Learning Jazz Improvisation
Learning Jazz Improvisation
Learning Jazz Improvisation
Kathy Dyson
May 2007
i
Acknowledgements
Thank you to Jane Davidson for her sustained support, encouragement and guidance.
Thanks are also due to Walter Turkenburg for his interest, careful reading of draft
daughter Ruth, who entered into the research and learning process so enthusiastically
and wholeheartedly; I hope that at least some of you are still improvising. I am
particularly grateful to Lindsay Aitkenhead who not only spent endless hours being
taught, observed and questioned, but who also looked after me on numerous occasions
and provided me with generous hospitality whilst at her house. Discussions with Dave
Hassell and Ian Croal have been especially valuable and I am grateful to all of those
clarify thoughts and communicate ideas clearly. Thanks to Chris Hodgkins who read
the final draft; to Brian Hennessy for generously providing materials from his library.
for the work on Bill Evans, and to John Dickson who allowed me to participate in an
my husband John for his emotional, intellectual, financial and technical support, not
just through the period of this project, but during the past 27 years; thanks would-
hardly be enough.
I
Learning Jazz Improvisation
Abstract 8
..........................................................................................................................
Introduction `..................................................................... 9
.............................................
Research Questions 10
.....................................................................................................
1. Schema Theory 15
....................................................................................
1.1 Descriptions of Schemata 15
......................................................................................
1.2 Motor Schemata 19
.....................................................................................................
1.2.1 Schmidt's schema theory of motor skill acquisition 19
..........................................
1
1.5.5 How does `schema memory' change as a result of learning? 51
............................
1.6 Problems with Schema Theory 52
..............................................................................
1.7 Alternatives to Schema Theory: Dual. Coding 55
.......................................................
1.8 Kelso's Theory of Dynamic Self-organising Learning Systems 57
...........................
1.9 Computational theory of mind 61
...............................................................................
improvisers 70
..................................................................................................................
2.4 Schema theory used to understand the process of jazz improvisation 77
..................
2.5 Schema theory for jazz improvisation 79
...................................................................
2.6 Motor schema for jazz improvisation 80
....................................................................
2.6.1 Motor abstractions 84'
..............................................................................................
2.7 Conceptual and theoretical schema for jazz improvisation 86
...................................
2.7.1 Theoretical and conceptual abstractions 88
..............................................................
2.8 Aural schema for jazz improvisation 90
.....................................................................
2.8.1 Abstractions for the aural schema 93
......................................................................
2.9 Communicative and collective schema for jazz improvisation 93
.............................
2.9.1 Abstractions of the collective schema 95
................................................................
2.10 Audience and listeners schema for jazz improvisation 96
.......................................
2.11 Neurological background and possible correlates 96
...............................................
2.11.1 Neurological description of voluntary movement 96
...........................................
2.11.2 The role of voluntary movement in improvisation related to schema theory... 99
2
3.1.1 Berliner's Learning Model 107
...............................................................................
3.1.7 Traditional ways of learning jazz improvisation and schema theory 121
...............
3.2.1 Selected review of jazz methods and manuals since the1960s 127
.........................
4.3.1 Pressing's cognitive model and ideas about improvisational skill 145
...................
4.3.3 Later thoughts from Pressing on the development of improvisational skill..... 1,57
5.5 What are the major problems concerning the nature and practice of qualitative
3
5.6 Qualitative Research Methods 186
.............................................................................
5.10 Purpose and rationale of the empirical research-how does it test the schema
201
theory?.......................................................................................................................
6.6 Summary of the pilot project jazz workshops; lessons learned and significant
7.4 Discussion and Findings from the first session relating to melody 241
.....................
5
8.2 Schema theory related to understanding the harmony 290
........................................
8.2.1 Evidence for the developing/intermediate schema for harmony- the abstraction
and recognition of chord progression from one standard to another ......................... 291
8.3 Schema theory relating to expressive parameters- evidence that a different process
is at work 292
..................................................................................................................
8.4 Schema theory related to wider listening within the jazz idiom 293
.........................
8.5 Schema theory and evidence of the technical schema developing 294
......................
8.5.1 Schema theory and further evidence of technical/motor schema using the
extending the motor/technical schema with modal scale practice ............................. 295
key 299
.............................................................................................................................
8.6.2 Evidence of schema development too soon 299
-anticipating ................................
8.7 Evidence of intermediate schema- altering the ways of learning a tune from
8.7.1 Evidence of intermediate schema-not only playing notes that fit but creatively
6
8.7.3 Evidence of the developing schema -extending rhythmic and timbral parameters
303
.................................................................................................................................
8.7.4 Evidence of schema development: using general contours that are later filled in
8.7.5 Evidence of improvement in aural memory and the development of the schema
8.8 Evidence that the schema is still at an intermediate stage- lack of resolution 304
....
8.12 Indications of the developing viola schema - singing and violin playing are not
308-
...................................................................................................................................
8.14 Evidence of schema development related to memory 308'
.......................................
8.17 Closing the eyes and how that affected schema process. 311
..................................
References 318
.................................................................................................................
Bibliography 331
..............................................................................................................
7
PAGE
NUMBERING
AS ORIGINAL
Abstract
mind that both structures and is structured by experience. In this thesis, schema
and motor elements in order to relate these processes to jazz improvisation and thus
conceptual knowledge may be abstracted and generalised; how motor skill in musical
generated; how multi-modal aspects of the skill may be integrated; how novel ideas
may occur; how the individual voice is created and how improvised ideas may be
communicated. This schema theory for. jazz improvisation provides the theoretical
ground from which a series of educational workshops (involving both groups and-
qualitative research methodology is used to collect and then analyse data from the
workshops. Evidence from these practical investigations demonstrates the ability for
teaching also reveals the value of singing to improvisation development and the
and work in progress have wide ranging implications for understanding dynamic
adaptive skill and for educational practice, specifically, how knowledge of the schema
9
Introduction
musician and teacher, this approach begins to provide the ground for explaining how
novel behaviour and thinking can be accounted for. Schema theory usefully provides
yet adaptable framework which forms a basis for linking conceptual ideas, aural
imagination and physical skills. The central tenet of the thesis is that higher order
skills such as improvisation are guided by schemata which enable dynamic and
adaptive behaviour. I propose that during the process of learning jazz improvisation, a
series of integrated conceptual, motor, auditory and other schemata emerge and are.
adapted in the mind to enable the body to perform the requisite musical skills and to
allow the individual to create novel ideas and to develop their own individual'
learning process as it occurs rather than particularly focusing on outcomes and also-
tries to bridge the gap between what the teacher thinks is being taught, what the
10
o To relate schema theory to the understanding of jazz improvisation.
o To test the applicability of the theory to jazz by organising and carrying out a'
o To monitor the emergent and developing skills, treating them as schemata if and
assess teaching and learning strategies and analyse these processes as data for
Research Questions
1. What is a schema?
3. What should a schema for jazz improvisation be able to do? (Proposed model)
4. What are the other current theories of jazz improvisational process and how do
5. How did jazz musicians traditionally learn to improvise and how is this aspect
11
6. How do contemporary teachers propose that jazz is learned and how does this
relate to schematheory?
7. How can the schema theory of jazz improvisation be tested within a practical.
setting? t
The Concise Oxford Dictionary defines improvisation as the capacity: `to create and
music: `the art of performing music spontaneously, without the aid of manuscript,
composing" (Kernfeld, 1988), and many contemporary jazz educators view it as such.
Runswick (2004) has an even broader definition oT improvisation based on his notion
12
or more parameters in a musical performance. ' In Runswick's view, invention is any
extreme and tape playback in which no interpretive elements are present, at the other.
aleatoric practices and Indian classical music, all of which have varying degrees of
improvised elements that are viewed as being equally, rather than hierarchically
important. Most improvisation however, takes place within a cultural context, and in
the jazz idiom improvisation can be viewed from three different perspectives; as a
completely free endeavour in which no rules are required (as played for example by
Derek Bailey or Han Bennink); as a semi-free activity where certain rules or forms
invented by the performers are applied (Antony Braxton or Cecil Taylor improvise in
this way), and as a melodic process within a specific idiom based on a historical
period usually using some elements of swing (as exemplified by Paul Desmond or
Charlie Parker). In this thesis, it is the third perspective- melodic improvisation based'.
on a harmonic progression which is the focus of the study; specifically, how it. is:
In order to explain why schema theory is being applied to the jazz improvisation
teaching and learning context, I begin the first chapter of the thesis by defining
schemata and exploring the range of different meanings of the term, so as to prepare
the theoretical ground for my overall investigation. In chapter two, I explore further
areas specifically pertaining to schemata for adaptive learning and propose a model of
jazz improvisation schema theory which incorporates elements from two major
learning jazz improvisation are discussed and compared to the schema theory model.
Chapter four investigates how contemporary teachers of jazz propose learning jazz
from Jeff Pressing, Philip Johnson-Laird and David Sudnow. Chapter five proposes a
rationale for the methodology which was adopted to ground the practical work, and a
model for data collection and analysis. In chapter six, the planning and organisation
the proposed jazz schema theory. Later in this chapter, there is a description and
the teacher to monitor the emerging schema are made. This work enables me to refine
my teaching and overall research strategies ready for the main practical investigations. '
Chapter seven focuses on the main practical investigation which is a case study of one
individual engaged in learning improvisation over the period of five weeks. The
findings and implications are discussed in chapter eight in relation to the jazz schema
theory itself and to other educational and musical aspects. Finally, conclusions are
drawn and possible new ways of developing or extending the research, are discussed.
To begin the overall investigation, this chapter now devotes itself to describing what a
schema or schema assemblage is and how it might work during the process of
14
this core idea and help to illustrate the broadest view of what a schema is and how it
15
I. Schema Theory
1.1 Descriptions of Schemata
A schema is a dynamic, abstract framework in the mind that structures experience and
is structured by experience. The notion of the schema has been documented since the
I
time of Aristotle, who discussed schemata in The Metaphysics, and Plato, who wrote
about them in The Meno. In more recent times, Kant (1787) was the first to describe
of all kinds), and realised that the abstraction in a schema can represent many things
sharing the same features in a dynamic state, whilst still providing a structure for
understanding and for action. Kant also differentiated between a visual mental image
and a schema, defining the image as being essentially a static phenomenon and the
psychological research, Head and Holmes (1911) and Head (1920) described the
schematic model we have of our body in our mind which is extraordinarily resistant to
what it sees as discordant information (for example, persistent feeling and pain in an
amputated limb). An implication of Head's research is that the brain can construct a
16
that is formed when listening to a story and he discovered that what is remembered is
based upon the attitude and values that are brought to it i. e. the role of our existing
schemata on new information. Rather than passively replicating the facts of the story,
we actively recreate them to fit with our own existing schema. (Similarly, we also
envisage our own particular imagining according to our previous experience and
,.
memories. ) Bartlett proposed that memory is both selective and recreative: that is, we
remember those elements that have particular resonance for us, and which we choose
to remember, with each successive remembrance being altered according to our own
Piaget (1952) related schema formation and function to child development and he
cognitive functions in order to respond to every experience that can be related to the
with the experiences of childhood and defined the schema of an action as the structure
of the `generalisable characteristics of this action, that is, those which allow the
repetition of the same action or its application to new content' (1966: 235). In relation
something requires us to choose a schema that can account for it and in the face of an
inexplicable situation a new schema must be formed or an old one adapted. In this
view, the schema forms the basis for all understanding. Consider for a moment an
opposite view, and see why the idea of the schema is so compelling. Imagine
17
coming to each situation entirely fresh in the way that perhaps babies and young
children do, and having in each instance to create a suitable framework to understand
it. Fortunately many similar situations occur as the child interacts with first the
mother or carer, then the family and then the outside world, so that various similar
It also appears that schemata can be used to give meaning and coherence to random
information. For example, Howard (1976) cites an anecdotal tale of a man hired to
was in truth an actor who read a series of disjointed and disorganised nonsense text
interspersed with appropriate jargon and academic phraseology. His audience seemed
to make sense of what he said by applying their own schemata; in other words they
sought and created their own meaning, allowing them to make sense of the lecture. '
This example illustrates how meaning is made by the listener and imposed on the -
back and forth until a reasonably definitive idea is created. The same process could
The implications of the role of schemata in our understanding are that without the
understood if there are insufficient clues presented in order to call up the relevant
18
material that initially comes within the domains of students' existing schemata and
subsequently orders new concepts and materials so that they are developed, enriched
and linked to the formation of new frameworks. According to Howard (1976: 42)
material that is not understood within the student's existing schemata will be ignored,
Memory and schemata are obviously integrated in that the development of a schema
allows the individual to abstract the most important elements for them, in any given
situation. Rumelhart (1984) suggests that schemata are also used to reconstruct
events from diverse and fragmentary sensory information, so that the memory
becomes coherent and meaningful to the individual. This notion is in agreement with
Neisser's definition presented below, where the schema both modifies and is altered
A schema is that part of the entire perceptual cycle which is internal to the
perceiver, modifiable by experience and somehow specific to what is being
perceived. The schema accepts information as it becomes available at sensory
surfaces and is changed by that information; it directs movements and
exploratory activities that make more information available, by which it is
further modified. From a biological point of view, a schema is part of the
nervous system. It is some active array of physiological structures and
processes; not a centre in the brain, but an entire system that includes
receptors and afferents and feed-forward units and efferents.
Neisser (1976: 54)
The schema is also involved in the amount and quality of information that we
remember and the more highly developed and rich the schema, the more likely it is
schemata, there are also schema theories for n otot control which are highly relevant
19
1.2 Motor Schemata
There has been some groundbreaking work in this area and of the many
figures Richard Schmidt and Nikolai Bernstein that I turn to illuminate motor schema
Schmidt (1975) developed a schema theory to account for the learning of motor skill
and defines motor learning as: `a set of (internal) processes associated with practice or
Schmidt's schema model of motor control proposes that there are two different
memory systems; the recall schema and the recognition schema, both of which have
the amount of force needed to execute the movement. The recognition schema relates
acts as an internal reference point or comparison between the imaginary correct and
the execution of a movement but has to adapt to constantly changing situations and
20
(1975), for a schema to be established or used there have to be four sources of
information:
movement; 1
1) and 2) represent the recall schema - the requirements and conditions of the
movement or skill.
4) The sensory consequences of the response i. e. how it felt during and after the
After a movement is made with a generalised motor programme, the individual stores
the four pieces of information described above using the recall schema to create the
movement and the recognition schema to control and evaluate it. Schmidt proposes
that the schema is formed from the relationships between the elements that are crucial
and from the abstraction of such information over many practised repetitions. Of `
previously in order to be produced in the future: the individual can extrapolate a new
movement based upon imagination and the abstracting of information from previously
movement in response to a new musical idea in the aural imagination, which could
not happen without the previous focus on learning specific movements related to
21
learned musical ideas or exercises. The novel response is one of the predictions of
schematheory and this is becausethe basis for producing a new movement is a rule
Initial conditions
Desired outcome
Past 0 v
ýE Past Past sensory
actual g consequences
spresponecifications
ons
outcomes
(Controls (Estimates
rapid consequences)
movements)
Expected
Response
sensory
specifications
consequences
Figure 1.4. The brain anticipates the consequencesof movement from past ac-
tion to prepare and initiate a movement. Reading from top to bottom, antici-
pated movements begin with a set of initial conditions and a plan of action that
will lead to the desired outputs. These are compared with the results of past ac-
tions recalled by two types of memory: left, memory of motor commands, and
right, memory of sensory data associatedwith past movements and their ef-
fects on the environment. It is therefore possible to recall the expected sensory
consequences,or the messagesdetected by the receptors during and after ac-
tion over the course of movement. Control of movement involves estimating
discrepanciesbetween anticipated and actual sensory data.
22
Another aspect of schema theory that has clear implications for jazz improvisation is
the importance of feedback both in the results of the learning and in the sensory
feedback in the early stages of learning when the nature of the relationship between
new skill, by the modification of both the recall schema and recognition schema. This
type of transfer is also linked to the notion of a generalised motor programme for
similar tasks and movements. In addition, the amount of information given to the
learner at any one time is crucial in establishing the recall schema. Instructions should
therefore be clear and simple with key points, as too much information will cause
confusion and stretch the limits of working memory. Also, sufficient time is needed
in order to assimilate information and to allow its transfer to long term memory.
Bernstein (1946/68) described the plateaux, delays and occasional regressions that
occur in the learning of complex skills, as a necessary part of the process in which the
brain alters to enable flowing, adaptive and dextrous motor skills to occur under
conditions that vary widely. It is part of the teacher's role to recognise and accept
these perceived setbacks and to alter any learning programme to take them into
ceasing the instruction altogether for a while. Bernstein also proposed generalised
motor programmes and the idea that all movement was adaptive and therefore likely
nature of the skill requires constantly adaptive movement triggered and directed from
23
1.3 Bernstein's theory of motor learning
and it could be said, at least in part, that these adaptations are improvisatory in nature.
controlled only on the basis of sensory corrections, and repetition of the same
movement will be accompanied by different motor impulses from the brain to the
muscles. This is because the schema provides the framework for the realisation of any
pattern of neurons to fire in any place. Thus the schema can be activated by any
appropriate groups of neurons and perhaps never by the same group twice. Just as we
never think the same thought in quite the same way, so we cannot make the same
know exactly how heavy the cup is going to be and could not possibly calculate it in
advance. I have the schema and generalised motor programme for picking up a cup
(or any other object), yet I never know exactly how heavy any cup will be because the
materials, liquids and volumes vary. My brain does not have all the information,
therefore as I pick up the cup, the feedback from the sensory nerves in my hand sends
signals which are organised into commands for more muscle support in the arm,
greater care because the coffee is hot, better balance as the cup is raised to the lips and
so on. Certain movements like hand to target, for example, are so well practised that
we seldom get them wrong but we cannot predict the weight of objects in advance
(although we have a good idea from experience), and have to constantly adapt
a short step to consciously using the process for a musical or other outcome. In
24
Bernstein's view, the repetitions of a movement are not necessary in order to `imprint'
a trace or programme but rather to solve a motor problem many times and to find
According to Bernstein, after numerous repetitions of the movement, the motor cortex
(in conjunction with the cerebellum), will sooner or later develop dynamically stable
movement patterns. At this point the sensory corrections cease and the movement
becomes stable and automatic in order to protect it from any further changes which
might compromise its efficacy. Thus the motor skill is still adaptable for
improvement and greater dexterity but it cannot disappear or deteriorate. One never,
for example, forgets how to ride a bicycle, even after many years without practice.
usually after many years of practice. Playing is entirely automatic if the performer
wishes it to be so, and the source and thinking behind why a certain musical line is
words. People frequently ask jazz musicians what they are doing and thinking of as
they improvise and the response is usually a variation on: `I just blow, man! ' As the
conscious control and effort required to perform the skill fades from consciousness, it
(1992) have shown that activity in the primary motor cortex and supplementary areas
of the cortex actually increases once a movement has been learned and the
25
implication is that non-conscious elements come into play which release
consciousness for other activities whilst motor areas are organising information so
that the brain can control movement in response to a wide range of environmental and
kinaesthetic variables. A further point to note is that if activity in the primary motor
cortex increases once the movement is learned, perhaps the extra activity is indicative
of the schema organising itself i. e. abstracting the various elements needed to meet
demonstrates how the brain organises adaptive movement. The comparator within the
cyclical model (see Fig 1.2 which is taken from Berthoz, 2000: 14) establishes what
Berthoz (2000: 13) describes as the required value. This has the role of detecting errors
between the intended and actual movement, which in turn triggers a subsequent-
correction if necessary. The comparator also recognises when the movement is ended'
so that the next movement in the sequence or plan can be executed. Finally, it adapts
supplied by the receptors at the sensory surfaces and the neurological substrates that
make the corrections and reorganise the movement. In the middle of a movement, it
would not be useful to restart the action because of a changing circumstance, therefore
the movement is adapted as it unfolds taking into account the new information. As
such, the comparator is a dynamic element comparing changing external states with
some kind of internal model. According to the model in this case, the receptor acts as
an initiator rather than a regulator of the movement and the suggestion is that this
an adjacent trajectory.
26
5
ENCODER
CORRECTIONS AW
,, "`'".
PROGRAM "''ý
" ,,
64
REGULATOR COMPARATOR
Sw
ýýý.
ENERGY
"" 1
"""'"ý 3
EFFECTOR
RECEPTOR
(muscle)
Sensory
receptor
INFORMATION
Locus of
activity
Figure 1.2. Schematic diagram showing the cerebral organization for control of
movement proposed by Bernstein.; r
27
From (Berthoz, 2000: 13)
One of the significant elements of Bernstein's theory of motor learning is that only
target actions matter, with trajectories of limbs being widely variable and
interchangeable, although they usually take the shortest/straightest route. The motor
schema is orientated towards the goal twhich is dependent on the will), and the
jazz improvisation, the target action would relate to an aurally imagined or heard idea,
and the generalised motor framework would allow it to be played wherever required
by the player on the instrument. As Bernstein sees it, in the elaboration of a skill, the
brain's sensory systems gradually learn to be more and more skilful in making an
movement process into the `language' of corrective motor impulses that need to be
sent to the muscles. In music, the main difference between learning adaptive musical
motor skills in a composed and notated piece of music and in jazz improvisation, is
that whilst the notated piece can interpreted and played with flexibility, in
improvisation the melody has to be created from the imagination and can be
In Bernstein's view then, motor skill is the ability to solve a motor problem and the
learned skill must be practised many times in order for the brain to experience all the
sensations, which form the basis of the sensory corrections. But, what is the motor
auditory feedback with movement and developing the ability to do this task
complex because the imagined musical line is constantly changing, and may require
28
the execution of movements that have never been specifically practised, as for
example, in the execution of a novel idea. However, the same ideas pertain; the
`target' becomes the next musical idea and as that is unfolding and being played
does not proceed in incremental steps or in a smooth fashion. After arduous conscious
practice, and many failures, it appears that a person is suddenly able to perform a new
skill. Again, this may reflect the schema organising itself non-consciously, hence the
sudden acquisition of the skill once all the appropriate elements are assembled.
Bernstein is also clear that the skill learning process is relatively slow because of the
number and variety of visual, kinaesthetic, motor and cognitive elements that have to
be integrated smoothly and efficiently. Motor skill learning within the individual
therefore takes as long as the schema takes to form, which will vary enormously from
person to person, and Bernstein warns against artificially attempting to speed up the
process.
There are also plateaux in the learning process where, despite the best efforts of both
teachers and students, progress slows. Bernstein proposes that in such cases,
something interferes with solving the problems related to the movement. It may be
that some element of the schema has not been assimilated and because it requires a
technical problem might destabilise the process. It may require more automatic
cannot be rushed without compromising the quality of the movement. We have all
come across the pupil who has learned something fast and inaccurately which is then
29
very difficult to `undo' and correct. This is also borne out in the early stages of
learning a piece of music: the slow precise movements required to ensure the correct
notes are incompatible with very quick movements. If an attempt is made to play a
newly learned passage too quickly too soon, the movements will be smudged.
Conversely there seems to be an optimum tempo for practice; too slow and the music
to research the jazz improvisational process. Schmidt and Bernstein's theories, whilst
aural triggers and the increasing dexterity and fluency of the mature improviser. There
are other aspects to the schema assemblage of the jazz improviser as well as the
conceptual and motor elements, for example social schemata, that have an impact on
How do social relationships, in the form of social schemata, affect how jazz
improvisation is learned? The characteristic feature of social schemata is the way that
two or more people coordinate with each other so that: `their action, affect, evaluation
or thought are complementary. ' (Fiske, 1998: 1) In jazz improvisation how one player
behaves and performs makes both musical and social sense within the context of other
players; their actions complete each other. Indeed, in jazz, as a communal and
(1998: 1) points out, relationships are patterns of coordination among people; they are
not properties of individuals. Jazz musicians typically seek out others of like mind and
30
similar musical standard to join up with. There are numerous stories related by
bands with real or home made instruments, hanging out and playing at jam sessions,
asking questions and being mentored by peers or more experienced players and finally
Fiske proposes that all of the diverse, adaptive and multi-cultural and multi-facetted
social relationship interactions are based on only four basic models (Fiske, 1991a,
1992). These are: Communal Sharing, Authority Ranking, Equality Matching and
group as equivalent and undifferentiated with respect to the social domain in question.
which subordinates defer, respect and perhaps obey, whilst superiors take precedence'
and also pastoral responsibility for subordinates. Authority Ranking relationships are
based on perceptions of legitimate asymmetries, not coercive power and are not
people keep track of the balance or difference among participants and know what is
required to restore balance, for example, turn taking, one person one vote, equal share
meaningful ratios or rates such as price, wages, interest, rents, and so on although
money need not be the medium. Fiske remarks that people often use different models
For example, roommates may divide the rent evenly and take turns cooking
dinner for each other (both Equality Matching), buy ingredients for the meal at
31
the store (Market Pricing), share their food and drink at the table without
regard to who consumes what and share living and bath rooms (Communal
Sharing), pay for long-distance calls according to the costs they each incur
(Market Pricing), and one may sell her used car to the other. On the softball
field one is a coach, the other player (Authority Ranking); yet in their sexual
relations they like to reverse these roles of domination and submission. (Fiske,
1998:4)
The jazz musician will probably be involved in all four types of these particular social
schemata as he or she learns and hones their improvisational skill. Communal Sharing
will relate to the conducting of rehearsals, forming of bands and peer learning;
Authority Ranking will affect jazz musicians relationships with teachers and master
players who they will defer to (in an apprenticeship-type model) and who also have a
pastoral care role; Equality Matching will take place within performances and-
rehearsals and relate to the taking of solos, the sharing of composition duties and the
organising of material; and relationships involving Market Pricing will occur in the
promotion of gigs, payment for performance, CD production and sales, and payments
for performing rights amongst others. Jazz musicians will use these models of social
well as to interpret, plan and remember important aspects of the overall process. The
models appear to be cognitive by implication but are in fact integral with emotions,
motives, needs, evaluative attitudes and judgements. These four social schemata are
also generally implicit in nature; people use them without reflection and would be
unable to articulate them clearly. The four models are dependent on cultural
parameters, paradigms and prototypes to specify action and can therefore be infinitely
diverse. When interacting people use the same social schema model in the same way,
32
relationships tend to be harmonious because they understand each other, have
complimentary motives and expectations and judge actions in the same way (Fiske
and Haslam, 1998). Thus jazz musicians and their audiences know the kinds of
understanding. Players and audiences for free jazz know the kind of performance and
music that will take place and how it will be different for those of more mainstream or
popular jazz.
Fiske and Taylor (1984: 140) describe three types of processes guided by social
schemata: perception, memory and inference. How do these relate to social schemata
_
for jazz improvisation? The authors assume that schemata focus primarily on
information is assimilated into existing knowledge. In the thesis I make the case that
this is just one element of the schema which is relevant to the conceptual schema such
as Kant's triangle; motor, aural and other schemata are non cognitive in nature. The
authors (1984: 141) also stress their belief that the most fundamental principle
people simplify reality by interpreting specific instances in the light of the general
actively construct reality, by creating meaning and adding onto it the raw data of `the
objective world'.
Taylor and Crocker, (1981) propose that there are five basic types of social schemata:
person, self, role, event, and procedural. Person schemata focus on knowledge about
33
the traits and goals that shape a persons behaviour and capture the perceiver's
schema contains information about one's own personality, appearance and behaviour.
Role schemata focus on knowledge about broad social categories e.g. age, race, sex or
occupation and how the individual is placed within society. Event schemata explore
(1984: 149).
In terms of learning jazz improvisation, person schemata will affect how players
-
choose others to rehearse, perform and socialise with, in part depending on the
similarity of their goals and aspirations and also their musical abilities and tastes,
(which need to be suited to each other). The jazz musician's self schema, like anyone
else's, may be clear on some attributes and less clear on others, but tend to be self-
schematic on dimensions that are important to them. As jazz music and improvisation
would be important to the aspiring improviser, they would tend to be defined in some
self-schemata being defined by the music and their aspirations within it. The self-
schema allows the person to filter incoming information about that particular
others (see Markus and Sentis, 1982 for a review). Being schematic on a given trait
means that one is a rapid judge of oneself on that particular trait, which makes
behaviour quicker and easier. People also recognise an attribute from their self
community where goals and aspirations are likely to be similar. People's self
34
schemata also make them think harder about all kinds of schema relevant information
that comestheir way, and so their self schemaand commitment to the ideas within it
A primary area involving social schemata for the jazz musician would seem to be
motivation; the motivation to be an equal with and respected by peers and the
of the social schema being an altering force on the individual may be the fairly recent
(over the past 20 years) emphasis on instrumental technique, over perhaps more
melodic and holistic approaches. Due to the major influences of, for example,
saxophonist Michael Brecker who, like Charlie Parker before him raised the technique
of the saxophone to a new level, young players are almost required to reach this
standard (on all instruments) in order to be taken seriously by peers, promoters and
audiences alike.
behaviour towards what is viewed by Berliner and others as the `jazz life'. The jazz
life was formerly associated both within the jazz community and outside of it, with
performance and a range of social behaviours such as hanging out with musicians,
going to jam sessions, sometimes drinking and drug taking, argot, extensive touring
and travelling and so on. Brecker and to some extent certain British jazz musicians
such as Tim Garland and Gwilym Simcock, are examples of consummate musicians
who happen to play jazz, while operating within a wide range of musical situations.
Brecker and his brother trumpeter Randy recorded with most of the major rock and
funk artists in the USA throughout their career and viewed this as a valuable as well
35
as profitable area of their musicianship. That kind of social schema of musician as
journeyman rather than purely jazz musician is one strand. The other, is represented
by a player like Soweto Kinch who wrote a rap entitled `The Jazz Planet' which
celebrates all of those aspects of the jazz life that are so precious. He along with
Abram Wilson and a group of British and USA artists still have the jazz life strand
flowing through their work and probably have very different social schemata that
Similarly the social schemata around women jazz musicians, whilst being a highly
complex and contested area is also very different, in that although most male
musicians state that the most important aspect for them is the music, nonetheless there
is a dearth of top class performing female jazz musicians in an art form that claims to
be highly democratic. This area is beyond the scope of this thesis, but the social
schemata for jazz playing and socialising does appear to have a predominantly
confirms their self concepts (Swann and Read, 1981a, 1981b) and for most people the
other words those aspects of self schemata that are most important to our identity are
appears to be more easily accessible to memory than knowledge of others, due to the
constant nature of consciousness and the role of emotion and meaning on our self
way (Lord, 1980) because we are unable to `see' ourselves as well as we see and
therefore can visualise others. Self schemata include areas of specialist knowledge
36
and expertise which for the jazz musician would be related to music theory,
instrumental technique, the jazz genre (history, current players, and styles) and a
Role schemata for the jazz musician will involve the various social roles undertaken
in rehearsals, teaching, peer learning, performance, listening and so on and are used to
enable the smooth running and organisation of various social activities related to the
learning of jazz improvisation. Certain roles will be ascribed roles, like those of age,
sex and race and others, achieved roles, will be based on ability, training and
experience. Both kinds of roles carry with them expectations for appropriate
-
behaviour and standards and these are organised in people's minds as schemata. As
_
well as aiding perception, memory and inference as all schemata do, role schemata
have clear behavioural and affective consequences and are involved in the
guidelines for a variety of empowering and mainly nurturing roles. Band leaders set
the direction of the band but also provide work, a well known name for promotion,
adhered to and are in general responsible for the harmonious working and performing
of the band. Some of the negative attributes of bandleaders (including instances of,
being overpaid) are also accepted by the players, if they see overall benefits to being
in the band. As jazz is generally viewed as democratic music, roles can and do shift
with musicians being bandleaders of their own prbjects, sidemen in other bands, co-
37
1.4.4 Event schemata
Event schemata are rather like the scripts outlined by Shank and Abelson (1977)
whicji are frameworks that describe appropriate sequences of events in well known
situations. Event schemata for the jazz improviser (and also the listener) would follow
the sequence of appropriate events at club's, concert halls, rehearsals, jam sessions and
so on. Event schemata guide information processing in much the same way as for
other schemata and also enable the smooth running and organising of events due to
the expectations of all parties. Event social schemata are mostly non-consciously
carried out (unless there is some kind of inappropriate behaviour or infringement) and
profoundly affect how people behave at events. For example, the event schema at a
concert hall is for the audience to sit and listen in silence to the band, whereas the
event schema at a jazz club is more ambiguous; some clubs insist on and get silence
for performers, whilst at others people, talk, eat and clank glasses throughout the
and joining in is appropriate and part of the experience. In pantomime, loud audience
participation and feedback is encouraged as an integral part of the show, which would
abstract and generalise information from one or more instances and mature schemata
are likely to be more complex and organised than immature examples and therefore
increasingly likely to moderate the judgement and the behaviour of the person
assimilation, mature schemata are not only more organised, but contain more
38
elements that are also integrated and interrelated. The mature jazz improviser is able
information abstracted through wide experience and the integrated relevant musical
knowledge.
Although they have only briefly been discussed here, social schemata in all the forms
mentioned (person, self, role and event), are fundamental to the learning of jazz
improvisation given the informal nature of much jazz learning, considering the
democratic and collaborative nature of the music and for motivational and economic
purposes.
Having discussed the various elements that may comprise the schema assemblage of
the jazz improviser, the next section describes in more detail how the schema itself
might work. It is based on Minsky's (1974) five critical questions that he felt needed
Schemata are invoked, either purposefully and consciously for some specific task, or
learning contexts where problem solving is inherent, a schema will be invoked in the
individual to account for the current situation andtto assessits potential. Information
about the context or the subject matter will invoke specific schemata which will be
39
consciously used. On the other hand, habitually used schemata that are used everyday,
such as those for making breakfastor finding our way to work, are non-consciousand
is
question whether it is possible to think or act in any coherent way without invoking
only develops to solve a problem, and whether we agree with this or not depends on
how broadly this is defined. We could, of course, take the view that everything we
but schemata may be formed just by our interaction with the world and with others.
In this view, schemata may even form without specifically conscious attention
(although non-conscious attention may occur), and involve notions of tacit learning
From work done by Gibson (1979), Trevarthen (1999/2000) and Lee, (1985) it is
possible to argue that people are each other's richest `affordance" and that individuals
intonation and speech, in interaction with others. This kind of dynamic non-conscious
awareness may form the basis of non-cognitive and non-conscious schemata that
I
An affordance is a property of an object, or a feature of the immediate environment, that indicates
how to interact with that object or feature. The empty space within an open doorway, for instance,
affords movement across that threshold; a sofa affords the possibility of sitting down on it.
40
structures that enrich and further connect them. Johnson (1987) would argue that
movement and interactions between other humans and the environment. The `in and
You wake out of a deep sleep; per out from beneath your covers and into
.
your room. You gradually emerge out of your stupor; pull yourself out from
under your quilt, climb into your robe, stretch out your limbs, and walk in a
daze out of the bedroom and into the bathroom. You look into the mirror and
see your face staring out at you. (1987: 31)
All of the above bodily orientations establish relationships between the body and the
environment (and what it affords us), and it is the relationships that are `translated'
into purely thought-based schemata in order for the individual to make sense of a
relationships between elements that are abstracted and generalised for elaborated and .
extended uses, and in this view schemata are essentially embodied and non-cognitive.
Schemata that develop into conceptual and verbalisable states are therefore like a
dynamic virtual form of our physical interactions, extended to another more complex
level for the purpose of understanding ideas or situations. Thus the coherence and
unity of adaptive movement becomes the coherence and unity of adaptive thought,
which in turn is likely to enrich and extend our physical potential, interactions and
Marshall (1994) who studied schema formation in the solving of arithmetical story
problems, proposed that the network form of the schema develops from many
repetitions of similar actions, forms connections among the various parts of the
situation being repeated, and is idiosyncratic and unique to each individual based on
0",. //,,
41
past experience, though with generalised elements that are similar enough to be
communicable.
Ultimately, schema selection will depend upon what kind of schema is being invoked.
Of those that are purposefully or consciously invoked like conceptual schemata; one
will be selected which is suitable for any given situation based on matching patterns
from past experience. In any situation, there are usually many clues about what is
likely to happen and how to go about understanding it, in order for a schema to be
called up. Certain types of social schemata and scripts will be naturally or non-
restaurant will invoke the appropriate `eating out' schema regardless of the huge
variety of restaurants available. Or going to a party of whatever kind will invoke the
selected and hard to alter, whereas person, self and role schemata involving identity
In the case of the motor schema, a non-conscious process will call up the appropriate
schema for the target action based on a generalised program, because the motor
problem has been solved to enable this. How this occurs neurologically is still not
well understood, especially as we usually begin to make the movement before any
conscious awareness of it. The process of how a movement can be made, is better
understood. However, the trigger that initiates the motor schema for a voluntary
Schmidt are also essentially non-conscious except for the need for conscious attention
42
If we relate schema selection to jazz improvisation, the appropriate schema will
depend upon the level of skills already present. In the case of a person who is already
a musician, but a novice improviser, schema selection will involve initially invoking
their already highly developed musicians' schema for playing their instrument, and
for understanding and performing, within whatever the context of the musical genre
they learned in. This schema will be a complex assemblage of motor, kinaesthetic,
aural, auditory, visual and conceptual elements, with additional conceptual and/or
auditory connections from their past experience. The new improvisational schema
will emerge as an extension of already existing elements with more focus, for
example, on aural to motor skills without the mediation of musical notation. The
novice improviser will then develop the new aural, kinaesthetic, motor and conceptual
schemata.
By contrast, the mature improviser will have developed the largely non-conscious
schema assemblage for improvisation and be able to draw on it at will in a wide range
of different situations. They will no longer be able to say how they learned or what
they are doing and rather in the manner of conversation, will imagine an idea or a
sense of an idea and be able to alter it at will as it unfolds. In that respect there is no
selection of the schema as such, but action to create the sound generated by the non-
Marshall (1996) argues for four discrete stages in conceptual schema use:
from the previous stage. Identification knowletigeractivates the schema and is based
43
the individual's experience alongside general abstractions that describe these
experiences. The schema may, therefore provide both a link and an organising
framework so that these various experiences can be adapted for future use, and
Marshall argues that elaboration knowledge allows the individual to create a mental
framework that allows the individual to create a tentative scenario which anticipates
required in order to cope with the new situation and environment. The priming and
expectation element appear to be crucial to the process and any signposts given by the
teacher that anticipate what is likely to happen, seems to be important to the learning
process.
Planning knowledge refers to the way the schema can then be used to make plans and
create expectations and goals and Marshall suggests we can investigate the formation
knowledge. Although this may well be the case in areas that can be more easily
verbalised or where the learner can identify discrete stages in the process, it is more
difficult in music, and in jazz improvisation it would be virtually impossible for the
improvisation, except in a most general way (learners however, may be more able to
verbalise the process). Execution knowledge is used to carry out the plans and in the
jazz context, this would be playing the music and so involve the fully integrated
elements.
44
In jazz improvisation, schemata that may be related to and called up for the learning
process are many and include motor and auditory to motor schemata triggered from
an aural rather than visual source. There are schemata for recognition and
memorisation of melody and harmony and conceptual schemata for scales and other
related patterns. The basic schemata required to be a musician (for example from a
classically trained background) form the starting point for the schema assemblage for
improvisation, with changes of emphasis amongst the various component parts of the
skill - for example, the strengthening of aural ability and imagination and various
aural stimuli and developing high levels of concentration needed to create musical
during the process of learning the skill and in the case of a trained musician, will
include elements of previously learned skills or knowledge, but perhaps require "
experiences and the present circumstances, and also between all current elements, this
should encourage the invocation of associated or different schemata that will help
According to Marshall (1996), there are only two kinds of modifications: enlarging
the schema or changing the information that is already part of it. Piaget and Beth
(1966: 242) describe another process of `reflective abstraction' whereby each stage of
the schema develops into a more sophisticated one. Like Schmidt, Piaget sees this
structure thereby generalising them for future use. Piaget suggests that the abstracted
45
elements should be `reflected' onto a new plane of thought in a kind of mirror image,
but within a different context. He considers that the reflection both relates to previous
ideas and operations but also continues them into a new sphere, and it is this that he
believes allows for novel actions, thoughts or approaches i. e. the use of previously
permit new and previously separated systems to be combined and integrated into more
previously noted idea of the schemafor `in and out', which developsfrom the body of
the individual being physically inside or outside various environments. The child is
inside her cot or outside in the park; in her father's arms or out of them. From this
develops the more complex notion that physically the child could be out of her cot but
still inside the house, or out in the park but in the pram. From these basic bodily
environment, then inside and outside of the body as a speaking and thinking
state, in love, in a connection with others and so on. As adults we have a clear notion
of what being in or out of something is like, both physically and psychologically and
how that can be communicated, but the roots appear to be grounded in movement and
the orientation of the body in space, and on this basis, Johnson asserts therefore that
If we agree with Johnson's theory of the embodied schema as the basis for both
thought and action, then the schema may be modified by some form of bodily action
and experience, and clearly in the case of improvisation, physical action and
movement take a large role in the learning process, supported by various conceptual
46
frameworks. In this view, the schema can only change if it involves some kind of
physical or virtual movement, and indeed can only be used fully once the scattered
gestalt. This in turn poses a problem for exploring schema emergence and
and complete structure is what informs and makes coherence the skill.
Enriching the skills or knowledge base and practising the skill or using the knowledge
in new or different contexts will therefore also modify the schema. The issue is how
the knowledge is abstracted and used, not the knowledge itself. For example, scales
are learned by musicians to facilitate technique on the instrument and for memorising
and learning music. The basic knowledge of scales remains unchanged, but the uses to
which the information abstracted from the scales is put, changes enormously. For
example, intervals are used in different ways and in varying harmonic contexts and
can be perceived as extensions of chord tones (i. e. the fourth can also be the eleventh).
The musician knows intuitively that scales are not practised in isolation (nor are they
of much use in their standard format), but rather for what the musician is then able to
do with them, which allows them to extend technical, aural and conceptual
boundaries. Thus the basic knowledge is personalised to fit the growing unique voice
of the individual.
individual and social identity in order to function properly in society, and if they were
changed with every nuance or new example, the benefits of schema use would be lost.
Fiske and Shelley (1984: 173) suggest that well-developed social schemata are likely
to be more abstract, complex, organised, moderate and conservative than less well
47
developed schemata because wide ranging instances and experience have gone into
making them. Mature schemataare likely to be more complex than immature ones
(Linville, 1982b; Linville and Jones, 1980) and more organised in terms of the
number and structure of links. For jazz improvisation and the development of social
schemata, the mature improviser will have schemata that enable her to improvise
creatively in the widest possible social circumstances and be able (with greater
The many contested theories of how memory works are beyond the scope of this
-
thesis, but Bartlett's view of recreative memory (1932) which has been developed and
underpinned with potential neural correlates by Edelman and Tononi (2000), seems to
be the most useful model in relation to schema theory and adaptive skill. Similarly to
Hebb's proposition of generalised motor programmes, Edelman and Tononi posit that
Thus the triggering of any set of circuits that results in a set of output
responses sufficiently similar to those that were previously adaptive provides
the basis for a repeated mental act or physical performance. In this view a
memory is dynamically generated from the activity of certain subsets of
circuits... Under these conditions, a given memory cannot be identified with
any single specific set of synaptic changes because the particular synaptic
changes associated with a given output and eventually with an entire
performance, are subject to further change during that performance. So what is
called forth when an act is repeated must be any one or more of the neural
response patterns adequate to that performance, not some singular sequence or
specific detail. (2000: 98)
In this view, the brain can create a memory by linking sets of circuits within its
memory may be enhanced by the activity of emofions and other value systems. The
idea of recreative memory explains why memories alter as we age to include current
48
perceptions i. e. we see our memories in a different light as time proceeds and in fact,
every time we remember them. It also helps to explain how a schema can be adapted
and altered because each time a memory is recreated with a different neurological
circuit (although still with many of the generalisable elements in place), a different set
unrepeatable and often un-memorisable because new connections and frameworks are
being used. The analogy between musical improvisation and the way the brain works
If this view of memory is correct, then the schema is recreated (in generalised neural
circuits) and used purposefully whenever the skill itself or something similar is"
Clearly, one vital element for the memory involved in schema development is focused
concentration and full attention on the matter to be learned. James (1890) stated that
we all know what it means to give something our full attention and suggested that
attention itself results from a competition between various `trains' of thought. This
selective state of thinking leads to one `train' dominating over the others and thereby
do with the optimum amount of information that can be assimilated by the individual
2 Of course, the variety and range of different contexts in which the skill (once initially established) is
practised will encourage more associations and connections within the schema.
