Child - Language - Development - Kel 5 (1) - 1
Child - Language - Development - Kel 5 (1) - 1
Child - Language - Development - Kel 5 (1) - 1
Arranged by group 5:
2024
FOREWORD
Praise be to God Almighty, who has provided us with strength and health, enabling us
to complete this paper on the topic of child language development, specifically focusing
on the one-word stage. This paper is written as part of our efforts to better understand the
early stages of linguistic development in children and its critical role in shaping their
communication skills. Language is one of the most essential tools for human interaction,
and its development begins at a very young age. The one-word stage is a fundamental
phase where children first begin to express themselves verbally, marking a significant
milestone in their cognitive and communicative abilities. Through this paper, we hope to
explore the key linguistic features of this stage and its importance in the overall language
acquisition process.
The author would like to express our deepest gratitude to all those who have
supported and encouraged us throughout the process of writing this paper. Special thanks
go to our lecturers, family, and friends for their valuable feedback, guidance, and
motivation. The author realize that this paper is far from perfect, and we welcome any
suggestions or constructive criticism for its improvement. We hope this paper will
provide useful insights into child language development and serve as a reference for
future research in the field.
Authors
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
During the one-word stage, children tend to use nouns and verbs more
frequently than other word classes. This phenomenon is referred to as noun and
verb predominance. Nouns are often used to refer to familiar people, objects, or
animals, such as "mama", "papa", "ball", and "cat". At this stage, children begin to
associate words with the objects around them, helping them understand that each
item has a name or label. These words are typically related to things that are
important in the child’s daily life, reflecting their immediate environment and
experiences.
Verbs also emerge during this stage, though they are usually fewer in number
compared to nouns. Verbs are often used by children to indicate actions that occur
in their surroundings, such as "fall", "eat", or "run". Children learn to use these
verbs because they frequently engage with these actions or because the actions are
critical in fulfilling their needs, like asking for help or food. The significance of
verbs at this stage lies in how children begin to understand the relationship
between subjects (people or objects) and the actions they perform. The
predominance of nouns and verbs indicates the child's rapid cognitive
development in understanding the world around them. It also shows how children
begin to categorize their environment. Nouns and verbs serve as building blocks
for the child to form basic concepts about people, objects, and activities, which
later form the foundation for more complex sentences. (Cruttenden, 1980)
2. Intonation
Although children in the one-word stage only use a single word per utterance,
they rely heavily on intonation to convey additional meaning. Intonation refers to
the rise and fall in the tone of voice that children use to express meaning or
emotion. In this context, intonation becomes a crucial part of the child’s pragmatic
communication, allowing them to express requests, questions, confusion, or
statements by varying their tone of voice. For instance, when a child says "mama"
with a rising intonation at the end of the word, it might indicate a question, such as
asking where their mother is or seeking attention. On the other hand, if the child
says "mama" with a falling and more relaxed intonation, it could simply mean
they are identifying the presence of their mother or feeling content with her
presence. These variations in intonation enable the child to communicate different
intents, even when limited to just one word.
Furthermore, intonation often serves as a cue for adults to interpret the child’s
desires or needs. For example, a higher pitch may indicate urgency or a request,
while a lower pitch may convey affirmation or recognition. This demonstrates that
children are not only learning the sounds of words but also how to use those
sounds effectively to communicate. Intonation plays a key role in allowing
children to extend their communicative abilities beyond the constraints of a single
word.(Bloom, 1970)
3. Contextual Use
- Contextual Dependence
Children during the one-word stage often use words in specific contexts,
relying heavily on the environment or situation to convey meaning. This
reliance on context is a hallmark of their early language development. For
example a child might say "ball" while pointing at a ball on the floor. In this
context, the child is indicating their desire to play with the ball. This example
illustrates how the child uses the word "ball" in a situation where it is most
relevant, relying on the surrounding environment to convey their intended
meaning.
