Bleile, Ken Mitchell - The Late Eight-Plural Publishing (2018)
Bleile, Ken Mitchell - The Late Eight-Plural Publishing (2018)
Bleile, Ken Mitchell - The Late Eight-Plural Publishing (2018)
Third Edition
The Late Eight
Third Edition
e-mail: [email protected]
website: http://www.pluralpublishing.com
Every attempt has been made to contact the copyright holders for material
originally printed in another source. If any have been inadvertently over-
looked, the publishers will gladly make the necessary arrangements at the
first opportunity.
Preface vii
Contributors ix
v
vi The late eight
The first edition of The Late Eight provided clinical resources to help clini-
cians and students evaluate and treat late-acquired sounds. I likened the
resources to all-purpose tools — the clinical equivalents of a carpenter’s
hammers, screwdrivers, bolts, paintbrushes, and ladder. The third edition
keeps (and slightly modifies) the clinical resources while expanding the
tool kit to include new essential clinical concepts, including evidence-based
practice, Spanish-influenced English, the curriculum, and a new treatment
model. Chapter 1, A Late 8 Update, offers short question-and-answer sec-
tions on 18 clinical topics, ranging from practical issues (Any suggestions
for practicing speech?) to thoughts on current controversies (Do the Late 8
belong in school? Is there a best age to begin speech treatment?). Chapter 2,
Using Evidence to Guide Clinical Practice, describes tools to discover the
evidence base for evaluating and treating late-acquired sounds. Chapter 3,
Late Eight en español, is the first chapter in our professional literature to
focus exclusively on late-acquired sounds in Spanish-speaking students.
Chapter 4, Speech Sound Disorders, Literacy, and Curriculum, demonstrates
the importance of speech treatment in school settings. Chapter 5, Motor
Learning Guided Therapy, which appeared in the second edition, shows
principles that underlie decisions within an exciting new clinical approach.
Along with the new chapters, the third edition replaces the CD of previous
editions with an expanded and flexible interactive companion website. The
authors of the third edition hope you find these changes beneficial in your
clinical work with your students.
Ken M. Bleile
April 2017
vii
Contributors
ix
Part I
New Essentials
Chapter One
A Late 8 Update
Ken M. Bleile
This chapter discusses frequently asked questions about the Late 8. Topics
range from practical concerns (What are good phonetic placement and
shaping techniques for [r]?) to theoretical issues (Do the Late 8 belong in
school?). You can read the questions either in sequence or individually. The
format for discussion of each topic is the same: question, short answer, dis-
cussion — sometimes brief, other times longer — followed by a brief summary.
Questions:
n What are the Late 8?
n Why are the Late 8 late?
n Do the Late 8 belong in school?
n Should a clinician treat the Late 8 using an articulation approach?
n What major speech errors affect the Late 8?
n Any suggestions for practicing speech?
n What if a production is not quite right?
n Is there a best age to begin speech treatment?
n What are key environments?
n Should a student with good speech perception receive discrimination
training?
n What are phonetic placement and shaping techniques?
n What are good phonetic placement and shaping techniques for [r]?
n Are non-speech oral motor exercises different than phonetic place-
ment and shaping?
n How many sounds can I treat in a session?
n Should a clinician provide treatment for a stimulable sound?
3
4 The late eight
n What if you hear something but aren’t sure how to transcribe it?
n Any therapy tips for a speech-language pathologist (SLP) student or
a new clinician?
n What is the most important reason a person improves in treatment?
For convenience, here is the same list divided by the nature of the question:
practical or conceptual.
Short Answer
Discussion
The above list is a reanalysis of the Iowa articulation norms project and its
Nebraska replication (Smit, Hand, Frelinger, Bernthal, & Byrd, 1990). Within
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 5
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
A table and a concept help explain why these eight consonants are late.
Table 1–1 shows English consonants typically acquired by children 24 months
or younger. The table lists sound classes from most closed (no distance
between articulators) to most open (the most distance between articulators).
Most Closed
Stops b d g p t k m n ŋ
Affricates
Fricatives
Liquids
Glides h w j
Most Open
6 The late eight
Viewed this way, children prefer (or at least acquire at an earlier age) conso-
nants with complete closure of the articulators (stops) or with the articulators
spread as wide as possible (glides). That is, if consonant classes are viewed
as falling on a continuum from most closed to most open, children’s early
consonants occur at the ends of the continuum. Midpoint consonant classes
(affricates, fricatives, liquids) are later acquisitions. In all the languages with
which I am familiar, children typically acquire endpoints (stops and glides)
before midpoints (McLeod, 2007, 2011).
What is striking is that the Late 8 are all “midway” sounds — that is, they
are consonants produced midway from most closed (stops) to most open
(glides). [tS ] begins as a stop and ends with friction. [θ ð s z S ] are made
with friction throughout, and [l r] are made with less closure than fricatives,
resulting in a kind of liquid turbulence in the air. As Table 1–2 shows, being
a midway sound is not sufficient to make a sound a late acquisition (for
example, [f] is a relatively early acquisition in English). In other words, the
pattern is that all the Late 8 are midway sounds but that being a midway
sound is not sufficient to make a consonant a late acquisition.
Summary
The Late 8 are midway consonants made with the mouth with the articulators
either farther apart (glides) or entirely touching (oral and nasal stops).
Short Answer
Most Closed
Stops b d g p t k m n ŋ
Affricates tS dZ
Fricatives f v θ ð s z S
Liquids l r
Glides h w j
Most Open
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 7
Discussion
Speech sound disorders are frequently occurring, which makes their care
expensive and time-consuming. A generation ago, school districts often
asked, why treat speech sound disorders when other children have broader
developmental needs? A question being asked more frequently today is,
why treat speech when schools struggle to meet new curriculum standards?
Questions such as these often pit one group of children with developmental
difficulties against other groups, while also pitting a family’s legal right to
developmental services against the need of society to pay for services.
Six Reasons
At least six reasons support why we should not let the Late 8 become school
dropouts:
Summary
Difficulty with late acquired sounds is the world’s most frequently occur-
ring communication disorder, affecting both children and adults learning
English as a non-native language. The school is the natural habitat for the
treatment of the Late 8 because speech contributes to school success and
problems in speech may result in school difficulties, both academically and
socially. Because of the close relationship between speech sound disorders
and educational success, in treating the Late 8 the goal is speech, the avenue
is language, and the vehicle is the curriculum.
Short Answer
Not necessarily.
Discussion
On first consideration, it may seem that treatment of the Late 8 lends itself to
an articulatory approach, since midpoint sounds appear harder for children to
pronounce than those on either end of the continuum (see question Why are the
Late 8 late?). However, this view does not do justice to the complexity of treating
a person with a developmental difficulty, including those in the speech area.
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 9
Cause
Communication
Speech treatment entails not simply knowing a problem’s cause, but also
understanding how the difficulty affects communication. Speech for com-
munication requires both language knowledge (phonology) and production
ability (articulation). To give just one illustration, a student who communi-
cates through speech needs to know the role of [s] in language as well as
how to perceive and move the articulators in such a way to produce [s].
Because the ultimate goal of speech treatment is to improve communica-
tion, speech treatment benefits from inclusion of both speech components,
articulation and phonology. Articulation provides the motor practice neces-
sary for speech to occur; phonology provides the links from the articulatory
system to the language system, which in turn makes it possible for a student
to function socially and educationally. To give an analogy: without language,
speech is like a train that travels fast but has no place of arrival; speech
10 The late eight
without the articulation component is like knowing the destination you want
to reach but not having a vehicle to get there.
Person
Attached to every speech difficulty is a real person. Along with cause and com-
munication, success or failure in speech treatment also depends on person
variables, including a student’s interests, motivation, language abilities, views
about speech therapy, peer relations, and personality. And, of course, along
with the student comes family, the school, and race/ethnicity.
Summary
Short Answer
The major errors are lip rounding, lisping, blading, and lateralization.
Discussion
Early speech errors tend to (though not always, of course) replace mid
sounds with sounds on either end of the openness continuum (see ques-
tion Why are the Late 8 late?). To illustrate, stops may replace [s] and other
affricates, and glides may replace liquids. Later in development, even when
students may produce midpoint sounds, they often produce them in differ-
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 11
ent ways, prominent productions being lip rounding, lisping, blading, and
lateralization.
Lip Rounding
[r] is prone to being pronounced with lip rounding, sometimes called [w]
coloring because it may sound like a cross between [r] and [w]. The usual
transcription of lip rounding on [r] is a small w under the [r], like this: [ r ]
Lisping
[s] and [z] (and to a lesser extent other of the Late 8 as well) are prone to
being pronounced with the tongue tip between the front teeth, similar or
identical to [θ] or [ð]. The usual symbol for lisping is a small tooth under the
sound, like this: [ ]
Blading
[s] and [z] (and to a lesser extent other of the Late 8 as well) are prone to
being pronounced with the blade of the tongue body raised toward the roof
of the mouth, giving it a [s] quality. The symbol for bladed is a small half
circle (representing the roof of the mouth) under the sound, like this: [ ]
Lateralization
[s] and [z] (and, as above, to a lesser extent many other of the Late 8) are
prone to being pronounced with the air flowing over the sides of the tongue
rather than over its top. Lateralized sounds have an [l]-like quality, though
the lateralization can sound bladed, especially if the lateralized sound is
unvoiced. The symbol for lateralization is an [l] placed before the sound,
like this: [ls] []
Summary
The Late 8 are affected by many different speech errors. Highly frequent
errors include lip rounding, lisping, blading, and lateralization.
Short Answer
Discussion
Speed
Avoid having a student practice speech by saying sounds and words very
slowly. Extremely slow speech employs feedback mechanisms not typically
used in speech.
Vowels
When possible, work on a treatment sound from the level of words to con-
versation. Those levels have functional value to a student; they promote
generalization because students speak them outside of therapy, and they
appear to be units of motor planning.
Self-Monitoring
Self-Correction
Introduce Distractions
range from throwing a ball back and forth while practicing speech, holding
group treatment, having the student make oral reports to you containing the
treatment sound, and providing treatment or generalization in the classroom.
Communicative Value
Functional Activities
People of Importance
When to Dismiss
Follow-Up
Whenever possible follow up after dismissal to make sure a student has not
regressed in his or her speech.
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
Summary
A simple 1-2-3 scale usually is sufficient to describe clinical progress that has
not yet reached the point where a production sounds entirely “right.”
Short Answer
Discussion
1. Brain development
2. First language
3. Second language
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 15
4. Disability
5. Brain injury
1. Brain Development
Many essential aspects of brain development are “front loaded” to occur early
in life, including:
2. First Language
Speech (even the Late 8) is the earliest acquired language area. These are the
ages at which the Late 8 are acquired by fully 75% of students (Smit, Hand,
Frelinger, Bernthal, & Byrd, 1990):
3. Second Language
4. Disability
Studies of children with Down syndrome offer further evidence of the early
acquisition of speech. As noted in several studies, children with Down syn-
drome may appear less intelligible at 10 years than when they were 5 (Miller,
1988, 1999). The explanation is that speech stops or slows in development
around 5 years, while other areas of language continue to develop. As other
areas develop — for example, a child develops a larger vocabulary and speaks
in longer sentences — speech remains relatively unchanged, resulting in a
child who sounds less intelligible when older than younger.
5. Brain Injury
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
Syllable Position
Vowel Influences
High Front Vowels. Production of Late 8 consonants produced near the alveo-
lar area is facilitated by high front vowels.
n [θ] as in thin
n [ð] as in these
n [s] as in see
n [z] as in zip
n [r] as in read
n Light [l] as in leaf
n [tS ] as in chew
n [ S ] as in shoe
n Dark [l] as in cool
Summary
Key environments are the mirror image of phonological processes. Key envi-
ronments derive from the same cross-linguistic and child language research
as phonological processes, differing from phonological processes only in
that their focus is phonetic contexts that may facilitate, rather than hinder,
speech production. Table 1–4 summarizes key environments for the Late 8,
organized by individual sound.
Short Answer
Discussion
Traditional Approach
A Cognitive Explanation
The answer may be that students both with and without speech difficul-
ties typically give little attention to their speech. The only difference between
the two is that we notice the lack of attention in students with speech
difficulties because their speech problem shines a light on their inattention.
Attention
Not paying attention to our speech is normal. To give just a few illustrations,
few people (other than speech-language pathologists) are aware that when
someone says [p] in consonant clusters, such as in spy, the sound is voice-
less and unaspirated, similar to a sound in Thai, while [p] beginning words
such as pie is aspirated. Similarly, few people realize that they pronounce [r]
in pride with little or no voicing, and typically pronounce [r] in bride with
voicing throughout. We fail to perceive these speech variations not because
our auditory mechanism is broken, but because we focus our attention on
communication.
Considered in this way, students’ “discrimination problems” may be dif-
ficulty with awareness, implying that the therapeutic challenge is to focus
a student’s attention on the difference between an intended pronunciation
and the actual sound that comes out of the mouth. An awareness exercise
is a little verbal tap on the shoulder to say, “Remember what sound you are
working on. Focus on what you are doing.”
Clinical Differences
n Metaphors
n Touch cues
n Mirrors
n Descriptions
n Demonstrations
n Phonetic placement and shaping
These are also excellent tools within a cognitive perspective and have wide
uses in many activities in this book. Less traditionally, activities that promote
language and reading also help focus a student on speech, including:
n Minimal pairs
n Deletions
n Self-correction
n Old way/new way
n Similar sound
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
This (or one of its many variants) is a popular technique to establish [k].
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the lower front teeth. (If
needed, a clinician can use a tongue depressor to keep the tongue in place.)
2. Ask the student to hump the back of the tongue and say [ku].
Shaping [ S ] to [s]
This phonetically clever shaping technique shows that sometimes a smile is
all you need.
Summary
Short Answer
Many good techniques exist. The website for this book lists over one hundred
phonetic placement and shaping techniques, including dozens for [r] and
vocalic [r].
Discussion
I receive more questions about phonetic placement and shaping of [r] than
any other sound. I do not believe one single best technique exists: there exist
24 The late eight
multiple ways to produce a sound and there are too many different student-
learning portraits for there to be “a best technique for everyone.” Here is my
personal list of favorite techniques:
1. Instruct the student to lie on his/her back, relax the mouth, and say [2]
or another vocalic [r].
The first technique is for retroflex [r] and the second for bunched [r]. Both
variants contain the following steps:
3. Tongue in track
4. Curl it back
n A relaxed tongue resting on the floor of the mouth (tuck the chin)
n Spread lips (make a grin)
n Position the tongue so its sides rest against the insides of the lower
teeth (tongue in track)
n Curl the tongue tip back (curl it back)
For bunched [r], instruct the student to place the sides of the tongue in the
track between the upper teeth and then curve the tongue tip down behind
the lower front teeth.
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
Differences
Testimonials
How can clinical testimonials exist that attest that students improve in speech
using non-speech oral motor approaches if clinical research strongly sug-
gests that non-speech oral motor approaches rely on faulty empirical and
theoretical foundations?
The answer may lie in testimonials, which highlight an important truth
about treatment: when a student improves, it is notoriously difficult to deter-
mine why he or she improved. Any treatment approach contains multiple
components, including the theory behind the approach, the student, the
setting, the clinician’s treatment skills, time of day, etc. Careful research is
required to determine how the many components in any given approach
alone or in conjunction with each other result in treatment change. For this
reason, testimonials are better at telling you “a student got better” than telling
you why a student got better.
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
Summary
No single answer holds for everyone. As with many aspects of the clinical
enterprise, clinical judgment is (and should be) the final arbitrator of how
many sounds to treat.
Short Answer
Discussion
The Positions
A Hypothesis
Summary
Short Answer
Welcome to my life.
Discussion
Summary
Short Answer
Over the years, I have had the pleasure to supervise many students and
new clinicians. The following distills what I have learned from watching the
progression of their clinical skills.
30 The late eight
Discussion
Recipes
Functional Activities
Talk openly and respectfully to a student about his or her speech. A student
needs to understand why he or she is in therapy to improve. Don’t let a
student’s speech difficulty become “the elephant in the room” that no one
mentions.
Success
Keep It Fun
Be interesting and energetic in therapy. If you have fun, a student is more apt
to have fun, too. But while having fun, remember that games and entertaining
activities are tools and that the purpose of therapy is to improve a student’s
speech.
Summary
Short Answer
Discussion
One of the most essential clinical questions is, why do people improve in
treatment? Though recognizing that a student’s improvement in treatment
probably depends on multiple interacting factors (including motivation,
a clinician’s skills, the treatment approach, family involvement, treatment
frequency, etc.), several researchers in psychology have focused on two vari-
ables, asking, what is the major driver of clinical success, the clinician or the
approach? (Lambert, 2004; Staines, 2008).
Approach or Clinician?
Research
Psychology) is that the “individual therapists’ personal skills rather than their
theoretical orientation” make the most difference in effectiveness of treat-
ment. Importantly, both the approach and the clinician contribute to clinical
success — that is, both the approach the clinician uses and the clinician’s
clinical skills are important. The approach contributes approximately 25% to
success and the clinician contributes the other 75%.
A Hypothesis
The reason a clinician is primary to clinical success is that both the student
and the clinician share the same neurological wiring for learning in a social
context. The reason a clinician is primary to treatment success— and it is
only my hypothesis — is that social relations play the same central role in
the treatment of speech sound disorders as they do in other types of clini-
cal treatment. In the treatment of speech sound disorders, approaches are
important tools, just like a painter’s brushes, pigments, and canvases. But
mastery of the tools will not result in a masterpiece. To achieve art, you need
a clinician.
Summary
Clinicians are more central to treatment success than the treatment approach.
Social relations may be crucial to treatment success because the student and
the clinician share the same neurological wiring for learning in a social context.
Conclusions
References
McLeod, S. (2002). Part I: The plethora of available data on children's speech devel-
opment ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing, 4, 141–147.
McLeod, S. (Ed.). (2007). The international guide to speech acquisition. Clifton Park,
NY: Thomson Delmar Learning.
McLeod, S. (2011). Laying the foundations for multilingual acquisition: An inter-
national overview of speech acquisition. Journal of Linguistics and Language
Teaching, 2, 53–71.
Pascallis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. (2002). Is face processing species specific
during the first year of life? Science, 296, 1321–1323.
Miller, J. (1988). The psychobiology of Down syndrome. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Miller, J. (1999). Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome.
Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Muttiah, N., Georges, K., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). Clinical and research perspec-
tives on nonspeech oral motor treatments and evidence-based practice. American
Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 20(1), 47–59.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2002). Digest for education statistics. U.S.
Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences.
Overby, M., Carrell, T., & Bernthal, J. (2007). Teachers’ perceptions of students with
speech sound disorders: A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Language, Speech,
and Hearing in Schools, 38(4), 327–341.
Paul, R., & Shriberg, L. (1982). Associations between phonology and syntax in
speech-delayed children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 536–547.
Powell, T. (1991). Planning for phonological generalization: An approach to treat-
ment target selection. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 1, 21–27.
Powell, T. W. (2009). Non-speech oral motor exercises: An ethical challenge. In
C. Bowen, Children’s speech sound disorders (pp. 199–202). Oxford, UK:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Powell, T., Elbert, M., & Dinnsen, D. (1991). Stimulability as a factor in phonologi-
cal generalization of misarticulating preschool children. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 34, 1318–1328.
Ruscello, D. M. (2008). Nonspeech oral motor treatment issues in children with
developmental speech sound disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 39, 380–391.
Ruscello, D. M., St. Louis, K., & Mason, N. (1991). School-aged children with pho-
nologic disorders: Coexistence with other speech/language disorders. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 236–242.
Schade, J. P., & van Groenigen, W. B. (1961). Structural organization of the human cere-
bral cortex. I. Maturation of the middle frontal gyrus. Acta Anatomica, 47, 72–111.
Shine, R. (1989). Articulatory production training: A sensory-motor approach. In N.
Creaghead, P. Newman, & W. Secord (Eds.), Assessment and remediation of articu-
latory and phonological disorders (pp. 355–359). Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill.
Shriberg, L. D., & Austin, D. (1998). The speech-language connection. Baltimore, MD:
Paul H. Brookes.
Shriberg, L., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1988). A follow-up study of children with phono-
logic disorders of unknown origin. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53,
144–155.
Shriberg, L., & Tomblin, B. (1999). Prevalence of speech delay in 6-year-old children
and comorbidity with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 42, 1461–1481.
CHAPTER 1: A Late 8 Update 35
Simonds, R. J., & Scheibel, A. B. (1989). The postnatal development of the motor
speech area: A preliminary study. Brain and Language, 37, 42–58.
Slater, S. (1992). Portrait of the professions. Asha, 34, 61–65.
Smit, A., Hand, L., Frelinger, J., Bernthal, J., & Byrd, A. (1990). The Iowa articula-
tion norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 55, 779–798.
Staines, G. (2008). The relative efficacy of psychotherapy: Reassessing the methods-
based paradigm. Review of General Psychology, 12(4), 330–343.
Stark, R., Bleile, K., Brandt, J., Freeman, J., & Vining, E. (1995). Speech-language out-
comes of hemispherectomy in children and young adults. Brain and Language,
51, 406–421.
Tyler, A., & Haskill, A. (2010). Morphosyntax intervention. In A. Williams, S. McLeod, &
R. McCauley, (Eds.). Interventions for speech sound disorders in children (pp. 355–
380). Baltimore, MA: Brookes.
Van Dyke, D. C., & Holte, L. (2003). Communication disorders in children. Pediatric
Annals, 32(7), 436.
Werker, J., & Tees, R. (2005). Speech perception as a window for understanding
plasticity and commitment in language systems of the brain. Developmental Psy-
chobiology, 46(3), 233–251.
Chapter Two
Using Evidence to
Guide Clinical Practice:
Considerations for
the Late Eight
Lauren K. Nelson
Introduction
Using evidence to guide practice has been a part of academic and clinical
education in speech-language pathology for many years. The need for an
evidence-based approach to decision making became even more prominent
in the mid-2000s (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [ASHA],
2004, 2005). This greater emphasis occurred in part because of increased
reliance on evidence in the field of medicine (ASHA, 2004). These changes in
the field of medicine have carried over to other health care fields. The greater
emphasis on evidence also occurred because of input from speech-language
pathologists (SLPs) in clinical practice. SLPs in both health care and school
settings experienced increased pressure to document that the services they
provided had supporting evidence.
When you consider the term “evidence,” perhaps the first thing that
comes to mind is research evidence. The use of reliable research evidence
is an important component of evidence-based practice (EBP). However, you
might better think of EBP as a decision-making process that incorporates the
best available research evidence, the SLPs’ own expertise, gained through
37
38 The late eight
both their educational and clinical experiences, and the input from the client
and/or his/her family (ASHA, 2004; Fey, Justice, & Schmitt, 2014). Dollaghan
(2007) used the phrase “external evidence” to refer to research evidence and
noted that the other sources, the SLP, client, and family, provided valuable
evidence as well. The focus of this chapter is on finding, evaluating, and
using external evidence, but we will keep in mind the equal importance of
evidence from clinical practice, the client, and family.
EBP Resources
and publish the results for others to use in the form of an “Evidence-Based
Systematic Review.” You can find a list of N-CEP’s systematic reviews at
the following website: http://www.asha.org/Research/EBP/EBSRs/. Unfortu-
nately, N-CEP had not published any systematic reviews that address children
with speech sound disorders (SSDs) at the time this chapter was written.
Evidence Maps
A second option from the ASHA website was more helpful. ASHA’s Evidence
Maps included the topic “Speech Sound Disorders” (http://www.asha.org/
Evidence-Maps/). These evidence maps comprised information for all three
components of EBP: research evidence, clinician expertise, and the client
and family perspective. If you go to this website and select the topic “Speech
Sound Disorders” and then select “External Scientific Evidence,” you will find
at least 20 sources, most of which are systematic reviews. Two articles from
this list stood out, a narrative review of research evidence for treatment of
children with SSDs (Baker & McLeod, 2011a) and a systematic review on
the topic of treatment intensity for children with SSDs (Kaipa & Peterson,
2016). We will explore the Baker and McLeod article in greater depth in
a later section of this chapter. If your client with an SSD had a specific
diagnosis, you might consider another topic, such as “Apraxia of Speech
(Childhood),” “Cerebral Palsy,” or “Cleft Lip and Palate” (http://www.asha
.org/Evidence-Maps).
single person. The lowest level of evidence occurs when empirical evidence
on a topic is lacking and individuals with strong credentials related to the
topic generate guidelines based on their collective expertise.
When you search for research evidence using speechBITE, I suggest you try
the easy to use advanced search option. Using this advanced search option,
you can specify the area of practice (e.g., speech, language, swallowing);
the type of intervention (e.g., speech/articulation/phonological therapy, lan-
guage therapy, fluency/stuttering therapy), population (e.g., autism spectrum
disorder, speech sound disorder [developmental], traumatic brain injury),
age group, and service delivery method (e.g., individual, group, parent).
You also have the option of skipping some of the options. For an initial
search on our topic of SSDs and the Late Eight consonants, you could try
speech/articulation/phonological therapy and speech sound disorder (devel-
opmental) and children. This search turned up 105 articles and that would
be an overwhelming number to read through. Seven of those articles were
systematic reviews, so a good starting place would be to focus just on those
seven articles. This search yielded the same 2 articles we identified from
the ASHA evidence maps search: Baker and McLeod (2011a) and Kaipa and
Peterson (2016).
Among the randomized control trials, two somewhat recent studies
stood out, a treatment study in which intelligibility was an outcome measure
(Lousada, Jesus, Hall, & Joffe, 2014) and a study of the effectiveness of treat-
ment delivered in a typical clinical setting (Broomfield & Dodd, 2011). These
latter two studies were of interest because they were relatively recent and
were not included in the Baker and McLeod narrative review.
Learn by Doing
If you have waited to try some of the searches covered in
the previous section, now would be a good time to stop
reading, get out your favorite device for browsing the Inter-
net, and try a search. First, type “http://speechbite.com” to
get to the speechBITE website. Then, just under the search
box, select the advanced search option. You should see a
series of drop-down boxes. Let’s try the following options:
(1) for Type of Intervention select Speech/Articulation/
Phonological Therapy; (2) for Within This Population, select
Hearing and Visual Impairment; and (3) for Age Group.
select Children. This search yielded 17 articles, 2 of which
were systematic reviews.
42 The late eight
Thus far our search for research evidence to support clinical decision making
for children with SSDs has yielded research that reflects the highest levels
of evidence, including systematic reviews and randomized treatment and
control group studies. This body of evidence showed that treatment for
SSDs was efficacious under the controlled conditions of research studies,
and in some cases, under the conditions present in typical clinical settings
(Broomfield & Dodd, 2013; McCleod & Baker, 2011a). What is less clear is
the extent to which the existing research studies included children with
speech errors on the Late Eight consonants. One approach to address
this issue would be to filter the studies we discovered through our previ-
ous searches according to the age of the participants. The rationale for
filtering by age is that research with older children, ages 5 and above, is
more likely to include participants with errors on the Late Eight Admittedly,
this is a somewhat arbitrary assumption, but information about age is
often available in the article abstracts or in the summaries from the sys-
tematic reviews. Our search for the best available evidence yielded system-
atic reviews as well as a variety of research studies that included children
ages 5 and above. The best research evidence for our purposes would
be studies that included the Late Eight consonants among the participants’
treatment targets. For the most part, the only way to identify this detail
about a study is to read the full research report. The evidence we discovered
is listed in Table 2–1. The table includes information, when available, about
the extent to which the study addressed treatment for errors on the Late
Eight.
One thing apparent in Table 2–1 was that research articles covering
treatment of SSDs do not always include information about the specific
speech sounds targeted in treatment. Nevertheless, the research evidence
provided some guidance regarding treatment of the Late Eight consonants.
If children have errors on multiple consonants, clinicians should consider
multiple oppositions treatment (Williams, 2000). This approach was effective
and comparatively efficient for children in more than one study (Allen, 2013;
Williams, 2000, 2005). Including either phonological awareness activities or
well-designed speech sound discrimination training in treatment appeared
to enhance treatment outcomes (Denne, Langdown, Pring, & Roy, 2005;
Rvachew, Nowak, & Cloutier, 2004). For children with both morphosyntactic
and phonological errors, either alternating treatment between morphosyn-
tactic and phoneme targets or targeting morphosyntactic errors first led to
greater overall improvement (Tyler, Lewis, Haskill, & Tolbert, 2002, 2003).
As noted in other reviews (Baker & McLeod, 2011b; Sugden, Baker, Munro,
& Williams, 2016), the considerable variability among studies and the many
different approaches to delivering treatment and selecting target sounds
made it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the best approaches
to treatment.
