Patterns of Nsenga Kalimba Music BLACKING
Patterns of Nsenga Kalimba Music BLACKING
Patterns of Nsenga Kalimba Music BLACKING
h
JOHN BLACKING
Introduction:
The recordings on which this analysis is based, were made between July 24th and
August 7th, 1961, in the Petauke District of the Eastern Province of Northern Rhodesia.
Some of them appear on Side 1 of Volume II in the series of three long-playing records,
Music from Petauke, published by Ethnic Folkways Library, New York.
Dr. Raymond Apthorpe, in his capacity as Research Sociologist of the Rhodes-
Livingstone Institute, Lusaka, had already done several months’ fieldwork amongst
the Nsenga, when he suggested to the acting director of the Institute, Mr. C. M. N.
White, M.B.E., that I should be invited to make a brief, but concentrated, study of
Nsenga music. I am most grateful to all those who have made this study possible, and
especially to the Council of the University of the Witwatersrand, whose Research
Committee made a most generous grant to enable me to have a Nagra IIIC Portable
Tape Recorder, and to cover some of the costs of preparing this analysis. Above all, I
should like to thank the Nsenga musicians who responded patiently and graciously to
my enquiries about their art.
The kalimba and ndimba hand-pianos are distinguished chiefly by the layout of their
keys (sec Fig. 2). Fig. 1 shows a kalimba with calabash resonator, on which tunes I to IV
were played. Its manufacture is similar to that of the Lala Kankowela described by A. M.
Jones in Africa, XX, 3, 1950, p. 324 if. The Nsenga instruments differ from the Lala
in their shape and the number of their metal keys, and in the covering of the calabash
resonator, which is made of animal skin fastened with wooden pegs, instead of with
sisal fibre. The Nsenga instruments vary in length from 4^ to 5f inches, the average
being about 5 inches. They are played with the thumbs only: the soundboard is held
by the two extended first fingers, and the resonator is held underneath with the remaining
fingers of both hands, so that it does not quite touch the soundboard.
considered to be in tune, and even the pitch of those keys that were tuned for performance
was not always corrected between items, although it sometimes slipped as a result of
vigorous playing. Until I have at my disposal satisfactory instruments for measuring the
exact pitches of the keys, I am quite satisfied with the approximate indications given by
the staff notation. As a matter of fact, the tuning of some instruments was remarkably
close to the European scale, and in any case performers were not always unduly concerned
by small variations in pitch; for instance, the player of XVI and XVII, who was consid
ered to be good, was not disturbed because the e* on his kalimba (No. 5) was distinctly
a quarter-tone flat, and he happily sang e in tune whilst playing it flat on the kalimba.
The diagrams and the numbering of the ‘fingering’ have been standardised to the
pattern of the more common, fourteen-note instruments. Thirteen-and-twelve-note
instruments usually lack keys 9, and 9 and 2 respectively; I came across one instrument
(No. 6, for tunes XVIII and XIX) which lacked key 7, but this was considered excep
tional.
The ‘fingering’ has been based on European piano fingering, although only the
thumbs are used, because contrary motion of the thumbs is more common than parallel
motion and is, in any case, inherent in the layout of the keys of the kalimba'. thus the
sequence of ‘fingering’, R—1 2 3 4 represents contrary motion of the thumbs, whilst
L—1 2 3 4
R—1 2 3 4 represents parallel motion. Similarly, in the graphic illustrations of thumb
L—4 2 3 1
movement which appear after the transcriptions, the contrary motion of tune I can be
distinguished at a glance from the parallel motion of Tune VI. It is hoped that this type
of graphic illustration may prove useful in comparing the music of hand-pianos from
different parts of Africa.
The kalimba is most frequently played by youths, who repeat simple phrases over
and over again, as they walk alone, or with friends. They are, of course much concerned
with girls and courting: although they generally play ‘walking songs’ (akimbo %ojendala-
mu) without words, such as tune XX, they give them titles which express the emotions
that either underlay their composition, or are at least associated with their performance.
The music of such songs appears to be entirely abstract, and the same type of brisk
tune can convey equally the resentment that the player feels when his girl goes off in a
richer man’s car (No. 1), or his pleasure when she shows that she loves him (No. III).
In many cases, the apparently different tunes are in fact rhythmic variations on a nuclear
pattern of ‘fingering’ (e.g. Nos. I and II), or even a complete pattern of ‘fingering’
(e.g. Nos. VIII and IX).
