Koch 2015 INTERDISCIPLINARYEMBODIMENTAPPROACHES - Chapter

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/285771341

Interdisciplinary Embodiment Approaches

Research · December 2015


DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.1.2642.8249

CITATIONS READS

0 414

1 author:

Sabine Koch
Alanus University Alfter, RIArT / SRH University Heidelberg
185 PUBLICATIONS 1,793 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Arts Therapies in Prevention View project

Active Factors of Creative Arts Therapies and a Model of Active Art Making View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sabine Koch on 05 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


INTERDISCIPLINARY EMBODIMENT APPROACHES. IMPLICATIONS
FOR CREATIVE ARTS THERAPIES
Embodiment Approaches
Sabine C. Koch
The chapter introduces interdisciplinary embodiment approaches from anthropology, linguistics,
and psychology and relates them to creative arts therapies. It particularly focuses on the role of the
body proper, body motion, basic dimensions of movement, body memory, and gender, fields that
lie at the intersection of cognitive sciences and dance/movement therapy. It is argued that the body
may be the place where the usefulness of the computer metaphor ends. The potential and
limitations of the embodiment perspective in relation to the creative arts therapies are discussed.

Keywords: Embodiment, human movement, body memory, creative arts therapy research,
dance/movement therapy

“Well, what do you think you understand with? With your head? Bah!” (from Nikos
Kazantzakis’ “Zorba the Greek”, cited after Andy Clark, 1997, p. 9)
Creative arts therapies (CATs), i.e. dance, music, art, drama, and poetry
therapy, have acquired increasing acceptance and application in recent years. The
professional fields are prospering internationally, accompanied by numerous
qualitative research studies and a smaller number of outcome studies, which altogether
almost unanimously support CATs’ effectiveness. CATs work well for all clients for
whom the verbal channel is not the primary means of expression. In dance/movement
therapy (DMT) for example, if indicated, “assessment and therapy can proceed
entirely in the nonverbal realm of movement, touch, rhythm, and spatial interaction”
(Goodill, 2005, p. 16). It has, however, continuously been emphasized that it remains
unclear how exactly creative arts therapy interventions work. New findings in
neuroscience and cognitive science increasingly shed light on the mechanisms
underlying CAT. Some embodiment approaches go so far as to proclaim a
paradigmatic change in favour of the inclusion of the phenomenal, lived, subjective
body into behavioural sciences’ major theorizing.

RECONCILIATION OF BODY AND MIND


Creative arts therapists have always assumed the union of body and mind as a
basic underlying principle of their work (e.g., Loman & Brandt, 1992), even though --
lacking a more adequate vocabulary -- they still have been talking about the two
realms as two separate capacities. On the basis of their clinical practice, for them self
and world are interconnected, and the mind speaks through the body in numerous
ways. Conversely, only now are the cognitive sciences on the verge of reconciling the
body and the mind. This new trend is reflected in the emerging embodiment
approaches.
Descartes (1596-1650) most clearly formulated the separation of body and mind
for the sake of the emerging bio-medical sciences being able to do research on and
with the body, expelling the mind from “the hard sciences” to the realm of philosophy
and theology. The heritage of Cartesian dualism has taught us to think in dichotomies,
and to value them as scientific ways of looking at life. Scientific psychology has
adapted much of this belief to develop its own theories and methods. Finally, at the
turn of the millennium, the body-mind problem has reached cognitive psychology as a
serious topic of empirical investigation. Ever since Merleau-Ponty’s groundbreaking
work on the ontological and epistemological meaning of the body for our human
condition (Merleau-Ponty, 1962), the reconciliation of body and mind emerged in
many scientific disciplines. Merleau-Ponty (1962) wanted us to start from the
experience of authentic perception. Asking where perception begins, he then
answered, in the body. The body is in the world from the beginning and
phenomenology as the descriptive science of existential beginnings leads Merleau-
Ponty to describe the body as a “general power of inhabiting all the environments,
which the world contains.” (Merleau-Ponty, 1962, p.311). In philosophy the problem
of embodiment has further been treated by Richard Zaner (1964; reflecting on the
philosophical contributions of Marcel, Sartre, and Merleau-Ponty) and lately by Fuchs
(2000) and Hurley (1998).
This chapter provides an overview of the interdisciplinary embodiment theories
that have developed in the tradition of Merleau-Pontys philosophical-
phenomenological approach. With them, a more holistic, and systemic thinking came
into the sciences. Embodied theories have their roots in philosophy, neurosciences,
medical anthropology and cognitive linguistics, but nowadays also include artificial
intelligence research, arts and art-related research, communication research, dynamic
systems approaches, educational sciences, linguistics and language-related research,
psychology, robotics, sociology, sports, and other fields. One of the main
characteristics of embodiment theories is that they take the body as the existential
ground for perception and action; they collapse such dualisms as body and mind,
subject and object (Sax, 2002), perception and conception (e.g., Lakoff & Johnson,
1999), and perception and action (e.g., Hurley, 1998; v. Weizsäcker, 1940/1996). They
assume a perceptual, modality-specific way of knowledge representation (as opposed
to an abstract, symbolic way), a way that leads via the sensory-motor system and uses
this system for thinking through embodied simulations (Barsalou, 1999; Barsalou,
Niedenthal, Barbey, & Ruppert, 2003; Gallese, 2003; Glenberg, 1997). Embodiment
approaches are furthermore closely related to approaches of situated cognition (e.g.,
dependency of cognition on cultural context) and dynamic systems approaches (e.g.,
Thelen, 1995) in fact overarching literature speaks of humans as situated, embodied,
dynamical agents (cf. Beer, 2000). Whether embodiment approaches offer a simple
shift in cognitive psychology or a full paradigmatic change remains to be determined
(cf. Clark, 1999).
This chapter focuses on theoretical developments in anthropology, linguistics,
and psychology. Within psychology it introduces the subdisciplines where
embodiment approaches presently emerge, and addresses connections to and future
directions in creative arts therapies research.