Table 2–1. Identified Sources of Evidence from 2000 to 2016 that Included Children Ages 5 and Above with Speech Sound
Disorders and/or Specified Target Sounds Among the Late Eight
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Baker & Narrative Multiple studies Ages 1;6 to 10;5 NA Children with SSDs
McLeod review/Ia across 134 studies benefited from treatment
(2011a)
Kaipa & Systematic Multiple studies Ages 2;6 to 8;2 NA Higher treatment intensity
Peterson review/Ia across 7 studies led to better outcomes
(2016)
Law, Garrett, Meta- Multiple studies Mean age 2;10 to NA Intervention effect was
& Nye (2004) analysis/ 6;10 across 6 studies significant for children with
45
Ia expressive phonological
disorders
McCauley Systematic Multiple studies Ages 1;3 to 16;11 NA No clear-cut evidence
et al. (2009) review/Ia across 5 studies supporting the use of
nonspeech oral motor
exercises to improve
speech production
Sugden et al. Systematic Multiple studies Ages 1;6 to 9;6 NA Evidence suggests that
(2016) review/Ia across 61 studies parents can be effective
intervention agents, but
the evidence covers many
different approaches and
the description of these
approaches lacks detail
continues
Table 2–1. continued
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Allen (2013) Randomized 55 children ages Multiple oppositions; Phoneme Children improved more
control trial/Ib 3;0 to 5;5 treatment frequency collapse (e.g., /g, when receiving treatment
1× or 3× per week, dZ, z, ð, dr, gr/) 3× per week compared with
and control 1× and when cumulative
intervention was the same
Broomfield & Randomized 730 children ages Usual speech and NA Children with language
Dodd (2011) control trial/Ib 0–16; including language services and speech disorders who
100 with speech averaging 5½ hours received treatment made
disorders ages in the 6 months significantly greater gains
5–11+ following the initial than no treatment control
46
assessment group
Denne et al. Randomized 20 children ages Phonological NA Children receiving treatment
(2005) control trial/Ib 5 to 7 randomly awareness scored significantly
assigned to treatment (TX) higher on phonological
treatment or in typical clinical awareness after treatment,
control group setting (i.e., 12 hours but not on literacy or
total) speech production tests
Rvachew Randomized 48 children in two Seven step TX from Later acquired/ Children treated on earlier
& Nowak control trial/Ib different groups imitated syllable least known (e.g., acquired sounds made
(2001) with mean ages to spontaneous /r, l, θ, S/ and significantly greater gains,
of 51.5 and 49.6 sentences earlier acquired but children treated on
months and most known later acquired sounds
(e.g., /t, k, l, v/ did make gains; authors
reported no differences in
generalization
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Rvachew Randomized 34 children in two Control group One outcome Experimental groups,
et al. (2004) control trial/Ib different groups received TX measure was receiving speech
with mean ages determined by percent correct perception training, made
of 52.9 and 50.3 the clinician (e.g., for “difficult” significantly greater gains
months cycles, tradition); consonants: /ŋ, k, in perception of speech
experimental g, v, S, tS, dZ, ð, θ, sounds and in accuracy of
group received s, z, l, r/ speech sound production
similar TX plus 15 compared with the control
added minutes of group; no differences were
computerized speech found for phonological
perception treatment awareness
47
Tyler et al. Randomized 27 children ages Compared Individually Phonological treatment
(2002) control trial/Ib 3;0 to 5;11 2 TX orders: reported for led to significantly greater
morphosyntax then each child improvement in phonology
phonological vs. and included compared with no
phonological, then consonant treatment; morphosyntax
morphosyntax clusters and treatment led to significantly
sounds such as greater improvement in
/k, g, f, s, S, tS, l/ morphosyntax than no
treatment; additionally,
cross-domain improvement
occurred from
morphosyntax treatment,
leading to improvement in
phonology as well, but not
for phonology treatment
continues
Table 2–1. continued
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Tyler et al. Randomized 47 children ages Four goal attack Three target For morphosyntax, both the
(2003) control trial/Ib 3;0 to 5;11 with 7 strategies sounds for each morphosyntax first TX and
children serving compared: blocks participant, such the alternating TX led to
as no-treatment of morphosyntax as /f, s, tS/; /l, S, k/; significantly greater gains
controls and the followed by /k, f, tS/; and compared with the control
other 40 randomly phonological /s, S, l/ group; for phonology,
divided among TX, blocks of morphosyntax TX first,
48
four different TX phonological phonological TX first, and
groups followed by alternating TX all led to
morphosyntax significantly greater gains
TX, alternating compared with the control
morphosyntax group
and phonological
TX, alternating
morph-syntax and
phonological TX,
and simultaneous
morphosyntax and
phonological TX
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Gillon (2000) Nonrandom- 91 children ages 23 children received Not specifically Post-treatment assessment
ized groups/ 5;6 to 7;6 with phonological targeted but revealed the children receiv-
IIa spoken language awareness (PA) TX, report pre- ing PA treatment scored
impairment 23 children received treatment and significantly higher on PA
typical speech and post-treatment tests and on several read-
language TX, 15 percent ing measures compared
children received consonants with the traditional TX and
minimal TX, and correct scores minimal TX groups; children
30 children were and use of in the PA group also made
typically developing phonological significantly greater gains
as a comparison processes in consonants correct, but
the authors stated they
49
needed a larger sample for
a meaningful effect size for
this difference
Crosbie et al. Nonrandom- 18 children ages 8 week block of NA For inconsistency,
(2005) ized groups/ 4;8 to 6;5 with contrast TX (minimal children with inconsistent
IIa with either consistent pairs) and 8 week speech sound disorders
baseline/ (n = 8 ) or block of core made significantly
treatment inconsistent vocabulary TX more improvement in
and group (n = 10) speech core vocabulary TX; for
comparisons sound disorders percent consonants
correct, contrast TX led to
significantly greater gains
and gains were greatest
for children with consistent
speech sound disorders
continues
Table 2–1. continued
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Forrest & Single subject Four male children Treatment focusing Targeted one Participants mastered the
Elbert (2001) experimental/ ages 4;11 to 5;3 on one fricative at fricative per child target sound in word final
IIb with inconsistent the word level, first /S, θ, s/ that was position and generalized
speech sound with modeled and missing from the to untreated words with the
production errors then unmodeled child’s phonetic sound in word final position;
and multiple productions of inventory only one child met the
consonants words with the criterion for generalization
missing from target sounds in final to other word positions
their phonetic position (68%+)
50
inventories
Gierut & Single subject 7 children ages Treatment of 3-element During TX the children
Champion experimental/ 3;4 to 6;3 who 3-element s-clusters, clusters /skw, skr, improved their accuracy
(2001) IIb scored below the in made up spl, spr/ in producing nonsense
6th percentile on (nonsense) words words with 3-element
an articulation clusters; post-treatment
test, who otherwise assessment showed little
had normal generalization to real words
hearing and with 3-element clusters;
nonspeech oral the authors identified
motor skills, and generalization to single
age-appropriate consonants (e.g., /g, f, θ, ð,
language skills s, z, S, l, r, tS, dZ/) and some
2-element clusters
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Miccio & Single subject Female child age Treatment in /v, z/ in word Child’s production of /v/
Ingrisano experimental/ 5;3 with history of two phases with initial position and /z/ improved with
(2000) IIb otitis media; pure the initial phase treatment and the authors
tone thresholds focusing on noted generalization
were 20 to 30 dB in acquisition and in different degrees to
the right and left imitation of the untreated sounds /f, θ, ð, tS,
ears; receiving target and the dZ/ after starting treatment
special education second phase on /v/ and additional
services for addi- on production of generalization to /s, S/ after
tional developmen- minimal pair words starting treatment on /z/
tal delays included with the target and
51
problems with fine error phonemes
motor skills
Morrisette & Single subject 8 children ages Four different TX One target sound Children in all four
Gierut (2002) experimental/ 3;10 to 5:4 with based on lexical for each child conditions acquired
IIb phonological properties of from the following their target sounds in
delays; scored target words: high set: /f, s, l, r/ treated words; treating
at or below 5th frequency words, high frequency words led
percentile on an low frequency to more generalization
articulation test words, words to untreated words and
from high density sounds than the other
“neighborhoods” conditions
and words from
low density
“neighborhoods”
continues
Table 2–1. continued
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Williams Single subject Female child age Multiple oppositions Multiple Three multiple oppositions
(2005) experimental/ 6;5 with moderate TX follow by minimal oppositions targets /f, θ, gl/ were
IIb phonological contrast TX targets were word acquired in spontaneous
impairment initial /f, θ, gl, words and generalized to
fr/; minimal pair conversational speech; one
targets were word minimal pair target /r/ was
initial /r, S/ and acquired in spontaneous
word final /θ/ words and conversational
speech
52
Jacoby et al. Retrospective, 234 children, ages Usual TX NA Found a statistically
(2002) nonrandom- 3 to 6, who had approaches; significant relationship
ized groups/III received speech all participants between amount of TX and
and language received functional improvement in functional
treatment from a communication communication rating for
university clinic ratings on children with articulation
National Outcome disorders (intelligibility),
Measurement and expressive language
System disorders; older children
(ages 5 & 6) needed more
TX to make functional
improvements than
younger children (ages 3
& 4)
Research
Design/Level Treatment Participants
Source of Evidence Approach (# and age) Target Sounds Outcomes
Pascoe et al. Nonexperi- Female child age Speech only and Multiple final TX yielded improvements
(2005) mental case 6;5 with severe speech-spelling TX consonants in CVC words productions,
study/III speech sound with CVC words as in CVC words spelling, and connected
disorder the targets and eventually speech; improvements
connected maintained at a 7-month
speech follow-up
Williams Nonexperi- 10 children ages Multiple oppositions, Targets varied Reported amount of time
(2000) mental case 4;0 to 6;5 with minimal pair, and from 4 to 16 in TX; maximum amount
53
study/III multiple speech naturalistic speech depending was 5 semesters and
sound production intelligibility TX; all on the child 105 sessions; average
errors children started with and included was 3.4 semesters and
multiple oppositions different places 60.3 sessions; 2 of the 10
approach of production children only required
and manners of multiple oppositions TX;
production remaining children required
addition TX – minimal pairs
and/or naturalistic TX
Note. This table only includes research evidence for children with speech sound disorders not related to specific diagnoses such as child-
hood apraxia of speech, cleft of the lip and palate, or dysarthria. Some high-level sources were omitted for this reason, such as the systematic
review on treatment for childhood apraxia of speech by Murray, McCabe, and Ballard (2014). Additionally, the table only includes studies in
which the participants spoke English.
54 The late eight
Another reason you might want to conduct your own literature search
is that your client has unique characteristics, suggesting that a client-specific
evidence search would be helpful (Gallagher, 2002). Let’s consider the possi-
bility of a 6-year-old child with Down syndrome, an SSD, and reduced speech
CHAPTER 2: Using Evidence to Guide Clinical Practice 55
Summary
References
Allen, M. M. (2013). Intervention efficacy and intensity for children with speech
sound disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 865–877.
doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0076)
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1997–2016). Evidence maps.
Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/Evidence-Maps/
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). Evidence-based practice
in communication disorders: An introduction [Technical report]. Retrieved from
http://www.asha.org/policy
American Speech-Language Hearing Association. (2005). Evidence-based practice in
communication disorders [Position statement]. Retrieved http://www.asha.org/policy
Baker, E., & McLeod, S. (2011a). Evidence-based practice for children with speech
sound disorders: Part 1 narrative review. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 42, 102–139. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2010/09-0075)
Baker, E., & McLeod, S. (2011b). Evidence-based practice for children with speech
sound disorders: Part 2 application to clinical practice. Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 140–151. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2010/10-0023)
Broomfield, J., & Dodd, B. (2011). Is speech and language therapy effective for
children with primary speech and language impairment? Report of a randomized
control trial. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
46, 628–640. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00039.x
Crosbie, S., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (2005). Intervention for children with severe speech
disorder: A comparison of two approaches. International Journal of Language
and Communication Disorders, 40(4), 467–491. Retrieved from http://www.tandf
online.com/toc/ilcd20/current
Denne, M., Langdown, N., Pring, T., & Roy, P. (2005). Treating children with expressive
phonological disorders: Does phonological awareness therapy work in the clinic?
International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40(4), 493–504.
Dollaghan, C. A. (2007). The handbook for evidence-based practice in communica-
tion disorders. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Eisenberg, S. L., & Hitchcock, E. R. (2010). Using standardized tests to inventory
consonant and vowel production: A comparison of 11 tests of articulation and
phonology. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 488–503.
doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0125)
Fey, M. E., Justice, L. M., & Schmitt, M. B. (2014). Evidence-based decision making in
communication intervention. In R. Paul (Ed.), Introduction to clinical methods in
communication disorders (3rd ed., pp. 55–79). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
CHAPTER 2: Using Evidence to Guide Clinical Practice 57
Morrisette, M. L., & Gierut, J. (2002). Lexical organization and phonological change
in treatment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 143–159.
Murray, E., McCabe, P., & Ballard, K. J. (2014). A systematic review of treatment
outcomes for children with childhood apraxia of speech. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 23, 486-504. doi:10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0035
O’Connor, S., & Pettigrew, C. M. (2009). The barriers perceived to prevent the suc-
cessful implementation of evidence-based practice by speech and language
therapists. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
44, 1018–1035. doi:10.3109/13682820802585967
Pascoe, M., Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (2005). Phonological therapy within a psy-
cholinguistic framework: Promoting change in a child with persisting speech
difficulties. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
40(2), 189–220.
Rvachew, S., & Nowak, M. (2001). The effect of target-selection strategy of phonological
learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 610–623.
Rvachew, S., Nowak, M., & Cloutier, G. A. (2004). Effect of phonemic perception
training on the speech production and phonological awareness skills of children
with expressive phonological delay. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathol-
ogy, 13, 250–263.
Shriberg, L. D., Lewis, B. A., Tomblin, J. B., McSweeny, J. L., Karlsson, H. B., & Scheer,
A. R. (2005). Toward diagnostic and phenotype markers for genetically transmitted
speech delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 834–852.
doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2005/058)
Speech Pathology Database for Best Interventions and Treatment Efficacy. (n.d.).
About. Retrieved from http://speechbite.com/about/
Speech Pathology Database for Best Interventions and Treatment Efficacy (n.d.). Group
comparison studies. Retrieved from http://speechbite.com/rating-research-quality/
group-comparison/
Sugden, E., Baker, E., Munro, N., & Williams, A. L. (2016). Involvement of parents
in intervention for childhood speech sound disorders: A review of the evidence.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(6), 597–625.
doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12247
Tyler, A. A., Lewis, K. E., Haskill, A., & Tolbert, L. C. (2002). Efficacy and cross-domain
effects of a morpho-syntax and a phonology intervention. Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 52–66.
Tyler, A. A., Lewis, K. E., Haskill, A., & Tolbert, L. C. (2003). Outcomes of different
speech and language goal attack strategies. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 46, 1077–1094.
Williams, A. L. (2000). Multiple oppositions: Case studies of variables in phonological
intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 289–299.
Williams, A. L. (2005). Assessment, target selection, and intervention: Dynamic interac-
tions within a systemic perspective. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(3), 231–242.
Yoder, P. J., Camarata, S., & Woynaroski, T. (2016). Treating speech comprehensibility
in students with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 59, 446–459. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-S-15-0148
Chapter Three
Introduction
In the United States, 13.0% of the population 5 years and older (38.4 million
residents) speak Spanish in the home (U.S. Census Bureau, 2013). In contrast,
approximately 94% of speech-language pathologists (SLPs) are monolingual
English speakers. Of the 6% who are bilingual SLPs, the largest group (approxi-
mately 65%) self-identify as bilingual in Spanish and English (ASHA, 2016).
Although more bilingual SLP programs are emerging (Crowley et al., 2014), for
the present and into the near future, Spanish-speaking students are more likely
to receive speech treatment from monolingual English-speaking clinicians.
The International Expert Panel on Multilingual Children’s Speech has
emphasized the need for monolingual and bilingual SLPs around the world
to address the speech needs of multilingual children (International Expert
Panel on Multilingual Children’s Speech, 2012). Essential areas include dis-
tinguishing a speech difference from a disorder and overcoming linguistic
and cultural barriers that may limit overall school-to-home communication
(Kohnert, Yim, Nett, Kan, & Duran, 2005; Williams & McLeod, 2012).
This chapter focuses on treatment of late acquired sounds in bilingual
Spanish-English children. The intended audience includes monolingual Eng-
lish-speaking SLPs, English-speaking SLPs with some “preschool” Spanish,
and bilingual SLPs. The chapter proceeds in the following steps:
59
60 The late eight
The Language
English and Spanish share both linguistic similarities and differences. The
historical connection between these two languages appears in their shared
Roman alphabet (Langdon, 2008). The historical connection between the
two languages also appears in shared overlapping sound-meaning word
pairs (cognates). Examples of English-Spanish cognates include rose-rosa
and lemon-limón. In any given linguistic area, linguistic features in the two
languages may be the same, share a partial overlap, or be different.
Further, just as in English, there may also be variations within Spanish
phonology (Goldstein, 2001). Phonemes and word structure are especially
important to clinical care of Spanish-speaking students. The discussion of
phonemes focuses on sound classes and consonant clusters. The discussion
of word structure focuses on word length and syllable structure.
Phonemes
Spanish has fewer consonants than English does, Spanish having 18 conso-
nants and English possessing 24 (Acevedo, 1993; Jiménez, 1987; Gorman &
Kester, 2003). Figure 3–1 shows a Venn diagram of sounds that are shared
in Spanish and English, sounds unique to Spanish, and sounds unique to
English. Major differences between Spanish and English occur in the fricative
and liquid sound classes (Morrow, Goldstein, Gilhool, & Paradis, 2014).
Fricatives
Spanish does not contain the following English fricatives: [z, Z, ʃ, v], except
perhaps in particular dialects. Without these phonemes, Spanish-influenced
English may result in the following productions.
Liquids
Spanish and English share the [l] phoneme, but Spanish does not contain the
English [ɹ]. Instead, Spanish has a vibrant trill /r/, where the tongue vibrates
against the alveolar ridge (e.g., “perro” = dog), and a tap /ɾ/ which is produced
with a quick touch to the alveolar ridge (e.g., “pero” = but), similar to the
flap in English (/bɾ2/ for butter) (Morrow et al., 2014). In this chapter, the
phonetic transcriptions will be: English [ɹ] (in “rabbit”) represented upside
down, the Spanish trill [r] right-side up, and the Spanish tap [ɾ] as such.
Consonant Clusters
Word Structure
n pared (wall)
n pan (bread)
n arroz (/aros/ = rice)
n flor (/floɾ/ = flower)
n sol (sun)
In the past, researchers debated whether bilinguals had one or two sepa-
rate phonological systems (see Ray, 2002 for a review). Grosjean (1989)
furthered the discussion by sharing that “the bilingual is NOT the sum of
two complete or incomplete monolinguals; rather, he or she has a unique
specific linguistic configuration” (p. 3). Presently, the commonly accepted
perspective suggests that bilinguals have two phonological systems which
overlap and interact (Gildersleeve-Neumann & Goldstein, 2015; Hambly,
Wren, McLeod, & Roulstone, 2013; Prezas et al., 2014). Some researchers
refer to this dual language model as the Interactional Dual Systems Model
(Paradis, 2001). Based on current research, SLPs can share that a child’s first
language (L1) may influence or interfere with his or her second language
(L2) and vice versa (Hambly et al., 2013). Thus, it is important to know the
linguistic differences between languages. These differences may be a source
of cross-linguistic interference or transfer.
In practical terms, what this means is that bilingual children have sepa-
rate phonological systems that overlap and interact with each other (Hambly
et al., 2013). A child’s exposure to his or her home language may influence
the child’s pronunciation of a second or third language (referred to as inter-
ference or transfer). For example, a school-age child may use the Spanish
tap /ɾ/ to pronounce the English word /fɹut/ as /fɾut/, or produce the English
word “shoes” as /tSus/ because the /S/ sound (and /z/ sound) are not present
in Spanish. Additionally, a Spanish-speaking school-age child who has not
mastered the trill /r/ may use the English /ɹ/ for the Spanish trill /r/ to say
the Spanish word “dog” as “pay-ro” for perro (Prezas et al., 2014).
64 The late eight
Dialects
tache), vela (candle), bañera (bathroom), rey (king), lluvia (rain), and burro
(donkey)” (p. 11). As seen in these examples, even common objects and foods
may have increased variation to a specific dialect.
One main difference between conservative and radical dialects is the pres-
ence or omission of final consonants (Bedore, 1999). Depending on the
speaker’s dialectal origin, the Spanish word for “more” could be pronounced
as más (México) or má’ (Puerto Rico). A preschool teacher who spoke with
a Puerto Rican Spanish dialect once shared with me “comemo’ la’ palabra’”
[comemos las palabras = we eat our words], referring to the final consonant
[s] omissions common in a Puerto Rican dialect. Based on socioeconomic
and/or social status, some Puerto Rican Spanish dialect speakers substitute
the syllable-final [s] with an [h] (“lah cosah” for “las cosas”) (Poplack, 1980).
Whether omitted or substituted, the [s] omissions occur only in syllable
final position; therefore, initial [s] omissions ([ol] for “sol” = sun) or final [r]
substitutions ([flo] for “flor” = flower) are indicators of further investigation
needed to determine an SSD.
Rican influenced Spanish (Yavas & Goldstein, 1998, p. 51). See Goldstein
and Iglesias (2001) and Martinez (2011) for a more in-depth review of the
phonological influence of Spanish dialects.
The Late 4: [l ð r ɾ]
The Middle 6: [s f p tS β ]
The Early 6: [ t m n k x]
Studies Participants f g x n d ð tS ñ b l ɾ s r
Mason, Smith, and Hinshaw Mexican children in CA X X X X X X X
(1976) replicated Medida (ages 4–9; n = 424)
Española de Articulación Sounds mastered later
(as cited in Jiménez, 1987) than 5 years of age
Terrero (1979) (as cited in Venezuela xprex xpost xpost xpost xprex xpre
Acevedo, 1993) (2;10–5;5; n = 64) xpost xpost
Monolingual Spanish
Jiménez (1987) Mexican American X X
children in CA
67
(3–5;7; n = 120)
Acevedo (1993) Mexican American in TX Xa X X
(3–5;11; n = 120)
Goldstein, Fabiano, & (see above) X X X X X X
Washington (2005) derived
from Acevedo (1993)
Fabiano-Smith & Goldstein Puerto Rican & X X X X
(2010) Dominican Spanish
speakers (8 bilingual,
8 monolingual)
continues
Table 3–1. continued
Studies Participants f g x n d ð tS ñ b l ɾ s r
Linares (1981) (as cited in Chihuahua, Mexico (3–6 X X X
Jiménez, 1987) years; n = 97) sounds
after 5
New Mexico X X X X X
68
(5–8 years; n = 148)
Totals 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 3 3 5 7 8
Leacox (2016) clinical Midwest, USA X X X X
estimate (K–5th grade; n = 100)
New Totals 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 3 4 6 8 9
Note. aNo other studies cite /g/ as late developing; may have been influenced by unfamiliar target ‘bigote’ (mustache) (Acevedo, 1993, p. 13).
CHAPTER 3: The Late Eight en español 69
Variation regarding which sounds are late acquisitions in Spanish may at least
in part reflect dialectal or regional influences. For example, both Jiménez
(1987) and Acevedo (1993) assessed 120 Mexican American children (ages
3–5) with the same articulation test items but found differences in which
sounds were late acquisitions. Jiménez (1987) studied children from Cali-
fornia and found that most children aged 3 to 5 years had not yet acquired
/s/ and trilled /r/. In contrast, Acevedo (1993) studied children from Texas
using the same articulation test items but found children of the same age had
not acquired /g/ and trilled /r/. It is possible the /g/ phoneme influenced
the target item “bigote” (mustache), as it was dialectally less familiar to the
students in Texas.
n Education programs
n Treatment approaches
n Treatment sounds
n Treatment techniques
Education Programs
Clinical care for late sounds in Spanish is best undertaken with awareness
of and contributing to a student’s education. Dual language (DL) programs
provide educational instruction in more than one language (Thomas &
Collier, 2002). Although a variety of programs exist in the United States and
around the globe, DL programs use different methods to group or “chunk”
language instruction. This “grouping” or “chunking” of language provides
opportunities for children to learn language in predictable ways.
70 The late eight
One-Way or Two-Way
DL programs may have a wide variation but may include either one-way or
two-way DL classrooms (Langdon, 2008). A one-way DL classroom means the
classroom includes only children whose home language includes exposure to
Spanish. To contrast, a two-way DL program means the classroom includes
children whose home language has Spanish as well as children whose home
language does not include Spanish (i.e., English-speaking children who are
learning Spanish). Two-way refers to the two different groups of children
with different home languages learning two languages (Gómez, Freeman, &
Freeman, 2005).
Language Ratio
Bilingual Resources
1. Traditional articulation
2. Minimal pairs
3. Core vocabulary
4. Perceptual and metalinguistic
5. Cuing for motor plan (for childhood apraxia of speech)
6. Home practice
Cognate Intervention
72
English-Punjabi
Holm & Dodd n = 2 (4;8, 5;2) Phonological (4;8) Traditional artic, minimal English-only
(2001) Cantonese at birth, Artic & pairs, core vocabulary,
Eng preK phonological (5;2) home practice
the translation equivalents plate-plato and lion-león are both cognate pairs.
As Kohnert and Derr (2004) mentioned, cognates are appropriate targets
for a bilingual approach. These cognate pairs may be appropriate targets
whether the SLP knows little or no Spanish or is bilingual. Choosing these
target words potentially creates a bridge between the home and school
languages.
Recent research has established that even younger children are sensitive
to cognates in vocabulary (Leacox, Wood, Sunderman, & Schatschneider,
2016; Pérez, Peña, & Bedore, 2010; Simpson Baird, Palacios, & Kibler, 2016).
However, to date no one has presented the empirical evidence of cognates for
phonological therapy. Nonetheless, “Words Their Way” for Spanish-English
DL learners provides phonological awareness activities, where the initial
sound pictures are cognates (Helman, Bear, Invernizzi, Templeton, & John-
ston, 2009). The images share the same first sound between English and
Spanish (examples below in Figure 3–2). Although cognates need further
research, it is an interesting strategy to consider for DL learners.
Treatment Sounds
Goal Selection
Yavaş & Goldstein (1998, pp. 57–58) Kohnert & Derr (2004, pp. 325–332)
Treat phonological patterns: Bilingual targets:
Similar rates in L1 and L2
1. A. Choose target phonemes
(frequently occurring) which are shared across the
two languages
Unequal frequency between
2.
L1 and L2 Cross-linguistic targets:
B. Intervention on the unshared
3. Only in one language features between the two
languages
Source: Kohnert, K., & Derr, A. (2004). Language intervention with bilingual children. In
B. Goldstein (Ed.), Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish-English
speakers (pp. 311–338). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes; Yavaş, M., & Goldstein, B. (1998).
Phonological assessment and treatment of bilingual speakers. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 49–60. doi:10.1044/1058-0360.0702.49
sounds in error that exist in both languages, then targeting sounds unique
to Spanish or unique to English (Perez, 1994 as cited in Yavas & Goldstein,
1998). Children with difficulties in production of late-developing sounds
may struggle with only one or a few articulation sounds in error. However,
we can apply Yavas and Goldstein’s (1998) phonological principles to late-
developing articulatory productions.
Second, Kohnert and Derr (2004) describe two broad bilingual treatment
perspectives to encourage gains in both languages: a bilingual approach
and a cross-linguistic approach. A bilingual approach focuses on the shared
skills in both languages, such as cognate words (English-Spanish translation
equivalents: salt/sal) and shared phonemes (e.g., [s], [l]). Kohnert and Derr’s
bilingual approach corresponds to Yavas and Goldstein’s (1998) primary
recommendation to target shared error patterns that have similar impact on
a child’s L1 and L2. Additionally, monolingual English SLPs can still choose a
bilingual approach by selecting phonemes that overlap between English and
Spanish. Prezas et al.’s (2014) findings align with the bilingual approach, as
bilingual 5-year-old children demonstrated equal error frequency on con-
sonant cluster deletion and liquid deviation errors between English and
Spanish. To provide services, an SLP would need knowledge of the child’s L1:
Spanish. Kohnert and Derr’s cross-linguistic approach supports an interven-
tion on the unshared features between the two languages, similar to that of
Yavas and Goldstein’s (1998) third recommendation to target error patterns
unique to one language.
CHAPTER 3: The Late Eight en español 75
In 2002, Ray published the first study with a trilingual student with
English-only treatment and found that generalization occurred on untreated
sounds in both Hindi and Gujarati. Then, there was a 13-year gap in the
bilingual SSD research literature until Gildersleeve-Neumann and Goldstein’s
(2015) study with the language of instruction including both English and
Spanish. It goes without saying that more evidence and clinical tools are
needed to guide SLPs to serve Spanish-English DL learners.