The ndimba seems to be an instrument for adults who are semi-professional
musicians: I never heard a walking song played on it, and its melodies are either ‘trans
criptions of songs normally heard in other contexts, such as the funeral song XV, or
‘songs for kalimbd* (n^imbo %a kalimba), such as XIII and XVI, which are frequently
composed by the performer. The layout of its keys renders it more suitable for melodic
work than the kalimba, which is ideally suited for ‘harmonic’ figuration, and this contrast
can be seen by a comparison of transcriptions I to X with XIII and XVII. The chief
differences between the two instruments, whose keys are tuned similarly, may be sum
marised as follows—though the features should not be regarded as mutually independent;
Kalimba: Ndimba:
For youths. For adults.
Generalized (most youths play). Specialized(semi-professional).
Private performance. Public performance (e.g. accompanying
songs at beer-parties, etc.).
‘Harmonic’ figuration and rhythmic Melodic basis of tunes, long periods,
variations, short periods.
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 29
plains why the constitute the bottom three notes of every scale. ‘Harmonic’ shifts away
from the tonic to intervals or broken chords that include these notes, drive the tunes on
and aehive an effect of shifting tonality, although there is no modulation.
nA B Q o o „
m :.
• o ------ ©-.....-
t| A 3“ Z J Z V 3 - f -
—
4 s* 1: i
O ° O O o
Z 4- Z 3 Z X
if f - : : ° g (* § ) s ° o O • 1
' —— O ' • © ° • R
-6-
Fig. 3. (A) ‘Harmonic’ framework of tune I, transposed into C.
(B) Rudimentary ‘harmonic’ fluctuation based on the tonic, mediant
and dominant.
The black notes in (A) and (B) are those generally played on the
kalimba with the left thumb; ‘fingering’ is indicated below.
(C) Synthesis of all the intervals played on the kalimba in the transcrip
tions. The black notes signify those that are most commonly used.
Tune 1 is a good example of shifting tonality ,and Fig. 3A shows its basic ‘harmonic’
framework. Fig. 3B shows the most rudimentary form of ‘harmonic’ fluctuation based
on the tonic, mediant and dominant; and a comparison of this with 3C shows that it is
these intervals which are in fact most frequently used. In 3A and 3B, the notes played
by the left thumb provide an ostinato-like figure which binds together the ‘harmonic’
fluctuations.
The left-thumb part of tune XII is closely related to the sung melody, and it was the
left-thumb part which was crucial in the correct performance of XIX (see detailed notes
on transcriptions); furthermore, there is more similarity between the left-thumb than the
right-thumb parts of different kalimba melodies. This all suggests that the left-thumb
part provides the nuclear theme of the music, though I was unable to clarify this by
conversation with Nsenga musicians. Fig. 3 shows its importance in the control of
‘harmonic’ fluctuations, and in addition Jones’s evidence that the left-thumb part of the
Lala kankowela reduplicates the melody {op. cit. p. 333) may be cited as evidence in support
of its thematic function in Nsenga kalimba music. The function of the right-thumb part,
except in ndimba music and those kalimba tunes which merely reduplicate vocal melodies,
such as No. XI, is therefore to embroider the nuclear pattern produced by the left thumb.
Thus we may say that for the Nsenga kalimba player the tunes are very frequently thyth-
mical variations on a pattern whose significance is primarily ‘harmonic’, rather than
melodic.
The analysis of overt intervals showed no relationship between Nsenga vocal
melodies and the kalimba tunes, but an analysis of the respective roles of the left and
right thumbs, and of the ‘harmonic’ foundations of the tunes has revealed certain tonal
principles that need not be specially related to the structure of the instrument, but may
rather be fundamental features of the Nsenga musical tradition. Although the structure
of several kalimba melodies is much affected by the physical layout of the instrument and
the way in which the thumbs move over the keys, the physical layout, in turn, may be
designed to meet the demands of certain tonal principles. It now remains to be seen to
what extent these tonal principles are typical of the main body of Nsenga music.
Notes on in dividual item s.
The numbers of the transcriptions are followed by (a) their numbers on the original tapes, which
are stored in the Ethnological Museum, University of the Witwatersrand, e.g. N8/31a (the numbers
represent the original sequence of performance); and (b) their numbers in the three records of Music
from Petauke, published by Ethnic Folkways Library, New York: since all the kalimba recordings appear
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 31
on Side 1 of Volume II, their cut numbers are simply preceded by the letters EF, and a dash indicates
any of the original taped items that does not appear on the record, e.g. EF/2a, —, b, c.
The names of performers are followed by their clan-names in brackets.
The transcriptions are based both on the recordings and on the instruction I received in kalimba
playing from Nsenga informants. I have not given any of the doodles with which several players begin
their performances: these usually consist of a run down the scale and then a few intervals played at random,
but always emphasising the key of the tune to be played. If the music of African hand-pianos is to be
understood and comparative studies are to be made, methods of transcription must be standardised: I
have devised a system of writing the right-and-left-thumb parts under the complete tune, so that the
physical foundations of the tune may be discovered, and the graphic illustrations are designed to show the
patterns of movement of the thumbs. I hope that these methods may provide a satisfactory basis on which
other workers may improve.
