas in which the
good object is praised, and the bad object is criticised. The term ‘neo-
liberalism’ has become ever more widely and loosely used, but we
will employ it here in the hope that it does still denote a meaning-
ful phenomenon: a political standpoint or philosophy in which the
central principle is that free markets should be the basis of human
68 I d e o l o g i e s
public who in other respects can be much more positive about the
state, and negative about markets. This convergence is seen in hostile
working-class views about the state’s indulgence of so-called work-
shy citizens, its weakness in tackling benefit fraud, and its excessive
generosity towards immigrants, at the expense of the nonimmigrant
population. The villainous figure of the parent who neglects their
own child yet provides for other children, or perversely favours the
least deserving amongst its own, is the psychological source of some
of the rage around the state’s allocation of housing and benefits.
This critique of neo-liberal ideology does not uniquely patholo-
gise its proponents. A measure of discomfort around dependency is
common enough, and there are other ways of dealing with it apart
from converting it into a political outlook. So neo-liberalism is not
alone in the psychological defence it expresses or draws upon. Nor
does our critique demolish it intellectually, or invalidate it as a politi-
cal project. There are important and complex issues around the nature
of markets and their relationship to the state, which obviously can-
not be condensed into psychological analysis. The value of psycho-
logical work of this kind is that it offers a way of understanding the
emotional needs and the defences which people bring to the table
of politics. This is especially important when we do so in such a way
that the emotional dimension then becomes the predominant one
in determining our opinions and actions. And even when that is not
the case, to have some insight into the links between our politics and
our more hidden selves should nudge us towards a more thoughtful
political discourse.
COMMUNISM
While neo-liberalism as we know it did not emerge until the second
half of the twentieth century, the ideology of which it is the clearest
‘opposite’ was already on the wane by then, though it still exerts some
influence today – the ideology of communism. In the communist
countries of the twentieth century, the state was, in its fusion with
the Communist Party, a massively revered parental object, at least in
Ideologies 71
FASCISM
Interestingly, that could also be a description of the state of mind
expressed in fascist ideology. There, the longed-for ideal object is once
again linked to the state, although in fascism there is typically a char-
ismatic leader into whose persona the state (and party) is absorbed,
as is also the case in some communist examples, such as Mao Zedong
or the Kim dynasty. In his merger with the state (whether there could
be a female fascist leader is an important question we will touch on
shortly), the leader is mandated by the people to use the power of
the state to protect them and to make them strong. (The term ‘fas-
cism’ is derived from the Latin word fasces, bundles of sticks with an
axe projecting which were carried into the Roman court by officials
to represent the authority of the state.) The glorified, omnipotent
leader/state is a perversion of a protective father. The ‘goodness’ of the
fascist leader is an illusion based on splitting, but is seen by some to
offer a promise of safety in membership of the societal family.
So acute is the fear that even this family may not be safe that it
has to be defined in very restrictive terms, to keep out all possible
sources of threat. All totalitarian mindsets are sustained by a vision
of safety in purity. Potentially overwhelming anxieties about con-
tamination, collapse or some other threat to the self are managed
by projecting the threat into others who must then be kicked out or
kept out of the family. Even then, they can remain a threat, and so
the need for a final solution. While fascism has its enemies within
its own camp, its construction of an external bad object which must
be annihilated is one of the most extreme and destructive states of
political mind, though as other genocides show, this catastrophic phe-
nomenon can also develop without much of the rationalisation which
an ideology provides. ‘External’ in this context does not necessarily
74 I d e o l o g i e s
experience of any sort, let alone a specifically Islamic one, nor as the
outcome of studying Islamic texts.
For most people, it is barely comprehensible that such a creed
could attract as many people as it has done, and continues to do,
across the world. However, we can get closer to an understanding of
its appeal if we analyse it as an ideology reflecting a state of mind. If
we can get beyond the fear and loathing which it inevitably produces
in us, we can see that it gives voice to an overwhelmingly fearful
condition.
The appeal of ‘Islamist’ ideology rests on two psychological foun-
dations. The first is an internal state of great fear. The terrorist does not
feel at all safe, and the infliction of terror on others is an attempt to
prevent the terrorist’s own self being consumed by terror. At the core
of this state of mind is a fear of punishment, not in a reality-oriented
way as the consequence of something which the individual has actu-
ally done that is wrong, but as a background state of mind rooted in
unconscious phantasy. The phantasy is of a brutal and infinitely pow-
erful superego figure, which is constantly seeking absolute obedience
and is ready to punish savagely any deviation.