Sound Selection
As depicted in Figure 3–3, late sounds divide into three categories: (1) treat-
ment of sounds found in both English and Spanish, (2) treatment of sounds
unique to Spanish, and (3) treatment of sounds unique to English.
Based on Figure 3–3, [s] and [l] are late sounds found in both English
and Spanish, tap [r] and trill [ɾ] are late sounds unique to Spanish, and SH,
Z, V, and English-R are late sounds unique to English.
Sounds in the first two groups are candidates for treatment. But what
about sounds in the third group, which occur in English but not in Spanish?
To treat or not to treat? Spanish-speaking children may have difficulty pro-
ducing the English [ʃ z v]. These sounds do not exist in the child’s home
language and result in a speech difference but not disorder. Therefore, a
Spanish-speaking student with only these sound errors would not typically
qualify for speech services (Yavas & Goldstein, 1998). Keep in mind, a child
with difficulty in pronunciation of Spanish trill [r] and/or tap [ɾ] may also
have difficulty with the English [ɹ], and in that case, may qualify for therapy
to remediate speech productions in the language of the classroom. Parents
In this retrospective anecdote, the sounds with speech goals from most
common to less common sounds were:
Treatment Techniques
1. Relative frequency
2. Expected errors
3. Shaping and elicitation techniques
4. Minimal pairs
[s]
Relative Frequency
In Spanish, [s] ranks first in relative frequency compared with all other
Spanish consonants, and its percentage of occurrence compared with all
Spanish phonemes is 9.4% (Guirao & Jurado, 1990).
78 The late eight
Expected Errors
Similar to English, a common error for [s] is lisping, with interdental tongue
placement [θ]. However, some dialects of Spanish may produce the interdental
“θeta” as a typical dialectal production (Martinez, 2011). Lateralizing [s] with
airflow escaping on each side of the tongue occurs but is less common than
interdental productions. Spanish-speaking children with SSDs are likely to
experience difficulties with [s] in consonant clusters (Gildersleeve-Neumann
& Goldstein, 2015; Yavas, 2010).
Minimal Pairs
It is a myth that there are “no rhyming words in Spanish.” Because Spanish
has fewer final sounds and a more syllabic CV word structure, there are fewer
minimal pair combinations compared with English, but minimal pairs are
still used in research and clinical practice as an appropriate tool for therapy
(Gildersleeve-Neumann & Goldstein, 2015) and with several languages (Holm
& Dodd, 2001; Holm, Ozanne, & Dodd, 1997).
For Spanish speakers with a conservative dialect, such as Mexican
Spanish, you might consider working on minimal pairs with final [s]. Final
[s] occurs in both English and Spanish. The phoneme [s] is only one of five
final sounds in Spanish. Examples with plurality include:
n Pato-Patos (duck-ducks)
n Dedo-dedos (finger-fingers)
n Mano-manos (hand-hands)
Or simple CV-CVC minimal pairs with a final [s] phoneme may be another
option:
For younger children, choosing one treatment word for home practice may
be a relevant strategy. One bilingual SLP colleague I worked with gave each
of her students a notebook for speech therapy. As her students became suc-
cessful with a sound, she would give them just one word to take home and
practice ( J. Leautaud, personal communication, December 22, 2016). The
child could color the notebook assignment with the one target word, if he
or she chose. The homework drawing could be artistic or simple, depending
on the age of the student (see Figure 3–5 for an example).
Although these children may have had consistent phonological or articula-
tion disorders, this type of homework assignment may align with principles of
a core vocabulary approach (Holm & Dodd, 1999). Clinically, this assignment
increased home-to-school communication and provided a feasible method
for the bilingual SLP to share treatment information with the child’s family.
[l]
Relative Frequency
In Spanish, [l] ranks seventh in relative frequency compared with all other
Spanish consonants, and its percentage of occurrence compared with all
Spanish phonemes is 3.9% (Guirao & Jurado, 1990).
Expected Errors
Figure 3–5. Therapy homework example for isolated [s] productions. (Practice
the “s” sound and draw the rays of the sun.)
80 The late eight
Minimal Pairs
As stated in the discussion of [s], Spanish has fewer final consonants than
English, which limits the number of minimal pairs. Nonetheless, minimal pairs
remains a useful treatment tool. Here are a few examples of minimal pairs:
English
n fawn-flan
n pain-plane
Spanish
n luego-fuego (then-fire)
n puma-pluma (puma-feather)
n lobo-globo (wolf-balloon)
Frequency
In Spanish, the tap [ɾ] ranks third in relative frequency compared with all
other Spanish consonants, and its percentage of occurrence compared with
all Spanish phonemes is 5.4% (Guirao & Jurado, 1990). This higher ranked
frequency may be because the tap [ɾ] is one of the few consonants that
occur in the final position of Spanish words. Additionally, tap [ɾ] occurs
in the Spanish initial and medial ɾ-blends. The trill [r] ranks sixteenth in
relative frequency, with its percentage of occurrence at only 0.4% (Guirao
& Jurado, 1990).
Expected Errors
Similar to English, a common error is gliding, substituting a [ j ] for an [r]
consonant; [w] for [r] substitutions are less common in Spanish, unless the
child has substantial exposure to English. Another developmental error is to
substitute a /ð/ for /ɾ/ (Prezas et al., 2014). Table 3–4 provides examples of
the substitutions, omissions, and distortions that Spanish-speaking children
may produce. Bilingual children have more substitution errors than Spanish
monolinguals (Goldstein & Washington, 2001). For example, monolingual
Spanish-speaking children may only substitute tap and trill sounds with
[l ɾ j], while bilingual children may present with more possible substitutions:
[l ɾ j s t tj dr ld] (Goldstein & Washington, 2001).
Shaping: “Engine lips”— bilabial trill to alveolar trill. If a child does not
produce any type of “r” in either language and if the trill [r] is a phoneme
in the child’s dialect, then I may start with this strategy. I start here in part
82 The late eight
Table 3–4. Error Patterns for the Spanish tap [ɾ] and trill [r]
Spanish Error
Profiles for Example
Tap r / Trill r Error (target → child’s production)
Substitution Bilabial trill for alveolar /pero/ → /peBo/ (dog)
Uvular trill // for alveolar /pero/ → /peo/
Tap /ɾ/ for trill /r/ /fɾuta/ → /fruta/ (fruit)
English /ɹ/ for Spanish /r/ /aros/ → /aɹos/ (rice)
/j/ for trill /r/ /pero/ → /pejo/
Trill /r/ for tap /ɾ/ /raɾo/ → “rrarro” (rare)
/l/ for tap /ɾ/ /fɾuta/ → /fluta/
because it can be a fun way to try making different sounds. Additionally, this
strategy provides tiered levels of support. Tiered levels benefit group therapy
when working with children at different levels of approximating accuracy of
the trilled /r/ sound.
The “Engine lips” strategy was inspired from a comical Youtube video
with “Mr. Ugly Teeth” that progresses from the trill [r] sound in isolation, to
syllables, words, and sentences. The video presentation ends with a well-
known Spanish tongue twister to practice the trill [r]: “erre con erre cigarro,
erre con erre barril, rápido corren los carros del ferrocarril” (an R with an R
cigar, an R with an R barrel, rapidly running the cars to the railroad) (Terrell,
1989, p. 17). http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=P9mMvuRGKY8
1. “Engine lips”: Ask the student to place lips together gently, keep lips
relaxed, and puff air through the closed lips. Repeat until you achieve
a bilabial trill. Cues at step 1 or 2 may include, “haz el sonido cómo un
barco a motor, un carro, bichos, a roncar” (make the sound like a motor
boat, car, bugs, snoring).
2. Tongue and lip: Ask the student to place tongue between upper lip and
front teeth. The tongue is in front of the teeth but behind the upper lip.
Again, ask the child to puff air through gently closed teeth-tongue-lip
placement. Modeling this placement and providing a mirror are helpful.
CHAPTER 3: The Late Eight en español 83
After several vibrant puffs, now the child’s tongue also has a sense of
vibrant movement.
3. Shape to alveolar trill: Ask the student to repeat step 2 and then slide
the tongue from between the upper lip and front teeth, back toward the
alveolar ridge. Repeat front-to-back tongue movements with sustained
puffs of air, until resulting in an alveolar trill.
1. Ask the student to bite (gently!) the sides of tongue (to close airflow
from escaping).
2. Ask the student to move tongue tip behind the front teeth.
3. Ask the student to take a deep breath and puff strong air (breathe) out.
84 The late eight
Shaping: Final [l] to trill [r] or tap [ɾ]. This technique is based on the
coarticulation and phonetic similarity among [l], [r], and [ɾ]. This strategy may
be the phonetic contrast needed to eliminate [l] substitutions.
1. Ask the child to say “el” (masculine definite article in Spanish for word
“the”).
2. Then name picture of “el río” (the river), but flip tongue back quickly
between words.
3. Ask the student to try again but flip the tongue forcefully for the [r] or
[ɾ], depending on target approximation.
4. Can also try with the Spanish words for “the king”: el rey.
Minimal Pairs
Simple activities to send home can increase repetitions and possible gen-
eralization. I found these activities as suitable to practice during group
therapy or for home therapy, relying on an element of independence and/
or self-monitoring.
Table 3–5. Example Lesson Plan for Early Treatment Elicitation of the
Spanish Trill /r/
Home practice Listen to correct trill /r/ productions and trial trill
/r/ productions in isolation or words.
http://www.studyspanish.com/pronunciation/
letter_rr.htm
_________________, CCC-SLP
Terapista del habla y lenguaje
CHAPTER 3: The Late Eight en español 87
Conclusions
The focus of this chapter was to review the knowledge SLPs need to treat
SSDs for Spanish-speaking learners of English. Essential to service provi-
sion, the key areas highlighted were the differences and similarities between
English and Spanish phonological systems, later acquired sounds in Spanish,
and specific treatment strategies for remediation. Presently, there are a mere
six studies on treatment with only one research study providing treatment
in Spanish and English (Gildersleeve-Neumann & Goldstein, 2015), empha-
sizing the necessity of further bilingual research guided by clinical insight.
Given the increase of multilingual speakers, clinical researchers and SLPs
need to continue to investigate strategies for multilingual children with SSDs.
References
Dinnsen, D., Chin, S., Elbert, M., & T. Powell. (1990). Some constraints on functionally
disordered phonologies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 33,
28–37.
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Barlow, J. A. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acqui-
sition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual
Education and Bilingualism, 13(1), 81–97.
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Goldstein, B. (2010). Early-, middle-, and late-developing sounds
in monolingual and bilingual children: An exploratory investigation. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 66–77.
Face, T. L., & Menke, M. R. (2009). Acquisition of the Spanish voiced spirants by
second language learners. In Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics
Symposium (pp. 39–52). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., & Goldstein, B. (2015). Cross-linguistic generalization in
the treatment of two sequential Spanish-English bilingual children with speech
sound disorders. International Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 17(1),
26–40. doi:10.3109/17549507.2014.898093
Goldstein, B.A. (1995). Spanish phonological development. In Kayser, H. (Ed.), Bilingual
speech-language pathology: An Hispanic focus (pp. 17–39). San Diego, CA: Singular.
Goldstein, B. (2000). Cultural & linguistic diversity resource guide for speech-language
pathologists. San Diego, CA: Singular.
Goldstein, B. (2001). The effect of dialect on phonological analysis: Evidence from
Spanish-speaking children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10,
394–406.
Goldstein, B., & Iglesias, A. (2001). The effect of dialect on phonological analysis:
Evidence from Spanish-speaking children. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 10, 394–406. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2001/034)
Goldstein, B., & Iglesias, A. (2006). Contextual probes of articulation competence-
Spanish. Greenville, SC: Super Duper Publications.
Goldstein, B., & Washington, P. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns
in 4-year-old typically developing Spanish-English bilingual children. Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 153–164. doi:10.1044/0161-1461
(2001/014)
Gómez, L., Freeman, D., & Freeman, Y. (2005). Dual language education: A promising
50-50 model. Bilingual Journal Research, 29, 145–164.
González-Bueno, M. (2005). Articulatory difficulties in the acquisition of Spanish
/r/ in the bilingual context. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan
(Eds.), ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp.
914–934). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Gorman, B., & Kester, E. S. (2003). Spanish-influenced English: Typical phonological
patterns in the English language learner. Austin, TX: Bilinguistics. Retrieved from
http://bilinguistics.com/wp-content/uploads/2013/01/abad_1001.pdf
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals
in one person. Brain and Language, 36(1), 3–15.
Guirao, M., & García Jurado, M. A. (1990). Frequency of occurrence of phonemes in
American Spanish. Revue québécoise de linguistique, 19(2), 135–149. doi:10.7202/
602680ar
Guitart, J. M. (2005). Sociophonetic knowledge of Spanish and control of style. In L.
Sayahi & M. Westmoreland (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the Second Workshop
CHAPTER 3: The Late Eight en español 89
Introduction
n Eligibility
n Linking Interventions with the Curriculum
Eligibility
Before treatment can begin, an SLP must qualify a student for school-based
service. Topics in this section include:
93
94 The late eight
For many children in the United States between ages 3 and 21, eligibility
for speech-language services are determined through regulations under the
Individuals with Disability Education Act (2004). The IDEA regulations define
speech sound disorders within the speech or language impairment category
with the following eligibility definition:
The statement means that to qualify for services under IDEA a child must
have an educational disability. “An educational disability requiring the ser-
vices of Special Education is a skills deficit, a health or physical condition,
a functional limitation, or a pattern of behavior that adversely affects educa-
tional performance” (IDEA, 2004).
The following rules guide the eligibility process:
Impact on Education
Eligibility guidelines often have gray areas regarding what constitutes edu-
cational impact. Multiple times, the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association has requested clarification from the Department of Education
regarding the educational justification of speech services. The Office of
Special Education Programs (OSEP) first responded to concerns about eli-
gibility of services in 1980 (Martin, 1980). At that time, some states or local
education agencies were narrowly defining eligibility as academic failure
instead of considering the impact of oral communication more broadly on
educational performance.
96 The late eight
After IDEA was updated in 2004, ASHA again asked for clarification from
the OSEP about eligibility for services. Posny (2007) stated: “it remains the
Department’s position that the term ‘educational performance’ is not limited
to academic performance. The determination that a speech and language
impairment adversely affects a child’s educational performance must be
determined on a case-by-case basis, depending on the unique needs of the
child, not based on discrepancies in age or grade performance in academic
subject areas” (Posny, 2007, p. 1). Recent updates to IDEA expand consider-
ations to include implications for nonacademic and extracurricular activities
which may include clubs, recess, and mealtime.
The following research describes the connection between speech sound
disorders and disorders of language and literacy:
Research indicates that SLPs and educators at minimum should monitor the
language and literacy skills of students who have speech sound disorders
CHAPTER 4: Speech Sound Disorders, Literacy, and Curriculum 97
Some SLPs may wonder how we know which kids may continue to struggle
with academics after remediation for a speech sound disorder. Under IDEA,
once a speech-language disorder has been remediated, a child no longer has
a disability and is dismissed from services. Farquharson (2015) states, “for a
puzzling group of students, even after services are rendered and the speech
sound disorder has remediated, academic difficulties persist. These academic
difficulties are often in the areas of reading, writing, and spoken language”
(p. 1). Unfortunately, we don’t have a crystal ball to predict. Using research
studies as guidance, we should carefully consider the following potential
areas as being red flags for later academic difficulties:
School-based SLPs may worry about issues of caseload and workload when
qualifying speech and language impaired students. Although caseload numbers,
minutes of services, or location of services cannot be a decision-making point
for qualifying a student, it is sometimes the elephant in the room.
The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association has stated that SLPs
have a role in working across all levels, serving a range of disorders, ensur-
ing educational relevance, providing unique contributions to curriculum,
highlighting language/literacy, and providing culturally competent services.
These services may include prevention, assessment, intervention, program
design, data collection and analysis, and compliance (ASHA, 2010). There
are no easy solutions to caseload and workload but SLPs must be advocates
for themselves, schools, and ultimately the students they service. The Ameri-
can Speech-Language-Hearing Association has resources available to help
address concerns and provide advocacy.
Most often, speech sound disorders affect a students’ ability to present infor-
mation or their self-confidence in sharing with teachers and peers. It may
also impact their long-term success in securing a job of their choice.
To justify treatment or to link interventions to content standards, SLPs
may look toward English Language Arts (ELA) or 21st Century Skills stan-
dards. In the Speaking and Listening Strand, a professional may find a stan-
dard worded similarly to the Common Core Standard of “Speak audibly and
express thoughts, feelings, and ideas clearly” (CCSS.ELA-LITERACY.SL.K.6).
In the Language Strand, a statement similar to the Common Core Standard of
“Acquire and use accurately grade-appropriate conversational, general aca-
CHAPTER 4: Speech Sound Disorders, Literacy, and Curriculum 99
Materials
n Semantics
n Syntax
n Morphology
n Reading fluency
n Spelling
100 The late eight
Semantics
These authors provide a list of words that are linked to standards in the
common core and might also be addressed in state standards. Words are
defined in a student-friendly way and cross-referenced with ELA and Math
Standards. Educators could cross-reference a list from this resource or similar
resources by targeted speech sound that would also go across content areas
(Table 4–1).
Using curricular material can be a good starting point for selecting vocab-
ulary. In additional, other resources are available for educators to narrow
down academic vocabulary. Coxhead (2000) conducted research and created
an extensive academic word list made up of 570 word families. Sublists of
words were created based on their frequency of use in English. Reviewing the
sublists, an SLP may select target articulation words that address the student’s
speech sound disorder. An example is provided in Table 4–2.
Finally, resources in books or on websites may provide vocabulary lists
that are linked to popular fiction and nonfiction books. Speech-language
pathologists should be critical consumers of these materials. Justice, Schmitt,
Murphy, Pratt, and Biancone (2014) found that SLPs often selected basic
vocabulary to target instead of academic rich vocabulary even though basic
vocabulary is often learned in context. Beck, McKeown, and Kucan (2002)
outline common books used by grade levels and the Tier II vocabulary that
could be targeted by educators. Even a classic story that might be considered
easy for children like Caps for Sale (Slobodkina, 2015), first published in
1940, has complex vocabulary like “ordinary,” “refreshed,” and “imitate” (Beck,
McKeown, & Kucan, 2002). If a child was working on /r/ targets, teaching
the words “ordinary” and “refreshed” would have more academic carryover
than selecting the words “street,” “red,” “gray,” “under,” and “tree,” which most
students would already have been exposed to in everyday conversations. SLPs,
along with other educational team members, need to use their best judgment
in selecting academic targets. Even though there are resources available, there
is no exact criteria or recommended sequence to decide if a word is Tier I,
Tier II, or Tier III and the order or grade level that words should be targeted.
Syntax
As students improve their speech sound production, often SLPs will ask
them to use words in sentences. If syntax is an issue for students, treatment
words can be selected to both address speech production and teach specific
syntactic structures. Eisenberg (2007) provides an in-depth look at later-
developing grammatical structures, including noun phrase expansions, verb
form expansions, predicate expansions, conjunctions, complement clauses,
adverbial constructions, and other sentence constructions. These advanced
grammatical structures may be overlooked as students remediate the most
basic syntactic skills. Using a discrete skills approach, an SLP may select
target words to address speech and model, drill, or sentence-combine with
students to work on syntax. Mini and micro lessons embedded in writing
can strengthen and assist with carryover of skills. An example of sentence
expanding is provided in Table 4–3.
Morphology
Sound Indicating
Plural /s/ /z/ /Iz/
cats dogs beaches
hats frogs couches
socks cars stitches
blankets bowls boxes
lamps games badges
coats bananas lunches
disks bands sandwiches
lights beads judges
books eggs buses
chips cubs quizzes
Reading Fluency
Educators often use short passage or grade level readers to target and measure
reading fluency. Reading fluency has been defined as reading accurately,
quickly, and with expression. After the National Reading Panel report was
released (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000),
assessments and instruction around reading fluency has increased. Reading
fluency improves through treating decoding skills and comprehension.
One popular intervention that can improve reading fluency while also
addressing motivation is Reader’s Theatre (Leahy & Justice, 2007). Reader’s
Theatre combines reading aloud for guided reading practice, supported
reading to help target decoding skills, repeated reading to improve fluency,
and performance reading to practice strategies. The ultimate goal is to
perform for an audience. Students who have reading disabilities along with
a speech sound disorder can identify words with treatment sounds within a
Reader’s Theatre script. This also allows multiple educational professionals
to be involved in using the same script for multiple purposes but providing
repeated readings to improve reading fluency. Below is an example script
that might be used to address reading fluency, motivation, and speech sound
targets. The SLP should read the script and select or modify the parts to treat
speech sounds. In the example in Table 4–5, the SLP has underlined target
/r/ words for the students to practice.
Spelling
Table 4–5. Three Billy Goats Gruff Script with /r/ Words Underlined
i.e., phonemes, are related to letters, i.e., graphemes. This phonological per-
spective of spelling is only one part of learning to spell; other sources of
knowledge include graphotactic (what words look like) and morphological
knowledge (understanding the relationship of roots to prefixes and suffixes)
(Bourassa & Treiman, 2014).
Wolter and Squires (2014) discuss a multilinguistic approach that inte-
grates phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and morphological
knowledge using explicit instruction. Another program that SLPs and other
educators could consider would be the SPELL-Links to Reading & Writing: A
Word Study Curriculum (Wasowicz, Apel, Masterson, & Whitney, 2004). For
students with speech sound disorders, lessons could incorporate linguis-
tic areas of deficit along with target words that include speech errors. For
example, Lesson 3 of the SPELL-Links program assists students in developing
the “ability to segment phonemes and map letters to the pre-vowel consonant
/r, l/ sounds” (Wasowicz, Apel, Masterson, & Whitney, 2014, pp. 1–26). The
starter word list includes words like red, lip, let, and ran, which are words
also commonly targeted by children working on /r/ and /l/ in articulation
therapy. This targeted area includes three lesson plans for students to be
taught and practice the skills. Overall, the SPELL-Links program provides 73
lesson topics with three to five plans for each area. The SPELL-Links program
is just one example of a commercially available program that can be used to
target spelling in a systematic way but also provide students the opportunity
to practice improving their articulation and phonology skills.
Conclusion
School SLPs play a vital role in remediating speech sound disorder. The use
of evidence-based interventions for speech sound disorders that relate to
curriculum should be a priority of all SLPs working with school-age students.
CHAPTER 4: Speech Sound Disorders, Literacy, and Curriculum 105
References
Motor Learning
Guided Therapy
Carlin Hageman
Introduction
107
108 The late eight
Thinking About It
Have we ever delineated all of the procedures and pro-
cesses that we currently use (e.g., immediate or constant
feedback)? Have we considered if behavior modification is
the correct model for practice of a motor skill? Do our tradi-
tional methods work? Of course they do for many learners,
but are they suitable for everyone and the most efficient
and cost effective?
Motor Skill
Motor Control
Thinking About It
What could disrupt motor learning of articulation? What are
the necessary capabilities for learning the skill of speaking?
Schmidt and Wrisberg (2008) described two methods of motor control —closed-
loop and open-loop control. Closed loop is characterized by slow movements
because the movements are constantly modified by sensory feedback about
the movement and the environment. Closed-loop control is slow and the
effort is intensive; consequently, it’s too slow to control the fast movements
of speech. In contrast, during open-loop control, the performer recalls a set
of instructions (or programs) for controlling muscle movements that, once
initiated, are completed without modification. Open-loop control systems are
fast because the movements (instructions) are completed without modifica-
tion. The disadvantage of open-loop control is that when the environment
changes, the performer is unable to change the movement once it has been
initiated. If the wrong movement is planned, it is likely to be executed before
the performer can stop it. Since the open-loop control system demands low
110 The late eight
Schema Theory
internal and external) are compared with the actual outcomes (internal and
external). When there is a mismatch between the predicted external (desired)
outcomes, the learner can tweak the GMP to attempt to make the next move-
ment more accurate or examine the intended internal sensations to ensure
that the intended movement was actually completed. In a sense, the learner
uses the errors to create a better GMP for the next attempt, either for the
specification of the invariant features of the plan (stay the same across many
different iterations of the movement) or the variant features of the plan (vary
112 The late eight
with specifics of the target [e.g., greater force]). From this writer’s perspec-
tive, we can hypothesize potential influences that may create problems for
the learner.
First, if the learner does not have an idea of the correct target, practice
attempts are not modifiable. For example, a deaf child or a child with audi-
tory processing problems may not have a clear notion of speech targets.
Second, the learner must be aware of the initial conditions. For example, the
learner must “know” the internal conditions (e.g., the position of the tongue,
jaw position, muscle tone, and respiratory system status). Further, even if the
learner knows the initial conditions, the learner must have feedback about
the dynamic changes that occur during the movements. If the learner does
not receive accurate feedback or no feedback at all, then the next attempt
will not be corrected in any systematic way — resulting in nearly random
trials. Finally, the learner must be aware of the external conditions, meaning
whether the movement had intended consequences on or for the target (e.g.,
did I make the correct phoneme or did the listener get the message?). Schema
theory suggests that all of this information is held in memory and compared
with the outcome of the movement. Thus, the learner must be able to hold in
memory the initial condition information long enough to compare it (which
also takes time and mental effort) to the outcome. Hence, the comparison is
dependent upon accurate and timely internal and external feedback as well
as the ability to remember and process the information. Factors that have
been shown to interfere with mental processing may include distraction and
arousal levels that are too high or too low (Kahneman, 1973). The sections
that follow are this author’s attempts to utilize this information to construct
therapy practice.
Thinking About It
Do we have diagnostic tests for these variables? Hearing—cer-
tainly, tests are available for hearing across the frequency
spectrum but maybe not for dynamic hearing. Internal con-
ditions — no, for ability to know tactically, kinesthetically,
etc., the conditions of the speech production mechanism.
What does that mean? Perhaps for some learners, we can be
making serious errors assuming that they know “articula-
tory movements.” And no, for testing the ability to hold
in working memory the motor plan and compare it with
results. What about attention? Do we measure the learner’s
ability to attend to information about a movement long
enough to make adjustments to the plan and commit the
adjustments to memory?
CHAPTER 5: Motor Learning Guided Therapy 113
Before we delve into the specifics of motor learning applied to speech prac-
tice, we need to make the distinction between acquisition performance and
learning. Acquisition performance refers to the momentary strength of a
pattern (skill) during practice, whereas learning refers to what the learner
remembers at a point remote from the practice. This can be measured as
retention, generalization, and spread. The important point for this discussion
is that during acquisition (practice), both the learner and the therapist will
do things to improve performance during practice that will be detrimental
to retention or generalization (e.g., constant feedback with description and
cuing). More will be said about this in the feedback section. Learning can also
be inhibited during practice by constant evaluation, which may affect arousal
levels. Many of us have seen the anxious child looking over our shoulder
trying to catch a glimpse of the scores (Hageman, Mueller, Burda, & Bleile,
2004; Maas et al., 2008; Schmidt & Wrisberg, 2008).
Thinking About It
What is the purpose of saying “good job” after each trial?
What are the consequences of praise after each trial? When
is the learner allowed to make the internal comparisons
about the adequacy of the movement? How much time is
necessary to make that comparison? What capabilities does
the learner need to have to make those comparisons and
use them? How or when does clinician behavior interfere
with learning?
Prepractice
Before practice begins, the learner should be prepared for practice (Schmidt
& Lee, 2005). Maas et al. (2008) suggested three goals of prepractice, includ-
ing: (a) motivation to learn, (b) adequate understanding of the task, including
a basic knowledge of correct, and (c) stimulability for acceptable responses
to avoid frustration. Although I think these are worthy goals, in my experi-
ence, it is important not to expect prepractice attempts to be accurate before
practice begins. For me, it is more important that the learner is able to vary
the productions. As a clinician, one must broaden acceptable responses to
include the ability to systematically vary movements even when they are
incorrect with respect to the “correct” response. This is a difficult task for
114 The late eight
Thinking About It
To learn to throw a baseball or softball, it is necessary
to learn the order of movements before worrying about
accuracy of the throw (e.g., correct starting point, opposite
leg motion, rotation of the hips, then rotation of shoulders,
then the arm forward motion and then the wrist snap).
What about speech? What are the correct starting positions?
What is the first movement, the second, and so on? How
much relative time should those movements consume? Do
we test or observe these events?
Practice
Thinking About It
Why does fatigue affect practice? Does it affect attention
through low arousal, which means that focused attention is
difficult? Does it mean the learner loses interest and does
not “tweak” the motor plan after a trial using mental effort?
Thinking About It
We defined motor learning as what the learner remembers
how to do at a time remote from the practice. If you score
acquisition behavior (i.e., during practice), the tendency is
to simplify practice and provide more feedback and cuing
to achieve higher acquisition scores, but that comes at the
price of poorer retention.
116
Figure 5–2. Principles of motor learning.