T unes I to IV (N8/31a, b, c, d: EF/2a, —, b, c).
Played by Ackson Zuly (Ng’oma) of Endawalo village, on the 14-note kalimba wrhich appears in the
photograph (Fg. 1), on 31/7/61.
He called these walking songs respectively:-
I. She has ridden in Daligeni’s motor-car.
II. Come, let us go together.
III. Wait, and I w ill open up for you.
IV. Come, man, let’s dance jive.
1 and II are closely related by nuclear patterns of ‘fingering’:-
1. R—(2c4b2) 42b II. R—lb (2c4b2) b
L—(2 4 2 3) L—21 (2 423) 1
The left-thumb ‘lingering’ of III and IV is similar—43 (2432) and (2432) 43 respectively. (This
might also be compared with the sequence (324) (324) in XII). In general, the thumbs move in contrary
motion: in I1IA, the right-thumb elaborates the general drift of contrary motion with parallel motion.
The sequences on the recorded performances of III and IV were:-
III. (3A + B + C )+ (4.4+ 2(B + D)+2(6 a 1+ 1(D+B) )+ (ll> l+ 2 (D + B ) )+ 2 A.
IV. 2A + 9B+ 4A + 7B + 4C+ 2A .
T unes V to X (N8/33c, d, b, e, f, a,: EF/lc, d, b, e, f, a).
Played by Taiad (‘tired’) Mwanza (Banda) of M awilili village, on a 14-note kalimba on 31v7/61.
The titles of the songs were:-
V. Good morning, mother of Booi.
VI. There are not many of you who are after women.
VII. Let us run out of the rain.
VIII. The wife should kill a chicken, the husband a cow.
IX. 1. Corn that is small gets dry soon.
2. I have been chewing you for a long time.
X. The house which has no child.
VIII and IX were originally played consecutively: they have identical patterns of ‘fingering’, and
differ only in rhythm. V has the same tempo as IX, and an almost identical rhythm; its pattern of fingering
is basically the same, though what is the first thumb-movement of IX occurs at the end of the phrase:-
V. R—(21421/21431)
L—(2414/2413)
VIII. R—431)(2/4212
L—133) (24/1424
IX. R—431)(2/4212
L—13)(24/1424
The nuclear pattern for the left thumb also occurs in VI (i.e. 2414). In general, parallel motion of the
thumbs is used more in tunes V to X than in I to IV.
Mwanza began both VIII and IX by playing 14, instead of 13, with the left thumb.
T unes XI, XII, and XIIA (N2/12b, 12a, and Nl/lb: EF/5b, —, —).
XI and XII played by Ackson Lungu (Ng’oma) of Congololo village, on a 12-note kalimba, on
26/7/61.
XIIA played by a youth of a Congololo village, on a 13-note kalimba, on 25/7/61.
XI is a funeral song, whose text means, ‘Camba has gone away, alas"
The words of XII are:-
1. You have confused me, my orphan child.
2. When I grow up, I will marry a European woman.
The topical words of XIIA are based on those of XII, and refer to a European farmer whose aero
plane crashed:-
‘Van der Steen, you have lost your aeroplane’.
The period of XI is unusual—2(4+ 5)+ 2(4+ 4 dotted crotchets. Although it is entirely different in
style from XII, both songs use the same nuclear patterns for the left thumb—324) and 224): there is also
considerable similarity between the right-thumb patterns:-
XI. —b c 4 2 c 4/2.
32 AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
/ 1 f l k. k
in sumbali tstosi, though with the same ‘fingering’, 2432. L—(2432) also appears as a nuclear pattern in
tunes III and IV.
Tune XX (N l/la.).
This was the first kalimba tune that 1 heard in Petauke, played on a badly tuned instrument at Con
gololo village, on 25/7/61. It was a ‘walking song’ with a very simple structure; but it embraces the
basic principle of polyrhythm between left and right thumbs, which applies to almost all kalimba tunes
•—R-(b2c4), found in I, II and XII; and L—(2413), found in V, VIII and IX. R—(b2c4) is, in fact, as
easily played, ‘natural’ sequence on the kalimba, in which the thumb slides from the short to the long keys,
first to the left and then to the right.
N ote: There is a considerable difference between the relative percentages of ascending and descending
intervals. When these are taken separately, there is a notable similarity in the frequency of thirds,
but differences between the frequencies of seconds and intervals greater than the third, the most
marked of which is the high percentage of fourths in the kalimba tunes.