EMBODIED COGNITION: EMBODIMENT APPROACHES IN ANTHROPOLOGY AND


COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS
Anthropological Perspectives: Human Nature and Culture are Grounded in the
Body
“The locus of the sacred is the body, for the body is the existential ground of culture.” (Csordas,
2002, p.87).
One of the early precursors of embodiment theories was Margret Lock’s and
Nancy Scheper-Hughes’ widely quoted article “The mindful body” (1987),
transmitting a medical anthropological analysis in order to “problematize the body
(…) by investigating the lived experience of the individual body-self and its
interwovenness with the social and the political body” (Lock & Scheper-Hughes,
1987; p.6).
Shortly after Lock and Scheper-Hughes, Thomas Csordas published his
influential article “Embodiment as a Paradigm for Anthropology” (1988; here quoted
from the reprint in 2002). To successfully confront the “hard sciences”, in his view, it
is necessary to formulate a “science of subjectivity” (and he quotes Shweder, 1986;
p.178, here) “the real world it seems is populated with subject-dependent and object-
like subjectivity, two types of phenomena for which there is no place in the mutually
exclusive and exhaustive realms of the symbol-and-meaning-seeking hermeneuticist
and the automated-law-seeking positivist.” (Csordas, 2002, p.86). Csordas suspects
that “embodiment has paradigmatic scope” for example for the dissolution of
dichotomies, such as cognition and emotion. Andrew Strathern in “Body Thoughts”
(1996) continues this line of thought. Strathern (1996) asserts that the term
embodiment has reached the status of a major concept in cultural analysis. Yet, in his
view it is not enough to switch from one side of the Cartesian dichotomy to the other.
It might, however, be a necessary step in the process to a more adequate
conceptualization. Creative arts therapies work within and between body/mind. For
instance, in searching with the patient for an adequate metaphor as an anchor that
allows them to work toward health.

Linguistic Perspectives: Concepts and Language are Grounded in the Body


„Our ability to move in the ways we do and to track the motions of other things gives motion a
major role in our conceptual system. The fact that we have muscles and use them to apply force
in certain ways leads to the structure of our system of causal concepts. What is important is not
just that we have bodies and that thought is somehow embodied. What is important is that the
peculiar nature of our bodies shapes our very possibilities for conceptualization and
categorization“. (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999, p.19)
In their 1999 book “Philosophy in the Flesh” Lakoff and Johnson define an
embodied concept as “a neural structure that is actually part of, or makes use of, the
sensorimotor system of our brains. Much of the conceptual inference is, therefore,
sensorimotor inference” and not just preceded by or followed by sensorimotor
inference (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999, p. 20). They state that the embodied mind
hypothesis radically undercuts the distinction between perception and conception. In
an embodied mind, the same neural systems engaged in perception or in motion play a
central role in conceptualization and reasoning. This implies that movement is a direct
part of reasoning. Lakoff and Johnson emphasize that replacing traditional
disembodied with embodied concepts is a gain for science, and how it is in line with
the most recent neuroscience and cognitive findings. In their view, traditional
scientific thought misses that “what has always made science possible, is our
embodiment, not our transcendence of it, and our imagination, not our avoidance of
it.” (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999, p.93). It is exactly the human embodiment, the human
experience, and the use of metaphor and imagination that makes science possible. The
authors’ theory on metaphor as a central human capacity could benefit from creative
arts therapists knowledge on how metaphor on a nonverbal body-level and art-related
level is part of that capacity.

