Exercício e Estimulação Cognitiva (Alzheimer)
Exercício e Estimulação Cognitiva (Alzheimer)
Exercício e Estimulação Cognitiva (Alzheimer)
Abstract
Background: Exercise is often proposed as a non-pharmacological intervention to delay cognitive decline in
people with dementia, but evidence remains inconclusive. Previous studies suggest that combining physical
exercise with cognitive stimulation may be more successful in this respect. Exergaming is a promising
intervention in which physical exercise is combined with cognitively challenging tasks in a single session. The aim
of this study was to investigate the effect of exergame training and aerobic training on cognitive functioning in
older adults with dementia.
Methods: A three-armed randomized controlled trial (RCT) compared exergame training, aerobic training and an
active control intervention consisting of relaxation and flexibility exercises. Individuals with dementia were randomized
and individually trained three times a week during 12 weeks. Cognitive functioning was measured at baseline, after the
12-week intervention period and at 24-week follow-up by neuropsychological assessment. The domains of executive
function, episodic memory, working memory and psychomotor speed were evaluated. Test scores were converted into
standardized z-scores that were averaged per domain. Between-group differences were analysed with analysis of
covariance.
Results: Data from 115 people with dementia (mean (SD) age = 79.2 (6.9) years; mean (SD) MMSE score = 22.9
(3.4)) were analysed. There was a significant improvement in psychomotor speed in the aerobic and exergame groups
compared to the active control group (mean difference domain score (95% CI) aerobic versus control 0.370 (0.103–0.
637), p = 0.007; exergame versus control 0.326 (0.081–0.571), p = 0.009). The effect size was moderate (partial η2 = 0.102).
No significant differences between the intervention and control groups were found for executive functioning, episodic
memory and working memory.
(Continued on next page)
* Correspondence: [email protected]
2
Radboud University Medical Center, Radboudumc Alzheimer Center,
Nijmegen, the Netherlands
3
Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Institute for Brain Cognition
and Behaviour, Department of Medical Psychology, Nijmegen, the
Netherlands
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
© The Author(s). 2019 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to
the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver
(http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 2 of 13
The primary aim of the current study is to investigate screening visit. The study was conducted in community
the efficacy of a 12-week exergame training and aerobic centres in Nijmegen, the Netherlands. Participants were
training compared to a control group on executive func- randomly assigned to one of the intervention groups or
tioning in older adults with dementia. We hypothesize the control group by an independent statistician. The
that exergame training results in greater improvement minimization method [31] was used to balance groups
on executive functioning than aerobic training. Second- for gender, severity of cognitive impairment (MMSE ≥
ary aims are: to assess the feasibility of exergames; to 20 or < 20), use of medication for Alzheimer’s disease,
compare effects of exergame training with single aerobic training location and level of education. The Dutch
training on the cognitive domains of psychomotor speed, classification of education levels [32] was used to clas-
episodic memory and working memory; to measure the sify the educational attainment of participants as low
follow-up effects of exergame training and aerobic train- (levels 1–3), average (levels 4–5) or high (levels 6–7).
ing; and to determine whether the cognitive effects of
training are modified by the APOE ε4 carrier state. Interventions
The study included three arms: exergame training, aer-
Methods obic training and active control. Participants in each
Study design arm received three training sessions per week for 12
The current study was a 12-week single-blind randomized weeks. Training sessions were given on a one-on-one
controlled trial (RCT) with two experimental intervention basis, and trained students or research assistants super-
groups and one active control group. Participants were in- vised the participants. Adherence to the intervention
cluded from January 2016 to September 2017. The Med- was calculated by dividing the number of sessions the
ical Ethics Committee of Radboud University Medical participant followed by the total number of sessions
Center in Nijmegen, the Netherlands approved the re- that were offered.
