Screening Violence
Screening Violence
Screening Violence
Rutgers
University
Press
New Brunswick,
New Jersey
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
British Cataloging-in-Publication Data for this book is available from the British Library
vii
vm
Contents
Contributors 267
Index 269
l
Screening Violence
Stephen Prince
1
2
Stephen Prince
Figure 1. In earlier decades, gunshot victims gracefully expired with minimal fuss. In Casablanca,
Major Strasser (Conrad Veidt) takes a bullet in the chest at close range and drops out of the frame
without losing his grip on a telephone.
monster tossing a little girl into a lake (she drowns) was deleted from
the final cuts of those pictures. And in countless Westerns and urban
crime dramas, shooting victims frowned and sank gracefully out of
frame, with their white shirts immaculate.
With its roots in Catholic Church doctrine, the Production
Code aimed to enforce a strict morality and healthy-mindedness in the
movies. This policy was the industry's response to a prolonged period
in the 1910s and 1920s of social agitation over movie content,2 during
which time many citizens' groups and state and local agencies tried to
censor the movies. To protect itself, the industry passed the Production
Code and created in 1934 an enforcement agency, the Production Code
Administration (PCA). For decades, the Code and the PCA held vio¬
lence on screen to a minimum. ,
Inevitably, though, some films and filmmakers pushed the
boundaries of what was deemed acceptable. The gangster film cycle of
the early 1930s, in particular, inflamed national controversy about the
unwholesome effects of the movies. Little Caesar (1930), The Public
4
Stephen Prince
Figure 2. Tony Montana (Al Pacino) expires in an ultrabloody showdown at the end ojScarface.
The ferocity of his death scene could not have been shown in earlier decades of American cinema.
Figure 3. In the 1930s, Hollywood gangster films aroused public outcry over their violence and
romantic depiction of criminals. In The Public Enemy (1931), Tom Powers (James Cagney) pre¬
pares to coldly execute a boyhood pal.
The beginning of the shift, that point when the graphic representation
of physical violence first became a distinct stylistic possibility in the
American cinema, is easy to date. Ultraviolence emerged in the late
1960s, and movies have never been the same since. The factors that
helped produce this new violence were instigated by two watershed
events in Hollywood history: the revision in September 1966 of Holly¬
wood's thirty-six-year-old Production Code and the creation two years
later of the Code and Rating Administration with its G-M-R-X classifi¬
cation scheme. These changes were responses to the more liberal and
tolerant culture of the period, particularly the revolution in social
mores tied to the youth movement. Shackled by the Production Code,
movies were thirty years behind the times. Accordingly and led by the
Motion Picture Association of America (MPAA), the Hollywood indus¬
try mounted an aggressive campaign to make films relevant again for a
society whose attitudes and practices no longer coincided with the
morality institutionalized in the Production Code.
Thus, the 1966 revision scrapped the Code's injunctions about
specific content areas and substituted in their place a few broadly
phrased guiding principles that, as MPAA head Jack Valenti pointed
out, significantly expanded the creative license of filmmakers. Where
the old Code told filmmakers precisely how they were to approach and
show scenes of violence, the revised Code merely recommended that
filmmakers exercise discretion in showing the taking of human life.
"Discretion" is an elastic and relative concept, and as the intensifying
violence in American cinema in the late 1960s shows, it was mostly an
ineffective principle. But the scrapping of specific content injunctions
was not the most significant revision. In a development that was far
more important and influential for subsequent filmmaking, the MPAA
created a new Suggested for Mature Audiences (SMA) designation for
films that had harder, more adult content. This new designation en-
7
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
abled filmmakers for the first time to target an adult audience and on
that basis take sex and violence much further than in the past when the
audience mix included young viewers.
An additional revision a few years later accelerated these devel¬
opments. In 1968, the MPAA unveiled a four-way classification system—
G-M-R-X—that differentiated films by audience segment. G-rated films
were suitable for the entire family, y/hile underage viewers were prohib¬
ited from seeing X-rated films and could only view R-films if accompa¬
nied by a parent or adult guardian. The G-M-R-X scheme made even
greater freedoms available to filmmakers because films could now be
niche-marketed to adult audiences, bypassing the content restrictions
that the presence of young viewers had hitherto necessitated. These mod¬
ifications of the Production Code helped produce a wave of tougher,
harder-edged, and controversial films whose graphic violence, profanity,
and sexuality exemplified the new artistic freedoms that the MPAA had
been seeking and promoting. Films like Bonnie and Clyde (1967), The
Fox (1968), The Detective (1968), In Cold Blood (1967), Point Blank
(1967), and Barbarella (1968) would have been unthinkable a mere five
years previously, and they collectively demonstrate the emergence of the
new, adult-themed cinema that the MPAA helped inaugurate.
Indeed, the MPAA's advocacy of new filmmaking freedoms was
a remarkable feature of this revolution in American cinema. The
MPAA pushed aggressively for these freedoms despite vocal protest
from groups opposed to these changes. Testifying before the National
Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence on December
19, 1968, Jack Valenti proudly announced, "There is a new breed of
filmmaker. And mark you well this new filmmaker, because he's an ex¬
traordinary fellow. .. . He's reaching out for new dimensions of expres¬
sion. And he is not bound—not bound—by the conventions of a
conformist past. I happen to think that's good."3 In another context,
Valenti stressed the importance of bringing "old movie standards out of
the archaic and arcane and into current trends."4 Criticizing viewers
who protested the rising levels of violence, sexuality, and profanity in
the movies, an MPAA community relations associate cited the "new
revolutions" in taste and morality in society. She recommended that
viewers reorient themselves to these social changes and the new cin¬
ema they were helping to produce. "They [the public] fail to realize that
films have changed to reflect our changing culture."5
The MPAA's responsiveness to late 1960s culture, in this re-
8
Stephen Prince
spect, was strikingly different from the posture and policy it assumed
in the early 1980s, when a more conservative national climate produced
vehement criticism of movie violence by government officials and
social watchdog groups. Brian De Palma's lurid and bloody thriller
Dressed to Kill (1980) aroused tremendous criticism because of its vio¬
lence against women. Anticipating problems, the MPAA threatened
during post-production to slap an X-rating on the picture unless De
Palma trimmed its.violence. De Palma protested the cuts but never¬
theless made them so that the picture could be released with an R-
rating. Explaining the tough line taken by the MPAA with regard to
Dressed to Kill, Jack Valenti pointed to the political realignments of the
Reagan era and implicitly announced that the agency's late 1960s lib¬
eralism was over. He said, "The political climate in this country is
shifting to the right, and that means more conservative attitudes to¬
ward sex and violence. But a lot of creative people are still living in the
world of revolution."6
During the 1960s, however, in its period of high liberalism, the
MPAA aimed to bring films into closer accord with the youth audience
and its general questioning of Establishment values. This was a vital
demographic and one that the agency was determined to court; a 1968
MPAA audience survey showed that 16- to 24-year-olds were responsi¬
ble for 48 percent of national ticket sales.7 To capture this young audi¬
ence, the MPAA believed that films would have to become more
attuned to contemporary mores, which adherence to a thirty-six-year-
old Production Code prevented. In addition to the youth audience, how¬
ever, a multitude of other factors influenced and helped shape the new
direction of American cinema. The period's general social turmoil, its
climate of political violence, and, most especially, the war in Vietnam
convinced many filmmakers and the MPAA that movie violence paled
next to the real-life bloodshed in the nation's cities and the jungles of
Southeast Asia. The savage bloodshed of the Vietnam War established
a context whereby filmmakers felt justified in reaching for new levels
of screen violence. Moreover, the war and the political assassinations
of the 1960s fed a general cultural fascination with violence to which
the movies responded.8
Jack .Valenti remarked on the interconnections between the
era's social and media violence and did so in a manner that claimed
film's prerogative in responding to real-world violence. In 1968, he re¬
marked, "For the first time in the history of this country, people are ex¬
posed to instant coverage [on television] of a war in progress. When so
9
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
many movie .critics complain about violence on film, I don't think they
realize the impact of 30 minutes on the Huntley-Brinkley newscast—
and that's real violence."9 Bonnie and Clyde was the most explicitly
violent film that had yet been made, and its director, Arthur Penn,
claimed that he didn't consider it especially violent when taken in con¬
text of the Vietnam era. "Not given the times in which we were living,
because every night on the news wejsaw kids in Vietnam being airlifted
out in body bags, with blood all over the place. Why, suddenly, the cin¬
ema had to be immaculate, I'll never know."10
Resulting from the industry's efforts to connect with a young,
contemporary audience and the period in which that audience lived,
motion picture violence began its remarkable escalation in 1967.
United Artists released Sergio Leone's Dollars trilogy of Westerns—A
Fistful of Dollars, For a Few Dollars More, and The Good, the Bad, and
the Ugly—on staggered dates throughout that year to accentuate the se¬
ries nature of the films. Their release triggered a storm of protest over
Leone's more cold-blooded and brutal depiction of the West. A reviewer
for Variety, the film industry's trade journal, called Fistful a "blood¬
bath," with "sadism from start to finish, unmitigated brutality, a pil¬
ing up of bodies."11 The paper's subsequent review of The Good, the
Bad, and the Ugly objected, "One sequence in particular, a five-minute
torture session that climaxes in an attempted eye-gouging, may well
serve as the battle cry for opponents of screen violence."12
The Leone films, which had been made in Italy, aroused con¬
siderable protest from groups opposed to the new screen violence, but
it was two American films that year—The Dirty Dozen and Bonnie and
Clyde—that ignited the loudest cries. The Dirty Dozen was an uncom¬
monly cynical World War II action picture about hardened criminals re¬
cruited for a suicide mission into Nazi Germany. In the climax of the
picture, the Americans incinerate their Nazi enemies by locking them
in closed quarters, dousing them with petrol, and setting them afire.
The sadism of the sequence appalled many commentators. Among the
most vocal of these was Bosley Crowther, the prominent New York
Times critic. He condemned the film, and the new movie violence of
which it was part, for its "glorification of killing" and for blatantly ap¬
pealing to an audience's aggressive and sadistic appetites.13 Bonnie and
Clyde went even farther by explicitly portraying the details and physi-
cality of violent death, concluding with an extended slaughter sequence
that shows the outlaws raked with automatic weapons' fire, their bod¬
ies riddled by bullet strikes. Both Time and Newsweek condemned the
10
Stephen Prince
Figure 4. Squibs registering bullet strikes are an essential component of contemporary movie vi¬
olence. His face obscured with blood spray, Mr. Blonde (Michael Madsen) is gunned down by Mr.
Orange (Tim Roth) in Reservoir Dogs. The character’s white shirt provides maximum contrast
for the squibs.
11
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
Figure 5. In The Wild Bunch, Peckinpah innovated in the use of squibs by employing them to
represent exit wounds. When a bounty hunter shoots an army soldier, viewers see the exit wound
but not the bullet strike on the victim’s chest.
sent in film prior to 1967 and which helped give violence in these ear¬
lier periods an unreal and sanitized appearance. To be sure, certain pic¬
tures in these earlier periods anticipated the modern staging of gun
violence. In Shane (1953), for example, a gunfire victim is hurled back¬
ward by the force of a bullet's impact, and in Penn's own The Left-
Handed Gun (1958), a hapless deputy is literally blown out of his boot
by a gun blast. But despite such striking details, these scenes lacked
squib-work and the palpable physicality that it lent gun violence. Dur¬
ing and after 1967, by contrast, the savage impact of gunfire on human
flesh became an enduring feature of screen killing. Films released in the
same year as Bonnie and Clyde and which lacked squib-work—The
Professionals, El Dorado—seemed bloodless, irrevocably part of a now-
archaic era in screen violence and not at all contemporary with Penn's
film. In the film's climactic sequence, Bonnie (Faye Dunaway) and
Clyde (Warren Beatty) are ambushed by the Texas Rangers, who are
armed with machine guns. The actors were rigged with multiple squibs.
When detonated in sequence and augmented by the writhing of the ac¬
tors, these provided a horrifying visualization of the outlaws' bodies
being punctured by scores of bullets.
Penn filmed this action with four cameras running at different
speeds. Most operated at very high speeds to provide slow-motion
footage for the construction of a complexly edited sequence. Penn and
his editor Dede Allen assembled a brief but sophisticated montage of
the ambush. The editing juxtaposes differential rates of slow motion to
extend the outlaws' death agonies and to capture, as Penn put it, the
balletic and the spastic qualities of their violent deaths. Penn said,
12
Stephen Prince
"What I did do, which I think had not been done, was to vary the speeds
of the slow motion so that I could get both the spastic and the balletic
qualities at the same time."14 These are qualities in tension with one
another, yet Penn and Allen's masterful editing conjoin movements
that are equally dancelike and convulsive. The scene's alternately beau¬
tiful and horrifying qualities are the result of its choreography and
specifically of Penn's determination to yoke dance and convulsion to¬
gether in a novel combination. The results were truly seminal. The
montage of slow-motion, squib-fired death agony powerfully influ¬
enced subsequent filmmakers, especially Sam Peckinpah, who took the
design to new extremes the following year in The Wild Bunch.
The Dirty Dozen and Bonnie and Clyde sent the industry a
clear signal that would not be ignored. These controversial pictures
were extraordinarily popular. The Dirty Dozen was the biggest money¬
maker of 1967, and so extraordinary was repeat business for Bonnie and
Clyde that Variety, tracking its box-office performance, placed it in an
"impossible to project" category.15 The picture's popularity placed it on
Variety's weekly list of the top dozen box-office earners twenty-two
times, a record topped at the time only by Mary Poppins (thirty-two
times on the list in late 1964-early 1965). Time magazine noted the in¬
dustry's response to this success and its implications for the future:
"There is an almost euphoric sense in Hollywood that more such
movies can and will be made."16
The outstanding commercial success of Leone's Dollars trilogy,
The Dirty Dozen, and Bonnie and Clyde showed that the public had
hitherto unappeased appetites for screen carnage and that the industry
could make a lot of money from filming hyperviolence. Thus, in short
order, the threshold that Bonnie and Clyde had crossed in 1967, with
its images of slow-motion bloodletting, was surpassed. Director Sam
Peckinpah's The Wild Bunch (1969) offered two extended slaughter se¬
quences, opening and closing the film, that were far more gruesome,
graphic, and protracted than the gun battle in Penn's picture had been.
Nothing in the American cinema had been as remotely violent as what
Peckinpah now put on screen in The Wild Bunch.
Its gun battles had ferocity and an intensity that Bonnie and
Clyde only approximated. Peckinpah and his producer Phil Feldman
knew exactly what they were doing in crafting blood-soaked images
that were the most audacious yet conceived. While Peckinpah was in
pre-production on The Wild Bunch, Feldman wrote to congratulate him
for offending the MPAA with the savagery of the picture's proposed
13
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
bloodshed and urged him to view the Leone films before deciding how
far to go. “I think it might be good for you to have a comparative basis
before you finally decide just how far other people have gone in the field
of blood and gore and what the public is comparing us to."17 Further¬
more, on March 19, 1968, Warner Bros, sent a letter to Peckinpah's pro¬
duction manager confirming that a print of Bonnie and Clyde would be
shipped to Parras, Mexico, where^Peckinpah was on location for The
Wild Bunch.18 The film would arrive for screening the weekend of
March 23-24. This was just prior to the start of principal photography
on March 25. Peckinpah evidently wished to take another look at
Penn's achievement, to reinforce his understanding of that achieve¬
ment and his desire to surpass it.
Many viewers were appalled by the film's gore. An early test
screening of the film elicited such negative responses as "The movie is
nothing but mass murder" and "Nauseating, unending, offensive
bloody violence."19 But the film also had its passionate defenders, and
these included ordinary viewers as well as prominent film critics. Penn
and Peckinpah were enormously talented filmmakers, and they had
crafted the two most vivid, audacious, and ambitious films that Holly¬
wood had seen in many years. Thus, these pictures gained a significant
measure of respect and critical stature that helped legitimize the in-
your-face bloodletting that otherwise made them so notorious. Penn
and Peckinpah were both radical social critics, disturbed by the cor¬
ruption of America in its Vietnam years, and they proved that film¬
makers in the late 1960s could use graphic violence for serious
purposes. Peckinpah, for example, repeatedly remarked that he wished
to deglamorize movie violence in order to show how ugly and awful real
violence was.20
Unfortunately, and perhaps inevitably, the stylistics of graphic
violence proved to hold tremendous fascination for subsequent gener¬
ations of filmmakers who did not share Penn and Peckinpah's radical
social objectives. The explicitness of this violence quickly escalated.
Made only a few years after The Wild Bunch, for example, Taxi Driver
(1976) was far bloodier and much more graphic, with images of dis¬
memberment and a gunshot victim's brains splattered on a wall. Penn
and Peckinpah helped establish the stylistic features of ultraviolence,
while subsequent filmmakers have replicated and exaggerated them.
Squib-work, multicamera filming, and montage editing utilizing dif¬
ferential rates of slow motion—this combination of elements became
one of the two dominant aesthetic forms of ultraviolence. It is today
14
Stephen Prince
Figures 6 & 7. One of the most horrific moments of violence in recent cinema occurred in Alien,
when a baby monster bursts from the chest of its human host. Graphic imagery of physical mu¬
tilation would become a staple of horror films.
very difficult to find gun battles in movies that have not been stylized
in this fashion. (The elaborate gun battles in L.A. Confidential [1997]
are notably deviant because they lack slow motion.) Moreover, the form
has been internationalized. Hong Kong director John Woo (The Killer
[1989], Hard Boiled [1992]) is the best-known contemporary exponent
of the furiously bloody gun battles in the style elaborated by Penn and
Peckinpah. Despite the fact that Woo's work is situated in another cul¬
ture and country (at least until his current Hollywood period), the for¬
mal design bf violence in his films follows the now-familiar and
conventional parameters of the Penn-Peckinpah stylistic.
Contemporary .ultraviolence exhibits a second predominant
aesthetic form. In addition to the Penn-Peckinpah stylistic, ultravio¬
lence includes graphic imagery of bodily mutilation. This type of im-
15
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
agery was not part of the Penn-Peckinpah stylistic, beyond the use of
squib-work, because that style stressed the kinetic effects of montage,
making violence balletic, a dance of death. But graphic mutilation—eye
gouging, impalement, and dismemberment—surfaced in the horror
film in the late 1970s and the 1980s, as that genre abandoned the at¬
mospherics of earlier decades and offered instead stomach-churning
and gut-wrenching experiences. As Carol Clover notes,
Figure 8. In the prelude to a highly detailed killing scene, a knife-wielding and masked assailant
grabs his victim (Drew Barrymore) in the opening sequence of Scream. Because she does not die
easily or quickly, the sadism goes on at length.
stop the productions, although by the late 1990s the slasher cycle was
nearly played out, as evidenced in part by the degree of self-conscious¬
ness it attained in Wes Craven's Scream films. But ultraviolent horror
films remain very popular, as the huge collections of gory movies in
video stores demonstrate and as the Web pages devoted in cyberspace
to Jason, Freddy Kreuger, and the other killer heroes indicate.
Ultraviolent films since 1967 have utilized these two aesthetic
formats—montage-slow motion and graphic mutilation—to provide
very powerful and intense experiences for spectators. The medium's
power to agitate, horrify, and excite movie audiences has generated per¬
sistent concern about the psychological and social effects of violent
films. These concerns have appeared throughout film (and television)
history. The 1930 Production Code, for example, was premised upon
characteristics of the medium and the audience, which in combination
were deemed socially undesirable. These characteristics were formally
written into the Code as its reasons for being. They included cinema's
intense appeal to mature as well as immature viewers, the spread of cin¬
ema to all sectors and regions of society and consequent erosion of local
community standards, the suggestibility of a mass audience, and the
vividness and emotional appeal of the medium. "In general, the mobil¬
ity, popularity, accessibility, emotional appeal, vividness, [and]
straightforward presentation of fact in the film make for more intimate
contact with a larger audience and for greater emotional appeal. Hence
the larger moral responsibilities of the motion pictures."27
An empirical examination of the effects of movies on young
18
Stephen Prince
viewers, the Payne Fund Studies (1933), closely followed passage of the
Production Code. The studies were not concerned with violence per se
but with juvenile delinquency more broadly conceived in terms of
young lives given over to petty crime and sexual promiscuity, possibly
resulting from Hollywood's influence. The studies are an early mani¬
festation of a persistent fear that visual media may have harmful effects
on viewers due to the media's vivid presentation and modeling of un¬
desirable behaviors. In Movies and Conduct, a volume in the series, the
sociologist Herbert Blumer developed the concept of "emotional pos¬
session" to explain the impact of film on viewers and the potential of
the medium to exert strong effects.28 For Blumer, emotional possession
had three components. Viewers identify strongly with characters and
situations on screen (e.g., romantic lovers in a kissing scene), and this
leads to the arousal of normally repressed feelings. Heightened arousal
produces a loss of ordinary self-control as the unleashed feelings gain
or threaten to gain expression. Blumer speculated that, for some view¬
ers over time, the emotions and behavioral repertoires associated with
emotional possession may become entrenched and lead to a long-term
alteration of character.
Blumer's terminology—emotional possession—tends to sound
a bit lurid, and it certainly overstates the affective components of cin¬
ema viewing at the expense of their cognitive correlates and the
viewer's cognitive framing of the experience. Most viewers, for exam¬
ple, doubtless realize they are watching a movie and understand that
there are boundaries between movie experience and real-life experi¬
ence. But Blumer was right to consider the special vividness and emo¬
tive power of the medium as key variables affecting viewer responses.
By varying camera positions, editing rhythms, and using music and spe¬
cial effects, filmmakers stylize violence and heighten its emotional and
visceral impact. The history of cinema is, in part, a history of increas¬
ingly vivid presentational techniques. Public fears that the medium's
intense appeals might bypass traditional avenues of socialization and
reconfigure social mores were acute in the 1930s, as the Payne Fund
Studies demonstrate, but these anxieties did not receive definitive an¬
swers in the period's research. The evidence furnished by the Payne
Fund Studies was ambiguous, and it pointed toward limited media ef¬
fects. Moreover, graphic violence was not a feature of American movies
in that period. After 1967, though, when the censorship barriers came
down and explicit sex and violence filled the screen, the old fears about
the medium's impact were again relevant. The rise of ultras iolence
19
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
with MPAA president Jack Valenti. "I was interested in the responses
about Bonnie and Clyde," Boggs said. "I might tell him that we had a
murder in my town committed by an 18-year-old boy who had come
out of Bonnie and Clyde one hour before. He killed a young man who
was running a drive-in grocery store. And it was just a senseless mur¬
der. Now, whether or not what he saw in Bonnie and Clyde had any im¬
pact on the murder, I don't know. But I know that what I say to you is
a fact—that he saw this movie which glorifies violence."59
picture yet conceived." "I hope the viciousness of the fights stays in the
film so people can really know how bad killing is." "The violence ob¬
viously was handled at a point near brilliance. The really shocking hor¬
ror of this mass killing definitely makes one think."
As these responses indicate, disturbing or outre images of vio¬
lence may stimulate some viewers to reflect upon the filmmaker's
moral point of view and probable intentions in crafting the images, al¬
though such viewers may arrive at very different conclusions about
these issues. At the same time, other viewers of The Wild Bunch clearly
exhibited less reflective and more excitatory reactions. Contemporary
reviewers commented upon the laughter that greeted the film's violent
episodes: "At all this the audience laughed (and so did I), not with mer¬
riment, exactly, but in tribute to such virtuosity of gore."62 Screen¬
writer Charles Higson recalled his first reaction to the film as a
teenager, one marked by a rush of excitement: "Once the film was over,
Figure 9. Covered in blood after a frenzy of shooting and stabbing that claims multiple victims,
Travis Bickle (Robert De Niro) taunts the police by mimicking suicide. Taxi Driver escalated the
threshold of permissible violence well past what Sam Peckinpah had shown in The Wild Bunch.
27
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
stuff like thait, you'd be cringing, turning away from the telly."70 Zill-
mann has discussed a similar concentration on special effects by male
viewers of horror films.71
Even a film whose violence is as harrowing as that in Saving
Private Ryan employs an elaborately artificial audiovisual design.
Spielberg and cinematographer Janusz Kaminski used a number of un¬
usual techniques to get the striking; vivid, and unexpected visual qual¬
ities of that film's battle scenes. For some shots, they employed a
shutter set at 45 and 90 degrees, instead of the more usual 180-degree
configuration, in order to pixelate the action. They stripped the coating
off the lenses to flatten contrast and get a foggy but sharp look. They
shot with the camera shutter out of sync to produce a streaking effect
from the top to bottom of the image. They used a Clairmont Camera
Image Shaker to produce horizontal and vertical shaking of the camera,
and they flashed the film to desaturate the color and used ENR to add
contrast. (Named after Technicolor technician Ernesto N. Rico, ENR is
a bleach-bypass process that leaves silver in the print during develop¬
ing. This gives the film image richer, deeper shadows and desaturated
pastels.) These effects made the battlefield carnage more vivid, but they
also supplied the visual effects that viewers could use to create some
emotional and cognitive distance from the depicted violence. These
techniques gave the violence an elaborate and explicit aesthetic frame,
which was intensified by the picture's narrative of heroism and moral
redemption. The violence was not raw, that is, it was not 'real.' It was
staged for the cameras and filtered through the various effects and tech¬
niques employed by the filmmakers. By contrast, when screen violence
lacks this aesthetic dimension, its evocation of negative emotions can
be markedly unpleasant for viewers, as in the McCauley study.
These considerations suggest some uncomfortable and unpleas¬
ant facts about the medium of cinema. Filmmakers who wish to use
graphic violence to offer a counterviolence message—that is, to use vi¬
olence in a way that undercuts its potential for arousing excitatory re¬
sponses in viewers—may be working in the wrong medium. The
medium subverts the goal. In significant ways, the aesthetic contract
that the filmmaker must honor with viewers entails that screen vio¬
lence be made to offer sensory pleasures. These pleasures are implicated
in the aggression responses studied in the empirical literature. The prob¬
lem is especially serious due to the phenomenon of divergent audience
responses. The difficulty here can be stated very simply: filmmakers
cannot control the reactions of their viewers. Neither Peckinpah nor
30
Stephen Prince
Figure 10. The-Basketball Diaries has been implicated in several real-world shootings in which
the killers cited the inspirational nature of the film sequence where a high school kid (Leonardo
Di Caprio) shoots his teacher and classmates. Note how the lighting and camera angle aestheti-
cize the violence by accentuating the drama and making it visually striking.
lective reaction: "It wasn't like people were laughing because people
were dying. The woman who got shot fell funny and people just
laughed."73 For some of the viewers, no doubt, the laughter was a dis¬
tancing response to alleviate tension, to insulate their feelings from the
depicted violence, and, in a high school environment rife with peer
pressure, to prevent peers from seeing that one is touched or sad and
therefore emotionally vulnerable. At the same time, however, such
laughter can signal a failure of empathy, an inability (or an unwilling¬
ness) to imaginatively place oneself inside the fiction and relate to the
pain or violence on an immediately personal level. In some films, like
Taxi Driver, the audiovisual structure of a scene might work to prevent
a viewer's empathic response by stressing the artifice and spectacle of
the violence, but here Spielberg evidently sought to show the violence
within an appropriately moral and empathy-inducing framework.
Viewer reactions were nevertheless somewhat unpredictable.
The Claremont High incident was not the only case of variant
responses elicited by the film. Washington Post columnist Donna Britt
encountered a similar incident when she went to see the film.74 She
reported that a group of middle-aged and elderly women were whoop¬
ing and laughing during scenes that prompted flinching, gasps, and tears
from the rest of the audience. A more extreme variant response oc¬
curred when an evidently deranged man in a San Diego theater shot the
woman sitting in front of him during a scene in the film where Nazis
32
Stephen Prince
The essays collected in this volume are drawn from a variety of sources
that demonstrate the complexity of the topic and the manner in which
it has cut across different realms of public discourse. These sources in¬
clude the popular press, scholarly journal articles representing work in
cinema studies and empirical social science, and testimony elicited by
congressional committees investigating problems of violence in soci¬
ety and the media. By examining movie violence in three contexts—its
history, its aesthetic characteristics and structure, and its potential ef¬
fects upon individual and society—this volume presents the topic in its
richness and complexity. As a result, readers will develop an under¬
standing of cinema violence as a representational construct and a social
phenomenon, a function of distinct historical periods, characteristics
of film form, and ongoing intellectual and research paradigms.
The first five essays in this volume set the historical context.
35
Graphic Violence in the Cinema
Stephen Prince
its seminal influence, she notes that "it was the first major film to allow
us the luxury of inspecting what frightened us-the senseless, the un¬
expected, the bloody. And most important, it kindly stylized death for
us; it created nobility from senselessness, it choreographed a dance out
of blood and death, it gave meaning and import to our mortal twitch-
ings." By choreographing violence in such a way as to impose an aes¬
thetic order on the experience of violent death, films of graphic
bloodshed impose this order upon the viewer's conceptual and emo¬
tional experience. Thus, Sobchack suggests that graphic violence per¬
forms a useful social function. Her essay and her reflections date from
the early 1970s. In the essay's afterword, Sobchack reassesses these
questions some twenty-five years later, in light of the intervening
decades of movie violence. The contrast between her reflections past
and present illuminates the trajectory that the nation's culture, as a
whole, has traveled in relation to cinema violence.
Carol J. Clover explicates the sexual and gendered aspects of
violence in horror films, specifically the slasher film. Since the 1980s,
these grisly slaughter-fests had taken over the horror genre, offering
viewers stalk-and-murder narratives overlaid with detailed imagery of
butchery and mutilation. While most critics were quick to condemn
these films as trash, Clover, instead, took a close view and found rich
psychological material in the format. Her illuminating analysis clari¬
fies the complex appeal of these films for viewers, amid all of the prob¬
lematic and prurient violence.
In the concluding essay in this section, I examine the origins
and aesthetic of slow-motion violence. Since Bonnie and Clyde and
The Wild Bunch, this has become the predominant way of stylizing vi¬
olent gun battles. Filmmakers insert slow-motion shots into extended
montages of death and destruction. I explore how filmmakers (chiefly,
Sam Peckinpah, who did more than any other director to popularize this
aesthetic) orchestrate these scenes and the emotional and moral impli¬
cations of these constructions.
Explorations of aesthetics inevitably open onto issues of effect.
The remaining essays examine these issues. The essays by Leonard
Berkowitz and Richard B. Felson offer somewhat contrasting portraits
of the state of research on media violence. Berkowitz provides a detailed
explanation of the role that cognitive factors (thoughts and ideas) play
in viewer reactions to media violence. Readers should not be put off by
the somewhat wordy title of this essay,- the discussion is clearly pre¬
sented and very straightforward. While arguing that the social costs of
40
Stephen Prince
violent imagery in the mass media are real and may be deemed unac¬
ceptable, Berkowitz shows the complexity of media-viewer interac¬
tions. Specifically, he explicates the role of thought (induced by media
programming) in activating bodily reactions. He terms this process the
"spreading activation" of thoughts related to media images and content
and which, under the right conditions, can dampen a viewer's inhibi¬
tions against acting aggressively. Berkowitz examines the factors that
can trigger and facilitate this "spreading activation." His account
shows that empirical research on media violence does not construct a
mechanistic portrait of media messages and passively reacting viewers.
Instead, while the bulk of the evidence clearly shows aggression-
inducing characteristics of media programming, .the processes involved
are complicated and multifactoral.
While a general consensus prevails among social scientists
about the aggression-inducing characteristics of movie and television
content, some controversy exists about the strength and duration of
these effects. In the final essay, sociologist Richard B. Felson undertakes
a thorough review of the empirical literature and offers a more tenta¬
tive assessment of media effects. He concludes that, while effects are
present, they are likely to be weak and to affect only a small percent¬
age of viewers, and are unlikely to be a significant factor in the high
crime rate in this country.
The essays by Berkowitz and Felson give the reader an exten¬
sive account of the available literature and theory on the effects of
media violence and a sampling of the controversy and range of opinion
that bear on this issue. There is as yet no definitive resolution of the
problem. The questions, though, remain urgent ones, from the stand¬
point of individual as well as social psychology. Neither Felson nor
Berkowitz suggests that the media play a clearly causative role in fos¬
tering real-world aggression. But they do implicate film and television
in the mix of social factors that incline individuals to behave violently.
In light of the empirical work on media effects, the fascination with
violence in popular culture should give us pause. It is an attribute
unlikely to be conducive to the general well-being or social health of
our society.
i
NOTES
3. Mass Media Hearings, vol. 9A: A Report to the National Commission on the
Causes and Prevention of Violence (Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office,
1969), p. 193.
4. "'Brutal Films Pale Before Televised Vietnam'—Valenti," Variety, February 21,
1968, p. 2.
5. "Theatre Operators and Public Require Updating on Social Point of View," Va¬
riety, March 6, 1968, p. 7.
6. Quoted in Peter Wood, "How a Film Changes from an 'X' to an 'R,'" New York
Times, July 20, 1980, sec. C. .*■
7. "Pix Must 'Broaden Market,'" Variety, March 20, 1968, p. 1.
8. I examine these factors in Savage Cinema: Sam Peckinpah and the Rise of
Ultraviolent Movies (Austin: University of Texas Press, 1998).
9. "Brutal Films Pale Before Televised Vietnam," p. 2.
10. Gary Crowdus and Richard Porton, "The Importance of a Singular, Guiding Vi¬
sion: An Interview with Arthur Penn," Cineaste 20:2 (Spring 1993), pp. 8-9.
11. Robert J. Landry, "It's Murder, Italian Style," Variety, February 8, 1967, p. 7.
12. Review of The Good, the Bad and the Ugly, Variety, December 27, 1967, p. 6.
13. Bosley Crowther, "Movies to Kill People By," New York Times, July 9, 1967, sec.
2, p. 10.
14. Crowdus and Porton, p. 9.
15. "'Bonnie and Clyde's' Booming Repeats," Variety, February 14, 1968, p. 3.
16. "Hollywood: The Shock of Freedom in Films," Time, December 8, 1967, p. 73.
17. Phil Feldman memo, May 13, 1969, folder no. 46, Sam Peckinpah Collection,
AMPAS Library, Los Angeles.
18. Wild Bunch—Bill Faralla, folder no. 43, letter of March 19, 1968, Sam Peckin¬
pah Collection.
19. Wild Bunch reaction cards, folder no. 65, Sam Peckinpah Collection.
20. I explore Peckinpah's efforts in this regard in Savage Cinema.
21. Carol J. Clover, Men, Women, and Chain Saws: Gender in the Modern Horror
Film (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1992), p. 41.
22. Variety, June 8, 1988, p. 24.
23. Roger Watkins, "'Demented Revenge' Hits World Screens," Variety, October 29,
1980, p. 3.
24. "Chi Tribune Blasts Gory X-Films in R-Rated Clothing," Variety, November 12,
1980, p. 6.
25. Ibid., p. 30.
26. Clover, p. 12.
27. Ruth A. Inglis, "Self-Regulation in Operation," in The American Film Industry,
rev. ed., ed. Tino Balio (Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1985), p. 378.
28. Herbert Blumer, Movies and Conduct (New York: Macmillan, 1933), p. 74.
29. Mark Crispin Miller, "In Defense of Sam Peckinpah," Film Quarterly 28:3
(Spring 1975), p. 13.
30. "Films, Like TV, Lack Research on 'Violence,'" Variety, December 25, 1968, p. 5.
31. Walter Kaufmann, Tragedy and Philosophy (New York: Anchor Books, 1969),
p. 34.
32. Dorothy L. Singer and Jerome L. Singer, "TV Violence: What's All the Fuss
About?," Television and Children 7:2 (1984), p. 30.
33. Leonard Berkowitz, "Some Effects of Thoughts on Anti- and Prosocial Influ¬
ences of Media Events: A Cognitive-Neoassociation Analysis," Psychological Bulletin
95, no. 3(1984), p. 414.
34. Haejung Paik and George Comstock, "The Effects of Television Violence on An¬
tisocial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis," Communication Research, 21, no. 4 (August 1994),
pp. 516-46. For a different view of media effects, see Felson in this volume.
42
Stephen Prince
74. Donna Britt, "Lights, Camera, Sad Reaction," Washington Post, March 15, 1994,
pp. Bl, B5.
75. "Gunman Gets 6 Years for 'Schindler's List' Shooting," Los Angeles Times, Au¬
gust 10, 1994, p. B8.
76. John Bailey, "Bang Bang Bang Bang, Ad Nauseum," American Cinematographer
75:12 (December 1994), p. 26.
77. Manohla Dargis, "Sleeping with Guns," Sight and Sound 7, no. 5 (May 1997),
p. 21.
78. "'Scream' Killer Fingers Film, TV," The Hollywood Reporter, July 25, 1999, on¬
line version.
<
The Historical Context
of Ultraviolence
v
■s
Joseph Morgenstern
Last week this magazine said that Bonnie and Clyde, a tale of two
young bank robbers in the 1930s, turns into a "squalid shoot-'em for
the moron trade" because it does not know what to make of its own
violence. I am sorry to say I consider that review grossly unfair and
regrettably inaccurate. I am sorrier to say I wrote it.
Seeing the film a second time and surrounded by an audience no
more or less moronic than I, but enjoying itself almost to the point of
rapture, I realized that Bonnie and Clyde knows perfectly well what to
make of its violence, and makes a cogent statement with it—that vio¬
lence is not necessarily perpetrated by shambling cavemen or quivering
psychopaths but may also be the casual, easy expression of only slightly
aberrated citizens, of jes' folks.
I had become so surfeited and preoccupied by violence in daily
life that my reaction was as excessive as the stimulus. There are indeed
a few moments in which the gore goes too far, becomes stock shockery
that invites standard revulsion. And yet, precisely because Bonnie and
Clyde combines these gratuitous crudities with scene after scene of
dazzling artistry, precisely because it has the power both to enthrall and
appall, it is an ideal laboratory for the study of violence, a subject in
which we are all matriculating these days.
Violent movies are an inevitable consequence of violent life.
They may also transmit the violence virus, but they do not breed it any
more than the Los Angeles television stations caused Watts to riot. Dis¬
tinctions can and must be made between violent films that pander and
violent films that enlighten, between camp, comment, and utter cyni¬
cism. And there is nothing like the movies for giving us historical per¬
spective on violence we have known, and in many cases loved.
No one but Charlie Chaplin's competitors ever deplored his
From Newsweek, August 28, 1967. © 1967, Newsweek, Inc. All rights reserved. Reprinted
by permission.
47
48
Joseph Morgenstern
Figure 11. Using multiple cameras and slow motion, director Arthur Penn was the first American
filmmaker to capture the spastic agonies of gunshot victims. Bonnie Parker (Faye Dunaway) ex¬
pires in a hail of machine gunfire.
leagues perform poignant and intricate wonders with a Loony Toon gang
of outlaws who bumble along from one bank job to another, from one
blood bath to another, in an inchoate, uncomprehending, and fore¬
doomed attempt to fulfill their stunted selves. Both Bonnie and Clyde
and St. Valentine's Day Massacre deal with the same slice of life, yet the
characters in the latter are gun racks and the characters in Bonnie have
a rushing of blood in their veins and a torrent of thought in their minds.
From time to time, however, all artistry falls by the wayside:
when a cop trying to stop the getaway car is shot in the face, when a
grocer is bludgeoned in a close-up, when'a grenade demolishes a police
tank, when one outlaw has his skull blown open and another has her
eye shot out. These scenes are reprehensible not because they are ugly
or shocking, but because they are familiar, gross, and demeaning. When
artists are able to bring characters to life and keep them alive, they
should not leave death to the special effects department.
There is, in the depiction of violence, a thin red line between
the precisely appropriate and the imprecisely offensive. Sometimes a
few too many frames of film may mean the difference between a shot
50
Joseph Morgenstern
that makes its point concisely and one that lingers slobberingly. These
few frames or scenes in Bonnie and Clyde will hardly change the course
of human events. When we talk about movies, we are not talking about
urban renewal programs, nuclear nonproliferation treaties, or rat con¬
trol bills. Art cannot dictate to life and movies cannot transform life,
unless we want to retool the entire industry for the production of prop¬
aganda. But art can certainly reflect life, clarify and improve life; and
since most of humanity teeters on the edge of violence every day, there
is no earthly reason why art should not turn violence to its own good
ends, showing us what we do and why. The clear danger, of course, is
that violence begets violence in life and engenders confusion in art. It
is a potent weapon, but it tends to aim the marksman.
I
Bosley Crowther
v
/
From The New York Times, July 9, 1967. Copyright © 1967 by The New York Times.
Reprinted by permission.
51
52
Bosley Crowther
promised them, they are elevated as heroes by the attitude that is blaz-
ingly assumed by Mr. Aldrich's direction of Nunnally Johnson and
Lukas Heller's script.
What is more, the initial recalcitrance and arrogance of these
brutes is given a tone of social defiance and individuality. The inculca¬
tion in them of a sort of hoodlum esprit de corps is developed as a fea¬
ture of excitement that is obviously meant to stimulate glee. And the
ultimate killing of the Nazis and their shrieking, cowering concubines
is a superfluous amount of shooting and slashing and throwing of fire.
If one could find in the structure of this picture or in the way it
was angled and staged the slightest hint of intentional, sardonic com¬
ment upon the fundamental nature of war—the slightest glimmer of
revelation that all killing is essentially criminal—then the hideous bru¬
tality of it might be regarded as subtle irony, and the glorifying of its
felons as a tragic travesty.
But there are no such hints or glimmers in it. It is a blatant and
obvious appeal to the latent aggressiveness and sadism in undiscrimi¬
nating viewers. And I would guess that the vast majority of the people
who are seeing it at the Capitol and at the 34th Street East, where it is
playing, are taking it for kicks and thrills and are coming away from it
palpitating with a vicarious sense of enjoyment in war.
A similar sense of fun out of killing is being induced, I feel sure,
by Mr. Leone's excessively violent For a Few Dollars More. This is
another of those clever Italian-made Western films, shot in Spain, with
a cast of actors speaking a babel of languages, but with English dialogue
dubbed, and it follows the further adventures of the character created
by Clint Eastwood in Mr. Leone's A Fistful of Dollars.
This gray-eyed, cool-cat gunman is a bounty killer here—that
is, a self-commissioned huntsman whose prey are bandits with prices
on their heads—and he is after a gang of Mexican bank robbers who are
being stalked by another bounty killer, too. So the deadly confronta¬
tions are not only between the huntsman and the men he is out to bring
back dead, but also between the two huntsmen, who clearly have no
feeling for each other's lives.
Having found a market for bloodlust with his previous Dollar
film, Mr. Lepne is out to exploit it for all it's worth with this further
killing spree. He fills the screen with violence, men stalking other men
like animals—with blazing guns and whining bullets and bodies falling
in spasm-twitching heaps. And he makes the deadlines of Mr. East-
53
From The New York Times, July 30, 1967. Copyright © 1967 by The New York Times.
Reprinted by permission.
54
55
Another Smash at Violence
Bosley Crowther
»
Ronald Gold
Crowther’s ‘Bonnie’-
Brook: Rap at Violence
Stirs Brouhaha
New York Times reviewer Bosley Crowther has long been the most
influential critic in America, and during the course of his career he has
been one of the industry's staunchest supporters and defenders. But
almost as often he has been at odds with the Hollywood moguls, some¬
times leading a crusade of his fellow scribes against the current tide of
crass "commercialism."
Lately, Crowther has begun another crusade, against what he
calls a "wave of violent movies." But this time he seems to have taken
on not only the filmmakers, but his fellow critics, the bulk of whom
have pointedly disagreed with his "violent" blasts at two recent pix,
Robert Aldrich's The Dirty Dozen and Arthur Penn's Bonnie and Clyde.
Indeed some of them have gone so far as to suggest that his cru¬
sading spirit has clouded his perception, that he's been unable to sepa¬
rate a film's statement about violence from the thing itself—and,
particularly in the case of Bonnie, that he has hurt the cause of serious
filmmaking in America by shooting down a work of art.
Crowther's basic thesis, as he expressed it in the second of two
Sunday pieces, ironically entitled "A Smash at Violence," is that "By
habituating the public to violence and brutality . . . films of excessive
violence only deaden their sensitivities and make slaughter seem a
meaningless cliche." He added that "such deliberate exploitation of the
public's susceptibilities is poisoning and deadening our fiber and
strength," and he rejected the view that violence in the world is an
excuse for it on the screen. "It is precisely because there are vast areas
of violence and bloodshed in our world . . . that our media of so-called
entertainment should strive for balance and moral truth," he said.
57
58
Ronald Gold
While nobody has come out in favor of violence for its own sake, some
of those who've been arguing with Crowther have thrown back at him
his own arguments in favor of increased on-screen liberality in the area
of sex. Indeed, Moira Walsh, critic for the Jesuit weekly America and
reviewer for the National Catholic Office for Motion Pictures, sounds
like an echo of the Times reviewer himself, when he defended such
films as The Pawnbroker, Who’s Afraid of Virginia Woolf! and Blow-
Up from the onslaughts of the bluenoses.
"When I started looking at movies in a 'quasi-official' capacity,"
Miss Walsh said, "I was thoroughly indoctrinated with the prissy
notion that 'common folk' should be protected from 'bad' and 'danger¬
ous' ideas in films. By slow and painful steps I was disabused of this out¬
look. It simply cannot be squared with any coherent view of the
realities of mid-20th-century life.. .. Without being dogmatic about it,
I tend to the opinion that the hardest thing for a film to do is to change
anyone's point of view; that films reflect the realities of the age far more
than they influence them."
Rather than approaching the subject on this abstract level, most
of Crowther's confreres who've chosen to take up his challenge (usu¬
ally without mentioning him by name) have responded negatively to
his dismissal of work by two highly respected filmmakers as nothing
but crassly commercial trash, essentially "sympathetic" to violence
and devoid of irony.
They've directly contradicted his view and, by implication,
have accused Crowther of totally ignoring the technical felicities of the
films at hand as well as the intentions of the men who made them.
In his review of The Dirty Dozen, for example, and in the Sun¬
day piece which followed it, Crowther was unable to find "the slight¬
est hint of intentional sardonic comment on the fundamental nature of
war," and his overview of the film was that it is "a raw and preposter¬
ous glorification of a group of criminal soldiers" and "a studied indul¬
gence of sadism that is morbid and disgusting beyond words."
Dirty Dozen
None of the other critics ignored the violence in Dirty Dozen, and some
of them were quite unhappy about it too, accusing director and screen-
Figures 12 & 13. For 1960s audiences, one of the most shocking scenes in Bonnie and Clyde oc¬
curs following a bungled holdup, when Clyde shoots a bank official in the face at point blank-range.
The victim hangs for a moment on the side of the car, his face pressed against the shattered glass.
60
Ronald Gold
writers of copping out in the final third of the film and spoiling the
point. But all of them were prepared to see that there was a point—one
which some of them emphasized later when taking up the discussion
of violence.
"This particular film/' said Judith Crist on the "Today" Show,
"very clearly implies that it takes killers and psychopaths to do a suc¬
cessful job in war, which is a murderous and perhaps psychotic pas¬
time." And Miss Walsh said Dozen contained "at least implicitly, the
most uncompromising attack on the 'typical military mind' and expo¬
sition of the insanity and hell of war that I have ever encountered in a
potentially popular 'mass audience' movie."
Accepting the picture on its own terms, most other critics also
avoided Crowther's preoccupation with whether or not the central sit¬
uation of Dozen (allegedly taken from an actual incident) was "a fic¬
tional supposition that is silly and irresponsible." And despite the
Times man's accusation that "a raw and unmitigated campaign of sheer
press-agentry has been trying to put across the notion that Warner
Brothers' Bonnie and Clyde is a faithful representation of the desper¬
ado careers of Clyde Barrow and Bonnie Parker," they stayed away from
the question of historical accuracy in their reviews of this pic too—
most of them suggesting that what Penn had tried to create was a "folk
ballad." (Warners claims it's never said anything different.)
While a number of the Bonnie reviews (including Variety's own)
thought Penn had been unsuccessful in welding comedy to tragedy, all
without exception treated the concept respectfully, and all seemed to
understand what was intended when, in Crowther's words, Penn went
"out of his way to splash the comedy holdups with smears of the vivid
blood."
Some, like Cue's William Wolf, called Bonnie "a major artistic
accomplishment, but even those who turned thumbs down, like New
York Post's Archer Winston, said it had "qualities that can be praised
extravagantly." And none duplicated Crowther's accusations that it had
been "assembled in a helter-skelter fashion"; that its characters were
"ridiculous, camp-tinctured travesties" or that it is "strangely antique,
sentimental claptrap."
Penn’s View
If one accepts the view that Penn had no intention of being either
"antique" or "sentimental" and that both he and Aldrich were trying
61
Crbwther’s ‘Bonnie’-Brook
to make statements against violence, rather than in favor of it, how was
Crowther able to view the films as he did?
One answer may be that he is not only concerned about, but in
a sense shares the reactions of, that portion of the public which may be
besieging the box office in search of kicks rather than illumination, and
is more concerned with what it sees than what is implied. ("But will
the picture do well because of its more probing aspects or because of
the vivid violence with which it is filled?" asks the Saturday Review's
Hollis Alpert about Bonnie and Clyde.)
If this is so, then Crowther is in a better position than most to
express concern about the effect of violent films on the public.
But what some in the trade are worried about is that these con¬
cerns over violence might spark a return to the genuinely "antique"
days of filmmaking, when every "commercial" picture had to make an
explicit statement of its point, rather than relying on the ability of its
audience to see beneath the surface. And these folks have accused
Crowther of making no effort to comprehend what the new breed of
filmmakers is attempting—not only where violence is involved, but in
other areas as well.
Offered as a recent example of what these people are talking
about is Joseph Losey's Accident, which Crowther called "just a sad lit¬
tle story of a wistful don," and virtually everybody else referred to as
"disturbing," "thought-provoking," or "a glacial dissection of human
passion."
"We don't mind if Mr. Crowther doesn't like our picture," said
one distributor this week. "What we're concerned about is that the
Times readers may come away from his reviews with no idea of what
it's supposed to be about."
Statement by Jack Valenti,
MPAA President, before
The National Commission
on the Causes and
Prevention of Violence
As you are aware and as we are, there are many disagreements today on
the subject of violence. People say there is too much violence in the
society and more than there used to be; there is disagreement about
whether there is more violence in movies today than there was.
And there is disagreement among laymen as well as social sci¬
entists about the effect that violence in the media has, particularly on
children.
The problem of violence was one of the principal matters that
occupied my attention when I first became president of this Associa¬
tion. It is only one, I might say, because it's only one part of the human
condition.
But I recognized immediately, from May 1966 when I became
president of the MPAA, that for the filmmaker the treatment of vio¬
lence in scenes and incidents of a story that he is trying to tell really
involves the whole fundamental issue of the responsibility of the artist,
the creative artist, not only toward his art but toward the society in
which he lives.
And I think that as you talk to filmmakers you know that this
issue confronts them almost in everything that they do. The question
that confronts the artist is: How much is too much?
I have said on many occasions that what is important is for the
62
63
Figure 14. In 1968, the Motion Picture Association of America instituted its G-M-R-X ratings for
film content. To satisfy the MPAA and earn an R rather than an X rating, producer Phil Feldman
and editor Lou Lombardo had to abbreviate the throat-cutting scene in The Wild Bunch, delet¬
ing a side-angle view of the blood spurt.
67
Jack Valenti
Jack Valenti
today, for the first time, we are excluding children. We are excluding
children from pictures we think are unsuitable for them. So I would say
we have gone beyond the concept set by that statement.
Mr. Tone: I'm at the moment speaking only of the philosophy
of the 1966 Code. And I recall your statement at the time the Code was
announced in October, which I think is similar to your statement
today: "There is no valid evidence at this time that proves movies have
anything to do with antisocial behavior." Would you not say that that
statement represents some change in viewpoint from the statement of
principles in the 1966 Code? And if it does, I'm interested in how the
motion picture industry's thinking has changed on that subject. What
have the—
Mr. Valenti: Well, the principal change has been that the Asso¬
ciation has a new president. Administrations may change and points of
view—if indeed this is a change in point of view. That is a judgment.
I bear no responsibility for the 1966 Code, any more than the
incoming president who bears responsibility for our nation would bear
total responsibility for carrying forward something that his predecessor
did—as long as it doesn't violate a principle. I truly believe that I have
made the philosophy stronger by bringing it into an active program. To
me this is far more important. It is well to say words, but I believe that
to translate these words into an active program shows progress. That's
why we have taken a philosophy and hardened it into an active program
that keeps children under 16 out of certain movies. That in my judg¬
ment, Mr. Tone, is an advance. That's keeping in step with the chang¬
ing mores and customs of a society and taking that which was first
rooted in philosophy and constructing a living, breathing program
through our new film rating system.
Mr. Tone: One more question before we get to the rating system
specifically. You stated the children who are not disturbed, normal chil¬
dren who come from normal homes, are not likely to be affected by any¬
thing they see. Do you believe that motion picture producers have an
obligation with respect to disturbed children or children who because
of some problem or other could be affected by the amount of violence
they see? Or do you believe that the advantages of creative freedom out¬
weigh any obligation to such a small minority? What are your views?
Mr. Valenti: Mr. Counsel, my answer to that is quite obviously
we can't make pictures at the level of the disturbed child. As Mr. Jus¬
tice Marshall said in his 1968 decision, that would turn movies into a
wasteland. We would make what he called inane movies. And I surely
would agree with that. There is no rational person in the motion pic-
71
ture industry who would even suggest that we should make pictures
aimed at the level of the disturbed child. You simply can't do it. Any
more than you would write all books or portray all of life itself at this
level. . . .
There are some people in America who have no patience or tol¬
erance for the workings of a democratic society, and they would have
the government intrude by law to control motion pictures in the belief
that this is the way to control violence.
As a humble citizen of this land, I don't think there is any delu¬
sion so slippery or any act more perilous than the intrusion of the gov¬
ernment into making such judgments for the communications media.
Of course, I would hope that all thoughtful citizens would be opposed
to that alternative.
The question that needs to be asked is: Can censorship cure the
portrayal of violence in the media? That's the question everybody must
ask. Some people would answer yes. I would have a larger question. I
would ask: Can censorship curb violence in the society? I think it's a
truism that movies are not beacons but rather mirrors of society. They
don't lead; they follow the already established course.
That leads to another question and that is: How much is too
much violence, Senator? How do you determine that?
And, then, the really tough question to be asked is: Who would
make these judgments? Who would appoint them or who would anoint
them? And by what omniscient or divine authority would they claim
accurate judgment?
This is the crux. The more I get involved in even voluntary rat¬
ing of pictures, the more convinced I am of the lunacy and the absurd¬
ity of governmental involvement in making such cultural judgments.
I promise you, ladies and gentlemen, all you have to do is read
two leading critics and you will find one saying, "This is wonderful,"
another saying "It's pornography," and one saying, "This is the great
moral play of our time," another saying, "It's cheap violence."
Who's right? This is the thing that causes me the greatest con¬
cern—knowing we are frail humans and yet we are making these diffi¬
cult, even impossible, judgments. We do the best that we can. What's
important is that it's voluntary.
Senator, I can only state this as my answer to your question
regarding alternatives, which obviously must concern anybody looking
at the problem: what we are doing now is not quite to our liking. What
else can we do? It is a tormented question that is both attractive and
repulsive to different people.
72
Jack Valenti
Figure 15. The graphic violence of Bonnie and Clyde was tied to the social and political turmoil
of the 1960s. Director Arthur Penn, for example, included a visual reference to the death of Presi¬
dent Kennedy. When the Texas Rangers gun down Clyde, a piece of his head (like JFK’s) flies off.
Jack Valenti
>
.
The Aesthetics of
Ultraviolence
John Bailey, ASC
From American Cinematographer, vol. 75, no. 12 (December 1994), pp. 26, 28-29.
Reprinted by permission.
79
80
John Bailey
It may seem I've diverged from the nominal topic of this essay,
but there is for me an emotional "throughline" that remains intact.
And that is pretty simple. As filmmakers, like any artists, we inhabit
our work. Not only do we take it home at night, but often, especially
when we are on location, our location is our home. We form and re-form
ad hoc families and residences that are as real to us as our own spouses
and children. t
Because we live and work in such a violent society, it is natu¬
ral that our films reflect and explore this violence. But often we only
explore it deep enough to wallow in its muck. This is where the ques¬
tion of a demarcation arises. What is exploration and what is exploita¬
tion? And who decides?
I've always had difficulty with this question. It gets to the heart
of the issue of censorship. Because one's intent and point of view are
key factors, it is no easy subject to investigate. But recently I saw two
films that so crystallized the matter that everything started to fall into
place, and I found for myself a clear distillate of the dilemma.
I hope you have seen both Natural Born Killers and The Shaw-
shank Redemption. They are magnificently made films, brilliantly con¬
ceived and photographed. They cast an unflinching look at the strains
of violence in Americans and in our films. At times both films make you
want to avert your eyes. Otherwise, they could not be more different.
The philosophical stand these two films take on the issue of
violence may be fundamentally different. But just as telling and more
germane to this essay is the photography of that violence.
Natural Born Killers is a manic, all-stops-out journey through
the madness and darkness of the American obsession with violence as
acte gratuit, an existential fury that defines all that is alien, angry, and
antilife in our character. Its style is hyperkinetic and frenzied, mixing
media and formats in a non-logical way that is said to have been deter¬
mined at times by the flip of a coin. Its images seem to spin out of con¬
trol, held in place only by an incantatory sound track, pivoting around
the threnodic voice of Leonard Cohen.
The Shawshank Redemption is visually very measured and
controlled, often almost processional. Its character and narrative skeins
are nuanced and defined by circumspection, a tone set by the reflective
timbre of Morgan Freeman's elegiac voice-over narration. The plot
points are neatly resolved, although the ending may seem too attenu¬
ated to some. The depiction and eruption of violence is unflinching but
distanced. None of this makes the film remote. In fact, it draws you in
it and by the end the cumulative power of empathy is heroic.
82
John Bailey
Figure 16. Woody Harrelson as a multiple murderer in Natural Born Killers. The low-angle cam¬
era and the character’s dramatically extended arm manifest the film’s fascination with the lurid
violence it purports to satirize and critique. It is very difficult for filmmakers to resist the attrac¬
tions of aestheticizing violence.
83
critical or moral perspective but because that's where the action is. The
defining aesthetic is MTV. And if it is meant to be otherwise, if it is
meant to be a de facto critique or satire of the American and media
obsession with violence, I can only say that on this level it is, for me at
least, a complete failure. The filmmaking tools which are wielded so
artfully and with such panache distort the putative intent. The film
eroticizes violence, wallows in it, and struggles to incite the viewer.
If you are an impressionable child, an angry adolescent, or an
alienated sociopath, you will probably leave Natural Born Killers feel¬
ing jacked up. But if you are a rational adult you will feel drained, sullen,
enervated. You leave The Shawshank Redemption feeling renewed,
buoyant, sanguine. Both films spend a good part of their screen time
inside the confines of a prison. Your experience of that space could not
be more polarized by the two films.
The space and light of Natural Born Killers is that of a Sadean
stage set replete with Grand Guignol effects, the whole grotesquerie
awash in blazingly assaultive light and aggressive camera motion. The
space and light of Shawshank Redemption is, like its narrative, almost
classical. Roger Deakins and Frank Darabont brought close scrutiny
and a sense of the importance of detail to their work. You have to watch
this film closely. You have to live in its images. They don't beat you
over the head. And because your head is left intact, you experience the
film, not just watch it.
The restraint of Bresson's camera in A Man Escaped has no
inherent moral force superior to the first-reel frenzy of Truffaut's Jules
and Jim. Nor is The Shawshank Redemption with its formal pictori-
alism superior to the visual disjunction of Natural Born Killers. My
point is not to negate one style of filmmaking in favor of another.
Nor do I wish to anathematize in toto the efficacy of violence
in the arts. From the Greek tragedies, Jacobean drama, French theater
of the Revolution, German Sturm und Drang, to Antonin Artaud and
Peter Brook, the use of violence in theater has been the vehicle for prob¬
ing the complexities and dysfunctions of the human condition and the
means to catharsis and growth. Violence is as endemic to the human
soul as is love. Both emotions are interwoven with the fear of and
inevitability of death. Experiencing, violence and death in our art is a
very real way of affirming our life.
But we must know that there are dangerous shoals when we set
out to conceptualize and visualize violence, especially in film. A cam¬
era may be a mechanical recording device but the eye behind it is artful
84
John Bailey
and intentional—always. Storyboards are not comic strips. They are blue¬
prints for complex and powerful images. If "actions have consequences,"
so do images. We create images. We are conjurers. And, like it or not, we
are teachers. Our images are more haunting and more influential than any
of the words that will ever be written about them. If you can deny that
fact, you can deny the movie memories of your own childhood.
Several years ago during pre-production of China Moon I had to
deal with these issues as a director. There is a murder in the film: Made¬
line Stowe fires a handgun at close range into the head of her husband,
Charles Dance. So, in the interest of responsible research, I found
myself one afternoon on the police firing range in Lakeland, Florida,
9 mm semiautomatic pistol at the ready. I had never fired a handgun. I
felt I needed to experience the sensation. The emotional shock of the
first discharge far exceeded its ballistic "kick." The sense of power was
instant and terrifying. I understood. That same week, also for research,
I subjected myself to viewing the full autopsy of a local murder victim.
I had never seen a human body rendered into parts. The sadness of this
needless death and of my own empathic mortality haunted me for days.
I decided to film in a realistic way the scripted autopsy of the
woman found murdered in her kitchen at the beginning of the film. I
also filmed in detail Ed Harris's forensic investigation of the murder vic¬
tim, with emphasis on the "bonding" between detective and deceased.
Joe Laude, a homicide detective who served as our tech adviser, said
that even in death the murder victim often "speaks" to him. And I pho¬
tographed Charles Dance's murder as simply and as directly as I could.
The second gunshot, the one to his head, was rigged with an elaborate
prosthetic to cause a stream of blood to pulse out under pressure, just
as I'd seen in documentary footage. It was graphic and shocking.
And it is not in the finished film. The close scrutiny of the
autopsy, the even closer, humanizing look of the murder victim as Har¬
ris stares into her open eyes, and the shot in the head of Charles Dance
all made the preview audience uncomfortable and was reported to have
pulled down the survey scores. Some of the offending material quickly
went onto the editing room floor,- more fell victim to strategic com¬
promises. At first, I felt completely co-opted, but then my own anxiety
made me v,ery tentative and uncertain about the choices I had made in
being so direct—that is, so "uncinematic"—in the depiction of murder
and its aftermath.
But the ensuing time has convinced me that it was correct to
deglamorize the violence and to focus on its human consequences. In
85
The seven screenwriters are decent men and women. Four of their chil¬
dren have been in Non-Violence Awareness programs. They are all
devout in the faith that there are too many guns in America. Not to
mention greater Los Angeles. They have written letters to senators urg¬
ing resistance to National Rifle Association pressures. But they all have
a problem with this script.
“What do we do with Arthur?"
“Arthur's a loose end."
“He did love Dolores."
“And he was useful in our second act bridge."
“Arthur was the second act bridge. Now he's spare."
“We kill him?"
“We have to. We leave him around the audience is wondering.
How?"
“Arthur could get ill."
"Illness is a year, it's doctors."
“It's got to be quick. We're over two hours already."
“Suppose Roger shoots him? In a fit of hitherto repressed
anger?"
“Love it. It makes Roger stronger,which is good for Angie."
“And it gets Roger put away, too."
“Ground clearing."
“We do it as a sudden epiphany. Ten-second scene."
“Bang bang, Arthur."
From Film Comment, vol. 29, no. 5, Sept.-Oct. 1993. Reprinted by permission.
86
87
Gittes pulls the car door open and Evelyn falls out. Her face is cov¬
ered with blood. She is dead, shot through the back of the head,
coming out through her left eye.
—Robert Towne, Chinatown, the screenplay
• Luca Brasi, one hand pinned to the bar counter with a knife, stran¬
gulation inflating his horrified dignity
• Paulie, the treacherous driver, plugged in the back of the head in a
parked car, all in serene longshot, while Clemenza takes an easeful
leak in the wheatfield
• Sollozzo, shot in the forehead so that an odd Indian castemark puts a
dent in his pasta
• Police Chief McCloskey, shot in the throat and the forehead; these
two men are left with their tipped-over table in the small Italian
restaurant like lovers' clothes thrown off in haste—there is some¬
thing orgasmic in this double murder, it's Michael getting laid
• Sonny, at the toll booth, with 612 bullets (you count them)
• Apollonia, eager to drive, blown up in the car, her persimmon breasts
tossed to either end of the garden
• Vito Corleone, while playing in the tomato plants on a hot day with
his grandson
• two Tattaglia brothers shotgunned in an elevator
• Moe Greene, having just put on his glasses, so blood can creep
through a cracked lens
• a man in a revolving door
• a man and a woman astonished in bed, naked, and then quickly dead
• Barzini and Barzini's man, Barzini only after he has run to the top of
some steps so that he may topple down again
• Tessio: we don't see Tessio's death, only his stooped figure being
taken away and Abe Vigoda's dreadful glance
88
David Thomson
• and Carlo, Connie's husband, throttled in a car, his frantic feet kicking
out the windscreen so that we can see he didn't even take care of his
shoes
land that would become the Red River D ranch, but so much more lib¬
erating to be Don Diego's man who is tumbled from the saddle by
Wayne's shot. Or Edmond O'Brien running through the streets of
D.O.A., clutching his bright stomach, loaded with irreversible poison,
running for his death. Or Elisha Cook Jr. in Shane, lifted off his feet and
thrown back in the mud by Jack Palance's guns. Cagney in White Heat,
seeming to go up like a rocket. There was a blooming in these deaths,
a jete, reaching for beauty. We grazed our knees and dirtied our clothes
doing death falls. But we glowed in their rapture.
As time passed, I guessed some actors had the same fun and
release in extravagant death scenes. Cagney and Edward G. Robinson
were treasured for those cadenzas in which they occupied time and
space with their delirious dance of death. "Is this the end of Rico?"
Robinson asked in Little Caesar. Well, eventually. Was there anything
in movies so much in love with life as these whirling expirations? Slum
kids could at least look forward to the brilliant strut and fret of passing.
Cagney's stricken grace leaned toward the fits and fevers of
approaching rigor mortis. "Made it, Ma!" in White Heat was only a vet¬
eran's fond tribute to the death throes from Public Enemy, He Was Her
Man, Angels with Dirty Faces (one of the first "showtime" deaths, proof
Figure 17. Ever the dynamo, James Cagney dispensed death while staying in constant motion. In
White Heat, in one fluid move Cagney eats his lunch (a chicken leg) and ‘‘ventilates’’ a victim
stashed in a car trunk.
90
David Thomson
that acting can get you over that hump), and The Roaring Twenties.
Cagney yearned to throw himself about, to smell extinction, tousle his
hair, and let his eyes see oblivion. He expanded on death scenes, he grew
lithe and poetical. It was only then that he could disclose his passion
for movement and his love of the precarious. Death gave this would-be
real-life radical his greatest sense of insurrection.
If you doubt the nihilistic elan in Cagney's demises, then look
at how bitterly Bogart went to his deaths. He never liked losing control.
In such predicaments, we feel the truth in Louise Brooks's observation
that "Humphrey" was a socially correct young man who liked to keep
his cool and his distance. Bogart tensed up when he had to die: his body
often folded in on itself, like hired evening dress packed in a suitcase.
If he had to "act," in death, it was his worst acting. In The Roaring
Twenties Bogart is intimidated, whipped, and mocked by Cagney, as if
he were Liston being taught psychic danger by Cassius Clay. It's notable
that in High Sierra Bogart's Roy Earle dies in extreme longshot, with¬
out benefit of triumphant staggering against the skyline. High Sierra
was Raoul Walsh (who made White Heat), but Sierra is drugged by Bog¬
art's depressive reticence. Bogart's glory learned a lot from High Sierra:
it developed a smoldering, still fatalism, the chance for a few wry words
before conclusions he had foreseen. But Cagney's death hound was
always lit up by the surprise—the discovery!—of bullets.
We know of actors who held a nearly contractual right not to
perish in fiction. To stay the hero, Wayne, Gary Cooper, and, more
recently, Clint Eastwood walked up and down in the shadow of death,
yet kept a beacon keylight on their ever more haggard faces. As one of
those not swept away by Unforgiven, I note the greater historical and
artistic plausibility if William Munny had died on his mission, rather
than revert to that reassuring bringer of death. The film might then con¬
clude with a simple scene of the forsaken Munny children, dying in
their cabin from cholera or loneliness. But the Munny who had lost
such edge and youth did pick up quickness at the end. Then he rode
home in a grim spirit, condemning all the fates and movie conventions
that had made him be lethal.
The comfort in Munny's regaining deadly impact surely ex¬
tends to Eastwood himself. No matter how far this moviemaker has
poked and prodded old genres, no matter his candor with age and fatigue
lines, he cannot do without looking good and potent. He is—and he
knows it—the last classic star. Thus, allegedly, he spent time research¬
ing Secret Service agentry for In the Line of Fire, but still indulged the
cockamamy of a 60-year-old jogging along beside the limo and the sen-
91
Death and Its Details
David Thomson
for surely, one day, he will have to eliminate every family member and
anyone who knows.
It's during the grease-quiet, digestive mechanics of films like
The Godfather that we may recall how frequently film has appealed to
fascists. Not that I mean to suggest some directors are readier for the
jackboot than others. No, the dilemma is tougher for those of us who
love film: something essential to the medium cleaves to uncorrected
powers, the magic of plot (or organization), and the chance to stare at
death without honoring pain or loss. Still, for the moment let's pull
back from that comprehensive unease and offer an intriguing dis¬
claimer—roughly, that these corpses don't smell, none of these guys
were ever "alive" anyway. They're 'toons, for crying out loud!
There is something childlike in the easy dispatch of so many
people as quick as a wipe. My 4-year-old takes as much delight in hurl¬
ing himself to many deaths, and in inflicting them with pointed finger
and inner-mouth explosions, as ever I did. But did children have this
game, or its risk, before moving pictures? Perhaps boys keep the game
alive—and most things that depend on boyishness are becoming harder
to sustain. Yet there are plenty of decent film critics, some of them
women, who seem untroubled by the extended boyhood of, say, Sam
Peckinpah and the very cinematic motto, "Kill anything that moves"—
the line that introduces Peckinpah's credit on The Wild Bunch.
The deaths are easier to face when one gets in the habit of
knowing movie deaths are akin to a bucket of dip cleaning out the
premises. Dip, you may remember, is the green fluid that brings death
or erasure to all 'toons in Who Framed Roger Rabbit. It works in the
way water did on the Wicked Witch of the West in The Wizard of Oz—
Christopher Lloyd's judge dies with the same cry of frustration, "What
a world!", that Margaret Hamilton uttered in Oz.
Lloyd's judge actually dies twice. He is first flattened out by a
steamroller: this is what reveals his secret looniness, although it is a
cunning conceit of the Zemeckis film to say some characters are flat¬
ter than others in movie's two-dimensional illusion. Then the judge
reinflates his own balloon and comes on wicked again—with gestures
from The Texas Chainsaw Massacre—before dip takes him back to
primordial ooze. He is too good a villain for one death. Zemeckis is so
wantonly inventive in Roger Rabbit (and so inspired by other films), I
could believe in the judge bouncing back as often as the cat in Tom and
Jerry cartoons.
Did that cat ever die? Or were his lives infinite? He was reduced
to fragments, blown to smithereens, electrocuted, pancaked—you
94
David Thomson
Figure 18. Martin Scorsese’s films are often full of harsh, flamboyantly rendered physical violence.
In Cape Fear, the villain Max Cady (Robert De Niro) is subjected to an extended beating
sequence, one of several mutilating ordeals the character undergoes.
think of a way to go, Hanna-Barbera did it—and always there was the
swift fade-out, fade-in, and puss was back again ready for worse. He was
a character out of Bunuel. When killing is so easy and such fun, and
death so brief, it becomes a way of life. And not just for card-carrying
boons. We know how reluctant the film business is to let its most vital
killers take retirement. At the end of Halloween, the demonstrably
deceased Michael vanishes, so that he could return for sequels. As I
tried to puzzle out why Scorsese had made Cape Fear, I noticed the
clearly posed cue for Max Cady to grab back once more from the river,
just as Carrie came out of the black earth of her own grave. The moment
passed; it was presumably just a joke about such tricks. But Cady could
have escaped—so burned, so crushed, and so drowned that he would be
the harder to recognize next time.
With death so climatic or constitutional, ghastliness becomes
a subject for movies. Our watching from the dark, intensely "with" the
images yet powerless to intervene in their progress, is a model for sto¬
ries in which those left alive may keep some kind of community with
lost ones. After all, the thrust of movies is so much more imaginary
than actual. So there have been films in which ghosts come back, or the
living make a journey to the realm of the dead. From Nosferatu and A
Matter of Life and Death to Beatty's Heaven Can Wait and Ghost,
95
Death and Its Details
movies have played with the undead (without having to hire other than
the regular actors). Some of those films have resorted to "ghostly" spe¬
cial effects, superimposition, and so on. Yet, truly, no tricks are re¬
quired. No one on screen has a real life or corporeality. The films keep
playing long after the actors die. These are 'toons reread as appealing
solids by our fond credulity.
Ghost hints at a way movies- might—on the scent of Shirley
MacLaine and Marianne Williamson—burrow into the self-help of pro¬
jection. In several recent movies, there are wishful thinkings beyond
the grave, psychic schmoozings: in Field of Dreams, Kevin Costner has
the chance to meet his dead father again, so that the load of misunder¬
standing can be tolled away. That's not what I want an art form to offer.
But that won't deter the development, and who knows if movie isn't
less an art than just one of those fun boxes the Good Guys offer. We
may not be far from a household video facility that could take all the
hours of home movie of a loved one and then put that passed-away per¬
son into computer regeneration, so that those left behind have a house
guest/ghost to chat with.
Already, TV commercials (today's pioneering) have worked this
magic with the look and sound of dead stars. And there are rumors of
Jurassic Parks in northern California where that readiness is poised for
new feature films—with Bogart and Louise Brooks together again at
last—if only the legal details can be worked out. The one interest in the
actual Jurassic Park was that such ghosts shared a frame with the very
pale humans Spielberg had time for. Most of the time, the seams didn't
show. But suppose next that we could resurrect and write dialogue for
Elvis and Marilyn? Or you could have your own home video tete-a-tete
with the star/celebrity of your choice—the star as ultimate pet.
The bullet goes in, and life goes out. Movie prefers it as an
instant, switchlike adjustment, without suffering or waiting. Yet so
much of death is in those two grim departments. How do we stand up
for the very few deaths, and corpses, we may meet in our lives? They
are unscripted, no matter the anticipation; and they are not there for
slo-mo analysis. Are there moments in films when we have a better
than bang-bang understanding of what cessation means for the passer-
on and those left behind? I can think; of film deaths that move me, or
give me a sharper sense of the precariousness of life. In every case I'm
going to list, somehow, life remains the subject:
• the deaths in The Missouri Breaks are epic and lugubrious, undigni¬
fied yet very skilled, but the best is Lee Clayton's, asleep after his
96
David Thomson
horse pissed during his love song, then awake to the snap of bracken
or sinews, his own vain efforts to breathe or stay calm at Tom Logan's
dry inquiry, "You know what woke you up? Lee, you just had your
throat cut."
• in Pat Garrett and Billy the Kid, Slim Pickens realizes he has a death
wound while James Coburn is still fighting L. Q. Jones. But the music
rises for his death, and leads him down to the river (Pickens has been
building a boat to "drift out of this damn territory"). His woman,
Katy Jurado, follows him and they gaze at each other by the water as
he holds his startled face and his belly wound up to the evening light.
And then we see that Coburn (Garrett) is watching the death and tak¬
ing it in as evidence and responsibility. For he had hired Pickens away
from the boat-building. And then there will be Garrett camped by
another river, half an hour and months later in the film, as a vagrant
family drifts by on a raft.
• in Renoir's The River, the little boy Bogey is entranced by the cobra
at the end of the garden. He is warned. But he wants to charm the
snake, and one afternoon he goes too close. We never see the cobra
strike. There is just the boy's sprawled body. The loss comes not long
after a siesta in which the several members of the household are seen
sleeping, with the camera simmering on their breath. The boy dies,
a new child is born. This easily sounds trite as a philosophy, but
Renoir's structure, his camera, and the sense of breathing transform
the quietism so that it becomes as steady and flowing as the river and
the flooding sitar music.
• the moment when Isabelle dies in Amber sons, with the Major gazing
into the fire, speaking of the sun as the source of life—Richard Ben¬
nett rambling very near his own death—and then the fateful word,
the Major jerked out of his revery, ready to die, and the rapacious
embrace that Fanny has for George. Family in a few seconds
• and the death of Tom Joslin in Silverlake Life, the documentary made
by Joslin and his lover, Mark Massi, as they both faced the destiny of
AIDS. In this case, it is the entire film, the ending of which never
enjoys doubt. It takes a movie like this to remind us how gradually
and faithfully death comes. Bodies diminish and waste, the lesions of
sarcoma spread and join; courage and tact fail. Joslin sometimes rants
out of fear and horror at what is happening to him. Months and years
come down to ninety minutes or so of film, and we see Joslin seconds
after he has died—a skull with skin, yet freed or deserted by life and
the illness. We see so few authentic deaths on film, Silverlake Life
can put you off movies.
97
I saw Silverlake Life on June 15 on PBS. Two days later I flew to Eng¬
land for my father's funeral. I was reading Philip Larkin on the plane
because, for many Englishmen, Larkin has been like a life-sustaining
illness: he had a sensibility I loathed, and a capacity with words that
was piercing. And now, eight years after his death, Larkin is being
revealed—in Selected Letters and a biography by Andrew Motion—as a
furtive, less than honest man, darker and more afraid than he could
admit. I felt there was some kind of kinship between Larkin and my
father. My father left my mother when I was being born. He lived with
another woman for over forty-five years. But he came home to us at
weekends and Christmases, and he never once said anything about the
double life. This is Larkin in Aubade (he never sold the movie rights):
that he and I would ever talk about our history. You see, I had never quite
been able to make him tell me; and instead there were ways in which I
had imitated him. He was cremated and I came back to America.
Kieran had his operation. It was a complete success. The news
from friends in Dublin could not have been more positive. Two weeks
after the operation he was to be moved from the hospital to a nursing
home for further convalescence. But in a matter of hours he developed
a pulmonary embolism and died. The day after he died I got a card from
him, written from the hospital —"My improvement is marked." You
have to believe me: he would have chuckled and said, "Oh, dear, yes,"
for he loved irony.
The limit to death in most of our films is that it shows what
poor attention they pay to life.
I
Devin McKinney
For many of us, our earliest and most lasting moviegoing memories
involve acts of violence. Whereas an older generation was marked by the
murder of Bambi's mother, my epiphany came with seeing Taxi Driver
at the age of 12 and being disturbed nearly to the point of physical sick¬
ness by its violence. But more intriguing to me now is the nightmare I
awoke with two days later. Somewhere I realized that I had responded
so viscerally not only because the violence felt physically real but
because it was emotionally and morally complex: it brought up ambiva¬
lences and dreads that no amount of rationalization could overcome.
Time swings like a pendulum, and violence is once again an
issue. Over the past few years a new ethos of violence has been accru¬
ing in the commercial cinema; directors have been attempting to take
it further—but not necessarily deeper. More than ever, violence has
emerged as thematic matter, the true meat even of movies that claim
to be about something else. This has given a sharper edge to both
“strong" and “weak" (as I'll call them) portrayals of violence, since both
must compete in a media marketplace that is ever more vicious and in
a social context that is ever more apathetic.
Of all that films contain, violence may be the most resistant to
quantification. Only at its weakest does it yield to patterns and pre¬
dictabilities. (Robin Wood conceded as much in his theory of the "inco¬
herent text," a theory whose unvoiced admission is that many of the
most violent and interesting films are so multilayered and resistant to
singular meaning that their systematization can be built only on con¬
tradictions.) But perhaps some generalities can be discerned. Perhaps
movie violence must contain multitudinous meanings if it hopes to
99
100
Devin McKinney
Figure 19. In Goodfellas Tommy (Joe Pesci) is executed with a bullet to the head and a flam¬
boyant blood spray. Note how director Martin Scorsese overtly plays this violence to the camera.
The characters face the camera, and the composition gives the viewer the ideal vantage from which
to watch the violence.
Figure 20. The notorious ear-cutting scene in Reservoir Dogs is a moment of gratuitous screen
violence. Failing to connect with any previous material in the film, it performs no narrative func¬
tion. But it does offer a striking visual showcase for ultraviolence, in which the bloody effect
becomes its own end and purpose.
106
Devin McKinney
1 . X III- .--- - OO L L
ing approach to ultraviolence. Schwarzenegger’s heroes usually concluded their mayhem with a
quip that took the bloodshed in the direction of comedy and “weak” violence.
108
Devin McKinney
Violence and death do not only sell movie tickets today; they also are
the source of countless arguments, safely confined to paper, to panel
discussions, to cocktail parties. What is all that technicolor blood doing
to our youth? Are we a nation of voyeurs? Can the movies make us cal¬
lous, unfeeling? Are the films innocent, merely presenting to us some¬
thing which is already there? Is today's movie violence reflective of
some phenomenon presently existent in our society (good, "honest,"
documentary revelation like The French Connection) or is it teaching
us to regard blood and death with a blase aplomb we would not other¬
wise acquire in our own insular lifetimes? Answers to these questions
often boil down to whether one believes that cinema is reflective or
affective, mimetic or cathartic. At any rate, issues such as these have
been worried about and nosed over until one's response becomes, at
best, gentle boredom and, at worst, calculated oversight.
Violence and death have always been with us in the darkness of
the theater. They were there before I became a regular moviegoer in the
late 1940s and will be there long after I have bloodlessly expired in bed
or been juicily run down by an aggressive taxicab. They've always been
there, as familiar as the smell of popcorn, and yet the violent deaths I
remember from long ago—the deaths which have stuck to my ribs, so
to speak—are rare. It certainly was not because I was shielded from the
horrors of the screen. The first film I ever saw was Bambi and his
mother's death before the forest fire was a movie experience I'll never
From Journal of Popular Film 3 (Winter 1974), pp. 2-14. Copyright © 1974 Heldref Pub¬
lications. Reprinted with permission.
Afterword copyright © 1999 by Vivian Sobchack.
110
Ill
The Violent Dance: A Personal Memoir of Death in the Movies
the screen—it oozed rather than spurted, it was most often black or a
rusty Cine-color. Even in medium close-up (the man-to-man shoot-out
or duel, the pink traceries of a whiplashing), I can't remember a single
movie death which fired my imagination as much as the bright red
blood from my own finger. Then as now, we humans attempted to hide
from the frightening reality of our fragile innards by believing in the
strength of plastic and supermarkets. kYet we were fascinated, as we
have always been, by blood and tissue and bone. Snowden's secret
(Catch-22) is everyone's secret. In the late 1940s, however, and in the
1950s and early 1960s it was a bit easier to keep that secret; everyone
was comfortably mum.
Then, suddenly it seemed, in the mid-1960s, there was blood
everywhere. We didn't have to go to war to find it—or stretch our necks
to peer at it on the highway. Blood appeared in living color in more and
more of our living rooms. And it was there all around us in the streets,
is still there. Politicians became surprisingly more than caricatures,-
they became mortal. People who looked and lived exactly as we did shot
at us from water towers, slit our throats, went berserk, committed
murder next door. Even children died and supermarkets—meats neatly
packaged and displayed to exorcise the taint of the slaughterhouse—
were no longer havens of safety and sanity and civilization. No place,
however ordinary, was safe; blood ran in busy streets, on university
campuses, in broad daylight, everywhere. We were all threatened and
terrified, all potential victims of our not so solid flesh and some
unknown madman's whimsy.
Public figures splattered against our consciousness: John Ken¬
nedy, Martin Luther King Jr., Malcolm X, Robert Kennedy, Governor
Wallace. Little men with strange other-worldly lights in their eyes took
up arms against anonymity: Charles Whitman, Richard Speck, Richard
Hickoclc, Perry Smith, Charles Manson. Students, police, and the
National Guard fought it out on university campuses where the most
burning issue when I went to college had been whether we had the right
to wear Bermuda shorts to class. None of the blood spilt was pictur¬
esque or patriotic. Death by violence became a possibility for all of us
because it lacked sense and meaning much of the time; there was no
drama and catharsis. The blood in our lives had nothing of art or dis¬
tance about it and we all felt personally threatened. Each one of us could
die, each one of us could bleed, each one of us had to consciously
acknowledge the secret which Snowden so shyly concealed in his flak
suit—we all had pink and vulnerable guts.
One can hardly think that someone's careful calculations
114
Vivian C. Sobchack
somewhere in Hollywood read fear in all our faces and saw our hunger
for the seeming security of knowledge, our yearning to find meaning in
the senselessness of random violence. Films which had previously spo¬
ken to our unconscious desires and fears, like Topsy, "just growed."
And in the fear-ridden 1960s and 1970s, it has been no different.
There always is, of course, a first film—the film which tran¬
scends its surface intentions and burns into us some unstated message
with the intensity not of an arc lamp but of a laser. Bonnie and Clyde,
released in 1967, was just such a film. Although it was not the first film
to overtly bathe itself in blood, it was the first one to create an aesthetic,
moral, and psychological furor. Uneven in tone yet brilliantly con¬
ceived, it fired our imaginations not merely because it was a good film,
but because it was the first major film to allow us the luxury of inspect¬
ing what frightened us—the senseless, the unexpected, the bloody. And,
most important, it kindly stylized death for us; it created nobility from
senselessness, it choreographed a dance out of blood and death, it gave
meaning and import to our mortal twitchings.
There has always been violence and death in the cinema. But the
cinematic phenomenon in the films of our decade which is new and sig¬
nificant is the caressing of violence, the loving treatment of it by the
camera. The most violent of deaths today is treated with the slow-
motion lyricism of the old Clairol commercials in which two lovers
glide to embrace each other. The once abrupt drop into nonbeing has
become a balletic free fall. This, of course, is what has incensed those
who fear for the nation's children and the nation's morality. Making
death and violence "beautiful," they suggest, may seduce all of us into
a cheerful acceptance of gore, may whet our aesthetic appetites for more
and more artful and prolonged bloodletting, may even cause us to com¬
mit violence. After all, The Wild Bunch and their victims died so grace¬
fully. Sonny's body was a poem in motion when he was machine-gunned
to death in The Godfather. A Clockwork Orange kept our toes tapping
to the tunes of rape and beatings. The French Connection, while
certainly not musical, possessed a staccato tempo which was brutal
and bloody.
I suppose that what bothers the moralists most is that these
films are selling, are popular, are even critically acclaimed. The idea
that we are a nation of voyeurs is indeed disturbing. Our Wednesday
and Saturday night outings to see blood and gore while we eat buttered
popcorn is disturbing. But then being human has always been disturb¬
ing. People are no more corrupt, no more voyeuristic than they were in
the days of my childhood. But we are certainly more fearful. And it is
115
The Violent Dance: A Personal Memoir of Death in the Movies
this fear, rather than peeping Tom-ism, which has caused violence to
almost literally blossom—like one of Disney's time-lapse flowers—on
today's movie screens.
Fear has made even the most squeamish of us take our hands
from our eyes. We still are afraid of violence and blood and death, but
we are more afraid of the unknown—particularly when it threatens us
personally and immediately. Even those of us who couldn't stand the
sight of blood at one time find our desire to know and understand blood
stronger than our desire not to see what frightens and sickens us. In a
time when we seemed safe, not immediately threatened, we could
ignore our fear and indulge our squeamishness. Today, this is hardly
possible.
After many viewings of the film, I still cannot watch the scene
in Dali and Bunuel's Un Chien Andalou in which a woman's eyeball is
slit by a razor. I have read about it, heard about it, but I have not seen
it. I cannot keep my eyes open; I have tried, but I physically and men¬
tally cannot do it. The content of that scene is violent and bloody
(although in black and white), and I am deeply afraid to watch it, to
know it. On the other hand, I sat motionless and wide-eyed through
Straw Dogs; my eyes refused to leave the screen for even a moment. I
can remember my husband wanting to leave in the middle (he said his
arm had fallen asleep and we had already watched The French Con¬
nection). I didn't want to go; I didn't want to miss anything. If that
sounds voyeuristic, in essence it wasn't. I got no pleasure at all out of
watching Straw Dogs. I felt extraordinarily tense, upset, sick. And yet
I could not leave the theater until the film was over, even at the expense
of a family argument. For some reason, it seemed to be a matter of life
and death—mine—that I stay.
What is the difference between my responses to the two films?
Both play upon hidden fear in the audience. Both have moments of
extreme violence. Why should I be able to watch one, but not the other?
The answer lies, I think, in the qualitative nature of the violence
involved. I don't have the pressing need to see a woman's eyeball slit by
a razor; seeing it will do nothing but disturb me. And this particular vio¬
lent action—although terrifying with or without its Freudian implica¬
tions—seems to have little to do with my life as I live it every day. I am
not afraid of someone's slitting my eyeball while I passively submit.
Watching that scene, in other words, is not going to instruct me; it is
not going to reveal to me something that is terrible, but which I need
to know. The nature of the violence in Straw Dogs is different. It may
not be treated surrealistically, but it is not totally realistic either. (One
116
Vivian C. Sobchack
Figure 22. David Sumner (Dustin Hoffman) defends his home in the climax of Sam Peckinpah’s
treatise on violence, Straw Dogs.
remembers the little tailor in the fairy tale who wore a sash that pro¬
claimed "Seven at One Blow.") Yet the violence in Straw Dogs touches
contemporary nerves in a way that Un Chien Andalou doesn't. It
involves the kind of violence that one fears now, today. Sickened, ter¬
rified, I had to watch the film. I had to learn and know what I fear and,
however painful the experience was, for the moment I found a certain
security in the fact that I had not backed away from instruction. In
short, I was doing my homework—trying to learn how to survive. David
in that movie was much like myself, the people around me. We all just
wanted to mind our own business and yet found ourselves, our homes,
our lives, threatened by people and things which plainly didn't make
sense, weren't at all rational.
Popular films have always given us—the audience—what we
want; otherwise they would not be popular. Today (just recently
twenty-seven bodies were unearthed in Texas), we want to know blood
and death. Although we retain little of the optimism which sprang from
the Age of Reason, we still believe, even if it's half-heartedly and hope¬
lessly, in knowledge, enlightenment. Knowledge is the magic which
will save ys; cataloguing will restore our crumbling sanity; inspection
will cure our anxiety. Blood and tissue, death and killing, rape and beat¬
ing don't please us, don't titillate us, can't be glibly compared to the cen¬
terfold of Playboy. Yet we have the clear and present need to know
them, to have them made significant rather than senseless, to have
them dramatized. Hence the slow motion, the lingering look at physi-
117
The Violent Dance: A Personal Memoir of Death in the Movies
only briefly, that even the most senseless and violent and horrible of
deaths has at least a form, an internal order, and therefore a meaning.
Those bloody and brutal films which appear on our theatre screens
today perform, for us all, a kindness.
Figure 23. In Quentin Tarantino’s Reservoir Dogs, Mr. Orange (Tim Roth) spends the film bleed¬
ing out from a gunshot wound.
became expected—and funny. (Here Scream and its sequel are recent
examples.)
This heightened sense of reflexivity and irony that emerges
from quantities of violence, from "more," is not necessarily progressive
nor does it lead to a "moral" agenda or a critique of violence. (By virtue
of its excesses and its emphasis on quantity and despite his intentions,
Olive Stone's Natural Born Killers is quite ambiguous in this regard.)
Indeed, in its present moment, this heightened reflexivity and irony
merely leads to a heightened sense of representation: that is, care for
the film as experience and text, perhaps, but a lack of any real concern
for the bodies blown away (or up) upon the'screen. In recent "splatter"
films, in Tarantino films like Reservoir Dogs and Pulp Fiction, and in
quite a number of action thrillers, bodies are more carelessly squan¬
dered than carefully stylized. Except, of course, insofar as excess, as
hyperbole, itself constitutes stylization. Thus, most of the violence we
see on the screen today suggests Grand Guignol rather than Jacobean
tragedy. However, in our current cultural moment, tiredly described as
"postmodern" but filled with new forms of violence like "road rage,"
122
Vivian C. Sobchack
Figure 24. John Travolta and Samuel L. Jackson are buddies and professional killers in Pulp Fic¬
tion, which takes a cartoonish approach to graphic violence, using a character’s exploding head
as the basis for an extended comic sketch.
124
Vivian C. Sobchack
125
126
Carol J. Clover
among the segment of the population that forms its erstwhile audience,
than do the legitimate products of the better studios.
Before we turn to the generic particulars, however, let us review
some of the critical and cinematic issues that attend the study of the
sensation genres in general and horror in particular. We take as our
point of departure not a slasher film but Brian de Palma's art-horror film
Body Double (1984). The plot—a man witnesses and after much strug¬
gle solves the mysterious murder of a woman with whom he has
become voyeuristically involved—concerns us less than the three
career levels through which the hero, an actor named Jake, first ascends
and then descends. He aspires initially to legitimate roles (Shake¬
speare), but it becomes clear during the course of a method-acting class
that his range of emotional expression is impaired by an unresolved
childhood fear. For the moment he has taken a job as vampire in a "low-
budget, independent horror film," but even that job is threatened when,
during a scene in which he is to be closed in a coffin and buried, he suf¬
fers an attack of claustrophobia and must leave the set. A plot twist
leads him to the underworld of pornography, where he takes on yet
another role, this time in a skin flick. Here, in the realm of the flesh
with a queen of porn, the sexual roots of Jake's paralysis—fear of the
(female) cavern—are exposed and finally resolved. A new man, he
returns to A Vampire’s Kiss to master the burial scene, and we are to
understand that Shakespeare is the next stop.
The three cinematic categories are thus ranked by degree of
sublimation. On the civilized side of the continuum lie the legitimate
genres,- at the other end, hard on the unconscious, lie the sensation or
"body" genres, horror and pornography, in that order. For De Palma, the
violence of horror reduces to and enacts archaic sexual feelings.
Beneath Jake's emotional paralysis (which emerges in the "high" genre)
lies a death anxiety (which is exposed in the burying-alive of horror),
and beneath that anxiety lies a primitive sexual response (which
emerges, and is resolved, in pornography). The layers of Jake's experi¬
ence accord strikingly, and perhaps not Coincidentally, with Freud's
archaeology of "uncanny" feelings. "To some people," Freud wrote,
"the idea of being buried alive by mistake is the most uncanny thing of
all. And yet psychoanalysis has taught us that this terrifying phantasy
is only a transformation of another phantasy which originally had noth¬
ing terrifying about it at all, but was qualified by a certain lascivious¬
ness—the phantasy, I mean, of intrauterine existence [der Phantasie
vom Leben im Mutterleib}."10 Pornography thus engages directly (in
pleasurable terms) what horror explores at one remove (in painful
128
Carol J. Clover
Figure 25. joe Spinell plays a serial killer in Maniac, which devotes lingering and graphic atten¬
tion to the savagery he inflicts on his victims. The special effects makeup by Tom Savini details
gunshot wounds to the head, garrotings, and scalping. No manner of death was now off-limits for
the commercial cinema.
129
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
the body, our witnessing body. But what we witness is also the body,
another's body, in experience: the body in sex and the body in threat.
The terms flesh film (skin flicks) and meat movies are remarkably apt.
Cinema, it is claimed, owes its particular success in the sensa¬
tion genres (witness the early and swift rise of vampire films) to its
unprecedented ability to manipulate point of view. What written nar¬
rative must announce, film can accomplish silently and instanta¬
neously through cutting. Within the space of seconds, the vampire's
first-person perspective is displaced by third-person or documentary
observation. To these simple shifts can be added the variables of dis¬
tance (from the panorama of the battlefield to the close-up of an eye¬
ball), angle, frame tilt, lighting effects, unsteadiness of image, and so
on—again, all subject to sudden and unannounced manipulation.13 Fri¬
day the 13th (1980) locates the I-camera with the killer in pursuit of a
victim; the camera is hand- held, producing a jerky image, and the frame
includes in-and-out-of-focus foreground objects (trees, bushes, window
frames) behind which the killer (I-camera), is lurking—all accompanied
by the sound of heartbeats and heavy breathing. "The camera moves in
on the screaming, pleading victim, 'looks down' at the knife, and then
plunges it into the chest, ear, or eyeball. Now that's sick."14
Lagging behind practice is a theoretical understanding of effect.
The processes by which a certain image (but not another) filmed in a
certain way (but not another) causes one person's (but not another's)
pulse to race finally remains a mystery—not only to critics and theo¬
rists but even, to judge from interviews and the trial-and-error (and
baldly imitative) quality of the films themselves, by the people who
make the product. The process of suture is sensed to be centrally impor¬
tant in effecting audience identification, though just how and why is
unclear.15 Nor is identification the straightforward notion some critics
take it to be.16 Where commentators by and large agree is in the impor¬
tance of the "play of pronoun function."17 If the fantastic depends for
its effect on an uncertainty of vision, a profusion of perspectives, and a
confusion of subjective and objective, then cinema is preeminently
suited to the fantastic. Indeed, to the extent that film can present
"unreal" combinations of objects and events as "real" through the cam¬
era eye, the "cinematic process itself anight be called fantastic."18 The
"cinefantastic" in any case succeeds, far more efficiently and effec¬
tively and on a far greater scale than its ancestral media, in the pro¬
duction of sensation.
The fact that the cinematic conventions of horror are so easily
130
Carol J. Clover
ory sense of ourselves as tiny and vulnerable in the face of the enormous
Other; but the Other is also finally another part of ourselves, the pro¬
jection of our repressed infantile rage and desire (our blind drive to anni¬
hilate those toward whom we feel anger, to force satisfaction from those
who stimulate us, to wrench food for ourselves if only by actually
devouring those who feed us) that we have had in the name of civiliza¬
tion to repudiate. We are both Red"Riding Hood and the Wolf; the force
of the experience, the horror, comes from "knowing" both sides of the
story—from giving ourselves over to the cinematic play of pronoun
functions. It is no surprise that the first film to which viewers were not
admitted once the theater darkened was Psycho. Whether Hitchcock
actually meant with this measure to intensify the "sleep" experience is
unclear, but the effect both in the short run, in establishing Psycho as
the ultimate thriller, and in the long run, in altering the cinema-going
habits of the nation, is indisputable. In the current understanding, hor¬
ror is the least interruptable of all film genres. That uninterruptability
itself bears witness to the compulsive nature of the stories it tells.
Whatever else it may be, the slasher film is clearly "crucial
enough to pass along." Profits and sequels tell much of the story. Hal¬
loween cost $320,000 to make and within six years had grossed over
$75 million,- even a highly produced film like The Shining has repaid
itself tenfold.23 The Hills Have Eyes (1977), The Texas Chainsaw Mas¬
sacre, and Alien (a science-fiction/slasher hybrid) are [as of 1987] at Part
Two. Psycho and A Nightmare on Elm Street are at Part Three. Hal¬
loween is at Part Four, and Friday the 13th is at Part Six. These are bet¬
ter taken as remakes than sequels; although the subsequent part
purports to take up where the earlier part left off, it in most cases sim¬
ply duplicates with only slight variation the plot and circumstances—
the formula—of its predecessor. Nor do different titles indicate
different plots,- Friday the 13th is set at summer camp and Halloween
in town, but the story is much the same, compulsively repeated in
those ten films and in dozens like them under different names. The
audience for that story is by all accounts largely young and largely
male—most conspicuously groups of boys who cheer the killer on as he
assaults the victims, then reverse their sympathies to cheer the sur¬
vivor on as she assaults the killef.24 Our question, then, has to do with
that particular audience's stake in that particular nightmare, with what
in the story is "crucial" enough to warrant the price of admission and
what the implications are for the current discussion of women and film.
132
Carol J. Clover
Figure 26. Suddenly looming into view, Leatherface claims his first victim in The Texas Chain¬
saw Massacre.
133
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
grandparents, turns out to be right next door to the house of the hitch¬
hiker and his family: his brother Leatherface; their father; an aged and
only marginally alive grandfather; and their dead grandmother and her
dog, whose mummified corpses are ceremonially included in the fam¬
ily gatherings. Three generations of slaughterhouse workers, once
proud of their craft but now displaced by machines, have taken up
killing and cannibalism as a way of life. Their house is grotesquely dec¬
orated with human and animal remains—bones, feathers, hair, skins.
The young people drift apart in their exploration of the abandoned
house and grounds and are picked off one by one by Leatherface and
Hitchhiker. Last is Sally. The others are attacked and killed with dis¬
patch, but Sally must fight for her life, enduring all manner of horrors
through the night. At dawn she manages to escape to the highway,
where she is picked up by a passing trucker.
Likewise, the nutshell plot of Halloween: a psychotic killer
(Michael) stalks a small town on Halloween and kills a string of teenage
friends, one by one; only Laurie survives. The twist here is that Michael
has escaped from the asylum in which he has been incarcerated since
the age of 6, when he killed his sister minutes after she and her
boyfriend parted following an illicit interlude in her parents' bed. That
murder, in flashback, opens the film. It is related entirely in the killer's
first person (I-camera) and only after the fact is the identity of the per¬
petrator revealed. Fifteen years later, Michael escapes his prison and
returns to kill Laurie, whom he construes as another version of his sis¬
ter (a sequel clarifies that she is in fact his younger sister, adopted by
another family at the time of the earlier tragedy). But before Michael
gets to Laurie, he picks off her high school friends: Annie, in a car on
her way to her boyfriend's; Bob, going to the kitchen for a beer after sex
with Lynda; Lynda, talking on the phone with Laurie and waiting for
Bob to come back with the beer. At last only Laurie remains. When she
hears Lynda squeal and then go silent on the phone, she leaves her own
babysitting house to go to Lynda's. Here she discovers the three bodies
and flees, the killer in pursuit. The remainder of the film is devoted to
the back-and-forth struggle between Laurie and Michael. Again and
again he bears down on her, and again and again she either eludes him
(by running, hiding, breaking through windows to escape, locking her¬
self in) or strikes back (once with a knitting needle, once with a hanger).
In the end, Dr. Loomis (Michael's psychiatrist in the asylum) rushes in
and shoots the killer (although not so fatally as to prevent his return in
the sequels).
134
Carol J. Clover
Killer
The psychiatrist at the end of Psycho explains what we had already
guessed from the action: that Norman Bates had introjected his mother,
in life a "clinging, demanding woman," so completely that she consti¬
tuted his other, controlling self. Not Norman but "the mother half of
his mind" killed Marion—had to kill Marion—when he (the Norman
half) found himself aroused by her. The notion of a killer propelled by
psychosexual fury, more particularly a male in gender distress, has
proved a durable one, and the progeny of Norman Bates stalk the genre
up to the present day. Just as Norman wears his mother's clothes dur¬
ing his acts of violence and is thought, by the screen characters and also,
for a while, by the film's spectators to be his mother, so the murderer
in the Psycho imitation Dressed to Kill (1980), a transvestite psychia¬
trist, seems until his unveiling to be a woman,- like Norman, he must
kill women who arouse him sexually. Likewise, in muted form, Hitch¬
hiker/Chop Top and Leatherface in the Texas Chainsaw films: neither
brother shows overt signs of gender confusion, but their cathexis to the
sick family—in which the mother is conspicuously absent but the pre¬
served corpse of the grandmother (answering the treated body of Mrs.
Bates in Psycho) is conspicuously present—has palpably arrested their
development. Both are in their 20s (30s, in the later film), but Hitch¬
hiker/Chop Top seems a gangly kid and Leatherface jiggles in baby fat
behind his butcher's apron. Like Norman Bates, whose bedroom dis¬
plays his childhood toys, Hitchhiker/Chop Top and Leatherface are per¬
manently locked in childhood. Only when Leatherface "discovers" sex
in The Texas Chainsaw Massacre, Part 2 does he lose his appetite for
murder. In Motel Hell (1980), a send-up of modern horror with special
reference to Psycho and The Texas Chainsaw Massacre, we are repeat¬
edly confronted with a portrait of the dead mother, silently presiding
over all manner of cannibalistic and incestuous doings on the part of
her adult children.
No less in the grip of boyhood is the killer in The Eyes of Laura
Mars (1978). The son of a hooker, a hysterical woman gone for days at
136
Carol J. Clover
a time, the killer has up to now put his boyish anger to good use in
police work—the film makes much of the irony—but the sight of
Laura's violent photographs causes it to be unleashed in full force. The
killer in Hell Night (1981) is the sole member of his family to survive,
as a child, a murderous rampage on the part of his father; the experience
condemned him to an afterlife as a murderer himself. In Halloween the
killer is a child, at least in the first instance: Michael, at the age of 6, is
so enraged at his sister (evidently for her sexual relations with her
boyfriend) that he stabs her to death with a kitchen knife. The remain¬
der of the film details his return rampage at the age of 21, and Dr.
Loomis, who has overseen the case in the interim, explains that
although Michael's body has attained maturity, his mind remains
frozen in infantile fury. In It’s Alive (1974), the killer is literally an
infant, evidently made monstrous through intrauterine apprehension
of its parents' ambivalence (early in the pregnancy they considered an
abortion).
Even killers whose childhood is not immediately at issue and
who display no overt gender confusion are often sexually disturbed. The
murderer in A Nightmare on Elm Street is an undead child molester.
The killer in Slumber Party Massacre (1982) says to a young woman he
is about to assault with a power drill: "Pretty. All of you are very pretty.
I love you. Takes a lot of love for a person to do this. You know you want
it. You want it. Yes." When she grasps the psychodynamics of the situ¬
ation in the infamous crotch episode of Texas Chainsaw, Part 2, Stretch
tries a desperate gambit: "You're really good, you really are good," she
repeats; indeed, immediately after ejaculation Leatherface becomes pal¬
pably less interested in his saw. The parodic Motel Hell spells it out.
"His pecker don't work; you see when he takes off his overalls—it's like
a shriveled prune," Bruce says of his killer-brother Vincent when he
learns of Terry's plans to marry him. Terry never does see, for on her
wedding night he attempts (needless to say) not sex but murder. Actual
rape is practically nonexistent in the slasher film, evidently on the
premise— as the crotch episode suggests—that violence and sex are not
concomitants but alternatives, the one as much a substitute for and a
prelude to the other as the teenage horror film is a substitute for and a
prelude to the "adult" film (or the meat movie a substitute for and prel¬
ude to the skin flick).26 When Sally under torture (The Texas Chainsaw
Massacre) cries out, "I'll do anything you want," clearly with sexual
intention, her assailants respond only by mimicking her in gross terms;
she has profoundly misunderstood the psychology.
137
>
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
Female killers are few and their reasons for killing significantly
different from men's. With the possible exception of the murderous
mother in Friday the 13th, they show no gender confusion. Nor is their
motive overtly psychosexual; their anger derives in most cases not from
childhood experience but from specific moments in their adult lives in
which they have been abandoned or cheated on by men (Strait-jacket
[1964], Play Misty for Me [1971], Attack of the 50-Foot Woman [1958]).
(Films like Mother’s Day [1980], Ms. 45 [1980], and I Spit on Your Grave
[1978] belong to the rape-revenge category.) Friday the 13th is some¬
thing of an anomaly. The killer is revealed as a middle-aged woman
whose son, Jason, drowned years earlier as a consequence of negligence
on the part of the camp counselors. The anomaly is not sustained in the
sequels (Parts Two to Six), however. Here the killer is Jason himself, not
dead after all but living in a forest hut. The pattern is a familiar one; his
motive is vengeance for the death of his mother, his excessive attach¬
ment toward whom is manifested in his enshrining of her severed head.
Like Stretch in the crotch episode of Texas Chainsaw, Part 2, the girl
who does final combat with Jason in Friday the 13th Part 2 (1981) sees
the shrine, grasps its significance (she's a psych major), and saves her¬
self by repeating in a commanding tone, "I am your mother, Jason; put
down the knife." Jason, for his part, begins to see his mother in the girl
(I-camera) and obeys her.
In films of the Psycho type (Dressed to Kill, The Eyes of Laura
Mars), the killer is an insider, a man who functions normally in the
action until, at the end, his other self is revealed. The Texas Chainsaw
Massacre and Halloween introduced another sort of killer: one whose
only role is that of killer and one whose identity as such is clear from
the outset. Norman may have a normal half, but these killers have
none. They are emphatic misfits and emphatic outsiders. Michael is an
escapee from a distant asylum,- Jason subsists in the forest; the Sawyer
sons live a bloody subterranean existence outside town. Nor are they
clearly seen. We catch sight of them only in glimpses—few and far
between in the beginning, more frequent toward the end. They are usu¬
ally large, sometimes overweight, and often masked. In short, they may
be recognizably human, but only marginally so, just as they are only
marginally visible—to their victims and to us, the spectators. In one
key aspect, however, the killers are superhuman: their virtual inde¬
structibility. Just as Michael (in Halloween) repeatedly rises from blows
that would stop a lesser man, so Jason (in the Friday the 13th films) sur¬
vives assault after assault to return in sequel after sequel. Chop Top in
138
Carol J. Clover
Terrible Place
The Terrible Place, most often a house or tunnel in which the victims
sooner or later find themselves, is a venerable element of horror. The
Bates mansion is just one in a long list of such places—a list that con¬
tinues, in the modern slasher, with the decaying mansion of The Texas
Chainsaw Massacre, the abandoned and haunted mansion of Hell
Night, the house for sale but unsellable in Halloween (also a point of
departure for such films as Rosemary’s Baby and The Amityville Hor¬
ror [1979]), and so on. What makes these houses terrible is not just their
Victorian decrepitude but the terrible families—murderous, incestu¬
ous, cannibalistic—that occupy them. So the Bates mansion enfolds the
history of a mother and son locked in a sick attachment, and so the
mansion/labyrinth of the Texas Chainsaw movies shelters a lawless
brood presided over by the decaying corpse of the grandmother. Jason's
forest hut (in the Friday the 13th sequels) is no mansion, but it houses
another mummified mother (or at least her head), with all the usual
candles and dreadful paraphernalia. The terrors of the Hell Night man¬
sion stem, we learn, from an early owner's massacre of his children. Into
such houses unwitting victims wander in film after film, and it is the
conventional task of the genre to register in close detail those victims'
dawning understanding, as they survey the visible evidence, of the
human crimes and perversions that have transpired there. That per¬
ception leads directly to the perception of their own immediate peril.
In The Texas Chainsaw Massacre, Part 2, house and tunnel
elide in a residential labyrinth underground, connected to the world
above by channels and culverts. The family is intact, indeed it thrives,
but for reasons evidently having to do with the nature of their sausage
business has moved residence and slaughterhouse underground. For
Stretch, trying desperately to find a way out, it is a ghastly place: dark,
full of blind alleys, walls wet with blood. Likewise the second basement
of the haunted mansion in Hell Night: strewn with decaying bodies and
skeletons, lighted with masses of candles. Other tunnels are less famil¬
ial: the one in Body Double that prompts Jack's claustrophobic faint,
and the horror-house tunnel in He Knows You’re Alone (1980) in which
the killer lurks. The morgue episode in the latter film, certain of the
139
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
Weapons
In the hands of the killer, at least, guns have no place in slasher films.
Victims sometimes avail themselves of firearms, but like telephones,
fire alarms, elevators, doorbells, and car engines, guns fail in the
squeeze. In some basic sense, the emotional terrain of the slasher film
is pretechnological. The preferred weapons of the killer are knives,
hammers, axes, ice picks, hypodermic needles, red-hot pokers, pitch-
forks, and the like. Such implements serve well a plot predicated on
stealth, the unawareness of later victims that the bodies of their friends
are accumulating just yards away. But the use of noisy chainsaws and
power drills and the nonuse of such relatively silent means as bow and
arrow, spear, catapult, and even swords would seem to suggest that
closeness and tactility are also at issue.27 The sense is clearer if we
include marginal examples like Jaws (1975) and The Birds, as well as
related werewolf and vampire genres. Knives and needles, like teeth,
beaks, fangs, and claws, are personal, extensions of the body that bring
attacker and attacked into animalistic embrace.28 In I Spit on Your
Grave, the heroine forces a rapist at gunpoint to drop his pants, evi¬
dently meaning to shoot him in genitals. But she changes her mind,
invites him home for what he all too readily supposes will be a volun¬
tary follow-up of the earlier gang rape. Then, as they sit together in a
140
Carol J. Clover
bubble bath, she castrates him with a knife. If we wondered why she
threw away the pistol, now we know: all phallic symbols are not equal,
and a hands-on knifing answers a hands-on rape in a way that a shoot¬
ing, even a shooting preceded by a humiliation, does not.29
Beyond that, the slasher evinces a fascination with flesh or
meat itself as that which is hidden from view. When the hitchhiker in
The Texas Chainsaw Massacre slits open his hand for the thrill, the
onlookers recoil in horror-all but Franklin, who seems fascinated by the
realization that all that lies between the visible, knowable outside of
the body and its secret insides is one thin membrane, protected only by
a collective taboo against its violation. It is no surprise that the rise of
the slasher film is concomitant with the development of special effects
that let us see with our own eyes the "opened" body.
Victims
Where once there was one victim, Marion Crane, there are now many:
five in The Texas Chainsaw Massacre, four in Halloween, fourteen in
Friday the 13th Part III (1982), and so on. (As Schoell puts it, "Other
filmmakers figured that the only thing better than one beautiful
woman being gruesomely murdered was a whole series of beautiful
women being gruesomely murdered.")30 Where once the victim was an
adult, now she is typically in her teens (hence the term teenie-kill pic).
Where once she was female, now she is both girl and boy, although most
often and most conspicuously girl. For all this, her essential quality
remains the same. Marion is first and foremost a sexual transgressor.
The first scenes show her in a hotel room dressing at the end of a lunch
hour, asking her lover to marry her. It is, of course, her wish to be made
an honest woman that leads her to abscond with $40,000, an act that
leads her to the Bates motel in Fairvale. FFere, just as we watched her
dress in the opening sequences, we now watch her undress. Moments
later, nude in the shower, she dies. A classic publicity poster for Psy¬
cho shows Janet Leigh with a slightly uncomprehending look on her
face sitting on the bed, dressed in a bra and half-slip, looking backward
in such a way as to outline her breasts. If it is the task of promotional
materials to state in one image the essence of a film, those breasts are
what Psycho is all about.
In the slasher film, sexual transgressors of both sexes are sched¬
uled for early destruction. The genre is studded with couples trying to
find a place beyond purview of parents and employers where they can
have sex, and immediately afterward (or during) being killed. The
141
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
are not equal, and the scenes not equally charged; but the fact remains
that in most slasher films after 1978 (following Halloween), men and
boys who go after "wrong" sex also die. This is not the only way males
die,- they also die incidentally, as girls do, when they get in the killer's
way or try to stop him, or when they stray into proscribed territory. The
victims in Hell Night and in the Texas Chainsaw Massacre and Friday
the 13th films are, respectively, those who trespass in Garth Manor,
those who stumble into the environs of the slaughterhouse family, and
those who become counselors at a cursed camp, all without regard to
sex. Boys die, in short, not because they are boys but because they make
mistakes.
Some girls die for the same mistakes. Others, however, and
always the main one, die—plot after plot develops the motive—because
they are female. Just as Norman Bates's oedipal psychosis is such that
only female victims will do, so Michael's sexual anger toward his sis¬
ter (in the Halloween series) drives him to kill her—and after her a
string of sister surrogates. In much the same way, the transsexual psy¬
chiatrist in Dressed to Kill is driven to murder only those women who
arouse him and remind him of his hated maleness. In The Eyes of Laura
Mars, the killer's hatred of his mother drives him to prey on women
specifically—and, significantly, one gay male. He Knows You’re Alone
features a killer who in consequence of an earlier jilting preys exclu¬
sively on brides-to-be. But even in films in which males and females are
killed in roughly even numbers, the lingering images are inevitably
female. The death of a male is always swift; even if the victim grasps
what is happening to him, he has no time to react or register terror. He
is dispatched and the camera moves on. The death of a male is more¬
over more likely than the death of a female to be viewed from a dis¬
tance, or viewed only dimly (because of darkness or fog, for example),
or indeed to happen offscreen and not be viewed at all. The murders of
women, on the other hand, are filmed at closer range, in more graphic
detail, and at greater length.
The pair of murders at the therapy pool in Halloween II illus¬
trates the standard iconography. We see the orderly killed in two shots:
the first at close range in the control room, just before the stabbing, and
the second as he is being stabbed, through the vapors in a medium-long-
shot; the orderly never even sees his assailant. The nurse's death, on the
other hand, is shot entirely in medium close-up. The camera studies her
face as it registers first her unwitting complicity (as the killer strokes
her neck and shoulders from behind), then apprehension, and then, as
143
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
she faces him, terror; we see the knife plunge into her repeatedly, hear
her cries, and watch her blood fill the therapy pool. This cinematic stan¬
dard has a venerable history, and it remains intact in the slasher film.
Indeed, "tits and a scream" are all that is required of actresses audi¬
tioning for the role of victim in Co-Ed Frenzy, the Active slasher film
whose making constitutes the frame story of Blow Out (1981). It is
worth noting that none of the actresses auditioning has both in the
desired amount and that the director must resort to the use of doubles:
one for the tits, one for the screams.
Final Girl
The image of the distressed female most likely to linger in memory is
the image of the one who did not die: the survivor, or Final Girl. She is
the one who encounters the mutilated bodies of her friends and per¬
ceives the full extent of the preceding horror and of her own peril; who
is chased, cornered, wounded; whom we see scream, stagger, fall, rise,
and scream again. She is abject terror personified. If her friends knew
they were about to die only seconds before the event, the Final Girl lives
with the knowledge for long minutes or hours. She alone looks death
in the face,- but she alone also finds the strength either to stay the killer
long enough to be rescued (ending A) or to kill him herself (ending B).
She is inevitably female. In Schoell's words: "The vast majority of con¬
temporary shockers, whether in the sexist mold or not, feature cli¬
maxes in which the women fight back against their attackers—the
wandering, humorless psychos who populate these films. They often
show more courage and levelheadedness than their cringing male coun¬
terparts."32 Her scene occupies the last ten to twenty minutes (thirty
in the case of The Texas Chainsaw Massacre) and constitutes the film's
emphatic climax.
The sequence first appears in full-blown form (ending A) in The
Texas Chainsaw Massacre with Sally's spirited self-defense and eventual
rescue. Her brother and companions' were dispatched suddenly and
uncomprehendingly, one by one, but Sally survives the ninth round—
long enough to see what has become of her fellows and what is in store
for her, long enough to meet and- even dine with the whole slaughter¬
house family, long enough to undergo all manner of torture (including
the ancient grandfather's effort to strike a fatal hammer blow on the tem¬
ple as they bend her over a washtub), and long enough to bolt and rebolt,
be caught and recaught, plead and replead for her life, and eventually
144
Carol J. Clover
escape to the highway. For nearly thirty minutes of screen time—a third
of the film—we watch her shriek, run, flinch, jump through windows,
and sustain injury and mutilation. Her will to survive is astonishing; in
the end, bloody and staggering, she finds the highway, Leatherface and
Hitchhiker in pursuit. Just as they bear down on her, a truck comes up
and crushes Hitchhiker. Minutes later a pickup driver plucks Sally up
and saves her from Leatherface. The final shots show us Leatherface
from her point of view (the bed of the pickup): standing on the highway,
wounded (having gashed open his abdomen during the truck episode)
but upright, waving the chainsaw crazily over his head.
Halloween’s Final Girl is Laurie. Her desperate defense is
shorter in duration than Sally's but no less fraught with horror. Limp¬
ing from a knife wound in the leg, she flees to a garden room and breaks
in through the window with a rake. Neighbors hear her scream for help
but suspect a Halloween prank and shut the blinds. She gets into her
own babysitting house—by throwing a potted plant at a second-story
window to rouse the children—just as the killer descends. Minutes
later he comes through the window and they grapple,- she manages to
fell him with a knitting needle and grabs his butcher knife—but drops
it when he seems dead. As she goes upstairs to the children, the killer
rises, takes the knife, and goes after her. She takes refuge in a closet,
lashing the two doorknobs together from the inside. As the killer
slashes and stabs at the closet door—we see this from her inside per¬
spective—she bends a hanger into a weapon and, when he breaks the
door down, stabs him in the eye. Again thinking him vanquished, she
sends the children to the police and sinks down in pain and exhaustion.
The killer rises again, but just as he is about to stab her, Dr. Loomis,
alerted by the children, rushes in and shoots the killer.
Given the drift in just the four years between The Texas Chain¬
saw Massacre and Halloween—from passive to active defense—it is no
surprise that the films following Halloween present Final Girls who not
only fight back but do so with ferocity and even kill the killer on their
own, without help from the outside.33 Valerie in Slumber Party Mas¬
sacre (a film directed by Amy Jones and scripted by Rita Mae Brown)
takes a machete-like weapon to the killer, striking off the bit from his
drill, severing1 his hand, and finally impaling him. Alice assaults and
decapitates the killer of Friday the 13th. Pursued by the killer in Hell
Night, Marti pries the gate key from the stiff fingers of a corpse to let
herself out of the mansion grounds to safety; when the car won't start,
she repairs it on the spot; when the car gets stuck in the roadway, she
inside and killer on top, she releases it in such a way as to cast the killer
145
Her Body,'Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
Figure 27. Freddy Kreuger stalked and slashed his way through the popular Nightmare on Elm Street
films. Unlike other killers in the slasher cycle, Kreuger leavened his sadism with an urbane wit.
on the gate's upper spikes. The grittiest of the Final Girls is Nancy of A
Nightmare on Elm Street. Aware in advance that the killer will be pay¬
ing her a visit, she plans an elaborate defense. When he enters the
house, she dares him to come at her, then runs at him in direct attack.
146
Carol J. Clover
boyfriend) detains Norman at the motel while Lila snoops about (tak¬
ing note of Norman's toys). When she perceives Norman's approach,
she flees to the basement. Here she encounters the treated corpse of
Mrs. Bates and begins screaming in horror. Norman bursts in and is
about to strike when Sam enters and grabs him from behind. Like her
generic sisters, then, Lila is the spunky inquirer into the Terrible
Place—the one who first grasps, however dimly, the past and present
danger, the one who looks death in the face, and the one who survives
the murderer's last stab. There the correspondences end, however. The
Psycho scene turns, after all, on the revelation of Norman's psychotic
identity, not on Lila as a character—she enters the film midway and is
sketchily drawn—and still less on her self-defense.
The Final Girl of the slasher film is presented from the outset
as the main character. The practiced viewer distinguishes her from her
friends minutes into the film. She is the girl scout, the bookworm, the
mechanic. Unlike her girlfriends (and Marion Crane), she is not sexu¬
ally active. Laurie (Halloween) is teased because of her fears about dat¬
ing, and Marti (Hell Night) explains to the boy with whom she finds
herself sharing a room that they will have separate beds. Although
Stretch [Texas Chainsaw, Part 2) is hardly virginal, she is not available,
either,- early in the film she pointedly turns down a date, and we are
given to understand that she is, for the present, unattached and even
lonely. So too Stevie of Carpenter's The Fog, like Stretch a disk jockey.
Divorced mother and a newcomer in town, she is unattached and lonely
but declines male attention. The Final Girl is also watchful to the point
of paranoia; small signs of danger that her friends ignore she takes in
and turns over. Above all, she is intelligent and resourceful in extreme
situations. Thus Laurie even at her most desperate, cornered in a closet,
has the wit to grab a hanger from the rack and bend it into a weapon;
Marti can hot-wire her getaway car, the killer in pursuit; and the psych
major of Friday the 13th Part 2, on seeing the enshrined head of Mrs.
Voorhees, can stop Jason in his tracks by assuming a stridently mater¬
nal voice. Finally, although she is always smaller and weaker than the
killer, she grapples with him energetically and convincingly.
The Final Girl is boyish, in a word. Just as the killer is not fully
masculine, she is not fully feminine—not, in any case, feminine in the
ways of her friends. Her smartness, gravity, competence in mechanical
and other practical matters, and sexual reluctance set her apart from
the other girls and ally her, ironically, with the very boys she fears or
rejects, not to speak of the killer himself. Lest we miss the point, it is
148
Figures 28 & 29. The shower murder of Marion Crane in Alfred Hitchcock’s Psycho exerted a
seminal influence over the subsequent slasher horror cycle.
spelled out in her name: Stevie, Marti, Terry, Laurie, Stretch, Will. Not
only the conception of the hero in Alien and Aliens but also her name,
Ripley, owes a clear debt to slasher tradition.
With the introduction of the Final Girl, then, the Psycho for¬
mula is radically altered. It is not merely a question of enlarging the fig¬
ure of Lila but of absorbing into her role, in varying degrees, the
functions of Arbogast (investigator) and Sam (rescuer) and restructuring
the narrative action from beginning to end around her progress in rela¬
tion to the killer. In other words, Psycho's detective plot, revolving
around a revelation, yields in the modern slasher film to a hero plot,
149
v
revolving around the main character's struggle with and eventual tri¬
umph over evil. But for the femaleness, however qualified, of that main
character, the story is a standard one of tale and epic.
Shock
One reason that the shower sequence in Psycho has "evoked more
study, elicited more comment, and generated more shot-for-shot analy¬
sis from a technical viewpoint than any other in the history of cinema"
is that it suggests so much but shows so little.35 Of the forty-odd shots
in as many seconds that figure the murder, only a single fleeting one
actually shows the body being stabbed. The others present us with a
rapid-fire concatenation of images of the knife-wielding hand, parts of
Marion, parts of the shower, and finally the bloody water as it swirls
down the drain. The horror resides less in the actual images than in
their summary implication.
Although Hitchcock is hardly the first director to prefer the
oblique rendition of physical violence, he may, to judge from current
examples, be one of the last. For better or worse, the perfection of special
effects has made it possible to show maiming and dismemberment in
extraordinarily credible detail. The horror genres are the natural reposi¬
tories of such effects,- what can be done is done, and slashers, at the bot¬
tom of the category, do it most and worst. Thus we see a head being
stepped on so that the eyes pop out, a face being flayed, a decapitation, a
hypodermic needle penetrating an eyeball in close-up, and so on.
With this new explicitness also comes a new tone. If the horror
of Psycho was taken seriously, the "horror" of the slasher films is of a
rather more complicated sort. Audiences express uproarious disgust
("Gross!") as often as they express fear, and it is clear that the makers
of slasher films pursue the combination. More particularly, spectators
fall silent while the victim is being stalked, scream out at the first stab,
and make loud noises of revulsion at the sight of the bloody stump. The
rapid alternation between registers—between something like "real"
horror on one hand and campy, self-parodying Horror on the other—is
by now one of the most conspicuous characteristics of the tradition. In
its cultivation of intentionally outrageous excess, the slasher film inter¬
sects with the cult film, a genre devoted to such effects. Just what this
self-ironizing relation to taboo signifies, beyond a remarkably compe¬
tent audience, is unclear—it is yet another aspect of the phenomenon
that has lain beyond criticism—but for the time being it stands as a
defining characteristic of the lower genres of popular culture.
150
Carol J. Clover
The Body
On the face of it, the relation between the sexes in slasher films could
hardly be clearer. The killer is with few exceptions recognizably human
and distinctly male; his fury is unmistakably sexual in both roots and
expression; his victims are mostly women, often sexually free and
always young and beautiful ones. Just how essential this victim is to
horror is suggested by her historical durability. If the killer has over
time been variously figured as shark, fog, gorilla, birds, and slime, the
victim is eternally and prototypically the damsel. Cinema hardly
invented the pattern. It has simply given visual expression to the abid¬
ing proposition that, in Poe's famous formulation, the death of a beau¬
tiful woman is the "most poetical topic in the world."36 As slasher
director Dario Argento puts it, "I like women, especially beautiful ones.
If they have a good face and figure, I would much prefer to watch them
being murdered than an ugly girl or a man."37 Brian De Palma elabo¬
rates: "Women in peril work better in the suspense genre. It all goes
back to the Perils of Pauline. ... If you have a haunted house and you
have a woman walking around with a candelabrum, you fear more for
her than you would for a husky man."38 Or Hitchcock, during the film¬
ing of The Birds: "I always believe in following the advice of the play¬
wright Sardou. He said 'Torture the women!' The trouble today is that
we don't torture women enough."39
What the directors do not say, but show, is that "Pauline" is at
her very most effective in a state of undress, borne down upon by a bla¬
tantly phallic murderer, even gurgling orgasmically as she dies. The case
could be made that the slasher films available at a given neighborhood
video rental outlet recommend themselves to censorship under the
Dworkin-MacKinnon guidelines at least as readily as the hard-core
films the next section over, at which that legislation is aimed; for if some
victims are men, the argument goes, most are women, and the women
are brutalized in ways that come too close to real life for comfort. But
what this line of reasoning does not take into account is the figure of the
Final Girl. Because slashers lie for all practical purposes beyond the
purview of legitimate criticism and, to the extent that they have been
reviewed at all, have been reviewed on an individual basis, the phe¬
nomenon of the female victim-hero has scarcely been acknowledged.
It is, of course, "on the face of it" that most of the public dis¬
cussion of film takes place—from the Dworkin-MacKinnon legislation
to Sislcel and Ebert's reviews to our own talks with friends on leaving
the movie house. Underlying that discussion is the assumption that the
151
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
sexes are what they seem; that screen males represent the Male and
screen females the Female,- that this identification along gender lines
authorizes impulses toward sexual violence in males and encourages
impulses toward victimization in females. In part because of the mas¬
sive authority cinema by nature accords the image, even academic film
criticism has been slow—slower than literary criticism—to get beyond
appearances. Film may not appropriate the mind's eye, but it certainly
encroaches on it; the gender characteristics of a screen figure are a vis¬
ible and audible given for the duration of the film. To the extent that
the possibility of cross-gender identification has been entertained, it
has been in the direction female-with-male. Thus some critics have
wondered whether the female viewer, faced with the screen image of a
masochistic/narcissistic female, might not rather elect to "betray her
sex and identify with the masculine point of view."40 The reverse ques¬
tion—whether men might not also, on occasion, elect to betray their
sex and identify with screen females—has scarcely been asked, pre¬
sumably on the assumption that men's interests are well served by the
traditional patterns of cinematic representation. Then too there is the
matter of the "male gaze." As E. Ann Kaplan sums it up: "Within the
film text itself, men gaze at women, who become objects of the gaze;
the spectator, in turn, is made to identify with this male gaze, and to
objectify the women on the screen; and the camera's original 'gaze'
comes into play in the very act of filming."41 But if it is so that all of
us, male and female alike, are by these processes "made to" identify
with men and "against" women, how are we then to explain the appeal
to a largely male audience of a film genre that features a female victim-
hero? The slasher film brings us squarely up against a fundamental
question of film analysis: Where does the literal end and the figurative
begin,- how do the two levels interact and what is the significance of the
particular interaction,- and to which, in arriving at a political judgment
(as we are inclined to do in the case of low horror and pornography), do
we assign priority?
A figurative or functional analysis of the slasher begins with the
processes of point of view and identification. The male viewer seeking
a male character, even a vicious one, with whom to identify in a sus¬
tained way has little to hang on to in the standard example. On the good
side, the only viable candidates are the schoolmates or friends of the
girls. They are for the most part marginal, undeveloped characters;
more to the point, they tend to die early in the film. If the traditional
horror film gave the male spectator a last-minute hero with whom to
identify, thereby "indulging his vanity as protector of the helpless
152
Carol J. Clover
Social critics make much of the fact that male audience members cheer
on the misogynous misfits in these movies as they rape, plunder, and
murder their screaming, writhing female victims. Since these same
critics walk out of the moviehouse in disgust long before the movie is
over, they don't realize that these same men cheer on (with renewed
enthusiasm, in fact) the heroines, who are often as strong, sexy, and
independent as the [earlier] victims, as they blow away the killer with
a shotgun or get him between the eyes with a machete. All of these men
are said to be identifying with the maniac, but they enjoy his death
throes the most of all, and applaud the heroine with admiration.44
What filmmakers seem to know better than film critics is that gender
is less a wall than a permeable membrane.45
No one who has read "Red Riding Hood" to a small boy or par¬
ticipated in a viewing of, say, Deliverance ([1972], an all-male story that
154
Carol J. Clover
women find as gripping as men) or, more recently, Alien and Aliens,
with whose spaceage female Rambo, herself a Final Girl, male viewers
seem to engage with ease, can doubt the phenomenon of cross-gender
identification.46 This fluidity of engaged perspective is in keeping with
the universal claims of the psychoanalytic model: the threat function
and the victim function coexist in the same unconscious, regardless of
anatomical sex. But why, if viewers can identify across gender lines and
if the root experience of horror is sex blind, are the screen sexes not
interchangeable? Why not more and better female killers, and why (in
light of the maleness of the majority audience) not Pauls as well as
Paulines? The fact that horror film so stubbornly genders the killer
male and the principal victim female would seem to suggest that rep¬
resentation itself is at issue—that the sensation of bodily fright derives
not exclusively from repressed content, as Freud insisted, but also from
the bodily manifestations of that content.
Nor is the gender of the principals as straightforward as it first
seems. The killer's phallic purpose, as he thrusts his drill or knife into
the trembling bodies of young women, is unmistakable. At the same
time, however, his masculinity is severely qualified: he ranges from the
virginal or sexually inert to the transvestite or transsexual, is spiritually
divided ("the mother half of his mind"), or even equipped with vulva and
vagina. Although the killer of God Told Me To (a.k.a. Demon [1979]) is
represented and taken as a male in the film text, he is revealed, by the
doctor who delivered him, to have been sexually ambiguous from birth:
"I truly could not tell whether that child was male or female,- it was as
if the sexual gender had not been determined... as if it were being devel¬
oped."47 In this respect, slasher killers have much in common with the
monsters of classic horror—monsters who, in Linda Williams's formu¬
lation, represent not just "an eruption of the normally repressed animal
sexual energy of the civilized male" but also the "power and potency of
a non-phallic sexuality." To the extent that the monster is constructed
as feminine, the horror film thus expresses female desire only to show
how monstrous it is.48 The intention is manifest in Aliens, in which
the Final Girl, Ripley, is pitted in the climactic scene against the most
terrifying "alien" of all: an egg-laying Mother.
Nor,can we help noticing the "intrauterine" quality of the Ter¬
rible Place, dark and often damp, in which the killer lives or lurks and
whence he stages his most terrifying attacks. "It often happens," Freud
wrote, "that neurotic men declare that they feel there is something
uncanny about the female genital organs. This unheimlich place, how¬
ever, is an entrance to the former Heim [home] of all human beings, to
155
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
the place where each one of us lived once upon a time and in the begin¬
ning. ... In this case too then, the unheimlich is what once was
heimisch, familiar,- the prefix 'uri ['un-'] is the token repression."49 It
is the exceptional film that does not mark as significant the moment
that the killer leaps out of the dark recesses of a corridor or cavern at
the trespassing victim, usually the Final Girl. Long after the other par¬
ticulars have faded, the viewer \yill remember the images of Amy
assaulted from the dark halls of a morgue [He Knows You’re Alone),
Sally or Stretch facing dismemberment in the ghastly dining room or
underground labyrinth of the slaughterhouse family (the Texas Chain¬
saw films), or Melanie trapped in the attic as the savage birds close in
(The Birds). In such scenes of convergence the Other is at its bisexual
mightiest, the victim at her tiniest, and the component of sado¬
masochism at its most blatant.
The gender of the Final Girl is likewise compromised from the
outset by her masculine interests, her inevitable sexual reluctance (pen¬
etration, it seems, constructs the female), her apartness from other
girls, sometimes her name. At the level of the cinematic apparatus, her
unfemininity is signaled clearly by her exercise of the "active investi¬
gating gaze" normally reserved for males and hideously punished in
females when they assume it themselves. Tentatively at first and then
aggressively, the Final Girl looks for the killer, even tracking him to his
forest hut or his underground labyrinth, and then at him, therewith
bringing him, often for the first time, into our vision as well.50 When,
in the final scene, she stops screaming, looks at the killer, and reaches
for the knife (sledgehammer, scalpel, gun, machete, hanger, knitting
needle, chainsaw), she addresses the killer on his own terms. To the
critics' objection that Halloween in effect punished female sexuality,
director John Carpenter responded: "They [the critics] completely
missed the boat there, I think. Because if you turn it around, the one
girl who is the most sexually uptight just keeps stabbing this guy with
a long knife. She's the most sexually frustrated. She's the one that killed
him. Not because she's a virgin, but because all that repressed energy
starts coming out. She uses all those phallic symbols on the guy-She
and the killer have a certain link: sexual repression."51
For all its perversity, Carpenter's remark does not underscore
the sense of affinity, even recognition, that attends the final encounter.
But the "certain link" that puts killer and Final Girl on terms, at least
briefly, is more than "sexual repression." It is also a shared masculinity,
materialized in "all those phallic symbols"—and it is also a shared fem¬
ininity, materialized in what comes next (and what Carpenter, perhaps
156
Carol ]. Clover
rare expression of abject terror on the part of a male (as in I Spit on Your
Grave) does one apprehend the full extent of the cinematic double stan¬
dard in such matters.53
It is also the case that gender displacement can provide a kind
of identificatory buffer, an emotional remove, that permits the major¬
ity audience to explore taboo subjects in the relative safety of vicari¬
ousness. Just as Bergman came to realize that he could explore
castration anxiety more freely via depictions of hurt female bodies (wit¬
ness the genital mutilation of Karin in Cries and Whispers [1972]), so
the makers of slasher films seem to know that sadomasochistic incest
fantasies sit more easily with the male viewer when the visible player
is female. It is one thing for that viewer to hear the psychiatrist intone
at the end of Psycho that Norman as a boy (in the backstory) was abnor¬
mally attached to his mother; it would be quite another to see that
attachment dramatized in the present, to experience in nightmare form
the elaboration of Norman's (the viewer's own) fears and desires. If the
former is playable in male form, the latter, it seems, is not.
The Final Girl is, on reflection, a congenial double for the ado¬
lescent male. She is feminine enough to act out in a gratifying way, a
way unapproved for adult males, the terrors and masochistic pleasures
of the underlying fantasy, but not so feminine as to disturb the struc¬
tures of male competence and sexuality. Her sexual inactivity, in this
reading, becomes all but inevitable,- the male viewer may be willing to
enter into the vicarious experience of defending himself from the pos¬
sibility of symbolic penetration on the part of the killer, but real vagi¬
nal penetration on the diegetic level is evidently more femaleness than
he can bear. The question then arises whether the Final Girls of slasher
films —Stretch, Stevie, Marti, Will, Terry, Laurie, and Ripley—are not
boyish for the same reason that the female "victims" in Victorian flag¬
ellation literature—"Georgy," "Willy"—are boyish: because they are
transformed males. The transformation, Steven Marcus writes, "is
itself both a defense against and a disavowal of the fantasy it is simul¬
taneously expressing"—namely, that a "little boy is being beaten ... by
another man."54 What is represented as male-on-female violence, in
short, is figuratively speaking male-on-male sex. For Marcus, the liter¬
ary picture of flagellation, in which girls are beaten, is utterly belied by
the descriptions (in My Secret Life) of real-life episodes in which the
persons being beaten are not girls at all but "gentlemen" dressed in
women's clothes ("He had a woman's dress on tucked up to his waist,
showing his naked rump and thighs.... On his head was a woman's cap
tied carefully round his face to hide whiskers") and whipped by prosti-
159
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film.
such a striking resemblance to the classic (male) hero story? Does the
standard hero story featuring an anatomical female "mean" differently
from one featuring an anatomical male?
As Marcus perceived, the relationship between the Georgy sto¬
ries of flagellation literature and the real-life anecdote of the Victorian
gentleman is a marvelously telling one. In his view, the maleness of the
latter must prove the essential or‘functional maleness of the former.
What his analysis does not come to full grips with, however, is the cloth¬
ing the gentleman wears—not that of a child, as Marcus's "childish"
reading of the scene contemplates, but explicitly that of a woman.60
These women's clothes can of course be understood, within the terms
of the homoerotic interpretation, as a last-ditch effort on the part of the
gentleman to dissociate himself from the (incestuous) homosexuality
implicit in his favored sexual practice. But can they not just as well, and
far more economically, be explained as part and parcel of a fantasy of
literal femaleness? By the same token, cannot the femaleness of the
gentleman's literary representatives—the girls of the flagellation sto¬
ries—be understood as the obvious, even necessary, extension of the
man's dress and cap? The same dress and cap, I suggest, haunt the mar¬
gins of the slasher film. This is not to deny the deflective convenience,
for the male spectator (and filmmaker), of a female victim-hero in a con¬
text so fraught with taboo; it is only to suggest that the femaleness of
that character is also conditioned by a kind of imaginative curiosity
about the feminine in and of itself.
So too the psychoanalytic case. These films do indeed seem to
pit the child in a struggle, at once terrifying and attractive, with the
parental Other, and it is a rare example that does not directly thema-
tize parent-child relations. But if Freud stressed the maternal source of
the unheimlich, the Other of our films is decidedly androgynous:
female/feminine in aspects of character and place (the "intrauterine"
locale) but male in anatomy. Conventional logic may interpret the
killer as the phallic mother of the transformed boy (the Final Girl), but
the text itself does not compel such a reading. On the contrary, the text
at every level presents us with hermaphroditic constructions—con¬
structions that draw attention to themselves and demand to be taken
on their own terms.
For if we define the Final Girl as nothing more than a figurative
male, what do we then make of the context of the spectacular gender play
in which she is emphatically situated? In his essay on the uncanny, Freud
rejected out of hand Jentsch's theory that the experience of horror pro¬
ceeds from intellectual uncertainty (curiosity?)—feelings of confusion,
162
Carol J. Clover
By the same literalistic token, then, Stretch's success must lie in the
fact that in the end, at least, she "speaks loudly" even though she car¬
ries no "stick." Just as "Dorothy's" voice slips serve to remind us that
her character really is male, so the Final Girl's "tits and scream" serve
more or less continuously to remind us that she really is female—even
as, and despite the fact that, she in the end acquits herself "like a
man."66 Her chainsaw is thus what "Dorothy's",skirt is: a figuration of
what she does and what she seems, as opposed to—and the films turn
on the opposition—what she is. The idea that appearance and behavior
do not necessarily indicate sex—indeed, can misindicate sex—is predi¬
cated on the understanding that sex is one thing and gender another,- in
practice, that sex is life, a less-than-interesting given, but that gender
is theater. Whatever else it may be, Stretch's waving of the chainsaw is
a moment of high drag. Its purpose is not to make us forget that she is
a girl but to thrust that fact on us. The moment, it is probably fair to
say, is also one that openly mocks the literary/cinematic conventions
of symbolic representation.
It may be just this theatricalization of gender that makes pos¬
sible the willingness of the male viewer to submit himself to a brand of
spectator experience that Hitchcock designated as "feminine" in 1960
and that has become only more so since then. In classic horror, the
"feminization" of the audience is intermittent and ceases early. Our
relationship with Marion's body in Psycho halts abruptly at the
moment of its greatest intensity (slashing, ripping, tearing). The con¬
siderable remainder of the film distributes our bruised sympathies
among several lesser figures, male and female, in such a way and at such
length as to ameliorate the Marion experience and leave us, in the end,
more or less recuperated in our (presumed) masculinity. Like Marion,
the Final Girl is the designated victim, the incorporation of the audi¬
ence, the slashing, ripping, and tearing of whose body will cause us to
flinch and scream out in our seats. But unlike Marion, she does not die.
If Psycho, like other classic horror films, solves the femininity problem
by obliterating the female and replacing her with representatives of the
masculine order (mostly but not inevitably males), the modern slasher
solves it by regendering the woman. We are, as an audience, in the end
"masculinized" by and through the very figure by and through whom
165
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
we were earlier "feminized." The same body does for both, and that
body is female.
The last point is the crucial one: the same female body does for
both. The Final Girl (1) undergoes agonizing trials and (2) virtually or
actually destroys the antagonist and saves herself. By the lights of folk
tradition, she is not a heroine, for whom phase 1 consists in being saved
by someone else, but a hero, who rises to the occasion and defeats the
adversary with his own wit and hands. Phase 1 of the story sits well on
the female; it is the heart of heroine stories in general (Red Riding Hood,
Pauline), and in some figurative sense, in ways we have elaborated in
some detail, it is gendered feminine even when played by a male.
Odysseus's position, trapped in the cave of the Cyclops, is after all not
so different from Pauline's position tied to the tracks or Sally's trapped
in the dining room of the slaughterhouse family. The decisive moment,
as far as the fixing of gender is concerned, lies in what happens next:
those who save themselves are male, and those who are saved by oth¬
ers are female. No matter how "feminine" his experience in phase 1,
the traditional hero, if he rises against his adversary and saves himself
in phase 2, will be male.
What is remarkable about the slasher film is that it comes close
to reversing the priorities. Presumably for the various functional or fig¬
urative reasons we have considered in this essay, phase 1 wants a female:
on that point all slashers from Psycho on are agreed. Abject fear is still
gendered feminine, and the taboo anxieties in which slashers trade are
still explored more easily via Pauline than Paul. The slippage comes in
phase 2. As if in mute deference to a cultural imperative, slasher films
from the 1970s bring in a last-minute male, even when he is rendered
supernumerary by the Final Girl's sturdy defense. By 1980, however, the
male rescuer is either dismissably marginal or dispensed with alto¬
gether,- not a few films have him rush to the rescue only to be hacked to
bits, leaving the Final Girl to save herself after all. At the moment that
the Final Girl becomes her own savior, she becomes a hero; and the
moment that she becomes a hero is the 'moment that the male viewer
gives up the last pretense of male identification. Abject terror may still
be gendered feminine, but the willingness of one immensely popular
current genre to rerepresent the hero as an anatomical female would
seem to suggest that at least one of the traditional marks of heroism, tri¬
umphant self-rescue, is no longer strictly gendered masculine.
So too the cinematic apparatus. The classic split between "spec¬
tacle and narrative," which "supposes the man's role as the active one
of forwarding the story, making things happen," is at least unsettled in
166
Carol J. Clover
the slasher film.67 When the Final Girl (in films like Hell Night, Texas
Chainsaw, Part 2, and even Splatter University [1985]) assumes the
"active investigating gaze/' she exactly reverses the look, making a
spectacle of the killer and a spectator of herself. Again, it is through the
killer's eyes (I-camera) that we saw the Final Girl at the beginning of
the film, and through the Final Girl's eyes that we see the killer, often
for the first time with any clarity, toward the end. The gaze becomes,
at least for a while, female. More to the point, the female exercise of
scopic control results not in her annihilation, in the manner of classic
cinema, but in her triumph; indeed, her triumph depends on her
assumption of the gaze. It is no surprise, in light of these developments,
that the Final Girl should show signs of boyishness. Fler symbolic phal-
licization, in the last scenes, may or may not proceed at root from the
horror of lack on the part of audience and maker. But it certainly pro¬
ceeds from the need to bring her in line with the epic laws of Western
narrative tradition—the very unanimity of which bears witness to the
historical importance, in popular culture, of the literal representation
of heroism in male form—and it proceeds no less from the need to ren¬
der the reallocated gaze intelligible to an audience conditioned by the
dominant cinematic apparatus.
It is worth noting that the higher genres of horror have for the
most part resisted such developments. The idea of a female who out¬
smarts, much less outfights—or outgazes—her assailant is unthinkable
in the films of De Palma and Hitchcock. Although the slasher film's
victims may be sexual teases, they are not in addition simple-minded,
scheming, physically incompetent, and morally deficient in the man¬
ner of these filmmakers' female victims. And however revolting their
special effects and sexualized their violence, few slasher murders
approach the level of voluptuous sadism that attends the destruction of
women in De Palma's films. For reasons on which we can only specu¬
late, femininity is more conventionally elaborated and inexorably pun¬
ished, and in an emphatically masculine environment, in the higher
forms— the forms that are written up, and not by Joe Bob Briggs.
That the slasher film speaks deeply and obsessively to male
anxieties and desires seems clear—if nothing else from the maleness of
the majority audience. And yet these are texts in which the categories
masculine and feminine, traditionally embodied in male and female,
are collapsed into one and the same character—a character who is
anatomically female and one whose point of view the spectator is
unambiguously invited, by the usual set of literary-structural and cin¬
ematic conventions, to share. The willingness and even eagerness (so
167
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
NOTES
I owe a special debt of gratitude to James Cunniff and Lynn Hunt for criticism and encour¬
agement. Particular thanks to James (not Lynn) for sitting with me through not a few of
these movies.
1. Morris Dickstein, "The Aesthetics of Fright/' American Film 5, no. 10 (Sep¬
tember 1980): 34.
2. "Will Rogers never met a man he didn't like, and I can truly say the same about
the cinema," Harvey R. Greenberg says in his paean to horror, The Movies on Your Mind
(New York: Dutton/Saturday Review Press, 1975); yet his claim does not extend to the
"plethora of execrable imitations [of Psycho] that debased cinema" (p. 137).
3. William Schoell, Stay Out of the Shower: Twenty-Five Years of Shocker Films
Beginning with Psycho (New York: Dembner, 1985).
4. "Joe Bob Briggs" was evidently invented as a solution to the Dallas Time Her¬
ald's problem of "how to cover trashy movies." See Calvin Trillin's "American Chroni¬
cles: The Life and Times of Joe Bob Briggs, So Far," New Yorker, December 22, 1986, pp.
73-88.
5. Lew Brighton, "Saturn in Retrograde,- or, The Texas Jump Cut," Film Journal 2,
no. 4 (1975): 25.
6. Stephen Koch, "Fashions in Pornography: Murder as Cinematic Chic," Harper’s,
November 1976, pp. 108-9.
7. Robin Wood, "Return of the Repressed," Film Comment 14, no. 4 (July-August
1978): 30.
8. Robin Wood, "Beauty Bests the Beast," American Film 8, no. 10 (September
1983): 63.
9. Dickstein, "Aesthetics of Fright," p. 34.
10. Sigmund Freud, "The Uncanny," in Standard Edition of the Complete Psycho¬
logical Works of Sigmund Freud, ed. and trans. James Strachey (London: Hogarth Press,
1964), 17:244.
11. Steven Marcus, The Other Victorians: A Study of Sexuality and Pornography in
Mid-Nineteenth-Century England (New York: Basic Books, 1964), p. 278.
12. William Castle, Step Right Up! I’m Gonna Scare the Pants Off America (New
York: Pharos Books, 1992), p. 262.
13. Given the number of permutations, it is no surprise that new strategies keep
emerging. Only a few years ago, a director hit upon the idea of rendering the point of view
of an infant through the use of an I-camera at floor level with a double-vision image (Larry
Cohen, It's Alive). Nearly a century after technology provided a radically different means
of telling a story, filmmakers are still uncovering the possibilities.
14. Mick Martin and Marsha Porter, Video Movie Guide, 1987 (New York: Ballan-
tine, 1987), p. 690. Wood, "Beauty," p. 65, notes that the first-person camera also serves
to preserve the secret of the killer's identity for a final surprise—crucial to many films—
but adds: "The sense of indeterminate, unidentified, possibly supernatural or superhu¬
man Menace feeds the spectator's fantasy of power, facilitating a direct spectator-camera
identification by keeping the intermediary character, while signified to be present, as
vaguely defined as possible." Brian De Palma's Blow Out opens with a parody of just this
cinematic habit.
15. On this widely discussed topic, see Kaja Silverman, The Subject of Semiotics
(New York: Oxford University Press, 1983), pp. 194-236; and Lesley Stern, "Point of View:
The Blind Spot," Film Reader, no. 4 (1979): 214-36.
16. In this essay I have used the term identification vaguely and generally to refer
to both primary and secondary processes. See Mary Ann Doane, "Misrecognition and
170
Carol J. Clover
Identity," Cine-Tracts, no. 11 (1980): 25-32; and Christian Metz, "The Imaginary Signi-
fier," The Imaginary Signifier: Psychoanalysis and the Cinema (Bloomington: Indiana
University Press, 1982).
17. Mark Nash, "Vampyr and the Fantastic," Screen 17, no. 3 (Autumn 1976): 37.
Nash coins the term cinefantastic to refer to this play.
18. Rosemary Jackson, Fantasy: The Literature of Subversion (London and New
York: Methuen, 1981), p. 31.
19. As Dickstein puts it, "The 'art' of horror film is a ludicrous notion since horror,
even at its most commercially exploitative, is genuinely subculture like the wild child
that can never be tamed, or the half-human mutant who appeals to our secret fascination
with deformity and the grotesque." "Aesthetics," p. 34.
20. James B. Twitchell, Dreadful Pleasures: An Anatomy of Modern Horror (New
York: Oxford University Press, 1985), p. 84.
21. Donald Spoto, The Dark Side of Genius: The Life of Alfred Hitchcock (Boston
and Toronto: Little, Brown, 1983), p. 421.
22. Wood, "Return of the Repressed," p. 26. In Wes Craven's A Nightmare on Elm
Street (1985), it is the nightmare itself, shared by the teenagers who live on Elm Street,
that is fatal. One by one they are killed by the murderer of their collective dream. The
one girl who survives does so by first refusing to sleep and then, at the same time that
she acknowledges her parents' inadequacies, by conquering the feelings that prompt the
deadly nightmare. See, on the topic of dream/horror, Dennis L. White, "The Poetics of
Horror," Cinema Journal 10, no. 2 (Spring 1971): 1-18. Reprinted in Film Genre: Theory
and Criticism, ed. Barry K. Grant (Metuchen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press, 1977), pp. 124—44.
23. It is not just the profit margin that fuels the production of low horror. It is also
the fact that, thanks to the irrelevance of production values, the initial stake is within
the means of a small group of investors. Low horror is thus for all practical purposes the
only way an independent filmmaker can break into the market. Add to this the film¬
maker's unusual degree of control over the product, and one begins to understand why it
is that low horror engages the talents of such people as Stephanie Rothman, George
Romero, Wes Craven, and Larry Cohen. As V. Vale and Andrea Juno put it, "The value of
low-budget films is: they can be transcendent expressions of a single person's individual
vision and quirky originality. When a corporation decides to invest $20 million in a film,
a chain of command regulates each step, and no person is allowed free rein. Meetings with
lawyers, accountants, and corporate boards are what films in Hollywood are all about."
Incredibly Strange Films, ed. V. Vale and Andrea Juno (San Francisco: ReSearch #10,
1986), p. 5.
24. Despite the film industry's interest in demographics, there is no in-depth study
of the composition of the slasher-film audience. Twitchell, Dreadful Pleasures, pp. 69-72
and 306-7, relies on personal observation and the report of critics, which are remarkably
consistent over time and from place to place; my own observations concur. The audience
is mostly between the ages of 12 and 20, and disproportionately male. Some critics
remark on a contingent of older men who sit separately and who, in Twitchell's view, are
there "not to be frightened, but to participate" specifically in the "stab-at-female"
episodes. Roger Ebert and Gene Siskel corroborate the observation.
25. The development of the human-sausage theme is typical of the back-and-forth
borrowing in low horror. The Texas Chainsaw Massacre hints at it; Motel Hell turns it
into an industry ("Farmer Vincent's Smoked Meats: This is it!" proclaims a local bill¬
board); and The Texas Chainsaw Massacre, Part 2 expands it to a statewide chili-tasting
contest.
26. The release of sexuality in the horror film is always presented as perverted, mon¬
strous, and excessive, both the perversion and the excess being the logical outcome of
repressing. Nowhere is this carried further than in The Texas Chainsaw Massacre. Here
171
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
sexuality is totally perverted from its functions, into sadism, violence, and cannibalism.
It is striking that there is no suggestion anywhere that Sally is the object of an overtly
sexual threat; she is to be tormented, killed, dismembered, and eaten, but not raped."
Wood, "Return of the Repressed," p. 31.
27■ With some exceptions—for example, the speargun used in the sixth killing in
Friday the 13th, Part III.
28. Stuart Kaminsky, American Film Genres: Approaches to a Critical Theory of
Popular Film (Dayton, Ohio: Pflaum, 1974), p. 107.
29. The shower sequence in Psycho is ptobably the most echoed scene in all of film
history. The bathtub scene in I Spit on Your Grave (not properly speaking a slasher,
though with a number of generic affinities) is to my knowledge the only effort to reverse
the terms.
30. Schoell, Stay Out of the Shower, p. 35. It may be argued that Blood Feast (1963),
in which a lame Egyptian caterer slaughters one woman after another for their body parts
(all in the service of Ishtar), provides the serial-murder model.
31. This theme too is spoofed in Motel Hell. Farmer Vincent's victims are two hook¬
ers, a kinky couple looking for same (he puts them in room 1 of the motel), and Terry and
her boyfriend Bo, out for kicks on a motorcycle. When Terry (allowed to survive) won¬
ders aloud why someone would try to kill them, Farmer Vincent answers her by asking
pointedly whether they were married. "No," she says, in a tone of resignation, as if
accepting the logic.
32. "Scenes in which women whimper helplessly and do nothing to defend them¬
selves are ridiculed by the audience, who find it hard to believe that anyone—male or
female—would simply allow someone to kill them with nary a protest." Schoell, Stay
Out of the Shower, pp. 55-56.
33. Splatter University (1984) is a disturbing exception. Professor Julie Parker is
clearly established as a Final Girl from the outset and then killed just after the beginning
of what we are led to believe will be the Final Girl sequence (she kicks the killer, a psy¬
chotic priest-scholar who keeps his knife sheathed in a crucifix, in the groin, runs for the
elevator—and then is trapped and stabbed to death). So meticulously are the conventions
observed, and then so grossly violated, that we can only assume sadistic intentionality.
This is a film in which (with the exception of an asylum orderly in the preface) only
females are killed and in highly sexual circumstances.
34. This film is complicated by the fact that the action is envisaged as a living
dream. Nancy finally kills the killer by killing her part of the collective nightmare. See
note 22 above.
35. Spoto, Dark Side of Genius, p. 454. See also William Rothman, Hitchcock: The
Murderous Gaze (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1982), pp. 246-341.
36. Edgar Allan Poe, "The Philosophy of Composition," in Selected Prose, Poetry
and Eureka (San Francisco: Rhinehart Press, 1950), p. 425.
37. Quoted in Schoell, Stay Out of the Shower, p. 56.
38. Quoted in ibid., p. 41.
39. Spoto, Dark Side of Genius, p. 483.
40. Silvia Bovenschen, "Is There a Feminine Aesthetic?" New German Critique 10
(Winter 1977): 114. See also Doane, "Misrecognition and Identity."
41. E. Ann Kaplan, Women and Film: Both Sides of the Camera (New York and Lon¬
don: Methuen, 1983), p. 15. The discussion of the gendered "gaze" is lively and extensive.
See, above all, Laura Mulvey, "Visual Pleasure and Narrative Cinema," Screen 16, no. 3
(Autumn 1975): 6-18; also see Christine Gledhill, "Recent Developments in Feminist
Criticism," Quarterly Review of Film Studies 3, no. 4 (Fall 1978): 458-93.
42. Wood, "Beauty Bests the Beast," p. 64.
43. The locus classicus in this connection is the view-from-the-coffin shot in Carl
172
Carol J. Clover
Dreyer's Vampyr [ 1932), in which the I-camera sees through the eyes of a dead man. See
Nash, "Vampyr and the Fantastic," esp. pp. 32-33. The 1986 remake of The Little Shop
of Horrors (originally a low-budget horror film, made the same year as Psycho in two days)
lets us see the dentist from the proximate point of view of the patient's tonsils.
44. Two points in this paragraph deserve emending. One is the suggestion that rape
is common in these films; it is in fact virtually absent, by definition (see note 26 above).
The other is the characterization of the Final Girl as "sexy." She may be attractive
(although typically less so than her friends), but she is with few exceptions sexually
inactive. For a detailed analysis of point-of-view manipulation, together with a psycho¬
analytic interpretation of the dynamic, see Steve Neale, "Halloween: Suspense, Aggres¬
sion, and the Look," Framework 14 (1981): 25-29; reprinted in Planks of Reason: Essays
on the Horror Film, ed. Barry Keith Grant (Metuchen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press, 1984), pp.
331-45.
45. Wood is struck by the willingness of the teenage audience to identify "against"
itself, with the forces of the enemy of youth: "Watching [The Texas Chainsaw Massacre]
recently with a large, half-stoned youth audience, who cheered and applauded every one
of Leatherface's outrages against their representatives on the screen, was a terrifying
experience" ("Return of the Repressed," p. 32).
46. "I really appreciate the way audiences respond," Gail Anne FFurd, producer of
Aliens, is reported to have said in the San Francisco Examiner Datebook, August 10,
1986, p. 19. "They buy it. We don't get people, even rednecks, leaving the theatre saying,
'That was stupid. No woman would do that.' You don't have to be a liberal ERA supporter
to root for Ripley." Time, July 28, 1986, p. 56, suggests that Ripley's maternal impulses
(she squares off against the worst aliens of all in her quest to save a little girl) give the
audience "a much stronger rooting interest in Ripley, and that gives the picture reso¬
nances unusual in a popcorn epic."
47. "When she [the mother] referred to the infant as a male, I just went along with
it. Wonder how that child turned out—male, female, or something else entirely?" The
birth is understood to be parthenogenetic, and the bisexual child, literally equipped with
both sets of genitals, is figured as the reborn Christ.
48. Linda Williams, "When the Woman Looks," in Re-Vision: Essays in Feminist
Film Criticism, ed. Mary Ann Doane, Patricia Mellencamp, and Linda Williams (Freder¬
ick, Md.: University Publications of America/American Film Institute, 1984), p. 90.
Williams's emphasis on the phallic leads her to dismiss slasher killers as a "non-specific
male killing force" and hence a degeneration in the tradition. "In these films the recog¬
nition and affinity between woman and monster of classic horror film gives way to pure
identity: she is the monster, her mutilated body is the only visible horror" (p. 96). This
analysis does justice neither to the obvious bisexuality of slasher killers, nor to the new
strength of the female victim. The slasher film may not, in balance, be more subversive
than traditional horror, but it is certainly not less so.
49. Freud, "The Uncanny," p. 245. See also Neale, "Halloween," esp. pp. 28-29.
50. "The woman's exercise of an active investigating gaze can only be simultane¬
ous with her own victimization. The place of her specularization is transformed into the
locus of a process of seeing designed to unveil an aggression against itself." Mary Ann
Doane, "The Woman's Film: Possession and Address," in Re-Vision, p. 72.
51. John Carpenter, interviewed by Todd McCarthy, "Trick or Treat," Film Com¬
ment 16, no. 1 (January-February 1980): 23-24.
52. This is not so in traditional film or in heterosexual pornography, in any case.
Gay male pornography films female bodies.
53. Compare the visual treatment of the (male) rape in Deliverance (1972) with the
(female) rapes in Hitchcock's Frenzy (1972), Craven's Last House on the Left (1972), or
Bergman's The Virgin Spring (1959). The latter films study the victims' faces at length
173
Her Body, Himself: Gender in the Slasher Film
and in close-up during the act; the former looks at the act intermittently and in longshot,
focusing less on the actual victim than on the victim's friend who must look on.
54. Marcus, The Other Victorians, pp. 260-61. Marcus distinguishes two phases in
the development of flagellation literature: one in which the figure being beaten is a boy,
and a second in which the figure is a girl. The very shift indicates, at some level, the irrel¬
evance of apparent sex. "The sexual identity of the figure being beaten is remarkably
labile. Sometimes he is represented as a boy, sometimes as a girl, sometimes as a combi¬
nation of the two—a boy dressed as a girl, or the reverse." The girls often have sexually
ambiguous names as well. The beater is a female but, in Marcus's reading, a phallic one—
muscular, possessed of body hair—representing the father.
55. Ibid., pp. 125-27.
56. "Suspense is like a woman. The more left to the imagination, the more the
excitement.... The perfect 'woman of mystery' is one who is blonde, subtle, and Nordic.
... Movie titles, like women, should be easy to remember without being familiar, intrigu¬
ing but never obvious, warm yet refreshing, suggest action, not impassiveness, and finally
give a clue without revealing the plot. Although I do not profess to be an authority on
women, I fear that the perfect title, like the perfect woman, is difficult to find." Quoted
in Spoto, Dark Side of Genius, p. 431.
57. This would seem to be the point of the final sequence of De Palma's Blow Out,
in which we see the boyfriend of the victim-hero stab the killer to death but later hear
the television announce that the woman herself vanquished the killer. The frame plot of
the film has to do with the making of a slasher film (Co-Ed Frenzy), and it seems clear
that De Palma means his ending to stand as a comment on the Final Girl formula of the
genre. De Palma's (and indirectly Hitchcock's) insistence that only men can kill men, or
protect women from men, deserves a separate essay.
58. The term is Judith Fetterly's. See her The Resisting Reader, A Feminist
Approach to American Fiction (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1978).
59. On the possible variety of responses to a single film, see Norman N. Holland,
"Ting Film," Critical Inquiry 12 (Summer 1986): 654-71.
60. Marcus, The Other Victorians, p. 127. Marcus contents himself with noting that
the scene demonstrates a "confusion of sexual identity." In the literature of flagellation,
he adds, "this confused identity is also present, but it is concealed and unacknowledged."
But it is precisely the femaleness of the beaten figures that does acknowledge it.
61. Freud, "The Uncanny," esp. pp. 219-21 and 226-27.
62. Paymond Durgnat, Films and Feelings (London: Faber and Faber, 1967), p. 216.
63. Not a few critics have argued that the ambiguity is the unintentional result of
bad filmmaking.
64. So argues Susan Barrowclough: the "male spectator takes the part not of the
male but of the female. Contrary to the assumption that the male uses pornography to
confirm and celebrate his gender's sexual activity and dominance, is the possibility of his
pleasure in identifying with a 'feminine' passivity or subordination." "Not a Love Story,"
Screen 23, no. 5 (November-December 1982): 35-36. Alan Soble seconds the proposal in
his Pornography: Marxism, Feminism, and the Future of Sexuality (New Haven, Conn.:
Yale University Press, 1986), p. 93. Porn/sexploitation filmmaker Joe Sarno: "My point
of view is more or less always from the woman's point of view; the fairy tales that my
films are based on are from the woman's point of view; I stress the efficacy of women for
themselves. In general, I focus on the female orgasm as much as I can." Quoted in Vale
and Juno, eds., Incredibly Strange Films, p. 94. "Male identification with women," Kaja
Silverman writes, "has not received the same amount of critical attention [as sublima¬
tion into professional 'showing off' and reversal into scopophilia], although it would seem
the most potentially destabilizing, at least as far as gender is concerned." See her discus¬
sion of the "Great Male Renunciation" in "Fragments of a Fashionable Discourse," in
174
Carol J. Clover
»
Stephen Prince
From Savage Cinema: Sam Peckinpah and the Rise of Ultiaviolent Movies by Stephen
Prince, copyright © 1998. Courtesy of the University of Texas Press.
175
176
Stephen Prince
We watch our wars and see men die, really die, every day on television,
but it doesn't seem real. We don't believe those are real people dying
on that screen. We've been anesthetized by the media. What I do is
show people what it's really like—not by showing it as it is so much
as by heightening it, stylizing it. . . . The only way I can do that is by
not letting them gloss over the looks of it, as if it were the seven
o'clock news from the DMZ. When people complain about the way I
handle violence, what they're really saying is, "Please don't show me;
I don't want to know; and get me another beer out of the icebox."1
Figure 30. Sam Peckinpah, the modern poet of screen violence, popularized the essential film style
for depicting gun violence.
events around them. His belief in this narcotizing function was consis¬
tent with the radical critique of popular culture in that period (as repre¬
sented in the writings of Susan Sontag and Herbert Marcuse).4 By using
graphic imagery of bloodletting and the montage aesthetic, Peckinpah
aimed to bring the era's violence inside the movie theater, which would
no longer function as a place of refuge by shielding viewers from horrific
images. His work would place the filmic representation of violence into
proper synchronicity with the era, whose convulsions engulfed the sen¬
sibilities of filmmaker and audience alike. His object in doing this was
to create a socially beneficial effect. "To negate violence it must be
178
Stephen Prince
that he began using it well before The Wild Bunch, as Weddle and Sey-
dor have pointed out), who seemed to have only an occasional interest
in the device, the essentials of Peckinpah's usage are clearly already
contained in Seven Samurai.
Contrary to Weddle's claims, Kurosawa does cut in and out of
the slow-motion footage in a dynamic manner. During the scene where
the leader of the samurai, Kambei (Takashi Shimura), rescues a kid¬
napped child from a crazed thief and kills the thief with a short sword,
Kurosawa dynamically intercuts footage filmed at normal speed with
slow-motion footage so that the rhythms of the scene oscillate between
these two different temporal modes. The mortally wounded thief
crashes through the hut's doorway to the village square outside, where
amazed onlookers witness his dying. Kurosawa intercuts three slow-
motion shots of the thief crashing through the door, running a few steps
forward, and rising up on tiptoe with three normal-speed shots of the
onlookers' reactions. Since movement also occurs in these shots, the
scene builds an internal tension between these differing rhythms. After
Figure 31. In Seven Samurai, Kurosawa intercuts slow-motion shots of the dying thief (fore¬
ground) with normal-speed shots of onlookers. This scene provided the essential template for mod¬
ern movie violence, showing other filmmakers how to integrate footage shot at differing camera
speeds to aestheticize violence.
182
Stephen Prince
these six shots, Kurosawa shows the thief fall to the ground in slow
motion but without the sound of an impact. This sound has been with¬
drawn from the scene, setting up a dynamic visual-acoustic conflict
that accompanies the temporal conflicts.
But the visual-acoustic conflicts in the film are more subtle
still and hold yet greater relevance for Peckinpah's work and for the
slow-motion aesthetic. During the scene's slow-motion shots, Kuro¬
sawa includes amplified sound effects—the baby's cry, the mother's
scream, howling wind—which involve no temporal distortion. Ampli¬
fied, but temporally unmodified, sound accompanies the slow-motion
images, and this disjunction emphasizes the heightened artifice of these
images, their uniquely expressive power. What we have yet to appreci¬
ate is not only how fundamentally Kurosawa's brief exploration of
slow-motion effects influenced Peckinpah's work, but also how Kuro¬
sawa had already made the essential discovery that a temporal non¬
synchrony of image and sound accentuates the contrast of footage shot
at differing camera speeds. The normal-speed sound emphasizes the
otherness of the slow-motion image. This visual-acoustic principle is
basic to the expressive power of such sequences.
Following Kurosawa, Peckinpah manipulates sound during the
slow-motion shots by amplifying selected effects. The cries of the baby
and mother and the howling wind are selectively amplified during the
slow-motion shots in Seven Samurai to accentuate the temporal mis¬
match between the audiovisual tracks. In the opening shoot-out in The
Wild Bunch, when an outlaw crashes his horse through a glass window
in slow motion, the sound of shattering glass heard at normal tempo is
mixed above the general battle sounds to accentuate the temporal mis¬
matches, and when Dutch (Ernest Borgnine) falls off his horse into a
wooden structure in slow motion, the amplified crunch of wood is the
dominant sound. In Peckinpah's work, the slow-motion image is care¬
fully contrasted with amplified sound effects to create an intermodal,
cross-sensory montage, and, as we have established, the expressive
power of these combinations had been explicitly demonstrated by
Kurosawa.
Kurosawa's disjunctive editing and audiovisual combinations
are indebte4 to the montage tradition exemplified by Eisenstein and so,
too, therefore, is Peckinpah's work. Care needs to be exercised in
extending these comparisons, however. Eisenstein's montage princi¬
ples belong to a rich and specific ideological and social context that
informed his filmmaking, a context that does not translate to either
183
The Aesthetic of Slow-Mption Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
motion work, remarking "they did all my shtick."16 Despite this claim,
however, wardrobe supervisor Gordon Dawson recollected that Peck¬
inpah wanted to surpass Bonnie and Clyde's violence and stylistics
while in production on The Wild Bunch. Furthermore, in a letter of
March 19, 1968, Warner Bros, confirmed with the film's production
manager that a print of Bonnie and Clyde would be shipped to Peckin¬
pah's Mexico location for a screening the next weekend (March 23-24).
This was immediately prior to the start of principal photography on the
25th. Peckinpah was studying Penn's film and wanted to see it before
commencing work on his own. He knew exactly what he was doing on
The Wild Bunch and how it related to Penn's achievements in Bonnie
and Clyde.17
If Peckinpah did not discover anything new about intercutting
slow-motion shots into a montage sequence, he undeniably extended
and built upon the principles informing Kurosawa's editing of multi¬
camera footage. Like Penn's use of slow motion within the bloody mon¬
tage that concludes Bonnie and Clyde, which Penn said conveyed
"both the spastic and the balletic" qualities of the gangsters' death ago¬
nies,18 Peckinpah's editing emphasized the brutality of physical vio¬
lence while also giving it a graceful beauty. This contradiction between
the aesthetic beauty of the visual spectacle and the emotional and phys¬
ical pain that Peckinpah also dramatized as part of his screen violence
is a complex and important one, and Peckinpah used slow motion and
montage to stylize screen violence in ways that far surpassed what Penn
had done in Bonnie and Clyde. An comparison with Penn's films can
help illuminate why Peckinpah's slow-motion inserts are more strik¬
ing and achieve a more heightened stylistic intensity than do Penn's.
Penn began to explore slow motion in The Left-Handed Gun
(1958), about the legend of Billy the Kid, during the scene where Billy
(Paul Newman) kills Deputy Ollinger (Denver Pyle), a scene that Peck¬
inpah closely re-creates in Pat Garrett and Billy the Kid. It is important
to note here that Penn's film contains numerous images and bits of
business that Peckinpah borrowed for his own film, including the
Christ-pose Billy adopts when Garrett arrests him following the Stink¬
ing Springs shoot-out. Both films also reference the fascinated reaction
of children, to violent death, a major Peckinpah preoccupation. In
Penn's film, a little girl runs out and laughs at the dead Ollinger, who
has been blown out of his boot, while Peckinpah's film shows kids play¬
ing and laughing on the gallows that has been erected for Billy. And,
again, much of this can be traced back to Kurosawa, who in Seven
185
The Aesthetic of Slow-Motion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
Samurai had shown children playing and climbing on the fortress walls
built to protect the village from the bandits and across which much
killing would occur.
When Billy shoots Ollinger, Penn cuts from a slow-motion shot
of Ollinger waving his arms and starting to fall backward to a fast-
motion shot of his body hitting the ground. The transition from slow
to fast motion is abrupt, and as a stylistic design it is clearly an exper¬
imental effort. "I was just playing with the medium/' says Penn.19 The
experimentation doesn't work very well because the action rendered in
slow motion—Ollinger flailing his arms—is not effectively suited for
the temporal manipulation. It is neither balletic nor spastic. By contrast,
in Bonnie and Clyde, Penn more shrewdly incorporates slow motion by
intercutting it at multiple points with the jerky convulsing of the gang¬
sters as they are riddled with bullets. By alternating between slow and
apparently accelerated tempos (the apparent acceleration produced at
normal film speed by virtue of the Texas Rangers' fast rate of fire), Penn
successfully brings out the balletic and spastic qualities of the scene.
Furthermore, Penn switches to slow motion at a more judicious
moment than in The Left-Handed Gun. After a quick series of glances
between Bonnie and Clyde that conveys their awareness of what is about
to happen, Clyde runs toward Bonnie, at which point they are raked
with machine-gun fire. As Clyde starts to fall, Penn switches to slow
motion for the first time so that the arc of Clyde's dying fall is poeti¬
cally extended.
The imagery is extremely vivid, and it discloses a fundamental
principle that Peckinpah would observe in his own films: slow motion
is especially powerful when it correlates with a character's loss of phys¬
ical volition. Clyde's dying arc; the trajectories of falling, dying men shot
from the rooftops of San Rafael or the army personnel blasted off the flat¬
cars of the train in The Wild Bunch; the Gorch brothers dead on their
feet but kept up convulsively by the impact of bullets fired by Mapache's
men.; Holly's mortal fall, blasted backward across the saloon by the force
of Pat Garrett's shot; Lt. Triebig's grotesque writhing under Steiner's
machine gun fire in Cross of Iron—all of these slow-motion images
derive their poetic force from the metaphysical paradox of the body's
continued animate reactions during.a moment of diminished or extin¬
guished consciousness. Slow motion intensifies this paradox by pro¬
longing it. It is not just the moment of violent death which is extended,
but the mysteries inherent in that twilit zone between consciousness
and autonomic impulse, that awful moment when a personality ceases
186
Stephen Prince
Slow-Motion Inserts
the climax of Straw Dogs, when David Sumner (Dustin Hoffman) grap¬
ples with Charlie Venner following the shooting of Norman Scutt (both
Venner and Scutt are members of the gang that has invaded the Sum¬
ners' house), Peckinpah presented their struggle in a lengthy series of
slow-motion shots. It is an interesting usage, but it softens the hard
edge the film's violence has heretofore had. The viewer feels that little
harm can come to either David or Charlie while they slog slowly
around as if underwater. Significantly, when David brings the mantrap
down on Charlie's head, the film reverts to normal speed. Furthermore,
because the slow-motion shots have lasted an uncommonly long time,
the transition back out, to normal tempo, feels abrupt and harsh. The
paradoxical thing about the brief slow-motion inserts that typify Peck¬
inpah's work is that they mesh so well with the ordinary temporal con¬
tinuum. When the insert is brief, the editor can slip into and out of the
decelerated moment in a highly fluid manner. Despite the temporal dis¬
ruption, strong continuity prevails, unlike the just-described scene
from Straw Dogs, where the return to normal time occasions a percep¬
tible loss of continuity.
Slow-motion images are not of themselves dynamic. Their ten¬
dency is toward inertia, a deceleration not only of represented time but
of the internal rhythms and pacing of the sequence in which they
appear. They become dynamic with reference to their surrounding con¬
text—the normal-tempo actions against which they play as stylistic
opposites. By maximizing this opposition, Peckinpah and his editors
could give the slow- motion inserts a dynamism which they do not in
themselves possess. Intercutting slow motion with normal speed
became an essential and highly effective way of achieving this. When
two thugs in Bring Me the Head of Alfredo Garcia lose control of their
station wagon, it skids off the road, churning up a huge spray of dirt.
This is rendered in two slow-motion shots that are crosscut with the
startled reactions of passengers on a passing bus. The decelerated action
of the skid is slowed down so much that the resulting images seem
robbed of nearly all movement, which heightens the dynamic contrast
with the surrounding normal-tempo imagery. The viewer experiences
a perceptual shock because of this radical misalignment between the
alternate tempos. Intercutting the two accentuates the misalignment
and the dynamic contrast and works against the tendency for slow
motion to create inertia and a brake on the action.
Later in that film, when the protagonist, Bennie (Warren Oates),
ambushes these thugs and shoots them, Peckinpah had his editors
crosscut three slow-motion shots of one thug's dying fall with normal-
191
The Aesthetic of Slow-Motion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
intercut all the separate lines of action. I might start with this guy
being hit, then cut to that guy being hit, cut to this guy falling, that
guy still falling, then cut to somebody else over there getting hit, to a
horse spinning over there, somebody going through a window there,
and then back to the first guy just landing on the ground. I meshed it.
I took every piece of action and intercut it with another.26
193
The Aesthetic of Slow-Motion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
Indeed, the shot lists that survive among Peckinpah's papers, corre¬
sponding to the opening and closing shoot-outs in San Rafael and Agua
Verde, present a linear and chronological list of images.
As Lombardo's description indicates, and as a careful viewing
of the film demonstrates, the basic device used to mesh the lines of
action is crosscutting. However, instead of a simple cutting back and
forth, the lines of action are interrupted for extended periods by cut¬
aways to other things before they resume. In its elaborate patterns of
crosscutting, Peckinpah's editing performs four distinct functions. It
slows down, interrupts, parallels, and returns to ongoing lines of action.
These functions collectively establish the collage-like structure of the
montage set-pieces, and they are clearly illustrated by a scene within
the San Rafael massacre that opens The Wild Bunch, one that Lom¬
bardo alludes to in his description of how he meshed the lines of action.
The Bunch have just robbed the depot and are being fired upon by the
rooftop snipers. They return fire, hitting two victims on the rooftop. An
early, written visualization of this scene differs substantially from the
final montage and describes mainly a linear progression of images with
little intercutting.
23. Bounty hunter Shannon rises up to fire and is hit and wounded,
starts to pitch forward over the parapit [sic],
24. Another bounty hunter (who?) grabs for him, exposes himself and
is hit and killed, slumps back releasing Shannon who rolls down
the roof and pitches to the street.
25. Burt sees the body fall beside him and jumps on his horse racing out
firing steadily until he is caught in the bounty hunter fire, causing
both he [sic] and his horse to plunge through the window of the
dress shop.
26. Wild Bunch:—Pike stays in the street with Dutch as the others die
or mount and ride out.
27. Then following, Frank who has been wounded while mounting, is
shot while riding out and goes into a drag still clutching the bags of
silver from the pay station.
28. As Frank is dragged down the street, the horse is shot down.27
The editing in the finished film parallels the falls of both rooftop vic¬
tims by crosscutting between them, but it also interrupts these lines of
action by cutting away to other, ongoing lines of action before return¬
ing to the two victims. As victim one ('Shannon' in the early shot con¬
tinuity) topples forward, off the roof, the first cutaway occurs to a shot
194
Stephen Prince
Figures 32 & 33. Peckinpah uses slow motion and montage editing to mesh together multiple
lines of action in the violent gun battles that open and close The Wild Bunch, as in this inter¬
cutting of the trajectories of two victims shot off the rooftops of the town of Starbuck.
of Pike running out of the depot office. The next shot returns to a con¬
tinuation of the previous action as victim one falls below the bottom
of the frame line. The next three shots are all cutaways. Angel, another
of the Bunch, dashes out of the depot office, returning fire as he runs.
A low-angle longshot of the rooftop snipers is followed by a high-angle
longshot of several of the Bunch shooting toward the snipers from
inside the depot. After these three cutaways, the montage returns to
victim one, and slow motion is introduced to retard the rate of his fall.
The composition is very dramatic. The victim arcs against the blue sky,
the slow motion suspending him weightlessly in space.
The next three shots are more cutaways, but they also intro¬
duce the fate of the second rooftop victim. In the first cutaway, a
medium shot shows the Bunch firing from inside the depot. Then, in
the next shot, the second rooftop victim is struck and falls forward,
195
The Aesthetic of Slow-Motion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
toward the camera. Next, a medium shot shows Angel running from
the depot, firing, continuing an action introduced six shots previously.
The next shot, in slow motion, returns to the first victim, continuing
his lethargic descent to earth. Next, the montage cuts to the second vic¬
tim still toppling forward, but not off the roof. His fall will be broken
by the rooftop ledge. A medium close-up shows Deke Thornton firing
from the roof. Then, in the next two ^shots, both victims land simulta¬
neously, in a matched cut, victim one in the street and victim two on
the rooftop ledge.
The editing reconfigures, stylistically transforms, the deaths of
the two rooftop victims. The action cuts away from victim one's fall
four times, initially for one shot, then for three shots, again for three
shots, and finally for two shots. Two cutaways interrupt the fall of the
second victim, for two shots each time. The cutaways and the use of
slow motion impose a marked distortion upon the time and space of
the represented action. The editing creates a false parallel between the
two victims. Victim one falls off the roof in slow motion, and victim
two, hit later in the sequence, falls at normal speed, yet they strike
ledge and street at precisely the same moment, as the matched cut that
closes off these events indicates. The editing imposes a false parallel
between normal time and decelerated time. The simultaneous impact
of the two victims represents an impossible time-space relationship
within the sequence, yet Peckinpah and Lombardo convincingly inter¬
cut normal speed and slow motion to extend this discontinuity.
This is certainly what Peckinpah meant when he rejected alle¬
gations that his work represented a greater realism, stressing instead
that it stylized violence, heightened it through artistic transformation.
The elaborate montages of The Wild Bunch effect such an artistic trans¬
formation of space, time, and perception by rendering space and time
as totally plastic and unstable entities. Time slows, stretches, folds
around on itself and becomes the fourth dimension of a spatial field in
which the ordinary laws of physics do not apply. When the outlaw try¬
ing to flee the rooftop sniper ("Burt" in the early visualization) crashes
his horse through a storefront window, three slow-motion shots of this
action are intercut with three normal-speed shots of another rider
crashing to the street after his horse is shot out from under him. The
cutting creates a false spatial parallel. The amount of space traversed
by each rider who takes a spill is roughly the same—from saddle to
ground—but the different rates of time indicate different spaces, since
time and space alike are part of a four-dimensional continuum. By
196
Stephen Prince
intercutting the falls of these riders in slow motion and normal speed,
Peckinpah and Lombardo reconfigured space as well as time. The slow
motion implies an alternate spatial field in which the reconstituted
dynamics play out.
Peckinpah and Lombardo also employed the pattern of inter¬
rupting ongoing lines of action with cutaways to other events in a way
that is analogous with their use of slow motion—to extend the dura¬
tion of the represented events and retard their completion. In the con¬
cluding shoot-out at Mapache's headquarters, the film's crowning
montage set-piece, Pike bursts into a room where a woman stands,
shoots into a mirrored door to kill a soldier hiding there, and is then
shot by the woman on whom he had turned his hack. This simple series
of events is broken up and extended with a very long set of cutaways.
After Pike shoots the soldier, the action cuts away from him for seven¬
teen shots, which show, primarily, Dutch using a woman as a shield as
he exchanges gunfire with Mapache's men. Then, returning to Pike, a
single close-up shows him glancing outside the woman's room. A sec¬
ond lengthy cutaway, twenty-eight shots long, shows Lyle Gorch
behind the machine gun, howling like a demon. Then the action returns
to Pike, still in the room, at which point he is shot. The elaborate cut¬
aways extend the duration of the action and thereby expand time.
As this description indicates, Peckinpah and Lombardo did not
employ slow motion in any simple capacity. The slow-motion inserts
are placed within a complex montage that crosscuts multiple lines of
action so that the slow motion functions in concert with the extended
cutaways to reconfigure time. Furthermore, the temporal manipula¬
tions include not just deceleration but also parallelism, disruption, and
resumption. The resulting stylistic transcends a naturalistic presenta¬
tion of violence, and it is notable that critics have discussed Peckin¬
pah's work as if its use of bloody squibs and slow motion was more
realistic than previous generations of Flollywood gunfights. It certainly
is bloodier, but Peckinpah's is far from a realist's aesthetic. Peckinpah's
montage set-pieces in The Wild Bunch work primarily on the level of
form rather than by their representational content. They work as
exquisitely crafted artifacts that emphasize physical spectacle. Their
design foregrounds the hyperkinetic spectacle so that it becomes a
detachable part of the film. Although, within the narrative, complex
issues of character and theme lead up to and into the film's climactic
slaughter at Mapache's headquarters, in terms of its montage design
this scene is complete, self-contained, and utterly sufficient unto itself.
For the filmmakers involved, the scene must have been a lot of fun to
197
The Aesthetic of Slow-Mvtion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
craft and edit: Herein lies a significant problem for the didactic uses to
which Peckinpah wanted to put his screen violence. This problem has
persisted, and it affects the work of virtually all filmmakers who have
chosen to stylize graphic violence by using this aesthetic approach.
Peckinpah himself noted that audience reactions were at vari¬
ance with the responses he intended viewers to have. Writing to Paul
Stamford, who had condemned the brutality of The Wild Bunch, Peck¬
inpah observed that "better than 50% of the people who saw the pic¬
ture felt as you did. However, better than 30% of the people thought it
was an outstanding and much needed statement against violence."28
Elsewhere, he admitted, "unfortunately most people come to see it [vio¬
lence] because they dig it, which is a study of human nature, and which
makes me a little sick."29
As Peckinpah recognized, viewer reactions to The Wild Bunch
included outrage and shock over the scale and explicitness of the vio¬
lence but also excitement and exhilaration. By aestheticizing it, Peck¬
inpah's montages made violence pleasurable and beautiful and turned
it into an exciting spectacle. The very stylization that Peckinpah
thought would wake people up to the horror of violence instead excited
and gratified many. This pleasure is an inevitable result of the aes¬
theticizing functions of Peckinpah's montage editing and its balletic
incorporation of slow motion, and it is a virtually inescapable effect of
the montage aesthetic that characterizes the work of all filmmakers
who have employed it. The director occasionally acknowledged his
own culpability in eliciting a dualistic response. "In The Wild Bunch I
wanted to show that violence could be at the same time repulsive and
fascinating."30
Many critics noted these contradictions in 1969 when The Wild
Bunch was released. The reviewer for The Nation confessed his own
complicity in sharing the audience's gleeful reaction to all the carnage.
"At all this the audience laughed (and so did I), not with merriment,
exactly, but in tribute to such virtuosity of gore."31 The reviewer for
The Christian Century noted that "even while gasping at Peckinpah's
bloodbath, the people seated near me in the theater continued to cram
their mouths with popcorn."32
Several perceptive reviewers pointed to the contradiction be¬
tween Peckinpah's stated aims and his montage style: the style is so rit-
ualistically elaborated in the opening and closing scenes of The Wild
Bunch that it turns violence into a pleasurable spectacle. Furthermore,
the excessive elaboration of these montages indicates that the filmmaker
took an overwhelming interest and delight in the mechanics of crafting
198
Stephen Prince
them. Arthur Knight in Saturday Review pointed out that the stylistics
of the film tend to displace the intended moral commentary. "But when
the movies attempt to show violent killing in detail, the mind turns
against the fact of death and toward the mechanics that produced it. And,
curiously, one comes away convinced that the director was also more
concerned with the mechanics than with the fact."33 Joseph Morgen-
stern in Newsweek stressed the apparent falsity of Peckinpah's premise
that the repetition of stylized violence could become an artistic device
commenting on itself.34 These reviewers are wrong in their dismissal of
Peckinpah's moral involvement with his material and in their objections
that the film is without ethical content. But they are dead right in argu¬
ing that the tendency of Peckinpah's montage -set-pieces in The Wild
Bunch is to spectacularize violence in ways that will incite the aggres¬
sive fantasies of many viewers. Recalling his first encounter with The
Wild Bunch when he was a teenager, screenwriter Charles Higson
stressed that the film's violence stimulated an "orgiastic" release of
energy in him. "Once the film was over, I was exhausted and in a state
of high nervous excitement. I wanted to go out in a blaze of glory. I
wanted a Gatling gun. I wanted to be pierced by a hundred bullets."35
Peckinpah did not overtly intend to elicit this kind of reaction in
his viewers, any more than Martin Scorsese wanted to arouse vigilante
responses in viewers of his bloody Taxi Driver (1976), but both film¬
makers crafted sequences that have done so. If we ask why, the answer
lies in the way that Peckinpah, and Scorsese, too, was seduced by the
artistic excitement of putting those violent montages together, of
manipulating time and space with images of bodies flying this way and
that. The sheer pleasure of crafting these montage scenes would have
been exhilarating for Peckinpah. That pleasure is plainly evident in the
the San Rafael massacre and the flamboyant Agua Verde shoot-out, vio¬
lent set-pieces that open and close the film. Through their dynamic
energies, these montages convey the excitement and thrill of a film¬
maker no longer in moral control of his material to the viewer, who
reacts accordingly. Discussing Taxi Driver, which climaxes with an
astoundingly bloody shoot-out, Scorsese exhibits confusion about his
intellectual intent and the seductive excitement he derives from craft¬
ing screen Violence. The audience, he says, was
human, very much the same way as you get an exhilaration from the
violence in The Wild Bunch. But the exhilaration of the violence at the
end of The Wild Bunch and the violence that's in Taxi Driver—because
it s shot a certain way, and I know how it's shot, because I shot it and
I designed it—is also in the creation of that scene in the editing, in the
camera moves, in the use of music, and the use of sound effects, and
in the movement within thefr^me of the characters. . . . And that's
where the exhilaration comes in.36
template. Thus this aesthetic, when it becomes the chief means for rep¬
resenting violent death, as it all too often has been, is an insufficient
means for probing the meaning and consequences of violence, should
those be a filmmaker's intentions. The montage aesthetic conveys
a filmmaker's delight to the audience, who is encouraged to respond
in kind. The aesthetic evokes vigilante responses. Thus does death be¬
come pleasurable and its protracted physical agonies become emptied
of human meaning.
NOTES
1. "Playboy Interview: Sam Peckinpah," Playboy 19, no. 8 (August 1972), p. 68.
2. Sam Peckinpah Collection, Margaret Herrick Library, Wild Bunch—response
letters, statement on violence, folder no. 92.
3. SPC, Wild Bunch—response letters, May 13, 1969, folder no. 92.
4. See Stephen Prince, Savage Cinema: Sam Peckinpah and the Rise of Ultravio-
lent Movies (Austin: University of Texas Press, 1998), pp. 27^4-5.
5. SPC, Wild Bunch—response letters, statement on violence, folder no. 92.
6. SPC, Wild Bunch—response letters, September 3, 1969, folder no. 92.
7. David Weddle, If They Move . . . Kill ‘Em: The Life and Times of Sam Peckin¬
pah (New York: Grove Press, 1994), pp. 99, 333.
8. SPC, Alfredo Garcia—daily log, folder no. 45.
9. SPC, funior Bonner—daily shots, folder no. 19.
10. Nat Segaloff, "Walon Green: Fate Will Get You," Backstory 3: Interviews with
Screenwriters of the 1960s ed. Pat McGilligan (Berkeley: University of California Press,
1997), pp. 143,135-56; and author's telephone conversation with Paul Seydor, July 22,1997.
11. Ernest Callenbach, "A Conversation with Sam Peckinpah," Film Quarterly 17,
no. 2 (Winter 1963-64), p. 10.
12. Weddle, p. 271.
13. SPC, Cross of Iron—script notes, letter of February 5, 1976, folder no. 26.
14. SPC, Feldman memos, October 29, 1967, folder no. 45.
15. Gary Crowdus and Richard Porton, "The Importance of a Singular, Guiding
Vision," Cineaste 20, no. 2 (Spring 1993), p. 9.
16. Stephen Farber, "Peckinpah's Return," Film Quarterly 23, no. 1 (Fall 1969), p. 11.
17. SPC, The Wild Bunch—correspondence, letter of March 19, 1968, folder no. 43;
and Weddle, p. 331.
18. Ibid.
19. Ibid., p. 5.
20. Garner Simmons, Peckinpah: A Portrait in Montage (Austin: University of
Texas Press, 1982), p. 19.
21. Bryson, "Wild Bunch in New York," p. 28.
22. Savage Cinema, pp. 72-97.
23. Seydor, p. 20.
24. Weddle, p. 271.
25. See, for example, Weddle, p. 356.
26. Weddle, p. 355.
27. SPC, Wild Bunch—editing, folder no. 40.
28. SPC, Wild Bunch—response letters, May 13, 1969, folder no. 92.
29. John Bryson, "Wild Bunch in New York," New York (August 19, 1974), p. 28.
201
The Aesthetic of Slow-Mdtion Violence in the Films of Sam Peckinpah
■
The Effects
of Ultraviolence
'
Leonard Berkowitz
Some Effects of
Thoughts on Anti- and
Prosocial Influences of Media
Events: A Cognitive-
Neoassociation Analysis
Well before the onset of the twentieth century, the French sociologist
Gabriel Tarde discussed what he termed suggesto-imitative assaults.
He noted how news of a sensational violent crime often seemed to
prompt similar incidents elsewhere, and pointed to the aftereffects of
the Jack the Ripper murders as an example. According to Tarde, the
great national attention given these brutal crimes led to eight
absolutely identical crimes in London as well as "a repetition of these
same deeds outside of the capital (and abroad)" (Tarde, 1912). "Epi¬
demics of crime," he concluded in a once famous phrase, "follow the
line of the telegraph."
Regardless of how well known Tarde's principle was several
generations ago, social scientists have only recently obtained system¬
atic quantitative support for his thesis. Berkowitz and Macaulay (1971)
reported a significant jump in violent, but not property, crimes above
what would be expected from the prevailing trend after several spec¬
tacular murders in the early and mid-1960s, including those after Pres¬
ident Kennedy's assassination. Phillips published much more
impressive evidence in a series of studies of the consequences of widely
publicized suicides. He found that when the media paid considerable
attention to a famous person's suicide, there was an increase in the
number of people who also took their lives. According to Phillips (1974)
From Psychological Bulletin, vol. 95, no. 3, 1984, pp. 410-427. Copyright © 1984 by the
American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission.
205
206
Leonard Berkowitz
the more publicity given to the suicide story, the greater the apparent
impact. In the same vein, he later demonstrated that the number of
automobile accidents increased several days after a widely publicized
suicide; many of the accidents were single-vehicle incidents (Phillips,
1979). Interestingly, in these single-vehicle cases, the driver tended to
be more like the suicide victim than were any passengers in the car.
Phillips (1980) then cast his empirical net even further and showed that
news stories of suicide-murders were followed by an increase in non¬
commercial airplane crashes. There is evidence that even the portrayal
of suicides in television soap operas led to an increase in the number of
white Americans who end their own lives (Phillips, 1982).
The implications of this research seem fairly clear: real and fic¬
tional depictions by the media of violence—killings, shootings, or
suicides—can prompt audience members to act aggressively toward
others or themselves. The present essay also argues that some of the
now familiar effects of violent scenes on television and movie screens
are similar to those producing the suggesto-imitative assaults discussed
by Tarde and documented in the research into the contagion of violence
just mentioned. In addition to these negative consequences, the mass
media can also promote prosocial behavior (Liebert, Sprafkin, & David¬
son, 1982; Rushton, 1979). A comprehensive account of media effects
should deal with both positive and negative influences.
In attempting such an analysis, this essay considers a number
of psychological processes that can contribute to these anti- and proso¬
cial effects. In particular, it focuses on the role of the audience's
thoughts. I suggest that the ideas activated by the witnessed or reported
event tend to evoke temporarily other, semantically related thoughts
in the audience that, under some circumstances, justify conduct simi¬
lar to that portrayed by the communication and even lead the viewers
to anticipate the benefits they might derive from this behavior. These
ideas also tend to evoke associated expresive-motor responses or per¬
haps even behavioral reactions that might intensify the viewer's exist¬
ing behavioral tendencies.
Along with other similar analyses, the present formulation adds the
concept of spreading activation advanced by Collins and Loftus (1975)
and others to these structural notions. This argument maintains that
when a thought element is brought into focal awareness, the activation
radiates out from, this particular node along the associative pathways
to other nodes. This process can lead to a priming effect. Although the¬
orists are not in agreement as to why the priming takes place, they gen¬
erally maintain that for some time after a concept has been activated,
there is an increased likelihood that it and associated thought elements
will come to mind again. It is as though some residual excitation has
remained at the activated node for a while, making it easier for this and
other related thoughts and feelings to be activated. Thus, the aggessive
ideas suggested by a violent movie can prime other semantically related
thoughts, heightening the chances that viewers will have other aggres¬
sive ideas in this period. All of this can happen automatically and with¬
out much thinking. Cognitive theorists now differentiate between
208
Leonard Berkowitz
The present conception does not stop with the individual's thoughts
and memories. Making use of recent theoretical developments in cog¬
nitive psychology, it holds that externally presented ideas can activate
particular feelings and even specific action tendencies as well as certain
kinds of thoughts and recollections. This formulation builds on the net¬
work analyses of emotion, advanced by Bower (1981), Clark and Isen
(1982), Lang (1979), and Leventhal (1980), which postulate associative
connections among thoughts, memories, and feelings. As an example,
Bower (1981) posited a memory-emotion network in which feelings or
emotion nodes are associatively (and, especially, semantically) related
to particular concepts and propositions and also to the autonomic
209
Some Effects of Thoughts on'Anti- and Prosocial Influences of Media Events
Figures 34 & 35. Director Paul Verhoeven (Robocop, Total Recall, Starship Troopers) has cre¬
ated some of the most extreme and outlandish violence in mainstream films. In Robocop, a police
robot machine-guns a corporate executive, whose body explodes in a shower of blood and torn
clothing. The MPAA threatened the film with an X rating unless Verhoeven trimmed its violence.
person who had insulted them earlier. The experiments all found that the
subjects attacked their tormentor more severely after seeing the "bad
guy" being assaulted than after watching the "good man" being hurt.
Berkowitz interpreted the results of his studies as being due to
differences in the perceived justification of the witnessed aggression.
That is, the subjects presumably regarded the villain's punishment as
justified aggression, whereas the "good guy's" beating was viewed as
morally unwarranted. The observers then interpreted their own possi¬
ble attacks on their tormentor in the same way. In essence, the thought
of the movie violence as good, justified aggression presumably activated
other ideas of morally proper aggression. However, other researchers
extended the original findings in a way that suggests a slightly differ¬
ent interpretation. Beginning with Hoyt (1970), investigators (Geen fit
Stonner, 1973, 1974; Meyer, 1972) showed that provoked observers
were most punitive toward their insulter if they had been led to think
of the witnessed violence as vengeance for an earlier injustice. Believ¬
ing themselves to have been ill-treated, the subjects evidently applied
the movie-activated thoughts of revenge to their own situation so that
their inhibitions against aggression were lessened. Those who had not
been angered, however, might have regarded these ideas as inappropri¬
ate or even wrong, which might have produced increased restraints
(Geen St Stonner, 1973). Geen (1976) concluded that the viewers com¬
pared their own motivational states with those of the actors in the vio¬
lent incident and used cues from this incident "as information
concerning [what is] appropriate behavior" (p. 212). Whatever the exact
meaning of the observed aggression, the context in which it occurs
clearly can affect the viewers' inhibitions against aggression.
young adults. The angry college men in their experiment were more
punitive toward their provocateur after watching a war scene that was
described as realistic rather than a staged depiction.
Research by Geen and his colleagues provides important addi¬
tional information. An experiment by Geen and Rakosky (1973) sug¬
gests that the observers' definition of the witnessed aggression as only
fictitious seemed to distance them from the event psychologically so
that it had less of an impact on them. Subjects who were reminded that
the fight they were about to see was fictional were less physiologically
aroused by it than were those subjects who were not given this
reminder. Consistent with this finding, in a later study Geen (1975)
reported that angry men shown a real fight instead of a staged one were
more likely to maintain their high level of physiological arousal during
the movie and to give their tormentor strong punishment soon after¬
ward. The subjects in these investigations were less likely to have
aggression-activating thoughts as they watched the violent incident if
they regarded the event as only fictional.
Another experiment indicates that the observer's focus on the
artistic nature of the scene can have a similar result. Leyens, Cisneros,
and Hossay (1976) asked some of their subjects to attend to the aesthetic
qualities of the aggression-related pictures shown to them. These sub¬
jects were less aggressive toward the person who had insulted them ear¬
lier than were those subjects who were also exposed to aggressive
pictures but who were not given this "decentration" instruction. In
other words, the subjects seeing the aggressive pictures without the
aesthetic focus were more aggressive than controls seeing aggressive
pictures with the aesthetic focus. The researchers noted that the atten¬
tion to the aesthetic qualities had not altered the aggressive meaning
of the pictures, so it could only be that it led to a weaker activation of
aggression-facilitating ideas.
Leyens and his associates pointed out an intriguing implication
of their findings. It may be possible to lessen the aggression-inciting
capacity of violent communications without imposing censorship by
teaching viewers to focus on the aesthetic or physical aspects of the
movies they watch. A research team led by Huesmann and Eron (Hues-
mann et al.’ 1983) followed a comparable line of reasoning, emphasiz¬
ing the reality versus fictional dimension. As part of their research
project, schoolchildren in the experimental condition were required to
write essays on why televised violence was unreal and bad.. This pro¬
cedure, based on social-psychological studies of role playing, was
intended to inculcate an unfavorable attitude toward violent programs
229
Some Effects of Thoughts on Anti- and Prosocial Influences of Media Events
Conclusion
REFERENCES
Aderman, D., St Berkowitz, L. (1970). Observational set, empathy, and helping. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 141-148.
Anderson, C. A.(1983). Imagination and expectation:The effect of imagining behavioral
scripts on personal intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
293-305.
Anderson, J., St Bower, G. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: V. H.
Winston.
Andison, F. (1977). TV violence and viewer aggression: A cumulation of study results
1956-1976. Public Opinion Quarterly, 41, 314—331.
Archer, D., St Gartner, R. (1976). Violent acts and violent times: A comparative approach
to postwar homicide rates. American Sociological Review, 41, 937-963.
Bandura, A. (1965). Influence of models' reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition
of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 589-595.
Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning Press.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A., Ross, D., St Ross, S. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3-11.
Bargh, J., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social per¬
ception: The influence of trait information presented outside of conscious aware¬
ness on impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,
437^149. Berkowitz, L. (1965). Some aspects of observed aggression. Journal of Per¬
sonality and Social Psychology, 2, 359-369.
Berkowitz, L. (1970). Aggressive humor as a stimulus to aggressive responses. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 710-717.
Berkowitz, L. (1973). Words and symbols as stimuli to aggressive responses. In J. Knutson
(Ed.), Control of aggression: Implications from basic research (pp. 113-143).
Chicago: Aldine.
Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated asso¬
ciations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychological Review, 81, 165-176.
Berkowitz, L., St Alioto, J. (1973). The meaning of an observed event as a determinant of
its aggressive consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28,
206-217.
Berkowitz, L., Corwin, R., St Heironimus, M. (1963). Film violence and subsequent
aggressive tendencies. Public Opinion Quarterly, 27, 217-229.
Berkowitz, L., St Geen, R. (1967). Stimulus qualities of the target of aggression: A further
study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 364-368.
Berkowitz, L., St Macaulay, J. (1971). The contagion of criminal violence. Sociometry, 34,
238-260.
Berkowitz, L., Parke, R., Leyens, J., St West, S. (1974). The effects of justified and unjus¬
tified movie violence on aggression in juvenile delinquents. Journal of Research in
Crime and Delinquency, 11, 16-24.
232
Leonard Berkowitz
Berkowitz, L., & Powers, P. (1979). Effects of timing and justification of witnessed aggres¬
sion on the observers' punitiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 71-80.
Berkowitz, L., & Rawlings, E. (1963). Effects of film violence on inhibitions against sub¬
sequent aggression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 405-412.
Blackman, J., St Hornstein, H. (1977). Newscasts and the social actuary. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 41, 295-313.
Bollen, K., St Phillips, D. (1982). Imitative suicides: A national study of the effects of tel¬
evision news stories. American Sociological Review, 47, 802-809.
Bower, G. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.
Buvinic, M., St Berkotyitz, L. (1976). Delayed effects of practiced versus unpracticed
responses after observation of movie violence. Journal of Experimental Social Psy¬
chology, 12, 283-293.
Carver, C., Ganellen, R., Froming, W., St Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in
terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19,
403-421.
Clark, M., St Isen, A. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling
states and social behavior. In A. Hastorf St A. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychol-
ogy (pp. 73-108). New York: Elsevier.
Collins, A., St Loftus, E. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic memory. Psy¬
chological Review, 82, 407-428.
Collins, W., St Getz, S. (1976). Children's social responses following modeled reactions to
provocation: Prosocial effects of a TV drama. Journal of Personality, 44, 488-500.
Comstock, G. (1975). Television and human behavior: The key studies. Santa Monica,
CA: Rand.
Comstock, G. (1980). New emphases in research on the effects of television and film vio¬
lence. In E. Palmer St A. Dorr (Eds.), Children and the faces of television (pp.
129-148). New York: Academic Press.
Comstock, G., Chaffee, S., Katzman, N., McCombs, M., St Roberts, D. (1978). Television
and human behavior. New York: Columbia University Press.
Cook, T. D., Kendzierski, D. A., St Thomas, S. V. (1983). The implicit assumptions of tel¬
evision research: An analysis of the 1982 NIMH report on television and behavior.
Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 161-201.
Donnerstein, E., St Berkowitz, L. (1981). Victim reactions in aggressive erotic films as a
factor in violence against women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,
710-724.
Donnerstein, E., St Berkowitz, L. (1983). Effects of film content and victim association
on aggressive behavior and attitudes. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wis-
consin-Madison.
Doob, A., St Climie, R. (1972). Delay of measurement and the effects of film violence.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 136-142.
Doob, A., St MacDonald, G. (1979). Television viewing and fear of victimization: Is the
relationship causal? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 170-179.
Dorr, A. (1981). Television and affective development and functioning: Maybe this
decade. Journal of Broadcasting, 25, 335-345.
Drabman, R., St Thomas, M. (1974). Does media violence increase children's toleration
of real-life aggression? Developmental Psychology, 10, 418-421.
Eron, L. (1982)! Parent-child interaction, television violence, and aggression of children.
American Psychologist, 37, 197-211.
Eron, L., Huesmann, L., Lefkowitz, M., St Walder, L. (1972). Does television violence
cause aggression? American Psychologist, 27, 253-263.
Feshbach, S. (1972). Reality and fantasy in filmed violence. In J. Murray, E. Rubinstein,
St G. Comstock (Eds.), Television and social behavior (vol. 2, pp. 318-345). Wash¬
ington, DC: Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
233
Some Effects of Thoughts on Anti- and Prosocial Influences of Media Events
Friedrich, L., St Stein, A. (1973). Aggressive and prosocial television programs and the nat¬
ural behavior of preschool children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 38 (4, Serial No. 151).
Geen, R. (1975). The meaning of observed violence: Real versus fictional violence and
effects of aggression and emotional arousal. Journal of Research in Personality, 9,
270-281.
Geen, R. (1976). Observing violence in the mass media: Implications of basic research. In
R. Geen St E. O'Neal (Eds.), Perspectives on aggression (pp. 193-234). New York:
Academic Press. t *
Geen, R. (1981). Behavioral and physiological reactions to observed violence: Effects of
prior exposure to aggressive stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
40, 868-875.
Geen, R., St Berkowitz, L. (1966). Name-mediated aggressive cue properties. Journal of
Personality, 34, 456-465.
Geen, R., St Berkowitz, L. (1967). Some conditions facilitating the occurrence of aggres¬
sion after the observation of violence. Journal of Personality, 35, 666-676.
Geen, R., St Rakosky, J. (1973). Interpretations of observed violence and their effects of
GSR. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 6, 289-292.
Geen, R., St Stonner, D. (1973). Context effects in observed violence. Journal of Person¬
ality and Social Psychology, 25, 145-150.
Geen, R., St Stonner, D. (1974). The meaning of observed violence: Effects on arousal and
aggressive behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 55-63.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., St Signorielli, N. (1980). The "mainstreaming" of
America: Violence profile No. 11. Journal of Communication, 30, 10-29.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Signorielli, N., Morgan, M., St Jackson-Beeck, M. (1979). The demon¬
stration of power. Violence profile No. 10. Journal of Communication, 29, 177-195.
Goranson, R. (1969). Observed violence and aggressive behavior: The effects of negative
outcomes to the observed violence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University
of Wisconsin.
Goranson, R. (1970). Media violence and aggressive behavior: A review of experimental
research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 5,
pp. 1-31). New York: Academic Press.
Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With
special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73-99.
Hawkins, R., St Pingree, S. (1980). Some processes in the cultivation effect. Communi¬
cation Research, 7, 193-226.
Hearold, S. (1979). Meta-analysis of the effects of television on social behavior. Unpub¬
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado.
Holloway, S., Tucker, L., St Hornstein, H. (1977). The effects of social and nonsocial infor¬
mation on interpersonal behavior of males: The news makes news. Journal of Per¬
sonality and Social Psychology, 35, 514-522.
Hornstein, H. A., LaKind, E., Frankel, G., St Manne, S. (1975). The effects of knowledge
about remote social events on prosocial behavior, social conception, and mood.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1038-1046.
Hoyt, f. (1970). Effect of media violence "justification" on aggression. Journal of Broad¬
casting, 16, 455-464.
Huesmann, L., Eron, L., Klein, L., Brice, P., St^Fischer, P. (1983). Mitigating the imitation
of aggressive behaviors by changing children's attitudes about media violence. Jour¬
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 899-910.
Iwao, S., de Sola Pool, I., St Hagiwara, S. (1981). Japanese and U.S. media: Some cross-
cultural insights in TV violence. Journal of Communication, 31, 28-36.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.
Josephson, W. (1982). The effects of violent television upon children’s aggression: Elici-
234
Leonard Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 10, pp. 136-172). New York:
Academic Press.
Phillips, D. (1974). The influence of suggestion on suicide: Substantive and theoretical
implications of the Werther effect. American Sociological Review, 39, 340-354.
Phillips, D. (1979). Suicide, motor vehicle fatalities, and the mass media: Evidence toward
a theory of suggestion. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 1150-1174.
Phillips, D. (1980). Airplane accidents, murder, and the mass media: Towards a theory of
imitation and suggestion. Social Forces, 58, 1001-1024.
Phillips, D. (1982). The impact of fictional television stories on U.S. adult fatalities: New
evidence on the effect of mass media on violence. American Journal of Sociology,
87, 1340-1359.
Phillips, D. (1983). The impact of mass media violence on U.S. homicides. American Soci¬
ological Review, 48, 560-568.
Rosenfeld, E., Maloney, S., Huesmann, R., Eron, L., Fischer, P, Musonis, V., St Washing¬
ton, A. (1978). The effect of fantasy behaviors and fantasy-reality discriminations
upon the observational learning of aggression. Paper presented at the annual meet¬
ing of the International Society for Research on Aggression, Washington, DC.
Rushton, f. (1979). Effects of prosocial television and film material on the behavior of
viewers. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 12,
pp. 322-351). New York: Academic Press.
Shiffrin, R., St Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information pro¬
cessing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psycho¬
logical Review, 84, 127-190.
Silverman, L., St Sprafkin, J. (1980). The effects of Sesame Street's prosocial spots on coop¬
erative-play between young children. Journal of Broadcasting, 24, 135-147.
Singer, J., St Singer, D. (1981J. Television, imagination and aggression. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Sprafkin, J., Liebert, R., St Poulos, R. (1975). Effects of a prosocial televised example on
children's helping. Journal of Experimental Child, 20, 119-126.
Tannenbaum, P, St Gaer, E. P. (1965). Mood changes as a function of stress of protagonist
and degree of identification in a film-viewing situation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 2, 612-616.
Tarde, G. (1912). Penal philosophy. Boston: Little, Brown.
Thomas, M. (1982). Physiological arousal, exposure to a relatively lengthy aggressive
film, and aggressive behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 16, 72-81.
Thomas, M., St Drabman, R. (1977). Effects of television violence on expectations of oth¬
ers’ aggression. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychologi¬
cal Association, San Francisco, CA.
Thomas, M., Horton, R., Lippincott, E., St Drabman, R. (1977). Desensitization to por¬
trayals of real-life aggression as a function of exposure to television violence. Jour¬
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 450-458.
Turner, C., St Berkowitz, L. (1972). Identification with film aggressor (covert role taking)
and reactions to film violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21,
256-264.
Turner, C., St Layton, J. (1976). Verbal imagery and connotation as memory induced medi¬
ators of aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 755-763.
Tyler, I. R. (1980). Impact of directly and indirectly experienced events: The origin of
crime-related judgments and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol¬
ogy, 39, 13-28.
Vaughan, K., St Lanzetta, J. (1981). The effect of modification of expressive displays on
vicarious emotional arousal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 16-30.
Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for the induction of mood states. Behavior Research
and Therapy, 6, 473-482.
236
Leonard Berkowitz
i
Richard B. Felson
With permission from the Annual Review of Sociology, vol. 22, 1996, pp. 103-128,
© 1996, by Annual Reviews.
237
238
Richard B. Felson
Figure 36. Harvey Keitel takes aim in Quentin Tarantino’s Reservoir Dogs, a film awash in
blood and mayhem.
Laboratory Experiments
Laboratory experiments examine short-term effects of media violence.
Most studies show that subjects in laboratory experiments who observe
media violence tend to behave more aggressively than do subjects in
control groups. A meta-analysis of these studies reveals consistent and
substantial media effects (Andison, 1977). However, research is incon¬
sistent in showing whether it is necessary to provoke subjects before
showing violence to get an effect (Freedman, 1984). Thus, it is not clear
whether media exposure acts as an instigator of aggression in the labo¬
ratory or merely as a facilitator.
Researchers have raised questions about the external validity of
laboratory experiments in this area (Freedman, 1984; Cook et al., 1983).
They point out that the laboratory situation is very different from
situations leading to violence outside the laboratory (e.g., Tedeschi &
Felson, 1994). For subjects to engage in aggressive behavior in the lab¬
oratory, the behavior must be legitimated. Subjects are told, for exam¬
ple, that the delivery of shocks is a teaching method or part of a game.
Subjects are then subjected to an attack by a confederate and given a
chance to retaliate. Unlike aggressive behavior outside the laboratory,
there is no possibility that this will be punished by third parties or that
this will subject them to retaliation from the target. It is unknown to
what extent these differences limit the generalizability of experimen¬
tal studies. Evidence suggests that aggression measures in many labo¬
ratory studies do involve an intent to harm (Rerkowitz &. Donnerstein,
1982). Experimental subjects may not be so different from those who
engage in violence outside the laboratory, who see their behavior as
legitimate, and who do not consider its costs.3
The demand cues in these studies are probably a more significant
problem. Demand cues are instructions or other stimuli that indicate to
subjects how the experimenter expects them to behave.4 Experimenters
who show violent films are likely to communicate a message about their
attitudes toward aggression. A violent film may imply to subjects that
the experimenter is a permissive adult or someone not particularly
240
Richard B. Felson
offended by violence. Just a few subjects aware of the demand and com¬
pliant could account for the mean differences in aggression found
between experimental conditions.
The laboratory is a setting that exaggerates the effects of con¬
formity and social influence (see Gottfredson & FFirschi, 1993). The
extent of compliance in laboratory settings is dramatically demon¬
strated in Milgram's (1974) well-known research on obedient aggres¬
sion. Subjects' behavior is easily influenced for at least three reasons:
(1) The standards for behavior are unclear and the situation is novel
(Nemeth, 1970); (2) subjects are influenced by the prestige of the ex¬
perimenter and the scientific enterprise,- (3) subjects want to avoid
being perceived as psychologically maladjusted by the psychologist-
experimenter (Rosenberg, 1969).
Field Experiments
These studies take advantage of the fact that television was introduced
at different times in different locations. They assume that people who
are exposed to television will also be exposed to a high dose of televi¬
sion violence. This is probably a reasonable assumption given the
extremely high correlation between television viewing and exposure to
television violence (Milavsky et al., 1982).
Hennigan et al. (1982) compared crime rates in American cities
that already had television with those that did not. No effect of the
presence or absence of television was found on violent crime rates in a
comparison of the two kinds of cities. Furthermore, when cities with¬
out television obtained it, there was no increase in violent crime. There
was an increase in the incidence of larceny, which the authors attrib¬
uted to relative deprivation suffered by viewers observing affluent
people on television.6
Joy et al. (1986) examined changes in the aggressive behavior of
children after television was introduced into an isolated Canadian town
in the 1970s. The town was compared to two supposedly comparable
towns that already had television. Forty-five children in the three
towns were observed on the school playground in first and second grade
and then again two years later. The frequency of both verbal and phys¬
ical aggression increased in all three communities, but the increase was
significantly greater in the community in which television was intro¬
duced during the study. Some of the results were not consistent with a
television effect, however. In the first phase of the study, the children
in the community without television were just as aggressive as the chil¬
dren in the communities that already had television. Without televi¬
sion they should have been less aggressive. The children in the
community where television was introduced then became more aggres¬
sive than the children in the other communities in the second phase,
when all three communities had television. At this point, the level of
aggressive behavior in the three communities should have been simi¬
lar. To accept the findings, one must assume that the community with¬
out television at the beginning of the study had more aggressive
children than the other communities for other reasons, but that this
242
Richard B. Felson
effect was counteracted in the first phase by the fact that they were not
exposed to television. That assumption implies that there are other dif¬
ferences between the communities and thus casts doubt on the findings
of the study.
Centerwall (1989) examined the relationship between homi¬
cide rates and the introduction of television in three countries: South
Africa, Canada, and the United States. Television was introduced in
South Africa in 1975, about twenty-five years after Canada and the
United States. The white homicide rate increased dramatically in the
United States and Canada about fifteen years after the introduction of
television, when the first generation of children who had access to tel¬
evision were entering adulthood. The white homicide rate declined
slightly in South Africa during this time period. While Centerwall ruled
out some confounding factors (e.g., differences in economic develop¬
ment), causal inference is difficult, given the many differences between
the countries involved. In addition, Centerwall could not determine at
the time he wrote whether the level of violence had increased fifteen
years after the introduction of television in South Africa,- thus an impor¬
tant piece of evidence was missing.
Centerwall also examined the effect of the introduction of tel¬
evision in the United States. He found that urban areas acquired tele¬
vision before rural areas, and their homicide rates increased earlier.
However, social changes in general are likely to occur in urban areas
before they occur in rural areas. He also found that households of
whites acquired television sets before households of blacks, and their
homicide rates increased earlier as well. It is difficult to imagine an
alternative explanation of this effect.
Still, the methodological limitations of these studies make it
difficult to have confidence in a causal inference about media effects.
The substantial differences between the comparison groups increase
the risk that the relationship between the introduction of television
and increases in aggression is spurious.
fighter and the crime victims were similar in race and sex. The loss of
prize fights by white fighters was followed by increases in deaths
through homicide of white males on days 2 and 8. The loss of prize
fights by blacks was followed by an increase in homicide deaths for
black males on days 4 and 5. The rise in the homicide rate was not can¬
celed out by a subsequent drop, suggesting that the prize fights affected
the incidence and not just the timing of homicides.
Baron and Reiss (1985) attribute these effects to the fact that
prize fights tend to occur during the week and homicides are more
likely to occur on weekends. They were able to replicate Phillips's find¬
ings by selecting weeks without prizefights and pretending that they
had occurred. In response to this critique, Phillips and Bollen (1985)
selected different weeks and showed that the weekend effect could not
account for all of the findings. Miller et al. (1991) replicated some of
Phillips's results, but found that the effect only occurred on Saturdays
following highly publicized fights.
Freedman (1984) has criticized Phillips's research on other
methodological grounds, and Phillips (1986) has addressed these criti¬
cisms. There are still unresolved questions, such as why effects tended
to occur on different days for different races. In addition, experimental
results suggest that watching boxing films does not affect the viewer's
aggressive behavior. Geen (1978) found that, when provoked, college
students were more aggressive after viewing vengeful aggression but
not after viewing a boxing match (see also Hoyt, 1970).
Longitudinal Surveys
Richard B. Felson
to television violence, and Milavsky et al. did not replicate that effect.
The findings reported in the cross-national studies were inconsistent
and had as many negative findings as positive ones. Therefore one must
conclude that longitudinal studies have not demonstrated a relation¬
ship between the amount of violence viewed on television and subse¬
quent aggressive behavior.9
Cognitive Priming
whose name was either Bob or Kirk (Berkowitz St Geen, 1966). Subjects
gave the confederate the most shocks in the condition when they had
been provoked, had viewed the violent film, and the confederate had
the same name as the film's star.
Tedeschi and Norman (1985) attribute the results from these
studies to demand cues (see also Tedeschi &. Felson, 1994). They point
out that experimenters mention the1'fact that the confederate's first
name is the same as Kirk Douglas's in their instructions, and that they
justify to subjects the beating that Kirk Douglas received. A series of
studies have shown that it is necessary to provide this justification to
get a violent film effect (Geen &. Berkowitz, 1967; Berkowitz, 1965;
Berkowitz et al., 1962; Berkowitz St Rawlings, 1963; Meyer, 1972b).
Josephson (1987) examined the combined effects of exposure to
a violent film and retrieval cues in a field experiment with second- and
third-grade boys. The boys were exposed to either a violent film (in
which a walkie-talkie was used) or a nonviolent film. The boys were also
frustrated either before or after the film. Later they were interviewed by
someone holding either a walkie-talkie or a microphone. After the inter¬
view, the boys played a game of field hockey and their aggressive behav¬
ior was recorded. It was predicted that boys who were exposed to both
violent television and a walkie-talkie would be most aggressive in the
game, since the walkie-talkie would lead them to retrieve scripts asso¬
ciated with the violent film. The hypothesis was confirmed for boys who
were, according to teacher ratings, aggressive. Boys who were identified
as nonaggressive inhibited their aggression when exposed to the walkie-
talkie and the film. Josephson suggested that for these nonaggressive
boys, aggression may be strongly associated with negative emotions
such as guilt and fear which, when primed, may inhibit aggression. If we
accept this post hoc interpretation, it suggests that media violence may
increase or inhibit the violent behavior of viewers, depending on their
initial predisposition. Such effects are likely to be short-term, and they
may have no effect on the overall rate of violence.
Sponsor Effects
/
Richard B. Felson
It is widely believed that people are more violent because they learn to be
violent from their parents, their peers, and the mass media. These social¬
ization effects tend to endure since they involve changes in the individ¬
ual. The evidence on the versatility of criminal offenders casts doubt on
the importance of this socialization process. Considerable evidence
252
Richard B. Felson
suggests that those who commit violent crime tend to commit nonvio¬
lent crime and other deviant acts as well. Studies of arrest histories based
on both official records and self-reports show a low level of specializa¬
tion in violent crime. For example, West and Farrington (1977) found
that 80 percent of adults convicted of violence also had convictions for
crimes involving dishonesty. Violent acts were also related to non¬
criminal forms of deviant behavior, such as sexual promiscuity, smok¬
ing, heavy drinking, gambling, and having an unstable job history.
The evidence that most offenders are versatile challenges the
notion that violent offenders are more violent because of a special pro¬
clivity to engage in violence, due to exposure to media violence or any
other factor. Individual differences in the propensity to engage in crim¬
inal violence reflect for the most part individual differences in antiso¬
cial behavior generally. Variations in the socialization of self-control
and other inhibitory factors are probably important causal factors
(Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Theories that emphasize specific social¬
ization to violence are likely to be limited in their utility, since most
violent offenders are generalists.
The versatility argument should not be overstated. Some peo¬
ple do specialize in violence, and exposure to media violence may play
a role in their socialization. There are a variety of reasons one might
expect viewers to learn aggressive behavior from the media. First,
media depictions of violence may suggest novel behaviors to viewers
that they otherwise might not have considered. Second, vicarious rein¬
forcements and legitimation of violent actions may increase the ten¬
dency to model media violence. Third, viewers become desensitized
about violence after a heavy diet of it on television. Finally, people may
get a false idea of reality from observing a great deal of violence on tel¬
evision and develop unrealistic fears. I now examine each of the
processes more closely.
Bandura (1983) has argued that television can shape the forms that
aggressive behavior takes. Television can teach skills that may be use¬
ful for committing acts of violence, and it can direct viewers' attention
to behaviors that they may not have considered. For example, young
people may mimic karate and judo moves, or they may learn effective
tactics for committing violent crime. This information may give direc¬
tion to those who are already motivated to engage in aggression. Such a
253
Bandura (1983) also suggested that television may inform viewers of the
positive and negative consequences of violent behavior. Audiences can
be expected to imitate violent behavior that is successful in gaining the
model's objectives in fictional or nonfictional programs. When violence
is justified or left unpunished on television, the viewer's guilt or con¬
cern about consequences is reduced. Thus Paik and Comstock's (1994)
meta-analysis found that the magnitude of media effects on antisocial
behavior was greater when the violent actor was rewarded or the behav¬
ior was legitimated.
It is not at all clear what message is learned from viewing vio¬
lence on television. In most plots, the protagonist uses violence for
legitimate ends while the villain engages in illegitimate violence. The
protagonist usually uses violence in self-defense or to mete out an
appropriate level of punishment to a dangerous or threatening criminal.
Television conveys the message that while some forms of violence are
necessary and legitimate, criminal violence is evil.
The consequences of the illegitimate violence portrayed in fic-
254
Richard B. Felson
tional television and film are more negative than the consequences of ille¬
gitimate violence in real life. In real life violent people often evade pun¬
ishment, while in television, the villain is almost always punished. Thus,
one could argue that television violence might reduce the incidence of
criminal violence, since crime doesn't pay for TV criminals. Another
difference is in the appeal of those who engage in illegitimate violence.
In fictional television, those who engage in illegitimate violence tend
to lack any attractive qualities that would lead to sympathy or identi¬
fication. In real life, illegitimate violence may be committed by loved
ones or others who are perceived to have desirable qualities.
Other factors may limit the effects of any message about the
legitimacy, or the rewards and costs of violence. First, the lessons
learned from the media about violence may be similar or redundant to
the lessons learned about the use of violence conveyed by other sources.
In fact, most viewers probably approve of the violent behavior of the
protagonists. The influence of television on viewers who already agree
with its message would be weak at best. Second, the audience may not
take the message from fictional plots seriously. Modeling is more likely
to occur after viewing nonfiction than after viewing fiction (Feshbach,
1972; Berkowitz &. Alioto, 1973).11 Third, the violent contexts and
provocations observed on television are likely to be very different from
the contexts and provocations people experience in their own lives. Evi¬
dence suggests that viewers take context and intentions into account
before they model aggressive behavior (Geen, 1978; Floyt, 1970). Straus
(Baron &. Straus, 1987), on the other hand, suggests that people are
likely to be influenced by the violence they observe regardless of its
context, message, or legitimacy. According to cultural spillover theory,
violence in one sphere of life leads to violence in other spheres.
Finally, some young children may miss the more subtle aspects
of television messages, focusing on overt acts rather than on the inten¬
tions or contexts in which such acts occur. Collins et al. (1984) found
that kindergarten and second-grade children were relatively unaffected
by an aggressor's motives in their understanding of a violent program.
They focused more on the aggressiveness of the behavior and its ulti¬
mate consequences. However, even if young children imitate the vio¬
lence of models, it is not at all clear that they will continue to exhibit
violence as they get older. When they are older, and they pay attention
to the intentions and context in violent television, their behavior is
more likely to reflect the messages they learn. It is also at these later
ages that violent behavior, if it should occur, is likely to be dangerous.
255
%
Desensitization
Frequent viewing of television violence may cause viewers to be less
anxious and sensitive about violence. Someone who becomes desensi¬
tized to violence may be more likely to engage in violence. This argu¬
ment assumes that anxiety about violence inhibits its use.
Desensitization has been examined indirectly using measures
of arousal. Research shows that subjects who view violent films are less
aroused by violence later on (Thomas et al., 1977; see Rule &. Ferguson,
1986 for a review). In addition, heavy viewers of television violence
tend to respond less emotionally to violence than do light viewers.
There is no evidence that desensitization produces lower levels of
violent behavior.13 Nor is it clear what effect should occur. Studies of desen¬
sitization measure arousal not anxiety) and arousal can facilitate violent
behavior, according to the literature cited earlier (e.g., Zillmann, 1983). If
viewers are exposed to a heavy diet of television violence, one might argue
that they will be less aroused by violence and therefore less likely to engage
in violence. In addition, if viewers become desensitized to violent behavior
on television, they may become indifferent to its message. Desensitization
could thereby weaken the effect of a heavy diet of television violence.
Richard B. Felson
1989). Other scholars have concluded that the causal effects of expo¬
sure to television have not been demonstrated (Freedman, 1984;
McGuire, 1989).
Given the pervasiveness of media violence, it would be sur¬
prising if it had no effect on viewers. I agree with those scholars who
think that exposure to television violence probably does have a small
effect on violent behavior (Cook et al., 1983). The reason that media
effects are not consistently observed is probably because they are weak
and affect only a small percentage of viewers. These weak effects may
still have practical importance since, in a large population, they would
produce some death and injuries. However, it seems unlikely that
media violence is a significant factor in high crime rates in this coun¬
try. Changes in violent crimes mirror changes in crime rates generally.
In addition, the people who engage in criminal violence also commit
other types of crime. An explanation that attributes violent behavior to
socialization that encourages violence cannot easily explain the versa¬
tility of most violent criminals.
It seems likely that some people would be more susceptible to
media influence than others. Therefore it is puzzling that research has
not shown any consistent statistical interactions involving individual
difference factors and media exposure. The failure to find individual dif¬
ference factors that condition the effects of media exposure on aggres¬
sive behavior contributes to skepticism about media effects.
It seems reasonable to believe that the media directs viewers'
attention to novel forms of violent behavior they might not otherwise
consider. The anecdotal evidence is convincing in this area. There
appear to be documented cases in which bizarre events on television
are followed by similar events in the real world; the similarities seem
too great to be coincidental. In addition, hijackings and political vio¬
lence tend to occur in waves. Many parents have observed their chil¬
dren mimicking behaviors they've observed in films. Whether this
process leads to a greater frequency of violence is unclear.
There is some evidence that the effects observed in laboratory
experiments, and less consistently in field experiments, are due to spon¬
sor effects. The fact that children who are exposed to violence tend to
misbehave generally casts doubt on tiiost of the other theoretical expla¬
nations of media effects. The issue has particular significance for labora¬
tory research, where subjects know they are being studied and may be
responding to demand cues. Research is needed in which sponsor effects
are isolated and controlled. A field experiment in which subjects imitate
260
Richard B. Felson
violent behavior they have observed in the absence of the sponsor, but
do not misbehave otherwise, would be convincing. Alternatively, there
may need to be further development of the theoretical argument that
self-control behavior is modeled.
It is not clear what lesson the media teaches about the legiti¬
macy of violence, or the likelihood of punishment. To some extent that
message is redundant with lessons learned from other sources of influ¬
ence. The message is probably ambiguous and is likely to have different
effects on different viewers. Young children may imitate illegitimate
violence if they do not understand the message, but their imitative
behavior may have trivial consequences. Out of millions of viewers,
there must be some with highly idiosyncratic interpretations of televi¬
sion content who intertwine the fantasy with their own lives, and as a
result have an increased probability of engaging in violent behavior.
NOTES
REFERENCES
Richard B. Felson
Berkowitz, L., Corwin, R.; St Heironimus, M. (1962). Film violence and subsequent
aggressive tendencies. Public Opinion Quarterly, 27, 217-229.
Berkowitz, L., fit Donnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than skin deep: Some
answers to criticism of laboratory experiments. American Psychologist, 37, 245-257.
Berkowitz, L., & Geen, R. G. (1966). Film violence and the cue properties of available tar¬
gets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 525-530.
Berkowitz, L.; fit Rawlings, E. (1963). Effects of film violence: An inhibition against sub¬
sequent aggression. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 66, 405-412.
Boeringer, S. (1994). Pornography and sexual aggression: Associations of violent and non¬
violent depictions with rape and rape proclivity. Deviant Behavior, 15, 289-304.
Bushman, B. J., & Geen, R. G. (1990). Role of cognitive-emotional mediators and indi¬
vidual differences in the effects of media violence on aggression. Journal of Person¬
ality and Social Psycholology, 58, 156-163.
Centerwall, B. S. (1989). Exposure to television as a cause of violence. In G. Comstock
(Ed.), Public communication and behavior (vol. 2, pp. 1-58. Orlando: Academic.
Collins, W. A., Berndt, T. J., fit Hess, V. L. (1984). Observational learning of motives and
consequences for television aggression: A developmental study. Child Develop¬
ment, 45, 799-802.
Cook, T. D., Kendzierski, D. A., fit Thomas, S. V. (1983). The implicit assumptions of tel¬
evision: An analysis of the 1982 NIMH Report on Television and Behavior. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 47, 161-201.
Demare, D., Lips, H. M., St Briere, J. (1993). Sexually violent pornography, anti-women
attitudes, and sexual aggression: A structural equation model. Journal of Research
in Personality, 27, 285-300.
Dimmick, J. W., McCain, T. A., & Bolton, W. T. (1979). Media use and the life span. Amer¬
ican Behavioral Scientist, 23, 7-31.
Donnerstein, E. (1980). Aggressive erotica and violence against women. Journal of Per¬
sonality and Social Psychology, 39, 269-277.
Donnerstein, E. (1984). Pornography: Its effect on violence against women. In N. M. Mala-
muth St E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Pornography and sexual aggression (pp. 53-81). New
York: Academic.
Donnerstein, E., Donnerstein, M., St Evans, R. (1975). Erotic stimuli and aggression: Facil¬
itation or inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 237-244.
Dworkin, A. (1981). Pornography: Men possessing women. New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons.
Eron. L. D., Huesmann, L. R., Lefkowitz, M. M., St Walder, L. O. (1972). Does television
violence cause aggression? American Psychologist, 27, 253-263.
Felson, M. (1986). Routine activities, social controls, rational decisions and criminal out¬
comes. In D. Cornish St R. Clarke (Eds.), The reasoning criminal: Rational choice
perspectives on offending. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Feshbach, S. (1972). Reality and fantasy in filmed violence. In J. P. Murray, E. Rubinstein,
St G. A. Comstock (Eds.), Television and social behavior (vol. 2, pp. 318-345). Wash¬
ington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Feshbach, S., fit Singer, R. (1971). Television and aggression. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
Fisher, W. A., St Grenier, G. (1994). Violent pornography, antiwoman thoughts, and anti¬
woman acts: In search of reliable effects. Journal of Sex Research, 31. 23-38.
Fraczek, A. (19^6). Socio-cultural environment, television viewing, and the development
of aggression among children in Poland. In L. R. Huesmann St L. D. Eron (Eds.), Tele¬
vision and the aggressive child: A cross-national comparison (pp. 119-160. Hills¬
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Freedman, J. L. (1984). Effects of television violence on aggressiveness. Psychological Bul¬
letin, 96, 227-246.
Friedrich, L. K., St Stein, A. H. (1973). Aggressive and prosocial television programs and
263
the natural behavior of preschool children. Monograph of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 38 (4, Serial No. 151).
Friedrich-Cofer, L., St Huston, A. C. (1986). Television violence and aggression: The
debate continues. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 364-371.
Garcia, L. T., St Milano, L. (1990). A content analysis of erotic videos. Journal of Law Psy¬
chiatry, 14, 47-64.
Geen, R. G. (1978). Some effects of observing violence upon the behavior of the observer.
In B. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (vol. 8). New York:
Academic. ,
Geen, R. G. (1983). Aggression and television violence. In R. G. Geen St E. I. Donnerstein
(Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (vol. 2, pp. 103-125). New
York: Academic.
Geen, R. G, St Berkowitz, L. (1967). Some conditions facilitating the occurrence of aggres¬
sion after the observation of violence. Journal of Personality, 35, 666-676.
Gentry, C. S. (1991). Pornography and rape: An empirical analysis. Deviant Behavior, 12,
277-288.
Gerbner, G., St Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence profile. Journal of
Communication, 26, 173-199.
Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford: Stanford Uni¬
versity Press.
Gottfredson, M., St Hirschi, T. (1993). A control theory interpretation of psychological
research on aggression. In R. B. Felson St J. T. Tedeschi (Eds.), Aggression and vio¬
lence: Social interactionist perspectives (pp. 47-68). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Gunter, B. (1994). The question of media violence. In J. Bryant St D. Zillman (Eds.), Media
effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 163-212). Hillsdale, Nf: Erlbaum.
Hall, E. R., Howard, J. A., St Boezio, S. L. (1986). Tolerance of rape: A sexist or antisocial
attitude. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 10, 101-118.
Hearold, S. (1986). A synthesis of 1043 effects of television on social behavior. In G. Com¬
stock (Ed.), Public communication and behavior (vol. 1, pp. 65-133). San Diego, CA:
Academic.
Hennigan, K. M., Del Rosario, M. L., Heath, L., Cook, T. D., Wharton, J. D., St Calder, B.
f. (1982). The impact of the introduction of television on crime in the United States.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 461-477.
Howells, K., St Wright, E. (1978). The sexual attitudes of aggressive sexual offenders.
British Journal of Crimnology, 18, 170-173.
Hoyt, J. L. (1970). Effect of media violence 'justification' on aggression. Journal of Broad¬
casting, 14, 455^-64.
Huesmann, L. R. (1982). Television violence and aggressive behavior. In D. Pearl, L.
Bouthilet, St f. Lazar (Eds.), Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific progress
and implications for the eighties (vol. 2, pp. 220-256). Washington, DC: National
Institute of Mental Health.
Huesmann, L. R., St Eron, L. D. (1986). The development of aggression in American chil¬
dren as a consequence of television violence viewing. In L. R. Huesmann St L. D.
Eron (Eds.), Television and the aggressive child: A cross-national comparison (pp.
45-80). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
fosephson, W. L. (1987). Television violence and children's aggression: Testing the prim¬
ing, social script, and disinhibition predictions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 53, 882890.
Joy, L. A., Kimball, M. M., St Zaback, M. L. (1986). Television and children's aggressive
behavior. In T. M. Williams (Ed.), The impact of television: A natural experiment in
three communities (pp. 303-360). New York: Academic.
264
Richard B. Felson
Kanin, E. J. (1969). Selected dyadic aspect of male sex aggression. Journal of Sex Research,
5, 12-28.
Koss, M. P., Gidycz, C. A., St Wisniewski, N. (1987). The scope of rape: Incidence and
prevalence of sexual aggression and victimization in a national sample of students
in higher education. Journal of Consultation in Clinical Psychology, 55, 162-170.
Koss, M. P., Leonard, K. E., Beezley, D. A., St Oros, C. J. (1985). Non-stranger sexual aggres¬
sion: a discriminate analysis classification. Sex Roles, 12, 981-992.
Kruttschnitt. C. (1989). A sociological, offender-based, study of rape. Sociological Quar¬
terly, 30, 305-329.
Lagerspetz, K., St Viemero, V. (1986). Television and aggressive behavior among Finnish
children. In L. R. Huesmann St L. D. Eron (Eds.), Television and the aggressive child:
A cross-national comparison (pp. 81-118). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leyens, J. P., Camino, L., Parke, R. D.; St Berkowitz, L. (1975). Effects of movie violence
on aggression in a field setting as a function of group dominance and cohesion. Jour¬
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 346-360.
Linz, D. (1989). Exposure to sexually explicit materials and attitudes toward rape: A com¬
parison of study results. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 50-84.
Linz, D., St Malamuth, N. (1993). Pornography. Newbury Park: Sage.
Loren, R. E. A., St Weeks, G. (1986). Sexual fantasies of undergraduates and their percep¬
tions of the sexual fantasies of the opposite sex. Journal of Sex Education Therapy,
12, 31-36.
MacKinnon, C. (1984). Not a moral issue. Yale Law Policy Review, 2, 321-345.
Malamuth, N. M. (1986). Predictors of naturalistic sexual aggression. Journal of Person¬
ality and Social Psychology, 50, 953-962.
Malamuth, N. M., St Check, J. V. P. (1981). The effects of mass media exposure on accept¬
ance of violence against women: A field experiment. Journal of Research in Person¬
ality, 15, 436-446.
McGuire, W. J. (1986). The myth of massive media impact: Savagings and salvagings. In
G. Comstock (Ed.), Public communication and behavior (vol. 1, pp. 175-257).
Orlando: Academic.
Messner, S. F. (1986). Television violence and violent crime: An aggregate analysis. Social
Problems, 33, 218-235.
Messner, S. F., St Blau, J. R. (1987). Routine leisure activities and rates of crime: A macro¬
level analysis. Social Forces, 65, 1035-1052.
Meyer, T. P. (1972a). The effects of sexually arousing and violent films on aggressive
behavior. Journal of Sex Research, 8, 324-331.
Meyer, T. P. (1972b). Effects of viewing justified and unjustified real film violence on
aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 21-29.
Milavsky, J. R., Stipp, H. H., Kessler, R. C., St Rubens, W. S. (1982). Television and aggres¬
sion: A panel study. New York: Academic.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York: Harper St
Row.
Miller, T. Q., Heath, L., Molcan, f. R., St Dugoni, B. L. (1991). Imitative violence in the
real world: A reanalysis of homicide rates following championship prize fights.
Aggressive Behavior, 17, 121-134.
Neiss, R. (1988f). Reconceptualizing arousal: Psychobiological states in motor perform¬
ance. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 345-366.
Nemeth, C. (1970). Bargaining and reciprocity. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 297-308.
Ogles, R. M. (1987). Cultivation analysis: Theory, methodology and current research on
television-influenced constructions of social reality. Mass Communication Review,
14, 43-53.
Padget, V. R., Brislin-Slutz, J., St Neal, J. A. (1989). Pornography, erotica, and attitudes
265
toward women: The effects of repeated exposure. Journal of Sex Research, 26,
479-491.
Paik, H., Si Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of television violence on antisocial behav¬
ior: A meta-analysis. Communication Research, 21, 516-545.
Palys, T. S. (1986). Testing the common wisdom: The social content of video pornogra¬
phy. Canadian Psychology, 27, 22-35.
Parke, R. D., Berkowitz, L., Leyens, J. P., West, S., St Sebastian, R. J. (1977). Some effects
of violent and nonviolent movies on the behavior of juvenile delinquents. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 10, pp. 135-172).
New York: Academic.
Phillips, D. P. (1983). The impact of mass media violence on U.S. homicides. American
Sociological Review, 48, 560-568.
Phillips, D. P. (1986). The found experiment: A new technique for assessing the impact
of mass media violence on real-world aggressive behavior. In G. Comstock (Ed.), Pub¬
lic communication and behavior (vol. 1, pp. 259-307). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Phillips, D. P., St Bollen, K. A. (1985). Same time last year: Selective data dredging for
unreliable findings. American Sociological Review, 50, 364-371.
Rapaport, K., St Burkhart, B. R. (1984). Personality and attitudinal characteristics of sex¬
ually coercive college males. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 216-221.
Reiss, I. L. (1986). Journey into sexuality: An exploratory voyage. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Rosenberg, M. f. (1969). The conditions and consequences of evaluation apprehension. In R.
Rosenthal Si R. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifacts in behavioral research. New York: Academic.
Rule, B. G., St Ferguson, T. J. (1986). The effects of media violence on attitudes, emotions,
and cognitions. Journal of Social Issues, 42, 29-50.
Sapolsky, B. S. (1984). Arousal, affect, and the aggression-moderating effect of erotica. In
N. M. Malamuth St E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Pornography and sexual aggression (pp.
83-115). New York: Academic.
Sheehan, P. W. (1986). Television viewing and its relation to aggression among children
in Australia. In L. R. Huesmann St L. D. Eron (Eds.), Television and the aggressive
child: A cross-national comparison (pp. 161-200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Smith, D. D. (1976). The social content of pornography. Journal of Communication, 29,
16-24.
Straus, N. (1991). Discipline and divorce: Physical punishment of children and violence
and other crime in adulthood. Social Problems, 38, 133-154.
Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior.
(1972). Television and growing up: The impact of televised violence. Report to the
Surgeon General, U.S. Public Health Service. HEP Publ. No. HSM 72-9090.
Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health, U.S. Government Printing
Office.
Sykes, G., St Matza, D. (1961). Juvenile delinquency and subterranean values. American
Sociological Review, 26, 712-719.
Tedeschi. J. T., St Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions. Wash¬
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Tedeschi, J. T., St Norman, N. (1985). Social mechanisms of displaced aggression. In E. J.
Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes: Theory and research (vol. 2). Greenwich,
CT: JAI.
Tedeschi, J. T., Smith, R. B. HI, Si Brown, R. C. Jr. (1974). A reinterpretation of research
on aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 540-563.
Thomas, M. H., Horton, R. W., Lippincott, E. Q, St Drabman, R. S. (1977). Desensitiza¬
tion to portrayals of real-life aggression as a function of exposure to television vio¬
lence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 450-458.
266
Richard B. Felson
Valkenburg, P. M., Vooijs, M. W., & Van der Voort, T. H. (1992). The influence of televi¬
sion on children's fantasy styles: A secondary analysis. Imagination, Cognition, Per¬
sonality, 12, 55-67.
West, D. J., & Farrington, D. P. (1977). The delinquent way of life. London: Heinemann.
White, L. A. (1979). Erotica and aggression: The influence of sexual arousal, positive
affect, and negative affect on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 591-601.
Wiegman, O., Kuttschreuter, M., & Baarda, B. (1992). A longitudinal study of the effects
of television viewing on aggressive and antisocial behaviors. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 31, 147-164.
Wood, W., Wong. F. Y., & Chachere, J. G. (1991). Effects of media violence on viewers'
aggression in unconstrained social interaction. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 371-383.
Zillman, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in communication-mediated aggressive behavior.
Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 7, 419-434.
Zillman, D. (1983). Arousal and aggression. In R. G. Geen & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.),
Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (vol. 1, pp. 75-101). New York:
Academic.
Zillmann, D., Bryant, J., Comisky, P. W., & Medoff, N. J. (1981). Excitation and hedonic
valence in the effect of erotica on motivated intermale aggression. European Jour¬
nal of Social Psychology, 11, 233-252.
Zillmann, D., Floyt, J. L., & Day, K. D. (1974). Strength and duration of the effect of aggres¬
sive, violent, and erotic communications on subsequent aggressive behavior. Com¬
munication Research, 1, 286-306.
Contributors
Bosley Crowther was the film critic for The New York Times.
Ronald Gold covered the Hollywood industry for Variety, the trade
paper for the motion picture business.
267
268
Contributors
269
Brown, Rita Mae, 144 Cruising, 141, 156, 162
Buckets of Blood, 134 Crying Game, The, 38, 100-101
Bugs Bunny Movie, The, 23
Bunuel, Luis, 94, 115 D.O.A., 89
Burton, Richard, 97 Dali, Salvador, 115
Dance, Charles, 84
Caan, James, 188 Dantine, Helmut, 188
Cagney, James, 4, 5, 37, 48, 89-90 Darabont, Frank, 83
Cape Fear, 94 Dawn of the Dead, 15
Carlson, Roy, 85 Dawson, Gordon, 184
Carpenter, John, 130, 141, 147, 155, Days of Heaven, 80
156 De Palma, Brian, 2, 5, 8, 125, 127,
Casino, 1 141, 150, 157, 159, 166
Casualties of War, 100 Deakins, Roger, 83
Catch-22, 113 Death Wish, 92
catharsis, concept of, 19-20; work of DeBord, Guy, 108
Seymour Feshback and, 21 Deer Hunter, The, 23; viewer
Champion, The, 246-247 responses to, 24
Chaplin, Charles, 47 Deliverance, 153
Chase, The, 48 Demon, 154
Child’s Voice, A, 97 Detective, The, 7
China Moon, 37, 84-85 Dickinson, Angie, 91
Chinatown, 87 Die Hard, 92
Christ, Judith, 60 Dirty Dozen, The, 9, 12, 35, 36, 48,
Christian Century, The, 197 57; critical ambivalence toward,
Clarke, Alan, 88 51-53, 54, 55-56, 58-60
Classification and Rating Adminis¬ Douglas, Kirk, 91, 246, 247
tration (CARA), 6, 33 Dracula, 125
Clockwork Orange, A, 38, 114 Dressed to Kill, 8, 135, 137, 141, 142,
Clover, Carol, 15, 16, 39 162
Coburn, James, 96 Dreyer, Carl Theodor, 125
Co-Ed Frenzy, 143 Dunaway, Faye, 11
Cook, Jr., Elisha, 89 Dutchman, 48
Cook, the Thief, His Wife and Her Duvall, Robert, 188
Lover, The, 103
Cooper, Gary, 90 Eastwood, Clint, 37, 52, 53, 90-91
Coppola, Francis Ford, 88 Ebert, Roger, 16
Costner, Kevin, 95 effects on viewers. See empirical
Craven, Wes, 17 research on media effects
Cries and Whispers, 158 Eisenstein, Sergei, 107, 178, 182-183,
Criminal Conversation, 97 187
Crimson Pirate, The, 48 El Dorado, 11
Cronenberg, David, 103, 159 Elephant, 88
Cross of Iron, 183, 185, 188, 192 Ellis, Bret Easton, 102
Crowther, Bosley, 9, 35-36; criticisms empirical research on media effects,
of movie violence by, 51-56; reac¬ 19-29; consequences of depicted
tions to Crowther, 57-61 aggression, 224-226; cultivation
271
Index
Index