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 33
TABLE 1
Comparative interval frequencies of k-alimba tunes 1 to XII and vocal melodies IX
and XI to XVII.
Percentage Percentage
Ascending Descending A see tiding Descending
2nd—minor 2.81 7.8 20 I 2 5 .7 '
L 7.0 >32.8 V30 1-74.3
—major 4-2 . 25.0 10 J 48.6
3rd-minor 28.2 12.5' 33.3 10.5 ‘
j-53.6 ^28.1 53.3 ^20.0
—major 25.4 15.6 20 9 .5 ,
4th 32.4 15.6 8.3 4.75
Aug. 4th — 3.1 — —
lOth-minor — 1.6 — —
TOTAL 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
34
Grachic illustrations of thumb movement in kalimba tunes.
AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 35
w
36 AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
TABLE 2
Metrical basis of left and right thumb parts
V J r "j j / j j J
i j j -r
IX r i j
Lp r p r p r p r
YTT r i
j j J j
Lr t r c r r r
rJ s J j -r j «r
Lp r r r rr
£/ J j j -r
1 X J.
if r r r p
VI ri 11 1 1 1 1 n i l u n . j n
VII p j — “i j~ -j. J. jr ~ i.
r r r r r ..r r r r r r_r
&
VIII
x v u ['.
j i j j j' j j i
_f r ___ r r r
i-------- ~ , ~ ; ~ _T>
x vi XIII ■j j 3J J J
Lf r p- tc , •r r r f
; r t j r i 1 rJ J j J
■t r __r C1_J it r r ft
[ cl r £ (f r r r
v iriiu rJ J LT1 j j j J
xviu
r r r r U r P- P r
AX A
r
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 37
38 AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
L lf= 4 h r l h tH
J.=IOV-I08M.M.
a
R £ 2f e e
C. 2 2 4 - 4 - 4 4 2. C 2 2 I Z \ z
14-1 4- 1 4- z W z 4- Z 4
L h
J. =q6KM. ^ 3
ar 'l f. f ^
&
R ■r m
4 8
I ___ 3___ 3 4 4 f %Z 4 2.1 /4 * /4
L
^Ji=l20-I2.fc^«
v.k«( £ m
K 1. K*-no*-#e - le k»-le ku- ku - »k»- t«.
kftlii
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 39
40 AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
I 12 3 \ * L
l— M- u. - ■U
'&
4-7 * .s -*° 3 R E ^ & E * = £ = = 4
% 'O’
($ % 7 Tz ~—
i'fr fr p £ g -~ P -
S 3 3
H g'ar-tnba-ni, c u -a a oa au-ngu
J P ^ T . - J
m ^ m /* f f f P
JS 1 5 2 M.H.
- - --------------- -----------m - 1
v . / P S p S r p r y — j p .................... — — . " .. • .
Yoica 1
^ P a - l i n ja -m a ,
c ;* f
p a - l i n ja -m a ,
'
sa-
M
- S z ~
[ _ f ^ t s
c . 4 £7
ftr c * c r tJ r ...r c _ r c
2. 2 2 2. 2 1 3 2. i i i i
M t A- 3 s- 3 s- 3 J j a ! _ _ r ___ 3 ...... S \ .3 ___5L _ _4___
:i u
^ j :
n r r
v\ «
4 1 1 c f ^ cT gr I I I J ]'
n * * r - li- le «± - 11 - ! • - 11 - l e pa - 11 - l a Nya - Lo - Ta <
-j j — j------ ;
=?= L r ^ ^ :
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 41
42 AFRICAN MUSIC SOCIETY JOURNAL
V
PATTERNS OF NSENGA KALIMBA MUSIC 43
)L 4 , c 3 b__ 2. a ( ) R I g Z b 3 c 4
6.fejUo"- ° 0 >P J,0 " ° B+ °
112H.M.
Voice f e ^ J5 Ji bJ
U 1 . Su - mba *u - li - le .
M J * I3 2 M .M .
Voice f b /
” 2. Tu - 1» - t* - mbo B -
KpJ;>«u|
G^
j - J;
•|
s
ir
R
i
b
o
b b b
,
w
P li . ,2J.
3 -2
j t ,j m
M 132 H.M.
J5 t e r j- ^ ^ ^ / b j
Su - mb* - li t »0 - tml n l - mu - 1 1 - 1 * - ko.
® b J
t _ f ............... V >
1| jI--- ]
'.. J - J^ *
r '-
^ J—
>
■ L=
b .. - J
z 2 2 b t : Z.
L 4 o 2 » b 2 < x I R I ( ) 2 b 3 c 4
o
7 © O
I &
d» \0O^-lO4M.M.
7 P J r "tf J ?
I *R j / ^ j-
b 2 2 c 4 2
2 4-______ I 3j