In their exhortations to potential recruits, and to each other,
Islamist terrorists refer constantly to the absolute necessity of obey-
ing the will of Allah. The rewards of doing so are unending, as are the
punishments for not doing so. Their Allah is a monstrous god who
insists on the annihilation of all non-Muslims, and who provides,
where it may be useful to do so, for this to be preceded by their sub-
jection to unspeakable torture. The category of non-Muslims includes
those many Muslims who takfiri Islamists do not recognize as ‘real’
Muslims (because they do not accept the terrorists’ primitive defini-
tion of Islam), so simple profession of faith is not enough. Safety
can be found only by obeying those who say that Allah demands this
commitment to the violent creation of the global caliphate, since
any Muslims who object that this is not Allah’s will cannot be real
Muslims. Anyone listening to those dissenting voices will be booking
their own passage to hell.
76 I d e o l o g i e s
LIBERAL DEMOCRACY
Let us turn now to the ideology which those of us living in lib-
eral democracies may not spontaneously think of as an ‘ideology’,
because it is our ‘common sense’, a set of principles which we often
take for granted as defining the best kind of society. If we take the
same approach to it as we have done to the various ‘-isms’, will we
arrive at the same sort of critique, the same exposure of the defences
and perversions to be seen in the state of mind which the ideology
expresses? To some extent, the answer is yes. One feature of this ideol-
ogy is its tendency to underplay the more destructive sides of human
nature, a tendency which can amount to denial, a defensive refusal to
acknowledge the extent to which antagonisms can develop or persist
even in situations where there is no external reason for them to do so.
The internal drivers of negativity and conflict sit uneasily in the more
rationalistic versions of liberal democratic thought, for which the
continuation of tensions around religious and ethnic differences in
basically free and democratic societies is hard to explain.
We might also detect a measure of narcissism in the individualistic
tone of some strains of this ideology (which, like the other major
ideological formations we have discussed, is actually a broad collec-
tion of different positions). Liberalism can offer space to neo-liberal
refusals of collective responsibility, although this might be attenu-
ated or partially displaced by genuine concern for others and by an
accompanying rhetoric of ‘one society’, as in the philosophy of the
82 I d e o l o g i e s
ENVIRONMENTAL CATASTROPHE
There is widespread denial about climate change. This is not the
denial of the climate change deniers, who are relatively few in
number; rather it is the attitude of the many who do not dispute
there could be a problem but turn away from it or distract them-
selves elsewhere. This is not outright cognitive denial, but a denial,
88 P r o s p e c t s
FAKE NEWS
Our political cultures are awash with suspicion of, and often con-
tempt for, those involved in politics. There is a broad cultural context
for this in the decline of deference, a process which has been going
on at least since the 1960s era of protest and counter-culture. So
this is not a recent development; by the 1990s a cynical outlook on
politics was established amongst many journalists and reflected in
the tone of their coverage in the mainstream media. But this century
has seen the negativity about politics become much wider and more
emphatic, not least because, in an ironic twist, the political media
themselves have become objects of deep suspicion. The concept of
‘fake news’ brings a new level of threat to democratic politics because
it attacks the legitimacy not of particular politicians or policies, but
of the whole public sphere of debate and deliberation, leaving people
free to withdraw from argument with anyone they disagree with,
and to ignore any critical scrutiny of their actions. In the age of the
web and social media, falsities can obviously proliferate freely, and
pose a major problem for democratic deliberation by confusing and
misleading us. But the even greater threat probably comes from the
accusations of falsity made in order to close down debate and inquiry,
in effect to close down politics.
While there are many examples of politicians who have acted in
ways that deserve public opprobrium, and of mendacious journalists,
something further is needed to push people into the incoherence
of the radically anti-political politics which have emerged in recent
years. It is as if we have become collectively unable to trust, an
attitude which if it gained sway over the economic, transactional
sphere of life would bring most things to a halt. It may yet do so
in the political sphere. How do we understand this crisis of trust?