CHAPTER 5: Motor Learning Guided Therapy 117
Using random practice can be more beneficial for learning than blocked
practice. In our experience, blocked practice may be more effective for the
early learners, whereas random practice is more effective for the advanced
learner. However, when we use blocked practice, we must allow for learning
from each repetition of the production. Therefore, we impose delays of up
to 5 seconds between reiterations of the practice item. During the pause,
we do not allow the learner to talk, play games, or fidget or the clinician to
speak because we have hypothesized that one variable underlying the poor
learning of speech is a lack of attention devoted to the recognition schema
and modification of the GMP.
Focus
Where should the learner focus his/her attention during practice? The learner
has two options in that he/she can focus on the internal aspects of the
movement (e.g., kinesthetic, kinematic, and somatosensory, as in the feel of
the tongue during the production of the /r/) or the external aspects (e.g.,
how does the /r/ sound or how does the listener respond?). The evidence is
very strong that an external focus produces a strong advantage for learning
in the nonspeech motor domain. The movements tend to be more accurate
and less variable, but even so, there may be some interaction between the
level of performance (new or more experienced learner) and the task (Wulf,
Shea, & McNevin, 2003).
Thinking About It
This issue may lie at the heart of some children trying
to learn to speak. When clinicians suggest that attention
be directed to the feel of the act, they assume that the
mechanisms of perception are normal. Our current state of
the art does not allow us to test those senses. Because the
movements of the pharynx, velum, and tongue essentially
are invisible to the learner, poor sensory ability to detect,
identify, and remember sensations of speech may make
internal focus of feedback and instruction frustrating. On
the other hand, some ability to know what the structures
are doing is necessary (not necessarily at the conscious
awareness level) or the next try is essentially random.
Thinking About It
But here we are, right back at the point at which we need
to know, what are the sensory capabilities of the learning
with respect to movements of the oral structures? Indeed,
another assumption we often make is that if the hearing
sensation levels are adequate, then the learner can hear the
sounds. Is that always true?
Feedback
that duplicates that which the performer can perceive has a neutral effect at
best and more commonly is detrimental to learning. Consider how irritating it
would be for a coach to keep telling you that you made the basket when you
can see that you did (literature is clear that arousal levels that are too high
interfere with learning). In addition, Schmidt and Wrisberg (2008) reported
that excessive feedback contributes to dependency upon the coach or thera-
pist for correct performance rather than internal direction or evaluation.
Thinking About It
The key word about feedback is duplication. How do we
know when we duplicate the intrinsic feedback of the
learner? Because we cannot measure internal feedback, in
which direction should we err? In my opinion, we should
err on the side that the learner can do it — albeit with com-
munication that focuses his or her attention on it and the
learner is provided with plenty of time to use it. How much
time is plenty? Note that focusing attention on internal feed-
back is not the same as giving feedback. Using cognitive
“questioning,” like how did that feel? etc., is counterpro-
ductive because it misdirects attention and effort involved
in evaluating performance and is a difficult cognitive task
(e.g., can you put into words how it feels to elevate your
velum during speech?).
Thinking About It
Why make the learner wait for it? From my perspective, I am
hypothesizing that with practice the learner may begin to be
able to use internal feedback. Forcing him or her to wait for
KR or KP may provide an opportunity to use that internal
feedback and give the learner time to compare the actual
movement/result with what was intended. Of course, with this
model you run the risk of duplication of what the learner can
do. Limiting the frequency of feedback may address that issue.
For example, it is obvious that the deaf child needs KR; however, it is
not so obvious that the deaf child needs KP. The deaf child would need KP
providing that he/she could not perceive the movements that led to the KR
received. Further, even KR would not be needed provided the child could
discern the reaction of the environment to the attempted production. In other
words, when the child can determine whether the production resulted in the
desired effect in the real world, then he/she would not need to be told. Of
course, during practice, it might take considerable ingenuity by the clinician
to create communication interactions that lead to independent judgments
by the learner rather than just direct feedback from the clinician. At this
point, the clinician should use every tool available to determine what the
learner can perceive.
Since few, if any, tools are available to the clinician to measure a child’s
ability to perceive the position and movement of the articulators, we need to
go back to the model of the schema whereby the learner modifies behavior
by comparing what he/she intended to do with what actually occurred and
then evaluating the effect on the environment. If the learner — in our case,
the child — is unable to acquire and remember internal and accurate feedback
about the results and the movement, then the comparison cannot be made
in order to adjust the next trial. External feedback is then required.
How frequent should the feedback be? A few investigations have exam-
ined the frequency of feedback in speech (e.g., Kim, LaPointe, & Stierwalt,
2012; Steinhauer & Preston Grayhack, 2000; Wambaugh, Kalinyak-Flizzar,
West, & Doyle, 1998). Consistently, they have demonstrated that feedback
of between 20% and 60% is more effective for retention than 100% percent
feedback. However, for the tasks they examined, they did not control for
learner experience. In other words, in keeping with Schmidt and Wrisberg’s
(2008) notions, if the learners were just beginning, 100% feedback might be
appropriate for a few trials.
How precise should the feedback be? Schmidt and Wrisberg (2008)
described several types of feedback; among them are summary feedback,
average feedback, and bandwidth feedback. Their guiding principle was that
CHAPTER 5: Motor Learning Guided Therapy 121
Measurement
Movement Complexity
Thinking About It
How do you define the speed of movements in speech?
Stetson said that the fundamental unit of speech is the
syllable. We know that the temporal relationships between
transitions and the vowel, voice onset time, the relative
lengths of sounds within syllables, and the relative lengths
of syllables are critical components of speech perception
and prosody. Therefore, I would not practice at a speech
rate that is so low that it changes any of those aspects.
Thinking About It
The notion of coupling has clear implications for clinicians
trying to figure out the length of a motor program — for
speech I would say at minimum a syllable. However, there
is considerable evidence that speech motor programming
can be in elements considerably longer than a syllable. For
example, two investigations of the effect on phrase length
of inhibition of short and longer phrases clearly demon-
strated that speakers, including young children, program
across several syllables (Anderson, Meuting, Woolston, &
Hageman, 2011; Backlin et al., 2008). Carr (2004) also dem-
onstrated open- and closed-loop control differences across
syllables in speech rate tasks.
124 The late eight
Summary
Several variables have been described that are known to affect motor learn-
ing for nonspeech activities and some speech activities. We have also pro-
posed certain other aspects of motor practice that improve nonspeech motor
control and extrapolated them to speech motor practice. Figure 5–3 shows
a sample therapy hierarchy with explanations.
Contains personalized practice materials representing For creating unpredictable practice
universe of targets sequences
Pre-practice set up: From a previously collected pool of practice items, the client randomly draws five words, phrases, and/or
sentences to use during steps 1–5.
Clinician draws a set of words to use as a generalization measure (not Note the pool contains items of various complexities, which can be
practiced) controlled by the clinician. Challenging stimuli are better.
Step 1 Clinician produces utterance depicted on stimulus card and then waits 3 seconds.
For learners who are showing some mastery of the target, omit step 1 Pre-performance time can be filled with movement rehearsal
randomly (50%) such as imagining saying the target. Time to wait is
adaptable to the client.
125
Step 2 Client attempts utterance 2 times without feedback-waiting 3 seconds after each production.
Post performance reflection-internal comparison of intent with movement allowing time to process internal feedback and adjust motor program.
The time may be too long for some and too short for others. No talking by either person. Minimize motor activity and excitement (e.g., game
anticipation)
Step 3 After two attempts, clinician repeats the same utterance, waits 3 seconds, then has client judge the productions which
allowed time for learner to process intrinsic feedback and adjust motor program.
Important – this step is optional. Judgments vary from correct/incorrect or closer/further from target (or other notions governing understanding of the effort). In Step 4
the client actually indicates the judgment. (Preference should be given to broad focus on production – how it sounds versus how is made).
An extension could be that the client receives more value for a correct production judged as correct compared to a wrong production judged as wrong
though the “wrong/wrong” act would still have value. The learner should not fear error. No value for correct/wrong or a wrong/correct judgments.
However feedback could be delivered as summary or average feedback across the set of productions with motor learning suggesting that the less precise
the feedback the better. Summary or average feedback is best with learners who have made some progress.
126
The outcome feedback can be 100% early but should be reduced quickly. As learner shows movement toward more correct, I would reduce feedback
systematically to as little as 25%. Similarly, prescriptive feedback should be reduced as well to zero as learner shows the ability to change productions
closer to correct. The post KR delay time can be shortened or lengthened. Important thing is to keep the post KR time quiet (no talking by either person.
Motivational feedback should be given when needed but definitely not during post KR time or every time.
Repeat steps 1-5 with a new set of randomly drawn stimulus cards and continue until session ends.
Five items in a set is arbitrary – could be more or less depending upon the client’s ability to attend. KR or KP could be provided across the entire set; thus,
the learner should show evidence that they can integrate more general feedback such as summary. Note that the evidence is not overt (asking client to tell
you); rather it is shown by changing performance.
References
Anderson, K., Mueting, E., Woolston, L., & Hageman, C. (2011). Reliability, accu-
racy and refractoriness of a transit reaction reaction: Replication with speech in
children. Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, San Diego, CA.
Backlin, J., Corbett, A., Halbur, T., Gaughan, L., Howard, L., Williamson, C., et al.
(2008). Reliability, accuracy and refractoriness of a transit reaction: Replication
with speech. Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, Chicago, IL.
Carr, L. (2004). Temporal stability of speech motor programs in young children
(Unpublished master’s research). University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, IA.
Gaughan, L., Howard, L., & Hageman, C. F. (2009). Speech inhibition in aphasia:
A measure of motor speech programs. Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.
Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge point: A framework for conceptual-
izing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 36(2), 212–224.
Hageman, C. F., Meuller, M., Burda, A., & Bleile, K. (2004). A motor learning guided
approach to the treatment of developmental apraxia of speech. Presentation at the
10th Symposium of the International Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics Associa-
tion, Lafayette, LA.
Hageman, C., Stierwalt, J., & Burda, A. N. (2004). A motor learning guided approach
to the treatment of nonfluent aphasia. The 10th Symposium of the International
Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics Association, Lafayette, LA.
Hall, P. K., Jordan, L. S., & Robin, D. A. (1993). Developmental apraxia of speech:
Theory and clinical practice. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kim, I., LaPointe, L. L., & Stierwalt, J. A. (2012). The effect of feedback and practice on
the acquisition of novel speech behaviors. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 21, 89–100.
Lisman, A. L., & Sadagopan, N., (2013). Focus of attention and speech motor perfor-
mance. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46, 281–293.
Maas, E., & Farinella, K. A. (2012). Random versus blocked treatment for child-
hood apraxia of speech. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 55,
561–578.
Maas, E., & Mailend, M.-L. (2012). Speech planning happens before speech execution:
Online reaction time methods in study of apraxia of speech. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1423–1534.
Maas, E., Robin, D. A., Austermann Hula, S. N., Freedman, S. E., Wulf, G., Ballard,
K. J., Schmidt, R. A. (2008). Principles of motor learning in treatment of motor
speech disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 277–298.
Netsell, R. (1991). A neurobiologic view of speech production and the dysarthrias.
San Diego, CA: Singular.
Redle. E., Vannesi, J., Maloney, T., Tseval, R. K., Eikenberry, S., Lewis, B., Shriberg, L.
D., Tkach, J., & Holland, S. (2015). Functional MRI evidence for fine motor praxis
dysfunction in children with persistent speech disorders. Brain Research, 1597,
47–56.
128 The late eight
Robin, D. A., Jacks, A., Hageman, C., Clark, H. M., & Woodworth, G. (2008). Visuomo-
tor tracking abilities of speakers with apraxia of speech or conduction aphasia.
Brain and Language, 106(2), 98–106.
Schmidt, R. A. (1988). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Cham-
paign, IL: Human Kinetic.
Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: A problem-
based learning approach (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Stetson, R. H. (1951). Motor phonetics: A study of speech movements in action (2nd
ed.). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland.
Steinhauer, K., & Preston Grayhack, J. (2000). The role of knowledge of results in
performance and learning of a voice motor task. Journal of Voice, 14(2), 137–145.
Wambaugh, J. L., Kalinyak-Flizzar, M. M., West, J. E., & Doyle, P. J. (1998). Effects of
treatment for sound errors in apraxia of speech and aphasia. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 725–743.
Wulf, G., Shea, C. H., & McNevin, N. H. (2003). Increasing the distance of an external
focus of attention enhances learning. Psychological Research, 67, 22–29. doi:10.1007/
s00426-002-0093-6
Part II
Clinical Resources
Chapter Six
Overview
This chapter provides an overview of the resources in the second half of the
book and on the accompanying companion website. Resources described
in the first half of this chapter include 14 “tools of the trade” to evaluate
and treat late-acquired sounds. The second half of the chapter offers an
illustration showing how a clinician might use the resources in evaluations
and treatment. The chapter concludes with a description of resources on the
accompanying companion website.
1. Definition
2. Acquisition
3. Relative frequency
4. Errors
5. Key environments
6. Metaphors
7. Touch cues
8. Initial screening
9. Screening for stimulability
10. Demonstrations
11. Phonetic placement and shaping techniques
12. Exercises
13. Language activities
14. Word lists
131
132 The late eight
Warning!
This chapter provides necessary background for using the
resources. Most of the information, though important, is
fairly dry to read.
1. Definition
[s] is made in either of two ways. Some people produce [s] with
the tongue tip up behind the upper front teeth, others say it with the
tongue tip down behind the lower front teeth. Neither one is
the “right way.” Follow a student’s lead in deciding which way to
teach [s]. If a student appears to find it easier to say [s] with the
tongue tip up, teach the sound that way; if a student appears to
find it easier to say [s] with the tongue tip down, teach the sound
that way. For both varieties of [s], the airstream is continuous and
the vocal folds are apart.
A brief technical definition of the sound is also provided. This is the technical
definition of [s]:
2. Acquisition
What It Is: Acquisition data show the ages at which 50% and 75% of children
acquire a sound.
Its Uses: Acquisition data are widely used for two purposes:
CHAPTER 6: Overview 133
3. Relative Frequency
4. Error
What It Is: This resource shows the speech errors a student is likely to make
when unable to pronounce a sound.
Its Uses: Knowledge of speech errors is widely used for at least two reasons:
Illustration: These are common [r] errors among students with errors affect-
ing late-acquired sounds:
The most common error is gliding — that is, [w] for [r]. Deletion of
[r] after vowels and in consonant clusters also is a common error.
5. Key Environment
Its Use: A key environment is widely used for one important purpose:
6. Metaphor
7. Touch Cue
8. Initial Screening
Instructions: I’m going to say some words. Please say the word
after me.
Medial
7. Wishing _____________
8. Ocean _____________
9. Washer _____________
Final
10. Dish _____________
11. Crush _____________
12. Irish _____________
13. Mars _____________
14. Marsh _____________
15. Borscht _____________
Comments/Notes:
CHAPTER 6: Overview 137
Imitation
1. chip _____________
2. catch _____________
Favorite Words
Names of family members:
Favorite people, heroes, and activities:
Phonetic Placement
1. Ask the student to make the train sound “choo choo.”
138 The late eight
Shaping
1. Instruct student to say “Bet you” slowly.
2. Next, instruct student to say “Bet you” fast, resulting in the produc-
tion of “Betcha.”
3. If “betcha” doesn’t work, repeat with “Got you.”
10. Demonstration
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student, “Please open your mouth.”
2. Once the mouth is open, with Q-tip dab a little peanut butter or
other favorite food on alveolar ridge (for tongue tip raised [s]) or
behind lower front teeth (for tongue tip lowered [s]).
3. Ask the student to touch the food with the tongue tip.
What They Are: Phonetic placement and shaping techniques are procedures
to teach sounds. Phonetic placement techniques entail instructing a student
how to place the articulators to make a sound. Phonetic placement techniques
are similar to demonstrations, the difference being that phonetic placement
techniques require a production from a student and demonstrations do not.
Shaping techniques rely on similarities between sounds to shape a sound a
student can pronounce into one he or she cannot pronounce. Techniques
presented in this book were culled from many sources, published and unpub-
lished. The main published sources were books long out of print, most espe-
cially Nemoy and Davis (1954). Unpublished sources include many talented
CHAPTER 6: Overview 139
and creative clinicians that the author has had the pleasure to interact with
over the years.
Clinician: What is a word for ocean that starts with the snake sound?
Student: Sea.
Clinician: That’s right. Do you remember how you used to say the s
sound?
Student: th
Clinician: Good. Now say sea the new way, the old way, and then the
new way again.
Student: Sea. Thea. Sea.
Clinician: Great. Now say sea three times, listening to yourself and
trying to make the sound the new way.
Student: Sea. Sea. Sea.
CHAPTER 6: Overview 141
Illustration: This is an awareness activity for [z] and a speech activity for [r],
followed by an example showing ways in which an activity might be varied.
What It Is: This book contains a list of approximately 4,000 different com-
monly found words containing late-acquired sounds. With very few excep-
tions, the words are short (one to two syllables) and easily pictured. The
word lists for each sound are divided into major phonetic environments in
which the sound occurs. Included in the word lists are lists of minimal pairs,
deletions, and themes.
A Good Question
Someone may ask: Yes, but how do these word lists differ
from those in the 40,000 words book — in addition to that
book having 40,000 words and this book having only 4,000?
An Answer
The author has a well-worn copy of 40,000 Selected Words
(Blockcolsky, Frazer, & Frazer, 1987). The major differences
between the word lists in that fine book and those in this
book include:
Illustration: The following is a sample of the entry for [s] at the beginning
of words, followed by a theme for [l]:
Companion Website
n A “cheat sheet”
n Initial screening form
n Initial stimulability testing form
n Demonstrations
n Phonetic placement and shaping exercises
n Shells for speech exercises
n Shells for language activities
n Complete word list
n Minimal pairs
n Deletions
n Themes
Evaluation
n Initial screening
n Screening for stimulability
n Definition
n Acquisition
n Relative frequency
n Errors
Guide to Resources
Evaluation
Treatment
The most typical referral source for a student is a parent or teacher. A student
may then be observed in a classroom or playground before receiving an
initial speech screening and a screening test for stimulability.
Initial Screening
Definition
Acquisition
Acquisition data indicate the age at which 50% and 75% of children acquire
a sound. If all other things are equal (they seldom are), a clinician may
decide to first treat an earlier acquired sound. Indeed, for some clinicians
the importance of acquisition data is the primary criterion used in the selec-
tion process.
Relative Frequency
Errors
Treatment
Once a sound or sounds have been selected for treatment, the following
resources are used:
n Metaphors
n Touch cues
n Demonstrations
n Phonetic placement and shaping techniques
n Key environments
n Word lists
n Awareness and speech exercises
n Language activities
Metaphors, touch cues, and demonstrations all provide useful ways to refer
to treatment sounds, and especially during early treatment phases may help
focus a student on the task at hand. As treatment progresses, they serve as
reminders and prompts.
Metaphors
Touch Cues
Touch cues are finger positions that represent a treatment sound, allowing
a clinician to refer to a treatment sound using modalities other than speech.
Touch cues grossly mimic speech movements. An older student understands
that, for example, the touch cue for velar consonants is made parallel to
the back of the mouth, representing where the tongue is raised. A younger
student may benefit from a touch cue without realizing its mimicking quality.
For such a student, a touch cue is a visual and tactile reminder, a way to say,
“Remember: this is the sound we are working on.”
Demonstrations
one sound into another. These techniques are used when a nonstimulable
sound is selected for treatment. Students 7 years or older typically possess
sufficient attention and language skills to benefit from these techniques. With
a younger student, success with these techniques is more hit-or-miss. The
techniques are typically inappropriate (and ineffective) with a child under
4 years.
Though every clinician has favorite phonetic placement and shaping
techniques, no single technique works for every student. In general, a clini-
cian selects one that makes intuitive sense and then engages in trial-and-error
dynamic assessment. Often, from a few to 5 to 10 minutes is sufficient to
determine if a particular technique will prove successful. In general, when
selecting a technique, most clinicians prefer those that are simpler and have
shorter instructions. Longer techniques are turned to when the shorter,
simpler ones do not yield results.
The phonetic placement and shaping techniques listed in this book are
“bare bones recipes” to expand and modify as a clinician desires. Often, the
actual phonetic placement or shaping technique used with a student contains
the following elements:
The following illustrates one possible way to fully expand a bare bones
phonetic placement technique:
Instructions:
1. First demonstrate the method on yourself.
2. To begin, place your tongue between your upper and lower front teeth.
3. Place a feather or small piece of paper in front of your mouth, about a
half inch to an inch from your tongue.
4. Blow air over your tongue to move the feather or paper.
5. Explain, “That’s how you make the leaking tire sound. Now it’s your
turn.”
6. Instruct the student to stick out his or her tongue just as you did.
7. When the tongue is out, place the feather or paper before the mouth.
150 The late eight
Key Environments
Beginning of Word
Establish [s], [l], and [r] before a high front vowel. Once established, expand
the number of different vowels that follow. For a student who is strongly
affected by the adjacent vowel, back high vowels are likely to be more chal-
lenging than front ones.
End of Word
[s] is more likely to be established here than [l] and [r]. Establish after a high
front vowel. Next, to make word-initial sounds, have the word-final sounds
be followed by a word beginning with a vowel, such as “bus and.” This
encourages the sound to “migrate” to start the following word, resulting in,
for example, “bu sand.”
Between Vowels
[l] and [r] are more likely to be established here than [s], though some
students find [s] easier to make here, too. For all three consonants, establish
between two high front vowels, as in ili. Once established, add different
adjacent vowels. To expand to word-initial position, have the student drop
the first vowel, resulting in, for example, [li]. To expand to word-final posi-
tion, follow the same procedure, resulting in, for example, [il].
Consonant Clusters
For [s], establish after [t] as in “pizza” or the nonsense word [tsi]. To expand
the environments in which [s] occurs, have [ts] be followed by different
CHAPTER 6: Overview 151
Word Lists
Word lists are used to generate stimuli to help establish a sound in a student’s
speech, and then to practice it. Words, rather than nonsense syllables, are the
vehicle for teaching a treatment sound for two reasons:
Language Activities
Language activities use school books and other outside materials, including
stories a student is reading, favorite stories from home, and articles from
152 The late eight
newspapers and magazines. These materials, because they are familiar and
widely used, are easy to adapt by families, aides, and teachers. Many times
their use also has the practical advantage of improving a student’s academic
skills. Though the purpose of therapy is speech, much is gained if in the
process of learning speech a student also does better on classroom assign-
ments or gives a better oral report.
Summary
Resources contained in the first half of this book are “tools of the trade”
to assist a clinician in evaluating and treating such speech disorders. The
discussion in the second half of the chapter illustrates one way care might be
conceptualized and carried out. The resources are flexible and can be used
within a wide variety of therapeutic approaches and with a range of students,
including children and adults and both first and second language learners.
The hope in assembling these resources is to help provide clinical care to the
many students with this highly prevalent developmental difficulty.
References
[θ]
Definition
[θ] is made with the tongue tip between the upper and lower front teeth.
The airstream is a continuous hiss between the upper tongue and the
upper teeth. The vocal folds are apart. The technical definition of [θ] is
voiceless interdental fricative.
Acquisition
Fifty percent of children acquire [θ] by 4;6 and 75% of children acquire
[θ] by 6;0.
Relative Frequency
[θ] is ranked seventh in relative frequency compared with the other
late-acquired consonants. It ranks twenty-first in relative frequency com-
pared with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occur-
rence compared with all English consonants is 0.9%.
Errors
[s] for [θ] is a common error, as is [f] for [θ]. A less common error for [θ]
among school-aged students is [t] or [p] for [θ].
153
154 The late eight
Key Environments
End of a syllable or word, as in teeth
Before a high front vowel, as in thin
Possible Metaphors
Select metaphors based on the aspect of speech that is the focus of therapy.
Touch Cue
Finger in front of lips.
Instructions
Place the student’s finger in the middle of the front of the lips.
Initial Screening Test for [θ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. Thigh _____________
2. Thunder _____________
3. Thorn _____________
4. Threw _____________
5. Thriller _____________
6. Throne _____________
Medial
7. Nothing _____________
8. Python _____________
9. Without _____________
Final
10. Bath _____________
11. Oath _____________
12. Teeth _____________
13. Sixth _____________
14. Ninth _____________
15. North _____________
Comments/Notes:
155
Stimulability Tests for [θ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. Thumb _____________
2. Tooth _____________
Key Environments
End of a syllable or word
1. teeth _____________
2. [iθ] _____________
Favorite Words
Names of family members:
Favorite people, heroes, and activities:
Notes/Comments:
156
CHAPTER 7: [T] 157
Place: Interdental
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Fricative
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encourage the student
to move the flower in the wind.
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Run your finger or the student’s finger down the student’s arm while making
several long voiceless fricatives to demonstrate the “hissing” quality and
length of fricatives.
Voicing: Voiceless
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Instruct the student to listen to and identify the difference between a voice-
less and voiced [a].
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her to hum, which
heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
CHAPTER 7: [T] 159
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
If the student is able to produce a voiced and voiceless fricative, ask him or
her to cover the ears and make these sounds. Alternatively, ask the student
to make [h] and [a].
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
You and the student place one hand on your throat and the other on the
student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds together, telling
each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
Instructions:
If the student is able to produce a pair of voiced and voiceless oral stops,
attach a small piece of paper or a paper flower to the end of a tongue depres-
sor or pencil and ask the student to “make the paper (or flower) move.” The
paper is more likely to move when a voiceless consonant is produced than
when a voiced consonant is produced (be careful in providing instructions
to the student, however, because a strongly articulated voiced oral stop will
also move the flower).
160 The late eight
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Shaping Exercises
Objects: None
CHAPTER 7: [T] 161
Instructions:
1. Demonstrate the difference between the places of production for [f] and
[θ].
2. Ask the student to say [f] while moving the tongue to lie between the
upper and lower front teeth, resulting in [θ]. (Note: To facilitate [ð],
develop from [v].)
This method approaches [θ] from the opposite direction as the first method:
rather than from slightly anterior [f], this method approaches [θ] from slightly
posterior [s].
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Demonstrate the difference between the place of production for [s] and
the place of production for [θ].
2. Next, instruct the student to say [s] while moving the tongue to lie
between the upper and lower front teeth, resulting in [θ]. (Note: To facili-
tate the [ð], develop from [z].)
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
162
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Thin 1. __________________
Thumper 2. __________________
Thick 3. __________________
Thief 4. __________________
Thumb 5. __________________
Thank you 6. __________________
Thigh 7. __________________
Thunder 8. __________________
Thorn 9. __________________
Thumbtack 10. __________________
163
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then say
the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
164
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
Thin In 1. __________________
Thick Ick 2. __________________
Thumb Um 3. __________________
Thigh I, eye 4. __________________
Thug Ugh 5. __________________
Thor Or 6. __________________
Thaw Awe 7. __________________
Thought Ought 8. __________________
Think Ink 9. __________________
Theory Eerie 10. __________________
165
166 The late eight
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Thin 1. __________________
Thumper 2. __________________
Thick 3. __________________
Thief 4. __________________
Thumb 5. __________________
Thank you 6. __________________
Thigh 7. __________________
Thunder 8. __________________
Thorn 9. __________________
Thumbtack 10. __________________
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note. Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Thin 1. __________________
Thumper 2. __________________
Thick 3. __________________
Thief 4. __________________
Thumb 5. __________________
Thank you 6. __________________
Thigh 7. __________________
Thunder 8. __________________
Thorn 9. __________________
Thumbtack 10. __________________
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with * and then say the word
with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un, and
then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note. Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Thin 1. __________________
Thumper 2. __________________
Thick 3. __________________
Thief 4. __________________
Thumb 5. __________________
Thank you 6. __________________
Thigh 7. __________________
Thunder 8. __________________
Thorn 9. __________________
Thumbtack 10. __________________
168
CHAPTER 7: [T] 169
Beginning of Words
Thermos
Theme song
Thebe dweeb
Thoughtful
Thatch batch, hatch
Theory eerie dearie
Thorough burrow
Thirst burst, nursed
Thistle missile
Three tree
Throw row
Thrill rill
Throttle
Thrift shop
Thread read tread
Throat wrote
Threw rue true, crew
Throw rug
Thriller
Throne roan grown, prone
Thrifty
Throwing rowing
Threshold
Throng wrong prong
Thirsty
Thrive
Thrash rash crash, brash, lash
Through rue grew, true, crew
Thrush rush brush, crush
Threat
Thrust rust crust
Throb rob
CHAPTER 7: [T] 171
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
All Environments
Ends of Words
Mammoth
Bath baa
Oath oh
Dishcloth
Ruth rue
Teeth tea
Babe Ruth
Steam bath
Macbeth
Sith
Goldsmith
Sloth slaw
Keith key
Sweet tooth
Bike path
Swath
Birdbath
Tooth two
Sleuth slew
Math
Phone booth
Kenneth
Plymouth
Faith fey, Fay
South sow
Blacksmith
Sheath she
Path
Faith Fay, fey
Mouth Mao
Cheesecloth
Beth
Breath
Myth
Broth braw
Booth boo
Death
Wreath
CHAPTER 7: [T] 173
Fourth four
Fifth
Sixth six
Seventh seven
Eighth eight
Ninth nine
Tenth ten
Eleventh eleven
Twelfth
North nor
Warmth warm
Themes
Toothache
Toothless
Teeth T, tea wreath, Keith
Mouth Mao
Breath
Thirst burst, nursed
Sweet tooth
Tooth two
Toothbrush
Thirsty
Mouthwash
Toothpick
Toothpaste
174 The late eight
Thumb gum
Thigh I, eye bye, high, pie, tie
Mouth
Throat wrote
Bathmat
Bathtub
Bathrobe
Bathroom
Bath baa
Playthings
Sith
Darth Mal
Darth Vader
Throne roan grown, prone
Threat
Bathmat
Bathtub
Thermos
Threshold
Bathrobe
Bathroom
Thimble nimble, cymbal
Throw rug
Thumbtack
Mouthwash
Playthings
Toothpick
Broth bra
Thread read tread
Toothpaste
Thong long, song
CHAPTER 7: [T] 175
Athlete
Author
Sleuth slew
Blacksmith
Cutthroat
Thief chief, beef, leaf
Thug bug, hug
Path
Thistle missile
Thatch batch, hatch
Thrush rush brush, crush
Thicket picket
Mammoth
Sloth slaw
Bike path
Thaw awe saw, paw
Panther
Earthquake
Python
Thorny corny
Thunder
Thorn corn, torn, horn, worn
Throwing rowing
Thrive
Think ink rink, wink, pink
Thrash rash crash, brash, lash
Threaten
Thrust rust crust
Throb rob
Throw row crow, pro
Threw rue true
176 The late eight
Three tree
Fourth four
Fourth grade
Tenth ten
Monthly
Thursday
Third bird, word, nerd, heard
Third base
Thirty dirty
Thousand
Sixth grade
Fifth grade
Thirteen
Third world
North nor
South sow
CHAPTER 7: [T] 177
North Star
Jethro
Thumper jumper, bumper
Swarthmore
North Pole
South Seas
Faith fey, Fay
Babe Ruth
Ruth rue
Thebe dweeb
Mathew
Thebes dweebs
Beth
Athens
Nathan
Kathy
Carthage
Martha
Nathan
Macbeth
Goldsmith
Keith
Kenneth
Plymouth
Thelma Selma
Thor or soar, core, shore, floor
Thoreau
Chapter Eight
[ð]
Definition
[ð] is made with the tongue tip between the upper and lower front teeth.