TABLE I: OVERVIEW OF MAJOR EMBODIMENT THEORIES


Major Embodiment Approaches (Overview)

Philosophy Merleau-Ponty, 1962 Perception is grounded in


Hurley, 1998 the body
Unity of perception and
action
Anthropology Lock & Scheper-Huges, 1987 The phenomenal body
Csordas, 1988, 2002 Culture is grounded in the
body
Linguistics Lakoff & Johnson, 1999 Concepts are grounded in
the body
Language/metaphor is
grounded in the body
Unity of perception and
conception
Neurosciences Gallese, 2003 Embodied
Cognition/Simulation
Artificial Clark, 1997 Intellect/being is grounded
Intelligence in the body
Psychology Varela, Thompson & Rosch, 1991 The embodied mind
Social Psychology Barsalou, Niedenthal et al., 2003 Cognition is grounded in
Niedenthal et al., 2005 the body
Attitudes/emotions are
grounded in the body
Memory Research Glenberg, 1997 Memory is grounded in
the body
Developmental Thelen, 1995; 2000 Development is grounded
Psych. in the body
Primacy of Motion

THE ROLE OF BODY AND MOVEMENT: EMBODIMENT APPROACHES IN


PSYCHOLOGY
Cognitive Psychology and Neurosciences: Cognition and Emotion are Grounded
in the Body
“We explicitly call into question the assumption -prevalent throughout cognitive science- that
cognition consists of the representation of a world that is independent of our perceptual and
cognitive capacities by a cognitive system that exists independently of the world. We outline
instead a view of cognition as embodied action“. (Varela, Thompson, & Rosch, 1991, p. XX)
Varela, Thompson and Rosch (1991) were the first authors to introduce
embodiment approaches into cognitive psychology. Their approach is revolutionary.
They state that cognitive science has hardly brought forth any applicable knowledge,
and therefore it was time to change this trend. Since their original work, embodied
mind theories gain influence in the context of explaining situated social cognition and
behavior (e.g., Jeannerod, 1997; Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber, &
Ric, 2005). Action simulation is an integral part of embodiment theorizing. Empirical
evidence for action simulation comes from Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Gallese
(2002) study on “mirror neurons” and subsequent research into similar “hard wired
evidence”. Rizzolatti et al. (2002) have shown that in apes observation of a grasping
movement, led to the same activation in motor centers that in the individual that was
actually performing the grasp, just less strong. Hormonal and endocrinal findings also
suggest convergent evidence for the central relevance of the body as an organ of
perception, internal communication, memory, and other human capacities (cf. Pert,
1997). Meanwhile a strong body of convergent evidence has accumulated in favor of
cognitive simulations while perceiving motor activity (Barsalou, 1999), of the
interconnectedness between perception and action (Hurley, 1998), on the sensory-
motor qualities of thought, shown by the activation of the same sensory-motor
pathways while thinking about the activity as if actually performing the activity
(Gallese, 2003; Rizzolatti et al., 2002). For CATs this means evidence for functional
mechanisms in relaxation and mental imagery exercises.