Drawing on the approach taken in this book, we would assume that
there is always some form of internal driver alongside the external
ones. As many commentators have suggested, cynicism and hostility
towards elites is partly grounded in feelings of abandonment and
90 P r o s p e c t s
GROWING INEQUALITIES
Finally, there is the problem of growing inequality, focussed on in recent
years by, amongst many others, the geographer Danny Dorling, the
economist Thomas Piketty and the epidemiologists Richard Wilkinson
and Kate Pickett. The general emphasis in this book has been away
from economic issues, towards questions of leader-follower relations,
cultural identity and ideological mindset. ‘Economistic’ approaches
to politics have been unfavourably compared here with psychosocial
ones, alongside which they seem simplistic and rationalistic. So it may
seem surprising that we should end on the topic of socio-economic
inequality.
In discussing how the psychological defences of narcissism, split-
ting, projection and so on can shape our experience of politics, we
noted the origins of these defences in experiences of unsafety and
humiliation. If it were possible to make early and skilled interventions
into the family lives of all babies and young children whose emotional
development was at risk, our politics, and societies as a whole, might
look a lot brighter. Hopefully, access to sophisticated psychotherapeu-
tic help will increase across the world, with benefits to individuals
and their communities in all areas of life, and this ought to be a strate-
gic political priority. But the problems we have been describing in this
book, though located in the minds of individuals, need addressing
at systemic and primary level; they are too endemic to be dealt with
otherwise. The most fundamental primary level is that of culture, and
how cultural values shape parenting and family life. While cultures
are always changing, changing them by design in specific directions
is likely to be a slow and uncertain process. However, some cultural
values can be strongly influenced by social arrangements, and some
of those can be directly changed through political action.
Specifically, economic inequality can be changed through politics,
and there are reasons to think that reducing inequality would impact
on the socio-cultural life of a society in ways that would change the
values and the experiences of people in that society. These changes
92 P r o s p e c t s
Hoggett, P. (2009) Politics, Identity and Emotion. Boulder, CO: Paradigm. An exploration
in more scholarly detail of many of the key issues in this book and
others.
Lindholm, C. (1990) Charisma. Oxford: Blackwell. An integration of sociological
and psychoanalytic thinking about charisma, with case studies.
Lindner, E. (2006) Making Enemies: Humiliation and International Conflict. Westport, CT:
Praeger Publishers. One of the few psychological studies which puts dig-
nity/humiliation at the centre of its theory.
Mintchev, N. & Hinshelwood, R.D. (2017) The Feeling of Certainty: Psychosocial Perspec-
tives on Identity and Difference. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave. Psychoanalytic
essays on the psychic bases of prejudice, dogma and fundamentalism.
Mitchell, J., ed. (1986) The Selected Melanie Klein. Harmondsworth: Penguin. A col-
lection of many key readings in the Kleinian development of Freud.
Richards, B. (2007) Emotional Governance: Politics, Media and Terror. Basingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan. Offers a framework for understanding public feeling, with
responses to terror as a case study.
Rustin, M. (1991) The Good Society and the Inner World. London: Verso. Essays on Klei-
nian psychoanalysis, politics and philosophy by a leading psychosocial
thinker.
Volkan, V. (2004) Blind Trust: Large Groups and their Leaders in Times of Crisis and Terror. Char-
lottesville, VA: Pitchstone Publishing. Volkan’s theory of large group
dynamics, with many major case studies.
Volkan, V., Itzkowitz, N. & Dod, A. (1997) Richard Nixon: A Psychobiography. New York:
Columbia University Press. Another demonstration of how psychoanalytic
psychobiography can help make sense of individual leadership.
Winnicott, D. (1986) Home Is Where We Start From: Essays by a Psychoanalyst. Harmond-
sworth: Penguin. Very readable and wide-ranging essays by a key figure in
post-Freudian psychoanalysis.
Yates, C. (2015) The Play of Political Culture, Emotion and Identity. Basingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan. Links psychoanalysis with cultural studies in the analysis of
electoral politics.
NOTES
CHAPTER 1
1 For a number of critiques of this rationalism, see Hoggett and Thompson
(2012).
2 This phrase, famously coined by the Hungarian/American psychiatrist and
psychoanalyst Thomas Szasz in 1961, was at the centre of the critiques of
psychiatry which in the late twentieth century began to have some influ-
ence on professional practices in the mental health field, though medicalised
approaches continue to be predominant.
3 We can see here the meaning of the term ‘object relations theory’. It derives
from the philosophical terminology in which a relationship is seen as com-
prising a ‘subject’ who is in relation to an ‘object’. It is a view of the mind
as consisting of the set of relationships which different parts of the self have
with people and things in the external world and with each other, i.e. with
the various ‘objects’ of their experience.