The airstream is a continuous hiss between the upper tongue and the
upper teeth. The vocal folds are together. The technical definition of [ð]
is voiced interdental fricative.
Acquisition
Fifty percent of children acquire [ð] by 4;6 and 75% of children acquire
[ð] by 5;6.
Relative Frequency
[ð] is ranked fourth in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks eleventh in relative frequency compared
with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence
compared with all English consonants is 4.1%.
Errors
[d] for [ð] is a common error. A less common error among school-aged
students is [b] for [ð].
179
180 The late eight
Key Environments
Beginning of syllable and between vowels, as in weather
Before a high front vowel, as in these
Possible Metaphors
Select metaphors based on the aspect of speech that is the focus of therapy.
Touch Cue
Finger in front of lips.
Instructions
Place the student’s finger in the middle of the front of the lips.
Initial Screening Test for [ð]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. This _____________
2. Them _____________
3. Then _____________
4. The _____________
Medial
5. Clothing _____________
6. Weather _____________
7. Father _____________
8. Feather _____________
Final
9. Breathe _____________
10. Teethe _____________
11. Sheathe _____________
12. Soothe _____________
Comments/Notes:
181
Stimulability Tests for [ð]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. the _____________
2. mother _____________
Key Environments
Between vowels
1. weather _____________
2. either _____________
Favorite Words
1. Demonstrate placing the tongue between the upper and lower teeth.
2. Instruct the student to put his or her hand in front of the mouth and
blow through the teeth to feel the airflow with the voice box turned on.
1. Demonstrate the difference between the place of production for [z] and
the place of production for [ð].
2. Next, instruct the student to say [z] while moving his or her tongue to
rest between his or her upper and lower teeth, resulting in [ð].
Notes/Comments:
182
CHAPTER 8: [D] 183
Place: Interdental
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Fricative
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions: Tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encour-
age the student to move the flower in the wind.
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your finger or the student’s finger down the student’s arm
while making several long voiceless fricatives to demonstrate the “hissing”
quality and length of fricatives.
Voicing: Voiced
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
To facilitate [ð], follow the steps for [θ] but also use demonstrations to instruct
the student to turn on the voice box.
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
186
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
That 1. __________________
This 2. __________________
Them 3. __________________
Then 4. __________________
The Hague 5. __________________
Their 6. __________________
Those 7. __________________
These 8. __________________
They 9. __________________
There 10. __________________
187
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
188
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
They A 1. __________________
Than Ann 2. __________________
Though O 3. __________________
Their air 4. __________________
Those Os 5. __________________
These ease 6. __________________
There air 7. __________________
That at 8. __________________
Then N 9. __________________
They A 10. __________________
189
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
That 1. __________________
This 2. __________________
Them 3. __________________
Then 4. __________________
The Hague 5. __________________
Their 6. __________________
Those 7. __________________
These 8. __________________
They 9. __________________
There 10. __________________
190
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
That 1. __________________
This 2. __________________
Them 3. __________________
Then 4. __________________
The Hague 5. __________________
Their 6. __________________
Those 7. __________________
These 8. __________________
They 9. __________________
There 10. __________________
191
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
That 1. __________________
This 2. __________________
Them 3. __________________
Then 4. __________________
The Hague 5. __________________
Their 6. __________________
Those 7. __________________
These 8. __________________
They 9. __________________
There 10. __________________
192
CHAPTER 8: [D] 193
Beginning of Words
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
—
194 The late eight
End of Words
Breathe Bree
Teethe Tea
Sheathe She
Soothe Sue
Bathe Bay
Unsheathe
Smooth
Clothe
Scythe
Sun bathe
Themes
Earthquake
Slither
Sun bathe
Leather feather, weather, heather
Weather feather, leather
Feather weather, heather, leather
Heather feather, weather, leather
Northern
The Hague
Arthur
Heather feather, weather, leather
CHAPTER 8: [D] 195
Bather Bay
Bathing
Bathrobe
Clothe
Clothing
Bather Bayer
Breathe Bree
Breathing
Teethe T, tea
Teething
[s]
Definition
[s] is made in either of two ways. Some people produce [s] with the
tongue tip up behind the upper front teeth, others say [s] with the tongue
tip down behind the lower front teeth. Neither one is the “right way.”
Follow the student’s lead in deciding which way to teach [s]. If a student
appears to find it easier to say [s] with the tongue tip up, teach the
sound that way; if the student appears to find it easier to say [s] with
the tongue tip down, teach the sound that way. For both varieties of [s],
the airstream is continuous and the vocal folds are apart. The technical
definition of [s] is voiceless alveolar fricative.
Acquisition
Fifty percent of children acquire [s] by 3;6 and 75% of children acquire
[s] by 6;0.
Relative Frequency
[s] is ranked first in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks 3rd in relative frequency compared with
all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence compared
with all English consonants is 7.1%.
197
198 The late eight
Errors
A common error for [s] is lisping. During lisping, [s] is pronounced with
the tongue tip between the teeth, as for [θ]. Another common error for [s]
is using the tongue blade instead of the tongue tip, which is sometimes
called bladed [s]. A third common error is lateralizing, in which instead
of air flowing over the top of the tongue, it flows over the sides of the
tongue, as for an [l].
Key Environments
End of syllables, as in bus
Next to [t], as in steep, pizza, beats
Next to high front vowels, as in see
Possible Metaphors
Select metaphors based on the aspect of speech that is the focus of therapy.
Touch Cue
Finger on the corner of the mouth (finger up for tongue tip raised sound
or finger down for tongue tip down sound).
Instructions
Place the student’s finger in the corner of the lips, and remind the
student to keep upper and lower teeth close together.
Initial Screening Test for [s]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Medial
10. Acid _____________
11. Messy _____________
12. Icing _____________
Final
13. Dice _____________ 17. Husk _____________
14. Moose _____________ 18. Toast _____________
15. House _____________ 19. Fierce _____________
16. Wasp _____________ 20. Wince _____________
Comments/Notes:
199
Stimulability Tests for [s]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. sun _____________
2. bus _____________
Key Environments
Favorite Words
200
Phonetic Placement _____________
1. Place a tongue depressor just behind the student’s upper or lower front
teeth, depending on which variety of [s] is being taught, and ask him or
her to use the tongue tip to hold it there.
2. Next, ask the student to keep the tongue tip still while you carefully
remove the tongue depressor.
3. Ask the student to breathe out, resulting in [s].
Notes/Comments:
201
202 The late eight
Place: Alveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Fricative
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions: Tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encour-
age the student to move the flower in the wind.
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your finger or the student’s finger down the student’s arm
while making several long voiceless fricatives to demonstrate the “hissing”
quality and length of fricatives.
Voicing: Voiceless
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
A key to making [s] is correct tongue tip placement. To make [s] the tongue
tip is behind either the upper or lower front teeth, the upper and lower teeth
are relatively close, and a narrow stream of air hisses between the tongue
tip and the teeth. Once this is achieved, the hiss of air is achieved simply by
breathing out, and getting the jaw relatively closed so the upper and lower
teeth nearly touch is usually achieved with a few simple instructions. All the
following phonetic placement techniques focus on placing the tongue tip for
[s]. The first two methods are used either for [s] with the tongue tip raised or
lowered, the third and fourth are for [s] with the tongue tip raised, and the
fifth is for [s] with the tongue tip lowered.
This method and the following method work whether the tongue tip is up
or down.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind either the upper or lower
front teeth and then to pull the tongue back a little bit.
2. Close the student’s teeth so the teeth barely touch.
3. Place your finger in front of the center of the student’s mouth, and
say, “Breathe slowly over your tongue toward my finger.” The sound
produced by the student when breathing out approximates [s].
Instructions:
1. Place a tongue depressor just behind the student’s upper or lower front
teeth and ask the student to use the tongue tip to hold it there.
2. Ask the student to keep the tongue still while you carefully remove the
tongue depressor.
3. Ask the student to breathe out, resulting in [s].
206 The late eight
This method is a little more involved than the previous two. It is useful for
a student who really doesn’t seem to “get” the idea that the tongue tip must
be raised.
Instructions:
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to raise his or her tongue so that the sides are firmly in
contact with the inner surface of the upper back teeth. (An alternative
method is to instruct the student to stick out the tongue slightly, lower
the upper teeth to come into contact with the sides of the tongue, and
then pull the tongue inside the mouth.)
2. Ask the student to groove the tongue slightly along the midline. (If
needed, ask the student to protrude the tongue and place a clean object
such as a drinking straw along the midline of the tongue. Then ask the
student to raise the sides of the tongue slightly around the straw.)
3. Carefully withdraw the straw.
4. Ask the student to place the tongue tip about a quarter of an inch behind
the upper teeth and then ask the student to bring the teeth together.
CHAPTER 9: [s] 207
5. Instruct the student to blow air along the groove of the tongue toward
the lower teeth. If the student has difficulty directing the air along the
tongue groove, insert a drinking straw into the student’s mouth and
instruct the student to blow through the straw, which often results in [s].
This method works surprisingly well with some students. It can be tried for [s]
with the tongue tip raised, though my clinical experience is that it is usually
more effective for the tongue tip-lowered variety of [s].
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to brush his or her lower gums with the tongue while
attempting to say [s].
2. Ask the student to stop moving the tongue and to bring the upper and
lower teeth close together, but not touching.
3. Instruct the student to breathe out through the mouth, resulting in [s].
Shaping Techniques
If a student can make a [z], he or she can usually be taught to make an [s]
fairly easily.
Objects: None
Instructions:
Instruct the student to say [z] and then to turn off the voice box. For some
students, this is sufficient to result in [s]. (Note: To facilitate [z], instruct the
student to turn on the voice box while saying [s].)
This method works well either for a student who lisps or for one who otherwise
has a well-established interdental consonant.
208 The late eight
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to protrude the tongue between the teeth and to
say [θ].
2. As the student says [θ], instruct him or her to bring the tongue back into
the mouth and behind the upper or lower front teeth, depending on
which variety of [s] is being facilitated. An alternative method is to ask
the student to scrape the tongue tip back along the back of the front
teeth. (If needed, the tip of the student’s tongue can be pushed inward
with a tongue depressor.)
3. Next, ask the student to either raise or lower the tongue tip slightly,
depending on which type of [s] is being taught.
4. Ask the student to blow air through the mouth. The sound produced
approximates [s]. (Note: To facilitate [z], develop from [ð].)
These four methods help a student to convert a lateral [s] into [s].
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Talk about the “butterfly position” for the tongue, or the position the
tongue is in when you prolong the i in pip or the ee in peep. Point out
to the student that the lateral margins of the tongue are in contact with
the teeth: like a butterfly with its wings up. Older students sometimes
like and are amused by the imagery of a butterfly assuming the “bracing
position.”
2. Draw the student’s attention to the way the tongue edges (wings) press
quite firmly on their teeth. You might mention that “floppy edges” or
“floppy wings” let the air out sideways, while “strong edges” or “strong
CHAPTER 9: [s] 209
wings” do not. Help the student imagine the midline of the tongue as the
butterfly’s body, visualizing the groove that forms along its center.
3. Tell the student the groove is there for her/him to “shoot” the air down,
straight out in front. It is there specially to guide the air in the right
direction for a super sounding ess. Use your hands to convey the idea of
“wings up,” “wings firmly tucked in against the teeth,” and language such
as, “a nice little groove where the butterfly’s body sits,” and a “straight
shot” of air. Employ imagery to talk about “shooting straight” and “shoot-
ing sideways.”
4. Next, model [t] and have the student imitate your production. Do the
same with [t-t], then [t-t-t] and then [t-t-t-t]. If the student is producing a
schwa or other vowel between the consonants, eliminate it if possible.
Aim for a “pure” sounding sequence of consonants. If the student needs
a vowel to get from one [t] to the next, use i as in pip (ti- ti- ti-) or ee
as in peep (tee- tee- tee-). Some clinicians prefer ee because it creates a
firmer “seal” between the tongue margins and the teeth, and, potentially
anyway, this discourages lateral airflow.
5. Increase the rate at which the student repeats [t-t-t-t]. Notice the subtle [s]
that starts occurring between the t’s. Point this out to the student in your
speech and in his/her speech. The sequence is now starting to sound
like [ts-ts-ts-ts].
6. The student will probably be unaware at this stage that the little “under-
articulated” [s] is there. In this step, tell the student to produce the [t] and
to let a little air come out at the end of the sound. Demonstrate what you
mean, without actually instructing the student to produce “t and then
s.” Just emphasize that you want to have “air happening” after the [t].
Gradually “sharpen” the [ts] thus produced, so that it becomes obvious
that there are two sounds, [t] and [s], being articulated clearly. Once it’s
perfect, have the student practice saying [ts] until he or she can do it very
easily. It is a good idea to stay on this level for several days.
7. Now put the [ts] combination into real words. Again using the vowels ee as
in feet or i as in fit to facilitate correct placement, present the student with a
practice list. For example, “He eats meat, she eats candy, it eats grain,” and
so forth; or, “It’s a boy, it’s a man, it’s a cow,” and so forth or, “It’s good, it’s
bad, it’s tall,” etc. In making up the phrases or sentences, do not include
other words containing [s] or [z]. This means don’t have items such as, “He
eats pasta” or “It’s a zoo” or “It’s silly.” Once [ts] is established in words,
practice [ts] in simple sentences that do not contain other s-words.
8. When the [s] the student is producing sounds clear and “adultlike,” it is
ready to separate from the [t]. Without mentioning the tongue too often,
instruct the student to say [ts] without moving the tongue, and then add
an ess, like this: [ts-s]. This may be difficult for the student at first, so take
it slowly and quietly and give plenty of support and encouragement.
9. Next, instruct the student to make the [ts] silent, resulting in [s].
210 The late eight
Instructions:
1. Demonstrate air flowing through a straw protruding from the side of the
mouth when a lateral [s] is made and air flowing through a straw placed
in the front of the mouth when a correct [s] is made.
2. Encourage the student to close the teeth and to direct the airflow through
a straw placed in front of the mouth. This typically results in [s]. (Note:
To facilitate [z], develop from lateral [z].)
Instructions:
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to gently bite the sides of the tongue, drop the tongue
tip, and put it behind the front teeth.
2. Ask the student to smile and blow air out the front of the mouth, result-
ing in [s].
CHAPTER 9: [s] 211
These methods rely on the fact that [t] and [s] are made in the same place of
production. It is extremely helpful for the many students who have a well-
established [t]. A limitation on its utility is that it requires the student to follow
steps which some students find challenging.
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to say [t] in tea with strong aspiration. If said quickly
and forcefully, [tsi] results. An alternative to this procedure is to ask the
student to say [tsi] instead of tea.
2. Instruct the student to say [tsi] without the vowel, resulting in [ts].
3. Ask the student to prolong the [s] portion of [ts], resulting in tsss.
4. Ask the student to make [t] silent, resulting in [s].
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to open the mouth and to put the tongue in position for
[t].
2. Instruct the student to lower the tongue slightly and to send the air over
the tongue.
3. Place the student’s finger in front of the mouth to feel the emission of
air, which typically results in [s].
Third Method
This is a clever variation of the [s] from [t] method that was developed by
Gillian Fleming of Dunedin, New Zealand, who kindly consented to let it be
placed in this book. It works especially well for establishing [s] at the end of
words. Gillian writes, “I feel that by the time a child reaches school age, he/
she is very aware of the [s] sound. People have been trying to correct it for
ages, saying things like, ‘Put your tongue in’ or ‘It’s not [θ], it’s [s]’ and other
212 The late eight
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to place the tongue tip on the ridge behind the top
teeth, resulting in a series of short [t] sounds (t — t — t — t — t).
2. Next, instruct the student to make [t] a little longer (ttt ttt ttt).
3. Next, make [t] a little longer still (ttttttt — tttttttt — ttttttt).
4. Lastly, put the long ttttttt on the end of a word (catttt — big cattttt — fluffy
cattttt — etc.), resulting in word final [ts].
5. Once clear [s] is established, practice final [ts] in other words (bats, boats,
kites, etc.).
6. When the clinician believes the time is right, point out that the student
is now making a correct [s] sound.
[s] from [ ʃ ]
As this method attests, sometimes the only thing that is needed to succeed is
a good smile. Retracting the lips to smile pulls the tongue forward into an [s].
Objects: None
Instructions:
This method, like the one above, converts one fricative into another. The
difference is that it approaches [s] from an anterior position rather than a
posterior one. I have found it a little less successful than the previous method.
Objects: None
CHAPTER 9: [s] 213
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to lift the tongue tip slowly while making a prolonged
[f].
2. Ask the student to bring the front teeth close together but not quite
touching. If needed, gently pull out the student’s lower lip slightly.
3. Ask the student to smile while making the sound, which often results
in [s]. (Note: To facilitate [z], develop from [v] or use [s] and instruct the
student to turn on his or her voice box.)
Admittedly, this and the method that follows are a somewhat far stretch. This
method relies on the similarity in the place of production of [i] and [s], and
the following method relies on similarity in the frication created by [s] and
[h]. Neither would be my first choice for a method to remediate [s], but each
has a place in the clinical repertoire.
Objects: None
Instructions:
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to gradually close the teeth while saying [h].
2. Ask the student to raise the tongue tip gradually while producing a pro-
longed [h] until the resulting sound is [s]. (Note: To facilitate [z], instruct
the student to turn on the voice box.)
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
214
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Sat 1. __________________
Salmon 2. __________________
Sail 3. __________________
Salad 4. __________________
Sailboat 5. __________________
Socks 6. __________________
Soap 7. __________________
Sew 8. __________________
Sun 9. __________________
Sally 10. ________________
215
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
216
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
Sat at 1. __________________
Sail ale, ail 2. __________________
Sew O, oh 3. __________________
Sally alley 4. __________________
Silk ilk 5. __________________
Seagull eagle 6. __________________
Sick ick 7. __________________
Sole ole 8. __________________
Sink ink 9. __________________
Sea E 10. ________________
217
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Sat 1. __________________
Salmon 2. __________________
Sail 3. __________________
Salad 4. __________________
Sailboat 5. __________________
Socks 6. __________________
Soap 7. __________________
Sew 8. __________________
Sun 9. __________________
Sally 10. ________________
218
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note. Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Sat 1. __________________
Salmon 2. __________________
Sail 3. __________________
Salad 4. __________________
Sailboat 5. __________________
Socks 6. __________________
Soap 7. __________________
Sew 8. __________________
Sun 9. __________________
Sally 10. ________________
219
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Sat 1. __________________
Salmon 2. __________________
Sail 3. __________________
Salad 4. __________________
Sailboat 5. __________________
Socks 6. __________________
Soap 7. __________________
Sew 8. __________________
Sun 9. __________________
Sally 10. ________________
220
CHAPTER 9: [s] 221
Beginning of Words
Circle
Sit it hit, mit, kit, knit, bit
Soldier
Sam am ham, lamb
Salesclerk
Six wicks, mix
Seaweed
Suit cute, hoot, boot
Cindy Indy windy
Soar or, oar four, more, tore, bore, core
Ceiling peeling, reeling, kneeling
Sub hub, cub
Sunlight
Saint ain’t paint, faint
Subway
Seal eel Neil, heel
Cement
Sour hour tower, shower, cower
Sunny honey, money, funny,
bunny
Suck uck buck, luck, shuck, muck
City kitty, witty
Soup loop, poop
Salt ought malt
Sign nine, fine, shine
Sandal handle
Sid id hid, kid, bid, lid
Song thong, long
Sank rank, lank, bank
Suds duds, cuds
Soak oak poke, choke, yolk, joke,
woke
Serve Merv, curve
Sound round, pound
Sue ooh boo, who, due
Soot foot
Sing wing, thing, king, ring
Sip whip, rip, lip, hip, dip
Cell L fell, dell, Mel
Sack back, rack, hack, pack, tack
Surf turf
Soil oil boil, coil
Sock lock, mock
CHAPTER 9: [s] 223
[sp]
Spot pot
Spell
Spicy
Spout pout
Spanish
Space pace
Spill pill
Spit pit
Spokes pokes
Spider
Sponge
Spear peer
Spark park
Spank
Spoon
Spareribs
Spin pin
Spy pie
Spade paid
Spooky Pooky
Spy pie
Spaceship
Spike pike
Spirit
Speak peak
Special
Spear peer
Speed peed
[st]
Stop top
Sting ting
Stoplight
Stink
Story Tory
Stack tack
Stork torque
Stagecoach
Steak take
Stick tick
224 The late eight
Storm
Stairs tears
Stone Age
Stalk talk
Stool tool
Stallion
Staff
Steam team
Steal teal
Starfish
Stamp
Stale tale, tail
Stock car
Stand
Stub tub
Star tar
Stew two
Staff
Stag
Stake take
Standard
Statue
Steady Teddy
[sk]
School cool
Ski key, C
Skin kin
Scale kale
Skinny
Skirt Kurt
Skier
Scarf
School bus cool bus
Scar car
Skate Kate
Scotland
Skunk
Skip Kip
Sky
Skillful
CHAPTER 9: [s] 225
Sketch
Skew Q
Skim Kim
Skull cull
Skid kid
Skill kill
[sf]
Sphinx finks
Sphere ear
Sphincter
[sl]
Slip lip
Sleigh lay
Slide lied
Sleep leap
Sled dog
Slug lug
Sling
Sled led
Slimy limey
Sleepy
Sleeve leave
Slippers
Sliver liver
Slime lime
Sloth
Slab lab
Slang
Slick lick
Slump lump
Slow low
Slash lash
Slink link
Slate late
Slave
Slop
Slay lay
Sleek leak
226 The late eight
[sw]
Swing wing
Swimmer
Sweater wetter
Sweatshirt
Sweet tooth
Sweat wet
Swiss
Sweet wheat
Switch witch
Swamp
Swim whim
Swan wan
Swelter welter
Swerve
Swear wear
Sweden
Swat watt
Sweep weep
Sway way
Swap
Swallow wallow
Swarm warm
Swift wift
[sm]
Small mall
Smell Mel
Smile mile
Smelly
Smog
Smoke
Smack Mack
Smoky
Smooth
Smallest
Smock mock
Smart mart
Smoky
Smash mash
CHAPTER 9: [s] 227
Smother mother
Smudge
Smite mite
[sn]
Snack knack
Snail nail
Snout
Snorkel
Snap nap
Snake bite
Snow no, know
Snarl gnarl
Snack bar
Snow bank no bank
Snow White no white
Sneakers
Snoopy
Snake
Sneeze knees
Snore nor
Sneer near
Snug
Sniff
Snippy nippy
Sniffle
Snub nub
Sneak
Snatch
Snob knob
Snort
[spr]
Spring
Spray pray
Sprite (soda)
Sprinkle
Spry pry
Spray gun
Sprint print
Sprinkles
228 The late eight
Springboard
Sprinkling
Springtime
Sprain
Spread
Sprig prig
Spree
Spruce
Sprout
[str]
Struck truck
Strum
Strike out
Strong
Stroller troller
Street treat
Stranger
Straw
Stronghold
Stripe tripe
String
Strike zone
Streetcar
Stream
Strap trap
Strike trike
Stretcher
Strict tricked
Stroke
Struggle
Strum
Strut
[skr]
Screen
Scribble
Scream cream
Scratch
Scrapper
CHAPTER 9: [s] 229
Scrooge
Screenplay
Scrub
Screw crew
Screen
Script crypt
Scramble
Scribe
Scrap crap
Scroll
Scrimp crimp
Scrabble
Screech
[skw]
Square
Squash
Squeal keel
Squeak
Squad quad
Squirt
Squaw
Squalid
Squirm
Squeeze
Squall
Squint
Squadron
[spl]
Splashdown
Splinter
Split
Splash
Splint
Spleen
Splat
Splice
Splurge
230 The late eight
Sting ting
Spicy
Skirt Kurt
Steak take
Spanish
Staff
Skull cull
Steady Teddy
Skinny
Spill pill
Spider
Sponge
Sketch
Skew Q
Skim Kim
Spear peer
Scotland
Spark park
Skid kid
Spareribs
Steal teal
Spin pin
Staff
Spy pie
Skill kill
Spade paid
Stoplight top light
Spooky Pooky
Spy lie
Stone Age
Stallion
Starfish
Spaceship
Stop top
Spot pot
Stag
Stake take
Spit pit
Story Tory
Spirit
Stack tack
CHAPTER 9: [s] 231
Spoon
Stork
Stagecoach
Space pace
Stick tick
Storm
Stairs tears
Spank
Stalk talk
Sky
Stool tool
Special
Skip Kip
Stamp
Spike pike
Speed peed
Stock car
Spokes pokes
Stand
Stub tub
Star tar
Stew two
School cool
Standard
Ski key
Speak peak
Skin kin
Spout pout
Scale kale
Spell
Skier
Scarf
Spear peer
School bus cool bus
Stink
Scar car
Steam team
Skate Kate
Statue
Skunk
Stale tale, tail
232 The late eight
Small mall
Snarl gnarl
Sneeze knees
Smell
Snail nail
Smock mock
Sneer near
Snort
Smile mile
Snoopy
Smog
Snow White no white
Smoke
Snug
Snob knob
Smart mart
Smite mite
Snack bar
Smooth
Snoopy
Smoky
Smudge
Snake
Smallest
Snack knack
Snub nub
Sneak
Smother mother
Snout
Smack Mack
Snorkel
Snap nap
Smash mash
Snake bite
Smelly
Snow no, know
Smoky
Sniff
Snippy nippy
Sniffle
Snatch
Snow bank no bank
Sneakers
Snore nor
CHAPTER 9: [s] 233
Squeal keel
Strike trike
Stretcher
Screw crew
Spread
Squeak
Spray pray
Squalid
Strap trap
Screen
Screech
Sprinkle
Sprinkles
Scrimp crimp
Scrabble
Street treat
Squash
Squirm
Sprinkling
Struck truck
Script crypt
Scramble
Scribe
Sprain
Squint
Squadron
Strum
Squall
Splash
Strike out
Squeeze
Springtime
Scrap crap
Scroll
Strong
Spray gun
Strict tricked
Stroke
Spruce
Stroller
Splint
Scream cream
Stranger
Sprig prig
