Article 2
Article 2
Article 2
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, can
be found online at: http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/68/6/983
MARGARET A. HOJEM
and KENNETH J. OTTENBACHER
We examined the inferential decisions made using either visual analysis alone
or in combination with a trend line to evaluate data from single-subject research
designs. Thirty-nine subjects were randomly assigned to either a Visual Group
(n = 20) that used a visual-inspection approach to analyzing graphed data or a
Quantitative Group (n = 19) that used a trend-line approach. After instruction in
interpretation, we asked the subjects to analyze graphs containing data from five
hypothetical AB single-subject designs. Results revealed a statistically significant
difference in the decision made between the two groups for four of the five
graphs. The group using the trend line to analyze graphed data exhibited more
confidence in the decisions they made and also demonstrated greater within-
group consistency as compared with the group using visual inspection. The
implications of various methods of data analysis in establishing the scientific
legitimacy of single-subject research methods are discussed, and the argument
is made that quantitative procedures can assist in the analysis and interpretation
of single-subject data.
Key Words: Clinical protocols, Physical therapy, Research, Research design.
Researchers in physical therapy have recently reported sev- nesses of single-subject methods before the designs can be
eral investigations using single-subject research designs to appropriately implemented and interpreted.
address questions of clinical and therapeutic interest.1-3 These One area of particular controversy in implementing and
methods, also referred to as single-case experimental, intra- interpreting single-subject designs is data analysis.10 Several
subject, single-system, or idiographic research designs, repre- analytic options are available to the therapist using single-
sent alternatives to traditional group-comparison procedures subject research procedures. In their article addressing the
commonly used in the evaluation of clinical interventions.4,5 issue of interpreting the results of single-subject research de-
In clinical settings, a primary advantage of single-subject signs, Wolery and Harris identify two strategies for analyzing
procedures is their compatibility with clinical practice, which single-subject data: 1) visual analysis of graphed data and 2)
generally focuses on one patient at a time. Other advantages quantitative (statistical) analysis.11
include an emphasis on individualized programs of treatment Graphic presentation and visual analysis have been the
and outcome measures and a focus on understanding the traditional methods used to interpret and draw inferences
process of change through repeated measurements versus from data collected using a single-subject framework.6 Ac-
focusing only on the product or posttest results.6 The com- cording to Kazdin, visual analysis "refers to reaching a judg-
patibility of single-subject designs with clinical practice has ment about the reliability or consistency of intervention ef-
led some investigators to advocate them as the preferred fects by visually examining graphed data."4(p232) Tawney and
method of clinical research in field settings.7,8 Single-subject Gast argue that graphic presentation and visual inspection of
research methods are practice based and practitioner oriented; single-subject data provide practitioners with a compact and
however, they are not an empirical panacea. Connolly et al detailed account of patient performance, including a sequence
have correctly advised physical therapists that single-subject of the design phases, an indication of the time spent in each
methods have advantages and limitations and that therapists phase, and the relationship between the treatment and out-
must understand and appreciate both the strengths and weak- come variable.12
One frequent criticism of visual analysis is the lack of any
formal set of decision rules on which to make inferences from
single-subject data.7 Several authorities have argued that the
M. Hojem, MS, is Physical Therapist, DeForest Public Schools, DeForest,
WI 53532. She was a student in the graduate program in therapeutic science, lack of formal decision rules for making visual judgments
University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, when this study was con- from graphed data introduces the possibility of bias and
ducted. subjectivity into the analytical process.13,14 As Kazdin has
K. Ottenbacher, PhD, is Associate Professor, School of Allied Health Profes-
sions, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 2120 Medical Sciences Center, 1300 noted, "the process of visual inspection would seem to permit,
University Ave, Madison, WI 53706 (USA). if not actively encourage, subjectivity and inconsistency in
Address correspondence to Dr. Ottenbacher.