49
at any one time, which varies greatly. If our current schemata act as filters for
information, then only those aspects that we can already comprehend or can act upon
will even be attended to. On the other hand,' the schema will have to change if
sufficiently new information is assimilated within it and perhaps this is where the
system tips into chaos and reforms as a stable entity on a higher level of
of both environmental and educational information and only really take notice and
give attention to those elements that are different, unusual or particularly resonant for
us as individuals.
existing schemata. Davis and Tall (2002) suggest that the focus of the individual's
attention determines the structure of their schema and describe von Glaserfield's
(1981) model of attention which may shed light on schema formation. His
understanding of the nature of attention was that it is not likely to be extended over
`Pulse-like attentional moments' may help to explain the idiosyncratic and individual
many different areas for varying lengths of time. We cannot assume that people hold
50
similar schemata for the same experiences especially when they involve abstract
central to facilitating the learning process and requires much thought about how
Given the general level of connectivity within the brain, it is natural to assume that
schemata will be created as network structures and that the more connections there are
between different aspects of the schema and between different schemata, the stronger
and richer the schema for a particular skill or understanding will become. Schema
information from another area. In a complex and adaptive skill such as jazz
improvisation, it is likely that connections of the schema will link across modalities,
so that for example, aural imagination can trigger motor schemata just as visual
patterns and hand, or other body movements can. In addition, conceptual information-
on how a melody is put together or how the harmony functions, will form other
Marshall (1995) proposes that to activate the schema, an initial `probe' is required to
one or more of its constituent parts. She implies that the stimuli usually come from
the outside environment, and once one part is `excited', it spreads to all the
independently within the brain itself). The notion is that a small idea or trigger of
hugely interconnected 3
one particular element can activate a complex and schema. In
51
could lead to the instantiation of the whole root and branch system of the schema.
Marshall also believes that connections can either be inhibitory or excitatory so that
some connections will promote the spread of activation from one node to another, and
others will block it. This suggests that whole schemata can be inhibited if they appear
to the mind/brain to be incompatible with other aspects or with the current situation.
In the model of recreative memory, each time the schema is used either consciously or
unconsciously, it will be recreated and therefore altered. In terms of how the schema
changes as a result of learning, the problem is how the schema can be both stable and
adaptive, and this is similar to the problem of memory itself; we have memories that
provide us with a stable idea of ourselves and our experience, but they are not fixed or-
replicated exactly as they were made. When the schema alters or incorporates new
alteration whereby the system properties of the schema cannot remain the same and so
perhaps tip briefly into chaos before forming a new point of stasis. As a self-
organising system, we may have little control over this process except the will to learn
the skill or understand the subject and the willingness to continue to practise solving
the physical and mental problems (or a combination of both), that relate to it. The key
to the stability yet adaptability of both the schema and memory itself may lie in the
creation of many memories of the same event or action across modalities and the
learning of movement may provide the model. Adaptive movement schemata are
formed and develop from initial focus on a specific task - for example, grasping an
object. At the point where the movement is learned and is stable, as mentioned
previously, Bernstein suggests that something' happens within the schema to stop the
skill deteriorating and a stasis point is reached that forms the basis for other adaptive
52
changes. So for example, once an infant can walk, the skill is stabilised and only then
Likewise, the skill of walking does not deteriorate during the process of learning to
run, and walking itself remains an adaptive skill (albeit innately evolved) because the
terrain we walk over is constantly changing. In this view, perhaps schemata are
embedded one within the other with core elements that remain stable while others
alter to adapt to changing conditions. Unless there is damage to the brain, certain
movements, skills, ways of thinking and language structures are never lost, whatever
The predominant problem is the breadth of the theory and the lack of consensus
dynamic problem solving tool. Bartlett, (1932) viewed the schema as a way of
on the personality and previous and current knowledge and attitudes of the individual.
Both Minsky (1976) and Rummelhart (1985) define the schema as a more static
framework with `slots' that are altered depending on individual circumstances. This
view of the schema works in relatively superficial social situations where we need to
know what to do when visiting a restaurant, for example, but does not explain novel
or truly adaptive behaviour. Neither does it explain how schemata may change
53
Social schemata (which include person, self, role and event schemata) as defined by
Shelley and Fiske (1984) are viewed as organising social interaction and, as with
construct reality and that generic prior knowledge allows us to function in complex
social situations with relative ease. Bernstein (1967) and Schmidt (1975) both focused
on the motor schemata involved with movement and motor skill learning. Bernstein's
view of the schema was as a circular action-perception cycle with a comparator that
alters the schema and therefore the movement process. With Berthoz (2000) and
Niesser (1976), Bernstein believed that this aspect of schema theory was a result of
the evolutionary thrust towards developing the ability to reorganise action according
links between the many layers of sensory and motor components involved in action
which can also account for novelty in skill production, and this is most important in
the notion that the generalised motor programme is at the heart of understanding how
write one's name in a variety of contexts. Like many theorists however, Schmidt is
vague on the mechanics of schema formation and the major question of how the
schema can be both stable and adaptive simultaneously. Piaget (1952) related schema
formation and function to child development and saw the schema as a unit of thought
that grows and differentiates with the experiences of childhood. He also defined the
this action, that is, those which allow the repetition of the same action or its
54
application to new content, ' Both Piaget (1966) and Johnson (1986) view embodied
action schemata (through assimilation, reflection and accommodation) as the basis for
abstract thought and their ideas provides a link between the conceptual and motor
aspects of schema theory. The scope and flexibility of schema theory and the way it
weakness or inconsistency of the theory. Allied to this is the difficulty of working out
ways to ascertain schema content and how it changes dynamically. In fact the answers
A second major problem with schema theory is the nature of the individually
developing mind and the vast array of stimuli and influences upon it, which would
cognitive structure are based on wide ranging developmental stages and highly
On the other hand, the outcomes of our learning do appear to be generalisable in part
question really is, if the schema is not an appropriate theory for understanding
complex adaptive thought and action then what is? Of the many different theories of
55
1.7 Alternatives to Schema Theory: Dual Coding
According to dual coding theory (DCT), cognition involves the activity of two distinct
sub-systems; a verbal system for dealing directly with language and a nonverbal
imagery system for dealing with non linguistic objects and events. In the theory, the
imagens that are activated when one recognises, manipulates, or thinks about words
and objects. These representations are assumed to be modality specific so there are
different logogens and imagens for the visual, auditory, kinaesthetic and motor
properties of language and objects. The representations are also assumed to be linked
and connected to sensory output and response output so that they can function
view cognition is a variable pattern of the interplay between the two systems. The
DCT system is therefore more concrete and less abstract than the proposed schema
theory.
The empirical evidence for DCT is based on the beneficial effects of concreteness and
uniformly from abstract words like truth or justice, to concrete words like chair or
is cued by concrete stimulus words. Whether this is more to do with mnemonics and
the use of image to aid memory rather than the basis for a cognitive system, is
unclear. The DCT hypothesis for improved memory is that concrete words are likely
to have an associated image; i. e. are dually coded, whereas abstract words are not.
The evidence from Paivio (1975) and Paivio and Lambert (1981), suggests that non
verbal `code' is mnemonically stronger than verbal code. In a way this should be
obvious, and still could be a result of the predominance and strength of the visual
56
cortex; any memory system that uses visual images will necessarily be more
powerful. Similarly with sounds, Thompson and Paivio (1994) showed that picture
objects and sounds had additive effects on memory, and it is known in teaching
practice that if tasks or knowledge are presented in a multi-modal way, then they will
be memorised more effectively. The hypothesis here is that there are more and
different memories created and that people have different preferred modalities for
learning. Some do better through kinaesthetic learning, some through visual and
others through verbal or auditory activities. Combinations of these ought to prove the
most successful for memorisation and recall. In neurological terms, brain scans have
uncovered distinct areas responding to the sensory motor modality of objects and their
attributes accessed both by perceptual stimuli and words (see review by Paivio 2006:
chapter 6). Dual code theorists make the case that because words that name colours or
actions activate the same brain areas as perceived colours and action patterns, this
supports the functional reality of imagens and logogens. They claim that the brain
houses auditory, kinaesthetic and motor imagens and logogens in different locations
which are accessed by different neural pathways. What then is the system that
The DCT theorists notably, Paivio, suggest that cognitive growth is built on multiple `
imitation. The salient stages are identified by Paivio (1971: 438) and begin with the
formation of a substrate of non verbal representations and imagery derived from the
child's observations and behaviour, related to concrete objects, events and relations
among them. Language builds on this foundation and remains functionally connected
to it as referential links are formed. As the events, relations and behaviours are
dynamically organised through repetition and variation, the DCT theorists state that a
57
`natural syntax' develops which is incorporated into the imagery, enriched by motor
programs from the child's actions and from which abstract verbal skills are attained.
The idea of natural syntax appears vague and not as clear as Piaget's concepts of
assimilation and accommodation. The only difference between Piaget and the DCT
theorists seems to be the emphasis on the, functional importance of the verbal and non
verbal components of cognitive development (Paivio, 2006: 31-32) and the lack of
The notion that non verbal cognition occurs before the development of verbal
cognition is obvious and also that it is scaffolded by the language of care givers and
later of the child itself. It is very unclear however, how the dual coding system
develops without the use of abstraction in some form. Educational implications are
also obvious; that early experience should be based on rich sensory motor experiences
with concrete objects and events, with gradually associated language experience
necessary for the verbal side. There are various research projects to confirm the
importance of focus on perceptual and motor skills in young children which result in
The definition of a self-organising system is that its structure appears without pressure
from the outside i. e. internal constraints result in alterations to the system but are
independent of it. The field seeks to discover rules under which such structures may
appear and the kind of forms they take. Similarly, the definition of a system is a series
58
Kelso (1995) believes that the difficulty in characterising the internal global states of
the individual mind means that we largely ignore self organising aspects of the
dynamically interacting human, so that in most learning situations, people are treated
in exactly the same way. He suggests that the unique background and capabilities that
the individual brings to the learning environment should be carefully evaluated before
exposure to a new task, although not how this might be achieved. This view is
However, Kelso takes issue with the notion of the schema as framework for
.a
Usually, some hypothetical construct located inside the head, such as a schema
or trace is said to be built up or strengthened as a result of the learning
process. Internal changes may only be inferred through correlated changes in
performance that occur with practice. Learning, in this somewhat impotent
view, is a covert process forever inaccessible to observation: only the effects
of practise may be seen......... Practice produces a directed drift of the
response in the direction of the task requirement. In plain terms the subject's-
performance improves and becomes less variable. Your grandmother could"
have told you that. (1995: 161)
process needs are tools that can identify changes in skill behaviour in terms of the
formation and change of patterns. Thus the key to understanding learning for him, lies
tendencies) and specific parametric influences acting on those dynamics. The term
before learning something new. In the broadest definition, schemata may well have
59
these self-organising attributes; Kelso's ideas do not preclude the more dynamic
word education comes from the Greek words educare and educe, meaning to bring
The other key to understanding learning lies in the way specific information,
in the form of a pattern to be learned, modifies the intrinsic dynamics.
Learning is the process by which the pattern becomes memorised. We say that
a behavioural pattern is learned to the extent that the intrinsic dynamics are
modified in the direction of the to-be-learned pattern. Once learning is
achieved, the memorised pattern constitutes an attractor, a stable state of the
now modified pattern dynamics. (Kelso, 1995: 163)
Kelso (1997) also thinks it highly likely that, in common with many natural physical
processes, that learning involves the passage from one organised state to another, (i. e.,
perhaps from one schema to another), rather than from disorder to order. In keeping
with the ideas about complexity by Lewin (1992) et al, he posits that dynamic, self-
organising learning systems exist at the. edge of chaos, so that relatively small local
As each person possesses their own individual `signature' or `attractor layout', they
will learn according to the individual lay out of their neuronal connections and
r
previous formations. Kelso makes a strong case therefore that the individual should be
60
the significant unit of analysis in learning research (rather than the group or the
species) and that generic laws of learning may emerge by studying the individual.
Kelso also states that, whether some tasks are learned more easily than others (e.g. in
terms of rate of learning and performance efficiency) depends on the extent to which
instantiated. For example, as previously noted, once they have learned it, people can
write their name large or small, in all kinds of different media, with the hand, big toe
or even a pencil attached to the nose! This should mean that learning involves changes
in the dynamics of the system that practises the task and also in other coordination
systemsas well and suggeststhat the degree of abstractionby the brain is very high
indeed. The ability of normal humans to write their name with any limb, on any kind
of surface, large or small, is directly related to the generalisation and abstraction that
occurs in schema theory. A skill learned by the fingers of the right or left hand, using
kinaesthetic, motor and visual modalities, can be transferred with little or no practise
to another part of the body and a range of surfaces. The multi-modal schema of the
varying and flexible conditions. The signature is still recognisable as the individual's
own whether written in stone or sand or paint or in tiny or huge letters, and its
61
1.9 Computational theory of mind
There are two principle ways to view computational theory of mind; the first is the
defining computability in a highly abstract sense (which reveals nothing about how
the mind actually works). The second is an information processing model based,on
the idea that the brain, as a biological entity, processes information from the
and behave adaptably. The idea of the computational brain is highly contested, and
whilst a full review of the computational theory of mind is beyond of the scope of this
thesis, the basic ideas are as follows, from a review by Churchland (2001). The
algorithmic school envisages the brain as a series of digital computers that run various
programmes (with the underlying analogy of software being run by the hardware of
anatomical brain structures), when for example, reasoning takes place. In this view
representations are like sentence structure, with syntax and semantic content that can.
narrowly defined brain processes such as thinking logically or playing chess. The
remains static and unchanging throughout life, although the software alters.
The information processing model emphasises the architecture of the brain itself and a
common theme running through the many theories, is that the brain is a highly
parallel system, with many interconnected elements, whose wiring and connectivity
change with human development and learning. In this view, representation is a pattern
of activation across neural nets and networks which can be formulated as vectors and
makes the networks more complex and the hardware/software analogy is rejected on
62
the basis that biological systems have evolved to perform highly specialised tasks.
Changes in neurological wiring occur due to experience, and much long standing
neural research backs this up, including how learning changes during infancy.
Connectionist models have been explored in memory, perception and motor control
research successfully but not in complex teal life experience of situations. This seems
learning and fails to account for the complexities and adaptation of the individual
Having discussed schema theory, and its strengths and weaknesses as a theoretical
proposition, the next chapter focuses on how schema theory might be useful for
63
2. Schema theory for learning
2.1 Abstraction in the emerging schema
abstraction may occur. However, in extensive research into chess players and
particularly in what distinguishes an expert player from merely a good one, both
visual and conceptual schemata have been described and explained in some detail. For
instance it was proposed by Binet (1894) that in learning to play chess, at the
elements which later give way to ever greater levels of abstraction, more dynamic
elements and less defined imagery. Binet illustrated this clearly in his observations of
blindfold chess games and interviews with players. Blindfold chess players need to
be able to: visualise and remember changes in the position of chessmen as they occur;
imagine the implications of the changes; follow and predict the play of their
opponents and plan their next sets of moves, without physically seeing the
chessboard. Novice players told Binet that they imagined the chessboard:
exactly as if they were looking attentively at the board and the pieces. They
retain a veritable mental photograph in which the board appears clearly with
its black and white squares, and all the pieces are present in colour and with
their characteristic shapes. (1894: 156)
Binet considered this to be the first stage of abstraction because whilst it was
imagined and contained strong visual images, it was fairly fixed in character and
certain insignificant details were missing; it was not therefore a purely replicative or
blindfold chess and the implication is that this emerging first stage schema is likely to
be less dynamic and so less useful to the player' because the visual image does not
64
The second intermediate category of players had more skill, but still tended to
represent the chessboard visually, although the image was less clear and more
impressionistic and simplified. Binet called this the second level of abstraction
because the schema was developing and there was much less emphasis on the purely
visual aspects, although the players were still unable to play blindfold chess with any
fluency. The third category of players, the most skilled, had fully developed schemata
(1894: 159). He states that the colour of the image had gone, along with all the specific
details of the chessmen, which were now seen purely as friend or foe. Concrete
aspects had ceased to be important to this level of player, but the position of the
pieces (or the geometrical schema as Binet refers to it), was retained particularly in
the direction of the movement of a piece and the square that it must stop. Binet
described this as `geometrical memory', and because of its dynamic and abstract
quality it is a highly developed schema which enables the player to manipulate the
elements of the game most effectively in his or her imagination. Thus the master
player calls up only general knowledge of where the piece stands in relation to other
aspects of his and his opponent's position. What the master has acquired that the
novice lacks, is salient and dynamic knowledge of previous games. Indeed, according
to De Groot (1978) a Grand Master has a working knowledge of more than 100,000
games. This knowledge may form the schema assemblage of the expert player who
uses it to make better and quicker moves. He or she does not analyse more
possibilities from the memories of previous games, as for example the Fritz chess
computers does, but rather knows intuitively the better moves and their implications
played and analysed. At the highest levels in chess playing, the master will not
65
consciously analyse anything, but rather `see' the best or right move in the same way
The changes in the emerging and developing schema are seen clearly in the blindfold
chess players because they are imagining visual elements and patterns, and as their
visual schema changes to allow greater adaptability, so their level of skill increases.
Changes to skill levels in chess players are to some degree quantifiable due to the
ranking system that pertains in the competitive arena, which is relatively accurate
with regard to skill level and predictive of the likely outcomes of games between
musical setting is more difficult however, because the starting parameters are much
less well defined, less reliant on visual elements and highly dependant on more
Despite the sheer breadth of mind and body processes explained by the various
schema theories, what they share is the need to understand how the human mind
works in a dynamic sense in real life, and particularly how the individual learns and
continues to learn and apply knowledge and skills to various different environments
and situations. From such a broad spectrum we need to narrow down and formulate
the aspects of schema theory that are relevant to jazz improvisation. The primary
motor, with aural memory and auditory feedback triggering and driving the requisite
motor schemata to produce the improvised musical line. The primary schema that
improvised ideas within the jazz genre, the player also requires some aural knowledge
66
of the sound and rhythmic feel of the music, and perhaps a reference to at least one
historical era for imitative purposes. They also need to have some understanding of
the musical theory surrounding jazz, which can be used to scaffold the learning-to-
improvise process. These latter schemata are also primarily aural although they are
related to listening rather than the imagination of ideas and also to getting a whole
aural sense of the jazz musical tradition. The conceptual elements will encompass
blues and 32 bar standard); musical theory related to melodic development; variation
components may relate to specific instrumental technique and the need to develop
left and right hands on the piano or the guitar in faster passages or the articulation and
The process of learning to improvise at its simplest involves the development and
perception cycle devised by Niesser (1976) From the available information (the
background musical knowledge will vary greatly from individual to individual), the
player imagines a phrase and in executing it adds to their aural memory, stock of `
theoretical knowledge and embodied motor skill within the schema assemblage.
Kinaesthetic and motor feedback on the flow and route of the movement plus auditory
feedback and comparison of the intended idea and the actual notes played, will be also
be added to the schema and alter its structure and connections. This schema
improvised line, which will once again increase the store of multi-modal information
67
To attempt to explore in more detail which aspects of the schema develop and what is
schema for jazz improvisation, i. e. what does one need to know and to be able to do,
in order to improvise within the melodic jazz idiom? At the simplest level, one needs
to be able to imagine a melodic line (within a harmonic context) or at least the outline
involves three primary aspects: aural memory and auditory feedback, motor skills of
varying types and cognitive/conceptual elements for scaffolding the skill, then the
three areas. Taking the motor schema first; one needs to be able to move the fingers
(and whatever other muscular centres are physically required to play a musical
instrument), from triggers directed by aural memory and also perhaps from notation.
The motor schema or assemblage has to control movement patterns that are not
previously learned patterns based around scales or harmonic content. It would seem
one timeframe. Both the motor and aural schemata have to emulate and develop the
rhythmic aspects of the jazz genre and to embody the all important `feel' of the music.
In addition, the motor schema has to embody and respond to the emotional content of
watching the movements in relation to the sound in the early stages), conceptual
68
The aural or auditory schema needs to initiate the evolution and imagination of a
melody within a harmonic context that is, at least initially, appropriate to the player's
own musical tastes and identity, and to the genre itself. To do this there has to be
conscious exercise, in order to build an. aural understanding of the sound, feel and
frameworks of the music. To be able to imagine a melody or even the gist of one,
requires a sense of what melody is; how the rhythmic feel of the jazz genre affects it;
how a cohesive melody is created; how ideas can be developed using a range of
strategies from analysis of melodic contour to variation technique and how rhythmic
energy affects melody. Having begun to understand what melody is, it is then a matter
of creating new examples and developing the ideas within them. This process can be
initiated by focusing on common jazz repertoire and using the melodic contour,
rhythmic energy and variation technique to expand on what is already a cohesive and
useful framework. Also within the aural schema will be a developing sense of the'
harmonic context of the melody and the extent to which it informs melodic
chords), and conceptual information about how functional harmony works within the
jazz genre.
and can scaffold the development of both the auditory/aural and motor schemata. It is
comprised of the more rational knowledge base related to repertoire, music theory,
technical aspects of instrumental learning and knowledge of jazz related theory and
involves integrating theoretical elements into motor patterns and aural imagination
and from there into the more emotional musical voice of the individual player. The
69
contemporary approach to formal jazz education generally employed in many music
schools is based on the practice and assimilation of theoretical aspects, from which
the more creative improvisatory elements are thought to evolve. This practice is
Other schemata that are also involved in the process of learning jazz improvisation are
about traits and goals that shape other people's behaviour; the self schema which
contains information about ones' 'own personality and behaviour and is particularly
important in developing self identity and in focusing on elements that are important to
them; role schemata that affect an individual's response to appropriate norms and
behaviours attached to a social position and event schemata that relate to what
range of social and musical situations and will have many effects on the way that the
improviser will develop. In addition there are other schemata for visual and
kinaesthetic aspects both of which are vital to the learning of jazz improvisation.
Clearly, visual schemata are needed to scaffold movements and motor programs on
the instrument. They are required for interaction and visual clues to musical ideas
with other musicians and to read and decode musical notation and other symbols, as
for motor development, bodily posture and orientation, and general bodily awareness
and feedback at the core of playing a musical instrument or singing. For the purposes
of this thesis, however, the primary focus will be on the motor, aural and cognitive
70
2.3 Possible schema abstractions for beginner, intermediate and advanced jazz
improvisers
Given that schemata at different stages of jazz improvisational skill will not only
differ between individuals but also between various skill levels; what might the
might the schema assemblages of a jazz musician look like at these three stages and
which elements are likely to be abstracted from one level to another? Starting with a
beginner and assuming that they possess sufficient technical musical skill and
instrumental control to begin the process, then one of the first abstractions of the
musical schemata is likely to involve altering the trigger of motor programmes from
visual to aural. Classically or formally trained players are used to playing and learning
music from written notation and `translating' it into movements by relying both on
visual and aural feedback for correction. The process of learning music from notation
is necessarily slow, especially if someone other than the musician has written it and
player in order to make complete sense of the music. Thus the process of altering
visual to aural triggers has to be elaborated in two different ways: the musician has to
both imagine their own music and also to develop the aural to motor learning process
so that what is heard and imagined can be played instantaneously. Information has to
be abstracted from the musical experience of, for example, playing a jazz standard,
which alters the trigger for motor programmes from the visual distance between a
71
The process of imagining musical ideas (in jazz improvisation) will be based on the
previous schematathat the musician has developed for melody and harmony. There
are literally thousands of songs, nursery rhymes, and fragments of melodies in the
mind which will provide a rich source, but which will vary in content between
individuals because of the huge range of musical tastes and styles they are likely to
have encountered and been influenced by throughout their life. For jazz however,
there is also a stylistic requirement and a harmonic basis, both of which will affect the
choice of melody for improvisation and the learner has to abstract enough information
Beginners will therefore be attempting to hear and find on the instrument ideas that
they have imagined rather than read in notated form, which will be influenced to a
high degree by the music they have previously experienced. As mentioned before, one
of the ways in which novices can improve their melodic sense is by focusing on
developing elements of a known melody and using that as a framework. Using the
tune (having first learned it by heart), provides a means of varying the learned
elements within a fairly tight framework, whilst keeping the contour in order to
orientate themselves around the whole 32 bar framework. This kind of schema
development is similar to the way that jazz musicians approached improvisation in the
1920s and 30s, which involved the development of alternative and similar singable
melodies that fit well within the standard framework and use elements or echoes of
the tune. The underlying process is still finding the intervals on the instrument from
the aural imagination and the main limitation with the strictly melodic approach,
believe that the beginner needs technical mastery of both the harmonic theory and the
72
instrument before any worthwhile improvisation can occur. They promote the idea of
studying vast arrays of scales,patterns, licks and exercisesin every key in order to
find the intervals automatically and in a way that is unrelated to a melodic line. This
The beginner using the harmony of the standard for improvisation has another
framework which provides a basis for further exploration and more scope for melodic
all of the theoretical knowledge required to improvise using harmony with a natural
and melodic improvisational voice. The beginner working on the harmonic aspects
will focus on integrating some of the theoretical elements (chord tones, key centres,
transpositions, related scales), in the imagination and on the instrument. They will
necessarily be concerned with what musical ideas will `fit' the harmonic progression
and be consonant with the harmony. This part of the learning process can be viewed
as mechanical because on the one hand this practice did not form part of the
traditional way of learning to improvise, and on the other, because beginners want to
play the right notes and get to grips with how to play a motif in several keys for
The schema at this stage then, may incorporate aural, motor and theoretical elements
of the skill that are focused on correctness and finding and using the `right' notes
within the harmonic context. In this first schema stage the novice is concerned
primarily with `what to do' which Schmidt (1976) calls the cognitive phase, where the
beginner is working out the most effective way to do the task in a problem solving
manner. Schmidt suggests that this initial phase is characterised by the largest and
most dramatic gains in the whole learning process although performance is usually
73
inconsistent because learners are trying different ways to solve the problem. At this
solving strategies they are using and what they think is happening. Teaching at this
stage would be characterised by detailed work first on the melody and then the
melody. Great emphasis would be placed on the sound and rhythmic feel of the
improvisation and wider timbral and expressive qualities explored. Listening would
also form an important and integral part of learning in order to build the background
listening schema which informs aural imagination and is the basis for jazz
improvisation.
Similarly, for beginners learning in the more traditional way (by transcribing
improvised solos from records), the first stage of schema development might be to
develop sufficient aural memory to remember for example, a four bar phrase and then
theoretical aspects in the first instance and focuses more on the direct translation of
heard sound to aural memory to hands (or other musculature) and instrument.. Here
the beginner is imitating the musical results of the fully developed jazz improvisation
elements about the development of ideas, the use of timbre and dynamics, the
interaction between band and soloist and the way that the solo is communicated.
An intermediate level, might be attained when the improviser can extemporise around
the contour of the melody using elements from within it; remember the chord
progression; know by sound and sight the various chord tones and appropriate scales
and play an improvised musical idea whilst anticipating the next chord or sequence of
74
chords. The intermediate improviser may also be able to play some phrases at will
and may be less prescriptive in terms of following every chord change, by focusing
instead on the unfolding line and being aware of the harmonic progression in the
later in the text), provides a tantalising glimpse of the schema developing sufficiently
again as the skill is still unstable through lack of concentration or the full integration
of all of the required elements. Schmidt (1976) calls this the associative phase where
the student makes more subtle adjustments to the performance of the skill. At this
stage the improvisation becomes more consistent and the progress of it more gradual.
This is the point at which Bernstein suggests widely varying the conditions and
aspects of the teaching context in order to strengthen and stabilise the skill. In this
phase Schmidt also suggests that the verbal cognitive aspects of the task have largely
been dropped and that the emphasis is now on the how rather than the which. The ear
and the ability to `hear' musical ideas, develops with practice alongside a more
would involve widely varying the repertoire learned to include many variations of
keys, rhythmic and melodic elements. Different approaches and strategies, which are
described in more detail in the practical investigation, could now be introduced, with
involve memorising and transcribing a whole solo by ear. At this stage, the player will
now be familiar with the aurally triggered intervals over some part of the instrument
and have developed sufficient aural memory to improve their skills through further
transcription or finding their own individual voice - they now have the schema in
75
place to be able to do this. The major advantage of transcription over other methods
would seem to be that the player absorbs and imitates in increasingly greater detail a
whole musical idea and conception with all its melodic, timbral and rhythmic
What then has been abstracted from the first level to the intermediate? Firstly, the
relationships between the intervals in the space on the instrument have been
abstracted from all of the tunes learned by heart and explored melodically. Secondly,
the motor programmes for executing the music have been expanded to include aural
as well as visual triggers. Thirdly, this motor/aural skill has then been integrated with
example, chord symbols which provide a short hand reference for the harmonic
sense of orientation within the progression and over the course of the 32 bars as they
are able to predict in their aural imagination, what is coming up next and respond to it
and this sense has been abstracted from repeated listenings and improvising over ever
Fully skilled players are able to transfer previously learned information to any new
standard and can even improvise well over tunes that are not well known to them,
because their schema now allows greater flexibility and the highest levels of
abstraction. At this level, players will not be consciously thinking about the melody,
key centres or the harmonic progression whilst improvising on standards with which
they are familiar. Like the advanced chess players, they are unconscious of all detail
and process, and think aurally or respond to ideas in terms of direction, resolution and
76
unfolding the musical ideas in the moment. The schema incorporates technical,
unique style and voice of the individual for the purpose of optimal musical
exploration and expression. As skilled improvisers do not have to focus on the details
any more, there is time and energy to listen and respond to band members, audience,
and new ideas from the imagination, all of which take the improviser to another level
the broadest and most metaphorical of terms, such is the automaticity of the process.4
The advanced stages of schema development are synonymous with Schmidt's third
This occurs most often after many years and the automatic nature of the skill means
that it can now be performed with less interference from many simultaneous
activities, which frees up the conscious mind to take in and respond to, other ambient
and environmental information. Schmidt (1976) points out that these high-level motor
processes are almost never studied in research environments because of the difficulty
in controlling and manipulating all the variables and the length of time involved in
such a process. At this stage, the teaching would involve: greater emphasis on
perhaps in the problem-solving mode and possibly collaborative work with other art
4 For this reason, I have focused the practical investigation on improvisers who are learning, so that at
least parts of the skill may be accessible to consciousness and some verbalisation on their part.
77
What elements are likely to have been abstracted between the intermediate stage and
that the advanced player is able to think of a line and to play it and is also able to
change its course at will in response to the improvised melody as it unfolds. The
theoretical knowledge has been integrated fully into the schema (through working on
specific examples), so that the improviser can now instantaneously translate chord
symbols, knowledge of key centres and so on, into sounds that not only fit the
harmony, but are also in keeping with what she actually wants to play. As more
repertoire has been added by the player, many different intervallic and harmonic
patterns have been assimilated and the relationships between them abstracted to
other band members have also been abstracted especially the speed at which these
occur and as further abstraction allows greater automaticity of playing, the mature
improviser is able to become more open and responsive to group and audience
dynamic.
To summarise, schema theory helps to explain how improvised jazz music is made by
this integration and connectivity can take the form of coordinating separate memories
in multi-modal areas or of abstracting the essential elements from the different parts
to form one generalised schema. For example, a visual clue may cause the schema
made of visual elements to emerge and at 'any point in the improvisation other
elements could take over. Schemata fit well with the notion that skill acquisition leads
78
to ever-greater levels of abstraction and automaticity. For example, what begins as an
awkward or slow movement or mental task requiring great effort and concentration
gradually becomes a graceful and rhythmic movement or thought that flows naturally
into others and becomes unconscious. The enabling mechanism for these greater
cognitive connections which are particularly significant in the playing of music. Thus,
from our environment and it appears that the richer the sensory environment for
learning, the greater the potential for memory creation and the formation of schemata.
In the case of jazz improvisation, where jazz `feel' and rhythm are especially
important to the music, Trevarthen's theory of the Intrinsic Motive Pulse (1999/2000)
useful not only because it suggests that body movement will enhance the facilitation
of the rhythmic schema but also because I am trying to find a theory for schemata that
states:
In the generating and regulating core of the brain, the Intrinsic Motive
Formation, movement-creating reticular networks and nuclei are intricately
combined with the neurochemical systems of emotion. The same activating
neurons that select movements and control their energy and smoothness also
cause changes in emotions felt, and the intensity and "colour" of
consciousness. (Trevarthen 1999/2000: 161)
This would suggest that schemata are overlaid with emotion both in formation and
function and may therefore provide motivating factors (amongst others) in the
learning process.
79
Although the schema provides us with a unique and idiosyncratic framework for
communication is possible between us at all. ) Thus if both performer and listener have
The central tenet of the current thesis is that higher order skills such as improvisation
are guided by schemata which enable dynamic and adaptive behaviour and the plan
for this section is to formulate a schema theory for jazz improvisation that
understanding the process. In this theory it is assumed that (as in the practical
investigation) the improvisers are beginners but already trained musicians who are
proficient instrumentalists and who already have a musical background from which
to draw for the development of a new skill. The rationale for using trained musicians
in the practical study was to be able to focus on the improvised musical ideas
instrument. With complete beginners it may have taken too long for them to organise
both the imagined idea and the fingering or technical requirements on the instrument
instrumental technique in tandem, but would have to be conducted over a much longer
time period.
,r
80
2.6 Motor schema for jazz improvisation
To we begin with movement, Schmidt (1975) has stated that when a person executes a
move to reach a goal, she `stores' up to four types of elements: i) the initial conditions
i. e. information about the starting point of the movement provided by the senses; ii)
the `programme' for the motor command built from previous schemata i. e.
information about the task to be executed; iii) the sensory consequences of the
movement, for example, the outcome and how it felt during and after the movement.
In jazz improvisation, the initial conditions are fairly complex, vary from individual to
individual and involve the musical and physical context in which the improvisation is
to take place. This may include an expectation or anticipation of the melodic and
player and their instrument which is integrated to the aural and motor aspects; a visual
referent in the form of notated music (if used); the level of concentration of the
individual, and an awareness of the role of the particular players in the group i. e. a
notion of what is expected in a communal way. The initial conditions will obviously
also be affected by the physical environment (in terms of heat, light, acoustic) and
also by the psychological state of the individual players and the group as a whole (in
terms of motivation, confidence, perceived and actual levels of ability and so on).
The programme for the motor commands relates to the schemata that have already
previously, with classically trained musicians, these will be invoked by visual triggers
81
from notated music and here previous schemata have to be altered or extended to
include triggers from aural imagination or other players. So whilst the motor areas
already form a firm basis for the learning of improvisation (complete beginners would
have to learn the movements from scratch), the way in which the movements are
awareness that the musician has developed with the instrument, which forms an
extension of the body schema. Different types of instruments will have varying
kinaesthetic properties and also involve different modalities, for example, pianists will
be able to use more visual schemata in the development of motor programmes simply
because of the visual layout of the keyboards Brass and wind players will be much
more aware of how breathing and embouchure affect the production of notes and the
way that these aspects are integrated with hand movements and aural imagination.
Their schema assemblages, therefore, may have less emphasis on visual information
There are other parameters in existing motor programmes for musicians that have to
be altered in order for them to be able to improvise and these relate to aural memory
to hand or body movements and the speeding up of response times both to internally
and externally generated music. Formally trained musicians are generally used to
which are both heard and imagined. Finally, in relation to motor command
background of the standard tune and this means that the beginning improviser has to
82
alter their perspective on their current harmonic understanding and adapt it to the new
situation. This process may entail using existing scales or patterns in a new way or
making different connections between harmonic and motor patterns and would be an
imagined idea with the actual music produced and relating that to required changes in
the movement or series of movements. In improvisation this may be the most crucial
aspect of the whole development of the integrated schema i. e. feeling for and finding
musical idea. The sensory consequences relate here to movements in space to find the
musculature required to support the notes or phrases and visual movements related to
monitoring hands in collaboration with aural triggers. As visual, aural, motor and
kinaesthetic modalities are processed in the brain at varying speeds, the integration of
this sensory information and feedback is a highly complex process. One can imagine
that the schema is organising these parameters so that eventually they flow in one
The outcome of the movement or series of movements, is the execution of the musical
line, which in turn is compared to an idea of the intended outcome and to the notion
that the player has in their aural imagination about what sounds good or right in a
particular musical context. Whilst the outcomes of the movement primarily affect the
aural sense of the player, they also affect the conceptual schema of what is required in
the musical context. This process involves musical meaning, and it is a common
experience among players, both beginners and mature improvisers, that to play `ideas
83
that fit' arbitrarily over the harmonic framework is not really a satisfying experience;
rather they strive for creating meaningful musical ideas that are expressive of their
own unique voice and musical taste. Therefore, the outcomes of the movements and
the musical consequences are bound up with the player's musical and personal
The sensory consequences and outcomes of the movements relate directly to actual
aural information, imagined aural information, visual information and various aspects
of musical taste, identity and theoretical knowledge of `notes that will fit'. Of primary
importance also in the process of following the outcomes of the movement, is how it
feeling of practising the emergent skill (at whatever level) is important because the
improvisers can experience the feeling of improvising, so that all the various elements
required in the skill can begin the process of integration through the emergence of the
schema.
As Schmidt points out, the schema is not the set of the data provided by the
between several sensory and motor components of action. The focus on the
relationship between the various elements allows the most salient aspects of the
84
2.6.1 Motor abstractions
The elements likely to be abstracted during the development of the motor schema for
various stages of progression in the skill. For example, the formally trained beginner,
who learns a tune by finding the ' intervals spatially, will then (probably
instantaneously) be able to abstract that information into new keys or positions on the
instrument. The player will also be able to alter the rhythmic parameters easily to
terminology, this represents a first stage of motor abstraction because the melodic
material has been learned, not created in the aural imagination e.g. the melodic
The second stage may involve initiating the motor program from imagined and heard
sounds and this could be developed by using the same melodic framework and
developed from specific intervals of the tune; motifs developed between phrases; lines
imagined using similar contours to the tune, and so on. This stage of the development
of the motor schema (which for simplicity we can call the intermediate), is also
knowledge of harmony and intervals. The improviser can practise a parallel series of
scales, patterns and arpeggios that assist in the development of the motor schema by
motor patterns, to solve the problem of how the imagined and unfolding improvised
line is actually executed and continued. The sound of the harmonic basis of the tune
provides another framework for the motor schema and aural imagination process to
develop, and the improviser can explore and experiment with sounds against this
85
background, which provides another reference point. The intermediate improviser is
still however, dealing with specific movements, scales, harmonic progressions and
The third stage of motor schema abstraction (i. e. that of advanced players) 6 will
involve being able to imagine and play a musical line without thinking consciously
about any outside parameter (e.g. where the notes are on the instrument, the key
centre or chord structure or the orientation within the progression). Information used
to initiate motor programmes is generalised across the repertoire (and possibly across
different musical genres), and there is likely to be little discrepancy between the
intended and actual movements (related to sounds) because the player can change the
flow of the movement instantaneously, much in the way that we alter the flow of any
other kind of bodily movement to reach a target. The improviser no longer relies "
exclusively on motor patterns but can choose to play what she hears or imagines
because the process in now entirely adaptive. This phase, which may continue to
develop, is characterised by the player being unable to remember what she has just
played, because the movement is within the moment and no longer based on any
particular framework. Given that the improvisation may also have been initiated and
The elements likely to be abstracted through the various stages or states of schema
development have already been described. They include: the extension or altering of
motor triggers from visual to aural (from notated music to aural imagination); the
6 This is a somewhat arbitrary grouping as, like Piaget's stages there may be no clear cut defining
difference between stages and no specific end to the development of the improvisational process.
86
within the instrument; the abstraction of the relationships between the spatial intervals
into different keys; the abstraction of the intervals from scales to use in different
sophisticated musical lines abstracted from more exposure to and assimilation of wide
ranging repertoire and developing the sound and ideas that most represent the
personality of the improviser and developing the ability to generate ideas and extend
them over longer time frames. The information to be abstracted comes from a number
The conceptual and theoretical schema scaffolds the more fundamental aural to
muscle and motor schema by providing a broader framework than a melodic line and
by more systematic use of intervallic patterns. The beginner is made aware of the
functional nature of jazz harmony, usually in the form of how chord families relate to
key centres and specific scales, and how to recognise these progressions within the
jazz standard tune. The separate chord tones have to be learned individually in order
to understand how chord symbols work, in relation to the overall key of the passage,
and also as sounds. The sound aspect involves the intervallic relationships between
chord tones, and the functional relationships between chords in the same and different
families. For example, the roles and functions of the minor seventh or dominant
chords, the `colour' of the chord i. e. how it sounds, and the kinds of expressive
87
fellow players and listeners also), without adhering too strictly to the chord tones or
scales that underpin chord families. The theoretical schema allows the knowledge to
be used like a tool for the more creative purposes of the improviser. Later elements of
the theoretical schema might include: knowledge and use of the higher chord tones
e.g. ninths, elevenths, thirteenths and altered colour tones like the flattened fifth or
which again, may provide a fresh strategy to an old standard without destroying
completely the underlying framework and enabling the improviser (and listener) to
The scales provide another reference point for the theoretical schema by linking single
lines, chord tones and keys and by providing a linear starting point for improvisation,
which is other than the melody. Usually in the jazz idiom, scales are learned in their
basic form and then practised in modes to allow any scale to be played on any note
which can be easily related to specific chords within a given family. This process
trained players begin and end scales on the same root starting note. After the whole
scale is learned, it is then broken into intervals, e.g. thirds, fourths, and fifths, to
enable the improviser not only to hear the different sounds but also to practise feeling
the intervals on the instrument. Specific repertoire can also be used to practise the
sound and spaces of particular intervals and there are innumerable ways to make up
scalic and intervallic patterns in order to improve technique and the ability to `get
round' a harmonic progression. Scales can be extended to include blue notes and
other colour tones, so that the sound palette and technical scope of the player is
broadened further. The varied and creative practice of scales ought to enable the
88
improviser to play what they hear anywhere on the instrument, because the sense of
the relationship between intervals is established and integrated aurally and spatially.
schema proceeds in a similar way to that of movement and that from deliberate
form the adaptive conceptual schema. Each change of key, altered scale pattern,
extension to the repertoire of tunes and chords and the exploration of expressive
For the beginner improviser, the first stage of abstraction within the theoretical
schema will be the assimilation of the factual theoretical information (e. g. component
parts of scales, chord tones etc.) from the musical notation, and its translation into
theoretically what notes will fit, and has to learn the specific instance in the musical
The abstraction is to understand what chord symbols mean in terms of notes and
sound so that they are recognisable in new situations. The symbols allow for and
placement within the bar, whilst retaining the basic harmonic framework. In terms of
89
scales, the abstraction comes from their complete memorisation in whole forms across
the range of the instrument and then their deconstruction into modes, intervals and
patterns. The initial process appears to be the learning of exact and accurate forms and
patterns, in ways that the beginner can recognise across the instrument and the
abstraction is that they have to be both extended and memorised from previous more
ideas within a specific context, i. e. knowing that a section of the standard was in the
key of Ab, and using clues from the harmonic progression, the improviser would
know to use notes within that key to improvise. The intermediate knowledge of scale
patterns and chord tones would provide the improviser with a number of different
ways in which to initiate improvised ideas (including the initial use of the melody
itself). However, the intermediate improviser would be unable to freely generalise the
information into new standards because of insufficient practice with varied conditions
and repertoire. They would also be able to recognise the chord symbols and
extrapolate various useful elements from them, to begin to inform not just what is
likely to be correct in their own improvised ideas but what is characteristic of their
own unique voice and what is creative about improvisation. The abstraction of
more intuitive and creative process rather than slavish adherence to correct notes
In the advanced stage of theoretical schema abstraction, the conceptual aspects have
been thoroughly absorbed and are now integrated with the aural and motor aspects of
the process. The improviser will be able play a convincing solo over a standard that
90
she may not know well, simply by using abstracted and generalised information
available from a notated copy or from the sound of the music itself. The scales, (their
constituent parts and altered notes), have by this time become completely assimilated
into the motor and aural pathways of the player, who is now able to call up any
interval or pattern of notes at will and can use them in new combinations to create
novel ideas. The advanced improviser will be able to cope with difficult keys or
embodied and at the service of more creative aspects of improvisation. In addition the
harmonic background has become (instead of the chord by chord progression of the
beginner), a flow of changing sounds or colours over 32 bars with the theoretical
information absorbed into the unfolding musical line of the improviser. This sense of
the progression that incorporates all the practised notes and harmonies provides yet
The aural schema is rather complex and involves not just learning to imagine, sing or
play melodic improvised lines but to do so in the melodic jazz genre, an essential
authenticity, the player must have previously heard and assimilated a reasonable
amount of jazz music. As I explain in more detail later, for jazz musicians growing up
in the early part of the twentieth century (1920s, 30s and 40s), the popular music of
the day was jazz and so those players tended to be surrounded by live or recorded
examples of it in their everyday environments. This is no longer the case and so the
first stage of aural schema development requires that examples of jazz improvisation
be heard and felt in the body through movement' For the aural schema to develop I
feel it is helpful to sing along with the music and to be able to sing improvised lines,
91
(although some players manage to learn without doing this). So the sense of style and
the idiom of the music and its rhythmic components have to be absorbed by the
The aural schema develops in tandem with the motor and theoretical schemata and
after a sense of what is required is established from listening to examples, the second
stage is the learning of tunes with a jazz feel in order to absorb the idea of contour,
phrase lengths, development of motifs, contrasting sections and the feel and sound of
transcribed (usually by ear) tunes and solos by their favourite players, thereby
absorbing the sound and style directly into aural imagination and thence to the motor
schema. The process enabled the player to develop the relationship between aural and
unconsciously) that provide the necessary schema framework for their own individual
improvised lines to emerge. The aural transcription process also allowed the novice to
absorb the more creative and cohesive aspects of the improvised solo by dint of
imitating an advanced player and learning the flow of the solo, timbral aspects and
various other nuances developed by the improviser over a long period of time.