Developmental Psychology: Human Development is Grounded in the Body


The notion that development is grounded in the body is not new, ever since
Piaget’s influential work, it is the state-of-the-art knowledge and the ground that
cognitive developmental psychology builds upon. However, there have been a number
of outstanding theorists lately whose names are related to the new embodiment
approaches, particularly: Ester Thelen. Thelen describes the dynamics and situatedness
of human development and human motion in infant stepping ability (Thelen and
Smith, 1994; Thelen, 1995; 2000; similarly, Kelso, 1996, for adult rhythmic finger
motions). She found evidence for the influence of motor behavior on the cognitive and
affective development of the young infant. She writes about rhythms as fundamental
properties of infant movement. Nava Lotan a DMT researcher from Israel adapted
Thelen’s new approaches for her work on behavior patterns of small children with the
Kestenberg Movement Profile (Lotan & Yirmiya, 2002).
Additionally, Barbara Tversky (2002) points out that bodies are special kinds of
categories integral to all our perceptions. She reports own and other’s findings that
support the critical function of the body and of body motion in human development.
Bryant, Tversky, and Franklin (1992) hypothesized that reaction times would be
correlated with the degree of mental rotation needed to face an object. The results,
however, were contrary to the hypothesis. Subjects responded fastest to objects located
on the head/feet axis, followed by the front/back axis, followed by the left/right axis.
Tversky argues that these results follow from using a “spatial framework” that is
sensitive to environmental asymmetries (such as gravity) and perceptual asymmetries
(we generally look and attend to the front). In other words, retrieval processes seem to
be sensitive to how we use our bodies, and in this case related to the movement planes.
In a similar vein, Pauen and Träuble (2002), two developmental psychologists
specialized in early infant cognition, describe the primacy of motion in human
cognitive development. Motion perception is a basic cognitive process. The ability to
distinguish animate from inanimate objects is one of the first cognitive functions we
acquire in life (Pauen & Träuble, 2002). To recognize intention from motion is
psychologically and evolutionarily important at the most elementary level of social
cognition (Blythe, Todd, & Miller, 1999; Krämer, 2001). Motion is a major cue to
infer intentions and motivations, and to make causal attributions (Heider, 1958; Heider
& Simmel, 1944).
Social- and Language-Psychology: Social Cognition, Emotion, and Interaction are
Grounded in the Body
“Ago ergo cogito – I act, therefore I think” (Glenberg, 2005)
Preverbal motor development does not happen in the individual space, but in
the interpersonal space. We are social beings and social psychology has contributed a
new line of body-based research culminating in the social embodiment approach of
Barsalou, Niedenthal, Barbey, and Ruppert (2003). By embodiment Barsalou et al.
(2003) mean that “states of the body, such as postures, arm movements, and facial
expressions, arise during social interaction and play central roles in social information
processing” (Barsalou et al., 2003, p. 43). Social information processing means
cognitive processing (or thinking) related to social situations. Four types of
embodiment effects have been reported by social psychologists. First, perceived social
stimuli next to cognitive states produce bodily states as well. Second, perceiving
bodily states in others produces bodily mimicry in the self. Third, bodily states in the
self produce affective states. Fourth, the compatibility of bodily states and cognitive
states modulates performance effectiveness (Barsalou et al., 2003). Movement
therapists might be strongly reminded of the empathy theory of Theodor Lipps (1903;
cf. Wallbott, 1991) for the first three types of findings. The social embodiment
approach bundles single empirical results and other body-based research strings and
propose an alternative theoretical account for the workings of mind and memory with
reference to recent results in the neurosciences. Embodiment theories offer a new view
on knowledge representation. While traditional theories assume that a symbolic system
“redescribes” sensory, motor, and introspective states, resulting in amodal
descriptions, embodied theories of cognition proclaim the following:
“embodied theories represent knowledge as partial simulations of sensory, motor, and
introspective states (…). When an event is experienced originally, the underlying sensory, motor,
and introspective states are partially stored. Later, when knowledge of the event becomes
relevant in memory, language, or thought, these original states are partially simulated. Thus,
remembering an event arises from partially simulating the sensory, motor, and introspective
states active at the time. (…) Depending on the situation, embodiment may range from
simulation, to traces of execution, to full-blown execution. (…) these embodiments are not
merely peripheral appendages (…) of social information processing - they constitute the core of
it. (Barsalou et al., 2003, p. 44).
Most embodiment effects are unconscious and occur automatically
(Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). Wilson (2002) and Niedenthal et al. (2005) distinguish
online and offline embodiment. Online embodiment refers to a present situated
embodiment effect where cognitive activity operates directly on real-world
environments or vice versa. Offline embodiment refers to effects from memory, where
cognitive activity that is de-coupled from real-world environments. For example,
sitting in the classroom being called upon by the teacher and not knowing the answer
to any of her questions might cause cold sweat, a dry throat, gaze aversion, and an
increased heart rate (online). Imagining this situation may have the same bodily effects
(offline). This distinction has implications for CAT work with traumatized patients.
A theory of embodied memory has been suggested by Arthur Glenberg (1997).
Glenberg started from the open research question of how language conveys meaning
and suggests that linguistic meaning is grounded in bodily activity. Glenberg and
Kaschak (2002) found that participants were faster at judging the meaning of a
sentence when it was compatible with the hand movement required for the response
(e.g., “close the drawer” with forward movement; “open the drawer” with backward
movement). This action-sentence compatibility effect occurred even when the
sentences referred to abstract actions that involved directional communication (i.e.,
participants were fastest in judging the sentence “You told Liz the story” with a
forward movement and the sentence “Liz told you the story” with a backward
movement). These findings are consistent with the claim that language comprehension
is grounded in bodily action, and are inconsistent with abstract symbol theories of
meaning.

Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy: Healing is Grounded in the Body


Clinical practice is increasingly involved with nonverbal techniques, basically
without being conversant of their explicit way of working (e.g., EMDR, NLP, body
feedback, mirroring, passing, etc.). Authors such as Damasio (1994/1999), LeDoux
(1996), and Schore (1994) have indicated important directions in clinical thinking.
There is, however, no explicit embodiment approach in clinical psychology and
psychotherapy research. Even though the saluto-genetic approach (Antonovsky, 1997)
and trauma-related approaches (e.g., van der Kolk, 2003) are closely linked to
concerns in embodiment and creative arts therapies (Koch, in press). CAT research
could be contributing to understand the common principles at work behind all
therapies by looking at the therapeutic intervention techniques applied by therapists of
any school, and organizing them along the lines of embodiment theories.
Evidence for clinical relevance of embodiment research is presently again
coming from social psychological research: Piotr Winkielmann from the University of
California St. Barbara compared autistic subjects to typical subjects (Winkielman,
2005). On the basis of the mirror neuron theory and the amygdala theory to explaining
autism, he finds that particularly social mirroring is impaired in autists (McIntosh,
Reichmann-Decker, Winkielman, & Wilbarger, 2005). The treatment implied by this
research is the use of mirroring interventions to create reciprocity, interventions dance
therapists have been using ever since they worked with autists (Adler, 1969). This
DMTs specific autism treatment is now finally science based.

Three Examples for Research Applications


1. Embodiment and Basic Dimensions of Motion
Social psychology has just started to investigate basic embodiment dimensions
and their psychological meaning. The movement planes for example have been
addressed in latest psychological research by Schubert (in press) in which the vertical
plane is related to power, and by Boroditsky (2001) in which the sagittal plane is
related to time.
The bi-directionality of affect/cognition and motor behavior has been
investigated since research on the body feedback hypotheses suggested that the
incorporation of distinct facial expressions or postures leads to distinct emotions or
evaluations (see Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). This line of research supports
that next to emotions and cognitions causing certain motor behaviors and bodily
expressions, certain motor behaviors cause certain emotions and cognitions. The
notion of bidirectionality of affect/cognition and motor behavior supports the basic
maxim according to which CATs have been working from the beginning of the
profession. Yet, only recently has research in social psychology rendered empirical
evidence that confirms this assumption.
Research working on the specific embodiment effect of approach and
avoidance motor behavior on attitude formation (Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson,
1993; Neumann & Strack, 2000) can be directly related to the DMT concept of Shape
and Shape-Flow. Cacioppo, Priester and Berntson (1993) placed participants at a table
and asked them while studying arbitrary Chinese signs (written in Chinese script) to
either activate the flexor or the extensor of their arm muscles by pressing against their
table either from below or from above (from the table surface). The resulting
movement was thus either an approach or an avoidance movement, either towards the
body or away from the body. Those signs that had been studied while pulling the arms
towards the body were later judged as significantly more positive, than the signs that
had been studied while pushing against the table. This finding is consistent with other
“motor congruency effects” (Förster & Strack, 1996). For a dance/movement therapist,
parallels to shape-flow and shaping stand out (Laban, 1960; Laban & Lawrence,
1974). The findings of Cacioppo et al. (1993) are directly supporting KMP theory
about unipolar shape flow (Kestenberg-Amighi, Loman, Sossin, & Lewis, 1999;
Kestenberg & Sossin, 1979; Kestenberg, 1995) that offers a classification system for
attraction and repulsion motion toward and away from objects.

2. Body Memory
Merleau-Ponty (1962) has almost entirely spared the issue of body memory
from his otherwise very exhaustive discussion of the body’s role in perception. The
philosopher who has taken on the challenge of treating the topic of body memory was
Edward T. Casey (1987; cf. Pylvänäinen, 2003, and this volume). Casey defines body
memory as “memory that is intrinsic to the body, to its own ways of remembering:
how we remember in and by and through the body.” He emphasizes the tacit and non-
deliberate nature of such memory. In fact, body memory is so much part of the ground
of our experience that the topic has been ignored from the ancient Greeks to Kant.
Casey distinguishes habitual body memory, traumatic body memory, and erotic body
memory. Habitual body memory becomes salient when it is disrupted, such as when
one gets a new keyboard for the typewriter and finds oneself in a “state of
disorientation” at first. What Casey calls habitual body memory has much in common
with what cognitive psychology calls implicit memory. For Casey body memory is
located both in the objective, material body (fibers and tissues; cf. Pringer, 1995/2005)
and in the phenomenal, lived body (after Leib in Husserl). “Because it re-enacts the
past, it need not represent it; (…) body memory includes its own past by an internal
osmotic intertwining with it.” (Casey, 1987, p.88). As Fuchs (2004) writes
“body memory mediates the basic experience of familiarity and continuity in the succession of
events. It unburdens us from the necessity to constantly find our bearings again. Bodily learning
means to forget what we have learned or done explicitly, and to let it sink into implicit,
unconscious knowing. By this we acquire the skills and dispositions of perceiving and acting
that make up our very personal way of being in the world. We might also say: What we have
forgotten has become what we are. (Fuchs, 2004, p.3).
In contrast to memory categories of cognitive psychology, such as
autobiographic memory only representing the past as the past, body memory mediates
the living presence of the past. It is therefore the essential basis of the self. As such it
can be found entirely intact in dancing, singing, or sculpting with a demented patient.
Fuchs notes “Freedom and art are essentially based on the tacit memory of the body.”
(Fuchs, 2004, p.3).
Evidence in neuroscientific research is accumulating that active remembering,
such as the recall of representations or mental imagery, activates sensory (e.g.,
Wheeler, Peterson, & Buckner, 2000) as well as motor responses (e.g., Nyberg,
Pertersson, Nilsson, Sandbloom, Aberg, & Ingwar, 2001), and that in fact, sensory and
motor processing is more intimately linked than ever assumed (Tucker & Ellis, 1998).