search protocol, which was published previously [27]. The The exergame training consisted of a combined cogni-
study was conducted in compliance with Declaration of tive–aerobic bicycle training developed by Bike Labyrinth
Helsinki ethical standards. Participants all verbally (www.bikelabyrinth.com). The aerobic training compo-
agreed to participate in the study and gave written in- nent consisted of cycling on a stationary bike, 30–50 min
formed consent. The trial is registered at the Dutch per session. The aerobic exercise was tailored to an indi-
trial register (http://www.trialregister.nl) with identifi- vidual fitness level and health status, and aimed to achieve
cation number NTR5581. an intensity of 65–75% of heart rate reserve after 12 weeks
of training [27]. For participants on medication that atten-
Participants and study procedures uates heart rate (e.g. beta-blockers), the Borg Rating of
Participants were approached via the memory clinic of Perceived Exertion (RPE) [33] was used to ensure that the
Radboudumc Alzheimer Center, day care centres for intended training intensity was achieved. In addition, the
older adults with cognitive disorders, advertisement in stationary bike was connected to a video screen. Partici-
local newspapers and word of mouth. Eligibility criteria pants followed a route through a digital environment and
for inclusion were: clinically confirmed diagnosis of de- simultaneously performed cognitive tasks targeting re-
mentia following the DSM-IV criteria [28] (vascular, sponse inhibition, task switching and processing speed.
Alzheimer or mixed type) with a Mini Mental Status The exergame training consisted of seven different cog-
Examination (MMSE) [29] score ≥ 17; aged 60 years or nitive training levels. The difficulty of the cognitive
older; if using anti-dementia medication, a stable dose tasks increased per level to ensure that the training
for at least 3 months before the start of the trial; and remained cognitively challenging. The exergame train-
being capable of giving informed consent [30]. Exclu- ing and different training levels are described exten-
sion criteria were: co-morbidity that limited exercising, sively in our protocol paper [27].
including severe cardiovascular, musculoskeletal or The single aerobic exercise group consisted of cycling
neurological disease; diagnosis of a depression, bipolar on a stationary bike that was not connected to a video
disorder or psychotic disorder at the moment of inclu- screen. The aerobic training was identical to the exer-
sion; current drug or alcohol dependency; exercising game training already described. Participants in the ac-
more than five times per week for at least 30 min at a tive control group received training that consisted of
moderate intensity; wheelchair bound; and severe hear- relaxation and flexibility exercises with a duration of
ing or visual problems that could not be corrected with 30 min and the same frequency as the training regimes
the use of hearing aids/glasses. When participants were of the intervention groups. The exercises required min-
recruited by newspaper advertisement or word of imal muscle strength and aerobic capacity and were
mouth, we confirmed the dementia diagnosis by inves- easy to perform. The level of social engagement was
tigating their medical record before planning a similar to the intervention groups.
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 4 of 13
were randomized. Fourteen participants did not Attendance, intensity and safety
complete the 12-week intervention (12%). The num- Table 2 presents the adherence per group; a trend was found
ber of drop-outs did not differ significantly between towards higher adherence in the exergame group compared
the groups (p = 0.930). The enrolment, allocation to the aerobic group (mean difference (95% CI) 6.85 (− 0.09
process and reasons for drop-out are presented in to 13.79), p = 0.053). Participants rated both exercise inter-
Fig. 1. Baseline characteristics for the randomized ventions and the active control group highly (see Table 2).