Obviously, the aural schema will develop in a different way if the improviser is not
using transcription, but is also likely to be more individual (if less dynamic) from the
beginning. Having learned and memorised a series of jazz tunes, the novice
improviser can then begin to vary and extemporise around them, imagining motifs
For the beginner, the aural harmonic background has many elements to assimilate
because of all of the possible sounds contained within it. In conjunction with the
92
theoretical and motor schemata, the beginner will listen and absorb the related sounds
and attempt to imagine improvised lines using the new harmonically based notes. As
with the motor and theoretical schemata, the focus will be initially on playing
consonant or correct sounding notes over the harmonic background, but as the schema
develops, more colours may be explored and experimented with. The process of
hearing the `right' notes develops in tandem with hearing (and playing) melodies that
the improviser likes and wants to hear, motivated by their musical tastes and identity.
Similarly, the kinds of ideas that the player imagines will also be influenced by the
music they are listening to and that in turn will be guided by the type of jazz or
improvisation that most appeals to them. The aural schema development is led by the
motor and theoretical schemata but also strongly influenced by the musical
personality and identity of the player and the need to play something that is
meaningful and not merely to reproduce a pattern or something that will fit. The aural
schema is also guided by an often intangible feeling of a musical idea that cannot be
The advanced state of the aural schema occurs when the player is able to imagine and
conscious scaffold or framework. The improvised ideas can be initiated from intervals
or the melody or from the contour or energy of the tune or from any aspect of the
harmonic basis, at will. Aural memory will have developed over time, so that the
player can respond to externally heard musical ideas from other players and can
develop ideas over longer time frames, again in a free-flowing way. The development
of aural memory also enables the player to quickly learn new tunes and to abstract the
information required in order to improvise effectively over them. At this level there is
a confidence in the aural skill that allows the player to explore, take risks and to move
93
away from ideas that are predictable, so that the creative skill itself is used to its full
potential.
The first level of abstraction is the hearing and assimilating of intervals from the
framework of the melody by memorising them and then using them as the basis for
process of hearing and imagining the intervals seems to have the same neural
correlate, so the process of hearing and imagining the sounds appears to be the same.
The subsequent stage of the abstraction is to imagine the intervals and melodic
phrases outside of the framework of the tune itself which occurs in tandem with the
theoretical understanding and the motor schema, and is based at first on correct and
_
The collective schema is based on elements from all of the above but focused
outwards towards members of the ensemble rather than inwards on the processes of
the individual improviser. It is an aural and visually based schema which is highly
attuned and responsive to the musical ideas and expression within the ensemble of
players. A first stage of such a schema is being aware of other players within the
group e.g. actually hearing them as the improvised solo is unfolding. In an educational
or other informal learning setting, players can be made aware of each others' roles by
being involved with them as well, i. e. all of the group can learn a bass line or a drum
rhythm. A second stage would be for the improviser to be responsive to the overall
I
feel and expressive qualities of the band and for them to reciprocate ideas. It may be
94
difficult for the novice to be responsive to both their own improvisation and to the
collective qualities of the group but as the skill develops so does the individual's
ability to step outside of the more internal processes and to listen to the musical
environment. The process is aided by the need to listen to the underlying harmony and
the different ways that chord players will perform harmonically and rhythmically.
The novice player is likely to focus on her own imagined and executed ideas until she
can develop through the schema, sufficient fluency and automaticity to become aware
of other players and the general musical environment. The player needs to develop a
enhances the more communal aspects of the music. This particular schema
assemblage will have aural, visual and kinaesthetic/motor interactive elements (i. e.
tempi and time feels. The information that has to be abstracted is the nature and form
of another's improvisational style and the kinds of ideas they are likely to be play (in
order to be ready to respond to them. ) This can only happen at a deep level if
A further stage in the collective schema allows the individual musicians to swap,
imitate or develop ideas initially as contours or rhythmic shapes, and later with more
7 Both the Duke Ellington band and the Count Basic band of this, where band
are good examples
members developed almost telepathic forms of communication and musical empathy in a collaborative
way. For many of the early years, there was little written music for either band: the musicians
developed `on the spot' arrangements of the compositions. Indeed it took newcomers several months to
become integrated into the existing structure and to get a sen§e of the style of the music and the other
players, before they could add to the ensemble. Additionally, Ellington wrote specifically to the
strength and sound of his players, adding to the generally collaborative schema of what the band
individually and collectively was likely to do in the improvised sections.
95
clarity and detail. In advanced improvisers there will be a high level of sensitivity
across the whole group and many layered communication both on harmonic and
melodic levels and also in expressiveness and the use of dynamics. The collective
band schema then turns outwards to communicate with the audience in a negotiated,
many stranded process, whereby in the ideal state, the audience is also able to
The levels of abstraction for the collective schema are similar to those described for
the other schemata for improvisation: a vague sense of being within a musical
more aware and responsive to the harmonic background and rhythmic feel. This in
_
turn is abstracted into specific and accurate responses to melodic or rhythmic ideas
performed by other players, initially in an outline or contour form and later more
exactly. As the group becomes collectively more attuned, so does the level of
responsiveness, in that the individual can influence the group in a dynamic and
creative way. This level of collective responsiveness and cohesion allows the band to
more easily communicate with the audience because the music has become fluid on so
many levels that the consciousness of the musicians can focus outwards toward other
(responsive) people.
The audience schema for jazz improvisation is based on their individual and collective
previous experience of listening to live and recorded examples of the music, and some
96
knowledge likely hear during the concert. 8
expectation and of the music they are to
The complexity of this negotiated process is compoundedby the fact that often jazz
players are unpredictable and may well have developed or altered their style since the
audience previously heard them. Conversely, some jazz players (for example, the Pat
Metheny Group) aim to replicate the recorded sound at the live gigs so that the
audience will know exactly what to expect. This has a negative effect on some
undertaking. On ideal occasions (i. e. where the players perform predictably enough to
be communicative but not so much as to be boring), the audience schema and the
learning jazz improvisation, is it possible to describe any neural correlates for schema
theory? The final part of the chapter briefly explores this area.
Although many different areas and structures in the brain are involved in the control
of voluntary movement, the primary motor cortex is a key area (No. 4, of the pre
central gyrus) which was identified by Penfield (1951) in the control of localised
the brain in the conscious patient before surgery for epilepsy, Penfield discovered that
stimulations to the pre central gyrus triggered localised muscle contractions to the
contralateral side of the body and that there Evasa somatotopic representation of the
8 Clearly there has to be some kind of starting point to listening to jazz improvisation where there has
been no previous experience of hearing it- the listening schema also has to start somewhere.
97
corresponding parts of the body in Area 4 in the primary motor cortex. The pre motor
to the motor cortex for any given movement. The supplementary motor area appears
to influence the planning and initiation of the movement on the basis of past
supplementary area. The posterior parietal cortex also plays a role in the control of
it
voluntary movements: receives somatosensory, proprioceptive and visual inputs and
uses them to determine the position of the body and the target in space. It is thought to
produce some kind of internal model of the movement before it is actually initiated.
This fits well with Bernstein's theoretical model (previously described in chapter 1) in
which the actual movement as it unfolds is checked with the comparator or ideal
version.
In Penfield's model of the somatotopic layout of the various body parts, the hand (and
face) have particularly large areas due to the complex nature of the movements and
combinations of muscles that both have. In fine motor skill movement, for example,
in musicians playing instruments, people learn to make very fine movements of body
parts that are not normally used in that way. Under such circumstances the surface
area of the cortex can grow with practice, suggesting plasticity of neuronal function
and anatomy.
In addition to the areas already mentioned the basal ganglia (a group of structures
which comprise the caudate nucleus, putamen, globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus
and substantia nigra), play an indirect role in the motor system. They project to the
motor cortex, pre-motor cortex and supplementary areas simultaneously, forming the
98
performed. It is known that the basal ganglia also act as an inhibitor or filter of
inappropriate movement and the breakdown of the inhibitory elements cause the
to Edelman (2000), it is the basal ganglia that `store' the automated movements
associated with fine motor skill, which allow automaticity and fluency of movement.
In terms of the schema, these structures may be involved in initiating such movements
as pre-learned patterns, which are then adapted by other structures. Not all circuits in
the basal ganglia are motor orientated. Some are thought to be involved in
memorisation and in cognitive and emotional aspects but their role is not well
The cerebellum plays several roles in motor control: it stores learned sequences of
elsewhere in the brain and subsequently integrates all of these elements to provide
somatosensory and posterior parietal areas of the cortex project massive numbers of
axons to the nuclei of the pons within the brainstem. The neurons of the pons then
project into the cerebellum. The corticopontinecerebellar tract is a nerve bundle which
contains 20 million axons and enables the complex integrated working of the
cerebellum. The cerebellum is' divided into three parts which connect to a specific
structure and function in the brain. The vestibule cerebellum is connected to the
vestibule of the inner ear and is involved in balance. The spinocerebellum consists
mainly of the vermis, is connected to the spinal cord and controls postural activity and
muscle tone. The cerebellum controls muscle tension at all times whilst releasing
those muscles needed for movement. The cerebrocerebellum consists of the cerebellar
99
the cortex and contributes to the co-ordination of voluntary movement by ensuring
that when one set of muscles initiates a movement, the opposing set acts as a brake
i. e. in order for one set of muscles to be used another set must be inhibited. Timing
elements are also included in the cerebellar system enabling it to sequence the various
functions it controls. For the body to make any given gesture or movement the
sequence and duration of each of the basic movements of each body segment involved
must be controlled in a very precise manner. A loop circuit in the cerebellum connects
it directly to the motor cortex and modulates the signals that it sends to the motor
neurons. The cerebellum also plays a role in analysing the visual signals associated
with movement and appears to calculate the speed of these movements (either as
objects within the field of vision or as moving body parts) and adjusts the motor
the generalised nature of movements learned in writing one's name, has been
mentioned previously and he understood that whilst we use our hand and wrist to
write our name on paper, we can instantaneously adapt the movement to also include
the arm and shoulder when writing our name on a blackboard, for example. Synergies
of muscles are incorporated for a wide range of activities related to writing ones'
name for instance, in any given setting and therefore ought to be based on a schema
whereby the generalised elements are abstracted and adapted to new situations. As
100
order skills such as improvisation). Voluntary movement can be both initiated by
external stimuli and imagination and similar neural substrates are used in actually
making the movement and in imagining making it; hence the value and power of the
The activation sequence for motor areas is organised hierarchically and is undertaken
jointly with the motor cortex and other neural systems. The first level defines the
motor objectives and strategies of the movement and the behaviours that have to be
key), the key is seen, identified and located in space. Firstly, the prefrontal cortex
prepares the plans for this movement. Meanwhile the frontal cortex is receiving
information from a large number of axons projecting from the parietal cortex, which
is involved in spatial perception. Its analysis of the position of the body and its
various parts in space, are essential in preparing for the movement. The basal ganglia
The brain formulates a plan of action to touch the key using schemata developed from
infancy and based on hand to target movements. In this second phase, the pre-motor
cortex and the supplementary areas work with the cerebellum to specify the precise
sequence of contractions of the various muscles required in the movement. The plan
involves the motor systems organising the muscle synergies, and the hand trajectory
needed to reach the key, all of which has been practised extensively to allow for
maximum adaptive purpose. Thirdly, the primary motor cortex, the brainstem and the
spinal cord, work in an integrated way to produce or inhibit the appropriate muscle
contractions for the movement. The primary motor cortex determines the force of
101
each muscle group. It then sends the information to the spinal motor neurons and
inter-neurons that generate the movement and initiate any postural changes. As the
movement is executed, commands are conveyed from the cortical and brainstem
descending pathways to the motor neurons and these specify the temporal sequence of
muscle activation, and the angles of bones and joints. The muscular shape of the hand
alters in order to prepare to touch the key and feedback occurs at every level so that
the brain can judge the effect of the movement i. e. it is not pre-programmed but
by the posterior parietal cortex, the planning of the movement occurs within the
premotor areas of the frontal cortex and the execution of the movement happens
within the primary motor cortex. All three areas are therefore intrinsically involved in
the process of improvisation and integrally linked to auditory cortex and aural
imagination both of which will provide triggers and ongoing stimulus for movement.
The auditory, motor, visual, proprioceptive and kinaesthetic areas play a key role in
the most important areas and starting from the auditory imagination, sounds are
imagined, sung or vocalised in the primary auditory cortex, setting off a chain of
determine whether auditory cortical areas active in perceiving a real auditory stimulus
would also be active when subjects imagined it. The findings showed similar cerebral
blood flow patterns in both real and imagined responses along with areas of common
activation in the frontal lobes, parietal lobes and the supplementary motor area. The
similarities in blood flow imply that the two processes of hearing and imagining
music share a similar neural substrate. The Zatorre research also indicated that
temporal lobe activation included primary cortical areas in response to real music, and
extended into association areas, whereas imagined music occurred exclusively in the
102
association cortex. The implication is that primary auditory cortex responds to and
are involved with higher order processes which may include internal representations
of complex familiar stimuli. Similarly the supplementary motor area has been shown
to be involved when a motor task is only, imagined (Rao et al 1993; Wise et al 1991).
The activation of this area may therefore be crucial for actual and imagined
Having set in motion the musical imagination, the next stage is to prepare and execute
the movements necessary to produce the sounds on an instrument. Initially, the motor
(which may also be embodied in the musculature of the vocal chords). The motor
i.
cortical areas, e. the particular chord or key in which the piece is being played.
Visual clues play an important part which is related to both the layout of the
instrument, (where this is useful, as in the piano or guitar), and also in relation to
notated music which provides further conceptual clues about keys, chords and
melodic ideas, in order to initiate the solo. An improviser may well begin with a
visual pattern of notes that will fit a particular chord or a contour or interval within the
melody. We can assume that all of these elements i. e. auditory, visual, motor and
kinaesthetic are at least partially within the conscious awareness of the improviser as
they begin to play. The cortex controls the muscles of the hands and fingers through
the direct projection from the primary motor cortex, which is integrated with and
linked to supplementary motor and association areas. As the imagined sounds are
executed by the player, auditory and motor feedback will occur involving perception
103
by various layers of motor control. These include the cerebellum in adjusting motor
commands and the auditory cortex in comparing what is being played with what was
The motor process of executing the improvised musical line appears to involve pre-
learned elements which have become automatic and are played as a phrase and more
sometimes produce completely novel ideas. The automatic elements of motor and
some cognitive routines are proposed by Ghez and Krakauer (2000) and Edelman and
Tononi (2000) to be `stored' in the long cortical loops of the basal ganglia which are
anatomically insulated from what are considered to be more conscious areas of the
cortex. Edelman and Tononi (2000: 183-189) propose that automated motor routines
and elements of cognitive routines such as speech and language are stored in the basal
ganglia and not open to general conscious integration. When needed by the conscious
brain, there are specific portals through which this information travels and in the case
of music, the automatic programme of, for example, a specific scale or other learned
pattern which might fit a particular chord, is executed almost outside of the realms of
in this regard actually interferes with their smooth running. It appears to be better to
let the body execute the task unimpeded by conscious cognition. The process of the
during a conversation, I have a vague idea of the kind of thing I want to say but do not
know exactly how it will come out until it is said and I have listened and watched the
9 Subjectively for
me as the improviser this evaluative process takes place on three different levels: a
basic level of consonance with the background harmony ih conjunction with technical accuracy and
cleanness of execution; a higher level evaluating whether the music is interesting or unusual (i. e. not
the usual licks) and finally whether or not it has feeling and is communicating with the audience.
104
response of my companions to see if they also understand. If I am unclear or not
conveying the subtleties or meaning I want, I might try to rephrasethe whole thing
again in another way. As with musical improvisation, I would also be unable to.
repeat what I had said word for word even a sentence ago, although the gist and the
ideas are remembered often over years, and what other people say can also strike me
and become highly memorable. Edelman and Tononi's point is that if we had to
consciously think about and organise all the grammatical structures and vocabulary
involved in speaking or conversing, it would take so long that all meaning would be
conscious will and the desire for meaning. Similarly with musical improvisation, there
point and to develop meaningful musical ideas when the imagination is lacking
inspiration. Ghez (1991: 652) suggests that the basal ganglia may also compare
commands for movement from the pre-central motor fields with proprioceptive
feedback from the evolving movement, for regulating or monitoring its consequences.
The basal ganglia may also be involved in the initiation of internally generated
movements.
thought to play a crucial role in the fine tuning of movement by indirectly adjusting
the output of the major descending motor systems. The cerebellum is thought to act as
It
performance. receives information about plans for movement and information about
motor performance during the course of the movement, both of which allow it to
compare actual to intended motor response. When the movement is repeated, the
cerebellum generates corrective signals to gradually reduce the error and these
105
corrective signals involve feed-forward or anticipatory actions that operate on the
descending motor systems of the brainstem and cerebral cortex. The challenge of
triggering and guiding the whole process, rather than any relatively fixed external
previously heard sounds in such a way that the individuality, cohesion of the music
and its communicating power are all preserved and developed simultaneously.
research related to mirror neurons and social cognition that may profoundly affect
understanding of how humans learn. Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi and Rizzolatti (1996)
and Gallese et al (2004) discovered that subsets of neurons in monkey and human
brains respond when an individual performs certain actions and also when the subject
observes the same actions being performed by others. In other words, these neurons
not only provide templates for action but direct internal understanding of another's
actions, including their intentions and emotions. The researchers above believe that
mirror neurons underlie our ability to imitate others and provide a basis for non
verbal, non cognitive communication and learning. In aural traditions such as jazz, the
ability to imitate not just the music but also the stance, posture and mannerisms of
master players has been significant and has enabled the novice to absorb and embody
the complete sound and style of a player. The mirror neuron system may also help to
explain the ability of the brain to translate visual information into motor programs
because the movement or sequence of movements is not only observed but imitated
within the motor cortex. It may also play a large part in the appreciation of jazz music.
106
Having presented the main body of the theory at the start of the thesis, the next
chapter explores some alternative perspectives on the art of improvisation and how it
is learned. It begins with a historically based review of how four jazz musicians
learned to improvise during the 1920s and 30s and how their schemata developed
contemporary jazz teachers in the academic field think that improvisation is learned
and includes a brief review of significant jazz methods and texts since the beginning
107
3. Alternative Perspectives on the Learning of
Jazz Improvisation
The aim of this chapter is to understand how jazz was learned as an aural tradition
within a particular cultural and historical context, in order to assess and compare it
with the schema theory model. For this purpose, I have chosen to discuss the early
musical development of four American jazz musicians who were learning their
improvising skills in the early part of the twentieth century and who developed as
professional jazz musicians within what Berliner (1994) refers to as the `jazz
community' at that time. Lester Young (1909-1959), Joe Pass (1929-1994), Charlie
Parker (1920-1955) and Bill Evans (1929-1980), were chosen for these comparisons
doubt that all were innovatory masters of their day, with Parker for example, bringing
the technique of the alto saxophone to a new level. All four players' technical
mastery played a major role in their improvisational skill and the emotional
communication of their musical ideas. How did these four diverse players come to be
The four players appear to have been introduced to jazz and learned within the
using data he collected from the fifty jazz muýicihns he interviewed. Their childhood
108
a) being surrounded by both live and recorded music, in the foreground and the
family and on the streets) at an early stage, often from the time they began to
play an instrument;
on the right one; many players made their own instruments as children, before
f) practising very hard for some particularly formative part of their early life; '
i) being responsible for their own learning, as a result of the jazz community's
responsibility to learn and to find out what they needed to know, which
j) using the jazz standard as a basic framework to provide the structure for both
individual and collective improvising and gradually developing the theory and
k) copying idols by ear from records and playing along with them, in order to
gain fluency and to develop aural memory before striking out on their own and
109
developing an individual style. This way of learning is described by Berliner
1) during their formative years as young professionals, they were in thriving and
By looking at some key aspects in turn and comparing the biographical information
about each musician, it may be possible to tell to what extent these generalised
aspects from Berliner are applicable to the four featured musicians. Berliner's
propositions about the key elements of jazz learning, and eventually excellence, can
then be compared with a range of research findings on music learning, which together
enable us to draw more detailed conclusions about the viability of schema theory as
Many of the jazz musicians interviewed by Berliner (1994) recalled that they were
surrounded by music as children and jazz, being the popular music of the day, was
as musical enculturation, and the effect on the musicians as children must have been
immense. It would have enabled them not only to absorb repertoire and the all
important swing feel, but also to get a sense of the structure and form of the music at a
deep level. This enculturation process is largely absent from the academic approach to
teaching improvisation although there are attempts in curricula (for example Jerry
r
Coker, 1989) to emulate it. Lester Young was the only one of the four to have been
coming evening, and to drum up support, (Shapiro and Hentoff, 1955:22). Likewise,
his experiences of playing and touring with the family vaudeville band grounded him
in the popular music of the day and provided the background to enable him to develop
Charlie Parker, by contrast, was apparently introduced to music via his father's blues
record collection (Russell, 1973:35/36). The influences of the sound, feeling and
style of the blues singers of the 1920s such as Bessie Smith and Ma Rainey,
alongside Blind Lemon Jefferson, Louis Armstrong and Duke Ellington, were
profound on Parker, and were evident in his mature playing, forming a bluesy
foundation with a bright, strong and emotional timbre for his individual and
sophisticated improvisation. His exposure to the blues was, however, intermittent and
Parker was not surrounded by music again until he ventured into the gangster-run club
All of these clubs had live music. The music started early in the evening and
carried straight through the night till dawn, the official closing hour, though
seldom enforced. At daybreak, roving musicians, finished with their regular
jobs, would circulate through the district carrying their instruments and jam
until well into the morning. (Russell, 1973:30)
Here Parker absorbed the sounds of the best jazz musicians in the country at the time
and although initially unable to emulate them, he began to mime along to tenor
saxophonist Lester Young, who was some ten years older, and to absorb the
fundamental elements of sound and style needed to understand the jazz form and to
develop as an improviser. Joe Pass was also surrounded by music on the radio, on
record and at the movies. Indeed, he began to May the guitar in the early 1930s,
because of his admiration for silver screen star, Gene Autry, and began improvising
111
by filling in the gaps between the melody lines from tunes he heard on the radio, at
the suggestion of his father (Clinton, 1974). Pass's father also encouraged him to pick
Bill Evans' spent much of his early childhood listening to his elder brother playing the
piano, and, wanting to imitate him, he began playing for himself at the age of five in
1934 (Shadwick, 2002: 48). Subsequently, the music that surrounded him was mostly
influenced by classical music, which later formed a key element of his style. Like the
other players mentioned here, he listened to and imitated the popular music of the day.
on the radio and could play boogie-woogie piano by the age of twelve. He also
played in his brother's dance band in his early teens which exposed him to dance
music and some jazz. Like Pass, he began to improvise on his own, mainly out of
curiosity and to alleviate the boredom of the many repetitions of the same tune
(Shadwick, 2002:50).
Evans, unlike the others, absorbed both classical repertoire and technique (which he
continued to practise throughout his life) and this is amply reflected in his mature
improvising style which displays an almost orchestral sense of chordal and harmonic
thinking, perfect timing and a concentration and purity of style and melodic line. His
approach was also fundamentally different from other jazz pianists and he spent a
great deal of time in preparation and thought about his musical ideas as a direct result
112
written music may also have aided his architectural sense of structure of a piece which .
From the start, therefore, all four players were surrounded by music and enculturated
into the kind of jazz that they later became actively involved in, thereby providing the
background elements necessary for their aural schema development. Diverse live and
recorded examples of music which included jazz were simply available to them in
the four players had parents who supported and actively encouraged their musical
activity. Evans' parents paid for piano lessons for him from the age of five and
subsequently provided flute and violin lessons in order to broaden the scope of his
playing and general musicianship. Joe Pass's father wanted his children to have a
future outside of the steel mills that he had worked in and so took every opportunity to
help his son with his music, including requiring him to do many hours of practice,
perhaps too much for a child. Joe's father also brought him home all kinds of sheet
music and sat down to practise with him on his improvising and scales, although he
Lester Young's parents and siblings were all musicians and the family made a living
by touring the vaudeville circuit and playing for circuses, dancers, singers and clowns.
Young was taught by his father to play a number of different instruments before
113
settling finally on the tenor saxophone in his teens. Parker's mother paid for his first
In all four cases, there was active involvement on the part of encouraging parents to
provide instruments, lessons and performance opportunities for their children. They
wanted their children to develop musically and created an environment in which they
activities (either themselves or through teachers), whereby the young players could
Lester Young appears to have been the only musician with daily access to a live
improviser as a child in the form of his multi-instrumentalist father and his live music
experiences in New Orleans. Parker heard records and live improvisers in the clubs in
his teens and Pass too probably heard improvisers on record, whilst Evans had scant
access to improvisers before he joined a dance band at the age of 13. Berliner
considers that access to an improviser was vital to the future development of jazz
musicians:
the skill or role is probably the richest form of communication available to another
and so will be deeply influential in assisting the schema formation of the novice. This
114
is quite aside from the fact that the improvisation is viewed as a normal everyday
activity.
Sloboda and Davidson (1996: 184) cite evidence in their research that high achieving
young classical musicians do moderate amounts of informal practice and high levels
of formal practice (i. e. structured and technical work) and this seems also to have
been true for the four jazz musicians described here. The main difference appears to
be in the nature of the practice: jazz musicians tended to follow more informal aurally
based practices and were largely self-motivating. Green (2002) describes how pop
musicians also use such practices and reveals that they are largely self-taught and self-
10,000 hours of practice by the age of 20, again showing high levels of personal self-
From their own accounts and descriptions, the four jazz musicians probably did
similar amounts of practice with high levels of motivation. They did varying amounts ".
of practice as children, however. Evans worked hard even as a child and claims to
have done three hours a day intensive classical practice (Shadwick, 2002: 50). His jazz
practice began on the band stand with a dance band at the age of 13, and here he
George would call out the (chord) changes for me without ever suggesting that
I should have learned them for myself. Finally, instead of thinking of them as
isolated changes, I worked out a system upon which traditional theory is based
and I gradually began to understand How the music was put together.
(Hennessy, 1985).
115
It is interesting to note that Evans worked out his own theory for jazz improvisation
over the chords, presumably by integrating knowledge from his formal classical
training and his aurally based informal tuition on the bandstand. It is also easy to see
how idiosyncratically a schema for the harmonic background might form given the
Pass, like Evans, also did enormous amounts of highly structured, formal practice
I guess it came sort of easy; I have certain difficulties, not a lot. But you've
got to remember that I grew up playing the guitar. I started when I was nine,
and by the time I was nine and a half or ten, I was doing seven or eight hours
practice every day. I did two hours practice at six o'clock in the morning
before I went to school, and another two hours as soon as I got home in the
afternoon. Then I did four hours at night before I went to bed. I did that until I
was fourteen or fifteen. I didn't like it -I hated it, but my father was very firm
about it: he saw a little something happening, so he figured he'd just push. I
don't remember too much how I felt about it except that I'd rather be outside
playing ball and things. I never could ride a bike, like even today I can't do
these things. But, I know how I learned, and what I practised. (Clinton, 1974).
Apart from the huge number of hours, Pass's practice was characterised by a wide
range of different musical activities, all of which contributed to his developing into a
highly skilled jazz musician. He was unusual in that he had no teacher, but his father
supervised his practice as a non-musician, which was a mixture of both formal and
informal practices involving set tasks but also a degree of experimentation. His daily
practice schedule included specific technical exercises for learning the guitar
melody notes of well known standards; making up tunes; classical guitar studies for
performing chord solos and reading from sheet music for piano and other instruments.
He too was performing at the age of fourteen with a trio and happily, making more
money on good gig than his father did in a whole week at the steel factory.
116
By contrast to the disciplined and supervised practice of Pass, Parker (who remained
without either a regular teacher or mentor), was beset with difficulties both with the
technicalities of the instrument and the nature of the music he was trying to play,
making his early progress painfully slow and fitful. Parker's early experiences of
informal learning practices, particularly when the novice is unaware of what he has to
learn to acquire the skill. He became interested in playing in his early teens and
according to Giddins:
His first flirtation with music was as fleeting as that of most children who ask
for music lessons. At thirteen, he expressed enthusiasm for the sound of Rudy
Vallee's saxophone on the radio, and his ever-obliging mother bought him a
used and unplayable alto for $45. After she invested a larger sum to repair it,
his interest waned and the instrument was loaned to a friend. (Giddins,
1987:28)
Once he picked up the instrument again a couple of years later however, (and
certainly by the age of fifteen), Parker claimed to have practised eleven to fifteen
hours a day for a period of three or four years. Despite the suggested exaggeration of
such a statement, Parker would have needed to have done something similar in order
to make the stunning progress that he achieved by the time he was eighteen. The
difference between his first attempts and his mature, highly inventive and technically
innovative playing a few years later, were so great that many people thought he must
be innately and mysteriously talented and had developed his improvising just through
performing. Significantly, fellow alto saxophonist Paul Desmond also thought this as
Desmond: Another thing that's a major factor in your playing is this fantastic
technique that nobody's quite equalled. I've always wondered about that
too... whether there was... whether that came behind practising or whether that
was just from playing... whether that evolved gradually.
Parker: Well you make it so hard for me to answer you, you know.... because I
can't see where there's anything fantastic about it at all. I put quite a bit of
study into the horn, that's true. In fact, the neighbours threatened to ask my
117
mother to move once. We were living out West. She said I was driving them
crazy with the horn; I used to put in at least 11-15 hours a day.
Parker: That's true yes. I did that for over a period of three or four years.
and focused on it. For example, after a particularly disastrous experience at a jam
session in which he was laughed off the stand (knowing how to play only one and half
tunes in one key), he was told by a fellow musician that there were in fact twelve
keys. Armed with this new information, Parker set out to learn all twelve so that he
would be equally at home in any one of them and in doing so extended the accepted
saxophone technique of the day, that basically required only knowledge of the flat
and B major, for example, was very beneficial for both his aural memory and his
technique on the instrument and at the same time he became aware of the varying
sound qualities of different keys and the effect that they had on the communicating
Apart from practising as he performed in the family band, Young transcribed solos
from records in order to learn his jazz and this practice had a deep effect not only on
his aural and technical abilities, but also on his sound because he idolised Frankie
118
When I had just started to play, I would buy all his records. I imagine I can
still play all those solos off the record. He played C melody saxophone. I tried
to get the sound of aC melody on tenor. That's why I don't sound like other
people. (Young, Downbeat1956)
He particularly liked the way Trumbauer `told a little story' (ibid) and how having
played the melody, he would then extemporise around it. It was Young's desire to
sound like the C melody sax of his hero, that transformed his tenor sound and made
him so distinctive from the heavier timbres of for example, Coleman Hawkins.
Young, like many of the musicians interviewed by Berliner (1994) believed strongly
that in order to play jazz one needed to imitate a player who was inspiring and to
absorb his or her style before it was possible to develop an individual voice. Berliner
calls this the `imitate, assimilate, innovate' model and it is based on the notion that
During the 1950s after Parker had been recorded, Pass also transcribed Parker solos
later in his teens and the high level of his technique and aural memory is apparent
because those particular solos were amongst the hardest, both musically and
technically. This helped to give Pass his be-bop orientation when he arrived on the
New York scene in the early 1950s. Parker in his turn transcribed Young's solos of
1939 with the small Basie band (Giddins, 1987:35) until the tracks were worn away
from endless listenings and also had a similar live idol called Buster Smith who was a
key influence.
It is clear from these accounts that the four players practised intensely for a period of
time during their youth; that they used a mixture of formal and informal methods to
119
learn, but that for the jazz, their practice was autonomous and self-motivated; that
they had mentors or role models to assist in their technical practice and in the
conceptual aspects of understanding how to improvise and that they also used
provide a focus for their learning, they all performed at an early age either in informal
jam sessions or other low key gigs where they could experiment and develop ideas in
a fairly supportive environment. With the exception of Parker (who seems to have
lived a speeded up life in all respects), the three other players had been practising for
ten to fifteen years before they hit the jazz scene as proficient young improvisers.
musical skills and knowledge outside formal educational methods, which can be both
aural approaches and rely mainly on self-teaching and peer learning. The cultural
environments in which the young musicians found themselves enabled them to learn
in a variety of informal and self-motivated ways. Evans was the only player to have
whilst most players used the materials to hand around them: the radio, the record
player, the movies and whatever access they could get to live music, (though it was
hard for youngsters to get into the clubs). Jam sessions provided Parker with ample
scope for trying out his newly learned techniques and ideas and for comparing himself
occasional mentors and found out all of his information that way, guided entirely by
120
his own musical leanings. His great breakthrough though, in terms of individual style
was described in his own words as happening whilst jamming with a friend:
This revelation led him to explore a completely new approach and to form a new style
of jazz (be-bop) which no-one at the time could begin to emulate from his live
performances but only assimilated later once he began recording from the mid 1940s
onwards. The style, using upper extensions of the chords, a new kind of technically
demanding fast phrasing and the elimination of swing in up tempo tunes, was entirely
For all of these players, the concept of being taught how to play jazz did not exist. At
the time, aspiring improvisers had to learn for and by themselves and because of this,
beginners took very different routes and therefore ended up sounding highly
individual as mature players. Being self taught in the jazz field, they had no-one to
constrain them and developed in ways that felt intuitively right and appropriate to
them. There is a question of how important informal music learning practices are in
developing the skill of jazz improvisation and whether these can indeed be emulated
121
being incorporated into the academy, jazz music would have died when its cultural
In summary, the most important elementsin learning to play jazz from this historical
context appear to be similar to many other musical skill learning processes: everyday
was clearly a key element, as was parental support and encouragement. Aurally based
learning using imitation played another crucial role alongside the opportunity to
perform what was being learned early on in an informal and relaxed way. With a
few exceptions (Joe Pass for example), learning was primarily self-directed, fairly
slowly paced and where musicians were taught classically, (as in Bill Evans' case),
developing from curiosity and a desire to extend aural and technical skills.
A number of aspects of the traditional ways in which jazz musicians learned seem to
described grew up surrounded and enculturated by the music that they would later be
actively involved in and so would have absorbed the form, sound and feel almost
emerging schema. The implication both for the theory and for present day learning
(outside of the historical and cultural beginnings) is that a fundamental feel and
122
understanding of the form of the music has to take place through listening, if the
A second issue arising from the traditional ways of learning is the emphasis on aural
approaches, imitation and performance in developing the skill. The implication is that
unmediated and direct method of learning that encompasses and incorporates the
schema of another. In this view, having assimilated the style of another more
masterful player (gradually absorbing the more subtle rhythmic and melodic nuances
and for example finally being able to play along with the master improviser), the
technical and imitative process becomes the foundation for developing the individual
schema. Essentially this process elaborates on previously learned ideas and imagining
a range of expressive parameters that presumably inspired him to play jazz in the first
place.
A third significant factor in traditional learning is the role of performance early on,
and all of the four players were performing as children or in their early teens during
10 The
absorption of jazz music as a natural part of everyday activities and interactions is a further
distinction between the past and the present: the learning of jazz improvisation is currently a more
cerebral or intellectual activity, rather than part of an all encompassing `jazz life'. No distinction was
made by the aforementioned players between their music and their life, and this I think has changed.
123
communication between musician and audience. 11 It seems reasonable to assume
then, that in the development of the schema, performance is a vital ingredient and one
enrich it.
A fourth factor is the self-directed nature of the learning, which Green (2002) includes
with aural and peer learning, as part of the informal practices that pop musicians use
today to develop their skills. It is true that whilst the four players took responsibility
for their own learning in jazz, they were all in a musical and cultural environment
where many aspects of the skill could be picked up in their everyday life. Whether
this is the same or different now is hard to tell. It could be argued that for example,
children now have access to a much wider range of music through their i-pods and the
internet, although experience of live music varies greatly between individuals. The
fact remains that unmediated and self-motivated learning (whilst appearing chaotic),
with specific support at the right time, does seem to provide a most effective form of
learning. The reasons are not difficult to understand; self-motivated and self-directed
learning will proceed from a starting point and at a pace that is most appropriate to the
learner, and will be based entirely on natural interest, enjoyment, and curiosity which
is unconstrained. 12The process falters when the learner encounters technical or other
problems that they do not know how to deal with or when they proceed along a path
124
of learning that turns out to have been a blind alley. Another perceived negative
aspect is the length of time this kind of experiential learning can take. Jazz musicians
who learned in this way and then went on to a lengthy apprenticeship within the jazz
community, describe the whole learning process as `paying your dues'; implying that
the music requires that kind of long haul commitment in order for the musician to be
does appear that it takes a long time (normally ten to fifteen years) before the player is
implication for the schema theory of jazz improvisation is that the learner should be
encouraged to take responsibility for their learning from the start and that the teacher
should be supportive and provide information when required. In other words the
learning should proceed under the direction of the learner and the teacher's
Finally it is clear that the four players engaged in large amounts of formal and
informal practice at various times, suggesting that both are definitely needed to
acquire the skill. As all of the musicians (except Parker) were learning to play their
instruments anyway, the jazz aspects were an extension of their normal practice and
develops the schema. In addition the four players continued to practise and to
develop, elaborating their skill, and in Evans' case creating a more distilled and
development means that the skill is never really learned fully and that the schema
must be enriching, refining and developing itself in a complex skill like jazz
125
Importantly then, although all of the musicians described were readers of music, their
jazz improvisational skills were developed primarily by aural means using imitation
first to provide the framework to explore their own ideas. This is in sharp contrast to
the way that jazz improvisation is taught today in formal educational settings where
generally, a notation based method predominates over more aural aspects of skill
taught and learned today, and how does this compare to traditional methods and also
to schema theory?
126
4. How teachers have proposed learning jazz
improvisation
4.1 Introduction
The major proponents of academic jazz education, since it began in the early 1960s,
with patterns and procedures using the be-bop style as a model. Their main focus is on
the harmonic framework underlying the jazz standard (and in outlining chord-scale
patterns which will fit it), which is used as a basis for the emergence of more novel or
creative ideas. A major criticism of the formal educational approach is that it has
fixed the `language of jazz' at the be-bop period of the fifties through the use of
notated patterns which are presented visually and which encompass this complex
harmonic based style. This approach appears to come directly from a European
cultural tradition in which notated music is given priority and in which improvised
jazz music is seen as a skill to be learned, rather than a way of life to be adopted. The
older, more organic and ad hoc `black' approach is centred around aural traditions,
infused with the notion of jazz playing as a way of life. Berliner argues that living the
The Euro-centric version of learning jazz has had a profound effect on both the
development of the music itself and how it is perceived outside of the original cultural
environment in which it flourished. The result of this narrow focus has been the
neglect of many of the styles prominent before and after the 1935-55 era: the
127
emphasis on solo over communal improvisation; the attainment of technique for its
own sake above expressive musical content; the placing of certain types of jazz and
star soloists as `art' music in concert halls and the perception that many young players
sound the same because their improvising skill has been based on similar foundations.
The traditional assimilation of the previous generations' ideas via imitation and
transcription, has also been neglected in the notated model. Likewise the presentation
of patterns and scales reveals little about how a cohesive solo is developed, indeed, a
review of contemporary jazz method books noted that beyond introductory lessons on
traditional devices, theme and variation technique, repetition and chord-scale patterns:
`little insight is provided on how to turn the theoretical materials "into melodic gold". '
Below is a table that includes some of the most influential jazz texts of the past forty
128
Berliner Jazz: The Infinite Art Ethnomusicological Interesting and
of Improvisation study based on insightful account
(1994) musician's own using language and
observations of their vocabulary analogies
learning processes. to illuminate the
Loosely based on the improvisation
`imitate, assimilate process. Musicians
innovate' idea, centred own experiences can
in oral traditions, be contradictory and
communal learning, unrevealing about
mentoring and so on. learning process.
Much of it not open
to consciousness and
glossed and
recreated in memory
to seem like a logical
and rational step-like
movements to
competency and then
artistry.
Jerry Coker Improvising Jazz One of the very first More balanced than
(1964) instruction manuals for Baker but still
jazz improvisation. notation rather than
Makes the case that the aurally based. More
intellect is the only emphasis on creating
factor over which there individual motifs
is conscious control rather than copying
therefore the intellectual others or reciting
approach to jazz is the patterns. Some basic
only useful one. Bases analysis of melody
the method of learning and melodic contour
and standard and as related to
applying correct. scales. improvisation as
Emphasises the need to well as functional
create and learn your harmony and chord
own motifs. Continues tones. Swing not
with ensemble work and addressed till late in
communal approaches. the method. More
holistic and natural
method.
Jerry Coker Patterns for Jazz Pattern book for all Useful for improving
(1970) chords in 12 keys technique but
beginning with triads emphasises written
and developing over the over aural learning.
extensions. .
129
Jerry Coker Teaching Jazz (1989) Curriculum approach Improvisation is
involving all aspects: again seen as
history, theory, needinga vast
improvisation, analysis, background of
composition and stylistic &
arranging. Long periods theoretical
spent on history to knowledge before it
inculcate the style and can be successfully
sound. No improvisation learned. Rather than
until the second year- as an extension of
learning various natural human
background and musicality.
harmonic structures Rhythmic and
before any improvisation collaborative
is attempted. Focuses on approaches
melodic improvisation neglected.
to the detriment of
developing good
rhythmic and rhythm
section players.
131
Sam Most Metamorphosis: Eleven standard A good way of
Transformation of a progressions are introducing
Jazz Solo (1980) presented and Most has beginners to a range
written examples of of possibilities over
solos over them, in a standard
systematic way; progressions and
beginning with minim encouraging them to
guide tones, then explore the different
crotchet chord tones and sounds, intervals and
passing notes. This is rhythmic approaches
followed by exercises for themselves. It
using thirds; varying the presupposes quite a
rhythmic context; lot of theoretical
chromatic tones; fourths; knowledge and
pentatonics and finally a background but
bitonal approach. Most provides a
suggests playing though constrained was of
the exercises with the learning about
chord accompaniment improvising that is
and then making up exploratory rather
similar examples. than imitative of any
particular style or
Jimmy Jazz Phrasing and Aimed at teaching jazz One of the few texts
Giuffre Interpretation (1969) phrasing and feel to work specifically
through notation, this and centrally on jazz
method emphasises feel and phrasing
good rhythmic skills and through notation but
internalising rhythm and focusing on
articulation. The text is expressive qualities
comprised of two stave in the music. Pre-
melodies; one represents requisites are good
the `straight' version of reading skills and
the music the other the improvisation is not
jazzy version. His aim is mentioned although
to make the music come beginners would get
alive and in later stages an elementary
students are encouraged understanding of
to provide their own jazzy lines through
interpretations. the melodic
Advanced work also examples provided.
covers melodic contour
and the moving forward
of phrases so that the
music has both
momentum and inner
form.
John Jazz Rhythm-The One of a number of texts This is another
Mehegan Improvised Line produced by Mehegan comprehensive
(1962) whilst he was jazz tutor approach that
at the Julliard School. defines and
After discussing the describes the
132
components of swing, he stylistic changes
notates 10 versions of a through the many
12 bar blues in parallel eras of jazz so that
so that the student can the learner gets a
compare the content and feeling of how the
phrasing of for example, music becomes more
Bessie Smith and sophisticated. In this
Hampton Hawes. He regard he is unique
provides chordal as most methods
progressions for the start at be-bop. The
blues in a similar way use of already
with examples from written
Louis Armstrong in transcriptions is
1924 to Bernie's Tune in limited to analytical
1944. The book ends value, because the
with a series of aural element
transcriptions taken from involved in
similar period and transcribing has been
featuring a range of done by others.
players. However, by having
all the transcriptions
in parallel the
learner is easily able
to compare and
contrast different
soloists and musical
eras and should be
able to make up their
own examples,
without requiring
huge amounts of
theoretical
understanding or
John Swing and Early Follows a similar format Here Mehegan maps
Mehegan Progressive Piano to the previous example the territory and
Styles(1964) focusing specifically on provides good
piano players beginning examples that may
at James P. Johnson pre be attempted by the
1920 and culminating in beginner without too
pianists from the early much background
1960's. The approach knowledge or
here is chordal, based on technique. The
real styles of the players opportunity to start
and also works on simply and then
walking basslines. develop
harmonically is
sound and enables
learners to begin to
practise
improvisation from
the beginning
133
incorporating
theoretical and
technical aspects as
they become
relevant.