3. Embodiment and Gender


Research is just starting to explore what differential implication embodiment
views have on the cognition and affect of men and women. Warren Lamb has worked
on gender differences in motion (Lamb, 1965; Davies, 2001). Recent findings from
embodiment research suggest, for instance, power related differences. Schubert (2004)
found that making a fist had different implications on cognition and emotions of
women and men. It influenced their automatic processing of words related to the
concept of power.
Embodiment theories are closely related to gender issues. As we increasingly
investigate embodiment we will also increasingly move to an understanding on the
different forms of embodiment in men, women, gays, transsexuals, etc. Development
of sex roles is necessarily intertwined with cognitive development, and both are
grounded in the body. It remains up to future research to start looking at the
implication gender has on embodiment (and vice versa). An example of recent
research relating gender and artificial intelligence comes from Alison Adam (2002).
Starting out to investigate the role of the body in the generation of knowledge, Adam
offers a critique of artificial intelligence research and embodiment in artificial life. She
points out that the specificities of gender (and other embodied variations; the author)
cannot be adequately addressed with methods such as computer simulations.

Outstanding Questions
Is the world centerless and is it just we searching for a center, an inner organizer (that some
may call god, some may call theory, some may call metaphor, some may call psyche, some may
call mind). Is it just we in search of sense and meaning (cf. Heider, 1958), contingency, and
predictability, that is not objectively there but merely constructed (by categorization,
dichotomization, attribution)? Or is the existential ground given in form of our bodies?
Does this existential ground bereave us humans of our special position in the universe?
Can we explain all perceptual and memory phenomena on the grounds of embodiment or are
embodiment theories just a constraint for specific cases? I.e., is the sensory-motor simulation
occurring in all cases or are there still other forms of processing information and of being? Is
embodiment a new paradigm that will replace cognitive science or will it find its place as a part
of it?
In the progression from cognitive sciences to embodied sciences, the mind is increasingly
viewed as part of the body. There are at present many attempts to find the neurological,
endocrinological, hormonal, etc. traces of the mind or consciousness. Is this the return to a
disenchanted view on life? Or is it, on the contrary, a reconciliation of the century-long
analytical separation of body, mind, and world?

CONCLUSIONS
Embodiment approaches offer new scientific perspectives for creative arts
therapies. Most embodiment approaches compile empirical results that are suited to
support CAT practice, to stimulate CAT research, and to explain how CAT work.
Particularly, the cognitive science model of modality specific knowledge
representation (Barsalou et al., 2003) lays a foundation for CAT theory development
and provides a rationale for CATs functioning. Topics of CAT relevance that thus far
have been standing isolated such as body memory, basic movement dimensions, or
body and gender can be tied to embodiment theories and find their place in a broader
scientific framework.
Embodiment approaches claim that the typical partition of the cognitive system
into a variety of neural or functional subsystems is often misleading. It blinds us to the
possibility of alternative, more explanatory categories that cut across the traditional
body/mind/world division (cf. Clark, 1997). For cognitive science this means that
researchers need to critically rethink their subject matter. For creative arts therapists
with their more holistic approach their task will be to diligently and thoroughly
formulate their embodiment ideas and make them available to other scientific
communities and outlets. The potential is a better visibility and a more explicit
formulation of CATs theory in the light of a new paradigm. The danger of the
embodiment view is a too explicit and one-sided focus on the body as mentioned
above. Nevertheless, all cognition and affect can be conceptualized as embodied and
grounded in the body and is describable in terms of its functions. Mind and body are
not two entities related to each other but a living inseparable whole.
Embodiment approaches strengthen the theoretical underpinnings of the bodily
basis of thought and affect. Theories of embodied cognition attribute new scientific
value to experience-based approaches and validate major theoretical assumptions in
DMT and CATs. In turn, DMT and CATs can offer their well-developed assessments
and experience-based knowledge to provide these theories with a higher degree of
exactness and differentiation. One starting point is the investigation of basic
dimensions of motion and their dynamic feedback on cognition and affect. This
research will be relevant to our fields wherever the manipulation of movement is
intended for the promotion of health, improvement of symptoms, or freedom from
symptoms. This applies to DMT, all other creative art therapies, all forms of body
psychotherapies, and physiotherapy. All of them work with the body as an instrument
of resonance and central relevance.