sample were well matched between the groups Training duration, training load, heart rate and rate of per-
(Table 1). The included participants had a mean (SD) ceived exertion did not differ between both intervention
age of 79.9 (6.5) years and a mean (SD) MMSE score groups. The mean training intensity was light in both inter-
of 22.4 (3.2). There were no differences in age, vention groups with an average of 41.8% (SD = 13.3) and
MMSE score and Katz index between the different 43.5% (SD = 18.2) of maximal heart rate in the exergame
dementia types (see Additional file 1). group and aerobic group respectively. For the exergame
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 6 of 13
Fig. 2 Mean z-scores and standard errors of mean (SEM) at baseline, after 12 weeks and after 24 weeks for domains of executive function,
psychomotor speed, episodic memory and working memory. Arrows represent SEM. *Significant effect (p < 0.05) of exergame training and
aerobic training on psychomotor speed compared to controls after 12 weeks; §maintenance effect (p < 0.05) of aerobic and exergame training on
psychomotor speed at 24-week follow-up
the control group (mean difference domain score (95% ε4 carriership did not influence the relation between
CI) aerobic versus control 0.267 (0.048–0.486), p = 0.057). training and cognitive function. Finally, we demon-
No significant intervention effects were observed in any strated that a newly developed exergame that comprises
of the other domains. both physical and cognitive training elements is feasible
for people with dementia.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first randomized con- Interpretation of results and comparison with previous
trolled trial to investigate the differential effect of exer- research
gaming versus aerobic training on cognitive functioning Contrary to our hypothesis, the current results did not
in people with dementia. We hypothesized that exer- show a larger effect of exergame training compared to
game training would result in greater improvement on aerobic training on cognitive functioning. Comparable
executive functioning than single aerobic training. Al- research on the differential effects of combined cogni-
though we did not find an effect of exergame training tive and physical training versus only cognitive or phys-
or aerobic exercise on executive function after 12 ical interventions in people with dementia is scarce.
weeks, we found that psychomotor speed improved in There is one previously published paper reporting that
both the exergame and the aerobic group compared to neither a 12-week combined cognitive–aerobic training
active controls. This effect was maintained at the nor aerobic training only improved global cognitive
24-week follow-up. We did not find an effect of both function in a smaller sample of 80 individuals with AD
intervention groups in the cognitive domains of epi- [47]. However, the type of intervention and used out-
sodic memory and working memory compared to the come measures are incomparable to the current study.
control group. Moderator analysis showed that APOE Research in individuals with MCI showed inconsistent
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 9 of 13
findings regarding the cognitive benefits of combined functions in this group. Executive functions include
interventions and its potential superiority compared to higher-order processes such as inhibitory control, men-
physical exercise or cognitive training alone [48]. In tal flexibility and planning, which are already affected
contrast, for older adults without cognitive impairment in the early stages of dementia [54, 55]. Assessment of
there is converging evidence that combined interven- executive function in people with dementia may conse-
tions (including exergames) are superior to physical or quently result in floor effects or missing data, which
cognitive training alone [48], with larger effect sizes for make it difficult to measure change over time.
interventions that are performed simultaneously com- In our study we found a moderate effect of exergame
pared to sequential interventions [12]. training and aerobic training on psychomotor speed
In healthy older adults, evidence for the efficacy of after a 12-week training period in people with demen-
physical exercise and combined cognitive and physical tia. This effect was not yet present after 6 weeks of
interventions on executive functions [12, 49], memory training. Firstly, this may imply that the improvement
[12, 49], working memory [12, 50] and attention [51] is due to the training and not due to non-specific treat-
have been well established. In our current study, both ment or practice effects. Secondly, this suggests that a
exergame and aerobic-only training did not positively longer training duration is necessary to improve psy-
affect executive functions, working memory or episodic chomotor speed. Although still under debate, there is
memory. This seems partly in line with previous re- some evidence that physical exercise leads to improved
search. A meta-analysis performed by our group [13] cognitive function through promotion of hippocampal
demonstrated positive effects of combined interventions neurogenesis [56], brain angiogenesis [57] and synaptic
on global cognitive function in older adults with MCI or plasticity [58] elicited by an increased expression of
dementia, but no effects in the domains of executive neurotrophic factors [59]. In cognitively healthy older
function and memory. In contrast, a recently published adults, physical exercise interventions have the largest