Transcription Example: Charlie Examples abound of These have proved
Books Parker Omnibook, books of transcriptions very popular over
transcribed by from those of Louis the years although as
J.Aebersold and K. Armstrong in 1924 to mentioned
Stone (1978) present. The idea is to previously, a major
gain insight into the component of
solos of others and to transcription is the
improve technique by aural aspect. Unless
learning to play them. It the transcription
also enables learners to book is accompanied
analyse solos, and to see by the music, both
how ideas are developed the feel and phrasing
over longer periods. is difficult to convey
using purely
notational methods.
Also many players
are unable to make
the leap from their
current technical
standard to that of
say, Charlie Parker
and need to be
shown how best to
use the
transcriptions.
.
Fake Books The New Real Books, These books now Prior to the advent of
Voll. (1988), Vo12. proliferate and contain a fake books, most
(1991), Vol 3. (1995) vast repertoire of jazz tunes were
standard American show learned by ear either
tunes; jazzy pop and on the bandstand, or
funk repertoire; jazz from recordings.
tunes sometimes related Sheet music was
to one particular also a source of
composer like Ellington I repertoire but
or Mingus; evergreens usually contained the
that can be played as voiced chords in the
standards and so on. piano and was more
sophisticated than
using just chord
symbols. The use of
fake books tends to
reinforce the notated
aspects and they are
now used in
performance to the
detriment of band
interaction.
134
George Lydian Chromatic A way of playing in a Places emphasis on
Russell Concept of Tonal controlled way outside the Lydian scale
Improvisation (1959) of the tonality of a over the standard
harmonic progression major scale and the
using a variety of concept of tonal
ambiguous and many gravity, thereby
coloured lydian scales. increasing both the
tonal and atonal
possibilities for
improvisation using
notation. An
interesting way to
broaden the ear.
Thom David I Ear Training for A listening based One of the few
Mason Improvisers: A Total approach using patterns aural- based method
Approach (1981) to learn to hear chords books using solfege
and scales and uniquely in an unusual way to
using solfege as an ear- define and learn
training tool for intervallic
improvisation. Begins relationships within
with the aural learning chords and
of intervals; develops progressions.
singing and playing Emphasis on singing
exercises based on first and then
solfege and the transferring the sung
intervallic relationships motif or pattern to
between notes, then the instrument.
applies this material to
various chord
progressions.
Jamey Play along records and Recorded backing tracks These tracks have
Aebersold CD's from 1980- with various rhythm provided a useful
present sections for improvisers way for novice
to use. improvisers and
those not working
regularly, to practise
their improvisation.
Used in isolation
however, the
approach emphasises
a `star' soloist over a
more communal
stance and has
tended to fossilise
certain standards
over others.
135
Two of the most influential pioneers of formal jazz education and among the first
authors to publish manuals on the subject in the early 1960s, are David Baker and
Jerry Coker. I focus on these particular teachers, because they characterise the
mainstream of jazz education in the USA (which has historically made a big impact
on the European approach), and have continued to organise and teach jazz curricula in
higher education institutions to the present day. They also embody the more rational
and consensual thinking about the way to learn jazz improvisation within formal
educational establishments.
Beginning in the 1960s, David Baker produced a series of improvisation methods for
but all based on the scale pattern approach that he pioneered. The books provide
notated variations of scales, altered scales, arpeggios, patterns for specific harmonic
features like II, V, I progressions, turnarounds, exercises over specific intervals such
as fourths and combinations of all of the above, with the expectation that they will be
transposed into all keys by ear. Baker was one of the first jazz musician and
educators to publish a formal method on learning to improvise in the jazz idiom and
Baker's overriding idea (exemplified in the method), is that beginners in particular but
knowledge, facility and fluency with scales, scale patterns and chord arpeggios and
also a fairly rigorous conceptual understanding of functional harmony, before they are
able to begin to create an improvised musical solo. His notion is that in learning the
136
scales modally in all keys, broken into patterns and then mixed with chord tones, the
musician builds technical facility and confidence on the instrument: the ability to
imagine scale and arpeggio patterns and how they might fit together and gradually the
framework.
provide practice in finding and learning all of the possible combinations of intervals
that the player may need in order to develop their schema for improvisation. Given
that all the exercises are intended to be played with metronomic accuracy and at
gradually increasing tempos, they should also improve fluency, technique and speed
of aural imagination and when practised thoroughly, this is the case. What is missing
however is any systematic way of turning the exercises and intervals into melodies or
cohesive ideas that might lead to the development of the individual voice of the
player. The result, where the method has been comprehensively adopted in formal
educational settings, is that players sound (at least initially), generic, as their
background work has been very similar. As both timbral and communal aspects of the
music are de-emphasised, there has been a tendency for players sound to lack timbral
in favour of a `star soloist' plus accompaniment model, which has been assisted by
traditionally, schema development was highly idiosyncratic and based on the music
surrounding the player and in particular the sound and style of an accessible idol.
137
If we compare this to the informal learning practices defined by Green (2002) and to
the early experiencesdescribedby the four jazz musicians in the last chapter,we see_a
diametrically different approach at work. Whereas, the informal practices are based
on enculturation and aural approaches and the learner being autonomous, Baker sets
out a notated and systematic framework. Whereas the social and cultural contexts in
which the players learned their musical skills enabled them to pick up elements
naturally and at the appropriate time, Baker's notated material is presented all at once
and purely from his perspective. Also in areas like functional harmony, Evans for
example, learned how it worked aurally and from clues given by a mentor, so that he
it
absorbed in an idiosyncratic and organic way. The notated approach presents the
material in a theoretical and one dimensional manner which may not be appropriate to
every learner. In addition, the sheer volume of information, harmonic concepts and
patterns to learn before improvisation can be attempted, goes against the general
learning process in which small amounts of information are absorbed and adapted to
the skill
based on and leads to a specific jazz style of be-bop which, as mentioned previously
was developed by Charlie Parker in the late 1940s. The individual improvising voice
of the player develops from assimilating the music of their era and then producing a
used by Mehegan (1964) and described briefly in the table, in which he compares and
contrasts, on a bar by bar basis, the varying approaches to the blues beginning with
improvisations by Bessie Smith in the 1920s and ending with Hampton Hawes in
1954. To begin with the elaborations and developments of the melody from
138
recordings in the 1920s for example, by the great exponents of the period: Louis
Armstrong, Bix Beiderbecke, the Duke Ellington band and then to follow the
increasing sophistication of improvised lines through the big band swing era, then be-
bop, birth of the cool and beyond to fusion, seems a more thorough and
developmental approach.
scale methods, I propose that there is already a wealth of melodic musical knowledge
present and available within the aural imagination of the player to draw on in
developing the improvisational process, concurrently with learning some of the very
necessary technical and stylistic aspects. This melodic sense, based on songs and
tunes that the player has learned throughout their life, should provide enough
background sources for the beginner to start improvising without having first to
Green (2002: 28) explaining how pop musicians learn, and the four jazz musicians,
describe learning repertoire aurally i. e. songs and melodic fragments (riffs, bass lines
etc.) as the basis for developing their musical and improvising skill. Green mentions
(2002: 24) that there appear to be three types of listening involved in aural learning:
purposive (with the aim of learning something specific); attentive (which has the same
level of detail without any particular objective in mind, and distracted, where the
music is listened to on and off. Pop musicians report using all three types of listening
while learning songs and are able to play the music naturally just by long periods of
listening. They do not consciously attempt to learn songs, but rather pick them up
through constant listening, and the same would probably be true of the jazz musicians.
The long periods spent listening, absorbing and imitating the music aurally is the main
139
difference to the more academic notation based approach, where the aural elements
What seems to occur using the Baker approach is that by neglecting the more natural
melodic entry point, musicians tend to get locked into translating a chord symbol or
key centre into a scalic pattern or arpeggio which can be executed to fit the theoretical
notes of the chord, rather like improvisation by numbers. Aside from being an
extremely laborious task, beginners seem often confused between the theoretical,
visual and aural complexities of this method and quite unable to play in a natural way.
response to the melody (including the feeling it evokes and the contour it provides),
adding conceptual and theoretical information as and when it is needed by the player,
to develop and extend ideas. It comes back to what does one need to know in order to
be able to improvise in jazz and Baker believes you need the framework of scales and
Like Baker, Jerry Coker was one of the first players and teachers (again, in the early
1960s) to think about and produce a method for the teaching and learning of jazz in
academicor more formal educationsettings. He also basedhis ideas on the jazz music
of the 1950s. Aware of the changing social and cultural environment of jazz
musicians, his first book, Improvising Jazz (1964) was intended, according to Gunther
Schuller in the foreword, `to fill the gap left by the demise of the jam session and the
road-travelling band. ' (1964: viii) and Coker's own stated aim in his introduction
follows:
140
This book is designed to equip you with the understanding of the theoretical
principles used in jazz, presented in a logical sequence as they apply to the
ultimate improvised performance. (1964: 2)
The stated aim highlights the fundamental difference in approach from traditional
aural methods which were anything but logical or theoretical, being primarily based
on sound and movement. Coker (unlike Baker) begins his first method with the
defining the basic chord symbols and outlining the relevant scales. In this way he
progressions for jazz improvisation, although still emphasising the theoretical over
blues line which is analysed as a form of theme and variations. Having also described
linear melodies consisting mainly of eighth notes, Coker suggests the beginner read
We cannot emphasise this practice too much; it will benefit you in two
important areas: (1) It will develop your ear and pitch memory to the extent
that you will eventually be able to transcribe your own ideas while you are
improvising; and (2) by studying the solos and styles of already proficient
improvisers, you will gain a deeper understanding of the improvised solo and
will discover various methods and ideas for the handling of improvised
material. Your sense for evaluating the merits of various soloists will also
increase as you discover which solos can bear the scrutiny of analysis.
(1964: 14)
Coker is suggesting the use of transcription but without the essential aural to motor
aspects crucial to the learning process. He does however believe that it is important
for the beginner to get a feel and idea for the underlying structure and deeper elements
of the solo rather than just the fragments and how they might be put together. He also
proposes that beginners create a `licks' book of original motifs which will eventually
become part of the player's repertoire, and suggests that they compose two a day. As
141
banal and simplistic as this sounds, trumpeter Randy Brecker13 and his brother
saxophonist Michael Brecker have been transcribing their own solos since they started
playing. Brecker tapes himself improvising over various different sets of changes and
then transcribes his own solos, writing any particularly good riffs, motifs or lines in
his book. He explained that these help him to hone his own sound and actually to
secondary benefit is the improvement in sight reading (which enabled both Breckers
to become top studio players). Coker believed that the conscious development and
performer and audience. The performer should create sufficient interest by balancing
the need for a certain amount of predictability and repetition with contrasting
In next explaining how the rhythm section works in jazz, Coker prepares the
improviser for the group sessions that form a large part of the book. In emphasising
with Collier (1995) and much closer to the organic experiential learning approach
described earlier in chapter 3. Coker sets out a disciplined method for learning to
improvise within the ensemble by suggesting that the performers analyse and study
the piece to be learned before they rehearse so that they are not struggling with the
basics and can focus on more creative elements. During the rehearsal itself, the
rhythm section plays some choruses on their own, so that the sound and feel of the
chords is absorbed in time, then the solo players play roots using long notes. The main
13Describing his
practice at a seminar at Leeds College of Music in 2001.
14It is interesting to note that most improvisers
cannot remember exactly what they have just played:
Brecker can only remember it by recording and transcribing his own solos. This is indicative of the
dynamic and emergent skill that jazz improvising is: like conversation or trains of thought, wholly new
ideas are created which are not memorised.
142
idea is to keep the progression fairly slow and simple at the start and players are
concentration and aural focus. Players are then assigned different chord tones for the
long notes and gradually layers of complexity are added with motifs, arpeggios, scales
and so on. Coker suggests that beginners play all together at first to build confidence,
focusing on chord tones so that a consonant sound is made. Once the basic
progression is learned and understood, guide tones are used to create a framework and
cohesive melodic line as a basis for improvisation. The aim is for improvisers to
decide on the most important chord tones by ear and to use them to outline the
harmony, then to write down a solo incorporating these so that the visual modality can
For the development of aural ability and technique, Coker proposes learning relative
pitch on the instrument to enable heard pitches and intervals to be played as spatial
patterns and the process is aided by the fact that each pitch has a slightly different
tone colour from the others. He develops this idea through dictation exercises directly
from recording or sound to instrument and then memorisation of the different timbral
qualities of certain pitches. More theoretical concepts are added at this point to
In relation to schema theory, Coker's early method appears to reinforce some of the
best elements of the experiential and traditional way of learning. He also uses the
framework of the standard as a referent for the more theoretical ideas thereby
communal way of practising that benefits all aspects of skill development and
143
encourages collaborative strategies for improvisation and learning, enabling the band
does not try at this early period to include every possible combination of intervals that
the beginner might need, but focuses on the specifics of a particular tune or
The ideas presented in the early works are in sharp contrast to later approaches in
(1989). Ideas for the curriculum comprise the usual areas of jazz history, theory,
How Coker organises his course is however, unusual because for a whole year he
proposes that the jazz students work on jazz history, jazz piano and jazz theory
improvise, (and against his previous advice to form proper bands), Coker suggests
that the class in conducted with `play-alongs', i. e. recorded backing tracks featuring
competent but unresponsive rhythm sections. His reason for this is that there is
insufficient time to teach the rhythm section players how to accompany properly and
that beginners need strong rhythmic support. The implication of his previous advice
was that all players benefit from understanding how the rhythm section works and
how rhythm section players approach their tasks. Indeed, there is much to be said for
beginners learning drum rhythms, bass lines and piano voicings to help them in their
improvisation. Yet in this book, Coker is adamant that the class should focus on solo
improvisation rather than the group concept, and in the process loses a lot of valuable
jazz education. This approach also consigns the rhythm section to the role of
accompanists providing a background for the `more important' soloists: a practice that
144
has led to many a boring jazz group. The workshop approach proposed by Collier
(1995) uses group improvisation as a starting point and the rhythm section (if there is
one) learns their skills in conjunction with everyone else. Beginner improvisers in a
group need to become aware of all the other players: how they might participate in the
group's sound and the overall structure of the music. Rhythm section players have to
learn how to improvise; soloists have to learn how to accompany and how to respond
to accompanists and so on. This process should begin early, when everyone is of a
similar standard so that the group can cohere and develop as a unit.
In Coker's later curriculum, the improvisation classes began by applying digital and
`essential patterns and licks' to exercise tracks of play alongs. The first song is a John
Coltrane tune called Giant Steps which is highly complex chordally with fast moving
when he was already a competent player, and seems overly difficult as a tune foT
extemporisation: the elements that Coker chooses are purely chordally based. This
procedure is continued for several weeks until the patterns are learned and then
extended to other be-bop tunes, II, V, I progressions and the like. The process then
includes modal tunes and the aural familiarisation of all scale tones, pentatonics and
fourth intervals, followed by the blues. Thus it is clear that by the 1980s Coker has
altered his initial thinking about how to learnjazz improvisation and replaced it with a
chord and scale method similar to Baker's. The assumption again is that a long period
improvisation.
145
What are the other theories of improvisation and how do they relate to how players'
written probably the most commonly cited review of the theory of improvisation and
proposed his own cognitive model linked to the computational theory of mind
prevalent at the time of writing. I argue that his theory is too defined by separate
question on the basis that the computer programme he devised simply failed to
produce music remotely like jazz. The reason being that motivational, generalising
suggests that it is the hand which learns, scaffolded by various cognitive processes
and not vice versa, as is often assumed. This notion directly supports the schema
theory of jazz improvisation because it links aural memory with physical space on the
instrument (in the form of intervals) and to practising the movements required to find
Pressing (1988) was one of a few researchers to have engaged fully with the
146
physiological, as well as psychological terms. He emphasised the need to distinguish
between the different approaches and processes of learners and skilled improvisers in
any proposed theory and his stated intention in the 1988 paper was to answer the
learned and taught? How can novelty (within improvisation) be explained? Included
in the introductory literature review which defines the scope of the theory, he
describes briefly the many possible substrates of the improvisation process including
an outline of the control of movement by the central nervous system; motor and
skilled performance generally and motor schema theory by Schmidt (1975). Those
and feedback from an ongoing improvised musical line. Discussing time scales for the
control of movement, Pressing (1988: 137) makes the point that neurological
`processing' speed seems to be greatest for audition and touch/kinaesthesia, out of all
the sensory systems. Both relate directly to improvisation and provide a psychological
interpretation for why improvisation has been developed in music to a far higher
Before proposing his own theory, Pressing discusses the role of intuition and
creativity in the light of much anecdotal evidence from musicians about how truly
creative improvised music comes to them seemingly from outside sources, and
appears only marginally within their control. He also discusses how computer
147
ow
:j ti ý1 ocn CD a
cC G v' Fr o ýý
aw "i 0oZ ýc C (D 07
-
m
ýN rn ec
O- wc 'ný ä ys i ýp
w a / e
fD O Nc iý
N3 J
zo
ý':JO n. ý. r+ O
w-. yn CJ ~oC. ö
ö O
(D "0 w"-
(D
w-. ý. aW
Z. ýD s ýr
'LJ n=
°ö
C md CJ
C
Q'
CD 9o -, a
ýö
O
G
N T --
`""
cl \ý
:ý0f,
0 c, Il i
i 4
=ww CD 3 lol
cr, _`
I 'i //
,ZO ,o
r r. n' _J 3/
/
O wO
Gý `t OÖ
w fi
.b0r
CD w -I ,"-Qß. I IF
f7 `;: °On=, CD ý\ O/
oa.. n" o- E
n3 °o _-
/0
rf- 7ýe
M0 oy--.
/N
r- 0
ý"'O GC
CD n
"'°. CD +M Vi CD CD
E3 a" \n
\O
";
oE- oh C
c CD 4
CID 0+
CL r, 5
'0
ý' (Dn lD Vt ä" O
ä" 0^GoG
\Cý
Ný
0.
cýc 'd
ß Ö. W d
y
OGO
ýr K
N0
r> CD- / ti
ýooa8ao[: 7
L2_L\
. i
or
ý`
"^mý'ý'ý ^ý+can f/
N
Jr y-
D 111
r-C
Pmý
C9 n0-
..
_0
0
' 0
CL
v0
in' CD
G0Ga CL
148
Pressing's cognitive model of improvisation is examined in detail here because it is
model is useful both in terms of defining what improvisation might be and in what
may be happening in the mind and body as the performer improvises. It does not
begin to explain how improvisation is learned or how coherent ideas and an individual
voice develops. Pressing begins by stating that any improvisation may be partitioned
into a sequence of non-overlapping sections. It may be, but is it actually put together
like that or in more of a gestalt way? The way that certain improvised solos develop
over time, implies that deep structural frameworks are at work, even if we do not
understand how they are formed and are not consciously aware of them. These
sections are described as `event clusters' and they are put into the form of an equation
as Ei. The equation is then: I (improvisation) = {EI, E2..... En). Pressing assumes
that every improvisation is actually generated by triggers at specific time points which
he calls tl, t2... tn that initiate the movement patterns appropriate to affect intended
musical actions. At each time point, schemata for action are triggered and as schemata
are adaptive, subsequent fine motor tuning based on feedback, goes on after each time
point. In other words, these are not discrete time points but like the event clusters, will
overlap. There may also be a referent R which is an underlying guide or scheme used
by the musician to generate improvised behaviour. This could be the standard tune
Also within the equation, G will be the goal (if there is one) and M is long term
memory. The goal may be a point of resolution within the standard tune i. e. at the end
of the first eight bars or driven by the internal logic of the musical line, or it may be
149
The process of event cluster generation is now described by Pressing like this: ({E},
R, G, gI -Ei+l. Decision making in the (i+I) situation may extend back before tl
mature improviser is influenced by his musical environment and other band members,
so for group improvisation, other players would add their own distinct time - point
sequences and also interact. Pressing's equation now looks like this, with K as
k's cognitive representation of all previous event clusters produced by the other
Pressing then breaks each event cluster into partly redundant aspects: the acoustic
aspect which is produced and sensed sounds; the musical aspect which is a cognitive
and the movement aspect which includes the timing of muscular actions,
Visual and emotional aspects will also be involved and there may be others. Pressing
suggests that each aspect exists in intended and actual form: the intended form is
specified at a particular time point and the corresponding actual form is constructed
from sensory feedback. I feel it is more likely that the intended form, whilst initiating
line. As it is played, the improviser then responds to that, not to some wholly
preconceived idea. In a previous quote, Bill Evans mentions this and the desirability
and delight for the improviser of not knowing what is coming next. To return to the
in the sense of a dynamic and adaptive process), we may change what we say in
150
response to the feedback (both verbal and in the form of body language) we receive
but the process is to elaborate or extend the idea we have just put into words.
It may be that we have the whole phrase in non-conscious neural structures (such as
the basal ganglia) which becomes conscious as it is spoken, but given the nature of
schemata and their generalisable features, I find this hard to imagine. There has to be
some kind of comparator (as in the Bernstein model) but what it is and how it actually
functions, is very unclear. Pressing states that the gap between the intended and actual
very often novice improvisers do not even know what they intend, but play
Similarly, many play phrases that they feel have come `out of blue', which again they
try to repeat, refine or emulate with varying results. I feel the process is much less
linear and more chaotic than described by Pressing, especially in the early stages
when the schema may be organising itself. Pressing also considers that in the mature
improviser, the intended and actual form, are almost complete, which I question
because jazz musicians like best to create new and different ideas rather than relying
on licks and so forth. New ideas cannot often be intended but rather arise from a
musical contexts.
151
B. Each aspect of E is decomposed into objects, features and processes. An
over time.
indicates attentional loading, and the importance that a given factor has in
D. The production of E depends on the basis of long-term factors like the use of
the referent, current goals, stylistic norms and so. Pressing posits that there
are only two methods used to continue a solo: associative and interrupt
features and processes with which to effect continuity between the event
clusters, Ei, Ei+1. In this way he proposes that novel ideas could be
generated. For interrupt generation, the improviser breaks off from current
idea into a new direction resetting many component objects, features and
processes.
of the array components stay approximately the same. In the contrast type of
array must alter. He suggests that the most powerful and general types of
152
G. The choice between association and interrupt generation may be modelled by
H. Once the requisite objects, features and processes are selected for all relevant
aspects, tuneable cognitive and motor sub programmes are set in motion that
generate a specific action design. At this point we are at ti+1 in the equation
and the first loop of the process Ei-Ei+1 is complete. Pressing suggests that
There are a number of questions arising from this model: first, is it the case that there
are only two ways of extending the improvisation? There are famous recordings (for
example, John Coltrane playing A Love Supreme) of musicians repeating the same
non-linear system, triggered by schemata with a longer time frame than Pressing
envisages. Harmony is not mentioned by Pressing and yet is a unifying referent for
both the improviser and listener and provides a constraint and a framework for
cohesion that purely melodic or free improvisation lack. Given that novice
improvisers spend many years getting to grips with harmony in the process of creating
melodic lines that fit it without being prescriptive, the model ought to take this aspect
into account.
flexibility in the choice of musical ideas so that whatever creative impulse presents
itself, the motor program will be able to execute it. In terms of the schema theory, it
153
allows the same information to be generalised throughout a range of changing
contexts and is part of the process of abstracting the relationships between notes
rather than replicating the phrases per se. Pressing believes that the control of these
to the feeling of letting go, and going with the flow. This notion relates to the idea of a
elements, with different parts of the system leading the improvisational line at any
particular point. I feel there is no doubt that the development of the skill leads to non-
Pressing suggests that object, feature and process arrays are formed due to the
interaction of innate creative abilities and the environment, and that over the course of
the improviser's life, new arrays and components are constantly being created by new
perceptions and perceptual groupings. The schema theory view would be that the
which is creative to a greater or lesser extent depending on the kind of social and
cultural environment the person finds themselves in, and the amount of practice they
of innate and natural forms of skill assisted by schema development, (like movement
and language) which allow for adaptive and dynamic interaction with the world.
Given this, we have to ask why everyone is not naturally a musical improviser.
Necessarily, novel ideas are rare, but can occur because the schema will provide the
scope for new movements in response to a new musical idea. Pressing proposes a
154
combinations of array components. For example, if a child has played loud and fast
and soft and slow but not soft and fast simultaneouslythen the new combination will
provide a novel experience. How does this relate to the notion of the schema? New
the view that novel actions are built by primarily distorting existing ones, but it is
more that existing actions generated by the schema can be endlessly adaptive among
continuations of a three note ascending motif. The main question is why and how one
particular phrase is chosen over another. At the heart of these questions is the nature
of volition and the mind-body problem about which there is no consensus. Pressing
":" The intuitive perspective, where the individual taps into a powerful source that
dictates the way the music flows naturally. Although this may appear fanciful,
V The residual decision making process (after specific learning) reflects the
the course of the music in ways that they are not even conscious of, based both
155
":" complex characteristics of the system. In other words it is the hand which
generators take over. It certainly feels like this at the beginning of the learning
Pressing feels that it should be possible to distinguish between the validity of intuitive
and random perspectives (although it is not clear how he would do this), but not
between free will and physicalist ones, because it is in the nature of improvisation to
The development of improvisational skill is only touched on at the end of the model,
and clearly, it has much in common with the emergent results of practice in other
capacity for error correction and expressiveness. Added to this in the practice of
in fact opposite in aspect: novelty is sought to avoid repetition and coherence is only
achieved through some degree of structural unity, which is possible only through
repetition. I would add that also related to the practice of musical improvisation is the
development of the improvisers' individual and unique voice. The individual voice is
bound up with timbre, phrasing, musical inventiveness and a predilection for the kinds
of musical lines that express the individuality and identity of the improviser.
Pressing proposes that the specific cognitive changes that allow inventiveness,
cohesion and the raft of other skills needed for improvisation are:
156
1. An increase in the memory store of objects, features, and processes in
perceptual information.
generalisation, abstraction and integration through the range of requisite skills and
honing of motor, auditory and theoretical information. The main difference between
improvisation as an open skill and the more fixed skills of say a classical musician, is
that the fixed skill situation evolves towards a minimal size attention set whereas the
4.3.2 Conclusion
The central features of the model are as follows: it is reductionist because the
cognitive structures of processing and control are broken down into acoustic, musical,
and movement aspects, and each of these are then broken down into characterising
novelty due to the extensive redundancy of the cognitive representations and the
157
series of event clusters during each of which, a continuation is chosen based on
association or interruption which act as constraints on the new material. The model
underlying major philosophical questions about free will and the origins of decision
freewill, physical causation and randomness and to test the basic assumptions of the
The major problem with the model is that it describes well what may be happening as
improvisation takes place (albeit in a complex way) but does not explain how
improvisational skill develops to allow for fluency and cohesion as well as novelty.
holistic activity, and the question of how both flow and cohesion are developed within
of much skill in jazz which is acquired in the normal way through concentrated
(such as jamming and performing) and peer and mentor input. Pressing argues that all
in real time which involves co-ordinating a range of cognitive, auditory and motor
158
features. But this is true of everything we do and in improvisational process we are
relatively unconstrained - that is the whole point. Pressing believes that deliberate
practice helps to minimise the effect of these constraints (if that is indeed what they
are), by creating tools which allow the improviser to concentrate on higher order
aspects, and here he is more explicitly aware of for example, cohesion and coherence
Exactly so, and it is perhaps schema theory that enables cohesion to be developed
unfolding, within which adaptations and changes can be made without altering the
(constraints) that guide and aid in the production of materials. ' (1984) In jazz, an
example of a referent would be the standard American 32 bar song form comprising
As a notated and aural generic framework, the standard song provides a private and
communal melodic, harmonic and rhythmic starting point and reference point for
practising and performing improvisation. Standard tunes are also useful for
providing a framework for recognition and expectation. The common element of the
referent provides internal and external cohesion and enables both thought and action
to be channelled quickly and smoothly*so that higher order skills are more likely to
159
emerge. In keeping with his extensive work on computer programming and Artificial
Intelligence, Pressing is fond of the idea that we have limited `real time processing
skills such as interaction, highly attuned perception and fine motor skills. If we agree
that `real time processing power' is an issue in the human mind, then the schema
provides a short cut to the laborious moment by moment view of action in which
every parameter has to chosen and re-chosen by the brain. The schema provides a way
of initiating movement (or thought or speech) purely through a vague, goal directed
Another tool for improvisational fluency arises from the creation, maintenance and
enrichment of an associated knowledge base in long term memory. The richness and
refinement of this base is reflected in a master's ability to find faster better solutions
and to respond flexibly to diverse musical situations. The novice has incomplete
knowledge which is strongly context specific in other words she has not learned yet
-
to abstract the information - and can therefore only apply a particular riff or pattern or
idea to one key or one situation. Connecting various elements is a key quality of the
mature improviser's skill, and again the schema provides a framework in which the
various motor, cognitive and physiological elements can be linked so that cohesion
and other high level properties begin to emerge. Pressing suggests that declarative
generalised motor programmes, and that efficiency of action and artistic expression
are the two main forces that shape how the individual selects the appropriate
160
The culturally predicted knowledge described above and related to referents, is added
Overall, the knowledge base will include musical materials and excerpts,
repertoire, sub-skills, perceptual strategies, problem-solving routines,
hierarchical memory structures and schemas, generalised motor programmes
and more. It is a cauldron of devices collected and fine-tuned on the basis of
optimising improvisatory performance. (1998b: 11)
framework of these elements, but he argues that it could never be complete. Baker
(1969) and Coker (1989) would argue that this is what they have done with their
method books but perhaps the notated approach represents only one strand of a hugely
rich and diverse skill. The fact remains that jazz improvisers develop their skills in a
very idiosyncratic way and any pedagogy should enable them to develop more easily
or creatively.
The theory concerns what the mind has to compute in order to produce an
acceptable improvisation. A theory of what is computed is not, of course, a
theory of how the computation is carried out. (1991: 291).
the mature skill and are therefore unable to verbalise and explain their experience of
it, except in the vaguest of terms. He posits therefore that those aspects necessary to
illuminate the process are hidden from both the individual improviser and the
161
observer alike and require a separate `disembodied' theory to account for. them. This
idea is questionable: firstly, the novice (like anyone learning anything) is much more
can independently listen to the novice and their accounts, in conjunction with the
music and attempt to ascertain how the learning process is proceeding. Thirdly, a
and systems within the individual but also the musical context and the social
interactions with other band members and audience, all of which affect the outcome
The question of whether the mind `computes' at all, is also controversial given the
human brain is unique in its anatomy and organisation especially at the level of its
contrast are created from standardised components and work with similar operating
development gives to the human brain allows for a wide range of responses and
Tononi's (2000) theory that memory is recreative, this is diametrically opposite to the
`remembers' the exact information that has been stored within it through a digital
system. Recreative memory has no storage `place' as such and is a quality of the
constraints and behavioural dynamics), which serves to select an output that repeats a
162
designed to seek meaning rather than `process' information and therefore the
world. In this regard, the Russian neurologist Luria (1968) described the life of a man
with almost perfect multi-modal and vivid recall, who was quite unable to lead a
normal life, or indeed to create meaning or abstract information from his experiences.
If we agree with Hebb, Arbib and Bernstein that many neural `programmes' are
generalised, then once the skill is learned, any neural circuit appropriate to the
movement or thought can be called up to execute it; the skill is not fixed into a
permanent trace in a specific place, but is an attribute of the whole neural system.
This is quite unlike a computer where information is fixed and stored in a particular
place.
I have previously made the case that improvisation is fundamentally a natural activity
changed with feedback as the action unfolds (Bernstein 1946/68). The brain appears
unable to compute the weight of a cup before it is lifted or the incline of a hill before
the body walks up it. Neither would it be able to predict the physiological changes
needed to move the body forwards over the change in terrain and gradient. These are
all adaptations based on feedback and generalised motor programmes, appropriate and
unique to any given situation, and there is no reason to believe that other cognitive
and brain systems work any differently. Lastly, the environment of the embodied
brain is complex, multi-modal and constantly changing; the individual cannot exist
outside of her worldly environment (for long), and therefore the brain is an adaptive
163
organism receiving and responding to information from which it seeks meaning, quite
unlike the information fed (passively, consciously and using codes), into a computer.
For the reasons above, whatever happens in the mind when a player improvises
computational model based on illuminating one particular aspect of the process might
be a more fruitful endeavour. Schema theory on the other hand can incorporate the
Johnson-Laird's theory proposes that the mind generates improvisations either by the
which explicit structure is not present. The use of symbols to describe cognitive
process is now considered to be problematic, due to the realisation that all thought and
behaviour is adaptive.
Our goal of understanding the brain and replicating human intelligence with
computers will no longer be served well by using the term "symbol" (or
"representation") to refer interchangeably to experiences of representing
something to oneself, to neural structures/processes, and to forms in the world
such as words in a computer programme. (Clancey, 1993)
feel but finds that both the concept and sound of swing impossible to analyse because:
164
He is forced to judge as a human listener when the rhythm sounds `right' because this
the individual player is responding and adapting as the music unfolds, to a range of
internal and external factors which will never happen in exactly the same way twice.
The `natural' sounding way in which a good bass player is able to move seamlessly
through various time changes and styles in the course of a performance, requires an
computer.
No evidence is provided for this claim and it is contradicted by the fact that
improvised solos at the highest level have a unity and cohesion about them that
take one example, Wes Montgomery's improvised solo on Round Midnight (1959)
displays all the attributes of a through composed piece; with development of ideas and
unity of conception, although Wes himself could not read music and never studied it
formally. How this happens simultaneously within the flow of an improvised solo is
one of the key questions about the process, because it is clear that underlying
principles of form are at work even if they are poorly understood. As described
previously, various external factors (like the standard tune) act as an individual and
communal reference point and as a means for internal and external cohesion, therefore
165
melodic improvisation in the jazz idiom clearly does not operate in a vacuum, but
Johnson-Laird's assertion that melodic jazz solos are phrases held together by the
harmonic progression of the standard, is also simplistic for the reasons above, and
reduces the entire improvisational process for both musician and computer to the
generation of a pattern for the rhythmic phrase and the generation of correlated series
of pitches for the notes in the phrase, but the problem of how the jazz musician
`processing power' (1991: 297). The linguistic grammar analogy originating from
Chomsky (1968) is based on the building of meaning through single words into
and deep structure in language have yet to be proven in terms of musical form, as
even at the most basic level, language structures can be broken down into ever smaller
Jackendoff and Lerdahl (1983) used a similar. linguistic model to create a theory of
structures in western classical music. They analysed the grouping structure of motifs
and phrases, their metrical structure, the hierarchy of pitch structure and tension and
classical music and suggested that there are universals in music and that the ability to
organise musical structures, is innate. However, the generative theory of tonal music
cannot apply to atonal music or indeed any music, which does not have a
166
conventional `western art music' hierarchical harmonic structure. We are certainly
able to compose, improvise and hear musical wholes i. e. we have auditory gestalt,
which seems to imply a structural system, but our visual modality also seeks meaning
in wholes and patterns which are completely unrelated to language or any kind of
narrative form.
speculate with a theory that the basal ganglia parallel neural system works for both
language and music, in isolating elements of verbal and musical skill, and
unconsciously. They propose that the conscious elements in the cerebral cortex and
time is speeded up. They also argue that having to think of every aspect of, for
as the flow of thought and response. We are not consciously aware of the neural
process in a vast number of bodily functions including voluntary movement, and the
automaticity of learned actions frees up the conscious mind to respond and adapt to
generated series of chord progressions based on II, V, VI, III, IV and I chords are
underwhelming. The computer operates in a context free zone with no sense of the
167
idiom; no awareness of the melody associated with the progression; without any
feedback about its appropriateness and with no interaction from other players. The
fundamental driving force for a beginner improviser; the desire to learn the skill, is
also absent along with the inspirational experience that prompted the wish to learn in
the first place. All of these aspects including the vital role of imitation, are only
available to the embodied mind acting and interacting with the unpredictable and
changing world in which chance and chaos play a fundamental part. To help to
illustrate this, listening to the great Scottish tenor sax player Tommy Smith live for a
1. The structure of his solos was only loosely based around the harmonic
progression; he took great flights outside of the time and key centres and
was able to change his approach depending on the mood and the
2. He did not appear to `choose' any phrase, but just let phrases unfold in a
coherent and cohesive way that could be followed by the band and the
collaborative action and cannot reach its highest form and flow without
168
one or two notes at any tempo, they could change rhythm, feel, and
the dimensions of the music at will, and was equally influential in this
respect, setting off on a different track which all of the others followed
quickly. Levels of concentration were so high that this exciting but risky
and `on the edge' strategy came adrift briefly only once in 2hrs 40
4. All of the music was invested with a wide-ranging emotional power the
They made perfect sense within the context of the standard and the
The adaptive nature of the skill is of paramount importance and the key issue is the
sublimation of all technical elements to the movement and the creation of a particular
mood and feeling, the integration of that mood with other players and the
imagination, but they all serve the same purpose: to move the music at will in the
direction determined by the individual and the collective in such a way as to be fully
169
In attempting to program the computer to create a walking bass line, Johnson-Laird
1. Each note in a bass line is chosen from the set of notes comprising the current
chord. (This happens in part because there are always some chord tones within
the bass line in order to outline the harmonic framework. However, pure chord
2. Bass players have a series of motifs and phrases, which they can call on
have learned, which they call upon in order to be able to play something
tones, progressions, and melodic contour. I would add: known licks and
with the audience. Perhaps the choice of notes depends upon the exact
Johnson-Laird writes a programme that selects the next interval in a melodic contour
and then its pitch and also programs the computer to recognise chord types and use
acceptable passing notes between chord tones. The example of a generated bass line
170
which he provides sounds very strange indeed, with uncharacteristic leaps, overuse of
b5 passing notes and a complete lack of cohesion and natural flow. Novice human
bass players coming up with a similar line, would be responding to information about
what they sounded like and would realise something was wrong even if it took them a
while to work out exactly what. It is unlikely however that they would produce
something so unnatural sounding: beginners tend to focus on root and fifth notes until
they can keep time. As a next step, they may begin to add passing notes and try to
produce a coherent flowing line. These processes are relatively slow and organic and
the learning process, the novice is interacting with other musicians and receiving
feedback, which causes modifications to their ideas and skills or indeed, at times,
complete revisions of them. The computer can only work on information given to it
and lacks the basic motivation of the human to improvise, hence the random sounding
It may be that any attempt to create a standard computer programme (as opposed
perhaps to a robotic version), for jazz improvisation will be beset with problems
because of the importance of cultural, social and emotional contexts within which it is
played. Fundamentally, the embodied mind never replicates any action or thought
implying that the whole working processes of the brain are adaptable and flexible.
Edelman and Tononi (2000), also make a strong case that brain operations in
perception and memory (both of which are integral to schema formation and
development), involve selection not instruction. This implies that the brain works in
codes or indeed any kind of representational or symbolic codes like those used in a
computer. They propose that instead of being guided by effective procedures (which
171
would suggest codes), the brain is governed more by a degenerative set of effective
structures- the complete opposite of the way a computer works. Add to this the
effects of the wide rage of neurotransmitters; blood flow; hormones and electrical
signals and it becomes clear how difficult it is to create dynamic higher order skills on
a computer. Lastly, rules of logic appear only to apply to learned rational thought, not
to brain process per se and information from the environment is perceived and
received in a multi-modal and non hierarchical manner, completely different from the
computational processes and suggests that these might occur algorhythmically in three
clearly not the case since his 20. experiments at randomly generated computer
improvisations sounded so un-jazz like. The second suggestion is the constrained used
choice of materials. The third suggestion is that some criteria are used to initiate the
ideas and others to develop and continue them depending on evaluative feedback. All
three notions seem limited and reductionist and cannot adequately explain the
disembodied isolation because they have to adapt to changing internal and external
172
implement them, and although some practice for the novice requires imitating and
replicating ideas and patterns, the mature skill requires the developmentof high levels
of concentration so that all the learning can be adapted to the requirements of the
moment and the music at hand. It is a dynamic and never ending process completely
David Sudnow's (1978) account of how he learned to improvise on the piano presents
another perspective on the skill, by describing the process (or as much of it as he can
verbalise), as it unfolds in time from his own viewpoint. He proposes that it is the
body and specifically in this case, the hand, that learns the skill and rejects the notion
movement. He suggests instead that the hands `learn' where to place themselves in
the notion that the learning of a skill progresses from specific detail to abstract
principle and suggests the opposite: that we move from abstraction or rather I think he
proposes the generalisation of motor programmes in the brain suggesting that this is a
neurological process.
Sudnow describes how he began to improvise by using basic scales, phrases and
chords that he learned at the keyboard and played to fit, with specific chords and
harmonic progressions, much in the manner specified by Baker and Coker. He had
173
lessons from a teacher who showed him a repertoire of musical ideas and what he
called `pathways'. Yet, despite much effort did not understand how the teacher chose
the appropriate phrases, and so remained for some time bound by rules he thought
existed for the whole process. This is partly because Sudnow's teacher was unable to
explain how he decided what to play, because the process had become unverbalisable
The next stage he referred to as the `gestalt' stage in which musical wholes were
recognised and reached by the hand with a degree of automaticity. These included
scale and melodic patterns, but also finding chords. Incidentally and unsurprisingly,
the path of his learning to improvise did not run smoothly, although he was directed
by a teacher, and he was faced with various difficulties including a harrowing (but
common) experience at a jam session where he was quite unable to reproduce the
ideas he had practised at home. His major point is that the only way to find out how
to perform the skill is by doing it, and enabling the hand (and the other synergies of
muscles that support it), to learn spatially in relation to the keys and scaffolded by
cognitive processes.
At the third stage, instead of solely visual or conceptual patterns triggering the hand,
he began to hear ideas and combined learned and heard music which resulted in
another intermediate stage where some of the ideas came off and some do not. Finally
he `goes for the jazz' and found his hand reaching for notes without any particular
(verbalised) instructions from the conscious brain. This is the stage that is marked by
dexterity, flow, cohesion and all those other fine movementsand effortless elements
174
Throughout his account, Sudnow is preoccupied with and intent on playing fast and
the crucial turning point in his development as an improviser came through watching
the slow and utterly co-ordinated body and hand movements of pianist Jimmy
More than any other single experience, it was listening to Jimmy Rowles that
marked the crucial turning point in my fourth year of study, when very
significant changes began to occur in my path to improvisation. (1978: 73).
By concentrating on breathing life into melodies and copying the exact body
movements of Rowles, (as many young jazz players used to copy the stance and
movements of their idols), Sudnow developed a new way of being at the piano which
later also affected his faster improvisations. At this point, he heard auditory
`glimpses' of how it would be to really play jazz and to call up phrases at will. He
also realised that his hands resembled other jazz piano player's hands i. e. his hands
The copying of Rowles posture and movement is highly significant and Sudnow
watched him (in a way that resembles Trevarthen's (1999/2000) body to body
communication), not just once, but `for months, night after night... ' (1978: 74) From
this deep level of observation Sudnow was able to abstract a sense of how Rowles
came to improvise musically, and in turn was able to embody the same in
process his
own playing. It is interesting that the interaction with the teacher and the rational
explanation of musical pathways, was not enough for Sudnow to understand the
process, although by the time he came to watch Rowles, he had been playing for four
years and may have been at a point of readiness to assimilate the information. It is
also interesting to note that the knowledge was `out there' to be picked up in body
movement outside of cognition and language and yet related to an aurally based
175
activity. The implication for schema theory (along with other movement related
posture may be far more crucial to the learning of jazz improvisation that is perhaps
realised and that this should be reflected in teaching methods and pedagogy.
By the end of the book, (some five years later), Sudnow writes:
I sing with my fingers, so to speak, and only so to speak, for there's a new
being my body and it is this being (here to so to speak) that sings. (1978: 130)
The significance of Sudnow's ideas is that he focused on the body, implying that it
cognitive processes in the mind. His notion that the novice begins with the specific
(i. e. the pattern, or motif) and develops it into something altogether vaguer (because it
allows for complete freedom of aural imagination), is also interesting and supports the
Having explained the schema theory of jazz improvisation and argued that it provides
a useful theoretical framework through which to understand how the skill develops;
the next stage is to attempt to seek evidence for the schema through the practical
workshops with both groups and individuals in which the process is taught, observed
and recorded. In the intervening chapter before the practical work is described, there
is a discussion about the kinds of qualitative research methods that are most
appropriate to the practical investigation of the emerging schema and the rationale for
their use.