234 The late eight
Straw
Sprint print
Stronghold
Squad quad
Spree
Stripe tripe
Split
String
Strike zone
Squash
Struggle
Strum
Scrooge
Streetcar
Square
Scrapper
Splurge
Squirt
Stream
Splashdown
Spring
Sprite (soda)
Strut
Splinter
Screen
Spruce
Sprout
Scribble
Splat
Splice
Scratch
Screenplay
Scrub
Spleen
Squeak
Springboard
Squaw
CHAPTER 9: [s] 235
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
All Environments
All Environments
End of Words
Toss
White house
Dice die
Goose goo
Moose moo
Courthouse
Ice eye, I
Noose new
Nervous
Famous
Trace tray
Gas
Red Cross
Race ray
Lighthouse
Glass
Fireplace
Mace May
Janis
CHAPTER 9: [s] 237
Greece
Kansas
Bruce brew
Porpoise
Press
Grass
Police
Gross grow
Palace
Grease
Air base
Lewis Louie
Cross
Mattress
Paris
Pass
Geese Gee
Necklace
Carlos Carlo
Rice rye
Dress
Alice alley
Blouse
Yes
Mouse Mao
Bus
Briefcase
Chris
Kiss
Congress
Class
Vase
Face Fay, fey
Bernice
Ace A
Hiss
Horse
Mice my
Bus
Boss
Moss
238 The late eight
[st]
Waist wait
Chest Chet
Frost fraught
Signpost
Fast fat
Midwest
Crust
Rust rut
Artist
Post
Fist fit
Breakfast
Mist mitt
August
Feast feet
Toast tote
Twist twit
Chemist
Most moat
Nest net
Forest
Gust gut
Signpost
Toothpaste
West wet
Typist
Vest vet
Conquest
Priest
August
Gymnast
Ghost goat
Wrist writ
Last
Slowest
CHAPTER 9: [s] 239
Key West
Lost
East eat
[sk]
Desk deck
Mask Mack
Disk Dick
Corn husk
Tusk tuck
Mollusk
Gas mask
Husk Huck
Ask
School desk
Face mask
Whisk wick
Task tack
Mosque mock
[sl]
Missile
Axle
Fossil
Muscle
Wrestle
[ls]
Convulse
Pulse Paul
[ns]
Fence fen
Dance Dan
Defense
Florence
Wince win
Immense
240 The late eight
France Fran
Balance
Science
Pounce
Tense ten
Prince
Bounce
License
Rinse
Lance
Entrance
[rs]
Force four
Divorce
Pierce peer
Golf course
Fierce fear
Hoarse whore
Horse whore
Air Force
Sawhorse
[ks]
Ax
Alex
Fox
Smallpox
Comics comic
Larynx
Phoenix
Mailbox
Box Bach
Tax tack
[s] + Consonant
Fast fat
Wasp
Waist wait
CHAPTER 9: [s] 241
Mosque mock
Chest Chet
Frost fraught
Signpost
Midwest
Crust
Lisp lip
Rust rut
Artist
Gasp gap
Fist fit
Crisp
Breakfast
Mist mitt
School desk
August
Feast feet
Facemask
Husk Huck
Toast tote
Twist twit
Mollusk
Chemist
Most moat
Gas mask
Nest net
Forest
Wrestle
Gust gut
Signpost
Toothpaste
West wet
Corn husk
Typist
Fossil
Tusk
Frost
Vest
Conquest
Priest
Muscle
Gymnast
242 The late eight
Wrist writ
Last
Grasp
Slowest
Key West
East eat
Desk deck
Post
Mask Mack
Ghost goat
Disk Dick
August
Axle
Ask
Whisk wick
Lost
Task tack
Missile
Consonant + [s]
Convulse
Rinse
Fence fen
Comics comic
Larynx
Florence
Fierce fear
Wince win
Box
Immense
Air Force
Dance Dan
Horse whore
Sawhorse
France Fran
Ax
Balance
Fox
Smallpox
Science
Force four
CHAPTER 9: [s] 243
Pounce
Hoarse whore
Tense ten
Defense
Prince
Phoenix
Bounce
Pulse Paul
License
Prince
Alex
Lance
Entrance
Divorce
Tax tack
Pierce peer
Golf course
Mailbox
Bucks buck
Cats cat
Tops top
Punks punk
Picks pick
Spots spot
Pops pop
Shops shop
Tips tip
Carts cart
Skunks skunk
Sharks shark
Pops pop
Droughts drought
Types type
Spouts spout
Embarks embark
Plops plop
Sinks sink
Shouts shout
244 The late eight
Themes
Messy
Toast tote
Swallow wallow
Spare ribs
Soup loop, poop
Salt ought malt
Sip whip, rip, lip, hip, dip
Rice rye mice, lice
Salad ballad
Sundae fun day
Steak take
Sub hub, cub
Sour hour tower, shower, cower
Icy
Sprout
Icing
Squirm
Sprouts sprout
Squeeze
Squash
Skim Kim
Splurge
Spread
Spicy
Spree
Stew two
Sprinkle wrinkle
Snack bar
Snack knack
CHAPTER 9: [s] 245
Smell Mel
Sweet wheat
Sprinkles
Chopsticks
Pizza pita
Crust
Crisp
Frosting
Snack knack
Icing
Grease
Feast feet
Crust
Swallow wallow
Crisp
Frost fraught
Snack knack
Smell Mel
Sweet wheat
Sprinkles
Whisk wick
Teaspoon
Frosting
Spoon
Skim Kim
Splurge
Spread
Fox
Fence fen
Corn husk
Tusk tuck
Sprig prig
Spruce
Husk Huck
246 The late eight
Sliver liver
Ax
Salmon
Sun fun, run, one, bun, gun
Mouse Mao
Saddle addle
Sleek leak
Swerve
Swan wan
Swallow wallow
Sailboat
Sand and band, fanned, tanned, hand
Sunrise
Swarm warm
Swift wift
Sunlight
Sunny honey, money, funny,
bunny
Soil oil boil, coil
Possum
Stag
Moose moo
Bison
Grassy
Skunk
Skip Kip
Mossy
Moss
Stork
Sky
Slug lug
Stallion
Stick tick
Storm
Horse whore
Mice my
Grass
Goose goo
Geese gee
Stream
Splash
Cycle Michael
Forest
CHAPTER 9: [s] 247
Fence fen
Sun fun, run, one, bun, gun
Saddle addle
Sunlight
Sunny honey, money, funny, bunny
Grassy
Swift wift
Stallion
Storm
Sleek leak
Swerve
Swelter welter
Horse whore
Grass
Stream
Splash
Forest
Mollusk
Salmon
Sail ale, ail tail, pail, nail, veil, rail,
whale, mail, jail
Sailboat
Sun fun, run, one, bun, gun
Seagull eagle
Sole ole mole, goal, coal
Sea E pea, me, key
Surfboard
Sailboat
Sand and band, fanned, tanned, hand
Sunburn
Surfer
Sunrise
Squall
Sharks shark
Seaweed
Sunlight
Seal eel Neil, heel
Sunny honey, money, funny, bunny
Sandal handle
248 The late eight
Surf turf
Lighthouse
Porpoise
Sky
Stallion
Stick tick
Storm
Starfish
Splash
Alex
Lance
Sally alley tally
Sioux oo dew, coo
Lucy Louie
Susan
Russell
Sam am Pam, ram, lamb
Cindy Indy windy
Sid id hid, kid, bid, Sid
Sue ooh boo, who, due
Lassie
Tracy
Jason
Bessie
Casey
Janis
Bruce brew
Lewis
Carlos Carlo
Alice
Chris
Smoky
Snow White no white
Oscar
Shakespeare
Betsy Betty
Einstein
Casper
Snoopy
CHAPTER 9: [s] 249
Phoenix
France Fran
Florence
Sphinx
Sweden
Key West
Sydney kidney
Tucson
White House
Greece
Kansas
Scotland
Houston
Larynx
Smallpox
Skin kin
Smile mile
Sniffle
Splint
Spleen
Sprain
Stretcher
Stroke
Skull cull
Sneeze knees
Snore nor
Gasp gap
Lisp lip
Waist wait
Chest Chet
Wrist writ
Muscle
Pulse Paul
Fist fit
Sick ick tick, thick, kick, pick, lick,
wick
Snort
Speak peak
Squint
250 The late eight
Sniff
Slump lump
Slang
Speak peak
Lisp lip
Smile mile
Larynx
Swear wear
Splash
Steam team
Sting ting
Sponge
Spout pout
Spill pill
Face fay, fey ace
Faucet
Sing wing, thing, king, ring
Suds duds, cuds
Soak oak poke, choke, yolk, joke,
woke
Soap rope, pope
Sink ink think, pink, rink, wink
Dress up
Glasses
Necklace
Dress
Blouse
Scarf
Slippers
Strap trap
Costume
Silk ilk
Shoestring
Bracelet
Vest vet
Saber
Soldier
Spell
Story Tory
Saint ain’t paint, faint
Cellar teller
Sad add mad, bad, dad
Cell L fell, dell, Mel
Sack back, rack, hack, pack, tack
Casket
Sneak
Locksmith
Slave
Squalid
Stake take
Slay lay
Escape
Crystal
Straw
Swing wing
Scar car
Staff
Struggle
Spike pike
252 The late eight
Spirit
Splat
Strike trike
Spear peer
Speed peed
Scribe
Scroll
Sneer near
Slash lash
Slink link
Stairs tears
Spy pie
Spade paid
Spooky Pooky
Spider
Spear peer
Castle
Fireplace
Mace May
Palace
Cross
Spark park
Stalk talk
Swamp
Smoke
Stronghold
Squad quad
Conquest
Priest
Nest net
Forest
Mist mitt
Gust gut
Ghost goat
Lost
Mask Mack
Wrestle
Defense
Prince
Entrance
Force four
Pierce peer
CHAPTER 9: [s] 253
Fierce fear
Horse whore
Feast feet
[z]
Definition
[z] is made in either of two ways. As with [s], some people produce [z]
with the tongue tip up behind the upper front teeth, others say it with
the tongue tip down behind the lower front teeth. Neither one is the
“right way.” Follow the student’s lead in deciding which way to teach [z].
If the student appears to find it easier to say [z] with the tongue tip up,
teach the sound that way; if the student appears to find it easier to say [z]
with the tongue tip down, teach the sound that way. For both varieties
of [z], the airstream is continuous and the vocal folds are together. The
technical definition of [z] is voiced alveolar fricative.
Acquisition
Fifty percent of children acquire [s] by 4;0 and 75% of children acquire
[s] by 6;0.
Relative Frequency
[z] is ranked fifth in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks fifteenth in relative frequency compared
with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence
compared with all English consonants is 3.0%.
255
256 The late eight
Errors
[z] is affected by many of the same errors as [s]— lisping, bladed produc-
tions, and lateralizing are all common errors for [z]. Additionally, at the
end of words, [z] may be produced with little or no voicing, resulting
in an [s]-like sound.
Key Environments
Beginning of syllable and between vowels, as in zee and a zee
Before a high front vowel, as in zip
In consonant clusters after [d], as in beads
Possible Metaphors
Select metaphors based on the aspect of speech that is the focus of therapy.
Touch Cue
Finger on the corner of the mouth (finger up for tongue tip raised sound
or finger down for tongue tip down sound).
Instructions
Place the student’s finger in the corner of the lips, and remind the
student to keep upper and lower teeth close together.
Initial Screening Test for [z]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. Zen _____________
2. Zone _____________
3. Zombie _____________
Medial
4. Daisy _____________
5. Rosa _____________
6. Moses _____________
Final
7. Nose _____________
8. Buzz _____________
9. Hose _____________
Comments/Notes:
257
Stimulability Tests for [z]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. zoo _____________
2. buzz _____________
Key Environments
Favorite Words
258
Phonetic Placement _____________
1. Place a tongue depressor just behind the student’s upper or lower front
teeth, depending on which variety of /z/ is being taught, and ask him or
her to use the tongue tip to hold it there.
2. Next, ask the student to keep the tongue tip still while you carefully
remove the tongue depressor.
3. Ask the student to breathe out, resulting in [s].
4. Ask student to turn voice box on, resulting in [z].
Instruct the student to say [s] and then to turn on the voice box.
Notes/Comments:
259
260 The late eight
Place: Alveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Fricative
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions: Tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encour-
age the student to move the flower in the wind.
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your finger or the student’s finger down the student’s arm
while making several long voiceless fricatives to demonstrate the “hissing”
quality and length of fricatives.
Voicing: Voiced
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
To facilitate [z], follow the steps for [s] but also use demonstrations to instruct
the student to turn on the voice box.
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
263
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Zoo 1. __________________
Zip 2. __________________
Zero 3. __________________
Zigzag 4. __________________
Zipper 5. __________________
Zach 6. __________________
Zest 7. __________________
Czar 8. __________________
Zip code 9. __________________
Zoom 10. __________________
264
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
265
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
Zoo oo 1. __________________
Czar R 2. __________________
Z E 3. __________________
Zen N 4. __________________
Zone own 5. __________________
Zinc ink 6. __________________
Zeal eel 7. __________________
Zion ion 8. __________________
266
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Zoo 1. __________________
Zip 2. __________________
Zero 3. __________________
Zigzag 4. __________________
Zipper 5. __________________
Zach 6. __________________
Zest 7. __________________
Czar 8. __________________
Zip code 9. __________________
Zoom 10. __________________
267
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Zoo 1. __________________
Zip 2. __________________
Zero 3. __________________
Zigzag 4. __________________
Zipper 5. __________________
Zach 6. __________________
Zest 7. __________________
Czar 8. __________________
Zip code 9. __________________
Zoom 10. __________________
268
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Zoo 1. __________________
Zip 2. __________________
Zero 3. __________________
Zigzag 4. __________________
Zipper 5. __________________
Zach 6. __________________
Zest 7. __________________
Czar 8. __________________
Zip code 9. __________________
Zoom 10. __________________
269
270 The late eight
Beginning of Words
Consonant Clusters
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
All Environments
All Environments
End of Words
Tribes tribe
Bugs bug
Kings king
Dimes dime
Tadpoles tadpole
Bells bell
CHAPTER 10: [z] 273
Dwarves
Seashells seashell
Crabs crab
Weeds weed
Balls ball
Wolves
Sandals sandal
Nails nail
Waves wave
Ears ear
Lions lion
Wings wing
Drums drum
Claws claw
Seagulls seagull
Eggs egg
Roars roar
Leopards leopard
Rinds rind
Sneakers sneaker
Ponds pond
Trolls troll
Ribbons ribbon
Whales whale
Elves
Crowns crown
Spareribs sparerib
Stars star
Goblins goblin
Birds bird
Thumbs thumb
Tails tail
Wheels wheel
Kids kid
Baggins
Blackbirds blackbird
Worlds world
Dogs dog
Tears tear
Hands hand
Camels camel
Rugs rug
274 The late eight
Mountains mountain
Coins coin
Schoolrooms schoolroom
Pools pool
Skateboards skateboard
Themes
Wizard
Samwise
Nazgul
Baggins
Rosie
Dwarves
Rings ring
Blizzard
Freezing
Elves
Pools pool
Mountains mountain
Drums drum
Crowns crown
Kings king
Music
Roars roar
Claws claw
Maze May
Crazy
Goblins Goblin
Kansas
Oz
Tribes tribe
Zombie
Worlds world
Stars star
Dreams dream
Bells bell
Prison
Desert
Wolves
Lions lion
Wings wing
Leopards leopard
276 The late eight
Tuesday
Wednesday
Thursday
Chapter Eleven
[l]
Definition
[l] is made with the tongue tip raised and touching the mouth roof. Air
flows over the sides of the tongue, and the larynx vibrates. The techni-
cal definition of [l] is voiced alveolar lateral. Two varieties of [l] exist in
English: “light [l],” which occurs at the beginning of syllables, as in leak
[lik], and “dark [l],” which occurs at the end of syllables, as in cool [kul].
In “dark [l]” the back of the tongue is raised in the velar region.
Acquisition
Fifty percent of children acquire [l] by 3;6 and 75% of children acquire
[l] by 6;0.
Relative Frequency
[l] is ranked second in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks seventh in relative frequency compared
with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence
compared with all English consonants is 5.6%.
277
278 The late eight
Errors
A common error for [l] is called gliding — that is, [w] or [j] (glide con-
sonants) substitute for [l] (a liquid consonant). Another common error
for [l] is to produce the sound with a wet, slushy quality. Often, a wet,
slushy sound occurs when [s] is made with the tongue blade raised
instead of the tongue tip.
Key Environments
Beginning of syllable and between vowels, as in lake and alone
Light [l] before a high front vowel, as in leaf
Dark [l] after a high back vowel at the end of a syllable, as in cool
Possible Metaphors
Select metaphors based on the aspect of speech that is the focus of therapy.
Touch Cue
Tip of finger on the middle of the upper lip.
Instructions
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. Lamp _____________
2. Light _____________
3. Leg _____________
4. Plane _____________
5. Sleep _____________
6. Clip _____________
Medial
7. Sailor _____________
8. Billy _____________
9. Olive _____________
Final
10. Skull _____________
11. Mail _____________
12. Bell _____________
13. Apple _____________
14. Model _____________
15. Muscle _____________
Comments/Notes:
279
Stimulability Tests for [l]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. lie _____________
2. low _____________
3. all _____________
4. ill _____________
Key Environments
Light [l]:
1. leaf _____________
2. leap _____________
Favorite Words
1. Touch the student’s alveolar ridge with a tongue depressor, peanut butter,
or lollipop to indicate the place of production for [l].
2. Ask the student to place his or her tongue tip in the place indicated, to
relax and to let air flow out from the sides of the tongue. The resulting
sound is voiceless [l].
3. Instruct the student to turn on the voice box, resulting in [l].
280
Shaping [l] from Interdental [t] _____________
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip between the teeth as for an
interdental [t].
2. Lower the student’s jaw.
3. Ask the student to slowly draw the tongue tip backward but to keep the
tongue tip in contact with the back of the teeth and the ridge behind the
two front teeth.
4. Ask the student to say [l], being sure that contact between the tongue
and the roof of the mouth is maintained.
Notes/Comments:
281
282 The late eight
Place: Alveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Liquid
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your or the student’s finger down the student’s arm while
making several long glides or liquids to demonstrate the “flowing” quality
and length of this sound.
Second Method
Objects: Strip of paper, a feather, or a small paper flower taped on the end
of a pencil
CHAPTER 11: [l] 283
Instructions: Use a strip of paper, a feather, or the hand held in front of the
student’s mouth while you produce several glides or liquids to draw attention
to the “flowing” quality and continuous nature of the sounds. Alternatively,
tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encourage the student
to move the flower in the wind.
First Method
Instructions: Place a straw on the groove of the tongue and blow out to
demonstrate central emission of air.
Second Method
Instructions: Ask the student to breathe in with the tongue as for [s]. Cool
air is felt at the central groove. Alternatively, perform the straw technique
above, remove the straw, and ask the student to breathe in with the tongue in
position for [l]. Cool air should be felt on the sides of the tongue over which
the air was emitted. An alternative method is to perform the straw technique
above, remove the straw, and ask the student to breathe in.
Voicing: Voiced
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
284 The late eight
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
Most often, the challenge with [l] is to help the student direct the airflow over
the sides of the tongue. These four methods each focus on helping the student
achieve lateral airflow. The first three methods focus on [l] in isolation and
the fourth focuses on [l] in consonant clusters.
First Method
Instructions:
1. Place a tongue depressor under the student’s tongue tip and then raise
the tongue tip behind the upper front teeth.
2. Ask the student to say [l] while maintaining contact between the tongue
tip and the roof of the mouth. The resulting sound is [l].
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Touch the student’s alveolar ridge with a tongue depressor, peanut butter,
or lollipop to indicate the place of production for [l].
2. Ask the student to place the tongue tip in the place indicated, to relax,
and to let air flow out from the sides of the tongue. The resulting sound
is voiceless [l].
3. Instruct the student to turn on the voice box, resulting in [l].
Third Method
This method works, though the number of steps limits its effectiveness, unless
the student has good motivation and attention.
Instructions:
Fourth Method
This method is for a student who needs to learn to pronounce [l] in conso-
nant clusters. The method works for consonant clusters in which the other
consonant in the cluster is not alveolar — for example, a velar or labial stop.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to place the tongue in the position for /l/.
2. Instruct the student to say “blue,” resulting in a [bl] consonant cluster.
CHAPTER 11: [l] 287
Shaping Techniques
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip between the teeth as for [ð].
2. Lower the student’s jaw.
3. Ask the student to slowly draw the tongue tip backward but to keep the
tongue tip in contact with the back of the teeth and the ridge behind the
two front teeth.
4. Next, instruct the student to say [l], being sure that contact between the
tongue and the roof of the mouth is maintained. If [θ] is used, instruct
the student to turn on the voice box.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to open the mouth as wide as for [a] but to raise the
tongue tip as for [i].
2. Ask the student to keep the tongue up as for [i] but to say [a], resulting
in a light (alveolar) [l]. Note. For a dark (velar) [l], follow the same steps
but ask the student to say [u] instead of [i].
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
288
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Log 1. __________________
Leather 2. __________________
Lick 3. __________________
Lock 4. __________________
Lake 5. __________________
Litter 6. __________________
Lump 7. __________________
Look 8. __________________
Low 9. __________________
Lip 10. __________________
289
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then say
the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
290
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
291
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Log 1. __________________
Leather 2. __________________
Lick 3. __________________
Lock 4. __________________
Lake 5. __________________
Litter 6. __________________
Lump 7. __________________
Look 8. __________________
Low 9. __________________
Lip 10. __________________
292
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Log 1. __________________
Leather 2. __________________
Lick 3. __________________
Lock 4. __________________
Lake 5. __________________
Litter 6. __________________
Lump 7. __________________
Look 8. __________________
Low 9. __________________
Lip 10. __________________
293
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Log 1. __________________
Leather 2. __________________
Lick 3. __________________
Lock 4. __________________
Lake 5. __________________
Litter 6. __________________
Lump 7. __________________
Look 8. __________________
Low 9. __________________
Lip 10. __________________
294
CHAPTER 11: [l] 295
Beginning of Words
[pl]
[kl]
Clark quark
Clip Kip quip
Club cub
Classroom
Class
Clock cock crock
Clipper
Clap cap crap
Clifford
Clay K Kray
298 The late eight
Clamshell
Clerk Kirk quirk
Cliff
Clever
Cluck
Cloud cowed crowd
Clippers kippers
Clam cam cram
Clover
Clown crown
Claw caw craw
Climb crime
[bl]
Black back
Blue boo brew
Blind bind
Blizzard
Blossom
Bleed bead breed
Blackbird
Block Brock
Blindfold
Blender bender
Blank bank
Bloodhound
Blackmail
Blood bud
[gl]
Glove grove
Glass gas grass
Glacier
Glenn
Glad grad
Glitter
Globe
Glider
Glow go grow
Glue goo grew
CHAPTER 11: [l] 299
Gloomy
Glasses gasses grasses
[fl]
Floor four
Flashbulb
Flag
Fly fie fry
Flood food
Flight fight fright
Flagstaff
Flat fat frat
Flipper
Flash
Flea fee free
Flour
Florist forest
Flab fab
Flame frame
Flip
Flower
[sl]
[spl]
Splash
Splinter sprinter
Splendid
Splay play
Splashdown
Splatter
Splurge
Splice
Spleen
Splat
Split
Splutter
Splint sprint
Splotchy
Splashy
Splendor
All Consonant
Clusters Deletions Minimal Pairs
All Consonant
Clusters Deletions Minimal Pairs
All Consonant
Clusters Deletions Minimal Pairs
Clerk Kirk
Flip
Pliers
Flower
Blizzard
Plate pate
Bleed bead breed
Glass gas grass
Block Brock
Flood food
Florist forest
Slide side spied
Cliff
Sliver
Blood bud
Fly fie fry
Plaza
Glove
Bloodhound
Glacier
Flight fight fright
Glenn
Splashdown
Glitter
Blank bank
Globe
Blackbird
Glider
Blender bender
Glow go grow
Splash
Floor four
Splinter
Flashbulb
Glad
Flag
Splendid
Sleigh say stray, stay, sway
Sleepy
Blindfold
Flagstaff
CHAPTER 11: [l] 303
All Consonant
Clusters Deletions Minimal Pairs
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
All Environments
End of Words
Roll row
Wheel whee
Rail ray
Tile tie
Skull
Mail may
Towel
Bell
Dial die
Seal sea, see
Boil boy
Mall
Snail
Seashell
Hole ho
Veil
Pile pie
Mole
Peel pea
Spool
Cell
School
Cool
Bowl bow
Nail
Drill
Troll toll
Pail pay
Mel
Bill
Goal go
Eel E
Gail gay
Bull
Baseball
Coal
Pill
Ball
Churchill
Steal
306 The late eight
Fall
April
Drool
Doll
Daniel
Jail J
Phil
Carol
Yell
Steel
Cheryl
Hall
Beach ball
Seagull
Brazil
Wall
Tadpole
Hotel
Oatmeal
Jackal
Whale way
Cartwheel
North Pole
[pl]
Apple
People
Scalpel
Steeple
Maple
People
[bl]
Cable
Bible
Fable
Hubble
Bubble
CHAPTER 11: [l] 307
Scrabble
Pebble
[dl]
Model
Pedal
Candle
Cradle
[gl]
Eagle
Beagle
Bugle
[fl]
Cheerful
Eiffel
Sniffle
Jungle
Bungle
[nl]
Funnel
Colonel
Tunnel
Kernel
[kl]
Jackal
Ankle
Buckle
Knuckle
Snorkel
308 The late eight
[dl]
Devil
[sl]
Missile
Castle
Fossil
Pencil
Muscle
Capsule
[ ʃ ] + [l]
Marshal
[lv]
Twelve
[lt]
Salt sought
Belt bet
Vault
Bolt
Bank vault
[ld]
Child chide
Donald
World word
Old ode
Gold goad
Stronghold
Bald
Field feed
Windshield
Arnold
CHAPTER 11: [l] 309
[lf]
Elf F
Werewolf
Golf
Wolf woof
Shelf chef
Rudolph
Gandolf
[lb]
Light bulb
DeKalb
[lm]
Elm M
Helm hem
[lp]
Kelp
Alp
Scalp
[lk]
Milk
Silk
Elk
Hulk
Apple
Model
Muscle
People
Beagle
Steeple
Bugle
310 The late eight
Maple
People
Fossil
Cable
Scalpel
Bible
Pencil
Hubble
Snorkel
Bubble
Ankle
Pedal
Candle
Knuckle
Cradle
Eagle
Cheerful
Buckle
Eiffel
Sniffle
Jungle
Fable
Bungle
Funnel
Pebble
Colonel
Missile
Tunnel
Kernel
Jackal
Scrabble
Devil
Castle
Capsule
Marshal
[l] + Consonant
Stronghold
Twelve
Salt sought
Hulk
CHAPTER 11: [l] 311
Vault
Light bulb
Bolt
Bank vault
Child chide
Shelf chef
Donald
Bald
World word
Belt bet
Gold goad
Windshield
Arnold
Gandolf
Elk
Alp
Elf F
Werewolf
Silk
Wolf woof
Rudolph
Old ode
DeKalb
Golf
Elm M
Field feed
Scalp
Helm hem
Kelp
Milk
Themes
Chilly
Elk
Rudolph
Sled said sped
Sled dog
Sleigh bells
Sleet seat skeet, sweet
Glacier
Blizzard
North Pole
Cool coo
Wolf woof
Wolves
Sledding
Sleigh say stay, spay, spray
Snowflake
Lobster
Salt bought, taught
Snorkel
Pebble
Splash
Flipper
Clamshell
Beach ball
Seagull
Pail pay
Towel
Seal sea, see feel, meal
Snail
Seashell
Kelp
Whale way
Eel E
CHAPTER 11: [l] 313
Sea Voyage to
Old Hawaii Deletions Minimal Pairs
Sailor
Galleon
Clipper kipper
Lei A say, pay, may
Boil boy
Lava Java
Hilly
Island
Boiling
Lettuce
Lemon
Lime I’m dime, rhyme, mime
Milk
Apple
Bubbling
Cobbler
Boil boy
Slicing spicing
Slaw saw
Flour
Glass gas grass
Blender bender
Clam cam cram
Plate pate
Plum
Lunch bunch, munch, hunch
Lid id hid, bid, mid
Like Ike Mike, bike, pike
Peel pea
Loaf oaf
Olive
Bowl bow
Chili
Salad
Jello
Roll row
Oatmeal
314 The late eight
Duckling
Cornflakes
Gandolf
Elrond
Gimli
Legolas
Gollum
Elf F
Wolf woof
Wolves
Eagle
Troll stroll
Trolls
Goblins
Lady Sadie
Lad add bad, mad, sad
Make a
Healthy Lunch Deletions Minimal Pairs
Lettuce
Lemon
Milk
Apple
Slicing spicing
Glass gas grass
Plate pate
Plum slum
Peel pea
Loaf oaf
Olive
Bowl bow
Salad
Roll row
CHAPTER 11: [l] 315
Kelly
Gimli
Lance dance, chance
Lucy juicy
Gandolf
Lynn in win, sin, tin
Billy
Lincoln
Leonard
Larry airy berry, Barry, merry, cherry
Lou oo two, new, boo, goo, new,
chew, Sue, Rue, two
Gail gay
Julie
Legolas
Lawrence
Gollum
Lois
Luke nuke, kook
Elrond
Zulu
Shelley
Aileen
Arnold
Donald
Albert
Glenn
Cliff
Clifford
Clark quark
Plato
Douglas
(The) Hulk
Kelly
(The) Devil
Billy
Mel
Bill
Churchill
April
Daniel
Phil
Carol
Cheryl
316 The late eight
Ceylon
Ireland
Dallas
England
Delhi
London
Ireland
Chile
Dallas
Brazil
Pluto
(The) Hubble
Plymouth
Flagstaff
Boulder
Melbourne
Eiffel (Tower)
DeKalb
Valley
Lake ache bake, cake
Lock sock, knock, rock
Lamp amp ramp, camp, damp
Light night, write, white, bite,
fight, kite
Lawn Shawn
Elf F
Castle
Tunnel
Werewolf
Steeple
Ugly
Goblin
Lame aim game, same, name
Lace ace race, case, base
Lazy daisy
Lye I, eye tie, die, sigh
England
Flower
Flame fame frame
Jail J
Cell
Lady Sadie
Lad add bad, mad, sad
Veil
Slipper sipper
Fable
Lagoon
Chapter Twelve
Vocalic
[r]
Definition
Vocalic [r] is a vowel rather than a consonant, and is included in this
book because often the road to a good [r] leads through first learning
to make vocalic [r]. Vocalic [r] is heard in the words heard, butter, and
girl. As with its consonant counterpart (called consonantal [r] or simply
[r]), it is produced in either of two ways. In the first way, called bunched,
the lips are rounded slightly, the tongue tip lowered, and the tongue
body bunched up near the area of the palate. In the second way, called
retroflex, the lips are rounded slightly and the tongue tip is raised either
toward the alveolar ridge or curled back toward the alveolar ridge. In
both varieties of vocalic [r], the sides of the tongue touch the insides
of the teeth about halfway back. Both bunched and retroflex types
of vocalic [r] can result in a good sound, though the author’s clinical
experience is that more students find the bunched variation somewhat
easier to learn than the retroflex one. For both types of vocalic [r],
the vocal folds are vibrating and the tongue root is retracted, creating
a contraction in the pharynx. The technical definition of vocalic [r] is a
mid-central rounded rhoticized vowel.