RCT showed that both a mentally challenging exergame gains on executive control processes, psychomotor
and a passive exergame improve executive functioning speed and attention [49, 51, 60, 61]. In people with de-
in people with MCI [52]. However, the more challenging mentia there is little research about the benefits for dif-
exergame only yielded significant effects after 6 months ferent cognitive domains. From a neurobiological
of training, while the passive exergame already produced perspective, however, we do not have an explanation
gains after 3 months [52]. A possible explanation for this for why exercise would only improve psychomotor
discrepancy is that participants in the mentally challen- speed, but not the other cognitive skills assessed. We
ging exergame group needed more time to master the hypothesize, that only finding an effect on psychomotor
intervention, which may have delayed triggering the syn- speed, and not on executive functioning, may be related
ergistic effects of the combined intervention [52]. This to domain-specific responsiveness of the selected out-
might also explain the negative findings in our study, come measures. Processing speed tests typically are
since a mentally challenging exergame was used for a continuous outcome measures without ceiling or floor
relative short intervention period of 12 weeks. effects that are highly sensitive [62], which may explain
There is evidence that the severity of neurocognitive the sensitivity to change even in a dementia sample. In
disorder has a moderating impact on the cognitive ef- contrast, tests that measured executive functioning re-
fects of combined cognitive and physical training [53]. sulted in floor effects in our dementia sample, which
An increase in the severity of neurocognitive disorder made it difficult to measure change over time. Alterna-
may lead to a decrease of the intervention effect [53]. tively, one could also hypothesize that mood may be a
This could be explained by a reduced structural brain mediating factor for improvement on speed measures,
capacity (e.g. reduced number of neurons and synapses) as previous research showed that exercise and exer-
in participants with more severe neurocognitive disorder, game training can reduce depressive symptoms in
which may lead to limited resources necessary for healthy older adults [63, 64]. The positive effect on psy-
training-induced gains [53]. Therefore, it may be more chomotor speed was consistent across the different
difficult to induce cognitive benefits in people with de- neuropsychological tests used to measure psychomotor
mentia compared to those with MCI or healthy older speed (short form of Trail Making Test part A and the
adults. Moreover, the complexity to obtain valid neuro- abbreviated Stroop Color Word Test parts I and II),
psychological outcomes that are sensitive to change in which shows that the effect was robust and reliable. Its
persons who already have severe cognitive deficits due moderate effect size is slightly larger than to the
to their dementia complicates the assessment of cogni- small-to-moderate effect sizes commonly found in
tive functioning in this group. Even though we carefully studies examining the effects of cholinesterase inhibi-
selected and adjusted tests for use in mild-to-moderate tors on cognitive function [65, 66]. Given that interven-
dementia, it is particularly challenging to assess executive tions to ameliorate cognitive decline of people with
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 10 of 13
dementia are scarce, this effect size may be clinically of mood, since this might be a potential mediating fac-
relevant. Poor processing speed is a predictor of func- tor for the improvement in processing speed measures.
tional decline in basic and instrumental activities of
daily living [67]. In addition, poor processing speed is Clinical relevance and feasibility
reported to be a predictor for incident dementia [68] Both exergame training and aerobic training improved
and was found to be associated with shorter survival psychomotor speed after 12 weeks, with a moderate ef-
among older adults in Japan [69]. Furthermore, late-life fect size. This finding may be clinically relevant as psy-
cognitive decline is attributable to slower processing speed chomotor speed is an important predictor for
[70]. Thus, the reported improvement in processing speed functional decline. In our study, exergame training was
may be clinically relevant. not superior to aerobic training. However, there was a
The mean training intensity was light in both inter- trend for higher adherence in the exergame group
vention groups, with an average of 41.8% (SD = 13.3) compared to the aerobic group. Additionally, trainers
and 43.5% (SD = 18.2) of maximal heart rate in the who individually guided the training sessions reported
exergame group and the aerobic group respectively. We that it was easier to motivate participants in the exer-
expected that improved cardiorespiratory fitness would game group and to increase duration of the training
be a requirement to improve cognitive function [51], sessions. This was confirmed by our finding that no
and therefore we aimed to achieve moderate exercise participants dropped out in the exergame group due to
intensity (e.g. 65–75% of maximal heart rate) during low motivation (see Fig. 1). Accordingly, exergaming
the training sessions. However, the exercise training seems to be an effective method to stimulate
was tailored to an individual fitness level and health long-term physical activity participation in people with
status, and most participants were not able to achieve a dementia.