176
5. Qualitative Research: Questions, Issues and
Methods
Introduction
made that case that schemata in jazz improvisation may be non-cognitive in nature,
especially those related to aural and motor processing: cognitive schemata scaffold
what is essentially a physical skill driven by aural imagination and feedback. The
practical research later presented in this thesis, which focuses more on the aural to
methods are suited to this purpose. The emphasis on flexibility of design and in
studying holistic and dynamic events within their natural setting, provides a good
basis for the study. However, there are wide ranging problems and issues around the
need to assessand validate qualitative research in a rigorous way. There are also wide
disagreements in the field about the nature, purpose, practice and status of its
methods. It is for these reasons that the next section will justify a specific use of these
The dictionary definition is: concerned with or measured by quality. Smith (2003: 1)
defines it thus:
177
Qualitative analysis is concerned with describing the constituent properties of
an entity, while quantitative analysis is concerned with describing how much
of an entity there is. (2003: 1).
areas become more multi-disciplinary and broader in scope ( Murphy et al, 1998:4 ).
law from particular instances), and quantitative research as being deductive (inferring
logic: all birds have beaks; this creature before me has a beak; therefore it must be a
bird. Obviously this kind of logic only works when the first statement is absolutely
certain: here the duck billed platypus could also be said to have a beak, as could the
dolphin, and neither are birds. The inductive method formulated by Bacon in the
fifteenth century proposedthat the careful study of many individual caseswould lead
the two approaches. In addition qualitative' research can also be done in a deductive
whether the researcher can understand and explain the meanings of another. An
178
understand what people do as opposed to what they think. They question the
the way that the participant acts in this one particular situation may have no
bearing in another and thirdly, that the researcher does not and cannot simply
interpretation, the researcher inevitably filters, frames, and generally mediates the
difficulty, and the researcher has to address rigorously how they intend to deal
with the problems, especially avoiding anecdotal evidence and their own prior
itself because it is not a direct reproduction of reality. There are likely to be many
equally valid descriptions of a setting and again, there are inevitably theoretical
assumptions behind them. The key is for researcher to state their research
A great strength of the qualitative research process is the emphasis on holism and
opposite of isolating and controlling variables that takes place in most quantitative
research.
179
4. Emphasis on process
qualitative research is its emphasis on process and the dynamic nature of life. This
goes hand in hand with a preference for longitudinal studies and prolonged
engagement with the setting being researched. Under these circumstances (even
with the misgivings over objectivity), it seems more likely that real insights and
useful information will be found, because the researcher will be steeped in and
The emergent nature of research design in many qualitative methods also enables
real insights to be garnered because the researcher has not imposed prior
approach allows for great flexibility, depending upon what the researcher actually
finds.
The choice between one method and another (or both) depends upon what the
researcher wants to know. The questions in the present study require a qualitative
approach particularly as they focus on process rather than outcomes i. e. the process of
improvisation and on the interaction and perceptions of teacher and learner, which are
related to schema theory. The primary reason for using qualitative methods in the
practical investigation is that the subject and ideas in question (the emergence and
development of the schemata for jazz improvisation), are essentially invisible and
ranging enough that would reveal this process in a suitably measurable way. The
180
research process and purpose was to seek evidence for the schema theory in jazz
normal educational setting. Thus, the practical investigation was focused on real
in order to fully engage with the subject. The most important aspect is that the
method suits the question, and researchers need to be very clear not just about the
questions but also about the ultimate purpose of the research and whether the method
Qualitative work is often associated with idealism and quantitative with realism, but
the material world offers a form stable physical reality that is hard to refute. In
qualitative domains what matters is what people perceive to be true rather than what
might objectively be true. Ultimately we can only perceive the world in ways that are
consistent to the physical organisation of that world. In this view Gibson (1966)
proposed that humans (and animals) are in a `systems' relationship with their
environment that the organism is interacting with, rather than sensations that are
allow perception to occur, and the way these affordances are viewed can vary
considerably. For example, a wave may be felt or ignored by the whole body of a fish
as its natural environment; jumped through by a porpoise or provide a view of the fish
force an engineer to build a sea wall. Our perception has evolved in tandem with the
181
physical world which provides a continuous and stable flow of information to which
we can respond. This view is entirely consonant with Johnson's (1987) idea that
abstract thought evolves from physical activity and interaction within the
environment, and establishes the importance of movement and the physical world at
deep levels within the human psyche. In terms of qualitative research, it ought to
natural setting more, because the organisation of people within their physical
research process.
Realists believe that objective truth is possible in the physical world and that scientific
method based on deduction and logic provides descriptions and explanations that are
literally true. As mentioned previously, the problem with this attitude is that science
can only produce its `truths' within a particular historical context (informed by
beliefs) and using the tools available to it. As Kuhn (1962) reminds us, hard science
can produce spectacularly wrong ideas which persist until a new paradigm shift
ideas. is
previously accepted
qualitative work is equally valuable. Similarly, the usual dichotomy between artificial
experiments, is equally misleading as qualitative and quantitative work can take place
15The paradigm shift has a lot in common with the schema. For example, the eye was regarded as little
more than a camera for centuries and that particular schema with all its attendant implications, had a
negative effect on the development of the thinking about vision
182
in either setting. The same underlying principles apply: good research always
is
challengesor at least awareof the assumptionson which it is based.
which has recurred throughout Western thought from Ancient Greece to the present
day. The positivist position, based on Compte's nineteenth century viewpoint, is that
`all genuine knowledge is based on sense experience and can only be advanced by
means of observation and experiment' (Cohen et al, 2001: 8). Essentially this has
been taken to mean that the researcher sets up an experiment to produce certain results
based upon what they (by hypothesis and deduction) think will happen. All decisions
about the research. strategy are taken in advance and any thoughts or judgements that
might occur during the process are eliminated by `objective' systems that are usually
2003: 11):
":" The individual is part of this real world, and so such processes as memory,
emotion and thought are events in the real world with definite enduring
characteristics.
183
V The models (mathematically formulated where possible) will show how
variables interrelate, especially how they relate to each other in cause and
effect fashion.
In this view, as Ashworth goes onto mention, qualitative researchers set aside the
notion of the real world in favour of how people formulate their own versions of
reality. This pluralistic and diverse viewpoint is far more complex and less clear cut
than the positivist position and it realises that the world is seen from innumerable
different perspectives all of which are valid. It is clear that human meanings are
integral to human experience and thought and have little in common with casual
The problems associated with the positivist approach have stimulated what Robson
(1993: 59) calls `post-positivist' science. Under this title, he groups a large number of
new qualitative approaches, some of which I will describe is more detail later in the
subjective, humanistic and naturalistic. The new paradigm methods are seen as being
5.5 What are the major problems concerning the nature and practice of
Murphy et at (1998: 1) outlines four main areas that are problematic within the field of
qualitative research:
184
What can we know about the real world apart from our perceptions of it?
If understandingis only about personal perception, then the world can be infinitely
redefined to suit our own view of it. An alternative viewpoint is that the physical
world imposes a structure that is also explained by physical laws which are fairly
stable (at least until the paradigm shifts described by Kuhn, 1962). The natural
sciences have been highly successful in using scientific method and logic and have
disregarded the problem of what the relationship is between perception and reality,
whilst claiming objectivity. [In reality we now know (Bargar and Duncan 1982; and
and experiments, is actually closer to the qualitative approach than expressed in the
formalised logical way in which the research process is described in journals and
papers. Scientific activity in practice and the way it is presented are very different
things. Robson (1993: 62) goes as far as to say that the confirmatory process of doing
the experiment and writing up is `essentially run of the mill' because the inspirational
and imaginative effort has gone into the thinking and creation of the hypothesis and
this is because of the quantitative emphasis on what can be proved, measured and
forms the basis for action and behaviour and therefore must be taken into account in
any research approach. It could be argued that social organisation forms a framework
in a natural setting and that this provides the basis for analysing people's
Whilst induction is associated with qualitative work, and deduction with the
quantitative approach, good science involves both aspects and the decision about
185
which to use should be made on the basis of whether the method will answer the
Both qualitative and quantitative research can be committed to a certain set of values
(or ideas) and the data used to prove them. We can at all stages of enquiry question
our values and those of others, in relation to the study. We can also ask how someone
comes to hold a particular belief, and finally which ideals or beliefs should be
adopted.
We do not know what other people are thinking but accept that their actions and
behaviour are the result of some kind of internal process and that this process can be
perceived, interpreted and responded to. The problem is how to do it: historically in
attempt to put oneself in the position of the participant or respondent. This has
changed recently to a more process based approach in which researchers examine the
process might be necessary to produce them. But, how valid are findings that
reconstruct the motivations, intentions and perceptions of others and what kinds of
frameworks are used for interpretation? We return again to the presentation of data
by the researcher that allows the reader to check and verify the results to provide a
framework of corroboration and also for the researcher to consider actual or possible
186
5.6 Qualitative Research Methods
A major element of the empirical research process involved observation and the
research tool.
5.6.1 Observation
Observation is a central qualitative method and generally, the researcher adopts the
stance of a learner who is there to understand what is happening within the natural
setting (Fielding: 1993). The process of observation is refined and distilled as the
study progresses, because the researcher will, once again, not want to define the scope
of the study too narrowly at the start (Silverman 1993; Hammersley and Atkinson
1995). Observation studies can focus on process rather than inputs and outputs, and
done properly, this makes them very valuable. Qualitative observational work is
usually conducted over a long time frame with the interaction of the researcher and
observational schedules where the researcher avoids any interaction and is present for
only a short time, seeking to treat the observed behaviours outside of the social,
temporal and historical context. In these more defined studies, participants behaviour
knows what they are looking for (Emerson: 1981). Qualitative observation is much
more flexible and open-ended: the intention is to find what is there and understand it
187
5.6.2 Types of observations
There are a number of observational roles that the researcher can take within a
qualitative study, which range on a continuum from being completely hidden from the
participants, to full participation within the setting being observed. Silverman (1993)
discusses two different types of participant observation: the interactionist and the
participants, and attempts to understand their meanings of both the action and the
The role of observer and the effect that the role is likely to have in the ensuing
research process is crucial in qualitative work, and can range from full participation to
avoidance of any interaction. Fully participant observation aims to `take the viewpoint
1993:48) Silverman also makes the point that participant observers are interested in
understanding the routine or ordinary rather than the extraordinary (Silverman, 1993).
Most qualitative researchers adopt an intermediate position between the two extremes
but the researcher can also have different roles on different occasions or times of the
research process. Gans (1982) identified three roles for the observer: total participant,
researcher participant and total researcher. He proposed however, that being a total
participant is the best option because it allowed complete immersion in the situation
(1982: 54). He goes on to suggest that this period of immersion has to be temporary
particular observational role becausethe researcherwill always enter the field with a
broad perspective and it is important to be explicit about what that is. The research
report is a representation of the study setting not a replication of it, and as Silverman
states: `Contrary to crude empiricists, the facts never speak for themselves. '
(Silverman, 1993:36) In addition, the impact of the researcher on the situation being
observed should be taken into account, although longitudinal studies and long
engagement within the setting are likely to naturalise the effect of the researcher's
presence. A final key point is that researchers should be open in their observations
and clearly explain to participants the underlying purposes and methods of the
research, whilst being aware that the participants may not fully understand that
purpose.
activities and also respond to the behaviour of the participants. Clearly, this kind of
role has a significant impact on both the data that is obtained and the way that it is
subsequently analysed. I chose this particular role, (rather than assigning it to another
improvise (and regularly practice it). As a teacher, I continually observe students and
adapt information and methods to very different individuals. The aim of the practical
investigation was to test the schema hypothesis for jazz improvisation within a real
and natural educational context, with a range of individuals who were essentially
189
collaborators and who were encouraged to be autonomous and to direct their own
learning from the beginning. This required the creation of a safe psychological and
experience and especially working with volunteers, that I could facilitate the learning
environment.
sessions on tape and video enabled reflective analysis at a later date. As such, I
recorded the proceedings on mini-disk. In the case study, I chose to video the process,
having found that purely audio recordings were not detailed or subtle enough and that
the use of body movement, gesture and facial expression was another key to
understanding the internal learning process of the participant. All observations were
conducted openly and with the express permission of all of those involved, who were
in turn, invited to listen or watch the results and inform later analysis.
Descriptive observation is the usual starting point, and Spradley (1980) suggests nine
dimensions on which the data can be collected: space (the physical lay-out of the
room or place); actors (names and relevant details of the participants); objects (the.
physical elements within the space); acts (specific individual actions); events
(particular occasions); time (the sequence of events); goals (what actors are
190
step then is to build up a narrative account and then to look beyond or further into to
The practical research described in this thesis uses observation as a primary method as
I was seeking to find evidence of schema formation and development and therefore
(Denzin, 1970) whereby a hypothesis is put forward, (in this case the schema theory
of jazz improvisation), the situation is studied in the light of the hypothesis (i. e. is
there evidence of schema development? ) and then either new questions arise which
require a new hypothesis or the research is developed further by studying a new case
Even though, obviously, one human communicating and observing another is a rich
Robson (1993: 203) suggests the following `lay person's guide' to the recording of
comprehensively, (this was done with video recording), then added to with on the spot
observations taken during the event. (This was not really possible in my research,
because I was teaching, except to alter the flow of the lesson in order to work on
another area or to develop a particular idea). As a routine action, the notes and the
recording should be reviewed as soon after the event as possible while it is fresh in the
memory and to ensure that fine details are not lost. This process may take as long or
longer than the observation itself, and Lofland and Lofland (1984) suggest five types
forgotten materials; interpretive ideas (i. e. ones that support the research questions
191
and ones that elaborate on it); personal impressions and feelings, i. e. your subjective
As Robson points out (1993: 202), we are selective observers because of the nature of
i.
our perceptual processes e. we filter out those visual aspects that do not resonate for
us. Our*experience and personality will affect what we pay attention to and what we
actually `see' when we are observing, therefore some features will appear to be more
relevant to us than others. Robson suggests that to help to counteract the selective bias
that is built into our physiology, researchers should `make a conscious effort to
distribute your attention widely and evenly. ' (1993: 204). Similarly, there is the
1993:204) that seeks to interpret or categorise data on the basis of partial information.
This means that incongruent data or even later events are marginalised or not taken
into account in the final analysis, so it is important to keep an open mind and not just
seek (and find) the data that is sought. In the same vein, it is also important to write up
any notes especially in writing a narrative account, soon after the event has occurred
Robson (1993: 205) believes that the researcher should be able to get to know and to
interact with everyone in a small group project, again, to reduce the potential for bias.
The problem with group research, even on a small scale, is the amount of rich and
multi-layered data that it produces. In fact, the main reason for the change from
working with groups in the pilot stages, to a case study for the major part of my
192
practical research, was in order to focus attention and observation on the learning
process of one rather than several people. In the practical workshop research,
interactive elements between group members and the overall group dynamic were
interesting factors that were inevitably missing in the 1: 1 case study, and perhaps
those particular aspects could be studied separately at a later date. As Robson points
out, (1993: 205) there is such a wealth of varied information within a group setting
that the researcher could probably find supporting evidence for any kind of initial
behaviour, are not easily elicited through observation alone and may require other
continuing debate about the relative merits of interviewing and observation, (Murphy
et al, 1998:5) and observation is necessarily time consuming and labour intensive.
However, it does allow the researcher to see how people behave rather than how they
say that they behave, which is a problem with interview responses. Interviews may
generate large amounts of interesting data that may be relevant to the area being
5.7 Interviewing
approach), is used in order to account for the dialogic nature of human interaction and
to draw out and understand the meanings of another on a deeper level. Standardised
interviewing techniques treat all participants in the same way (despite the fact that
193
interviewees will probably interpret the questions differently), and remove the
interview itself from the context. The.strength of qualitative interviews that can assess
and elaborate on the participants meanings (Bloor, 1994; Charmaz, 1983b) must be
balanced against the view that all types of interviews are social situations where either
difficult for interviewees to know, understand or verbalise the basis of their actions or
accurate or objective report within the research process. Interviews are accounts of
participants' ideas, feelings and actions and should be understood within the context
of the circumstances in which they occur (Baruch 1981). Another major shortcoming
of interviewing is that the mass of rich data obtained from such sources is difficult to
validate because there is no way of telling whether it reflects people's true behaviour
or not.
Powney and Watts (1987: ch2), make a distinction between respondent interviews and
informant interviews and in the former case the interviewer is in control and has a
interviews are, by contrast, more focused on the agenda and perceptions of the
interviewee, who will be more in control of the process. The interviews (many of
which have turned out to be more like dialogues) that occurred with participants in the
practical investigation, were based more on the informant model as they were centred
on their perceptions, and direct responses to learning approaches and activities. Many
dialogues and narratives were initiated by the main participant in the case study.
Group interviews and questions were asked during workshops to ascertain whether
any part of the learning process was open to consciousness and whether indeed the
teaching approaches were working. In the longer case study, interviews of greater
194
length were conducted, again, focusing on the participant's views, perceptions, ideas
and aspirations and only led in a general way by the interviewer. The next section
(Nisbet and Watt, 1984:72). It provides a unique example of a real person in a natural
setting and takes the context into account as a powerful determinant of cause and
effect. Case studies also have a wholeness and integrity about them that is suited to
the dynamic nature of much complex natural human activity (Sturman, 1999: 103)
which requires in depth study over a long period of time. Hitchcock and Hughes
(1995: 322) suggest that the case study approach is particularly useful when one has
little control over events. They consider that the case study has the following
characteristics (1989: 317): it is concerned with thick and rich description of events
specific relevant aspects; the researcher is integrally involved in the research and the
richness of the case is portrayed in the writing up. The temporal, geographical,
organisational and other contexts, will enable a framework to be drawn up, but the
study could also be defined by the characteristics of the individual or the participant's
roles. Nisbet and Watt (1984: 91) warn against researchers falling into the following
traps in writing. up descriptions of case studies: journalism which focuses on the more
only those aspects of the account that support your particular ideas or conclusions; an
anecdotal style, instead of a rigorous analysis; striving to generate deep insights from
195
low-level data and blandness, i. e. blindly accepting the participants views or including
There are several different types of case study and Yin (1984) identifies three types in
(for testing theories). Merriam (1988) also identifies three types of case studies which
concur with Yin, as descriptive, interpretive and evaluative. The ideal use of the
general large scale research work. The advantage of case studies (from Adelman et al,
1980) is that they are reasonably publicly accessible, especially if they take a narrative
form, allowing for wider dissemination and effect. This is especially so if the
researcher explains clearly the rationale and the methods involved in the research
process of the case study, so that the reader can make up their own mind.
Case studies are a step towards action and the insights gained from them can be put
development; to shape policy or for evaluation. Case studies with sufficiently rich
description can be reinterpreted at a later date in the light of new knowledge or from a
complex and multi-stranded reality embedded within a social setting, case studies can
represent a range of different viewpoints and perspectives within the same situation
and therefore more truthfully reflect life itself. Although they only look at one
instance in depth, they can provide insights into other similar cases, to allow
196
and Watt's (1984) add that case studies speak for themselves, because the whole
context is made clear and captures unique features that might otherwise be lost in
larger-scale, more general research. In addition, case studies can be undertaken by one
equipment is required and the case study can embrace and include unanticipated and
The main weakness is that the results may not be generalisable or only with great
care: it is extremely difficult to validate the findings given the un-objective nature of
the case study and they are prone to observer bias particularly if the observer has
preconceptions or prior theories that they are attempting to prove. Smith (1991: 375)
states that: `The case study method...... is the logically weakest method of knowing.
essentially passe. Recurrent patterns are the main product of the enterprise of historic
scholarship. ' This is a prejudice against case study methods but the problem remains
of how to make this type of qualitative research both reliable and valid.
5.9.1 Validity
mentioned briefly and the whole area is highly contested. One main issue is that the
If this viewpoint is adopted however, it is hard to see how qualitative research can be
197
at
assessed all because if the findings cannot be represented as even an approximation
of the truth, then the value of the research itself is questionable. If the research
(i. e. the teaching and learning of improvisation), then the researcher, participants and
readers must have confidence that the findings are as true as the context and current
conceptual ideas allow. It is of course, impossible to ascertain that the findings are
towards validity whereby the researcher ticks all the appropriate boxes and can claim
valid findings. Unfortunately, the application of a series of rules does not necessarily
enhance the validity of a particular project and there are a series of assumptions
behind the requirements for validity that need careful questioning. For example,
respondent validation of research proposes that the researcher presents the analysis to
participants for feedback on the validity of the conclusions. Given that this is possible,
reports or descriptions. There is no basis in fact for. this: participants may have their
own agendas and, given that the research aim and purpose was outlined to them at the
beginning, they may be unable or unwilling to give an `objective view. ' Similarly,
they may present inconsistent responses over time or in the same interview, so that the
role for participant verification is where the researcher misinterprets or mistakes some
198
5.9.2 Triangulation
is used to study the same phenomenon. The word triangulation itself refers to a
military strategists), in which several locational markers are used to pin-point a single
the richness and complexity of human behaviour, by studying it from more than one
chemistry, or physics) may be enough to provide unambiguous data but is simply not
since it has been observed that research methods sometimes act as filters through
neutral (Smith 1975), it is important that the researcher knows that the results or
findings are not purely the result of using one particular method.
aspects of the data analysis, there are a number of problems regarding its role in
that a multiple data source is superior to a single data source or instrument (Silverman
1985). In addition, Lincoln and Gruba (1985: 307) argue that the search for theory and
for the following reasons: firstly, the importance of analysing data in context is
verifiable. More fundamentally, the agreement of data from two different sources is
not proof of their validity; they may both be wrong! Thirdly, there is the issue of
corroboration of findings: the researcher will be looking for ways to corroborate what
199
she thinks is the truth of the findings, whereas a more valid approach may be to search
for instances which refute the general thinking or theoretical bias. In other words the
inconsistent or negative cases, rather than the presentation of corroborative data using
different methods. Indeed, `negative case analysis' is proposed by Lincoln and Gruba
engagement in the field, and peer debriefing (by a disinterested and objective peer).
Maxwell (1992) argues that it is not necessary for qualitative researchers to use
positivist quantitative tests of validity because they are often irrelevant in naturalistic
settings and suggests that `understanding' is a more suitable term than validity in
qualitative research. Similarly, Guba and Lincoln (1989) suggest that authenticity is a
more useful notion and that it is the meaning that the participants and the researcher
give to the data and the inferences drawn from it that are important. Compte and
Preissle (1993: 338) argue that for authenticity to be a test of validity, the following
appreciation of the situation; catalytic authenticity whereby the research gives rise to
specific courses of action and tactical authenticity in which the research should be of
There are other forms of validity relating to the external aspects of the research i. e. the
degree to which the findings can be generalised and Lincoln and Gruba (1985: 316)
suggest that to aid external validity, researchers should provide sufficiently rich data
200
for readers to determine whether transferability is possible. In the case of the current
jazz research, there appear to be many areas of schema and skill development that
research is based on the premise that the researcher deliberately does not try to
where the learner improviser was a collaborator in the research and was encouraged to
be autonomous and to use materials and approaches presented as they saw fit. On the
other hand, the educational setting itself was manipulated to the degree that it was
chosen by the researcher, and is therefore, not an entirely naturalistic setting. For
activity depending on previous skills, current interests and the make up of the group.
Within that setting, individuals were given the choice of how they would proceed: but
many aspects were directed. To some extent also, my theories and pre-conceptions
(based on previous experience), about the best ways to encourage and facilitate this
Catalytic validity strives to ensure that the research leads to action and during the
practical investigation the action of changing people through learning took place as
part of the process regardless of the outcomes. The findings were intended to alter
(where necessary) the attitudes and perception of teachers about how people may
learn and more specifically to change the way jazz improvisation is taught within the
academic setting. Lincoln and Gruba (1986) suggest that again, the criterion of
`fairness' be applied to the research, meaning that it should augment and improve the
participants' experience of the world and that is should also empower them. From this
viewpoint, the jazz research developed and empowered the participants and is
intended to do the same for future players and teachers through the findings. The
201
teaching materials were specifically designed not only to test the schema theory but
also for effective learning and practice. Many approaches were designed to enable
has continued to work on and use the materials in her own teaching and performing.
The research process has also benefited my own understanding and teaching practice
myself and students and watching closely the learning process in a number of
different students.
Finally, it is perhaps also worth noting that Scheurich (1996) views the methodology
of empowerment, action and change are therefore crucial and central to the ethics of
the research process and it requires that the researcher question her role and
responsibilities throughout.
5.10 Purpose and rationale of the empirical research-how does it test the schema
theory?
The main research process and purpose was to seek evidence for the schema theory in
jazz improvisation by exploring and interpreting the teaching and learning process, in
a normal educational setting. The practical investigation was focused on real teaching
fully engage with the subject. The primary methodology was to plan a series of
described previously. The aim was then to observe the resultant learning process, the
teacher pupil interactions and dialogues and evaluate the improvised music being
202
produced, to seek evidence for the emergent and developing schema. The empirical
work therefore was attempting to track the emergence and development of the schema
assemblage for jazz improvisation by setting a series of tasks designed to enable that
development, and then to evaluate and reflect on the outcomes. It was seeking to
identify those areas where the novice improviser is able to abstract information from
specific instances and use it in new situations. For example, a jazz standard was
learned, to provide a referent for future jazz standards: the melody was memorised
with the harmonic framework heard behind. The purpose was to begin to develop
schema theory proposes that aural (actual and in memory) to motor connection is the
and conceptual information on the nature of melodic jazz music. The memorised jazz
standard (an example of specifically learned sounds and intervals) was then used to
vary the melody in some major way. In the case study, the melody was played
unrehearsed in 3/4as opposed to the original 4/4 to see if the information from the
intervals and the contour of the original tune could be abstracted. From this simplified
standpoint of altering a learned tune, the teaching developed melodic (and later
form and contour rather than harmony. Long periods of time were spent learning and
playing the first standard in the repertoire on the basis of both Bernstein and
Schmidt's proposal that the first parameters of skill learning have to be learned
thoroughly before being developed and stabilised through more varied and variable
practice strategies.
After the sense of melodic improvisation had been initiated, the sound and theory of
the harmonic framework was focussed upon as a scaffold for further schema
203
development and for to provide another referent. At this stage, the research process
to
was seekevidencefor abstractionof harmonic ideasto assesstheir assimilation and
generalisability. Throughout the later stages of the learning process I was looking for
transpositions that anticipate different sections of the tune, or ideas generated from the
melodic or rhythmic energy of the tune that may indicate the schema generalising,
indicating the gradual evolution of automaticity, flow and dexterity associated with
technique is the idea that novice and expert players view technical problems and
parameters differently and I wished to explore how the perception and actuality of
fundamental way, how the novice improviser approached the generation of musical
lines. The idea was to call on a different area of imagination, associative memory and
possibly visual memory, to connect with or unlock a new area of the existing `jazz
improvisation were used with the intention of assessing their effect on the individual
schema, but with the exception of imitation within the group, this proved too difficult
to monitor. In addition to facilitating the learning and observing its process and
outcomes, I was also interested to explore the individual learner's assessment of their
own skill learning and whether any aspects of the emergent schema were open either
204
to consciousness and verbalisation: hence the number of spontaneous verbal questions
asked of learners at various stages of the process. In particular I was keen to find out
what the novice improviser was thinking as they attempted to improvise (if anything):
whether it provided any insight into schema development or if conscious thought was
irrelevant to the process. I was interested in the extent to which conscious rational
thought was used to underpin the process or whether more intuitive processes were
used by the students. All of these aspects could of course, have provided enough
Underlying these questions is the major one of whether the schema is essentially a self
organising system in which the consciousness of the learner plays a secondary role.
With regard to motor learning, both Bernstein and Schmidt predict that variable
practice in the latter stages of skill learning will stabilise the skill, and evidence was
sought for this by varying the scope and type of standards learned, and also
improvisational strategies. Evidence of the stability of the skill was also explored by
introducing new standards and evaluating how the intermediate improvisers coped.
Schema theory predicts that novel ideas occur as a result of schema growth and I
therefore also sought examples of novel improvised ideas particularly in the case
study where new ideas were more clearly evident from more mundane and exercise
based material. Lastly, schema theory (with Hebb, 1948 et al) predicts that
evidence of that was sought during the learning of improvisation in conjunction with
205
6. Practical Investigation: Preliminary
Educational Workshop Study
6.1 Aims
The primary aim of the pilot project was to test initial theoretical hypotheses about the
aim was to facilitate the effective formation of schemata for jazz improvisation among
the learners using specially chosen and organised materials and exercises.
The main research question was whether schema development could be monitored
which the learning was happening i. e. whether any part of the process was open either
was whether the organisation of the material and teaching approaches into a schema
based model, affected the learning process, and if so, how to improve on the
6.2 Introduction
The theory posits that generalised aural, conceptual and motor frameworks (schemata)
are built up, (or abstracted from received information) during practice of a task to be
learned. It should follow therefore that if the participant focuses on a single aspect of
the task, later varying the contexts or ways in which it is performed, the schema
206
should be more effectively formed, and the knowledge and skills more easily
education methods and manuals), focuses on learning scales, motifs, fragments and
based model that views the creation of improvised musical ideas as learning a
construct `sentences' and then `paragraphs'. Implicit in this view is that musical
coherence and individuality develop from being able to manipulate the materials of
intervals and scales until all the patterns are assimilated and any combination of them
can be played. From this basis, (which takes a great deal of time and practice), more
creative approaches are assumed to emerge and evolve. The pattern based
chord/scale method has evolved from studies of be-bop phrases in which the smaller
motifs were learned individually and then reassembled in longer phrases. Having
argued against this method and for a more individual and musical approach from the
start, in the practical research, I wanted to begin by using the melodic contour of a
standard as the basis for improvisation and to explore all the possibilities within it,
before moving on to an analysis and understanding of the melody within the context
of the harmony. I was seeking to develop the idea of improvising a cohesive melodic
207
line over two, four, eight and sixteen bar sections and having achieved that, to see
how the developing schema was used by the students to learn similar standards but
each with a defining difference. To this extent, my approach to teaching was also
recordings and feedback questionnaires from the students were all used in an attempt
to triangulate and so assess what was achieved and how it may work both
6.3 Method
The research took place in a workshop setting in two-hourly sessions over a period of
nine consecutive weeks and consisted of 18 hours of playing and tuition with
Aitkenhead, viola, Jessica Hall, viola, Ruth Dyson, recorder, Steve Turner, guitar,
Carolyn Hill, piano and Konstantinos Dmitri, piano. These classical instrumentalists
were volunteers from Sheffield Music Department with one being an advanced
student from Chetham's music school. Classically trained players were chosen in
order to focus on investigating how improvisation skills emerged, rather than, for
example, basic technical problems associated with instrumental technique. The aim
was to isolate and work on the more musically imaginative aspects, knowing that
fundamental classical instrumental technique was already in place. This was the only
`experimental' constraint put on the research process. The other aim of the pilot
project was for the musicians to work as a jazz group, with a mix of chordal and
single line instruments and to create a natural and informal educational setting where
they felt free to take risks and were under no pressure to produce `great' music. Also,
given the importance of creative collaboration in jazz improvisation, there are many
208
benefits to learning the process in a group setting, the most important of which are
The intention was that all the teaching/facilitating, should be educationally useful both
individually and collectively, and therefore the most effective approaches were sought
out of a selection, all of which were intended to be beneficial to the student. There
was no control group, as this did not seem appropriate in the educational context of
the project. The case could be made that over a period of nine weekly lessons,
whatever the method, any reasonably motivated student would have learned some of
the basics of improvisation, if only assimilated from observing and listening to the
rest of the class. What was sought, however, was qualitatively different approach
based on melody which allowed the novice to improvise within a narrow framework
felt to improvise, so that the initial feeling could be developed (in order that the
schema could also develop), and to emphasise the strongly individual nature of
improvisation that need not necessarily be based on chord tones or patterns but rather
on the rich melodic resources of the individual player. The first task was to use the
melody of a standard tune, as a source of intervals, contour and rhythmic energy over
32 bars, so that the student was provided with both an easily learned framework to
enhance and develop, and the means to do the same in other contexts.
The second task was to attempt to follow this process as it occurred within the
individual and the group. This provides a number of challenges for the researcher
(some of which are common to all research into learning processes and some which
209
9 the varying lengths of time needed for individuals to learn material and skills;
9 the question of whether these processes are open to consciousness and if so, at
what level;
" the likelihood that learning does not proceed in discrete and rational stages
" whether we can gauge an improviser's skill and understanding from the
The third task was to create and alter materials and approaches in the light of the
above research, to develop better ways to facilitate the process and to try and
facilitated high-level individual and creative learning, for use by general music and
These were relevant to all sessions and were taken forward into all new tunes and
pieces.
improvising using the standard tune, Autumn Leaves. (This particular piece
210
was chosen because a) it has sequential melodic motifs in the tune; b) it
follows a fairly standard harmonic chord progression which changes key and
c) it can be played in many and variously different ways easily i. e. using time
> Learning the melody by heart and being aware of the structure and melodic
contour;
¢ Playing the learned tune by ear in different keys beginning with those
> Playing the learned tune in different tempi and rhythmic feel;
> Extemporising around the melodic contour and moving gradually away
> Using the rhythmic ideas and structures to develop ideas and using the
211
¢ Working specifically with jazz feel and swung quavers by. singing,
> Developing a progressively cohesive solo over longer phrases from two, to
¢ Constantly connecting and linking all these various aspects of the learning
My decisions on the curriculum or repertoire for the jazz research were made
student with the value being in the subject matter itself rather than any interaction
with the learner. Curriculum as product is essentially that which I have outlined in the
212
planning here, with clear objectives. This view assumes that learning works in a fairly
linear way and that scientific methodology (by clearly defining intentions and
where both teacher and learners participate in deciding and implementing what is
the. knowledge nor methodology is static. For the purpose of the practical
as a jazz improviser and teacher, in order to explore and experiment with a new
conceptual knowledge and very high levels of concentration and listening skills, all of
which need to be introduced to the students in a coherent way in order to enable high
To test the notion that melodic improvisation based on a standard tune is a good
interesting sequential melodies and fairly typical standard jazz harmony. Autumn
Leaves was chosen as the first tune because it is relatively simple to learn; easily
to
adaptable numerous variations and does not require any knowledge of swing feel to
make it work. In the workshops, the standard was learned by heart, then played in
213
many different keys by ear and used as a framework and contour for extemporisation
as a way into improvisational process. The second stage was to learn the chord
progression as a series of related chord tones, and then a progression of key centres,
and to broaden the initial melodic ideas to incorporate various harmonic elements.
Expressive qualities, exploration of timbre and jazz swing feel were incorporated in
the exercises and at every stage individual expression and musical ideas were
encouraged in conjunction with the very necessary imitation of each others ideas.
The second stage of the process began again at a slightly more sophisticated level
with the introduction of a new standard with a defining new element: a rhythmic
twist, a different time signature; an unusual melody or a broader range of key changes
within it. The aim of the educational approach was to link previous knowledge and
skills; to develop a new aspect related to previous work and to improve concentration,
listening skills and technique in the process whilst retaining a good humoured and
safe psychological environment. Had the course been allowed to continue beyond the
planned nine weeks, the curriculum would have expanded to take in the whole range
of jazz standards across eighty years and to introduce the students to the full scope of
jazz styles and approaches in order to better develop their own. Indeed, a true jazz
curriculum would ideally have involved in depth and concentrated listening to the
(Berliner 1994). However, for the current research, I chose to focus on the practical
improvising skills and on students listening to each other rather than listening to the
repertoire itself.
I considered it good teaching practice to base the lesson planning and delivery on
Ausubel's (1963) and Smith's (1998) methods which use the `advance organiser' to
214
lay out to students what will happen in the lesson as a preliminary taster; how this
particular lesson fits in with the whole subject; what I think the students should be
able to learn in the time available and how the activities relate to previous and future
work. According to Smith (1998), all lessons should be taught from visual, aural and
automatically contain all of those elements but it is useful to remember the various
modalities in other less practical areas. Theoretical musical concepts for example,
could be notated and visualised on instruments, heard and then played. At the end of
the lesson, the content, activities and subsequent implications were reviewed with
suggestions from students were incorporated into the following week's sessions to
6.6 Summary of the pilot project jazz workshops; lessons learned and significant
findings
The three students who attended the first session learned the tune Autumn Leaves by
heart easily and were able to play it immediately in various different rhythmic
formats: as a waltz, a tango and a Latin tune. They were also able to transpose it by
ear at will; to create rhythmic motifs between the long notes and to extemporise a new
melody based around the intervals of the tune. The transformation of the tune itself
were done by the students without practice as a natural extension of learning the
melody, indicating that even this simple process was schema based and therefore open
to adaptation. The feedback questionnaires provided some information about how the
students felt. They were pleased to find out how easy it was to alter and extemporise
around a simple tune and felt confidence in developing their skills over the coming
215
weeks. During the first session, they had already experienced the feeling of
possibilities. The results of the first session indicated that a schema based approach
improvisation in a natural and melodic way. The group workshops sessions were
helpful to students because of the imitation, musical ideas, knowledge and support
provided by the group, but it was also more difficult for me as teacher/researcher to
The second session was intended to develop the ideas of the first week by including
some harmonic elements: explaining the shorthand of chord symbols; learning chord
tones and integrating this knowledge with the work already done on the melody.
However, several new students came to the class and it was decided to revise the
previous work on melody for the benefit of all. Some of the intended work on chord
tones was done towards the end of the afternoon, which focused on roots and thirds of
the chords, and motifs based on them. The students appeared to find this activity
more difficult. The student feedback from the questionnaires was not particularly
revealing: most did not mention the use of the melody; one expressed the difficulty of
the same time and another wanted to visualise the chords on the piano rather than the
viola she was playing. It was useful to them as classically trained musicians to
analyse the harmony, as it helped them to understand the melody better. Therefore in
the future, with similar students, the two aspects may be fruitfully taught in tandem.
There was also both a concern that students could not hear themselves properly
because everyone was improvising at the same time, and an overall lack of confidence
in using the harmony as the basis for improvisation because of their stated lack of
216
theoretical knowledge. The introduction of chordal and theoretical concepts appeared
therefore to slow down and inhibit the more natural flow of the melody-based
simple standard, but perhaps this was due to unfamiliarity with chord symbols oj:
dispel anxiety or self consciousness amongst the students, but I soon discovered that,
improvise on their own, within the group. Students liked to listen to their peers and
In the third session, the aims and activities focused on continuing work on the chord
tones of Autumn Leaves and on extending the chordal work to include larger chunks
of harmony in the form of chord families and key centres. Modes were introduced as
(which the students absorbed easily), and all the previous melodic, chordal and
harmonic work ' was connected in a final `free blowing session'. The aim was to
integrate each new approach with a previous one and to work on areas that needed
specific attention, for example the harmonic background. All students improvised on
their own in front of the class and were able to construct a solo making musical sense
they were utterly absorbed with the process and played for one and a half hours
without a break. The student feedback reflected the positive aspects of this class with
higher confidence levels and better understanding of the harmonic background. One
student wrote that she was still envisaging the chords through `a mental image of the
217
To provide a contrast and to extend the rhythmic and harmonic parameters of the
skill, in the fourth session, a new tune was introduced; Bluesette by Toots Thielemans
jazz waltz with a sequential tune and more sophisticated harmony. The tune was
-a
learned by heart swiftly, although the students had more difficulty with the chords
-
especially those that did not fit in with the previously learned II, V, I model. There
were also many more changes of key to assimilate and get under the fingers. They
did however appear (by the end of the session) to have absorbed the new progression
relatively easily, having spent so much time on Autumn Leaves. In the feedback
questionnaires, they stated that they could relate knowledge and skills already learned
in Autumn Leaves, to the new tune. One student explained that it was easier for her to
learn the music in a `flowing, moving-through-time way, rather than breaking things
down into ever smaller components'. The same student also applied chord theory to
learning the melody so that it made theoretical as well as aural sense to her.
In response to the feeling from the students that we needed to work on the more
rhythmic aspects of jazz improvisation, the plan for Session 5 was to continue
working on Bluesette and also to do specific exercises on rhythm. For this I chose to
do a blues based workshop in which the chordal elements were greatly simplified and
other players and solid rhythmic feel. The result of exploring the blues changes after
so much more sophisticated jazz harmony was that the students felt easily able to
improvise and very confident in their new skills. As the blues progression is so
simple, they were able to concentrate on the overall development of ideas over the
twelve bars and to responding quickly to riffs and motifs played by others. A
collective piece was devised in which players improvised backing figures behind the
soloist and made an on the spot arrangement. Feedback from this lesson highlighted
218
the value placed on learning purely through listening, by the students. They
particularly enjoyed the musically interactive elements and the freeing up of the
improvisational process that playing the blues progression provided. The indication
was that the students learned best, and had most confidence in the learning, where
listening and group interactivity were at their highest. Theoretical and visually notated
approaches not only seemed to slow up and make the process more academic, but also
sidetracked the listening and responding elements that the students found most
effective. Predominantly aural approaches which formed the basis for traditional jazz
improvisation learning appeared to be the most effective and enjoyable for schema
development.
In session six, the intention was to continue developing the aural learning from the
previous week by extending the blues format to learn a be-bop blues by Thelonius
Straight No Chaser was used to spark new rhythmic ideas, because the phrases of the
melody (which is a repeating, slightly altering motif), go across the beat and the bar
and it has unusual offbeat accents. The standard blues chords were also extended to
provide new colour tones in the form of minor seventh to dominant chords and hence
different melodic possibilities. Rhythm was also the focus in Bluesette where a range
of rhythmic phrases were explored in 3/4, again as an extension of the 4/4 phrases
previously learned. To keep Autumn Leaves in mind, and to extend the work already
done, as a final activity, the students were presented with a re-harmonised version by
pianist Andy Laverne and a discussion ensued on the various sounds and effects
evoked by the new chords. On reflection, it was early in the improvisation process to
enjoyed playing through the new progression and contrasting it with the much simpler
219
original. The obvious next stage would have been to improvise over the re-
harmonisation, which was not done in the end: it was enough to alert the students to
other possible strategies and approaches. Student feedback was positive even though
I felt the class was quite tired. One student wrote: `the Monk (Straight No Chaser),
tune had significantly expanded my ideas about the improvising possibilities on the
blues. ' Interestingly they felt that they still had a problem with harmony and
After collaborative reflection on the process of Session 6, the focus on rhythm was
continued with the introduction of Well You Needn't by Thelonius Monk, to explore
more off-the-beat rhythms and unusual rhythmic emphasis. Work was also planned on
the use of chromatic chord patterns and improvising over relatively static
ideas and to keep the blues in mind, we also learned a walking bass line for the
blues. The students were very excited by the rhythmic energy and unusual phrasing of
Well, You Needn't and by its call and answer nature and by focusing again on the
melody initially, some very interesting improvised ideas resulted. There was also
practice on moving motifs up a semitone (from F7 to Gb7) to fit the chromatic and .
repeating chords, which the students found quite difficult at first, indicating that the
repetition of improvised phrases exactly in a new key was still hard for them, and that
this aspect of the schema was as yet underdeveloped. The implication was that whilst,
the general outline of a phrase or an answering phrase was relatively easy for them to
imagine and play, more exact forms and phrases presented a problem, implying that
the abstraction of intervals and movements to make them had not fully occurred.
220
The plan for Session 8 was to continue with Well You Needn't and to practice
transpose up a semi-tone from F7-Gb7, whilst keeping the rhythmic energy of the
tune. This still seemed to pose a few problems especially in the transposition section
of the tune. To incorporate work on the blues and to add to the repertoire, another tune
was learned - Sister Sadie by Horace Silver, (which is blues-based call and answer
piece) - with a view to exploring that format some more. The call and answer phrases
worked well, with much improved listening and response and the intense jazzy and
swing based nature of the melody provided good rhythmic energy for the students to
improvise. The notion of using space within the solo to pace the melodic ideas and
give breathing space for phrases was also introduced and students played through
various exercises leaving gaps and starting the phrase on different beats of the bar.
They also worked with density and sparsenessof notes within the phrase. For the first
time in the feedback questionnaire, I asked the students what kind of mental pictures
they had whilst improvising. One said: `A combination of shapes guided by the
lesser extent melodic/harmonic things. )', and another: `I'm looking at the piano keys,
trying to visualise chords and scales and shapes in them. I usually play according to
patterns that I see, rather than things that I hear, but sometimes I hear ideas in my
The final session was intended to consolidate material learned to date and to leave
those students who were interested, with ways of continuing to practice what they had
the interests of educational research, I wanted to find out if possible, what the
student's had learned over the nine weeks, and began with a revision of all the tunes
221
from memory, in order to find out how much had been remembered by heart. Autumn
Leaves, Bluesette, and Sister Sadie were well remembered but the. students had
problems with Straight No Chaser and Well You Needn't. The first three tunes were
relatively straightforward, both melodically and rhythmically, whereas the last two
required a new rhythmic sophistication and understanding that clearly need more time
to develop. The implication is that the first standards were easily assimilated into the
student's existing schemata being relatively straightforward, but that the more
abstraction which delayed schema development in that area. Also, overall, less time
had been spent on them during the nine week period. All of the various different
intervals; modal scales; call and answer phrases and rhythmic density and sparsity.