This study was completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements for Ms.
the evaluation of intervention effects."4(p239)
Hojem's master's degree in therapeutic science, University of Wisconsin-Mad- Evidence of inconsistent visual analysis of single-subject
ison. data has been provided by numerous investigators.13-15 Jones
This article was submitted June 8, 1987; was with the authors for revision
four weeks; and was accepted October 21, 1987. Potential Conflict of Inter- et al, for example, compared the use of visual inference and
est: 4. a statistical procedure and found frequent intrarater disagree-
GRAPH 1 GRAPH 2
GRAPH 3 GRAPH 4
GRAPH 5
REFERENCES
1. Laskas CA, Mullen SL, Nelson DL, et al: Enhancement of two motor 15. Ottenbacher KJ: Reliability and accuracy of visually analyzing graphed data
functions of the lower extremity in a child with spastic quadriplegia. Phys from single-subject designs. Am J Occup Ther 40:464-469, 1986
Ther 65:11-16, 1985 16. Jones RR, Weinrott MR, Vaught RS: Effects of serial dependency on the
2. Martin JE, Epstein LH: Evaluating treatment effectiveness in cerebral palsy: agreement between visual and statistical inference. J Appl Behav Anal
Single-subject designs. Phys Ther 56:285-294, 1976 11:277-283,1978
3. Harris SR, Riffle K: Effects of inhibitive ankle-foot orthoses on standing 17. White OR: The "Split-Middle": A "Quickie" Method of Trend Estimation.
balance in a child with cerebral palsy: A single-subject design. Phys Ther Seattle, WA, The University of Washington, Experimental Education Unit,
66:663-667, 1986 Child Development and Mental Retardation Center, 1974
4. Kazdin AE: Single-Case Research Designs: Methods for Clinical and Ap- 18. White OR, Haring NG: Exceptional Teaching: Individually Planned Educa-
plied Settings. New York, NY, Oxford University Press Inc, 1982 tions, ed 2. Columbus, OH, Charles E Merrill Publishing Co, 1980
5. Bloom M, Fischer J: Evaluating Practice: Guidelines for the Accountable 19. Bloom M: The Paradox of Helping: Introduction to the Philosophy of
Professional. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall Inc, 1982 Scientific Practice. New York, NY, John Wiley & Sons Inc, 1975
6. Barlow D, Hersen M: Single-Case Experimental Designs: Strategies for 20. Glass GV, Willson VL, Gottman JM: Design and Analysis of Time-Series
Studying Behavior Change, ed 2. Elmsford, NY, Pergamon Press Inc, 1984 Experiments. Boulder, CO, Colorado Associated University Press, 1975
7. Ottenbacher KJ: Evaluating Clinical Change: Strategies for Occupational 21. Harris RJ: A Primer of Multivariate Statistics. New York, NY, Academic
and Physical Therapists. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1986 Press Inc, 1975
8. Kazdin AE: Single-case research in clinical child psychiatry. J Am Acad 22. Ottenbacher KJ: A quantitative analysis of experimentwise error rates in
Child Psychiatry 22:423-432, 1983 applied behavioral science research. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science
9. Connolly BH, Craik RL, Krebs DE: Guest editorial: Single-case research- 22:495-501,1986
When is it valid? Phys Ther 63:1767-1768, 1983 23. Parsonson BS, Baer DM: The analysis and presentation of graphic data.
10. Kratochwill TR, Brody GH: Single-subject designs: A perspective on the In Kratochwill TR (ed): Single-Subject Research: Strategies for Evaluating
controversy over employing statistical inference and implications for re- Change. New York, NY, Academic Press Inc, 1978, pp 101-165
search and training in behavior modification. Behav Modif 2:291 -307, 1978 24. Parsonson BS, Baer DM: The graphic analysis of data. In Poling A, Fuqua
11. Wolery M, Harris SR: Interpreting results of single-subject research de- W (eds): Research Methods in Applied Behavior Analysis. New York, NY,
signs. Phys Ther 62:445-452, 1982 Plenum Publishing Corp, 1986, pp 157-186
12. Tawney JW, Gast DL: Single-Subject Research in Special Education. 25. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis in the Behavioral Sciences, rev ed.
Columbus, OH, Charles E Merrill Publishing Co, 1984 New York, NY, Academic Press Inc, 1977
13. DeProspero A, Cohen S: Inconsistent visual analysis of intrasubject data. 26. Currier DP: Elements of Research in Physical Therapy, ed 2. Baltimore,
J Appl Behav Anal 12:573-599, 1979 MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1984
14. Furlong MJ, Wampold BE: Intervention effects and relative variation as
dimensions in experts' use of visual inference. J Appl Behav Anal 15:415-
421,1982
http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/68/6/983#otherarticles
Subscription http://ptjournal.apta.org/subscriptions/
Information
Permissions and Reprints http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/terms.xhtml
Information for Authors http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/ifora.xhtml