4 This conjunction of splitting and projection is expressed in the formidable
technical phrase ‘paranoid-schizoid position’, which is the Kleinian term for
the earliest states of mind, and regression to which is the core problem in
adult psychological functioning.
5 For a searching examination of social work in this regard, see Cooper (2018).
98 N o t e s
CHAPTER 2
1 www.aljazeera.com/news/2017/02/duterte-kill-rid-drugs-170202073247
477.html, accessed 27.7.18.
2 www.reuters.com/article/us-philippines-drugs/philippines-duterte-says-
hes-been-demonized-over-drugs-war-idUSKBN1CU1QY, accessed 27.7.18.
3 www.hrw.org/tag/philippines-war-drugs, accessed 27.7.18.
4 See www.ipsos.com/ipsos-mori/en-uk/politicians-remain-least-trusted-
profession-britain, accessed 11.9.18.
5 See https://cms.edelman.com/sites/default/files/2018-01/2018%20Edel-
man%20Trust%20Barometer%20Global%20Report.pdf, accessed 11.9.18.
6 This was in his essay ‘Politics as a Vocation’, reprinted in From Max Weber. Essays
in Sociology.
7 For many of us there is some degree of difference between our outward per-
sona and inner character, and this difference is likely to be greater for public
figures whose public image is deliberately crafted. Still, the outside is usually
heavily influenced by the inside, so basically we are looking at the relationship
between leader character and public emotion. I choose to use the term ‘charac-
ter’ here instead of ‘personality’. In some contexts the two are synonymous, but
‘personality’, since it is often assessed by behavioural checklists and self-report,
may be less able to capture the idea of a deep inner self. Also ‘character’ has con-
notations of the person as a moral agent, and so is appropriate for psychological
analysis in political contexts, especially in the study of leaders.
8 This phrase, an important one in the psychosocial literature, was first
deployed by the cultural theorist Raymond Williams – see his 1977 book
Marxism and Literature.
9 www.americanrhetoric.com/speeches/jensstoltenbergbombingmemorial.
htm, accessed 27.7.18.
10 Fear: Trump in the White House (2018), the veteran journalist’s intensively
researched book on the Trump administration, was an instant best-seller.
CHAPTER 3
1 The influence of this constructionist view is also due to the book The Invention
of Tradition, edited by Eric Hobsbawm and Terence Ranger, in which historians
consider the construction of various nationhoods in Britain, the British
Empire and Europe.
2 Greenfeld’s very readable 2016 book, Advanced Introduction to Nationalism,
summarises the conclusions drawn from many years of research previously
set out in three major books, work which is inevitably over-simplified here.
Notes 99
CHAPTER 4
1 Not all Marxists would agree with this simplification. Some have wanted
to define Marxism as a science, which complicates matters because science is
widely seen as the antithesis of all ideology. Some readers of this book will
want to know whether the psychology which it is based on is scientific or
not. That is a very reasonable question, since we should always be checking
the veracity of things we read or hear. But we would have to clarify what is
meant by ‘scientific’, some definitions of which are too narrow. In the human
and social sciences, different forms of inquiry should not all be subject to
the same methodological strictures. Ultimately, the main question is whether
they generate beneficial understandings of human behaviour which can help
to improve how we do things.
2 See Fromm (1949).
3 Macpherson (1962), p. 3.
4 http://cbi.typepad.com/files/full-translation-5-yr-plan-2011-2015.doc,
accessed 21.7.18.
5 See, for example, Theweleit (1987) and Wieland (2015).
6 This analysis is set out in more detail in my chapters in the books edited by
Adlam et al. (2018 Vol. 1) and Mintchev and Hinshelwood (2017).
7 See Gray (2003).
8 See Elias (1939).
9 See, for example, Gerodimos (2015), for an analysis of the belief and state-
ments of anarchist groups who rioted for three weeks in December 2008.
CHAPTER 5
1 See Hoggett (2011) for a discussion of some of the complexities involved in
this, and Weintrobe (2013) for a selection of articles on the psychology of
climate change.
REFERENCES
Adlam, J., Kluttig, T. & Lee, B., eds. (2018) Violent States and Creative States. 2 vols.
London: Jessica Kingsley.
Anderson, B. (1983) Imagined Communities. Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism.
London: Verso.
Bar Tal, D. (2013) Intractable Conflicts. Socio-Psychological Foundations and Dynamics.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bowlby, J. (1988) A Secure Base. Abingdon: Routledge, revised edn., 2005.