REFERENCES
Adam, A. (2002). Gender/Body/Machine. Ratio, 15, 354-389.
Adler, J. (1969). Looking for me. A film documenting Adler’s work with autistic children. Center for Media and
Independent Learning, 2000 Center Street, Berkely, CA, 94704.
Antonovsky, A. (1997). Salutogenese: zur Entmystifizierung der Gesundheit [Salutogenesis: The demystification
of health]. Tübingen: DGVT.
Barsalou, L.W., Niedenthal, P.M., Barbey, A.K., & Ruppert, J.A. (2003). Social Embodiment. In B. H. Ross
(Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 43 (pp. 43-92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Barsalou, L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.
Beer, R.D. (2000). Dynamical Approaches to cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 91-99.
Blythe, P.W., Todd, P.M., & Miller, G.F. (1999). How motion reveals intention. Categorizing social interactions.
In G. Gigerenzer, P. Todd, & the ABC Research Group (Eds.). Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp.
256-285). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time.
Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1-22.
Bryant, D. J., Tversky, B., & Franklin, N. (1992). Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing
described scenes. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 74–98.
Cacioppo, J.T., Priester, J.R., & Berntson, G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes II: Arm flexion and
extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5-17.
Casey, E. S. (1987). Remembering: A phenomenological study. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being There. Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 345-351.
Csordas, T.J. (2002). Body Meaning Healing. New York: Palgrave MacMillan.
Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.
Damasio, A.R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Orlando,
FL: Harcourt Brace.
Davies, Eden (2001). Beyond dance. Laban’s legacy of movement analysis. London: Brechin.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J.A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social
perception on social behavior. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 33
(pp. 1-40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Förster, J., & Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: A case of
conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421-430.
Fuchs, T. (2000). Leib Raum Person. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.
Fuchs, T. (2004). The Memory of the Body. Unpublished Manuscript.
Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations. The quest for a common mechanism.
Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London, 358, 517-528.
Glenberg, A.M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1-55.
Glenberg, A.M., & Kaschak, M.P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9,
558-565.
Glenberg, A. (2005). Laboratory for embodied cognition. http://psych.wisc.edu/glenberg/glenberglab/GLindex.
html Retreived 05/03/05
Goodill, S.W. (2005). An Introduction to Medical Dance/Movement Therapy. Healthcare in Motion.
Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J.T., & Rapson, R.L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Paris: Cambridge University Press.
Heider, F. (1958). The Psychologie of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley.
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology,
57, 243-259.
Hurley, S.L. (1998). Consciousness in Action. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Press.
Kelso, S.C. (1996). Dynamic Patterns. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Kestenberg, J.S., & Sossin, K.M. (1979). The role of movement patterns in development, Vol. 2. New York:
Dance Notation Bureau Press.
Kestenberg, J.S. (1995). Sexuality, body movement and rhythms of development. Northvale: Jason Aronson.
(Originally published in 1975 under the title Parents and Children)
Kestenberg-Amighi, J.K., Loman, S., Lewis, P., & Sossin, K.M. (1999). The meaning of movement.
Developmental and clinical perspectives of the Kestenberg Movement Profile. Amsterdam: Gordon &
Breach.
Koch, S.C. (in press). Theoretical Perspectives in Dance/Movemnet Therapy: A Vision for the Future. In S.L.
Brooke (in press). Handbook of the Creative Arts Therapies: History, theories, methods and applications.
Springfield, IL: CC Thomas.
Krämer, N.C. (2001). Bewegende Bewegung. Sozio-emotionale Wirkungen nonverbalen Verhaltens und deren
experimentelle Untersuchung mittels Computeranimation. [Moving Movement]. Lengerich: Pabst.
Laban, R.v. (1960). The mastery of movement. London: MacDonald & Evans.
Laban, R.v., & Lawrence, F. C. (1974). Effort: Economy in body movement. Boston, MA: Plays. (Originally
published in 1947)
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to Western
thought. New York: Basic Books.
Lamb, W. (1965). Posture and Gesture. London: Gerald Duckworth.
LeDoux, J. (1995). Emotions. Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209-235.
Lipps, T. (1903). Leitfaden der Psychologie (Kap. 14: Die Einfühlung, pp. 187-201). Leipzig: Wilhelm