Suggestions were made about how students could continue to develop their
improvising skills once the workshops had ended by using `chops practice' (a
The second half of the last session was devoted to writing a short jazz piece for the
rest of the group to play with the students performing and improvising around it. The
aim was to assess how much of the jazz idiom the students had assimilated over the
nine weeks and to begin to assesstheir developing schemata. To simplify the process,
I suggested the students use a standard chord progression from one of the tunes
already learned, or a blues as a basis for the composition. After writing, the students
were asked to teach the piece to the group aurally. One of the tunes was then chosen
and worked on more fully, with students creating textures, background figures and
222
riffs and responding to solo phrases, as group members took it in turns to improvise.
The indication from this part of the session was that the students had absorbed
sufficient elements of the jazz form and style to compose and improvise over a basic
From listening to the players and taking into account responses from the student
feedback, on average, they could remember and improvise confidently on three of the
six tunes we had learned over the nine weeks. They enjoyed writing the jazz tune and
improvisation especially using existing chords and two said that they would write
more. Three said that they would continue improvising and one that they might
form a group or band for this purpose, not having understood that the basis for
learning in jazz is group collaboration and interaction. Two could improvise without
looking at their instruments whilst two could not, and all felt more confident about
improvising, with three out of the four stating that more practice would give them
more confidence. I asked what the most important aspect had been for them in the
¢ `Learning a new way of playing as I have been playing `classically' for most
¢ `I now feel that I have different techniques for improvising available to me. I
now have a way to approach it whereas before I felt overwhelmed and didn't
223
¢ `I now know the process for improvisation. I know how it is done even
I asked whether they could quantify what they had learned over the nine weeks and
the responses ranged from `producing and developing motifs' to `a better feel for
improvisation, implying that they would be able to continue those processes on their
own.
In summary, the main elements learned from initial sessionswere that the teaching
material and approaches worked in a general way; the questionnaires were not very
useful at eliciting insightful responses, for people are simply not able to articulate in
words what was happening to them in practice. Also, although I could structure my
what was being achieved by the students was in fact schema development. It became
clear that students had very different perceptions and perspectives on what they had
learned and that much of the learning process was not verbally definable at any deep
level.
musicians this would be hard to prove definitively, and would require a control group
224
of less technically able musicians to verify. It appeared to be useful to spend a
relatively long time working on the same tune (Autumn Leaves) because it enabled
students to improvise of over progressively longer time frames, and the success of
their first 32 bar improvisations were indications of this. Also, the constraints placed
on the improvisation by first focusing on the melody and then the chord tones kept the
process simplified at the beginning, allowing the students to build skill and
confidence. The overall approach was validated by the students learning of Bluesette
with ease and one student's point about the positive aspects of learning the music in a
`flowing, moving through time way' rather than breaking things down into small
components, also helped to give credence to the approach and materials. In terms of
schema theory, the focus on specific intervals and one tune enabled immediate
to
adaptation and continuing abstraction of elements new tunes. The harmonic aspects
of the schema appeared to take longer to assimilate and develop, perhaps because of
the introduction of theoretical elements or just the amount of information that the
students had to deal with. From an educational perspective, predominantly high levels
of confidence appeared to indicate that the teaching was working, mainly by focusing
feedback suggested that more work was required on rhythm, especially swing feel,
and on the particular sound qualities of different types of chords and how they can be
used expressively, and also on analysing the harmonic structure in conjunction with
listening elements and to encourage individual playing within the group in order to
facilitate it, and b) perhaps the harmonic structure should be introduced with the
225
melody to aid understanding. It was clear that more aurally based approaches were
effective for schema development and that these should predominate in any future
work.
Thirdly, the questionnaires did not provide deep insights into the learning process and
the possible reasons for this include varying individual interpretations of the same
question, preconceived notions about what the researcher wants to hear, and the
indicated a need either to alter the questions in order to elicit different responses or to
alter the way in which the questions were asked. Students were therefore asked more
questions spontaneously during the workshops, in the hope of eliciting more intuitive
insights and more work was done on asking the right questions for questionnaires at
Fourthly, it was clear that students had very different perceptions of what they had
learned from lessons that had seemed to me to be simply constructed and with very
clear purposes. As mentioned previously, the students may indeed learn similar things
but their individual perception of them is very different. The major learning for me
was that proving the existence and development of the schema will be difficult, and
that it would perhaps be better to focus on one individual, rather than a group for the
main study.
Finally, the last point is the perennial teaching problem of the gap between what was
planned (rationally) for the lesson, and what actually happened during its course. This
is particularly the case in the introduction of new information based on what has
226
already been assimilated (or not) and how this affects the existing schemata of the
students. The gap necessitates a certain flexibility between what the teacher thinks is
needed to acquire the skill and how long the student takes to actually do it, hence the
disparity between plans and actual activities. As a general rule, in the jazz
improvisation workshops, if the activities were producing a flow of music and fruitful
practice, I tended to let them continue regardless of the plan. Similarly, if students
wanted to continue or repeat certain exercises, these autonomous decisions also took
precedence and this kind of flexible practice is part of the way in which jazz
From later sessions I understood that students learn in very different ways. For
example, one will benefit from a highly theoretical and rational approach whereas
another will want to base everything on what they hear and can imagine aurally. Some
are orientated by visual patterns on the instrument (especially piano and guitar) and
others have to begin with aurally based patterns, which makes it harder for them to
envisage the shape and components of the chords. It is important, therefore, to ensure
that a wide range of activities and a good mix of rational, theoretical, aural and
intuitive approaches are covered in every lesson. In addition, students were willing to
improvise on their own given the judgement free environment; they preferred to hear
themselves clearly and also liked listening to other peoples solos for both interest and
and shortcomings and asked to work on specific areas, for example, rhythm and the
jazz swing feel. It was apparent too, that non-chordal instrument players would be
assisted if they could see visual representations of the chords on the piano. It might
therefore be useful to spend a session purely on harmony worked out at the piano.
227
6.8 Summary of second practical study
The second preliminary workshop consisted of ten students doing improvisation for
Aitkenhead, viola; Wei-Wei Kung, voice; Chris Scott, piano; Yuko Takahashi,
violin; Jacky Hill, flute; Nick Fletcher, double bass; Lisa Hoyland, baritone
saxophone; Peter Turner, flute and Bethan Lee, and Esther Trewinnard both on
clarinet. Chris, Nick and Alice had some previous experience of jazz improvisation,
whilst the others were beginners (or at least claimed to be.) The workshops were
recorded on mini-disk and as I was either teaching, playing or in dialogues with the
students, during the sessions, I responded to the flow of the class as I would do
the introduction of new materials and so on, depending on how the class was
responding. This process is part of ordinary educational practice and no attempt was
made to alter what I considered to be the natural flow and progression of the lessons.
The classes were planned in a similar way to the first study, whilst incorporating
extemporisation and improvisation using melody as ä starting point and the later
The main finding from this particular class was that the students collectively had a
significant effect especially in imitating and extending each other's improvised ideas.
In addition, like the first group, they were able to transpose improvised ideas into new
keys within a short space of time, indicating an area of schema development which
allowed them to generalise and transform elements of the tune into different keys i. e.
the information about the relationships between the intervals had been abstracted
quickly and easily. One of the tunes that were learned at this session was One Note
228
Samba by Jobim, and the technical difficulty of the piece lies in the transpositions of
the middle eight into difficult keys at a relatively fast tempo. During the improvisation
over this section, several players imitated the tune to help them through it and many
echoed elements of the melody throughout their own solos to orientate themselves
through the progression. Another clear strategy used by several players was to play a
note or phrase that fitted in both keys to ease transition into the new key and this was
imitated throughout the group. Whilst improvising four bar phrases around the group,
they also imitated and developed ideas and were able to follow the form of the 32 bar
tune without getting lost, again indicating a grasp of the whole piece, even whilst
playing fragments of it. These exercises were developed into eight bar phrases and
finally the students were able to improvise over the whole 32 bars individually.
Certain players then began to take more risks and to go for ideas, (also using timbre),
with variable results, but great energy, and when eventually they were distracted or
got lost in the progression, other players would play parts of the tune to reorientate
them, and backing figures on the roots and fifths of the chords to assist. Overall, this
creative and intuitive way by being able to be responsive to the musical environment
6.9 Can we learn anything about the developing schema from these two sessions?
facilitator and the participants, which seemed to provide more insight into the process.
I asked questions of the students as they played, and received some interesting
answers about their perception of the improvising process. A student (L) expressed
(probably on behalf of all), the notion that she found it `hard to feel the harmony
behind me' and so was unable to continue developing her improvised ideas. The idea
229
of `feeling the harmony' rather than hearing it implies that at a higher level it is sensed
and embodied rather than merely heard and that once it is felt (presumably through
the whole body), then it is possible to predict and to imagine ideas based on predicting
the sound of the harmony. W viewed the harmony as `a safety net for me'; a thing
that could rescue her if she tried something beyond her technical skill. As a result of
the metaphorical safety net she said: `initially I had the feel of the tune in my head
and then I got more adventurous in my approach. ' This was indeed true; she took
quite spectacular risks resulting in exciting and `on-the-edge' improvisations. She too
used the word feel, not hear, and it is either a deeper and more embodied sense that is
being referred to here by both students or verbal imitation suggesting a desire for
shared meaning (as well as music! ). The implication is that schema development
incorporates deeply felt (as well as heard) understanding of the sounds of the harmony
and that at this level it then becomes possible to improvise both more rationally (i. e.
using learned strategies) and creatively. W also said: `It's good to be experimental
and see how far you can test the boundaries', indicating that to her, normal
constraint and needs to move beyond that. W was testing the boundaries before she
really knew what they might be and she appeared to have by-passed the need for
personality.
W was the first person to experiment with timbre by using Louis Armstrong type
growls most effectively within her improvisation and this was appreciated and
imitated by the rest of the group on their various instruments. She explained that; `I
tried to imitate, you know, to get the gruff sound of the brass (demonstrates). '
She was already thinking about how the timbre and sound might affect the
230
improvisation and naturally experimenting with different sounds, whilst being aware
that the growl is a standard jazz trumpet style. Crucial to the idea of schema theory is
that each unique individual absorbs and assimilates information idiosyncratically and
whilst a lot of what they learn will be similar; they will each perceive what they have
W perceived that the boundaries are to be broken before really knowing what they
are; L perceived that she has to feel the harmonic framework before she can improvise
B liked playing over the middle section in One Note Samba, with the keys changing
from Eb to Db Major. She said: `I enjoyed working towards the contrast. Just
thinking of like letting it ...... but I always expect the second half, the third
section of C to be shorter (laughs) because was it last week? It was shorter than
the first section? ' Again B seemed to want to be able to just let the improvisation
flow by sensing the harmony at the right point and time. She also realised that the first
tune we played the previous week had only eight bars in the final A section (of the
AABA form) whereas One Note Samba has sixteen bars (it is an ABA form). So
already she had a sense of the 32 bar standard AABA format which she had to alter to
fit the different tune this week; the schema has incorporated the 32 bar format and
now has to adapt to a 40 bar tune of 16,8, and 16 bar sections respectively.
Y expressed concern that we had no bass and drums and it was difficult for her to
keep in tempo. She was probably hearing the standard jazz group here and the class
`rhythm section' of guitar and piano with other instruments filling in with long root
notes was not quite the same! Sometimes the ad hoc rhythm section also got out of
sync with players going three times round the A section and so on. Y said that she
231
was thinking about the harmony more sometimes (instead of a melodic line? ) which
she considered `dodgy' and then realised that the boundaries or stylistic parameters
are not as strict as they are in classical music, so she had more space to play what she
liked.
In the second session a week later, the consensus amongst the students after playing
One Note Samba, and Sister Sadie (by Horace Silver), was that the improvising
process had got harder for them. There could have been a number of reasons for this:
their idea of a `good' improvised idea had changed and developed so that they could
not play their former licks; they had added theoretical information about the chords
and harmony to cope with (and this is invariably confusing at first); or maybe they
were just more aware of what they were playing and how it sounded and were perhaps
getting bored with it. Y said: `I mean, this one, to follow the harmony, progression
or whatever, I think it's easy but erm, I'm getting used to the improvisation itself
but sometimes I don't have much ideas of a melody, so I think that improvisation
is in a sense, it's really improvisation just like the colour of your heart or
whatever. But at the same time you have to have some basic idea or some
experiments like for example I just used this (demonstrates idea). If you have like,
some materials which you can invent from it would be better but I don't have
much experience of that so I just like cant' get...... ' I think that Y was saying that
the improvisation process should be more like a feeling and yet you still need to have
some kind of framework or materials to use in order to express it. There is also a
sense of the frustration in the learning process: it feels (and often sounds to the
outside listener) that the ideas are almost coming together, but not quite. It is like a
glimpse of a shapeor shadow that takes some time to come into focus. The senseis
that the unifying yet intangible shape is the schema organising itself and coming
232
together briefly and intermittently at the beginning of the learning to allow the learner
B said: `I'm finding it a bit harder because I'm trying to think of new ideas.
(Everyone assents and laughs here). She continued: `Yeh, because I'm trying to
play something that I haven't experienced before. If I was doing it for a job I'd
just play the same things over and over again because you'd find something that
sounds good and just keep to it. If my lifeline depended on it I'd try and make it
sound good as opposed to try and think of new things. ' Thus for B, ideas that
sounded good and worked were not the same as pure improvisation, where thinking of
new ideas is more important. B is I think, referring to jobs which pay with the phrase:
`if my lifeline depended on it' where she would have a more pragmatic approach and
the idea is to make the music sound good for a paying audience. Indeed, in a later
which the player finds a musical idea that they like, then learns it and repeats it
forever after as a fixed idea. It is interesting that even at an early stage, the students
quickly become bored with ideas that work and that they can play easily, and actively
move on to acquiring the more dynamic and difficult aspects of the skill.
E said that she also found improvising more difficult and I asked why: `I'm not sure.
I think I was listening to the wrong thing: I kept listening to W singing the tune
and I don't know if I really should have been listening to the rhythm. (Everyone
laughs with W. No, but the piano and the guitar has got the chords and the
harmony and a bit more rhythm. I don't know why but I think maybe because
you need to know these more, my ear just sort of goes straight to the melody. ' E
was listening to the tune (which W was singing in order to orientate herself and the
233
rest of the class through the standard), and then found it hard to stray from that. A
strategy that should have aided her became a restrictive element not allowing her to
focus on the chords and the harmony properly. '? As no-one else mentioned this, I
assume that the rest of the students could have the tune in the background as an
was however, unable to filter it out and went on to try and explain: `I don't know, it's
just like, I think that when you play and you're trying to hear the melody,
there's more of a chance that it sounds wrong or sounds not so nice because
there's less notes - it's like in a line. ' E saw the improvised melody as a linear form
from which she had less choice of notes (and not those that might clash with the tune
itself), contrasted with the possibility of the more vertical harmonic possibilities
it
she saw as restrictive because any improvisation would have to fit in with it. E
continued: `I mean, it's just like from what you hear. If you're not like, listening
to the whole harmony you don't hear it fitting in with the harmony you just hear
W also realised that she had been singing the same interval to initiate her
improvisation every time: `I realise one thing I'm fond of doing for the past,
including this session, three sessions, whenever we are doing the Sister Sadie I
(demonstrates by singing the seventh to the tonic motif she likes). I'll start
improvising just from that spot. So somehow I feel that humming for me
personally, if I hum the melody it's like a safety cushion, ok, I'm on the right
track. It helped me actually - sorry if I put you off. (laughs)'. W viewed starting
"A similar situation occurred in a later case study with a student for whom the melodic approach to
improvisation just did not work.
234
with the same idea as a way of launching her into others; the imagery of flying is
significant with the notion of the same phrase as a `propeller' used to get her airborne.
She also again used the word safety in relation to the melody this time; she saw it as a
L had been singing and teaching Sister Sadie to her pupils (because she liked it) all
the previous week and so now knew the tune well, although admitted to not knowing
the harmony in the middle section. She was playing the violin rather than the viola
(which is her normal instrument), that particular week and said of her improvisation
on the violin: "Cause I can't work out where the notes are on this (the violin) I
don't really play it, it means you have to do it completely by ear. ' I think this
means that L was not using any of the usual patterns or signposts on the instrument
but rather responding with her aural imagination and somehow finding the intervals in
the smaller space of the violin neck. Her strategy in the middle section (which has
different dominant chords and a chromatic section), involved just responding to the
sound of the harmony: `I couldn't remember what the chords were in the middle
and I can't really play the violin so I just threw caution to the wind (laughs) to
see what happened. ' L found it liberating not to have to think either of the finger
patterns on the instrument (relating to chord tones, scales and so on) nor of the
theoretical aspects of the harmony, but just to respond to the sound slightly after the
event.
P had practised One Note Samba at home, and when I responded positively to this and
the class laughed, he said: `No, I mean I'd practised the tune a bit, last weekend: I
had a go with it and got it, know what it sounds like and tried to get round it. I
couldn't quite remember the chords but I think I could remember it roughly, it
235
wasn't too bad. ' He knew `what it sounds like', presumably both with the melody
and the harmony and so `got round it. ' Again, even at this early stage, students
realised the difference between `getting round it' i. e. playing something that will work
and which sounds alright and playing something creative or new for them.
C was one of the few students who said that improvising was not difficult for him: `I
found it easier. I think perhaps it's just in there and it probably won't leave me
for a few years. ' He seemed to be describing the sense of having internalised the
whole tune with melody and harmony. Previously he had (somewhat reluctantly)
admitted to having practised by `taking the thing apart' and learning the notes of the
chords and then the entire progression. Having internalised it, he wanted to do more
with it: `Yes, a little bit especially where this bit comes in (demonstrates the
rhythmic chords of the first section); I'm trying to do something a bit more on
that. ' He continued: `Yeh I was just trying to play around with that really. Yeh,
it's firmly written in my skull. ' This is interesting terminology; he does not talk
about feeling but about writing; something being firmly written, but in the body. I
ought to have asked whether he envisaged the piece in written notation or sensed it
through time as the solo unravels but did not. The schema would not consist of a
`firmly written' trace but rather of a stabilised pattern governed by probability which
could be adapted at will. Either way, C considered that he has learned the standard
and had the means to explore different ideas within his improvisation.
Later in the same session, freer improvisation was explored using silence and noise,
based on Peter Schaeffer's workshops, where the students are encouraged to listen to
ambient soundsaround them and to discusswhat they consider to be noise and music.
These ideas were used as a basis for collectively `composed' and improvised pieces
236
which the group (divided into three) then devised. During this session words were
also used to invoke musical ideas as a source for group improvisation. Essentially, I
unconstrained perspective; to see how the students responded and to work out whether
what they were doing was radically different to the more structured work and if so,
how and why. In all the freer sessions, there was a tendency for the students to come
up with musical cliches and to portray some of the words literally. For example the
word `train' nearly always elicited the standard Ellingtonian rhythmic model whereas
it could have been abstracted and more subtle, -and many of the contributions were
similarly like bad film music. The students' focus was also on creating a relatively
their previously desire not to fix something but to actively create ideas. It is clear that
some students have greater sensitivity to sounds than others: they can hear more and
concentrate better. Some (especially the younger ones), find experimenting with
timbres hilarious i. e. it is not seen as a serious activity to explore the effects of new
sounds and perhaps this is associated with their previous experiences of atonal music.
This is a pity because it limits the possibilities, but may be understandable given their
could be argued that the kind of schema the students appeared to have, was a limiting
and limited framework and that a lot more work, both in devising educational
approaches and in researching how they work, would have to be done in order to
understand how a schema or schema assemblage for free improvisation might work.
very difficult to monitor schema emergence and development, hence the reason for
moving to case study for the main body of the research. L had been a student in some
237
of the group improvisation classes, wished to continue and when asked, agreed to
take part in more intensive one-to-one teaching research. The next chapter describes
238
7. Main Practical Investigation
The classes took place on a one to one basis, for two hours per week, over five weeks
7.1 Aims
The aims of the first session were: to introduce the research; to explain the rationale
behind the activities; to establish an approximate starting point for the teaching and
baseline of L's existing knowledge in the aural, physical and conceptual areas of skill;
to find out what L aspired to learn in the short and long term and to create a safe and
7.2 Methodology
The case study research sessions were recorded on video with the agreement of L,
because during the pilot study it became clear that important visual elements relating
to student and teacher gestures and other interactions were necessarily absent from the
initial audio recordings. Video recording allowed the learning process to be explored
more deeply by analysing L's body movement in relation to both the music produced
and her verbal responses. Throughout the case study, I asked L for her perspective
and perception of what she was doing and thinking at frequent intervals and noted the
the teacher to be responsive to her suggestions and wishes. Analysing the video, it
became clear however, that most of the work was in fact teacher led although the
decisions made at the start about what L wanted and needed to learn (based on her
239
existing skills and understanding), were collaborative in nature. Making L aware of
what she needed to address and outlining the elements of the course to be followed
may also have assisted in preparing her for what was to follow. From L's own
feasible), over the series of lessons and a framework developed that was also
visualised on a mind map. The aim was still to track the emergence and development
of the schema and to use a similar form of activities to the group workshops, but to
focus on the process with one person and to engage in more dialogue to gain further
insights.
The initial exercises were intended to provide a starting point for the work on
improvisation and a baseline from which to attempt to follow the developing schema
over the period of five weeks. The first exercises involved assessing the extent of L's
aural memory for simple rhythmic and pitch exercises (i. e. short phrases to be clapped
or sung back) which were later elaborated to include call an answer responses and
internal rhythmic feel. L was asked first to imitate clapped rhythms to ascertain the
limits of her aural memory for rhythmic patterns, which began simply, and gradually
out to see whether L had consistent internal rhythm and responsiveness to another
player. Call and answer rhythmic phrases were used to see how L developed them and
to judge immediate and creative rhythmic response. Singing exercises were used to
extend the work on aural memory by adding pitch, to ascertain the length of phrase
and kinds of music that L could remember from a single listening and over several
repetitions. The tunes began with simple folk melodies and rounds and gradually
240
progressed to jazz and Latin tunes with offbeat and swung feel phrasing. The
intention was to begin with music that L was already familiar with in her existing
schema and then to broaden the examples to include jazz music which she had some
follow the process and strategies she used in memorisation. This activity was
important to understanding schema development because the theory posits that from
specific instances of skill (or theoretical learning) elements become generalised which
are transferable and which lead not only to adaptation but also to novel behaviour. In
addition, in this research, the learning of a melody forms the basis for improvisation
using the melodic contour as a way of developing the schema, and observing how the
tune was memorised provided insights into why and how elements of music are
learned at one particular point in time and not another. Learning the tune was
variations in tempi and rhythmic feels. Questions were asked about L's existing
improvisation i. e. those parts of the theoretical schema that will underpin the more
practical elements.
To develop the harmonic basis for improvisation and also to ascertain L's practical
knowledge of the subject, the creation of motifs was explored to see whether L could
make use of the chord tones within the harmonic framework and take the ideas
241
more scalic and linear approach affected L's improvisational ideas. For contrast, later
in the session, expressive parameters in improvisation were also explored to gauge the
effect on the improvised lines and to explore whether this approach would be useful.
Lastly, questions were asked to see what L aspired to learn over the five weeks and a
practice schedule was devised by L herself on the basis of the afternoon's activities.
In the following description, the letters and numbers refer to specific parts of the
transcription which are printed in full in the appendix. All of the above exercises were
developed within the previous group workshop settings, but were somewhat
condensed in this first session of the case study, both because L had worked in this
way before and in order to set some kind of starting point for the research into the
7.4 Discussion and Findings from the first session relating to melody
In the initial exercises to find a base line of skill, L's aural memory was challenged by
complex rhythms and melodies although it was highly attuned and focused on shorter
and more standard `on the beat' music. It was apparent and became more so as the
session progressed, that L's schema for listening and hearing jazz was
underdeveloped. It also became clear that L made up parts of the rhythm (and
melody) where she had forgotten or not heard the phrase correctly, although on the
occasions when she did this, both the rhythmic and melodic aspects had contours and
echoes of the original music. It was evident that L had not heard enough or
assimilated the sound and feel of jazz at this particular point in time, due to the lack of
jazz rhythmic and melodic sense and also on her own admission. She did not have the
sound of the music in her head and it did not feel natural to her musical sensibility.
R19: `When I first started playing around with this (jazz music), I felt that I
242
that I couldn't hear it because I wasn't used to it. ' (L's emphasis.) This proved to
be a problem, for without a background senseof the flow and sound of the music, it
was difficult for L to emulate the jazz feel and indicates the importance of the
background listening schema which has to evolve before any meaningful jazz
Similarly with melody: simpler folk type tunes were well remembered and sung back
by L but jazzier motifs and tunes were more problematic. Listening carefully to the
recording, it appeared that L's memory altered some part of the melody on one
occasion which was recreated in the next version and then it seemed to be difficult for
her to alter to the correct form later. What is entirely unclear is what happens on one
factor in terms of memory was how, when L had listened with high concentration to a
two bar phrase in order to memorise it, and then could not reproduce the aural
memory either on the voice or the instrument, she approximated it (in a similar way to
the rhythmic exercises), using the overall contour. She then either remembered the
from successive listenings to the same phrase. This is illustrated in more detail at S18
the transcript in volume 2. During a later dialogue about what L was thinking of
during the singing exercises she said: S35 L: `Whereas at the moment, when I'm
remembering, (I was aware of doing it then actually), I'm looking... I'm looking
for repetition and I'm looking for big intervals and remembering where they
are; and I'm not thinking of the phrasing or the overall shape of it at all, so yes,
we need to do some work on that. ' (L's emphasis.) The impression is that the
which actually interfere with the active process of concentrated listening. L seemed to
243
be consciously listening for the unusual details rather than the global structure of a
phrase, although what she actually remembered (perhaps non-consciously and through
the schema), was the contour or the overall outline. For L, it was almost as if highly
rational and conceptual elements came into play in what is an aural memory to hand
shapes/movements and spaces, executed on the instrument. During the first session, a
repeating motif was L's rational approach to music (she is also a mathematician), in
For example, she invariably remembered the highest notes of a phrase or melody in
What does this indicate about schema formation? Perhaps, that general outlines or
contours form the basis of aural memory from which specific instances are filled in
and as they are memorised, the relationships can be abstracted. Alternatively, the
particular learning strategy. It is also described by Sudnow (1976) who proposes that
the skill of improvisation moves from the vague and generalised to the more specific.
The indication from these initial exercises was that L's aural memory was highly
developed and attuned to the music that she was familiar with i. e. classical, folk and
Kletzmer. With the background listening schema for jazz dormant (L had listened to
jazz as a young person), it was hard for L to memorise or respond to jazz phrases or
typical swing rhythms. Within the new jazz idiom that incorporated unusual rhythms
and blues related patterns, L could not recall phrases over two bars in length.
However, (with hindsight and later information), it became clear that integral to L's
244
was entirely absent from the initial exercises. L may have been able .to deduce the
harmony from the more traditional songs but not with the jazz phrases,hence her
difficulty in accurately copying those particular melodies. I was not aware of this at
L offered to learn the Jerome Kern tune All the Things You Are on camera so that I
could observe the process and this exercise is described fully in volume 2. In this
particular activity, I was looking for any evidence for the self organising nature of the
schema: how the memorisation process appeared to be working and whether the
specific intervals learned within the tune were stable enough to be adapted in a
process without comment or any verbal input and L learned the tune without any
version of the harmony as she learned the tune and then later played the melody with
the notated chords at the piano. After playing the tune and the chords twice more on
the piano, I asked L whether the written harmony was as she had imagined it to be.
L28 L: `Most of it is how I thought: some of it isn't or it's not how I would
harmonise it if I was.... from what I'd... It's not how I'd harmonised it in. my
head from what I was playing. Er, some of that I put down... well it's been
harmonised in quite a complicated way and I think somebody's just been playing
with it for the fun of putting some extra harmonies in. K: `Right. ' L: But yeah,
This interesting response may indicate that L had some kind of harmonising schema
which did not yet fit the jazz idiom and it would be intriguing to know how she would
245
have harmonised it. The other possibility is that L had heard this tune somewhere
before and recognised the chord changesincompletely from a past memory of it. L
reiterated: L29 L: `It's not how I'd harmonise it in my head from what I was
playing. ' Perhaps L imagined the tune harmonised in a folk-based way: it seems
unlikely that she would have heard the intricate and sophisticated key changes unless
Having learned the tune of All the Things You Are by heart, I then asked L to perform
it in different ways, to see how easily she could adapt it by ear, and therefore to
ascertain how the schema was developing for the tune i. e. whether the learned
relationships between the intervals could be abstracted and adapted to a new context.
I began by demonstrating the jazz swing feel on the guitar and adding off beat
rhythms in the accompaniment. E12 At the third time of playing the tune by heart,
with the guitar accompaniment, L came in with a `straight' swing feel, altering the
rhythmic emphasis of the notes. It is interesting to note that L changed the feel of the
tune immediately in response to the energy and rhythm of the accompaniment: it was
not entirely jazzy but very different from her previous renditions and quite unlike the
notated form. L also added slides and slurs as well as shortening some notes and
lengthening others. Overall, L played with verve and confidence incorporating the
accompaniment. This would seem to indicate that the schema for this tune had
developed sufficiently for her to adapt it and to add small changes by being given
hints from and imitating the aural musical context. L's learning of the melody by that
stage also appeared to by automatic enough for her to add different articulations,
indicating that schema development was already enhancing the adaptable nature of
246
The same exercise was repeated in 3/4time, which required a very. different rhythmic
approach, a diminution of the notes and a general lightening of the phrasing. I counted
the tune in at a slower tempo, having realised that the pace was accelerating and then
played an example of the jazz waltz rhythm whilst singing the tune as a model for L.
E18 This was the fifth time with the guitar accompaniment and first time in 3/4and
now that L was concentrating on altering the rhythm, the intonation became less
focused. L came in with the 3/4rhythm, added some notes and slides and it felt more
jig-like than jazz waltz, in keeping with her background in the folk idiom. L played a
few extraneous strings but completely altered the learned tune both rhythmically and
melodically to fit with the new % accompaniment. This kind of immediate adaptation
of the tune that can be remoulded to fit a different time feel and L could do this with a
indication that a trained musician is a natural improviser and that the time taken to
learn the skill of improvisation in the jazz idiom, is involved with technical
parameters, creating ideas and cohesion within the style, not learning the act of
improvising itself. The process of immediate adaptation of imagining the new form of
the tune in relation to the altered accompaniment, whilst keeping similar contours and
outlines is strong evidence for the existence of the schema in that the relationships
between the intervals had already been abstracted and could be altered rhythmically at
will. In this case, the tune was compressed, lightened and moving more quickly
through the harmony and it is also noteworthy that this kind of adaptation can occur
After a 70 minute break L played the All the Things You Are by heart again R2 to
ascertain how it
much of she had remembered because she was very sceptical that she
247
could memorise the tune so quickly. This area relates to whether the schema is
sessions separated by time in order to complete memorisation. L's view from her own
past experience was that it would take several days to memorise the tune thoroughly.
With a couple of prompts from me, she had fully remembered it by the third time
through, but in a later version, with some altered rhythms, rather than the original
notated one that she first learned. The process appears to be that when the piece is
recreated from memory, it is not reproduced exactly: subtle changes are made based
incorporated into the next version. The piece, in this instance, was therefore in a
dynamic state, not fixed but being reworked or altered with each performance. For L
this seemed like a non-conscious process i. e. either she did not notice that the melody
was different at all or only when there was a great incongruence between the actual
In this section, L's knowledge of jazz harmony was sought, again for the background
schema, and the improvisation exercises were extended to include the functional
harmony of the tune in the first few bars of All the Things You Are, with a view to
and how they related to the first eight bars of All The Things You Are: VI, II, V, I, IV
(Dmin7, G7, C Major). As I played these chords, L played long notes on the roots,
thirds and fifths over them to memorise both the chord progression and the elements
of the chords. The intention was to provide another framework for the emerging
schema and more scope for the improvisation strategies. L then created one bar long
248
motifs to play across the chords with varying success by focussing on the theoretical
This vertical harmony exercise was followed by a more linear task using the scales of
Ab and C major and the aim was to find how the scalic approach affected L's
improvisational ideas. The overall effect of L's scalic improvisation, at this early
stage was a wandering, unfocused line which without the constraints of the chord
tones, bore only a vague relation to the harmony. L was however, already anticipating
the key changes well and quickly able to improvise through the changing keys. I
suggested that she approach the linear model from a strong chord tone to orientate
herself better within the progression, which she subsequently did to good effect KS.
This enabled L to play tones from the underlying chord root movements which made
more musical sense. On the eighth repeat of the first eight bars of the progression K58
L improvised a truly interesting and beautiful line which she continued to develop
over the whole sequence. This seemed to be an example of the creation of a novel
idea within the schema process and was quite unlike the improvised ideas played
preceding and after it, implying that the schema is self organising and outside of
conscious control. Even at such an early stage of skill development, the schema
appears to enable the execution of novel ideas if only for short periods of time.
Towards the end of the first session I introduced the idea of exploring more
timbre in conjunction with the previous work on harmony and melody. The aims were
schema and to find out whether focusing on expressive aspects altered the way the
249
beginner approached improvisation. Was there a qualitative difference between
melodic and harmonic improvisation and that based on a mood, feeling or particular
timbral quality? The aim within the exercise was still to provide some form of
constraint but to keep the theoretical and technical aspects to a minimum and to
explore more intuitive and imaginative areas. I suggested to La number of words that
evoked a different mood and subsequently she improvised over the chord progression
of All the Things You Are having decided to play it in turn sadly, madly, gently, in a
dancing mood and angrily. The effect of this more deliberately expressive approach
was to alter L's playing dramatically. The `sad' version produced a change of pace
and phrasing in which L seemed more in control of the music, which had greater
melodic interest. She simplified and slowed down many phrases and in the process
created more effective lines and stronger ideas. In the `mad' version, L initially
sounded as if she were playing dissonant circus music, using call and answer phrases
and chromatic lines. This was the first time that L had deliberately used dissonance in
her improvisation, and she added many string and bowing effects for example, slides,
swoops, fast chromatic passages, and double stops. The music had tremendous energy
and really came to life, indicating just how powerful the use of a word or emotion can
From this initial brief work on using expressive qualities to stimulate improvisation, it
is clear that more work should be done to explore why and how this approach works.
In this specific case, the music produced was so completely different from that based
on the melody and harmony of the tune that again, it appears that some aspects of
improvisation are as much about unlocking and bringing out what is already there, as
extension of the one already developing in melodic and harmonic ways, is not clear.
250
In a later lesson L used a particularly poignant image to inform her improvisation
In summary then, by the end of the first session it was clear that L was already using
chord symbols explained in a previous workshop and could play the piano using them
to understand the sound and structure of the harmony and melody better i. e. she was
already involved in the process of developing the theoretical and practical elements of
the harmony and specifically, using the clear visual patterns on the piano to help her
understand them. Looking at the chords, L was seeking structural, conceptual and
aural connections and working out how the melody fitted with the harmony. L could
not imagine a tune without a contextual harmonic background and in All the Things
You Are, at the start, when harmony was provided, she imagined her own from the
symbols, which did not match Kern's in places. The fingering and embodiment of the
and physical space on the instrument. During this first session, L was thinking in
terms of unusual intervals and fingering patterns, whilst simultaneously making the
sound, rather than visualising the notation. In general, L took a problem solving
aural and technical elements to decide which note or series of notes to play next.
Constraints (of melody and harmony) provided a framework for L that was useful,
and exploring expressive parameters was both creative and fruitful for her, as she
played much more musically and with melodic focus using these. In terms of schema
development, whilst remaining speculative, L's ability to play the tune of All the
Things You Are immediately in a 3/4 jazz waltz feel, having learned it in a
straightforward 4/4 rhythm, does imply a schema framework which has abstracted the
relationships between intervals and can now adapt them to a new context unrehearsed.
251
The background jazz music schema comprising of jazz heard and any previous
understanding of it, had enabled L to understand the chord symbols and harmony, and
the already learned motor schemata for playing the viola, was obviously being
enhanced with more emphasis on aural parameters and triggers. The sound and feel of
jazz music was however clearly not yet in her aural imagination and so could yet
7.7 Session 2
7.7.1 Aims
The aims of the second session were ascertain L's progress during the previous week
and to explore her perception of her own developing improvising skill. A second aim
was to continue work on All the Things You Are, to try to see how her musical ideas
were developing (with a view to seeking evidence for the growth of the schema
assemblage) and to extend them using a variety of different strategies and approaches.
One of the predictions of schema theory is that the specific parameters of the skill
have to be thoroughly learned before variable practice is introduced, hence the focus
on one standard and it referential framework. The work was to include the following:
the swapping of four bar phrases, requiring more interactive and quicker response;
transposing the tune by ear, again to work out the stability of the schema; improvising
on the transposed chords for a similar purpose; introducing a new, simpler tune (St.
Thomas) and learning a tune aurally to strengthen the primary aural to motor
connections proposed by schema theory, to increase repertoire and vary the style of
252
The second part of the lesson consisted of listening to a Maria Schneider Jazz
Orchestra piece (Allegresse) to develop aural abilities and to introduce L to some new
music but also to ascertain broader aural memory skills and to begin to enhance the
listening schema. In the final part of the lesson Monk's I Mean You was learned
aurally, to extend listening skills, incorporate more jazzy rhythms and develop
repertoire.
7.7.2 Methodology
The lesson was organised in a similar way to session 1 in that a range of activities
possible to include new pieces, techniques and approaches. The main aim, as
previously stated, was to help L to develop her improvisational skills in a holistic and
L began by describing her work and thoughts about it during the previous week. L
was becoming aware of the gap in her listening knowledge of jazz (highlighted in the
previous session) and was taking responsibility for her own learning by seeking
different players to listen to. She was also thinking about the kind of style and sound
that she wanted to imitate. L had found that she was unable to improvise on her own,
(mainly due to lack of accompaniment), in the same way as she had in the lesson and
felt at this juncture that the process was just too difficult for her: R2 L: `And I felt
this is just too hard-I just can't do it. So.... but I didn't give up; I thought I
would broaden the range of what I was listening to see if there were any clues to
253
be found there and I also thought I'd tackle technical things because that seemed
like something positive that I could do. ' L seemed to understand that she had to be
able to imagine more musical ideas within the genre, in order to be able to improvise
with more fluency and individuality. She listened to Jean-Luc Ponty a jazz violinist:
R5 `So, I listened to that and I thought, you know, there's lots of options for what
sort of sound to make: there's the following the voice option; then there's the
following the tradition of jazz violin playing or there's trying to make something
else out of nowhere, which is a bit, I suppose, what the viola is.. ' L was beginning
to be aware of and to explore the options for sound and approaches through wider
L had focussed her practice on technical aspects and significantly, had simplified the
complex position fingering on the neck of the viola by imagining it to be the neck of
the guitar. By moving her whole hand up and down the neck, to change keys and
chords, she was able to keep the same fingering which allowed her to focus on the
sound of the notes. Describing the process L said: R7 L: `I thought another way of
tackling the chords is to treat the fiddle neck like a guitar and instead of trying
to figure out the chords wherever they might be, to use a similar fingering for all
of them and shift up and down more. ' R8 L: `Which is loads easier because
you're using the same finger on the same part of the chord and all you have to
remember is whether it's major, minor or seventh. ' In other words, this changed
perspective gave a consistency to the fingering patterns and simplified the complex
technical problems in that area that may have been inhibiting L's improvisational
ideas. This strategy which used a visual pattern approach on the fingerboard is
interesting and perhaps stems from watching what I was doing on the guitar. L had
imposed imaginary frets on the viola and so instead of changing positions (with
254
altered fingerings and spacing); she simply moved the pattern back and forth as one
would on the guitar. This appears to indicate the formation or evolution of the schema
for technical parameters i. e. finding an individual way to internalise the physical and
conceptual aspects of the chord tones (and scale notes) using a logical and consistent
fingering pattern. Whether this simplified approach to fingering was carried over in
the folk and classical music that L also played, is not known but there seemed not to
be a conflict between the standard fingering procedure that she had used over many
In the second week L also realised that jazz improvisation is essentially a communal
art and that playing with others is crucial to and enhances both the learning and
performing process, so she had been practising with a bass player. She had also
played the violin for ease and contrast and found that the physical differences between
the viola and violin had altered her approach to improvisation, specifically because of
the higher register in the violin and the comparative smallness of the body and neck of
the instrument.
During the teaching/facilitating activities, All the Things You Are was revisited, with
L improvising in a more confident and controlled manner and more cohesively over
longer periods. Overall, L's phrasing was over a longer time-frame i. e. eight or
sixteen bars rather than two to four bars and had more cohesion melodically. When I
asked L how she felt about the improvising she said: A2 L: `Bits of it were nice',
implying that she was not really aware of the qualitative change in her playing. The
beginner improviser compares their current performance to the times when the solo
really came together briefly and they are not aware of the general qualitative
255
phrases played by me, were also developing well and because this requires a quick
answering response, indicated that higher level motor/aural skills were at work. This
simultaneously. It means that L was able to move into higher levels of skill albeit
briefly because her concentration was not yet sufficiently developed to consistently
integrate all the necessary elements. She also accompanied me by playing bass lines
and chord tones much more rhythmically and was finding her way through the
changes well.
More work was done on transposing All the Things You Are into a variety of keys to
assess the stability of the schema and the technical fingering aspects. L was asked
what she was thinking of during the transposition: Ti L: L: `The intervals. It sort
of, it goes off the map where (thinks and looks tip) it's a bit of the tune where it
goes into an easy key and now by shifting a semi-tone it goes into difficult keys.
So I suppose that bit felt like an anchor before. and now feels like.... blimey. '
Interestingly, L uses a known territory metaphor of the map and going off it,
presumably into uncharted areas. She is also aware that the key changes to a more
difficult one and perhaps that too holds her back in the middle section. Similarly, she
saw the middle eight of the original key as easier than the surrounding harmony and
therefore like a safe `anchor' point within a moving sea of changing chords. I
suggested to L not to think of the chords or notes conceptually but rather the
relationships between them. If the relationships between the original intervals had
been thoroughly abstracted, schema theory predicts that they can be adapted into a
new context (in this case a key). However, there may still be technical fingering
256
problems even with a simplified system because the neck of the viola is not long
enough to encompass every key as the guitar is, (hence the variable fingering patterns
To extend the exercise, L then improvised in the transposed key and came adrift in the
second section which she then related back to the framework of the original melody,
although now in a new key. In the middle section, she simplified the line somewhat
but in the correct keys and then again related the line back to the tune in the last
section, wandering slightly out of the key at the end. This indicates, however, a very
high level skill i. e. of improvising by ear whilst transposing what is already a complex
tune. L does supremely well at even attempting this at such an early stage. In terms of
the schema, it appears that it was developing for the harmony as well as the melody
because L was able to abstract the relevant information to improvise in the transposed
key, albeit whilst returning to the tune at times. This appears to be a large amount of
information to be able to extrapolate and adapt from one tune. I asked L what she felt
about her improvising at this point and she accurately assessedwhere she was: T4 L:
`There are places where it really goes and other places whereI completely lose it.
But when I lose it, it's like there are places I can catch up with it, just about. '
Again, there is a sense of the bigger picture forming and the overall knowledge of the
standard providing a framework for finding a way through it over time. L was also
aware, as before, of when the improvisation was working and when it was not,
although she was still not quite in control of the process. When the improvisation
works in the early stages, it seems almost arbitrary from the student's perspective and
outside the conscious will of the beginner improviser. This is perhaps indicative of the
257
directing the fingers and establishing neural links between hands (or other parts of the
After several successive choruses of All the Things You Are in the transposed key, we
returned to the original key of Ab and I asked L how she felt her improvisation went:
T5 L: `A big feeling of relief. ' `My fingers knew where to go in that one. ' This
response seems to endorse the view that letting the fingers `go' (out of conscious
control), and the schema organise the movement is a key element in successful
improvisation and we perhaps inhibit this process with the will, by consciously
thinking about it, or with too much theoretical information. Just relaxing or feeling
relieved seems to unlock more improvisational ideas and when this happens, the
player, counter intuitively, sounds more in control and as if they have more conscious
input into the process, which is the opposite of what appears to be taking place. The
notion of the fingers knowing where to go is also telling, like a glimpse of the
physicality of the learning process where the fingers move in space to find the
imagined sounds. The motor schemata are essential to this process and much of the
practice involves this spatial awareness guided by changing aural imagination and
solos learned the intervals on the instrument by memorising a fixed recorded solo in
small chunks and gradually improved speed, technique and aural ability to play the
whole piece along with the soloist. They were copying the musical ideas instead of
creating them and by this means were also able to absorb and assimilate the jazz style
of the period. By the end of this process or many similar exercises, the player could
feel for and find a whole range of jazz inflected intervals and phrases by ear and
258
having developed that facility, and having the sound of the music embedded in their
aural memory, they went on to develop their own ideas and move away from the close
the beginning in the more academic educational process, but seems more natural and
everything played is individual, if sometimes a bit mundane. The main problem for
a short (16 bar) calypso with a simple chord progression. After the harmonic
complexity of All the Things You Are, L found St Thomas relatively easy to improvise
on and to transpose quickly. As in previous activities, she also used the tune for
extemporisation and the rhythmic phrasing and stop sections within it. Gaining in
confidence over the next few choruses, L began to anticipate and sometimes got ahead
of the beat. This may be an indication that as the schema develops, with knowledge of
the repertoire; it leads to greater predictability and anticipation from the improviser. It
seems that here, the anticipation and the willingness to play something appropriate to
the changing chord, ignored the listening feedback where L should have heard the
mistake. For example, there were clashes between the anticipated and actual tonalities
Throughout the lessons, I had emphasised the need to be able to sing both the
melodies and the improvisations before transferring them to the instrument. The effect
of the singing exercises (which seem to encourage more coherent melodic lines in a
better jazz feel), appeared only to apply to the instrumental improvisation for a short
time, i. e. the stronger and more relaxed sung improvisation were carried over into the
next instrumental chorus, but not beyond. L, like most of the participants in the
259
research was able especially at the start to sing better and more coherent improvised
lines than she was able to play. Whether this indicates an innate improvising schema
for the voice, based on tunes learned or perhaps linked through vocal musculature to
the improvisation that takes place in speech, is unclear. Later in the improvisational
process, L felt that she was better able to improvise on the instrument instead of the
voice simply because of practice and the number of different frameworks that she
used, (including conceptual and fingering), to scaffold the process. With regard to the
listening schema, L was by this time, beginning to hear variations to the notated
During the second part of the lesson we listened to Allegresse by Maria Schneider, (a
intricate, thirteen minute long composition for a large jazz orchestra), with a view to
improvisation wise, timbrally and so on, and also to gauge her aural memory. Overall,
the listening session (which was the first concentrated work done in this area),
indicated that L needed to continue to listen to a wider range and type of jazz music in
order to understand the idiom more thoroughly and assist with her improvisation.