Acquisition
Vocalic [r] is acquired by 50% of children by 3;6 and 75% of children
by 5;6.
319
320 The late eight
Relative Frequency
—
Errors
The most common error is deleting the [r] part of the vowel, result-
ing in words such as fur, fare, and fear being pronounced fu, fa, fe,
respectively.
Key Environments
In a word consisting of a single stressed syllable, as in girl
Possible Metaphors
The best metaphor usually focuses on the way vocalic [r] closes (ends)
the vowel.
Touch Cue
Palm down and finger tips curled down (bunched) or palm up and
finger tips curled up (retroflex).
Instructions
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Open
1. Grocer _______________
2. Racer _______________
3. Sister _______________
4. Monster _______________
5. Chester _______________
Closed
6. Bird _______________
7. Shirt _______________
8. Burn _______________
9. Hurl _______________
10. Nurse _______________
Comments/Notes:
321
Stimulability Tests for Vocalic [r]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. her _______________
2. curl _______________
Key Environments
Favorite Words
Instruct the student to growl like a tiger (grrr). Alternatively, ask the student to
make the “arm wrestling sound” ([rrr]) while arm wrestling with the clinician.
Notes/Comments:
322
CHAPTER 12: [r] 323
Place: Alveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Liquid
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your or the student’s finger down the student’s arm while
making several long glides or liquids to demonstrate the “flowing” quality
and length of this sound.
324 The late eight
Second Method
Objects: Strip of paper, a feather, or a small paper flower taped on the end
of a pencil
Instructions: Use a strip of paper, a feather, or the hand held in front of the
student’s mouth while you produce several glides or liquids to draw attention
to the “flowing” quality and continuous nature of the sounds. Alternatively,
tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encourage the student
to move the flower in the wind.
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the lower front
teeth and to raise the body of the tongue toward the mouth roof.
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Touch behind the student’s lower front teeth with a Q-tip to demonstrate
where the tongue tip should be placed. Alternatively, place a small piece
of candy or other favored food behind the lower front teeth and ask the
student to hold it there with the tongue tip.
2. Once this is achieved, ask the student to raise the tongue body toward
the mouth roof.
CHAPTER 12: [r] 325
Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the upper front teeth.
2. Ask the student to curl the tongue backward without touching the roof
of the mouth until it cannot go back farther.
A good [r] often is more easily achieved if the student is encouraged to say the
sound while keeping the sides of the tongue touching the insides of the teeth.
The following simple demonstration can be practiced in isolation or as part
of the bunched and retroflex demonstrations.
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Tell the student that his or her tongue is sleepy and wants to rest.
2. Instruct the student to let the tongue spread out in “its bed” until it is
touching the insides of the teeth.
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Touch the inside teeth with a Q-tip or some favored food to demonstrate
where the tongue should go.
2. Tell the student that his or her tongue is sleepy and wants to rest.
3. Instruct the student to let the tongue spread out in “its bed” until it is
touching the insides of the teeth.
Voicing: Voiced
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
CHAPTER 12: [r] 327
Fifth Method
The first three techniques are simple and quick. If they don’t work, try the
fourth or fifth methods.
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to growl like a tiger (grrr). Alternatively, ask
the student to make the “arm-wrestling sound” (rrr) while you and the student
arm wrestle.
I like this method — it is simple and quick, needs no objects, and is good both
for vocalic [r] and for shaping vocalic [r] into consonant [r]. It comes from
Gillian Fleming of Dunedin, New Zealand, who posted it on Caroline Bowen’s
website and kindly consented for it to be presented in this book.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student that for this sound it is the tongue that does the work
and that the lips need to be “out of the road” in a half-smile so that the
upper and lower teeth can be seen.
2. Model a long vocalic [r], and then ask the student to curl the tongue
back to copy your model. Allow the student to move the tongue around
until a perfect production is reached. If the student has difficulty, liken
the sound to “a car starting on a frosty morning” to elicit a growly rrrrr
sound.
3. Once vocalic [r] is produced, ask the student to “feel” where the tongue
is.
4. Once established in isolation, practice vocalic [r] in syllables, such as
rrah and rrye.
5. Once rrah and rrye have been achieved, instruct the student to place the
tongue tip up and back until the correct [r] is achieved and then have
the student say ring.
CHAPTER 12: [r] 329
This method works by stretching and spreading the sides of the mouth. Some-
what surprisingly, for some students it places the tongue in just the right
position to make vocalic [r].
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to “make a face” by hooking the first finger of each hand
in the corner of the mouth and stretching the mouth apart and back. The
same result can be achieved by placing a thin straw horizontally in the
student’s mouth.
2. With the mouth stretched, ask the student to say vocalic [r].
This method works because if a student lies back and relaxes, the tongue
“bunches” and falls into the position for vocalic [r]. Though it seems funny,
sometimes after establishing vocalic [r] flat on the back, you need to work for
the student to say the sound when not reclining.
Objects: None
Instructions: Instruct the student to lie on his or her back, relax the mouth,
and say vocalic [r].
Objects: None
Instructions:
Shaping Techniques
For many students, it is easier to shape vocalic [r] from another sound than
it is to teach it using phonetic placement techniques.
This method requires a number of steps, but is effective with the right student.
Instructions:
This “stretching” technique is very similar to the third vocalic [r] phonetic
placement technique.
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to “make a face” by hooking the first finger of each hand
in the corner of the mouth and stretching the mouth apart and back. The
same result can be achieved by placing a thin straw horizontally in the
student’s mouth.
2. With the mouth stretched, ask the student to say a prolonged [n], [d], or
[l].
3. As the student says the prolonged [n], [d], or [l], ask him or her to curl
the tongue backward, resulting in ner, der, or ler.
4. Have the student delete the consonant, resulting in vocalic [r].
CHAPTER 12: [r] 331
Objects: None
Instructions: Lower the student’s jaw slightly as for [d]. While the student’s
jaw is lowered, ask the student to pull back the tongue slightly, to lower the
tongue tip, and to say vocalic [r].
Objects: None
Instructions:
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to trill the tongue tip at the alveolar ridge.
2. Ask the student to stop the trill but to continue vocalizing, resulting in
vocalic [r].
The authors of almost all phonetic placement and shaping techniques are
unknown. The exception is the first technique for shaping vocalic [r] from [l],
described by Shriberg (1975). The technique works well with a student who
can pronounce [l]. The other two techniques offer additional possibilities for
shaping [l] into vocalic [r].
332 The late eight
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Instruct the student to place the tongue tip on the alveolar ridge in the
position for [l].
2. Ask the student to say [l] several times.
3. Ask the student to say [l] for 5 seconds.
4. Next, ask the student to say a long [l] but to drag the tongue tip slowly
back along the roof of the mouth until it is so far back that the student
has to drop it, resulting in vocalic [r].
Objects: None
Instructions:
Instructions:
This clever technique relies on the surprising similarities between vocalic [r]
and [ ʃ ], both of which involve slightly rounded lips, raising of the tongue
CHAPTER 12: [r] 333
blade, and contact between the tongue sides and the insides of the teeth. Anne
Hommes of Corvalis, Oregon contributed to this technique.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to say [ ʃ ], and then ask him or her to turn on the voice
box, resulting in [3].
2. Next, ask the student to curl the tongue tip back while keeping contact
with the insides of the back teeth, resulting in vocalic [r].
Here are two simple methods for converting ah into vocalic [r], For both,
watch carefully that the student does not round the lips, which results in a
vocalic [r] with [w] qualities. If need be, ask the student to hold down the lower
lip while practicing.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to sweep the roof of the mouth with the tongue tip while
saying ah.
2. Tell the student to stop, but to keep vocalizing ah.
3. Instruct the student to lower the tongue tip slightly, which often results
in vocalic [r].
Objects: None
Instructions:
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
334
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Sir 1. __________________
Dagger 2. __________________
Finger 3. __________________
Singer 4. __________________
Bur 5. __________________
Winner 6. __________________
Glacier 7. __________________
Whisper 8. __________________
Fir 9. __________________
Author 10. __________________
335
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
336
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
337
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Sir 1. __________________
Dagger 2. __________________
Finger 3. __________________
Singer 4. __________________
Bur 5. __________________
Winner 6. __________________
Glacier 7. __________________
Whisper 8. __________________
Fir 9. __________________
Author 10. __________________
338
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Sir 1. __________________
Dagger 2. __________________
Finger 3. __________________
Singer 4. __________________
Bur 5. __________________
Winner 6. __________________
Glacier 7. __________________
Whisper 8. __________________
Fir 9. __________________
Author 10. __________________
339
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. **un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Sir 1. __________________
Dagger 2. __________________
Finger 3. __________________
Singer 4. __________________
Bur 5. __________________
Winner 6. __________________
Glacier 7. __________________
Whisper 8. __________________
Fir 9. __________________
Author 10. __________________
340
CHAPTER 12: [r] 341
Vocalic [r]
Open Deletions
Sir
Dagger
Finger
Singer sing
Bur
Winner win
Glacier
Whisper
Fir
Author
Spider spied
Barber
Trailer trail
Summer
Steamer steam
Grocer
Racer race
Sister
Monster
Chester chest
Cellar cell
Rooster roost
Dancer dance
Easter east
Hamster
Master mast
Thriller thrill
Dollar doll
Endure
Rider ride
Spencer Spence
Caesar seize
Plaster
Lobster
Toaster toast
Cancer
Freezer freeze
342 The late eight
Open Deletions
Satyr
Fur
Her
Over
Pepper pep
Closed
Themes
Creatures
It’s a Job
People to See
Creatures Deletions
Spider spied
Rooster roost
Hamster
Monster
Lobster
CHAPTER 12: [r] 343
Creatures Deletions
Songbird
Worm
Satyr
Lizard
Squirrel
Bird
Germ
Author
Barber barb
Singer sing
Racer race
Dancer dance
Rider ride
Wizard
Nurse
Chester chest
Spencer Spence
Caesar seize
Pittsburgh
Turk
Herb
Leonard
344 The late eight
Reference
Shriberg, L. (1975). A response evocation program for vocalic [r]. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Disorders, 40, 92–105.
Chapter Thirteen
Consonantal
[r]
Definition
Consonantal [r] (henceforth simply called [r]) is the consonant counter-
part of vocalic [r]. [r] occurs before vowels in read, red, and three, and
after vowels in beard, hear, far, and four. In common with vocalic [r],
the consonantal [r] has two variations, the first (bunched) in which the
lips are rounded slightly, the tongue tip lowered, and the tongue body
bunched up near the area of the palate, and the second (retroflex) in
which the lips are rounded slightly and tongue tip is raised to the alveo-
lar ridge or curled back toward the alveolar ridge. In both varieties, the
sides of the tongue touch the insides of the teeth about halfway back.
As mentioned in the description of vocalic [r], both the bunched and
retroflex variations can produce an acceptable [r], though the author’s
clinical experience is that more students find the bunched variation
somewhat easier to learn than the retroflex one.
Acquisition
[r] is acquired by 50% of children by 5;0 and by 75% of children by 6;0.
345
346 The late eight
Relative Frequency
[r] is ranked third in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks eighth in relative frequency compared
with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence
compared with all English consonants is 5.2%.
Errors
The most common error is gliding — typically, [w] for [r], though [j] also
occurs. Deletion of [r] after a vowel and in consonant clusters also is
common.
Key Environments
Before a high front vowel, as in read
Beginning of syllable and between vowels, as in row and teary
In a consonant cluster with [t] or [k], as in tree or creek
Possible Metaphors
The metaphors for [r] are the same as for vocalic [r].
Touch Cue
The same touch cue is used for [r] as for vocalic [r].
Palm down and finger tips curled down (bunched) or palm up and
finger tips curled up (retroflex).
Instructions
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Medial
10. Story _____________
11. Gary _____________
12. Earning _____________
Final
13. Oar _____________ 16. Airport _____________
14. Stair _____________ 17. Starve _____________
15. Spear _____________ 18. Mark _____________
Comments/Notes:
347
Stimulability Tests for [r]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
Key Environments
Between vowels
1. eery _____________ 2. teary _____________
Favorite Words
Instruct the student to make a sound like a motor starting up, rerrrr.
Notes/Comments:
348
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 349
Place: Alveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Liquid
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your or the student’s finger down the student’s arm while
making several long glides or liquids to demonstrate the “flowing” quality
and length of this sound.
350 The late eight
Second Method
Objects: Strip of paper, a feather, or a small paper flower taped on the end
of a pencil
Instructions: Use a strip of paper, a feather, or the hand held in front of the
student’s mouth while you produce several glides or liquids to draw attention
to the “flowing” quality and continuous nature of the sounds. Alternatively,
tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encourage the student
to move the flower in the wind.
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the lower front
teeth and to raise the body of the tongue toward the mouth roof.
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Touch behind the student’s lower front teeth with a Q-tip to demonstrate
where the tongue tip should be placed. Alternatively, place a small piece
of candy or other favored food behind the lower front teeth and ask the
student to hold it there with the tongue tip.
2. Once this is achieved, ask the student to raise the tongue body toward
the mouth roof.
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 351
Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the upper front teeth.
2. Ask the student to curl the tongue backward without touching the roof
of the mouth until it cannot go back farther.
A good [r] often is more easily achieved if the student is encouraged to say the
sound while keeping the sides of the tongue touching the insides of the teeth.
The following simple demonstration can be practiced in isolation or as part
of the bunched and retroflex demonstrations.
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Tell the student that his or her tongue is sleepy and wants to rest.
2. Instruct the student to let the tongue spread out in “its bed” until it is
touching the insides of the teeth.
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Touch the inside teeth with a Q-tip or some favored food to demonstrate
where the tongue should go.
2. Tell the student that his or her tongue is sleepy and wants to rest.
3. Instruct the student to let the tongue spread out in “its bed” until it is
touching the insides of the teeth.
Voicing: Voiced
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 353
Fifth Method
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to place the tongue tip behind the upper front teeth. (If
needed, place the student’s tongue tip on a shelf made with a tongue
depressor.)
2. Next, ask the student to curl the tongue backward without touching the
roof of the mouth until it cannot go back farther.
3. Lower the student’s jaw slightly and ask the student to say [ru].
Shaping Techniques
These two methods are for a student who has mastered vocalic [r]. They are
the methods the author uses most often to teach [r].
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
3. After the student pronounces the words with vocalic [r] followed by
consonantal [r], instruct him or her to say the vocalic [r] silently, which
typically results in initial [r].
Second Method
This method relies on the fact that a syllable that ends in vocalic [r] and is
followed by a syllable beginning with a vowel will often result in a syllable-
initial [r] as a type of transition between vocalic [r] and the vowel that follows.
Objects: None
Instructions:
This method is for a student who substitutes [w] for [r] (gliding), but whose
tongue is in the correct position for [r]. Such a student may need to be taught
to reduce lip rounding.
Instructions:
This and the following method are for students for whom you wish to establish
[r] at the end of syllables. The methods demonstrate the technique for [ar] as
356 The late eight
in car and [ir] as in hear. The same technique works for all other vowels. To
illustrate, to teach [or] simply replace [a] or [i] with [o]. When teaching [r] in
this position, a useful trick is to have the student keep contact between the
sides of the tongue and the inner teeth.
Objects: None
Instructions:
Objects: None
Instructions:
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
357
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Rain 1. __________________
Root 2. __________________
Relaxed 3. __________________
Rat 4. __________________
Rattle 5. __________________
Robot 6. __________________
Rug 7. __________________
Rainy 8. __________________
Red Sea 9. __________________
Raven 10. __________________
358
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
359
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
Rat at 1. __________________
Rattle addle 2. __________________
Read Ed 3. __________________
Rocks ox 4. __________________
Rhino I know 5. __________________
Red Ed 6. __________________
Reach each 7. __________________
Rose owes 8. __________________
Rita eat a 9. __________________
Rome om 10. __________________
360
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Rain 1. __________________
Root 2. __________________
Relaxed 3. __________________
Rat 4. __________________
Rattle 5. __________________
Robot 6. __________________
Rug 7. __________________
Rainy 8. __________________
Red Sea 9. __________________
Raven 10. __________________
361
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Rain 1. __________________
Root 2. __________________
Relaxed 3. __________________
Rat 4. __________________
Rattle 5. __________________
Robot 6. __________________
Rug 7. __________________
Rainy 8. __________________
Red Sea 9. __________________
Raven 10. __________________
362
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____ *, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Rain 1. __________________
Root 2. __________________
Relaxed 3. __________________
Rat 4. __________________
Rattle 5. __________________
Robot 6. __________________
Rug 7. __________________
Rainy 8. __________________
Red Sea 9. __________________
Raven 10. __________________
363
364 The late eight
Beginning of Words
Wrestle trestle
Ron on gone
Roof goof
Rainbow
Realm elm helm
Rake ache bake, make, lake, shake
Race ace face, chase, lace
Reef leaf
Ruth
Rust dust, bust
Ram am Sam, Pam, lamb
Raisin lazin’
Rod odd mod, sod, nod, god
Roll bowl, goal
Raincoat
Roar oar, or tore, soar
Rip whip, lip, hip
Rinse mince
Racket
Write night, light, fight
Ran an can, man, pan
Reel eel kneel, feel, deal, Neal
Robed
Ripe pipe, type
Rail ale nail, whale, mail, jail, veil,
tail, pail
Rind mind, signed, bind
Roy oi boy, toy
Ray bay, say, May
Ranch
Rip tip, lip, whip, ship
Rag bag, sag, nag
Rebel
Raid aid maid, jade
Rhine mine, fine, sign
Rend end bend, send, mend
Ramon
Rake ache bake, take, lake
Rich itch witch, hitch
366 The late eight
[pr]
Prize pies
Price
Prince
Preschool
Preach peach
Prairie Perry
Prowl Powell
Pray pay
Proof poof
Prison
Press
Princess
Prune
Priest pieced
Prick pick
Pretzel
Pray pay
Pro
[tr]
Trip tip
Track tack
Train
Trent tent
Tractor
Tree tea, T
Trunk
Tray
Travel
Tribe
Tragic
Trick tick
Tramp
Traffic
Trap tap
Trail tail
Trash can
Truck tuck
Troll toll
Tree house
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 367
Trouble
Treat teat
Trailer tailor
Trash
Tracy
Trish Tish
[kr]
Crab cab
Crib
Cradle
Crown
Crawl
Crayon
Crumbs comes
Crow
Crash cash
Cricket
Crete
Christmas
Chris kiss
Croak Coke
Creak
Crossbow
Crutch
Crust cussed
Crisp
Cry
[br]
Bread bed
Broom boom
Brownie
Brian
Braces bases
Britain
Brick Bic
Broken
Bridge
Branch
368 The late eight
Brake bake
Broomstick
Brazil
Brain bane
Bronze Age
Brush
Break bake
[dr]
Drink dink
Driftwood
Drive dive
Driver diver
Drill dill
Dry die
Drugstore
Drag race
Dragon
Drum dumb
Drawer
Driveway
Drag
Dreaming deeming
Drip dip
Drizzle
Dress
Drain Dane
Drummer dumber
[gr]
Grass gas
Grease geese
Great gate
Grapefruit
Grant
Grave gave
Grover
Grizzly
Group
Grouchy
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 369
Grapes gapes
Ground
Greece geese
Great Dane
Gray gay
Green
Grow go
Grill gill
Grocer
[fr]
Front
Frown
Fruit
French fries
Friday
Fry fie
Francis
Frank
Freeze fees
Franklin
Frog fog
Frosting
Friend fend
Friar fire
France
[θr]
Three thee
Throw though
Thrill
Throttle
Thrift shop
Thread
Throat
Threw
Throw rug
Thriller
Throne
Thrifty
370 The late eight
Throwing
Threshold
Throng thong
Thrive
Thrash
Through
Thrush
Threat
Thrust
Throb
[Sr]
Shrew rue
Shredded
Shrug rug
Shrimp
Shrub rub
Shroud
Shreveport
Shrivel
Shriek reek
Shred red
Shrill rill
Shrink rink
Shrine Rhine
Shrewd rude
[spr]
Spring
Spray spay
Sprite (soda) spite
Sprinkles
Spray gun
Spruce
Spry spy
Sprint
Sprinkle
Springboard
Sprinkling
Springtime
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 371
Sprain Spain
Sprinkler
Spread sped
Spring
Sprig
Spree
Spruce
Sprout
Sprawl
[str]
Struck stuck
Strum
Strike out
Strong
Stroller
Street
Stranger
Straw
Stronghold
Stripe
String sting
Strike zone
Streetcar
Stream steam
Strap
Strike
Stretcher
Strict sticked
Stroke stoke
Struggle
Strut
[skr]
Screen
Scribble
Scream scheme
Scratch
Scrapper
Scrooge
372 The late eight
Screenplay
Scrub
Screw
Script skipped
Scramble
Scribe
Scrap
Scroll
Scrimp skimp
Scrabble
Screech
Britain
Prize pies
Trap tap
Price
Prince
Throat
Preschool
Bridge
Prairie Perry
Frosting
Prowl
Pray pay
Grease geese
Prison
Trouble
Priest pieced
Trip tip
Crayon
Travel
Drummer dumber
Tribe
Drip dip
Trick tick
Prune
Tramp
Traffic
Grow go
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 373
Truck tuck
Frog fog
Troll toll
Driver diver
Trailer tailor
Trish Tish
Crib
Trash can
Crown
Prick pick
Crawl
Thrill
Crow
Track tack
Crash cash
Drugstore
Cricket
Trail tail
Crete
Dress
Croak Coke
Friar fire
Creak
Tree house
Crutch
Preach peach
Crust cussed
Princess
Crisp
Bronze Age
Cry
Bread bed
Tracy
Brownie
Tree tea, T
Brian
Tragic
Brake bake
Green
Broomstick
Crumbs comes
Brazil
374 The late eight
Crab cab
Brain bane
Shriveled
Brush
Pro
Break bake
Train
Crossbow
Trunk
Press
Tray
Drink
Friday
Driftwood
Throttle
Drive dive
Broken
Drill dill
Christmas
Dry die
Brick
Drag race
Friend fend
Drawer
Driveway
Trash
Drag
Tractor
Drizzle
Broom boom
Drain Dane
Grass gas
Prof
Great gate
Grapefruit
Grant
French fries
Grave gave
Freeze fees
Grizzly
Group
Grouchy
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 375
Drum dumb
Grapes gapes
Braces bases
Gray gay
Shroud
Grill gill
Branch
Grocer
Front
Frown
Pray pay
Fruit
Dreaming deeming
Fry fie
Trent tent
Francis
Chris kiss
Frank
Thriller
Franklin
France
Three
Shriek sheik
Throw
Thrush
Cradle
Thread
Dragon
Throw rug
Grover
Thrum thumb
Greece geese
Thrift shop
Shrimp
Pretzel
Shrub
Great Dane
Shrew shoe
Ground
Shrink
376 The late eight
Scribe
Spring
Scrooge
Strike out
Sprint
Sprawl
String bean
Sprinkle
Stream steam
Scream scheme
Straight state
Spray spay
Straw stall
Sprinkler
Strut
Spry spy
Scrawny
Sprinkling
Stride
Sprig
Street
Spruce
Stretcher
Springboard
Strudel
Stripe
Spread sped
Strap
Scratchy
Spree
Stretch
Scribble
Strange
Scrub
Stroller
Sprinkles
Screw
Springtime
Strong
Scrapbook
Spray gun
String sting
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 377
Sprout
Screech
Sprain Spain
Scratch
Sprite spite
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
[pr]
[tr]
Consonant Clusters
[kr]
[br]
[dr]
[gr]
[fr]
[ðr]
[ʃr]
Mushroom Washrag Washroom
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 379
Consonant Clusters
[spr]
[str]
[skr]
All Environments
All Environments
End of Words
End of Words*
*Deletions are not included for [r] at the end of words. The [r] lowers the
preceding vowel, making the deletions task for this sound less useful for
most students.
Car
Guitar
Oar
Stair
Spear
Door
Poor
Jar
Seashore
Tear
Year
Bazaar
Bar
382 The late eight
End of Words
Ore
Pier
Software
Soar
Share
Hear
Ear
Space bar
Bookstore
Pour
Air
Snack bar
Stare
Frontier
War
Pear
Nightmare
Cheer
New Year
Star
Scar
Swear
Scare
North Star
Chair
Downpour
Steer
Senor
Mare
Shore
Air
Bear
Boar
Square
Hair
Éclair
Four
Floor
Boar
Store
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 383
[rt]
Art
Airport
Court
Port
Head Start
Heart
Tart
Starve
[rk]
Arc
Mark
Bark
Pitchfork
Denmark
Ark
Dark
New York
Pork
Fork
[rb]
Rhubarb
Barb
[rd]
Beard
Blackboard
Lord
Flashcard
Hard
Bored
Hoard
Award
384 The late eight
[rg]
Morgue
Borg
[rm]
Arm
Farm
Storm
Snowstorm
[rn]
Born
Corn
Horn
Thorn
Torn
Popcorn
Acorn
[rs]
Horse
[rʃ ]
Borscht
[rtʃ ]
Arch
March
Porch
Starch
Torch
Research
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 385
[rd]
Barge
Large
Forge
George
[r] + consonant
Snowstorm
Art
Denmark
Airport
Starve
Mark
George
Acorn
Bark
Torn
Pitchfork
Farm
Dark
Popcorn
Fork
Rhubarb
Port
Beard
Pork
Blackboard
Borg
Lord
Barb
Flashcard
Court
Hard
Tart
Bored
Arc
Hoard
New York
386 The late eight
Award
Morgue
Arm
Porch
Storm
Born
Forge
Horn
Torch
Thorn
Horse
Borscht
Arch
Heart
March
Corn starch
Research
Barge
Ark
Large
Head Start
Themes
Garage
Roof goof
Room tomb
Door
Broom
Corn
Rhubarb
Pork
Tart
Borscht
Hear he
Ear E
Croak Coke
CHAPTER 13: Consonantal [r] 389
Creak
Scratchy
Screech
Shriek sheik
Drip dip
Cry
Bark
Roar oar tore, soar, more, boar
Trick tick
Treat teat
Hoard hoed
Heart
Rend end bend, send, mend
Afraid
Witchcraft
Strange
Shroud
Shriveled
Thriller
Thrill
Friar fire
Grave gave
Dreaming
Broomstick
Crossbow
Crown
Tree tea, T
Trunk
Princess
Scare
Nightmare
Spear
Arrow
Forest
Hero
Ring sing, wing, king
Rain vein, mane, pane, pain
Story
390 The late eight
Downpour
Hair hay, hey
Priest pieced
Secret
Children
Dark
Morgue
Storm
Thorn
Torch
George
Barb Bob
Mark
Chris kiss
Red Sea
Russia
Rome om home, gnome, roam
Europe
Far East
Peru
Cairo
North Star
Rhine mine, fine, sign
Crete
Brazil
Britain
Detroit
Greece geese
France
(The) Matrix
Akron
New York
Denmark
Madrid
Chapter Fourteen
[ʃ]
Definition
[ ʃ ] is made behind the alveolar ridge, a little behind the position for [s].