Cooper, A. (2018) Conjunctions. Social Work, Psychoanalysis, and Society. Abingdon:
Routledge.
Elias, N. (1939) The Civilising Process. Oxford: Blackwell, revised edn., 1994
Erikson, E. (1950) Childhood and Society. Harmondsworth: Penguin, revised edn.,
1965.
Frank, J. (2004) Bush on the Couch. Inside the Mind of the US President. London:
Politico’s.
Frank, J. (2012) Obama on the Couch: Inside the Mind of the President. New York: Free Press.
Freud, S. (1921) Group psychology and the analysis of the ego. In Penguin Freud
Library,Vol. 12: Civilization, Society and Religion. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1991.
Fromm, E. (1949) Man for Himself. London: Ark, revised edn., 1986.
Gellner, E. (1983) Nations and Nationalism. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Gerodimos, R. (2015) The ideology of far left populism in Greece: Blame, vic-
timhood, and revenge in the discourse of Greek anarchists. Political Studies
63, 608–625.
102 R e f e r e n c e s
Gray, J. (2003) Al Qaeda and What it Means to be Modern. London: Faber & Faber.
Greenfeld, L. (2016) Advanced Introduction to Nationalism. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
Hayek, F. (1944) The Road to Serfdom. London: Ark Paperbacks, revised edn., 1986.
Hobsbawm, E. & Ranger, T., eds. (1983) The Invention of Tradition. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press, revised edn., 1992.
Hoggett, P. (2011) Climate change and the apocalyptic imagination. Psychoanalysis,
Culture and Society 16(3), 261–275.
Hoggett, P. & Thompson, S. (2012) Politics and the Emotions. London: Continuum.
Honneth, A. (1995) The Struggle for Recognition, Cambridge: Polity.
Lauenstein, O., Murer, J., Boos, M. & Reicher, S. (2015) ‘Oh motherland I pledge
to thee . . .’: A study into nationalism, gender and the representation of an
imagined family within national anthems. Nations and Nationalism 21(2),
309–329.
Lindner, E. (2006) Making Enemies: Humiliation and International Conflict. Westport, CT:
Praeger Publishers.
Macpherson, C.B. (1962) The Political Theory of Possessive Individualism. Hobbes to Locke.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Margalit, A. (1996) The Decent Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mintchev, N. & Hinshelwood, R.D. (2017) The Feeling of Certainty. Psychosocial Perspec-
tives on Identity and Difference. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave.
Richards, B. (2017) What Holds Us Together. London: Karnac.
Sandler, J. (1987) From Safety to Superego. Selected Papers of Joseph Sandler. New York:
Guilford Press.
Sennett, R. (2003) Respect: The Formation of Character in an Age of Inequality.
Harmondsworth: Penguin, revised edn., 2004.
Smith, A. (2009) Ethno-Symbolism and Nationalism: A Cultural Approach. Abingdon:
Routledge.
Szasz, T. (1961) The Myth of Mental Illness. New York: Harper, revised edn., 2010.
Theweleit, K. (1987) Male Fantasies. Cambridge: Polity.
Volkan, V. (2001) Transgenerational transmission and chosen traumas: An aspect
of large-group identity. Group Analysis 34, 79–97.
Volkan, V. (2004) Blind Trust. Large Groups and their Leaders in Times of Crisis and Terror.
Charlottesville, VA: Pitchstone Publishing.
Volkan, V., Itzkowitz, N. & Dod, A. (1997) Richard Nixon. A Psychobiography. New York:
Columbia University Press.
References 103
Weber, M. (1919) From Max Weber. Essays in Sociology. Abingdon: Routledge, revised
edn., 2009.
Weintrobe, S., ed. (2013) Engaging with Climate Change: Psychoanalytic and Interdisciplinary
Perspectives. London: Routledge.
Wieland, C. (2015) The Fascist State of Mind and the Manufacturing of Masculinity. Hove:
Routledge.
Wilkinson, R. & Pickett, K. (2009) The Spirit Level. London: Penguin.
Williams, R. (1977) Marxism and Literature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Winnicott, D. (1986) Home Is Where We Start From. Essays by a Psychoanalyst.
Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Woodward, B. (2018) Fear:Trump in the White House. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Wouters, C. (2007) Informalization: Manners and Emotions since 1890. London: Sage.
Yates, C. (2015) The Play of Political Culture, Emotion and Identity. Basingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan.