DeVito Eichenbaum - J Neurosci 2011 - Sequence HPC MPFC
DeVito Eichenbaum - J Neurosci 2011 - Sequence HPC MPFC
DeVito Eichenbaum - J Neurosci 2011 - Sequence HPC MPFC
Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
Episodic memory involves remembering the incidental order of a series of events that comprise a specific experience. Current models of
temporal organization in episodic memory have demonstrated that animals can make memory judgments about the order of serially
presented events; however, in these protocols, the animals can judge items based on their relative recency. Thus, it remains unclear as to
whether animals use the specific order of items in forming memories of distinct sequences. To resolve this important issue in memory
representation, we presented mice repeatedly with two widely separated odor sequences and then tested their natural exploratory
preference between pairs of odors selected from within or between sequences. Intact animals preferred to investigate odors that occurred
earlier within each sequence, indicating they did remember the order of events within each distinct sequence. In contrast, intact animals
did not discriminate between pairs of odors from different sequences. These findings indicate that preferences were not guided by relative
recency, which would be expected to support graded discrimination between widely separated events. Furthermore, damage to either the
hippocampus or the medial prefrontal cortex eliminated order preference within sequences. Despite the deficit in order memory, control
recognition tests showed that normal mice and mice with hippocampal or medial prefrontal damage could correctly identify previously
experienced odors compared with novel odors. These findings provide strong evidence that animals form representations of the order of
events within specific experiences and that the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex are essential to order memory.
(day 7), the animals were presented with A versus D, N versus P, and B memory for items within sequence 1 and sequence 2, the opposite order
versus C. as the groups described above. After the sequence probes, both groups
On the third day of probe testing (day 8), the animals were presented were tested on novelty recognition probe trials.
with pairs consisting of one odor from sequence 1 and one odor from Histology. After behavioral testing, all animals were given an overdose
sequence 2. These were considered “between-sequence” probe tests be- of sodium pentobarbital and perfused transcardially with 4% Formalin.
cause they evaluated the ability of the animals to compare the times at The brains were removed and postfixed for 1 h in Formalin, and then
which the odors were presented over the course of each day, i.e., earlier in cryoprotected in 30% sucrose solution, in pH 7.4 PBS. Coronal sections
sequence 1 versus 3 h later in sequence 2. The animals were presented were cut (40 m) using a freezing microtome. Every section was mounted on
with A versus N, E versus O, and B versus L. gelatin-coated slides and dried overnight. Slides were soaked in xylenes and
Finally, on the fourth day (day 9), the animals were presented with an then run through a series of ethanol dehydrations, stained with cresyl violet,
odor from either sequence against a novel odor they had never experi- and then rehydrated. The extent of the lesion was determined using a light
enced. These were considered “novelty” probe tests because they evalu- microscope to study the stained sections.
ated the ability of the animals to recognize novel compared with familiar Four representative sections along the anteroposterior axis of the hip-
odors. The animals were presented with L versus caraway (McCormick), pocampus (AP ⫹1.7, 2.3, 2.8, and 3.08) were selected from the mouse
C versus mace (McCormick), and M versus sage (McCormick). After a brain atlas to determine hippocampal damage (Franklin and Paxinos,
3 h delay, the animals were presented with A versus baby powder (Shaw’s 1997). For each hippocampal lesion animal, the four brain slices most
Supermarkets), N versus onion (McCormick), and B versus strawberry closely corresponding to the representative sections in the brain atlas
(Nestlé). were used. Three representative sections along the anteroposterior axis of
Cohorts of four mice, containing two sham and two lesioned mice, the medial prefrontal cortex (AP ⫺2.58, 2.1, and 1.7) were selected from
were trained and tested within each 9 d consecutive testing sequence, the mouse brain atlas to determine prefrontal damage (Franklin and
allowing us to ensure that each animal received a 3 min delay between Paxinos, 1997). For each prefrontal lesion animal, the three sections most
each of the three sequence presentations of sequence 1 in the first session closely corresponding to the representative sections in the brain atlas
of the day and sequence 2 in the second session of the day. For each were used. Canvas 5.0 (Deneba Software; ACD Systems International)
cohort, training on sequence 1 began at 12:00 P.M., and training on was used to calculate the percentage of tissue damage, which was defined
sequence 2 began at 3:00 P.M. The presentation of each odor was done as as the amount of total damage divided by the total area of the brain in that
rapidly as possible such that, for each odor, the animal was allowed to section ⫻ 100; the average of those values represented the lesion extent.
enter the testing area, dig in the scented cup, retrieve the reward, and Additional sections were studied under the light microscope to identify
immediately exit, with ⬃3 s between odor presentations during which incidental damage outside the targeted regions and were reported in
the investigator switched the scented cups. Results.
A cardboard divider was used to separate the compartment into two
areas of equal width, a testing compartment and a waiting compartment.
At the start of each session, the animal would be placed into the waiting Results
compartment and a trial would be initiated by lifting the divider, allow- Histology
ing the animal to run into the testing compartment. After each trial, the In two animals, damage to the hippocampus was minimal, and
divider would again be lifted by the experimenter, allowing the animal to these animals were removed from additional analyses. Otherwise,
run back into the other compartment and wait for the next trial to be the NMDA infusions generally resulted in a substantial loss of
initiated. The apparatus was also covered to prevent mice from escaping cells within the hippocampal formation, including Ammon’s
the test box during the session. horn, the dentate gyrus, and the subiculum (Fig. 2 A). An average
During probe trial sessions, each animal was allowed to dig freely in of 45% of the hippocampus was damaged across animals, ranging
each cup; neither of the cups was baited. The animal was always presented
from 8 to 77% total. The size of the lesion was not correlated with
with the cups in six possible locations within the apparatus during the
first 5 d to prevent the development of spatial biases. The positions of the the extent of the behavioral deficit found (r(8) ⫽ ⫺03.92, p ⫽
cups were randomized each day for all trials and were different for each 0.337). Two animals had partial damage to the thalamus. There
mouse. On days in which probe trials were administered, the locations of was also incidental damage to multiple cortical areas above
the cups were pseudorandomly selected so that each mouse experienced the infusion sites in both sham and hippocampal animals. The
the cups in different locations and cup location was not repeated amount of damage in each cortical area is as follows: somatosen-
throughout the session. The time spent digging in each cup was recorded sory cortex (10.8% in sham group, 18.88% in hippocampal lesion
using a stopwatch, and these times were later used to calculate preference group; t(10) ⫽ ⫺1.892, p ⫽ 0.088), motor cortex (16.01% in sham
indices. To prevent digging behavior from extinguishing, immediately group, 16.92% in hippocampal lesion group; t(10) ⫽ ⫺0.07, p ⫽
after each probe trial, the animal was presented with a plain cup of sand 0.945), retrosplenial cortex (25.26% in sham group, 21.75% in
(with no odor) with chocolate sprinkles that the animal was allowed to
hippocampal lesion group; t(10) ⫽ 0.572, p ⫽ 0.580), parietal
eat. With the exception of the novelty probe trials on day 4 of testing in
which each animal received six probe trials, each animal received three
cortex (14.91% in sham group, 29.9% in hippocampal lesion
probe trials per day. group; t(10) ⫽ ⫺0.2337, p ⫽ 0.042), and visual cortex (8.92% in
To determine whether the preferences reported in the results could sham group, 27.54% in hippocampal lesion group; t(10) ⫽
have been attributable to innate biases toward or against particular ⫺4.130, p ⫽ 0.033). Although the damage in the parietal and
odors, a separate group of eight naive mice were shaped to dig in cups, visual cortices was significantly greater in the hippocampal lesion
just as the trained groups described above. Then, each animal was imme- group compared with the sham group, it is unlikely that slightly
diately administered the same set of probe trials without exposure to the greater damage to these particular cortical areas could account
sequences. Furthermore, to account for potential differences in perfor- for the pattern of impaired memory for odor order and spared
mance on within-sequence and between-sequence probes attributable to odor recognition in this odor guided task.
the order in which these probe trials were administered, two additional Ibotenic acid infusions resulted in a substantial loss of cells
separate groups of eight mice were shaped to dig in cups then exposed
within the medial wall of the prefrontal cortex, including the
repeatedly to the sequences as described above. One group was tested on
their memory for items within sequence 1 and sequence 2 and then tested prelimbic and infralimbic cortices and dorsal cingulate gyrus
on their memory for items between sequence 1 and sequence 2, the same (Fig. 2 B). An average of 43% of the medial prefrontal cortex was
order in which the probe trials were administered to the mice in the damaged across all animals, ranging from 12 to 81%. Two ani-
experiment described above. Another group was first tested on memory mals had additional damage to the medial orbital cortex, and two
for items between sequence 1 and sequence 2 and then tested on their animals had unilateral damage to the ventral and lateral orbital
3172 • J. Neurosci., March 2, 2011 • 31(9):3169 –3175 DeVito and Eichenbaum • Hippocampal–Prefrontal Circuitry Supports Sequence Memory
Table 1. Average digging times and preference indices for each group across the various groups of mice and judge the aberrant finding of one
within-sequence, between-sequence, and novelty probe trials significant between-sequence preference as a statistical outlier
SHAM HPX PFCX that is not confirmed even among different types of between-
Within sequence
sequence probes in our intact groups.
Less recent odor 4.22 ⫾ 0.39s 3.66 ⫾ 0.67s 4.08 ⫾ 0.53s The present findings indicate that, across repeated sequence
More recent odor 2.40 ⫾ 0.28s 3.80 ⫾ 0.54s 4.25 ⫾ 0.41s presentations, animals link the five odors within each complete
Preference index 0.28 ⫾ 0.04 0.01 ⫾ 0.06 ⫺0.08 ⫾ 0.05 sequence. Conversely, because the animals treat odors that ap-
Between sequence pear in different sequences as unrelated, we conclude that mice
Odor from sequence 1 2.48 ⫾ 0.42s 3.04 ⫾ 0.76s 2.70 ⫾ 0.44s viewed each sequence as a temporally organized experience that is
Odor from sequence 2 2.21 ⫾ 0.27s 2.69 ⫾ 0.53s 3.29 ⫾ 0.63s distinct from the other sequence. We suggest that having been
Preference index ⫺0.06 ⫾ 0.08 0.01 ⫾ 0.14 0.02 ⫾ 0.08 repeatedly rewarded for digging in the odors in a particular order
Novelty recognition may dispose the mice to spend more time searching for food in
Novel odor 4.22 ⫾ 0.45s 4.31 ⫾ 0.62s 4.51 ⫾ 0.50s
the odor for which digging had always previously been rewarded
Familiar odor 1.88 ⫾ 0.29s 2.18 ⫾ 0.77s 2.04 ⫾ 0.30s
Preference index 0.41 ⫾ 0.04 0.55 ⫾ 0.15 0.36 ⫾ 0.06
earlier; that is, they may have had a high expectancy of reward for
odors that had regularly been rewarded earlier over those re-
SHAM, Sham-operated mice; HPX, hippocampal lesion mice; PFCX, medial prefrontal lesion mice.
warded later. In addition, the abnormality of preferences in ani-
mals with hippocampal and prefrontal lesions suggests that, in
preferential exploration of odors that they had experienced earlier the absence of prefrontal– hippocampal contributions, other
within the same sequence of events but did not prefer an odor that brain systems lead to a very different pattern of choices.
they experienced hours earlier the same day in a different sequence.
Therefore, their memory for order must be supported by a represen- Contributions of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to
tation of the order of events specific to each sequence. We further sequence memory
demonstrated that this pattern of performance on within- and Memory for the order of events within each sequence was dis-
between-sequence comparisons is not dependent on the order in rupted by damage to the hippocampus, confirming previous
which these probe tests are presented. Our results also then confirm studies that have reported a critical role for the hippocampus in
that the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex contribute to memory for sequential events (Fortin et al., 2002; Kesner et al.,
remembering order within specific experiences, whereas these areas 2002), as well as in disambiguating items with overlapping ele-
are not essential to item recognition. ments (Agster et al., 2002; Ergorul and Eichenbaum, 2006; Ku-
maran and Maguire, 2006). Neuroimaging studies, as well as
Memory for the order of events in distinct sequences is not evidence from single-unit recordings, have also demonstrated
based on judgments of relative recency that the hippocampus is activated during recall of the temporal
Previous behavioral studies have demonstrated that animals can order of life-like events (Gelbard-Sagiv et al., 2008; Lehn et al.,
remember the temporal order of serially presented odors (Fortin 2009). Observations on ensemble neuronal activity have sug-
et al., 2002; Kesner et al., 2002) and the order of sequentially gested that the hippocampus supports memory for specific se-
presented objects (Mitchell and Laiacona, 1998; Hannesson et al., quences by the representation of an evolving temporal context
2004; Dere et al., 2005, 2007). However, to date, there has been no for serial events within each sequence (Manns et al., 2007). Cen-
conclusive evidence as to whether animals make sequential mem- tral to the findings presented here, Manns et al. (2007) reported
ory judgments based on a representation of order within each that hippocampal neural ensembles do not code for the relative
specific experience or based on relative recency. recency of sequential events nor do they tag items with their
We first demonstrated that animals can successfully discrim- ordinal position within the sequence. Instead, when ensemble
inate between the items occurring earlier compared with items representations of the temporal context of each event distin-
occurring later within a specific sequence. In addition, there was guished experiences that occurred at different times across the
no difference in memory for items that occurred farther apart sequence, memory for order was accurate. These results support
compared with items that occurred closer together within that the temporal context model of sequence memory, which pro-
sequence. If order judgments were based on relative recency, we poses that events are bound within a sequence by a gradually
would have expected strong preferences in the comparison of changing temporal representation (Howard and Kahana, 2002).
odors that were presented 3 h apart across sequences than for Substantial evidence also supports a role for the prefrontal
comparisons that involved odors presented only seconds apart. cortex in memory for the temporal order of events (Mitchell and
Additionally, there was no difference between items at shorter Laiacona, 1998; Fuster, 2001; Hannesson et al., 2004; Barker et al.,
compared with longer lags, suggesting that animals do not use 2007). Electrophysiological data collected from animals perform-
recency information to make judgments about items within each ing a task similar to that in this study suggest that an additional
sequence as well. Above-chance differential preferences on the region of the prefrontal cortex plays a role in anticipating sequen-
between-sequence probe trials were not observed in either of our tial events (Ramus et al., 2007). In that experiment, animals were
two intact groups or in either of our lesion groups; however, the presented repetitively with a series of eight odors as the activity of
sham group exhibited a significant preference for one of the three orbitofrontal neurons was monitored. In well-trained animals,
between-sequence probe trials. In other studies, we did not ob- neurons in orbitofrontal cortex fired selectively during the sam-
serve a significance preference on any between-sequence probes pling of particular odors, and some cells exhibited odor-selective
in two varieties of wild-type mice, mice lacking serine racemase firing 200 – 400 ms before odor onset. Furthermore, damage to
(DeVito et al., 2010), or mice lacking the vasopressin 1B receptor the hippocampus abolished this anticipatory firing pattern, sug-
(our unpublished findings), and all of these mice (except for the gesting that input from the hippocampus provides important
serine racemase knock-outs) showed the same strong within- information about the anticipation of sequential odors. In a study
sequence preferences as the mice in this study. Therefore, we view that involved recordings in monkeys performing a temporal or-
the absence of a between-sequence preference common among dering task, lateral prefrontal cortex cells fired selectively to items
DeVito and Eichenbaum • Hippocampal–Prefrontal Circuitry Supports Sequence Memory J. Neurosci., March 2, 2011 • 31(9):3169 –3175 • 3175
within a specific sequence, and this activity predicted behavioral dritic morphology. Genes Brain Behav. Advance online publication. Re-
judgments about order (Ninokura et al., 2003; Averbeck and Lee, trieved January 24, 2011. doi:10.1111/j.1601–183X.2010.00656.x.
Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2010) Medial temporal lobe activity
2007). These findings suggest that the prefrontal cortex may es-
during source retrieval reflects information type, not memory strength. J
tablish representations of orderly events and may retrieve these Cogn Neurosci 22:1808 –1818.
representations guided by input from the hippocampus. Dudchenko PA, Wood ER, Eichenbaum H (2000) Neurotoxic hippocampal
lesions have no effect on odor span and little effect on odor recognition
Hippocampal– cortical circuitry supports memory for the but produce significant impairments on spatial span, recognition, and
order of items within a sequence but does not support item alternation. J Neurosci 20:2964 –2977.
recognition Eichenbaum H (2004) Hippocampus: cognitive processes and neural repre-
There is a current controversy as to how the hippocampal– cortical sentations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron 44:109 –120.
Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2007) The medial temporal
circuitry supports memory strength judgments (Eichenbaum et al.,
lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:123–152.
2007; Squire et al., 2007; Shrager et al., 2008; Diana et al., 2010). Ergorul C, Eichenbaum H (2006) Essential role of the hippocampal forma-
Recent findings have suggested that both the hippocampus and tion in rapid learning of higher-order sequential associations. J Neurosci
prefrontal cortex play important roles in the recollection com- 26:4111– 4117.
ponent of recognition memory but do not contribute to famil- Farovik A, Dupont LM, Arce M, Eichenbaum H (2008) Medial prefrontal
iarity (Fortin et al., 2004; Farovik et al., 2008). Additional studies cortex supports recollection, but not familiarity, in the rat. J Neurosci
using object recognition have found that these structures do not 28:13428 –13434.
contribute to the detection of novel compared with familiar items Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB (2002) Critical role of the hippocam-
pus in memory for sequence of events. Nat Neurosci 5:458 – 462.
but instead are recruited when familiar items are combined in
Fortin NJ, Wright SP, Eichenbaum H (2004) Recollection-like memory re-
novel ways, such as when associations between these items must trieval in rats is dependent on the hippocampus. Nature 431:188 –191.
be formed (Dudchenko et al., 2000; Hannesson et al., 2004; Jen- Franklin K, Paxinos G (1997) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates.
kins et al., 2004; Aggleton and Brown, 2005). Despite a severe San Diego: Academic.
impairment in order memory, animals with damage to the hip- Fuster JM (2001) The prefrontal cortex—an update: time is of the essence.
pocampus or prefrontal cortex successfully recognized each of Neuron 30:319 –333.
the items in the two sequences. Gelbard-Sagiv H, Mukamel R, Harel M, Malach R, Fried I (2008) Internally
The current findings provide a novel method for evaluating generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during
free recall. Science 322:96 –101.
sequence memory and demonstrate that animals can remember Hannesson DK, Vacca G, Howland JG, Phillips AG (2004) Medial prefron-
the order of events within specific experiences, and, in a parallel tal cortex is involved in spatial temporal order memory but not spatial
study, this test was useful in distinguishing a cognitive impair- recognition memory in tests relying on spontaneous exploration in rats.
ment in a murine model of schizophrenia (DeVito et al., 2010). In Behav Brain Res 153:273–285.
addition, the present results resolve an important issue in the Howard M, Kahana M (2002) A distributed representation of temporal
development of animal models of episodic memory, demonstrat- context. J Math Psych 46:269 –299.
ing that normal animals can remember specific sequences with- Jenkins TA, Amin E, Pearce JM, Brown MW, Aggleton JP (2004) Novel
spatial arrangements of familiar visual stimuli promote activity in the rat
out relying on judgments about the mere relative recency of
hippocampal formation but not the parahippocampal cortices: a c-fos
temporally separated events and that hippocampal– cortical cir- expression study. Neuroscience 124:43–52.
cuitry supports the temporal organization of memories. Kesner RP, Gilbert PE, Barua LA (2002) The role of the hippocampus in
memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behav Neurosci
References 116:286 –290.
Aggleton JP, Brown MW (2005) Contrasting hippocampal and perirhinal Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2006) The dynamics of hippocampal activation
cortex function using immediate early gene imaging. Q J Exp Psychol B
during encoding of overlapping sequences. Neuron 49:617– 629.
58:218 –233.
Lehn H, Steffenach HA, van Strien NM, Veltman DJ, Witter MP, Håberg AK
Agster KL, Fortin NJ, Eichenbaum H (2002) The hippocampus and disam-
(2009) A specific role of the human hippocampus in recall of temporal
biguation of overlapping sequences. J Neurosci 22:5760 –5768.
sequences. J Neurosci 29:3475–3484.
Averbeck BB, Lee D (2007) Prefrontal neural correlates of memory for se-
Manns JR, Howard MW, Eichenbaum H (2007) Gradual changes in hip-
quences. J Neurosci 27:2204 –2211.
pocampal activity support remembering the order of events. Neuron
Barker GR, Bird F, Alexander V, Warburton EC (2007) Recognition mem-
56:530 –540.
ory for objects, place, and temporal order: a disconnection analysis of the
role of the medial prefrontal cortex and perirhinal cortex. J Neurosci Mitchell JB, Laiacona J (1998) The medial frontal cortex and temporal
27:2948 –2957. memory: tests using spontaneous exploratory behaviour in the rat. Behav
Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H (1996) Conservation of the hippocampal mem- Brain Res 97:107–113.
ory function in rats and humans. Nature 379:255–257. Ninokura Y, Mushiake H, Tanji J (2003) Representation of the temporal
Chiba AA, Kesner RP, Gibson CJ (1997) Memory for the temporal order of order of visual objects in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. J Neuro-
new and familiar spatial location sequences: role of the medial prefrontal physiol 89:2868 –2873.
cortex. Learn Mem 4:311–317. Ramus SJ, Davis JB, Donahue RJ, Discenza CB, Waite AA (2007) Interactions
Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recov- between the orbitofrontal cortex and the hippocampal memory system dur-
ery by scrub jays. Nature 395:272–274. ing the storage of long-term memory. Ann NY Acad Sci 1121:216 –231.
Dere E, Huston J, De Souza Silva M (2005) Episodic-like memory in mice: Shrager Y, Kirwan CB, Squire LR (2008) Activity in both hippocampus and
simultaneous assessment of object, place and temporal order memory. perirhinal cortex predicts the memory strength of subsequently remem-
Brain Res Brain Res Prot 16:10 –19. bered information. Neuron 59:547–553.
Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA (2007) The pharmacology, neuro- Squire LR, Wixted JT, Clark RE (2007) Recognition memory and the medial
anatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:872– 883.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:673–704. Tulving E (1983) Elements of episodic memory. London: Oxford UP.
DeVito L, Balu D, Kanter B, Lykken C, Basu A, Coyle J, Eichenbaum H (2010) Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ (1998) Episodic and declarative memory: role
Serine racemase deletion disrupts memory for order and alters cortical den- of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8:198 –204.