Engelmann.
Lock, M.M. & Scheper-Hughes, N. (1987). The mindful body: A prolegomenon to future work in anthropology.
Medical Anthropology Quaterly, 1, 6-41.
Loman, S.T., & Brandt, R. (Eds.). (1992). The body-mind connection in human movement analysis. Keene, N.H.:
Antioch New England Graduate School.
Lotan, N. & Yirmiya, N. (2002). Body movement, presence of parents and the process of falling asleep in
toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(1), 81-88.
McIntosch, D.N., Reichmann-Decker, A., Winkielman, P. & Wilbarger, J.L. (2005). When the Social Mirror
Breaks: Deficits in Automatic, but not Voluntary Mimicry of Emotional Facial Expressions in Autism.
Manuscript under review.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routeledge.
Neumann, R. & Strack, F. (2000). Approach and avoidance: The influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive
cues on encoding of affective information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 39-48.
Niedenthal, P., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in Attitudes,
Social Perception, and Emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184-211.
Nyberg, L., Pertersson, K.M., Nilsson, L.G., Sandbloom, J., Aberg, C., & Ingwar, M. (2001). Reactivation of
motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. NeuroImage, 14, 521-528.
Pauen, S., & Träuble, B. (2002, April). Causal attribution of animate motion in 7-months-olds. Paper presented
at the biannual meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, Toronto, ON, Canada.
Pert, C. (1997). The Molecules of Emotion: Why You Feel the Way You Feel. Scribner, NY.
Pylvänäinen, P. (2003). Body Image: A tripartite model for the use in dance/movement therapy. American
Journal of Dance Therapy, 25, 39-55.
Pringer, C. (1995/2005). The fascia-memory project. http://hometown.aol.com/bridge22/FasciaMem Project.html
Retreived: 05/02/05
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2002). From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and
speculations. In A.N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds). The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain
bases (pp. 247-266). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sax, W. (2002). Dancing the Self. Personhood and Performance in the Pandav Lila of Garhwal. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Schore, A. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schubert, T.W. (2004). The power in your hand: Gender differences in bodily feedback from making a fist.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 757-769.
Schubert, T.W. (in press). Your highness: Vertical Positions as Perceptual Symbols of Power. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology.
Strathern, A.J. (1996). Body Thoughts. Ann Arbour: University of Michigan Press.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action.
Cambridge: MIT Press.
Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development -- A new synthesis. American Psychologist 50, 79-95.
Thelen, E. (2000). Grounded in the world: Developmental origins of the embodied mind. Infancy, 1, 3-28.
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830-846.
Tversky, B., Morrison, J.B., & Zacks, J. (2002). On bodies and events. In A.N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds). The
imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual
development (pp. 247-266). New York: Cambridge University Press.
van der Kolk, B.A. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder and the nature of trauma. In M. F. Solomon & D.J.
Siegel (Ed.). Healing trauma. Attachment, mind, body, and brain (pp. 168–195). New York & London:
Norton.
Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cognitive Science and Human Experience.
Cambridge: MIT Press.
Wallbott, H.G. (1991). Recognition of emotion from facial expression via imitation? Some indirect evidence for
an old theory. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 207-219.
Weizsäcker, V.v. (1996). Der Gestaltkreis. Theorie der Einheit von Wahrnehmen und Bewegen (6te
unveränderte Auflage). Stuttgart: Thieme. (Originally published in 1940).
Wheeler, M.E., Peterson, S.E., & Buckner, R.L. (2000). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates
sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
97, 11125-11129.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625-636.
Winkielman, P. (2005). Embodied emotional responses as exemplified in impairments such as autism.
Presentation at the Biannual Conference of the European Association of Experimnetal Social psychology
(EAESP) 19.-23.July, in Würzburg, Germany.
Zaner, R.M. (1964). The problem of embodiment. Some contributions to a phenomenology of the body. Nijhoff:
DenHaag.
Zajonc, R.B., & Markus, H. (1984). Affect and cognition: The hard interface. In C. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B.
Zajonc (Eds.). Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 73-102). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

View publication stats

You might also like