A third tune was played in this session: I Mean You by Thelonius Monk, which
features offbeat and over the bar phrases in a be-bop style unlike any of the tunes
previously played. During the subsequent learning of the harmony, L recognised the
II, V, I pattern cropping up again and this recognition may indicate the developing
schema for harmony and an understanding that this pattern is one of the recurring
elements in the jazz standard and therefore transferable to similar situations. L said of
the piece: L10 L: `This one's got a complicated tune and easy chords. ' This was
260
L's perception of the tune but in fact, it is relatively simple with many repeating
motifs and a call and answer form. It is the placement of the rhythm that is more
difficult and the strength of the off beat rhythmic feel. L's classical and folk schemata
appeared not to be attuned to this yet. When the improvisation began, it had the
hallmarks of a folk tune, particularly because of the movement of the first two
dominant chords (F7-Db7) and the `hoe-down' feel of the soloing was difficult to
alter perhaps because the progression reminded L of something she already knew and
From this second session it was apparent (from descriptions by L of her activities
during the preceding week), that L was listening to a range of jazz violinists and
thinking about the kind of sound and type of improvising she would like to be doing.
Technically, she had simplified the complex position fingering on the neck of the
viola by imagining it as the neck of the guitar (with frets) and moving her whole hand
up and down, to change keys and chords. This process had helped her enormously
because it condensed the fingering problems, allowing her to concentrate more on the
sound and melodic elements. The schema here appears to encompass an efficient
short cut to improving fingering capabilities. At this point also, L realised that jazz
improvisation is essentially a communal art and that playing with others enhances
Overall throughout the lesson, L was improvising in a more confident and controlled
manner and more cohesively over longer periods. This was especially true of fours
exchanged with me, which were developing well, with swift and appropriate
responses and much imitation, indicating higher skills at work and the development of
261
the schema allowing aural and motor elements to be created over a longer time frame.
Significantly, as well as transposing the tune of All the Things You Are, L was able to
improvise in the aurally transposed key for some of the time, indicating high levels of
adaptation within the schema, especially with regard to the relationships between
intervals in the new key. However, in playing a new and much simpler tune, St
Thomas, L found it almost too easy to learn and transpose and in anticipating the
various parts of the tune and chords, tended to get ahead of the beat. The schema here
appeared to be predicting too strongly and overriding the actual aural elements as they
unfolded.
Although the singing exercises worked well for learning tunes by heart and for
improvisation, the positive effect of the singing (which appears to encourage more
coherent melodic lines in a better jazz feel), only lasted for a chorus or two, once
transferred to the instrument. The listening session (which was the first concentrated
work done in this area), indicated that L needed to continue to listen to a wider range
and type of jazz music in order to understand the idiom more thoroughly and assist
with her improvisation. During the learning of the final tune, which L found difficult
to play by ear because of the irregular placement of the riff, she recognised the II, V, I
chord progression cropping up again (even though I had yet to explain the prevalence
of this type of progression), and this recognition is an indication that the developing
is
schema recognising harmonic in
patterns new situations.
262
7.8 Session 3
This session was halfway through the case study research, and I asked L to fill in an
centred on how L thought her improvisation was developing; whether the initial goals
should be altered; what she had learned that week during practice; what the focus
should be for the next three weeks; what she had listened to; whether she was singing;
what she enjoyed most about improvising and which approaches were most fruitful in
the lessons.
Generally L felt that the improvisation was `coming together quite nicely', Q1 and
that she would keep her original goals (of being able to improvise competently over
any standard and to incorporate improvisation into other areas of her playing), evens
though she now realised they were probably too ambitious within the given time
frame. During the preceding week, L had taught the tune of I Mean You to members
of her family aurally, and this had enabled her to really assimilate it properly and to
understand the importance of learning it that way (rather than by reading notation).
Indeed, she had learned it herself not by using the notated copy, but by recording the
chords on the piano, with a metronome and singing the tune over them, which
represents a new strategy for her. She had also focused on the sound of the harmony
before seeing it in the conceptual rational form of notated symbols. Overall, L felt she
could see how elements of the solo fitted together over a longer time period so that
her solos were not just reacting to the next chord but to the flow of the progression.
Both singing and listening had become more important to her and central to the
263
When I asked L why she had not used the notated music (which had previously been
her preferred method of learning) she said: Q13 L: `Oh, I had a look at the music
and thought it was completely impossible (looks up, thinks) cm, to work it out off
that. ' This seems quite an odd response from a reading player. Perhaps because we
had focused so much on a purely aural approach in the lesson, the reading became
what it actually is in reality; a form of code and a very different process altogether. L
stated that during the translation of the sung line of I Mean You onto the viola, she
became aware of the `thinking note' that she had added during the lesson in order to
make the pentatonic phrase more logical to her Q14. The adding of the note to make
the phrase what L wanted it to be, rather that what it actually was, links back to the
first session where she was adding notes to All the Things You Are on one particular
occasion of recreated memory which then became part of the next version. In the case
of I Mean You, it is perhaps evidence of the previous schema for a pentatonic phrase
overriding the actuality of auditory feedback and imposing itself on memory. It was
only when L was accompanying herself and singing the melody with the added note
(which made the tune out of synch with the chords), that she realised her mistake.
L had continued to guide her own learning by widening her listening to include
pianist/composer Thelonius Monk and the trombonist Annie Whitehead. She also
made her own decisions about the content and style of her practice sessions. L felt that
she needed to work on rhythm more for the next three weeks and as previously seen in
the pilot studies, stated that what she most enjoyed about improvising was `When I
do something I don't expect (laughs). ' Q6. Like the players in the earlier
workshops, the implication is that it is the creative and more intangible aspects that
drive musicians to become improvisers and that the perception of the player is that at
least part of the process is accepted to be non-conscious and not fully controllable.
264
From the perspective of the developing schema, under optimum circumstances, the
schema organises the various aural, conceptual and motor parameters to present
something unexpected and almost outside of the conscious control of the improviser.
This feeling from improvisers, that ideas come out of nowhere, is elaborated by L: Q7
L: `Yeah, something really nice sounding comes out that you didn't plan or when
you get an idea, a sort of technical idea for something and you think you'll give it
a go but it sounds quite different from what you thought it was going to (shrugs)
L decided that singing was a most useful element in the lessons and described the
connection between the voice and the instrument: Q11 K: `Yeah, I think what
happens is your musical imagination develops and it's a bit like, well it's not
quite the same because we're not talking about meaning, but it's a bit like when
you understand in a foreign language, long before you can actually speak or
construct a sentence, you can actually attune yourself and hear; you can more or
less understand what people are saying to you. It's that kind of process. ' Q12 L:
`It's when you stop again listening to the individual words and you start listening
to the sense of the whole thing together, isn't it, because you don't understand all
the words. ' This seems to be the feeling of the schema developing; the gestalt feeling
of understanding the whole, which might disintegrate again if you begin to analyse or
7.8.2 Activities
During this session, work was focused on developing the improvisation for I Mean
You with the intention of using the more be-bop orientated energy of the tune to take
the improvisation into a new direction. I also asked L to close her eyes to see whether
265
focusing purely on the sound (rather than the visual patterns on the fingerboard),
made any difference to the quality of her improvised ideas. L used the last riff of the
tune (I Mean You) to begin her improvisation and was slightly insecure at the start
with the themes wandering more and moving in a stepwise scalic motion, but at the
end of the second eight she played a wide and high interval. Throughout the
continuing solo, it appeared as if she was exploring wider, different intervals and
trying out new ideas. L also used the pentatonic intervals from the tune and more
examples of crotchet triplets taken from the tune, especially in the middle section. In
the last section L imitated the tune and played a faster triplet quaver run before
landing on a distinct colour tone of the seventh which she played for a while.
distractions or lack of concentration, but L was already confident enough to take risks
by experimenting with bigger intervals and sounds and trying out different chord
tones and rhythmic elements. The question of whether there was any difference
musically when she closed her eyes is difficult to answer, but she appeared to take
more chances without the visual modality keeping her hand correctly in place on the
fingerboard. Without her eyes, she seemed to concentrate purely on aural to hand
movements and imagine a more varied set of sounds. There appeared to be a better
However, L's perception of what she had felt and played with her eyes closed was
very striking. Q27 L: `What happened was, (L kept looking outwards and forwards)
it focuses you much more on the sound and takes you away from being who you
are standing here and all of a sudden the sound sounded like somebody else
266
K: `What do you mean it sounded like somebody else?'
K: `It always sounds good; I've been listening to the videos, it always sounds
Q29 L: (laughing) `Yeah, (nods no) but you know what I mean, it doesn't usually
Q30 L: `I don't know. Anyway, (shrugs) it removed it from the sort of here and
now and it also meant that (stops, looks left) yes there was a picture in my head of
the relationships between the notes, I can't explain it really (shakes her head no). '
Q31 L: `No, it was nothing like a sheet of music; it was like a three dimensional
or more than three dimensional thing with some sort of strings stuck in the
middle. '
K: `Right, so it's like the relationship between notes or chord tones? '
Q33 L: (thinks-looks ahead and up to the left) `I think between notes but it's
alright because the background and the harmony is there now, I can think about
the notes, you know what I mean? I can think of the notes in relation to
L was concerned that having experienced this kind of expansive feeling she would not
be able to find it again Q35 L: `Yeah (laughs) - it won't be anywhere near as good
267
Q36 L: `That sometimes that sort of thing happens and you can't capture it. '
I explained that for me, the whole point of improvisation was not to capture anything
but to train yourself to be in a psychological state where you were open to a wide
range of improvising possibilities. L then described how in the past, she had
`captured it' and fixed the melody so that it was then the same each time. Later, L
said that closing her eyes had made her want to play more gently: Q42 L: `Yeah not
gent... that's the wrong word erm, (thinks) I suppose it made me pay more
attention to the sound that was coming out, to the shaping of each individual
Why should closing her eyes have such a profound effect of L's perception of her
improvising skill? Perhaps, with her eyes open, she was focusing on playing `the
correct' notes based on visual patterns on the viola fingerboard and removing the
visual modality forced her to actually listen and to respond to ideas aurally and
disconnected her self-consciousness so that she heard herself as if. she were listening
to someone else, perhaps in a more objective way. The visual modality tends to
ground the individual in the present situation because their identity is bound up within
a visual context in which they are present in mind and body and closing the eyes
lessens that. When L listened with her eyes closed, what she heard sounded good; she
said: Q28 `I was amazed. ' I tried to reassure her that nothing had changed from my
perspective; she always sounded good. So here we find out, not unexpectedly, that L
does not generally like what she plays. Does this negativity inhibit the learning
process? Does it help to enjoy and like what we are playing or is this just a by
product? Many of the great jazz improvisers had extraordinarily high standards and
268
expectations of their playing that were entirely different from those of their listeners
and audience. Trumpeter Dizzy Gillespie for example, stated in an interview towards
the end of his life, that he could recall having played what he considered to be a good
improvised solo, only once. L could not or did not wish to say why her improvisation
did not sound good to her, but that closing her eyes Q30 `removed it from the sort of
here and now and that yes, there was a picture in my head of the relationships
As to what `it' was, L describes the image as a three dimensional or more `thing' Q31
with an abstraction of the viola strings in the middle of it. We might imagine this as a
visual image of the schema but there is no reason to suppose that a schema can be
imagined in any visual form and is more likely to be L's imaginative response to my
ideas and attempts to encourage her to verbalise processes that may not be truly
interesting because that is how Schmidt (1975) envisages the schema working and
movements or elements are memorised, not their actuality. I had not mentioned this
aspect at all to L and so this is more objective and indicates that Schmidt's theory may
have a humanly definable, perceivable form, which can indeed be visualised and
imagined. L sees the relationship between the notes overlaid against a background of
harmonic knowledge that she has gained over the past weeks. The final interesting
L
aspect mentions is that closing her eyes made her pay more attention to the sound
and although this is obvious, beginner improvisers concentrate so hard on `getting the
notes right' that sound and timbre are virtually forgotten and yet are of paramount
importance. Focusing on the shaping and quality of each individual note seems a good
exercise in itself.
269
L began to improvise over I Mean You a second time, saying Q43 L: 'I'll just try
starting somewhere else.' She played again with her eyes closed and began in a
strikingly different manner, unfettered from the previously played arpeggio figures
and indeed from slavish adherence to the chord tones altogether. L used a much
simpler approach overall and appeared more controlled; she used the triplets but in a
quite unformulaic way and seemed to be following a tune, and not imitating riffs or
phrases
I asked L what it felt like on that occasion to play with her eyes closed: Q44 L: `Not
as stunning as last time obviously `cause it's not so much of a shock and I was
like thinking that time rather than focus on the fifth of the chord I'd focus on the
the same way. ' In this instance L had added conceptual information about chord
tones and so perhaps the feeling of pure sound and form was not as striking. She was
also expecting the change of focus from visual to aural, because of her previous
experience.
Later in the session L remarked about the experience again; it had clearly made a
great impression. Q51 L: `That shutting your eyes was just stunning and the most
staggering thing about it was just, as I say, I suddenly heard myself like I was
listening to somebody else and it sounded like somebody who knew what they
were doing and it was like... (Makes a wide eyed and shocked facial expression to
Q53 L: `Yeh (smiles) I was really surprised `cause when I shut my eyes there was
this enormous space (makes an open round gesture of the arm and hand) inside my
270
head and I was really amazed that there was all that space there and there was
all the time and space in the world just to put it together and make it happen.
Then when I shut my eyes a second time and I had spent a lot of time playing
around the fifths the first time and I'd not really meant to but that was how it
came out. The next time I thought I'd play around in the thirds and then the
space in my head had shrunk back to being sort of viola neck size with a few
K: `Yes, so what we should try and work on is creating the space again, so that
you feel like you've got the musical space to play what you want. '
L: (smiling) `What an amazing feeling to have that huge great thing just in
This expansion and shrinking of space in the imagination as a direct result of shutting
off the visual modality (by closing the eyes), is very striking, altering L's perception
of how much time and space she had to create her improvised solos.
In the second half of the lesson, we returned. to All the Things You Are and L found it
easier to play rather than sing an improvisation. She herself felt that this was because
the fingering `anchors you down' AT1 and provides another framework for memory
and orientation through the tune. Thus it was at this point easier for L to play rather
than sing because she had the fingering as well as the aural and conceptual
More work was done on the expressive parameters for an improvised solo, and as on
completely changed her whole approach AT5. Overall, her improvisation was more
271
adventurous (some ideas worked, some not) and experimental. L moved away from
basic chord tones to colour tones and non-chord notes and used longer and more
interesting phrases. It is hard to say why this should be the case except that by
focusing on a feeling and therefore on expressive sounds and their development, the
conceptual, theoretical and visual aspects are put in the background, and this makes
for a stronger, and more cohesive improvised musical line. Pressing's view
(1988: 139) that: `In a sense, the performer is played by the music. ' seems apposite
here: the schema appears to organise the aural, conceptual and motor elements so that
the body moves to the flow of the music with the barest minimum of conscious
control by the novice improviser. But to do so, the conditions have to be optimum on
every level, including concentration and various distractions can throw the beginner
off balance.
During this part of the process, L realised that `getting lost' was not such a major
problem. AT13 L: `So getting lost turned out to be quite creative because you just
carry on with what you're doing and you get back, sooner or later. ' This
indicates another turning point in which L realised that mistakes are going to happen
during improvisation, which could enable a more creative melodic phrase and that
once the progression was fully assimilated, it was easy to find the way back into it.
There was at this stage, a certain relaxing in her physical stance and attitude and some
together, sometimes remarkably good, for brief periods and then the whole process
seemed to disintegrate again, as if descending into chaos. L realised that (far from
being a problem), it can be creative to get lost and find your way out of it
272
interestingly, rather than play something predictable or easy. L could now improvise
in 5/4 as well as 3/4and 4/4 and was aware that quotes from other tunes could be used
during the improvisation, to give clues to listeners and to make solos sound more
effective. L was also using timbral effects, for example, pizzicato for contrast and
interest. I was interested to see whether L would incorporate these positive and
interesting elements from the expressive exercises, into improvised solos at other
times.
L had continued to learn I Mean You outside of the lesson aurally rather than refer to
the notated music and this had helped both the learning process and the improvisation.
Closing her eyes had an impact on L's perception of how and what she was
improvising, although it had slightly less impact on the actual music produced and
the process more. In terms of the schema, focusing on aural rather than
removing visual distractions as well as patterns), and allowing greater focus on aural,
auditory and motor elements of the solo. The focus on sound was explored fully
during work on the expressive parameters with the emphasis on the changing quality
of sound. The ensuing dialogue helped to illuminate several areas including the notion
(prevalent amongst beginners and classically trained musicians) that you have to
`capture' good ideas and fix them somehow, instead of creating a state of mind and
273
7.9 Session 4
7.9.1 Activities
L described her progress and activities during her own practice in the previous week.
She had explored further expressive parameters in a notated folk tune with her viola
resulting communicative power of the music she played had shocked both L and her
teacher. The creation of a powerful mental image accompanied with a feeling, is like
an extension of the expressive work that was done on a more superficial level in the
previous week and tapping into an emotional well spring is something that Berliner's
including playing the scales and modes of F# and Gb which had confused her in the
last session. She had used her simplified fingering approach to practice a range of
modal scales and was beginning to understand the principle of starting any scale on
any note to allow the maximum possibilities for improvisation. This could be viewed
harmonic picture and a move away from seeing scales as separate and specific
patterns. It is interesting that L often focused her practice on technical aspects that
aided her conceptual and technical prowess, rather than the more intangible
improvisation process. This was partly due I think, to her rational nature and also due
to the difficulty of practising improvisation on your own, even with pre recorded
backing tracks.
L had worked on her chops practice tapes'8 for forty minutes at a time in order to
learn the chord tones and progression of A11the Things You Are more thoroughly. The
18A `chops' tape is a recording of the chords of a standard performed by the player to a metronome,
initially at a very slow tempo, which is gradually speeded up over time. The aim is to learn the chords
progression slowly and systematically by focussing on improvising a continuous melodic line over the
basis recording (Roberts, 1972).
274
chops tape process initiated by Roberts (1978), forces the improviser to focus on
removing all rhythmic variation (the player performs quavers throughout) and by
playing the progression for ten minutes at a time non stop, so that all riffs, patterns etc
are exhausted within the first couple of choruses. Again L had focused C6 on playing
the tune in different ways `and then sometimes doing improvisation'. She had also
worked on diminished chords in All the Things You Are and St Thomas remarking: C8
L: `Yeah and there's a couple in St. Thomas as well and that led me into looking
at the way the notes change; you get a line out of the thirds or the fifths or
whatever that's quite interesting as well; notes that stay the same and notes that
change. ' This comment reflected L's rational and analytical style; she liked to
A new tune, La Fiesta by Chick Corea was learned aurally to introduce L to a fresh
type of jazz tune with several different sections, and the eight bar bolero rhythm was
used as a basis for improvisation. The aim of this activity was to focus on more
rhythmic elements. A section where we swapped improvised phrases over the eight
After a break, work resumed on All the Things You Are with exercises in which
deliberate gaps were placed in the solo to give L time to think and to create a slower
entrances and exits from musical phrases. The idea was to make the improvisation
memorable to both players- to catch attention whilst incorporating the previous ideas
of leaving spaces.
275
7.9.2 Outcomes and how they affect the schema theory for improvisation
L was using some of the techniques of the improvisation class i. e. learning tunes
aurally, in other musical contexts indicating that she felt comfortable with them and
could abstract useful information from them. She had also practised the more
expressive approaches in the context of her folk viola playing with significant effects.
Her modal scale practice allowed her to view the scales (starting on any note) in a
different and more practical way, instead of seeing them as fixed eight note patterns
and this may represent schema development in both technical and aural areas enabling
the broader picture. L also looked at the difference between Gb and F# scales because
that had been a point of confusion particularly in the Monk tune. L increased her
chops practice during the week which would have aided fluency, technique and the
creation of ideas.
The aural learning of La Fiesta was quite slow but followed a pattern (similar to
and phrases, until the notes were heard properly and the correct notes filled in
accurately. Also there was the usual confusion between various parts of the tune and
sections, which continued for several successive choruses and then suddenly fell into
place correctly. In addition, L asked questions about the melody indicating again that
she conceptualises musical information in order to help her to learn it. So for example,
she confirmed the names of the long notes in the melody, even though it was being
learned aurally. At this stage in learning a new piece, L was still imagining the notes
and looking to find the intervals knowing conceptually what they were rather than
hearing a sound and feeling for it, because previously, her motor triggers have been
visual rather than aural. Also, as previously, L added notes to fill in the tune if she
276
could not remember the original and this would not have occurred if she had learned
perfecting a good idea over several choruses. The first playing of a good or interesting
phrase appeared to be accidental or exploratory, and then the idea was recognised and
remembered. The next time around it was played again and imitated over the chords
or developed in another way, and although the process seemed almost compositional,
there was a sense of unfinished business about it; the idea did not quite come off the
first time and so needed returning to. At the twelfth chorus, L took the viola away
from her neck and looked up as if deciding what to play next. Starting in the low
register, she used the pasa doble rhythm and played an ascending scale adding double
stops in the next chorus. She adapted this as I played stops on the guitar, making the
sound and bow strokes more percussive and adding double stops later and then
turning to the low note roots of the double stops. L played them for a chorus
developing the motif into rising octaves for variation. Playing the same progression
over and over appears. to draw out of the individual's musical resources. L did not
play the same ideas for very long before varying or experimenting with them. After
this last idea, she stopped of her own accord and did not say anything illuminating
about what she had played; just that it was `ok'. The implication of perfecting an
improvised solo over several choruses was that L was hearing a different or better
version than the one she was actually playing and moving closer towards it on each
successive solo. This developing ability to remember and elaborate on previous ideas
whilst comparing them to another imagined strand, indicates a high level of skill and a
degree of automaticity (linked to schema growth) within the playing, to allow other
277
At this particular stage, it was also clear that L was using a much wider range of
strategies in her improvisation because the chords were simpler in a tune like La
Fiesta and because her confidence as an improviser was growing. She was
extemporising around the original melody; using different melody notes in the same
rhythm; using fragments of the melody to orientate herself and elaborate on and
employing a wider range of timbres, registers and effects. For example, double
stopping and bowing effects; creating space within the solo and listening for a few
bars to collect herself; taking motifs through the changes; perfecting an idea over
several choruses (being able to remember and develop the ideas) and experimenting
with more rhythmic elements using stops, gaps and octaves. L varied her approaches
and ideas frequently so that one element did not appear to dominate the process.
In All the Things You Are C18 after the break, I asked L to deliberately put spaces
into her improvising by playing four bars and leaving four bars rest, in order to see if
consciously making spaces affected the quality of the improvisation or the perceptions
play something all the time. This strategy appeared to allow L time to think and
produced a series of imitative sequential patterns indicating more control over the
process. During these exercises L was staring intently at the viola fingerboard and so
was perhaps relating visual patterns to aural sequences in a purely rational way. A
subsequent exercise on the same tune, intended to further develop the process,
involved L again focusing on making dramatic and memorable entrances and exits
using echoes of previous ideas as a way to create cohesion. The aim was to make
musical ideas distinctive enough to be memorable both to the improviser and the
audience and L experimented with discord and the effect of it on the improvised line
C21, C22.
278
The conscious and deliberate use of dramatic effects (dynamics, tremelos, pizzicato,
double stops), had an energising effect on L's improvisation and served (like the
previous work on expressive qualities) to give L more possibilities for sounds and
effects within an improvised solo. In the midst of several solos, there was one
strikingly beautiful line, at the end of which L lost track of the progression and did not
resolve the phrase. From this and other examples, it is clear that distinctive and
musical phrases appear to be remembered better and over longer time periods, just as
Bernstein proposed with the schema theory for movement; good and correct
movements are remembered whereas incorrect or badly organised ones are not.
Another characteristic of L's improvising at this stage, was that she did not hear the
necessity of resolving towards the ends of phrases or sections and this gave an
unfinished and wandering quality to the music, as if something was left in mid air.
The reasons for this were unclear, as most of the music she was experienced in
playing, had places where chords and sections of harmony were resolved. The focus
within the developing schema was still perhaps fragmentary, and L was not fully
Similar ideas and motifs were now cropping up throughout the solo as L attempted to
incorporate or develop them across keys, indicating a firmer grasp of the materials
and improved aural memory. Overall there was more clarity in the improvised lines; a
clear outline of the harmonic progression and when L was lost, she soon found her
way back in again. Again, the overriding impression is that the schema `tunes in' for a
while actively co-ordinating all aural, motor, visual and conceptual elements at a high
level to produce a fine and appropriate improvised line only just within the control of
the novice. The schema then `tunes out' (especially if the player becomes aware of the
279
idea and perhaps due to the lack of sustained concentration), and the line disintegrates
or wanders. The focus vanishes and the player is floundering about hearing remnants
and echoes which they then try to play. As the skill progresses these `tuned in'
At the end of nine successive choruses, I asked L how useful the exercises had been:
C29 L: (thinks) `Yeah, again it's like...... yeah I got warmed up when I stopped
thinking about what the chords were then all of a sudden I could play; it's
strange isn't it? ' The improvisation did not sound very different from previous work
in the lesson, but L felt freed up and it is this feeling that is most important because,
the actual notes of the improvisation can be changed in an instance but the feeling of
empowerment and being free to improvise takes longer to imagine and call up. These
development of the process and may perhaps be more important than the
technical/conceptual parameters.
For the last exercise, I asked L to think about density and sparsenessof notes within a
phrase and to consciously use more or fewer notes. I also asked her to experiment
with phrase length and to create a mood at the same time, which she decided, would
be a dance, using the waltz rhythm. The mood was intended to be delicate and wistful.
At the end of seven choruses which did not sound appreciably different from previous
ideas, I asked L for her views on this last activity: C33 L: `What was more
interesting is when you said wait for every third of fourth idea because then your
brain chucks out loads of ideas; it's surprising how many there are. ' L implied
that her aural imagination produced many variations and ideas, not just a single
280
option, especially if there was sufficient time given for them to develop. How or why
the current physical and motor skills of the player; i. e. they would not be able to play
an idea beyond their technique although they may be able to imagine it. This notion
L also mentioned several times the notion of playing the `safe' option as the first or
easiest thing that springs to mind, rather than giving herself the time and space to play
something interesting or more difficult. C38 L: `But it's also if you have to go
somewhere fast onto the next note, you go somewhere safe nine times out of ten,
so it gives you more room to do things that are less safe. ' Like the participants in
the pilot study groups, L was aware of playing safe in her improvisation and this
appears to be the same process whatever the level of experience of the player; the
more creative approaches are sought but sometimes do not happen. At this point also,
there was still a feeling from L that the music was controlling her. In response to a
comment from me that the tendency is to play lots of notes and leave no spaces L
said: C34 L: `And once you've started it you've got to carry on haven't you? '
7.9.3 Summary
The indications from the fourth session were that consciously directed exercises
creating drama and using effects, actually broadened the scope and possibilities of L's
improvising and that more exercises in this and the expressive domain could be
fruitfully explored and developed. By this time, L appeared to be using a wider range
some startling and interesting improvised lines. L's state of mind and perception of
how she was doing were clearly fundamental to her motivation, even though they
281
appeared (at this stage) to have a minimal effect on the actual music produced. Being
improviser and L seemed to have reached that state more often in this session. In
terms of schema development, there was still a strong sense of L being controlled by
the music in some sense and of the schema `tuning' in and out; providing glimpses of
great music and then chaos. However, L's perception of this was variable; at times she
was aware of a good idea, at others not and the process of improvisation was almost
independent of her state of mind or consciousness of it. The notion of playing `safely'
again cropped up and the exercise of not playing the first idea that came into her head
helped L to choose from a range of ideas and to be more actively creative and
`unsafe'! At this stage, L was only at times in complete control of the music, for the
7.10 Session 5
The main purpose of the final session was to review work done; to evaluate progress
where possible and to suggest areas for future practice in order for L to continue the
process.
7.10.1 Activities
During the previous week, L had been practising scales and tunes in different
positions: F2 `I've been doing a lot of playing things in different positions like
reworking the tunes in different positions and working all the scales in all the
different positions which suddenly made lots of technical things tie together. '
Elaborating about her previous understanding that scales (on the viola from a classical
training perspective), could only be played in one fixed `right' place with the correct
282
fingering L said: F2 L: `and of course, there isn't a right fingering; there's
wherever you happen to be when you want to play it. ' F4 `I pick a scale for
today's practice and. I've been doing all the hard ones as you suggested and then
just going through five or six different positions and playing it in each of those
positions. You realise that you use some of them an awful lot and some of them
not very much at all because they're awkward anyway. As you said about chord
positions, you're stretching your hand in ways that you wouldn't normally
because in a classical piece you tend to finger it in the easiest way and then keep
schema development to incorporate the understanding that any scale can start on any
note in any position. This allows the improviser complete technical freedom of
expression, because they can play the note they hear in a range of places on the
instrument and is about moving away from a fixed internalised pattern, and seeing a
L had tried playing the violin again in her improvisation practice and found that whilst
it used to be easier (because of the size, sound and non-transposition), it was now
harder, because she had worked more and solved various technical problems on the
viola. It is interesting to note the way the parameters change as skills are developed on
an instrument. The singing was less useful to L as she developed more understanding
and improvising skill on the viola and she could feel herself moving to the intervals
on the viola whilst she was singing them without the instrument! The physicality of
different instruments and the ways problems are solved on them during the process of
learning to improvise are clearly significant and integral to the more invisible
283
psychological and conceptual elements. Playing the violin (instead of the viola) also
just makes you think about the tunes in a slightly different way, so it's quite good
to play them on the violin and see what you can do on that, and it's so much
easier to go up high. I've been trying to put harmonics into things just because it
sounds nice and having listened to some of these CD's, it's an effect that people
use quite often and it's easy, so it's a good effect that. ' There is a lot more research
that could be done about the relationship between the instrument and the player and
how its layout and sound affects the way they approach and create improvised lines.
The activities in the final session began with La Fiesta, where L used a range of
approaches in the improvisation and where the overall impression was one of
confidence in the basic materials. The simple and repeating chord progression
obviously assisted the process here, but the rhythmic impetus of the tune and pasa
doble rhythm provided a basis for all kinds of new and inventive ideas. Interestingly,
during the last choruses (19-24) most of the phrases were resolving onto the root at
the end and although this reflected the tune at that point (as it did in the other
standards), this is the first time that L had deliberately resolved musical ideas at the
end of a phrase and this gave the music a stronger and less wandering quality.
The chorus of fours F8 between L and I was particularly interesting and successful in
that L responded well to my four bar ideas with imitative motifs and answering
appropriate musical response. Many of the phrases were also copied and extended or
the rhythmic elements of them developed and this was in sharp contrast to earlier
284
attempts which got bogged down in aural and technical problems. Included in the
fours were a range of dynamics, density of notes, timbral effects and varied scale,
arpeggio and motivic features i. e. the improvisation was not restricted to purely
melodic or rhythmic areas. This particular exercise was also lively and open to more
The overall feeling of this session was of great progress: L was more creative and
thinking of one chorus at a time rather than smaller phrases and there was generally
more energy and several instances of the improvisation coming together well over the
eight bar sequence. In addition, L was actively imitating, copying and responding to
between this and her first improvised choruses were the new ideas that were
generated; there is much less recurrence of the Kletzmer ideas and more rhythmic
energy. Most important was the feeling of fun, not laborious seriousness, but perhaps
this would not have been attained without the previous work enabling a freer
response.
I asked L what she felt the difference was between the improvisation of this and last
week: F10 `It feels more solid. ' F11 `Yes and it's sort of flowing. ' The
contradiction between solidity (or stability) and fluidity, highlights the problem of
It also sums up the main issue about what a schema is i. e. a stable structure that
allows the creation of adaptive changes and fluidity. It is hard to envisage how all
this line of questioning to ask L what she was thinking of at the time and she could
not really say. I took this to be a sign of total concentration in the moment during the
285
improvisation process so that the conscious mind was not `thinking' at all or
verbalising or judging. The improvisational process was happening in the present and
because it was unadulterated (or conceptualised) response, the schema was allowed
uncluttered free rein and the ensuing improvisation was at a high level of energy and
skill
I asked L to sing a few choruses again to ascertain any differences between sung and
instrumental improvisation at this stage. This exercise was not a success in that it
seemed to represent L's improvisational skills at a previous level rather than in the
present. I asked L how she felt: F14 L: `It was harder at first because the tune's
like it's in my fingers now. ' Having focused on played improvisation, it was now
harder to L to sing and she related this to her fingers; the music was in her fingers
now and not her vocal chords any more. F15 L: `and once the tune's in your fingers
you don't have to think about it any more. ' This is obviously so, except that with
the improvising, all the potential movements have to be in the fingers also, not just
the relatively fixed parameters of a tune. It is the potential movements relating to all
of the potential intervals (learned through a range of different tunes), that takes the
schema time to organise; there are so many variables including timbral ones.
I asked L to sing and play at the same time to help to reconnect the voice to
instrument process and she found this to be useful: F20 L: `It was and I was also....
it frees..... it takes all that thinking about what your fingers are doing away
because you're actually focusing on the voice. Well, you're focusing on the what
note and how it's going to be; on the sound of it rather than the technical
problems and I was thinking, I'm not sure if I can just like... it's weird isn't it?
286
Doing jumps and things and just making them bigger and bigger and still seeing
L's perception was that the improvisation is better with the voice which I'm not sure
is true but she seemed more confident doing it this way. L also said that she focused
on the sound of the voice and the note rather than any technical element although it
was clear from her own description of the process, that sometimes the fingers rather
There followed some work on I Mean You with L singing as well as playing and when
asked how different the process was when using the voice she replied: F20 L: `It
wasn't a huge amount; the voice got me going but then I wanted to play in parts
of the instrument that my voice wouldn't stretch to. ' This remark seems to
highlight again the apparent conflict between different physical parameters and also
indicates that L did not use her voice frequently under normal circumstances. More
research work needs to be done on the role of the voice in developing instrumental
The final questionnaire sought to elicit L's views and perceptions of her progress over
and ways of continuing her practice, and only succeeded in this aim on a practical and
spontaneously during the research were more revealing). Her first comment was that
the length of the lessons was too short, as were the one week gaps between them and
she always felt that she ought to have done more. L enjoyed the one to one lessons
better than the workshops becauseof the greater time spent on playing and not being
self-conscious amongst others. She also wanted to continue her improvisation but
287
within the context of a performing group and so was looking for jam sessions where
she could do this. She had become aware of the value of communal playing in jazz
during the lessons (especially the jazz feel in the accompaniment) and had also begun
to enjoy practising again because of all the new possibilities available to her.
288
8. Findings and implications of the practical
investigation and how they relate to schema
theory
One of the most compelling pieces of evidence for the schema, from the practical
research (both in the group workshops and the case study), is the ability of students to
immediately adapt a tune they have learned by heart into new rhythmic variations and
different keys, without prior rehearsal or practice. This was clearly observed in the
case study in the first session, when L learned the tune of All the Things You Are by
heart. She was then asked to perform it in different ways, to ascertain how the schema
was developing i. e. whether the learned relationships between the intervals could be
abstracted and adapted to a new context. As I accompanied her on the guitar with a
swing rhythm, L changed the feel of the tune immediately in response to the different
energy of the accompaniment, which was very different from her previous renditions
and quite unlike the previously learned notated form. L also changed the articulation
by adding slides and slurs, as well as shortening some notes and lengthening others
and played with verve and confidence incorporating the rhythmic changes without
missing a beat, just by being focused on and responding to, the accompaniment. This
would seem to indicate that the schema for this tune had developed sufficiently for her
to adapt it and even to add small changes by being given hints from and imitating the
musical context of the accompaniment. L's learning of the melody by that stage also
appeared to by automatic enough for her to add different articulations, indicating that
schema development was already enhancing the adaptable nature of the emergent
skill.
289
Similarly, when the same exercise was repeated in 3/4time in a jazz waltz feel, (which
needed a very different rhythmic approach requiring a shortening of the notes and a
general lightening of the phrasing), L was able to instantaneously `translate' the tune
into the new time feel and rhythm. She completely altered the learned tune both
rhythmically and melodically, to fit with the new 3/4accompaniment and this kind of
the tune that can be remoulded to fit a different time feel. L could do this with a new
and unfamiliar tune, spontaneously and completely naturally. The skill appears to be
an indication that a trained musician is a natural improviser and that the time taken to
learn improvisation in the jazz idiom, is involved with technical parameters, creating
ideas and cohesion within the style, not learning how to improvise itself. The process
the altered accompaniment, whilst keeping similar contours and outlines, is strong
evidence for the existence of the schema in that the relationships between the intervals
had already been abstracted and could be altered rhythmically at will. In this case, the
tune was compressed, lightened and moved more quickly through the harmony and it
is also noteworthy that this kind of adaptation can occur so soon after a tune is
learned. It indicates the first stage of schema abstraction in that it abstracts basic
information from the tune in the form of extemporisation i. e. there is no real creative
improvisation as yet.
The fingering and therefore the embodiment of the music also helped L to memorise
the tune (and the therefore the abstractions), by connecting a sound and written note
to a movement and physical space on the instrument. During this first session, L said
that she was thinking in terms of unusual and larger intervals and fingering patterns,
whilst simultaneously making the sound, rather than visualising the notation. In
290
general, L took a problem solving approach to exploring improvisational strategies
using a combination of theoretical, aural and technical elements to decide which note
or series of notes to play next. Constraints (of melody and harmony) provided a
framework for L that was useful, and exploring expressive parameters was both
creative and fruitful for her as she played much more musically and with melodic
L was presented with chord symbols that were explained to her in a previous
workshop and then played them on the piano in order to understand the sound and
structure of the harmony and melody better. She was therefore already engaged in the
process of developing the theoretical and practical elements of the harmony and
specifically using the clear visual patterns on the piano to help her to hear and
understand them. Looking at the chords, L was seeking structural, conceptual and
aural connections and working out how the melody fitted with the harmony. It became
clear during the research process that L was unable to imagine a tune without a
contextual harmonic background and in All the Things You Are, at the start, when
harmony was not provided in the accompaniment, she imagined her own from the
symbols, which did not match Kern's in places. This indicates a first stage schema
for harmony because L was still attempting to understand chord tones and sounds
from the written symbols and was not fully aware of the related functional harmony,
being at an early stage of listening and assimilating jazz harmony in general. The
background jazz music schema comprising of jazz heard and any previous
understanding of it, had enabled L to understand the chord symbols and harmony and
the motor schemata which was already learned for viola playing, was obviously
enhanced with more emphasis on aural parameters and triggers. The sound and feel of
291
jazz music was however, clearly not yet in her aural imagination and so could not
In the second session I Mean You by Thelonius Monk was played, in a be-bop style
unlike any of the tunes previously attempted. During the subsequent learning of the
harmony, L recognised the II, V, I pattern recurring again from All the Things You
Are, which may indicate the schema developing for harmony and an understanding
that this pattern is transferable to other standards in different keys. As a teacher, I had
terms of chord families in relation to a particular key. L was subsequently able to find
other examples in new material which helped her in improvising over new standards
Referring to the same tune (I Mean You) L said: L10 L: `This one's got a
complicated tune and easy chords. ' In fact, the tune was relatively simple, with
many repeating motifs in a call and answer form; it was the placement of the rhythm
that was more difficult and the strength of the off beat rhythmic feel. L's classical and
folk schemata were not attuned to this and as she began her improvisation, it had all
the hallmarks of a folk tune particularly because of the movement of the first two
dominant chords (F7-Db7). The folk feel of the soloing was difficult to alter, perhaps
because the progression reminded L of something she already knew and played. This
is evidence that the overall listening schema was as yet undeveloped in the jazz be-
bop idiom and that what L heard within the melody, she did not understand in relation
to her previous listening. As she learned the melody aurally, she came to understand it
292
better and to realise that she was adding a note (that she called a `thinking note', see
below) to make the phrase more like the pentatonic falling phrase she was used to.
process is at work
improvisatory ideas. Her attention to sound, melodic flow and timbral effects were
More research in this area would be highly beneficial because it seems to draw on
other more affective sources within the individual and this kind of approach might be
improviser. Overall, throughout the project, L brought a rational and conceptual way
phrases based on logical and known patterns, for example, the pentatonic scale and
these replaced the actuality of the aural phrase (i. e. in the first phrase of I Mean You).
The expressive work removed all of these aspects and the resultant improvisations
were of a different order and style, implying that there may be several different voices
within the improviser that require drawing out. Perhaps expressive work should be
done in tandem with more technical and conceptual practice from the beginning.
developing in melodic and harmonic ways, is not clear. It should also be noted that
work had already been done on the melodic and harmonic basis for improvisation
which perhaps L was able to draw on in expressive work in a more intuitive way. In a
293
later lesson, L used a particularly poignant image to inform her improvisation again,
8.4 Schema theory related to wider listening within the jazz idiom
Evidence of schema theory in the second session related to L's listening in the week
between lessons and her realisation that she needed an aural (as opposed to theoretical
or conceptual) model or series of them, in order to decide what kind of approach and
sound she would use in her own improvisation. This was indicated by her following
and I thought, you know, there's lots of options for what sort of sound to make:
there's the following the voice option; then there's the following the tradition of
jazz violin playing or there's trying to make something else out of nowhere,
which is a bit, I suppose, what the viola is.. ' This was the first occasion that L had
improvisation and to inspire her own ideas. It is part of the broader listening schema
During the second part of the second session when L heard Allegresse by Maria
Schneider, she was clearly unused to listening to contemporary large ensemble jazz
and unsure of the time feel, instrumentation and various elements involved in the
compositional structure and improvisation. The listening session (which was the first
concentrated work done in this area), indicated that L needed to continue to listen to a
wider range and type of jazz music in order to understand the idiom more thoroughly
and assist with her improvisation. The general jazz listening schema is vitally
important for developing aural memory and a sense of the rhythmic feel and harmonic
294
continued to guide her own learning by widening her listening to include Thelonius
Monk and the trombonist Annie Whitehead, and by making her own decisions about
Indications of the development of a technical schema were also present in the second
session when L focussed her practice on technical aspects and significantly, had
simplified the complex position fingering on the neck of the viola. She imagined it to
be the neck of the guitar (with imaginary frets) and moved her whole hand up and
down to change keys and chords, thereby keeping the same fingering and allowing her
to listen more on the sound of the notes. Describing the process L said: R7 L: `I
thought another way of tackling the chords is to treat the fiddle neck like a
guitar and instead of trying to figure out the chords wherever they might be, to
use a similar fingering for all of them and shift up and down more. ' R8 L:
`Which is loads easier because you're using the same finger on the same part of
the chord and all you have to remember is whether it's major, minor or seventh. '
This changed perspective gave a consistency to the fingering patterns and simplified
the complex technical fingering problems that may have been inhibiting L's
improvisational ideas. The strategy, which used a visual pattern approach on the viola
neck, is interesting and probably came from watching what I was doing on the guitar.
It appears to show the formation or evolution of the schema for technical parameters
i. e. finding an individual way to internalise the physical and conceptual aspects of the
chord tones (and scale notes) using a logical and consistent fingering pattern.