- Situational Cues
Parents and caregivers play a crucial role in providing situational cues that
help children understand and use words correctly. They often repeat the word
in context, which helps the child associate the word with its meaning. Impact
on Language Development This interaction supports vocabulary growth and
comprehension. For instance, when a caregiver says "ball" while pointing to a
ball, the child learns that "ball" refers to an object used for play. This repeated
exposure helps solidify the word's meaning in the child's vocabulary.(Lestari et
al., n.d.)
The words used by children in the one-word stage are often simple, but they
carry rich meaning. A single word uttered by a child can represent an entire
sentence or more complex intention. For example, when a child says "juice", it
might mean "I want juice" or "Where is the juice?" This shows that children in
this stage often use words holophrastically, where a single word encapsulates the
meaning of an entire sentence depending on the context.
Other examples include the word "car", which can be used by the child to
identify a vehicle or to ask where the car is. The word "more" is often used by
children to express their desire for something to be repeated, such as asking for
more food or requesting to play a game again. In these cases, a single word uttered
by the child can have a range of meanings depending on the situation and
interaction taking place. The diversity of meaning from one word demonstrates
the child’s developing pragmatic skills, where they begin to understand how
words can be used in social contexts to meet their communicative needs. Adults
around the child often interpret the meaning of the word based on the context or
the child’s behavior, which helps clarify the child’s communication even though it
is still limited to single words. Additionally, through repeated use of these words,
children begin to learn more about language structure and how words can convey
more complex meanings.
b. Under extension
Underextension is the opposite of overextension. Children who experience
underextension use words to refer to objects or concepts that are narrower than
their actual meaning. For example, the child only calls their own pet dog a “dog”
and does not call other dogs they meet. This shows that the child has a very
specific and limited understanding of the meaning of the word.
Underextension can happen because children are just learning to speak and are
still building their vocabulary. They may not yet have many words to describe
different types of objects and concepts. Therefore, they may only use words that
they already know to refer to very specific objects. Underextension can also occur
due to the child’s personal experience. A child who only knows one type of dog
may only use the word “dog” to refer to that type of dog.
The one-word stage, also known as the holophrastic stage, is a crucial phase in
early language development occurring between 12 to 18 months. During this
stage, children begin to use single words to express entire thoughts or needs, such
as saying "milk" to indicate they want milk or "up" to be lifted. This stage marks
the transition from non-verbal to verbal communication, highlighting significant
cognitive and social growth as infants learn to convey complex ideas with limited
vocabulary(Jaelani, 2019). The one-word stage, also known as the holophrastic
stage, is a critical period in early language development where children begin to
use single words to convey complex thoughts and ideas. This stage is significant
for several reasons:
Vocabulary Development:
During this stage, children learn and use a limited number of words, which are
often nouns and verbs, to express their needs and wants. For example, a child
might say 'milk' to indicate a desire for milk or 'daddy' to refer to their father.
Pronunciation Development:
Infants in this stage begin to master the pronunciation of vowels and consonants.
They typically start with easy speech sounds like vowels and then move on to
consonants, which are more complex to produce.
Interpretation Challenges:
The one-word stage can be challenging for adults to interpret accurately because
the child's intention may not always be clear. Infants use body language and
context to convey their meaning, which can sometimes lead to misinterpretation.
This stage lays the foundation for future language skills, as children gradually
move from using single words to combining them into more complex sentences.
The one-word stage is essential for developing the ability to form all the sounds
they can hear and for understanding the semantic relationships between words.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
https://doi.org/10.7916/D8S75GT1
2(2).
Lestari, N., Salsabila, N., & Noviyanti, S. (n.d.). Perkembangan Pemerolehan Bahasa
pada Anak Usia Dini. JECER (Journal Of Early Childhood Education And
Keffer, S. (2023). Overextension - when toddlers use 1 word for everything. Toddler
Talk.
https://toddlertalk.com/blog/overextension
Kay, D. A., & Anglin, J. M. (2008,). Overextension and Underextension in the Child’s
Expressive and Receptive Speech : Journal of Child Language. Cambridge
Core.
https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Kamhi, A. G. (2008, September 26). Overextensions and Underextensions: How
Different are
They?: Journal of Child Language.
https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/journal-of-child-language