moderate training intensity. The recently published De-
mentia and Physical Activity (DAPA) trial [71] showed Future directions
that moderate to high-intensity aerobic and strength Future studies should examine whether certain individ-
exercise training did not slow cognitive decline in ual characteristics (e.g. type of dementia) moderate the
people with mild to moderate dementia, and even effect of physical activity on cognition. Insight into
worsened cognitive impairment in those who complied these individual differences is important because it can
with the intervention, despite an improvement in phys- determine which people are most likely to benefit from
ical fitness. It is therefore unlikely that the light training physical activity. It can also help to personalize inter-
intensity in our study limited the beneficial effects of ventions, thereby stimulating physical activity. More-
exercise on cognitive functioning. over, additional studies are needed to explore the
optimal intervention design and dose–response for eli-
Strengths and limitations citing beneficial cognitive effects in people with demen-
The strengths of our study include the inclusion of a tia. Future intervention trials should include measures
relatively large sample of people with dementia, a high of psychomotor speed as these can reliably and validly
adherence rate, the use of a comprehensive neuro- be assessed in people with dementia and are closely re-
psychological assessment and follow-up measurement lated to everyday activities. Furthermore, studies should
for long-term maintenance effects. However, some limi- also focus on investigating neurophysiological mecha-
tations need to be taken into account when interpreting nisms that underlie the cognitive effects of exercise, for
our results. Firstly, only participants who were mobile example by including neuroimaging measures.
and motivated enrolled in our study, which may limit
the external validity of the current findings. Secondly, Conclusions
participants were not blinded to allocation, which is an Exergaming is a feasible and highly appreciated exercise
unavoidable limitation of exercise studies. Outcome method to engage older adults with dementia in phys-
assessors were masked for intervention allocation. ical exercise, mixed with cognitive stimulation. Both
Thirdly, although we used adapted versions of executive exergame training and aerobic training can improve
tests, making administration in people with dementia psychomotor speed, which may be clinically relevant as
more feasible, a floor performance was still found in a psychomotor speed is an important predictor for func-
number of individuals. This may have reduced the sen- tional decline. Although no effects were found on
sitivity to measure change over time, obscuring poten- executive function, episodic memory and working
tial positive results. Fourthly, the intervention period memory, the potential broad range of effects of exer-
was only 12 weeks, which may have been too short to games for older adults with dementia (e.g. physical
show beneficial effects of exergames on executive func- functioning, quality of life, activities in daily living)
tioning. Lastly, future studies should include measures should be studied in future RCTs.
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 11 of 13
22. Deeny SP, Poeppel D, Zimmerman JB, Roth SM, Brandauer J, Witkowski S, et 48. Bruderer-Hofstetter M, Rausch-Osthoff AK, Meichtry A, Munzer T,
al. Exercise, APOE, and working memory: MEG and behavioral evidence for Niedermann K. Effective multicomponent interventions in comparison to
benefit of exercise in epsilon4 carriers. Biol Psychol. 2008;78:179–87. active control and no interventions on physical capacity, cognitive function
23. Liu CC, Kanekiyo T, Xu H, Bu G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: risk, and instrumental activities of daily living in elderly people with and without
mechanisms and therapy. Nat Rev Neurol. 2013;9:106–18. mild impaired cognition—a systematic review and network meta-analysis.
24. Rovio S, Kåreholt I, Helkala EL, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, et al. Ageing Res Rev. 2018;45:1–14.
Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and 49. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, Welsh-Bohmer
Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:705–11. K, et al. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic
25. Etnier JL, Caselli RJ, Reiman EM, Alexander GE, Sibley BA, Tessier D, et al. review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med. 2010;72:239–52.
Cognitive performance in older women relative to ApoE-e4 genotype and 50. Rathore A, Lom B. The effects of chronic and acute physical activity on
aerobic fitness. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:199–207. working memory performance in healthy participants: a systematic review
26. Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Lopez OL, Carlson M, et al. with meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Syst Rev. 2017;6:124.
Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the 51. Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L.
Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161:639–51. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in
27. Karssemeijer EGA, Bossers WJR, Aaronson JA, Kessels RPC, Olde Rikkert MGM. older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database
The effect of an interactive cycling training on cognitive functioning in Syst Rev. 2008;3:1–73.
older adults with mild dementia: study protocol for a randomized 52. Anderson-Hanley C, Barcelos NM, Zimmerman EA, Gillen RW, Dunnam
controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17:73. M, Cohen BD, et al. The Aerobic and Cognitive Exercise Study (ACES)
28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of for community-dwelling older adults with or at-risk for mild cognitive
mental disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV). Arlington: the American Psychiatric impairment (MCI): neuropsychological, neurobiological and
Association; 2000. neuroimaging outcomes of a randomized clinical trial. Front Aging
29. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical Neurosci. 2018;10:76.
method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J 53. Bamidis PD, Fissler P, Papageorgiou SG, Zilidou V, Konstantinidis EI, Billis
Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98. AS, et al. Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical
30. Meulenbroek O, Vernooij-Dassen MJFJ, Kessels RPC, Graff MJL, Sjögren MJC, training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive
Schalk BWM, et al. Informed consent in dementia research. Legislation, disorder. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:152.
theoretical concepts and how to assess capacity to consent. Eur Geriatr 54. Baudic S, Dalla Barba G, Thibaudet MC, Smagghe A, Remy P, Traykov L.
Med. 2010;1:58–63. Executive function deficits in early Alzheimer's disease and their relations
31. Pocock SJ, Simon R. Sequential treatment assignment with balancing for with episodic memory. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21:15–21.
prognostic factors in the controlled clinical trial. Biometrics. 1975;31:103–15. 55. Miyake A, Emerson MJ, Friedman NP. Assessment of executive functions in
32. Verhage F. Intelligence and age in a Dutch sample. Hum Dev. 1965;8:238–45. clinical settings: problems and recommendations. Semin Speech Lang.
33. Borg G. Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign: Human 2000;21(2):169–83.
Kinetics; 1998. 56. Nokia MS, Lensu S, Ahtiainen JP, Johansson PP, Koch LG, Britton SL, et al.
34. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Trail Making Test as an initial screening Physical exercise increases adult hippocampal neurogenesis in male rats
procedure for neuropsychological impairment in older children. Arch Clin provided it is aerobic and sustained. J Physiol. 2016;594:1855–73.
Neuropsychol. 2004;19:281–8. 57. Lange-Asschenfeldt C, Kojda G. Alzheimer’s disease, cerebrovascular
35. Jensen AR, Rohwer WD. The Stroop color-word test: a review. Acta Psychol. dysfunction and the benefits of exercise: from vessels to neurons. Exp
1966;25:36–93. Gerontol. 2008;43:499–504.
36. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual 58. Voss MW, Vivar C, Kramer AF, van Praag H. Bridging animal and
tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson's disease: which aspects of gait are human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity. Trends Cogn Sci.
attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:1248–56. 2013;17:525–44.
37. Ruff R, Light R, Parker S, Levin H. Benton Controlled Oral Word Association 59. Lista I, Sorrentino G. Biological mechanisms of physical activity in
Test: reliability and updated norms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1996;11:329–38. preventing cognitive decline. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2010;30:493–503.