Whether this simplified approach to fingering was carried over into the folk and
classical music that L also played, is not known but there seemed not to have been a
conflict between the standard fingering procedure that she had used over many years
295
and the new fingering for improvisation. The schema here appears to encompass an
8.5.1 Schema theory and further evidence of technical/motor schema using the
Later in the second session L returned to All the Things You Are and found it easier to
play rather than sing an improvisation. She herself felt that this was because the
fingering `anchors you down' AT1 and provides another framework for memory and
orientation through the tune. Thus, it was at this stage easier for L to play an
improvisation rather than sing it because she had the fingering as well as the aural and
conceptual frameworks to guide her. This provides further evidence for the
singing proved less useful because there was not fingering framework: the muscles of
8.5.2 Schema theory and evidence of abstracting information into other contexts
L was using some of the techniques of the improvisation class for example, learning
tunes aurally in other musical contexts, indicating that she felt comfortable with them
and could abstract useful information from them. She had also practised the more
expressive approaches in the context of her folk viola playing with significant effects.
Her modal scale practice allowed her to view the scales (starting on any note) in a
different and more practical way, instead of perceiving them as fixed eight note
patterns and this may represent schema development in both technical and aural areas
296
enabling the assimilation of a broader and more flexible picture. L also looked at the
difference between Gb and F# scales because that had been a point of confusion
There was further evidence for the development of the technical/motor schema in the
fifth session, when L had been practising scales and tunes in different positions: F2
`I've been doing a lot of playing things in different positions like reworking the
tunes in different positions and working all the scales in all the different positions
which suddenly made lots of technical things tie together. ' Elaborating about her
previous understanding that scales (from a classical training perspective), could only
be played in one fixed `right' place with the correct fingering L said: F2 L: `and of
course, there isn't a right fingering; there's wherever you happen to be when you
want to play it. ' F4 `I pick a scale for today's practice and I've been doing all the
hard ones as you suggested and then just going through five or six different
positions and playing it in each of those positions. You realise that you use some
of them an awful lot and some of them not very much at all because they're
awkward anyway. As you said about chord positions, you're stretching your
hand in ways that you wouldn't normally because in a classical piece you tend to
finger it in the easiest way and then keep playing it like that. ' The alteration and
development to incorporate the understanding that any scale can start on any note in
expression (because they can play the note they hear in a range of places on the
instrument). This is about moving away from the fixed, internalised pattern and using
297
8.5.4 Evidence of intermediate technical/motor schema development-the fingers
`knowing' where to go
After several successive choruses of All the Things You Are in the transposed key, the
tune was played in the original key of Ab and I asked L how she felt her
improvisation was progressing: T5 L: `A big feeling of relief. ' `My fingers knew
where to go in that one. ' This response seems to endorse the view that letting the
fingers `go' (out of conscious control) and the schema organise the movement is a key
Schema theory predicts that motor programmes will become increasingly automatic
and non-conscious as the skill develops and L's explanation of her fingers knowing
improvisational strategies
During the second session it appeared that the schema for improvisation was
developing because when All the Things You Are was revisited, L was improvising in
a more confident and controlled manner and more cohesively over longer periods,
playing phrases of eight or sixteen bars rather than the two to four bars she had
that L was thinking in terms of moving through the harmony and attempting more
intricate lines. When I asked L how she felt about the improvising she said: A2 L:
`Bits of it were nice', implying that she was not really aware of the qualitative
change in her playing. The beginner improviser seems to compare their current
performance to the times when the solo really came together briefly and they are not
improvisational strategies especially on La Fiesta because both the melody and the
chords were simpler and because her confidence and ability as an improviser was
growing. L was extemporising around the original melody; using different melody
notes in the same rhythm and using fragments of the tune to orientate herself and
For example, she used double stopping and bowing effects and created space within
the solo by listened for a few bars before continuing with the improvised line. She
also played the same motif through the changes altering it to fit the harmony and
perfected an idea over several choruses because she could remember and develop the
ideas, whilst experimenting with more rhythmic elements using stops, gaps and
octaves. L varied her approaches and ideas frequently so that one element did not
appear to dominate the process, and this appears to indicate the developing schema
There was also evidence for continuing schema development through L's response to
two and four bar phrases initiated by me. This whole area developed progressively
over the course of the research period and appears to reflect the changing nature of the
schema and therefore the ability of the player to respond automatically and
299
8.6.1 Evidence of schema development with the ability to improvise in a
transposed key
In the second session, evidence of the developing schema was indicated by the fact
that L was not only able to transpose the melodies and rhythmically alter them at will
but also to improvise in the transposed keys. She used the framework of the melody in
the new key to guide her when she got lost, then explored the harmonic background of
the new keys. This indicates a growing level skill and increasing schema
melodically), a relatively complex tune. In terms of the schema, it appears that it was
developing for the harmony as well as the melody because L was able to abstract the
relevant information to improvise in the transposed key, albeit whilst returning to the
framework tune at various times. The information abstracted from the standard,
therefore involved not only the intervals of the melody but the root movements and
extrapolate and adapt from one tune. I asked L what she felt about her improvising at
this point and she accurately assessedwhere she was: T4 L: `There are places where
it really goes and other places where I completely lose it. But when I lose it, it's
like there are places I can catch up with it, just about. '
On a slightly less positive note, when L learned and played St Thomas by Sonny
Rollins (which is a short AAB format 16 bar calypso with a simple chord
progression), she found it relatively easy after the harmonic complexity of All the
Things You Are, and was quickly able to transpose it and to improvise over the chord
progression. As in previous activities, L also used the tune for extemporisation and
300
imitated the rhythmic phrasing and stop sections within it to good effect. However, as
she gained in confidence and fluency, L began to anticipate the chords and phrases
and often got ahead of the beat. This may be an indication that as the schema
seems that the anticipation and the willingness to play something appropriate to the
changing chords, overrode L's listening feedback which should have been hearing the
mistake. For example, there were strong clashes between the anticipated and actual
tonalities and harmonies, which were not intended. The ability of the brain to filter out
integral part of schema development because it is impossible for a person to take in all
of the sensory information presented to them on a daily basis. This indicates the
importance of recording improvisation (and other musical practice) in order for the
improviser to really hear what they are playing, as opposed to what the brain decides
is happening or is focussing on. The area of filtering also relates to Pressing and
Johnson-Laird's ideas about the `limited processing power' of humans: the very
understanding, may in certain circumstances also restrict or limit the same processes.
8.7 Evidence of intermediate schema- altering the ways of learning a tune from
notated to aural
Further evidence is provided for the development of the intermediate schema when L
decided to learn I Mean You aurally, rather than from the notated copy, which was
previously L's preferred method of learning. When I asked why L had not used the
notated music she said: Q13 L: `Oh, I had a look at the music and thought it was
completely impossible (looks up, thinks) em, to work it out off that. ' This seems
quite an odd response from a reading player, although perhaps because we had
301
focused so much on a purely aural approach in the lesson, the reading became what it
actually is in reality; a shorthand form and aide-memoire. L stated that during the
translation of the sung line of I Mean You onto the viola, she became aware of the
`thinking note' that she had added during the lesson in order to make the pentatonic
phrase more logical to her Q14. The adding of the note to make the phrase what L
wanted it to be, rather that what it actually was, links back to the first session where
she was adding notes to All the Things You Are on one particular occasion. of
recreated memory which was then repeated in the next version. In the case I
of Mean
You,- it is perhaps evidence of the previous schema for a pentatonic phrase overriding
the actuality of auditory feedback and imposing itself on memory. It was only when L
was accompanying herself and singing the melody with the added note (which made
the tune out of synch with the chords) that she realised her mistake. This activity also
indicates the positive use of the voice to learn a melody; something which altered in
8.7.1 Evidence of intermediate schema-not only playing notes that fit but
The intermediate schema is also indicated by the change in attitude from beginner
improviser wanting to play something correct which fits with the harmony, to the
person who takes risks and enjoys the fact that improvisational process will not
always be controllable. L said that what she most enjoyed about improvising was
`When I do something I don't expect (laughs). ' Q6. Like the players in the earlier
workshops, the implication is that it is the creative and more intangible aspects that
drives and sustains musicians to become improvisers and that the perception of the
player is that at least part of the process is accepted to be non-conscious and not fully
302
circumstances, the schema appears to organise the various aural, conceptual and
consciousness of the improviser. This feeling from improvisers, that ideas come out
comes out that you didn't plan or when you get an idea, a sort of technical idea
for something and you think you'll give it a go but it sounds quite different from
what you thought it was going to (shrugs) and that can be nice. '
During this latter part of the process, L realised that `getting lost' was not such a
major problem. AT13 L: `So getting lost turned out to be quite creative because
you just carry on with what you're doing and you get back, sooner or later. '
This indicates another turning point in which L realised that mistakes happen during
improvisation which could enable a more creative melodic phrase and that once the
at this stage, a certain relaxing in her physical stance and attitude and some lessening
of her anxiety and seriousness about the whole activity. The relaxation and calming
element is often referred to by sports coaches as the point at which the athlete is really
able to cope with and extend their new skills because a degree of competence, fluency
and automaticity has been achieved. This appears to be the case at this stage of L's
skill: there was a noticeable change of attitude from mildly stressed to light hearted
and relaxed, as if the process has been assimilated at a deep level and could now be
practiced fully.
303
8.7.3 Evidence of the developing schema -extending rhythmic and timbral
parameters
At this stage, further evidence for the developing intermediate schema was indicated
by the fact that L could now improvise in 5/4 as well as 3/4and 4/4 and was aware that
quotes from other tunes could be used during the improvisation, to give clues to
listeners and to make solos sound more effective. L was also using timbral effects, for
example, pizzicato and double stopping for contrast and interest. Some of these
elements were incorporated into the melodic and harmonically based improvisations
8.7.4 Evidence of schema development: using general contours that are later
Many times during the process, general contours of a tune were memorised and then
the specific details filled in. For example, the aural learning of La Fiesta followed a
general contours and phrases, until the notes were heard properly and the correct notes
filled in accurately. Also there was a similar confusion during the learning and
memorising process, between various parts of the tune and sections, which continued
for several successive choruses and then suddenly fell into place correctly. In
addition, L continued to ask questions about the notes of the melody indicating that
she conceptualised musical information in order to help her to learn it. For example,
she confirmed the names of the long notes in the melody, even though it was being
learned aurally. At this stage in learning a new piece, L was still imagining the notes
and looking to find the intervals knowing conceptually what they were rather than
hearing a sound and feeling for it, because previously, her motor triggers have been
visual rather than aural. Also, as previously, L added notes to fill in the tune if she
304
could not remember the original and this would not have occurred if she had learned
There is evidence for improvement in aural memory and the development of the
example, La Fiesta. It became apparent that L was perfecting a good idea over
several choruses and was only able to do this as a result of improved aural memory
and a sense of the longer, more developed musical idea. The first playing of a good
recognised and remembered, the following time, it was played again and imitated over
solo over several choruses was that L was hearing a different or better version than
the one she was actually playing and moving closer towards it in each successive solo.
comparing them to another imagined strand, indicates a higher level of skill and a
degree of automaticity (linked to schema growth) within the playing to allow other
8.8 Evidence that the schema is still at an intermediate stage- lack of resolution
There was a further indication that the schema was at an intermediate stage during the
fourth and fifth sessions, because a characteristic of L's improvising at this stage was
that she did not hear the necessity of resolving towards the ends of phrases or sections
and this gave an unfinished and wandering quality to the music, as if something was
305
left in mid air. The reasons for this are unclear; she may simply not have been aware
of the lack of resolution or was too bound up in the improvisatory processto focus on
it. By the final tune (La Fiesta) of the last session, L was resolving at the ends of
sections and where needed during the progression, indicating that the schema
Also by the fourth and fifth sessions, similar ideas and motifs were now cropping up
indicating a firmer grasp of the materials and improved aural memory. She was
Overall there was more clarity in the improvised lines; a clear outline of the harmonic
progression and when L was lost, she soon found her way back in again. This would
seem to indicate the beginnings of a generalisation within the schema and the
development of the individual voice in which certain phrases or motifs are preferred
over others.
L mentioned several times the notion of playing the `safe' option as the first or easiest
thing that springs to mind, rather than giving herself the time and space to play
something interesting or more difficult. C38 L: `But it's also if you have to go
somewhere fast onto the next note, you go somewhere safe nine times out of ten,
so it gives you more room to do things that arc less safe. ' Like the participants in
the pilot study groups, L was aware of playing safe in her improvisation and this
more creative approaches are sought. At this stage also, there was still a feeling from
306
L that the music was controlling her. In response to a comment from me that the
tendency is to play lots of notes and leave no spaces L said: C34 L: `And once
you've started it you've got to carry on haven't you? ' This provides evidence that
the schema is still at an intermediate level, although well beyond the initial `playing
Later on in the improvising process, I asked L what she felt the difference was in her
improvisation, between one week and the next: F10 `It feels more solid. ' F11 `Yes
and it's sort of flowing. ' The contradiction between solidity (or stability) and
fluidity, highlights the problem of attempting to describe what is happening during the
process of learning to improvise. It also sums up one of the main issues about what a
schema is i. e. a stable structure that allows the creation of adaptive changes and
fluidity. I followed up this line of questioning to ask L what she was thinking of
during her improvisation and she could not really say. I took this to be a sign of total
concentration in the moment during the improvisation process so that the conscious
mind was not `thinking' at all or verbalising or judging. The improvisational process
was happening in the present and because it was unadulterated (or conceptualised)
response, the schema was allowed uncluttered free rein and the ensuing improvisation
8.12 Indications of the developing viola schema - singing and violin playing are
Also in session 5 was some evidence that the schema for improvisation specifically on
the viola was developing because, L had tried playing the violin again in her
307
improvisation practice and found that whilst it used to be easier (because of the size,
sound and non-transposition),it was now harder. L had by this.time, worked more on
the viola and solved various technical problems on the instrument, particularly on
fingering and scales. It is interesting to note the way the parameters change as skills
are developed on an instrument. The singing was less useful to L as she developed
more understanding and improvising skill on the viola and she could feel herself
moving to the intervals on the viola whilst she was singing them without it! The
physicality of different instruments and the ways problems are solved on them during
the process of learning to improvise, are clearly significant and integral to the more
invisible psychological and conceptual elements. There is a lot more research that
could be done about the relationship between the instrument and the player and how
its layout and sound affects the way they approach and create improvised lines.
When I asked L to sing a few choruses, the exercise was unsuccessful, in that it
seemed to represent L's improvisational skills at a previous level rather than in the
present. I asked L how she felt: F14 L: `It was harder at first because the tune's
like it's in my fingers now. ' Having focused on played improvisation, it was now
harder to L to sing and she related this to her fingers; the music is in her fingers now
and not her vocal chords any more. F15 L: `and once the tune's in your fingers you
don't have to think about it any more. ' This is obviously so, except that with the
improvising, all the potential movements have to be in the fingers also, not just the
the potential intervals (learned through a range of different tunes), that takes the
schema time to organise; there are so many variables including timbral ones.
308
8.13 Indication that the schema provides cohesion and improvement in aural
memory
The overall feeling of the last session was that L had made great progress and was
more creative and thinking of one whole chorus at a time rather than smaller phrases.
There was generally more energy and several instances of the improvisation coming
together well over the eight bar sequence. In addition, L was actively imitating,
phrases. The other difference between this and her early improvised choruses were the
new ideas that were generated: there was much less recurrence of ideas in the folk
idiom and more rhythmic energy. Most important was the feeling of light heartedness,
but perhaps this would not have been attained without the previous work enabling a
freer response.
Other findings from the individual case study relate primarily to memory and how this
may work in relation to schema development. A significant finding at the start of the
research process was that during the learning of a melody from musical notation, L
would change a note or rhythm as part of the process of memorisation and for some
inexplicable reason that change would become fixed and integrated into the next
version as a dynamic process. This was part of (an essentially improvisatory) overall
the harmonic and rhythmic framework of the standard. L also remembered the
specific details on progressive listenings. She was however, only consciously aware
of seeking unusual features or large intervals, and perhaps her memory was providing
understood, but perhaps has to do with the neurological change from short term to
long term memory. It is also probably affected by the number of times that particular
occasion and not another? Perhaps the stabilising elements of the schema process
inhibit constant change, which would not allow anything to be learned, but on
occasion enable some element to be altered and over time, as the schema develops,
The mismatch that was often observable in L between what she remembered as the
tune and what she sang back was appears also significant to schema development
because the implication is that the memory has the original correct version or a
contour of it, with which to compare with the actual one being sung. Or it may be that
the brain, having evolved to detect patterns just `knows' that the sung version is
different in some way from the original in aural memory. In either case, the question
of whether a whole or partial solo is created in the mind which is then executed by
motor schema or whether we merely adapt an idea and go with the flow of it, remains
unanswerable.
Another finding related to memory was that the learning and memorising of a melody
appears to proceed in a chaotic fashion, not in a series of rational steps (as most things
are taught). It appears that suddenly after much confusion, preparation and problem
solving, the piece is learned all at once and played in one time frame. Viewed as
whole and gestalt framework, it could be that all of the elements required for the
schemato work have to be in place before it can operate.It may also be evidence of
the self-organising nature of the schema and might explain the sudden feeling of
310
knowledge or skills `falling into place' in much learning process, especially after long
and arduouseffort.
Still related to memory, another finding was that L's aural memory was challenged by
more complex jazz rhythms and melodies because she had not heard enough of the
music to comprehend them, as her background listening schema was firmly based
within the western classical and folk tradition. As a result L tended (initially at least),
to change rhythmic or melodic elements that appeared incongruous to her to fit her
incorporating alien rhythmic patterns appeared to almost cause her physical pain, as
evidenced in grimaces recorded on video. This relates to the point previously made in
the general findings about the need to have a background schema for the style and
genre of music through listening and deep assimilation, before embarking on the
Indications of the self - organising nature of the schema were present throughout the
individual case study research. At many times during the practice of improvising
using vertical and more horizontal aspects of the harmony, it became clear that after
solving the various aural and motor problems in various different ways, L would
suddenly play a perfect or interesting improvised line in which all the elements came
together in a highly integrated and musical way. The solo line would then drift away
again to be replaced with more prosaic music. It appears that the schema organises the
various multi-modal processes, and that at the beginning, it focuses in and out rather
like a radio signal until higher levels of skill and concentration are developed and
311
sustainable within the individual. Thus even at the beginning L was able to improvise
throughout the process. Sometimes it was useful to create more musical and coherent
lines and at other times it was a restraining influence because of motor skills being
developed on the instrument and there was a conflict between what L could hear, sing
and play. As an aurally based process, I felt that singing ought to be beneficial,
8.17 Closing the eyes and how that affected schema process
Asking L to close her eyes had a profound (and positive) effect on her perception of
her own sound and style and how much space and time was available to her in the
improvised solo. Closing her eyes focused L on the sound she was making by
removing all visual patterns (L often stared at the fingerboard of the viola for long
periods). It also seemed to remove time constraints allowing her to think and play in a
relaxed way. The image that she saw of an abstract viola is similar in dynamic quality
to that of the most experienced blind-fold chess players described by Binet (1966) and
also close to how Schmidt (1975) viewed the relationship between movements and
elements in schema development. It might be useful to expand on work with the eyes
closed and to encourage the development of images which may help the improvisation
process. A final issue here is that in L's case, closing her eyes also shut out her
consciousness of who and where she was so that she just listened with a positive
312
judgement. The effect of a negative `voice in the head' may be counteracted in some
The effect of this positive feeling was felt during the subsequent improvising, where L
realised that getting lost or making a mistake could turn out to be a creative sidestep
and that the actual mistake does not matter much (unlike in classical music). This
understanding relaxed L and the music generally. It was noticeable the way that L
created and perfected an idea over several choruses; it began as a vague contour or
motif and was gradually developed over successive choruses indicating the schema at
work in a compositional way. It was also noticeable that L was using a wide variety of
The most significant finding across all the practical research was that the participating
musicians were naturally able to improvise in some form from the beginning of the
process. In other words, in the trained musician, the schema for improvisation would
indicated by the fact that, without exception, students were able to take a jazz tune
that they had learned by heart and without prior practice, play it in a different time,
style or key. This in itself provides strong evidence for the existence of the schema;
the melody, (learned from notation, memorised in aural memory and spatially as
intervals on the instrument), was adapted instantaneously and without rehearsal to fit
altered parameters. In later exercises, the tune was transposed with the same ease
where the spatial relationships between the intervals were remembered but where
notated conceptual elements were used i. e. transposed keys, chords and chord tones,
313
these appeared to inhibit the physical process. The implication is that improvisation is
a completely natural activity for a musician and that the difficulties related to it are
technically, culturally and stylistically based. In addition, improvised ideas that were
sung first (in the initial workshop settings), sounded stronger and more musically
coherent and grounded although this element was compromised slightly as their
A second general finding was that the use of conceptual and theoretical knowledge to
scaffold the improvisational skill at the beginning was not particularly helpful to the
motor process that is the primary driver of schema development. In other words,
transferred to motor schemata, and only after this is established does theoretical
The schema for imagination to muscle movement again seems to be naturally present
A third finding was that individuals perceive and understand what they have learned
very differently, even though they may have assimilated and produce similar skills.
This finding is based on the many different responses to the question (what have you
learned today? ) asked during a workshop where only one participant answered with a
response relating to the material that was actually taught. This might be an issue of
individual brain operates but in any case, the schema appears to be idiosyncratic and
314
A fourth finding is that the schema appears to be self-organising given that the
improviser is practising solving the associated problems, so that it can organise and
elements of a successful solo come together periodically (often to the surprise of the
student), and then disintegrate again, until the schema is stabilised and well
help with this stabilising process as Bernstein and Schmidt both predicted in schema
theory. Extremely high levels of concentration are required in order for the brain to
juggle and organise the various parameters: (aural, motor, kinaesthetic, visual and
conceptual), and the process is mostly unconscious and difficult to verbalise on the
underlying reason for a process that is almost beyond their conscious control
(excepting the will and motivation to do it). Likewise, peer learning, imitation and
A fifth finding is that (unsurprisingly) unless the sound and feel of the music has been
for them to improvise in the jazz genre; hence the importance of listening and
occurring skill that largely remains dormant due to the educational and cultural norms
315
already present because there is much evidence to suggest that the brain is organised
in a schema based way: i. e. all skills from walking to thinking are learned in a
physically based way and are adaptable to the unique and individual circumstances we
come across through life. When improvisation is based around melody and voice
improvised melodic line (we have after all, a huge repertoire of songs to draw on).
However, all the musicians involved in the research project had difficulties in
improvising a similar line on an instrument without any kind of framework and the
process had to be learned rather than just woken or unlocked. The implication is that
if improvisation was begun at the start of learning the instrument and in tandem with
the learning of notation, then it would also be a prevalent and natural skill. The
problem of `translating' aurally imagined or sung sounds onto the instrument for
classically trained musicians is that they have habitually triggered motor patterns from
visual notation and this process has to be altered so that sounds move the muscles.
cases, hinders rather than scaffolds improvisational process at the beginning, is also
much theoretical information, particularly in harmony, before any real work begins on
improvisation per se. To contrast this with more traditional methods, historically,
most players learned by aurally transcribing solos from records, which would tend to
strengthen the aural to motor and muscle processes. There is no doubt that conceptual
necessary at some point in the learning process, but perhaps beginners need to focus
316
The idiosyncratic way that students perceived what they learned has implications for
all teachers and suggests that a number of multi-modal and very different approaches
to teaching the same subject would be most beneficial to students. It also has
implications for assessment if students have difficulties explaining clearly what they
have learned: learning process that cannot be verbalised requires different kinds of
evaluation.
The apparently self-organising nature of the schema also has significant implications
for teaching, which suggest that more effort should be put into creating the right
control of the student. Rather than focusing on outcomes or targets involving the
spent in creating physical and psychological environments in which the brain can
Of the many issues that remain unresolved (along with the many questions still to be
answered) about a schema theory, two seem to be highly significant. Firstly, the
physiological or neurological level. I have made the case in this thesis that the schema
is a systems property and given that it could be called up by any set of neurons in a
recreative memory model, i. e. never in the same way or place twice, it is unlikely that
any proof of its existence will be found in the near future. Yet, although it is hard to
equally difficult to envisage how dynamic and adaptable motor and thought process
317
could work without some kind of schema structure. Greater understanding of memory
is neededand in particular how and why memory is altered on one occasion and not
another, if we are to fully comprehend how the schema can be both a stable
framework for the recreation of a thought or skill and adaptable to new ideas or
elements, at the same time. What happens in the stabilising process (if indeed that is
what it is) and then in the altering process? How can it be both fluid and stable
simultaneously?
Lastly, the view put forward in this thesis, is that the brain appears to work in an
improvisatory way, based on assemblages of schemata that have been learned and
have profound implications for the ways in which we learn and educate others.
318
References
Adelman, C., Kemmis, S. & Jenkins, D. (1980). Rethinking case study: notes from the
Second Cambridge Conference. In H. Simons (Ed), Towards a Science of the
Singular. Centre for Applied Research in Education, University of East Anglia, 45-
61.
Aebersold, Jamey & Stone, Ken. (Transcribers). (1978). Charlie Parker Omnibook
(transposed for Bb instruments). New York: Music People.
Aiken, Jim. (1980). Interview with Bill Evans in Contemporary Keyboard Magazine,
June.
Anderson, John R. (1980) Cognitive Psychology and its Implications. San Francisco:
W. H. Freeman.
Arbib, Michael A. & Hesse, Mary B. (1986). The Construction of Reality. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Baker, David. (1980). Improvisation, its Nature and Practice. Chicago: Maher.
Baker, David. (1988). Arranging and Composing for Small Ensembles. California:
Alfred Publishing.
Baker, David. (1989). Jazz Pedagogy: a comprehensive method for teacher and
student. California: Alfred Publishing.
319
Baruch, G. (1981). Moral tales: parent's stories of encounters with the health
profession. Sociology of Health and Illness; 3: 275-96.
Berthoz, Alain. (2000). The Brain's Sense of Movement. Cambridge Mass: Harvard
University Press.
Bryman, Alan. (1988). Quantity and Quality in Social Research. London: Unwin
Hyman.
Campbell, F. A., and Ramey, C.T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual
and academic achievement: a follow-up study for children from low-income families.
In Child Development, 65,684-698.
Chomsky, Noam. (1968). Language and Mind. USA: Harcourt Brace and World Inc.
Clinton, George. (1974). Guitar Magazine. June edition. Interview with Joe Pass.
Cohen, Louis, Manion, Lawrence & Morrison, Keith. (2000). Research Methods in
Education. (2ndEdition). London and New York: Routledge/Falmer.
Cohen, Louis and Manion, Lawrence. (1994). Research Methods in Education. (4th
Edition). London: Routledge.
320
Coker, Jerry. (1964). Improvising Jazz. Englewood Cliffs New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Coker, Jerry, Casale, J., Campbell, G., & Greene, J. (1970). Patterns for Jazz.
Indiana: Studio P/R.
Dobbins, Bill. (1978). The Contemporary Jazz Pianist, 4 vols. New York: Charles
Colin.
Edelman, Gerald. (1992). Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On Matters of Mind. London:
Allen Lane.
Edelman, Gerald & Tononi Giulio. (2000). Consciousness: How Matter Becomes
Imagination. Allen Lane: the Penguin Press.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate
practice in the acquisition of expert performance. In Psychological Review, 100 (3),
363-406.
Fiske, Alan P. (1991a). Structures of Social Life: The Four Elementary Forms of
Human Relations. New York: Free Press
321
Fiske, Alan P. (1992). The Four Elementary Forms of Sociality: Framework for a
Unified Theory of Social Relations. In Psychological Review, Vol 99 No 4,689-723
Fiske, Alan, P. (1998). Human Sociality. In International Society for the Study of
Personal Relationships Bulletin, 14(2), 4-9.
Fiske, Alan P., & Haslam, Nick (1998). Prerequisites for Satisfactory Relationships.
In Luanna H. Meyer, Hyun-Sook Park, Marquita Grenot-Scheyer, Irene S. Schwartz,
& Beth Harry, (Eds). Making Friends: The Influences of Culture and Development
(pp. 385-392). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks
Fitts, P.M. & Peterson, J.R. (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor responses.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(2): 103-112.
Fodor, Jerry. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge MA: Harvard University
Press.
Gallese, V., Keysers, C. and Rizzalotti, G. (2004). A Unifying View of the Basis of
Social Cognition. In Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol 8, pages 396-403.
Ghez, Claude. (1991). Motor Systems of the Brain, Part IV. In Eric Kandel, James
Schwartz & Thomas Jessell (Eds), Principles of Neuroscience. (3rd edition). London:
Prentice Hall International.
Gibson, James J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. New Jersey:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Giddins, Gary. (1987). Celebrating Bird: The Triumph of Charlie Parker. New York:
Beechtree Books.
322
Grafton S.T., Mazziotta J.C., Woods R. P., Phelps M. E. (1992). Human functional
anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115:565-587
Green, Lucy. (2002). How Popular Musicians Learn: A Way Ahead for Music
Education. USA and England: Ashgate.
Guiffre, Jimmy. (1969). Jazz Phrasing and Interpretation. New York: Associated
Music Publishers.
Head, Henry & Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory Disturbances from Cerebral Lesions. In
Brain, Oxford, 34: 102.
Hennessy, Brian. (1985). Jazz Journal. June edition. Interview with Bill Evans.
Hitchcock, G. & Hughes, D. (1989). Research and the Teacher. London: Routledge.
Hitchcock, G. & Hughes, D. (1995). Research and the Teacher. (2nd Edition).
London: Routledge.
Johnson, Mark. (1987). The Body in the Mind-The Bodily Basis of Meaning,
Imagination and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kandel, Eric. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, M. Eds. (2000). Principles of Neural
Science. Stamford, CT: Appleton and Lange.
Kant, Immanuel. (1787). Critique of Pure Reason (Trans. Norman Kemp Smith) New
York: St. Martins Press. (Reprint 1968).
32)
Kelly, A. V. (1995). The Curriculum-Theory and Practice. London: Paul Chapman
Chapter 3.
Kemmis, S. & McTaggert, R. (Eds). (1992). The Action Research Planner, (3a
edition). Geelong, Victoria, Australia: Deakin University Press.
Krakauer, John W., Pine, Zachary M., Ghilardi, Maria-Felice, Ghez, Claude. (2000).
Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching
Trajectories. In Journal of Neuroscience; 20: (23) 8916-8924.
Lerdahl, Fred & Jackendoff, Ray. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music.
Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press.
Lewin, Roger. (1992) Complexity: Life at the Edge of Chaos. London: Macmillan,
Lincoln, Yvonna S. & Guba, Egon G. (1985). Naturalistic Inquiry. Newbury Park,
California: Sage Publications.
Lincoln, Yvonna S. & Guba, Egon G. (1986). But is it rigorous? Trustworthiness and
authenticity in naturalistic enquiry. In D. D. Williams (Ed) Naturalistic Evaluation.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 73-84.
Lord, C.G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: Two modes of processing
social information. In Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 38,357-269.
Luria, Alexander. (1968). The Mind of a Mnemonist. New York: Basic Books.
324
Lyons, Len. (1975). New Intuitions. Interview with Bill Evans in Downbeat Magazine
at the Monterey Jazz Festival.
Mandler Matter, Jean. (1984). Stories, Scripts and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory.
Hillsdale New Jersey & London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Markus, H., and Sentis, K. P. (1982). The self in social information processing. In
J.Suls (Ed) Psychological perspectives on the self. Vol 1. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Mason, Thom D. (1981). Ear Training for Improvisers: A Total Approach. CA:
Alfred Publishing Co.
Mehegan, John. F. (1959). Jazz Improvisation: Tonal and Rhythmic Principles. Vol
1. New York: Watson Guptial.
Mehegan, John. (1962). Jazz Rhythm- the Improvised Line. New York: Watson-
Guptial.
Mehegan, John. (1964). Swing and Early Progressive Piano Styles- Jazz
Improvisation Vol 3. New York: Watson Guptial.
Merriam, S.B. (1988). Case Study Research in Education. San Francisco: Jossey
Bass.
Montgomery, Wes. (1959). `Round Midnight (LP vinyl recording). On The Wes
Montgomery Trio. Riverside RLP 1156.
325
Most, Sam. (1980). Metamorphosis-Transformation of the Jazz Solo. California:
Professional Music Products Inc.
Niesser, Ulrich. (1976). Cognition and Reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 54.
Nisbet, J. & Watt, J. (1984) Case study. In J. Bell, T. Bush, A. Fox, J.Goodey, &
S.Goulding (Eds), Conducting Small-scale Investigations in Educational
Management. London: Harper and Row, 79-92.
Nolte, John. (2002). The Human Brain: An Introduction to its Functional Anatomy.
(5th edition). USA: Mosby. I
Nolte, John. & Angevine, Jay. (2000). The Human Brain in Photographs and
Diagrams. (2°d edition). USA: Mosby.
Paivio, Allen. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston.
Paivio, Allen and Lambert, W. (1981). Dual coding and bi-lingual memory. In
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20,532-539.
Paivio, Allen. (2006). Mind and its evolution; a dual -coding theoretical
interpretation. Mahwah New Jersey: Laurence Eribaum Associates Inc.
Piaget, Jean. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children (Margaret Cook Trans. ).
New York: International Universities Press.
Piaget, Jean & Inhelder, B. (1969). The Psychology of the Child. New York: Basic
Books. (Original work published in 1966)
Piaget, Jean. (1971). Mental Imagery in the Child Vol VIII. London: Routledge.
(Reprinted 1997).
Plato. (1981) Meno. (Trans G. Grube). New York: Hackett Publishing Co. Inc.
Powney, J. & Watts, M. (1987). Interviewing in Educational Research. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul.
326
Pressing, Jeff. (1988). Improvisation: Methods and Models, in Generative Processes
in Music. (Ed) John Sloboda. Oxford: Clarendon.
Pressing, Jeff. (1999b). Cognitive complexity and the structure of musical patterns.
Proceedings of the 4`h Conference of the Australasian Cognitive Science Society.
Newcastle, New South Wales, Australia: University of Newcastle.
Rao, S.M., Binder J.R., Bandetti, P., Hammeke, T. A., Yetkin, F., Jesmanowicz, A.,
Lisk, 1., Morris, G., Mueller, W., Estkowski, I., Wong, E., Haughton, V., Hyde, J.,
(1993). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of complex human movements.
Neurology, 43,2311-2318.
Reece, Ian & Walker Stephen. (2000). Teaching, Training and Learning a Practical
Guide. Tyne and Wear: Business Education Publishers Ltd.
Riding, Richard & Rayner, Stephen. (2000). Cognitive Styles and Learning
Strategies- Understanding style differences in learning and behaviour. London: David
Fulton Publishers.
Roberts, Howard & Hagburg, Gary. (1989). The Praxis System: Guitar Compendium:
technique/improvisation/musicianship/theory. Tubingen, Germany: Advance Music.
Robson, Colin. (1993). Real World Research. Oxford, UK, and Cambridge MA:
Blackwell.
Rummelhart, David. (1975). Notes on a Schema for Stories. In Daniel Bobrow and A.
Collins (Eds), Representation and Understanding. New York: Academic Press, (211-
36)
327
Rummelhart, David. (1977). Introduction to Human Information Processing. New
York: John Wiley and Sons.
Russell, George. (1959). The Lydian Chromatic Concept of Tonal Organisation for
Improvisation. New York: Concept Publications.
Russell, Ross. (1973). Bird Lives-The high life and hard tines of Charlie Parker.
New York: Da Capo. (Reprinted 1996).
Schank, Roger & Abelson, Robert. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding.
Hillsdale New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schmidt, Richard, A. (2003). Motor schema theory after 27 years; Reflections and
Implications for a new theory. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74,366-
375.
Shadwick, Keith. (2002). Bill Evans- Everything Happens to Me. San Francisco:
Backbeat Books.
Shapiro, Nat & Hentoff, Nat. (1955). Hear Me Talkin' to Ya: the story ofjazz as told
by the men who made it. New York: Dover Publications Inc.
328
Silverman, David. (1985). Qualitative methodology and sociology. Ist Edition,
Aldershot: Gower.
Silverman, David. (1993). Integrating qualitative data: methods for analysing talk,
text and interaction. London: Sage Publications.
Sloboda, John & Davidson, Jane, W. (1996). The Young Performing Musician. In
Musical Beginnings (Eds) John Sloboda & Irene Deliege. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Smith Gregory Eugene. (1983). Homer, Gregory and Bill Evans? The Theory of
Formulaic Composition in the Context of Jazz Piano Improvisation. PhD thesis
Harvard University.
Smith, Jonathan. A., Harre, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (Eds). (1995). Rethinking
Methods in Psychology. London: Sage Publications.
Spradley, J.P. (1980). Participant Observation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston.
Stanley, L. & Wise, S. (1983). Breaking out: feminist consciousness and feminist
research. London: Routledge and Keegan Paul.
Strong, P. (1979a). The ceremonial order of the clinic. London: Routledge and
Keegan Paul.
Sturman, A. (1999). Case study methods. In J.P. Keeves and G. Lakomski (Eds)
Issues in Educational Research. Oxford: Elsevier Science Ltd., 103-12.
329
Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., and Picton, T. W. (1995). A multi-
disciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. In J. Grafman, K. Hollyoak
and F. Boller (Eds), Structure andfiinctions of the human prefrontal cortex. Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences, 279: 191-211.
Sudnow, David. (1978). Ways of the Hand: The Organisation of Improvised Conduct.
Cambridge Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Swann, W. B. Jnr and Read, S.J. (1981a). Acquiring self knowledge: the search for
feedback that fits. In Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 41,
1119-1128.
Swann, W. B. Jnr and Read, S.J. (1981b). Self-verification processes: how we sustain
our self-conceptions. In Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 17,351-372.
Thompson, R. F. (2000). The Brain -a Neuroscience Prirner. (3`d Edition). New York:
Worth Publishers.
Thompson, V., and Paivio, A. (1994). Memory for pictures and sounds: Independence
of auditory and visual codes. In Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48,
380-398.
Toga, A. W. & Mazziotta, J.C. (2000). Brain Mapping: The Systems. California:
Academic Press
330
Whiting, H. T. A. (Ed) (1984). Human Motor Actions, Bernstein Reassessed. New
York: North.
Wise, R.J., Chollet, F., Hadar, U., Friston, K., Hoffner, E., Frackowiak, R. (1991).
Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word
retrieval. In Brain 114,1803-1817.
Yin, R. K. (1984). Case Study Research Design and Methods. Beverly Hills: Sage
Publications.
Young, Lester. (1956). Downbeat Magazine. July edition. Interview with Lester
Young, interviewer unknown. From the web: www. downbeat. com/archive
Zatorre R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., and Evans A. C. (1996a).
Hearing in the Mind's Ear; A PET investigation of musical imagery and perception.
In Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8,29-46.
Zeki, Semir. (1993). A Vision of the Brain: The Visible World and the Cortex. UK:
Blackwell Science.
331
Bibliography
Aiello, Rita, (Ed). (1994). Musical Perceptions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Anderson, John R. (1980). Cognitive Psychology and its Implications. San Francisco:
W. H. Freeman.
Austin, James. (1998). Brain in Overview-The Large of It' Part 111in Zen and the
Brain. Massachusetts: MIT Press.
Bachmann, Marie. (1991). Dalcroze Today: An Education Through and Into Music.
Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Bailey, David. (1980). Improvisation: its nature and practice in music. London:
Moorland.
Barrett, Margaret. (1996). Music Education and the natural learning model. In Gary
Spruce (Ed), Teaching Music. London: Routledge.
Braz, G. F., & Colley, T. J. (Eds). (1997). Shadows in the Field: New Perspectives for
Fieldwork in Ethnomusicology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Berliner, Paul. (1978). The Soul of the Mbira: Music and Traditions of the Shona
People of Zimbabwe. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Blackmore, Susan. (1999). The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Calvin, William. (1995). Cortical Columns, Modules, and Hebbian Cell Assemblies.
In Michael A. Arbib (Ed), The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks..
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, a Bradford Book. 269-272.
Calvin, William H. (1996). The Cerebral Code- Thinking a Thought in the Mosaic of
the Mind. Cambridge Mass.: A Bradford Book, The MIT Press.
Calvin, William H. (1996). How Brains Think- Evolving Intelligence Then and Now.
New York: Basic Books.
332
Carl Rodgers, (1998). The Carl Rogers Reader. Constable: London.
Choksy, Lois. (1974). The Kodaly Method- Comprehensive Music Education from
Infant to Adult. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall Inc. Englewood Cliffs.
Clarke, Donald. (2002). Billie Holiday: Wishing on the Moon. New York: Da Capo.
Claxton, Guy. (1989). Growth Points in Cognition. London and New York:
Routledge.
Coker, Jerry. (1975). The Jazz Idiom. Englewood Cliffs New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Cook, Nicolas. & Everist, M. (1999). Rethinking Music. Oxford and New York:
Oxford University Press.
Deliege, Irene & Sloboda, John. (Eds). (1997). Musical Beginnings: Origins and
Development of Musical Competence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
De Mallet Burgess, Thomas. & Skillsbeck, Nicolas. (2000). Singing and Acting
Handbook-Games and Exercises for the Performer. New York: Routledge.
Dean, Roger. (1989). Creative Improvisation. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.
Finke, R. A., Ward, Thomas B., & Smith, S. M., (1992). Creative Cognition- Theory,
Research, and Applications. Cambridge, Mass.: A Bradford Book, The MIT Press.
Fitts, P.M. and Peterson, J.R. (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor
responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67 (2): 103-112.
Fodor, Jerry (2001). The Mind Doesn't Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of
Computational Psychology. Cambridge Massachusetts.
Fontana, David. (1981). Psychology for Teachers. (Third Edition 1995). London:
Macmillan.
Ford, Alun. (1997). Antony Braxton- Creative Music Continuums. Devon, England:
Stride Publications.
333
Gardner, Howard. (1993). Multiple Intelligences: The Theory in Practice. New York:
Basic Books.
Gathercole, Susan E., & Baddeley, Alan D. (1995). Working Memory and Language.
Hove UK and Hillsdale USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Greenhough, W., Black, J.E., Wallace, C. S. (1993). Neural Basis of Learning from:
Experience and Brain Development. In Mark H. Johnson, Yuko Munakata, & Rick
O. Gilmore (Eds), Brain Development in Cognition Chapter 11. (2"d Edition). Oxford:
Blackwell.
Heron, John. (1989). The Facilitators Handbook. London: Kogan Page, New York:
Nichols Publishing.
Holiday, Billie. (1956). TheLady Sings the Blues. New York: Double Day.
Howell, P., West, R. & Cross, I. (Eds). (1991). Representing Musical Structure.
London: Academic Press.
Illich, Ivan. (1970). Deschooling Society USA: Harper and Row, 1973 England:
Penguin.
Jaques-Dalcroze, Emile. (1930). Eurythmics, Art and Education. New York: Ayer.
Kelso, J. A. Scott. (1995). Dynamic Patterns. Cambridge Mass: MIT Bradford Books.
Kelso, J.A. Scott., Tuller, B., & Fowler, C.A. (1982). The Functional Specificity of
Articulatory Control and Co-ordination. In Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 72, S 103.
334
Kodaly, Zoltan. (1944). Collection ofMelodiesfor Schools (1-11).Budapest.
Lakoff, George. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things -What Categories
Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leman, Marc. (1995). Music and Schema Theory. Heidelberg: Springer Verlag.
Mingus, Charles. (1971). Beneath the Underdog. New York: Alfred A, Knopf.
Nelson, Oliver. (1966). Patterns for Improvisation. New Albany IN: Jamey Aebersold
Page, Tim (Ed. ) (1987). The Glenn Gould Reader. London: Faber.
Pepper, Art & Pepper, Laurie. (1980). Straight Life: the Story of Art Pepper. USA:
Collier Mac.
335
Pratt, George. (1999). Aural Awareness-Principles and Practice. Milton Keynes:
Open University Press.
Ricker, Ramon. (1976). Pentatonic Scalesfor Jazz Improvisation. Hialeah, FL: Studio
Publications.
Rogers. Carl. R, & Stevens, B. (1967). Person to Person: The Problem of Being
Human. London: Souvenir Press.
Rose, Steven. (1993). The Making of Memory: From Molecules to Mind. London:
Anchor.
Scruton, Roger (1997). The Aesthetics of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Slonimsky, Nicolas. (1947). Thesaurus of Scales and Melodic Patterns. New York:
Coleman Ross.
Wallin, Nils C., Marker, Bjorn. Brown, Steve. (Eds). (2000) The Origins of Music.
Cambridge, Massachusetts. A Bradford Book, The MIT Press.
336
Wragg, Edward C. (1994). An Introduction to Classroom Observation. London:
Routledge.
Wragg, Edward C. (2002). The Art and Science of Teaching and Learning. London:
Routledge.
337