The tongue blade is raised, and the lips are rounded. The airstream is
continuous and the vocal folds are apart. The technical definition of [ ʃ ]
is voiceless postalveolar fricative.
Acquisition
[ ʃ ] is acquired by 50% of children by 3;6 and by 75% of children by 5;0.
Relative Frequency
[ ʃ ] is ranked sixth in relative frequency compared with the other late-
acquired consonants. It ranks nineteenth in relative frequency compared
with all other English consonants, and its percentage of occurrence
compared with all English consonants is 1.5%.
Errors
[s] for [ ʃ ] is a common error. A younger student may pronounce [ ʃ ] as [t]
or [d]. A less common, though not rare error, is pronouncing [ ʃ ] as [tʃ ].
393
394 The late eight
Key Environments
End of a syllable, as in fish
By a high back vowel, as in shoe
Possible Metaphors
The best metaphors for [ ʃ ] typically focus on the fricative nature of the
sound.
Touch Cue
Finger in front of lips and use the metaphor “quiet sound.”
Instructions
Place the student’s finger in the corner of the lips, and remind the
student that this is the quiet sound.
Initial Screening Test for [ ʃ ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. Shell _____________
2. Shutter _____________
3. Show _____________
4. Shrug _____________
5. Shrimp _____________
6. Shred _____________
Medial
7. Wishing _____________
8. Ocean _____________
9. Washer _____________
Final
10. Dish _____________
11. Crush _____________
12. Irish _____________
13. Harsh _____________
14. Marsh _____________
15. Borscht _____________
Comments/Notes:
395
Stimulability Tests for [ ʃ ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. shoe _____________
2. bush _____________
Key Environments
Favorite Words
Notes/Comments:
396
CHAPTER 14: [ S ] 397
Demonstrations for [ ʃ ]
Place: Postalveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to run the tongue to where the bump on the
mouth roof just begins to go down toward the back of the mouth, using an
analogy of a “hill and valley.”
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Fricative
First Method
Instructions:
Second Method
Instructions: Tape a small paper flower on the end of a pencil and encour-
age the student to move the flower in the wind.
Third Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Run your finger or the student’s finger down the student’s arm
while making several long voiceless fricatives to demonstrate the “hissing”
quality and length of fricatives.
Voicing: Voiceless
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
CHAPTER 14: [ S ] 399
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
Method
In this book there is only a single phonetic placement exercise for [ ʃ ]. Most
often, the sound is easily established through a demonstration or a shaping
exercise.
Instructions:
Shaping Techniques
[ ʃ ] from [s]
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to say [s]. While the student is saying [s],
instruct him or her to pucker the lips slightly and to draw the tongue back
a little until [ ʃ ] results.
[ ʃ ] from [Z]
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to say [Z] and then turn off the voice, which
often results in [ ʃ ].
[ ʃ ] from [i]
As for the [ ʃ ] from [i] shaping technique, this technique relies on the fact that
[ ʃ ] and [i] are made near the same place of production.
Objects: None
Instructions:
1. Ask the student to say [i], first with the voice on and then with the voice
off.
2. Next, ask the student to pucker the lips slightly.
3. Raise the student’s lower jaw slightly.
4. Ask the student to breathe out silently while raising the tongue. The
sound produced is [ ʃ ].
Shell for Speech Exercises
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
402
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Shoe 1. __________________
Sheep 2. __________________
Shiny 3. __________________
Shut 4. __________________
Sharon 5. __________________
Shovel 6. __________________
Shell 7. __________________
Shutter 8. __________________
Show 9. __________________
Shannon 10. __________________
403
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
404
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
405
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Shoe 1. __________________
Sheep 2. __________________
Shiny 3. __________________
Shut 4. __________________
Sharon 5. __________________
Shovel 6. __________________
Shell 7. __________________
Shutter 8. __________________
Show 9. __________________
Shannon 10. __________________
406
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Shoe 1. __________________
Sheep 2. __________________
Shiny 3. __________________
Shut 4. __________________
Sharon 5. __________________
Shovel 6. __________________
Shell 7. __________________
Shutter 8. __________________
Show 9. __________________
Shannon 10. __________________
407
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____ *, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Shoe 1. __________________
Sheep 2. __________________
Shiny 3. __________________
Shut 4. __________________
Sharon 5. __________________
Shovel 6. __________________
Shell 7. __________________
Shutter 8. __________________
Show 9. __________________
Shannon 10. __________________
408
CHAPTER 14: [ S ] 409
Beginning of Words
Shepherd
Shape ape cape, nape, tape
Shakespeare
Shark arc, ark dark, bark, lark
Sheriff
Ship chip, sip, hip, zip, whip,
rip, lip
Shampoo
Shush hush, mush
Shake ache lake, bake, make, rake
Short court
Shaft aft raft, laughed
Shuffle ruffle
Shoe oo two, new, boo, dew
Shadow
Shear ear tear, near, beer, seer
Shop hop, pop, mop
Sharp tarp, carp
Sham aim came, tame, same
Shed Ed bed, Ned, red
Shaggy Aggie baggy
Shift gift, lift
Shave pave, knave
Shanty ante
Shiver liver, giver
Shade aid laid, maid, paid
Shelter
Shabby abbey cabbie, tabby
Shape tape, nape
Shot hot, got, pot, rot
Shock rock, sock, knock
Shaking aching baking, making
Shower hour power, tower, bower
Shoulder older colder, bolder
Shelob
Shale ail, ale mail, nail, pail, tail
Shallow aloe callow, tallow
Shelf elf
Sham am ram, Pam, Sam, ham
Show O row, tow, mow, go
Share air, heir care, mare, tear
Shuttle
CHAPTER 14: [ S ] 411
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
—
412 The late eight
End of Words
Mustache
Trash
Dash
Goldfish
Irish
Dish
Crush
British
Crawfish
Mash
Crash
Toothbrush
Fish
Cash
Catfish
Hush
Spanish
Ash
Wash
Welsh well
Paintbrush
Leash Lee
Clash
Trish
Push
Nailbrush
Flush
Wish
Splash
Car wash
Bush
Josh
Rubbish
Smash
Danish
Mouthwash
CHAPTER 14: [ S ] 413
Harsh
Marsh mar
Borscht bore
Themes for [ ʃ ]
Themes
Sharon
Shannon cannon
Cheryl barrel, Carol
Sherlock
Shaw awe
Shane cane, bane, mane
Shakespeare
Irish
Ash
Wash
Welsh well
Spanish
British
Shaggy Aggie baggy
Shelob
Trish
Josh
Danish
Nashville
Shreveport
414 The late eight
Banshee
Shelob
Shape tape, nape
Shiver liver, giver
Shade aid laid, maid, paid
Sharp tarp, carp
Shadow
Shock rock, sock, knock
Shaking aching baking, making
Shrivel drivel
Shriek reek creek
Shortcake
Sugar
Shellfish
Fish
Crawfish
Catfish
Shrimp
Shake ache lake, bake, make, rake
Sherbet
Shortbread
Borscht bore
Dish
Rubbish
Splash
Trash
Dishes
Washcloth
Washing
Washer
Shine vine, sign, nine, line
Chapter Fifteen
[tʃ ]
Definition
[tʃ ] is made with the body of the tongue touching the mouth roof behind
the bumpy ridge (alveolar ridge). The lips are slightly puckered. The
airstream is stopped and then becomes continuous, making it a short
stop ([t]) followed by a fricative ([tʃ ]). The vocal folds are apart. The
technical definition of [tʃ ] is voiceless postalveolar affricate.
Acquisition
[tʃ ] is acquired by 50% of children by 4;6 and by 75% of children by 5;6.
Relative Frequency
[tʃ ] is ranked eighth in relative frequency compared with the other
late-acquired consonants. It ranks twenty-second in relative frequency
compared with all other English consonants, and its percentage of
occurrence compared with all English consonants is 0.7%.
Errors
Errors for [tʃ ] typically keep either the stop or fricative component of
the sound. Both [t] for [tʃ ] and [s] for [ʃ ] are common.
417
418 The late eight
Key Environments
End of a syllable, as in batch
By a high back vowel, as in chew
Possible Metaphors
The best metaphors for [tʃ ] typically focus on the affricate (stop-fricative)
quality of the sound.
Touch Cue
None
Initial Screening Test for [tʃ ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Instructions: Say to the student, “I’m going to say some words. Please say
the words after me.”
Example: “Dog. Now you say it.”
Word Student*
Beginning
1. Chain _____________
2. Chip _____________
3. Chunk _____________
Medial
4. Peaches _____________
5. Itchy _____________
6. Teacher _____________
Final
7. Witch _____________
8. Sketch _____________
9. Itch _____________
10. Porch _____________
11. Starch _____________
12. Punch _____________
Comments/Notes:
419
Stimulability Tests for [tʃ ]
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Referral:
Imitation
1. chip _____________
2. catch _____________
Key Environments
Favorite Words
Shaping _____________
Notes/Comments:
420
CHAPTER 15: [tS ] 421
Place: Postalveolar
First Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to run the tongue to where the bump on the
mouth roof just begins to go down toward the back of the mouth, using an
analogy of a “hill and valley.”
Second Method
Instructions:
Manner: Affricate
Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Have the student hold his or her hands together tightly and
then separate them quickly to indicate the stop onset and fricative release
of affricates.
Voicing: Voiceless
First Method
Objects: None
Second Method
Objects: None
Instructions: Place the student’s hands over the ears and instruct him or her
to hum, which heightens the sensation of vocal cord vibration.
Third Method
Objects: None
Fourth Method
Objects: None
Instructions: You and the student place one hand on your throat and the
other on the student’s throat while making voiced and voiceless sounds
together, telling each other when the voicing goes on and off.
Fifth Method
Method
Objects: None
Instructions:
Shaping Technique
[tʃ ] from [ ʃ ]
Objects: None
Instructions: Ask the student to say a quick [ ʃ ] with the tongue tip touching
“the bump,” resulting in [tʃ ]. (Note: To facilitate [dZ], develop from [d].)
Objects: None
Instructions:
Objects: None
Instructions:
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
1. __________________
2. __________________
3. __________________
4. __________________
5. __________________
6. __________________
7. __________________
8. __________________
9. __________________
10. __________________
425
Imitation
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say it after me. Here’s an example. I say sat, and then you say sat.”
Cheese 1. __________________
Chicken 2. __________________
Chief 3. __________________
Cheat 4. __________________
Chain 5. __________________
Chip 6. __________________
Chunk 7. __________________
Church 8. __________________
Chuck 9. __________________
Chop 10. __________________
426
Minimal Pairs
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
say the rhyming word, and then say the word with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word that begins with
our sound. Please say the word, then replace our sound with another sound
to make the word have a different meaning, and then say the word with our
sound again. Here’s an example. I say seal. You say seal, then change [s] to
[w] to make wheel, and then say seal again. Like this: Seal. Wheel. Seal.”
427
Deletion
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
without the treatment sound, and then with the treatment sound.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, and then say it with our sound deleted, and then say it
with our sound included. Here’s an example. I say red. You say red, then Ed,
then red. Like this: Red. Ed. Red.”
428
Self-Correction
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word three times, self-correcting if errors in
the treatment sound occur.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word three times, listening to how you say our sound and
changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Here’s an example.
I say cheese, and then you say cheese three times, listening to how you say
our sound and changing it to make it correctly if you say it incorrectly. Like
this: Cheese. Cheese. Cheese.”
Cheese 1. __________________
Chicken 2. __________________
Chief 3. __________________
Cheat 4. __________________
Chain 5. __________________
Chip 6. __________________
Chunk 7. __________________
Church 8. __________________
Chuck 9. __________________
Chop 10. __________________
429
Old Way/New Way
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student say the word the new way, the old way, and then
the new way again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then say it the old way you used to say our sound, and
then say it the new way you say our sound. Here’s an example. I say thin.
You say thin, then *in, and then thin. Like this: Thin. *in. Thin.”
Note: Replace * with the way the student used to say the sound.
Cheese 1. __________________
Chicken 2. __________________
Chief 3. __________________
Cheat 4. __________________
Chain 5. __________________
Chip 6. __________________
Chunk 7. __________________
Church 8. __________________
Chuck 9. __________________
Chop 10. __________________
430
Similar Sound
Student’s Name:
Date: __________________
Treatment Sound: __________________
Goal: Have the student first say the word with the treatment sound, then
with the most similar sound the student can make, and then with the treat-
ment sound again.
Instructions to Student: “You are going to hear a word with our sound.
Please say the word, then replace our sound with _____*, and then say the
word with our sound. Here’s an example. I say sun. You say sun, then *un,
and then sun again. Like this: Sun. *un. Sun.”
Note: Replace * with a sound the student can pronounce that is phonetically
similar to the treatment sound.
Cheese 1. __________________
Chicken 2. __________________
Chief 3. __________________
Cheat 4. __________________
Chain 5. __________________
Chip 6. __________________
Chunk 7. __________________
Church 8. __________________
Chuck 9. __________________
Chop 10. __________________
431
432 The late eight
Beginning of Words
Consonant Clusters
—
434 The late eight
Medial
Single Consonants
Consonant Clusters
All Environments
End of Words
Teach tea, T
Witch
CHAPTER 15: [tS ] 435
Peach pea, P
Sketch
Roach row
Coach
Ouch ow
Speech
Track coach
Watch
Ski coach
Wristwatch
Beach bee, B
Catch
Couch cow
Sandwich
Watch
Cockroach
Scratch
Ostrich
Fetch
[rtʃ ]
Arch R
Scorch score
March mar
Porch poor, pour
Starch star
[ntʃ ]
Trench
Punch pun
Branch
French
Bunch bun
Lunch
Bench Ben
Wrench wren
436 The late eight
Themes
Roach row
Cockroach
Ostrich
Branch
Roaches
Nature
Vulture
Beach bee, B
Itchy
Cheetah
Chicks six, mix
Cherry airy Sherry, merry, scary
438 The late eight
Preacher
Teacher
Ski coach
Rancher
Track coach
Coach
Peaches
Lunch
Chew oo zoo, moo, two, shoe
Chill ill pill, gill, Bill, mill
Lunchtime
Chip ship, hip, sip, rip, lip, whip,
zip, dip
Scorch score
Lunchroom
Cheese ease peas, keys, knees
Chicken sicken
Chili hilly, Willy, Billy
Nachos
Ketchup
Chop shop, cop, pop, top, hop, mop
Sandwich
Punch pun
Bunch bun
Peaches
Peach pea, P
Cherry airy Sherry, merry, scary
Pitcher
Lunches
Chapter Sixteen
Introduction
Resources in this book may be used in diverse ways within many different
approaches. This chapter illustrates how they are employed to evaluate and
treat students in one school setting. The illustration is descriptive rather
than prescriptive, suggesting how the resources might be used rather than
indicating how they should be used.
Clinical Orientation
439
440 The late eight
Articulation
A primary reason the 8 are acquired late is that they are hard to pronounce.
What late-acquired sounds share is that none are made with the articulators
touching throughout (as stop consonants and nasals are) or with the articu-
lators relatively far apart (as glides and vowels are). Instead, late-acquired
sounds require a student to position the articulators not touching, not far
apart, but somewhere in the middle, making the airflow variously stop and
start (affricates), hiss (fricatives), flow over the sides of the tongue (lateral),
or flow around and over an atypical tongue configuration ([r]). Examples of
resources focusing primarily on the articulation aspect of speech include
definitions, metaphors, demonstrations, phonetic placement and shaping
techniques, and key environments.
Phonology
Being a skilled motor movement is only half of speech’s dual nature. Speech
also is an aspect of language, requiring language knowledge similar to but
distinct from knowledge that underlies syntax, morphology, semantics, and
discourse. A critical aspect of treating late-acquired sounds entails drawing
a student’s attention to the communication value of speech. In addition to
learning how to pronounce a sound, a student must also learn how sound
affects meaning, how a sound is similar to and different from other sounds,
and how to self-monitor and self-correct to ensure that communication
occurs. Examples of resources focusing primarily on the phonologic aspect
of speech include speech exercises, language activities, and word lists.
Human Variables
A speech problem does not exist independently from the person who has
the problem. Factors such as motivation, intelligence, family support, atten-
tion, and desire to learn are equally as important for clinical success as are
linguistic ones. To give just one illustration of the diversity of the people
attached to speech problems, one recent morning three students in the same
grade and school received speech therapy, one after the other. The first
was embarrassed by his speech problem, the second thought his speech
problem sounded pretty cool, and the third didn’t know he had a speech
problem. Later that same day another student was treated — a teenager with
a severe [r] difficulty who desperately wanted speech help for an upcom-
ing radio presentation with his class. He promised to do anything to have
better speech — except give up basketball practice for speech therapy. Radio
address or not, basketball had a higher priority. The point is not to criticize
this priority — only to emphasize that students bring far more than their
linguistic system to the therapy setting. Examples of resources focusing pri-
marily on human variables arising in the treatment of late-acquired sounds
include acquisition, relative frequencies, and errors.
CHAPTER 16: Evaluation and Treatment: An Illustration 441
Human Variables
What is more trite and less controversial than the assertion
that treatment success depends as much on human variables
as linguistic ones? Nonetheless, considering that most clini-
cians believe such variables are important, it is amazing how
poor our knowledge base is about human variables compared
with our knowledge of linguistic factors. Far more research is
needed before we understand how learner attributes interact
with speech factors to influence treatment outcome.
Functional Activities
All students, children, and adults, gifted or delayed, learn and grow through-
out their lives. Time devoted to treatment of late-acquired sounds should
further this learning whenever possible. In practice, this entails using activi-
ties that encourage learning and personal growth. Class materials, books,
and projects are excellent sources of activities for school-aged students, as
newspapers and movies and upcoming social events are for older school-
aged students and adults. In addition to providing support for learning, a
critical benefit of treatment relying on such activities is that it encourages
use of a treatment sound in contexts that matter to a student. Examples of
resources that most directly support classroom and life-based activities are
the speech exercises and language activities.
Clinical Resources
The order in which resources are described in the following sections approxi-
mate their use with a hypothetical student from the evaluation through
conclusion of treatment. The sequence is indicated in Table 16–1.
Evaluation
n Initial screening
n Screening for stimulability
n Definition
n Acquisition
n Relative frequency
n Errors
442 The late eight
The most typical referral source for a student is a parent or teacher. A student
may then be observed in a classroom or playground before receiving an
initial speech screening and a screening test for stimulability.
Initial Screening
Definition
Acquisition
Acquisition data indicate the age at which 50% and 75% of children acquire
a sound. If all other things are equal (they seldom are), a clinician may
decide to first treat an earlier acquired sound. Indeed, for some clinicians the
importance of acquisition data is the primary criterion used in the selection
process.
Relative Frequency
Errors
Errors are the major errors to which a sound is susceptible. This information
weighs in sound selection in at least two respects:
Treatment
Three Questions
The assessment typically yields one or more possible treatment sounds. This
raises the following questions:
The author’s view is that answers to these questions have more to do with
human variables than linguistic factors. Some students — especially older
ones — can work on a sound from beginning to completion, while others
benefit from alternating between sounds, sometimes within a single treat-
ment session, sometimes in alternate sessions. A useful dynamic assessment
CHAPTER 16: Evaluation and Treatment: An Illustration 445
Once a sound or sounds have been selected for treatment, the following
resources are used:
n Metaphors
n Touch cues
n Demonstrations
n Phonetic placement and shaping techniques
n Key environments
n Word lists
n Awareness and speech exercises
n Language activities
Metaphors, touch cues, and demonstrations all provide useful ways to refer
to treatment sounds, and especially during early treatment phases may help
focus a student on the task at hand. As treatment progresses, they serve as
reminders and prompts.
CHAPTER 16: Evaluation and Treatment: An Illustration 447
Metaphors
Touch Cues
Touch cues are finger positions that represent a treatment sound, allowing
a clinician to refer to a treatment sound using modalities other than speech.
Touch cues grossly mimic speech movements. An older student understands
that, for example, the touch cue for velar consonants is made parallel to
the back of the mouth, representing where the tongue is raised. A younger
student may benefit from a touch cue without realizing its mimicking quality.
For such a student, a touch cue is a visual and tactile reminder, a way to say,
“Remember: this is the sound we are working on.”
Demonstrations
The following illustrates one possible way to fully expand a bare bones
phonetic placement technique:
Instructions:
2. To begin, place your tongue between your upper and lower front teeth.
3. Place a feather or small piece of paper in front of your mouth, about a
half-inch to an inch from your tongue.
4. Blow air over your tongue to move the feather or paper.
5. Explain, “That’s how you make the leaking tire sound. Now it’s your turn.”
6. Instruct the student to stick out his or her tongue just as you did.
7. When the tongue is out, place the feather or paper before the mouth.
8. Explain, “Now blow to make it move.”
9. If the sound is made correctly, say, “That’s right. You did it. You made [θ]
— the leaky tire sound.” If the sound is made incorrectly, say something
like, “Good try. Let’s try again.”
Key Environments
Beginning of Word
Establish [s], [l], and [r] before a high front vowel. Once established, expand
the number of different vowels that follow. For a student that is strongly
affected by the adjacent vowel, back high vowels are likely to be more chal-
lenging than front ones.
End of Word
[s] is more likely to be established here than [l] and [r]. Establish after a high
front vowel. Next, to make word-initial sounds, have the word-final sounds
be followed by a word beginning with a vowel, such as “bus and.” This
encourages the sound to “migrate” to start the following word, resulting in,
for example, “bu sand.”
Between Vowels
[l] and [r] are more likely to be established here than [s], though some
students find [s] easier to make here, too. For all three consonants, establish
between two high front vowels, as in ili. Once established, add different
adjacent vowels. To expand to word-initial position, have the student drop
450 The late eight
the first vowel, resulting in, for example, [li]. To expand to word-final posi-
tion, follow the same procedure, resulting in, for example, [il].
Consonant Clusters
For [s], establish after [t] as in “pizza” or the nonsense word [tsi]. To expand
the environments in which [s] occurs, have [ts] be followed by different
vowels. To help expand [s] to syllable-initial position, encourage the student
to drop the [t]. For [l] and [r], establish after a consonant with a different
place of production than [l] and [r] (most often, select [p] or [b]) followed by
a high front vowel. Next, to help expand [l] and [r], ask the student to drop
the initial consonant in the consonant cluster.
Word Lists
Word lists are used to generate stimuli to help establish a sound in a student’s
speech, and then to practice it. Words, rather than nonsense syllables, are the
vehicle for teaching a treatment sound for two reasons:
Language Activities
Language activities use school books and other outside materials, including
stories a student is reading, favorite stories from home, and articles from
newspapers and magazines. These materials, because they are familiar and
widely used, are easy to adapt by families, aides, and teachers. Many times
their use also has the practical advantage of improving a student’s academic
skills. Though the purpose of therapy is speech, much is gained if in the
process of learning speech a student also does better on classroom assign-
ments or gives a better oral report.
Summary
The discussion in this chapter illustrates that one way care might be concep-
tualized and carried out. Within this perspective, treatment for late-acquired
sounds is conceptualized as helping a student learn new ways of speaking
and to unlearn old ones. This requires a treatment approach focusing on
452 The late eight
both the articulation and phonologic aspects of speech. Within this view,
a treatment program focused solely on articulation is like building a train
that doesn’t go anywhere, while a treatment program focused solely on
phonology is like a destination without a vehicle to get there. Treatment of
late-acquired sounds requires careful attention to building a good train to
reach a worthwhile travel destination. An additional characteristic of this
framework is that human variables such as motivation, intelligence, family
support, attention, and desire to learn are recognized as being at least equally
important for clinical success as linguistic ones. Lastly, the framework empha-
sizes the importance of using functional activities that contribute to the
advancement of a student’s education and social development.
References
Language Activities
453
454 The late eight
Awareness Activities
1. Read aloud from a book, having the student listen and raise a hand, clap,
or ring a bell whenever a word containing the treatment sound is heard.
An easy variation is to tell a story aloud rather than read aloud from a
book.
2. Ask a student to silently read a newspaper or story and then circle or
write down words that contain the treatment sound.
3. Read a story to a therapy group, having the students compete to be the
first to raise their hand when they hear the treatment sound.
4. Read a story to the student and a stuffed animal, having the two “compete”
for who raises a hand first when a treatment sound is heard. Let the stuffed
animal often give the wrong answer and have the student correct it.
5. Create minimal pairs using household objects or school materials (e.g.,
wing/ring, light/write). Then say a minimal pair and ask the student
which word (first or second) contains the treatment sound.
6. Tape-record several word lists and ask the student to rate them as being
either the new way or the old way of producing the treatment sound.
7. Read paragraphs from stories to the student and occasionally mispro-
nounce the treatment sound (e.g., “Once upon a time there was a print-
Appendix A: Language Activities 455
heth who lived in a cathle”). Have the student raise a hand whenever
the treatment sound is mispronounced.
8. Make up and read silly stories/sentences that contain the treatment
sound produced the “old” way (e.g., “He gave her a diamond wing”).
Draw silly pictures to match the silly sentences.
9. In a therapy group, have a student who can pronounce the treatment
sound read sentences aloud, sometimes saying the treatment sound cor-
rectly and other times incorrectly. The student with difficulty on the
treatment sound does a thumb up/thumb down, depending on whether
the treatment sound is correct or not.
10. Work with the student to make a book of words, each containing the
treatment sound. An easy variation is to have the student find pictures
depicting objects that contain the treatment sound and then have the
student paste the pictures into the book.
Speech Activities
1. Ask the student to read a story or article and to say each word that
contains the treatment sound.
2. Create a therapy group containing some students who can pronounce a
treatment sound and others who cannot. Either read aloud or tell a story
to the group, asking the students to listen for the treatment sound and
having a student who can pronounce it do so every time it occurs. Ask
the student who cannot pronounce it to say it after the student who can.
3. Give the student a printed story that has a sticker over words that contain
the treatment sound. Ask the student to read the story aloud and “to
guess” at the words under the stickers.
4. Read the student a story, stopping at words that contain the treatment
sound. The student then says the word. For younger students, have a
puppet or stuffed animal tell the story.
5. For a younger student, find cards or objects that contain the treatment
sound. Place them around a room, turn off the lights, and give the
student a flashlight. The student then finds the cards or objects and says
them as they are found.
6. Have the student make a book of words or pictures, each containing the
treatment sound, and then say each word to you.
7. Play a board game and adapt the rules to make it a therapy tool (e.g.,
move ahead two spaces with an accurate production; go back one space
with an “old way” production).
8. Use checkers with words containing the treatment sound attached to the
back. Have the student say the word taped on the back before moving
a piece.
9. Adapt familiar song tunes to include treatment sounds and words (e.g.,
Larry had a little lamb).
456 The late eight
10. Have the student make a collage with pictures containing the treatment
sound and then name the pictures.
11. Have the student collect and name pictures of friends/teachers who have
the treatment sound in their names.
12. Have the student write a letter to someone (family/friends) using words
with the treatment sounds as many times as possible.
13. Ask the student to list all the toys, animals, foods, and so forth that
contain the treatment sound.
14. Have the student look through spelling words, classroom vocabulary
words, and reading group books to find as many words as possible that
contain the treatment sound.
15. Have the student substitute the treatment sound for another sound in
words in stories or books (e.g., pronounce polled as rolled).
16. Ask the student to fill in the correct treatment word in sentences from a
bank of words or have the student choose his or her own words.
17. Make up sentences for the student containing words with the treatment
sounds for the student to say. Alternatively, ask the student to make up
the sentences.
18. Place a word or picture containing the treatment sound on a card, make
two copies of each card, and play “Go Fish.”
19. Tell the student a story containing words with the treatment sound, and
then have the student retell the story to you.
20. For a student who likes jokes, you and the student develop tongue twist-
ers containing the treatment sound.
21. Create a scavenger hunt using words containing the treatment sound.
22. Have the student say a word in a book with the treatment sound, then
say it without it, then with it again (e.g., rain, -ain, rain).
23. For a younger student, set toys containing the treatment sound around
the room. Blindfold a stuffed animal and have the student tell it the name
of the toys.
24. Lay out picture sequence cards that contain the treatment sound and ask
the student to tell you the story.
25. Adapt a song to make a silly version containing the treatment sound (e.g.,
“Christmas is coming, a rooster ate a rat”).
Evaluation
Treatment
457
458 The late eight
459
460 The late eight