38. Schmand B, Groenink S, Van den Dungen M. Letter fluency: 60. Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and
psychometric properties and Dutch normative data. Tijdschr Gerontol cognition across the life span. J Appl Physiol. 2011;111:1505–13.
Geriatr. 2008;39:64–76. 61. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older
39. Wilson BA, Evans JJ, Alderman N, Burgess PW, Emslie H. Behavioural adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:125–30.
assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. In: Methodology of frontal and 62. Gontkovsky ST, Beatty WW. Practical methods for the clinical assessment of
executive function. Abingdon: Psychology Press; 1997. p. 239–50. information processing speed. Int J Neurosci. 2006;116:1317–25.
40. Bucks RS, Willison JR, Byrne LMT, Kessels RPC. Location Learning 63. Arent SM, Landers DM, Etnier JL. The effects of exercise on mood in older
Test—Revised edition (Dutch version). Amsterdam: Hogrefe; 2011. adults: A meta-analytic review. J Aging Phys Act. 2000;8:407–30.
41. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition (WAIS-III): test 64. Rosenberg D, Depp CA, Vahia IV, Reichstadt J, Palmer BW, Kerr J, et al.
manual. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1997. Exergames for subsyndromal depression in older adults: a pilot study of a
42. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III): administration and scoring novel intervention. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18:221–6.
manual. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1997. 65. Blanco-Silvente L, Castells X, Saez M, Barcelo MA, Garre-Olmo J, Vilalta-
43. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. New Franch J, et al. Discontinuation, efficacy, and safety of cholinesterase
York: Oxford University Press; 2004. inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of 43
44. Brands AM, Kessels RPC, Hoogma RP, Henselmans JM, van der Beek Boter randomized clinical trials enrolling 16 106 patients. Int J
JW, Kappelle LJ, et al. Cognitive performance, psychological well-being, and Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;20:519–28.
brain magnetic resonance imaging in older patients with type 1 diabetes. 66. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–9.
Diabetes. 2006;55:1800–6. 67. Iwasa H, Gondo Y, Yoshida Y, Kwon J, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, et al.
45. Yi L, Wu T, Luo W, Zhou W, Wu J. A non-invasive, rapid method to Cognitive performance as a predictor of functional decline among the
genotype late-onset Alzheimer's disease-related apolipoprotein E gene non-disabled elderly dwelling in a Japanese community: a 4-year
polymorphisms. Neural Regen Res. 2014;9:69. population-based prospective cohort study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;
46. Pedersen AB, Mikkelsen EM, Cronin-Fenton D, Kristensen NR, Pham TM, 47:139–49.
Pedersen L, et al. Missing data and multiple imputation in clinical 68. Rapp MA, Reischies FM. Attention and executive control predict Alzheimer
epidemiological research. Clin Epidemiol. 2017;9:157. disease in late life: results from the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Am J Geriatr
47. Venturelli M, Sollima A, Cè E, Limonta E, Bisconti AV, Brasioli A, et al. Psychiatry. 2005;13:134–41.
Effectiveness of exercise-and cognitive-based treatments on salivary cortisol 69. Iwasa H, Kai I, Yoshida Y, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H. Information processing
levels and sundowning syndrome symptoms in patients with Alzheimer’s speed and 8-year mortality among community-dwelling elderly Japanese. J
disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53:1631–40. Epidemiol. 2014;24:52–9.
Karssemeijer et al. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy (2019) 11:3 Page 13 of 13
70. Salthouse TA. Speed mediation of adult age differences in cognition. Dev
Psychol. 1993;29:722–38.
71. Lamb SE, Sheehan B, Atherton N, Nichols V, Collins H, Mistry D, et al.
Dementia And Physical Activity (DAPA) trial of moderate to high intensity
exercise training for people with dementia: randomised controlled trial.
BMJ. 2018;361:k1675.