Process of Learning To Read
Process of Learning To Read
Process of Learning To Read
http://www.nap.edu/catalog/6023.html
41
Learning to read and write begins long before the school years,
as the biological, cognitive, and social precursors are put into place.
One of the most important preconditions for literacy is the integrity
of a child’s health and sensory organs, since the window for the
establishment of such skills as language is relatively brief. The child’s
intelligence, as long as it is in the normal range, does not have much
of an impact on the ease of learning to read (Stanovich et al., 1984).
The capacity to learn to read and write is related to children’s age-
related developmental timetables, although there is no clear agree-
ment on the precise chronological or mental age nor on a particular
developmental level that children must reach before they are “ready”
to learn to read and write.
Children gain an increasingly complex and decontextualized un-
derstanding of the world as their brains develop during their first
years of life. As they grow and gain experience, new neural connec-
tions are established at irregular rates, with spurts and plateaus
(Peterson, 1994). Although this process is orderly, it is variable
among individual children due to differences in both biological and
experiential influences.
Children who become successful readers tend to exhibit age-
appropriate sensory, perceptual, cognitive, and social skills as they
progress through the preschool years. Through the interaction of
maturation and experience, they become increasingly adept at mas-
tering physical dexterity and locomotion, at categorizing and con-
structing relationships between physical objects, at remembering
facts and events over time, at engaging in imaginative play, at form-
ing social relationships, and so forth.
Of course, many factors in an infant’s life can affect develop-
ment, ranging from maternal mental and physical health to condi-
tions of housing, temperament, nutrition, and emotional stress and
support. Although all these can have an impact on later literacy
sual word recognition can flourish only when children displace the
belief that print is like pictures with the insight that written words
are comprised of letters that, in turn, map to speech sounds. Even as
children begin to learn about spellings, they must also develop more
sophisticated understandings of the forces beyond pictures and indi-
vidual words that direct text meaning. These include, for example,
the nature of word, sentence, paragraph, and text structures and the
sorts of thinking and devices that hold them all together. Whereas
each such type of learning depends on experience and exploration, it
must also depend on certain conceptual insights.
For the child, Downing (1979:27) suggests, language is not an
object of awareness in itself but is “seemingly like a glass, through
which the child looks at the surrounding world, . . . not [initially]
suspecting that it has its own existence, its own aspects of construc-
tion.” To become a mature reader and writer, charged with con-
structing and corroborating the message of an author, this percep-
tion must change. Moreover, each such change must be guided by
the metalinguistic insight that language invites inspection and reflec-
tion. Indeed, literacy growth, at every level, depends on learning to
treat language as an object of thought, in and of itself (Halliday,
1982; Olson, 1995). See Box 2-1 for definitions of metacognition
and metalinguistic.
For most children, growing up to be a reader is a lengthy process
that begins long before formal instruction is provided in school or
elsewhere. The following sections offer a brief sketch of what is
BOX 2-1
More Definitions
Language Development
BOX 2-2
Key Definitions of the Components of Language
Phonological Awareness
BOX 2-3
Key Definitions of Some Terms
That Are Often Confused
Phonemic awareness is the insight that every spoken word can be con-
ceived as a sequence of phonemes. Because phonemes are the units of
sound that are represented by the letters of an alphabet, an awareness of
phonemes is key to understanding the logic of the alphabetic principle
and thus to the learnability of phonics and spelling.
The term phonological decoding or, more simply, decoding, refers to the
aspect of the reading process that involves deriving a pronunciation for a
printed sequence of letters based on knowledge of spelling-sound corre-
spondences.
Literacy Development
BOX 2-4
Goodnight Moon
1Routines with cultural significance as powerful as that of book reading do not appear to
be widespread in the area of writing, although this may be due to lack of relevant research
(Burns and Casbergue, 1992; Anderson and Stokes, 1984; Teale, 1986).
its linearity and use of recursive features. Read (1971) and Chomsky
(1975) were among the first to examine the writing of children whose
untutored spellings reflected phonetic and phonological analysis of
speech. Read (1975) demonstrated that children at these ages have
already developed conceptual categories for consonant and vowel
sounds in spoken English and that these categories, which were lin-
guistically sound, appeared to underlie the invented spellings found
in the children’s writing.
Although it appears that children are hard at work as scholars of
language, observations of children engaging in literacy activities in
homes and preschools depict them as playful and exploratory in
most of these activities.
Table 2-1 shows a set of particular accomplishments that the
successful learner is likely to exhibit during the preschool years.
This list is neither exhaustive nor incontestable, but it does capture
many highlights of the course of literacy acquisition that have been
revealed through several decades of research. Needless to say, the
timing of these accomplishments will to some extent depend on
maturational and experiential differences between children.
Comprehension
ments are only implicit (Trabasso and van den Broek, 1985; van den
Broek, 1994). Because texts cannot be fully explicit, situation mod-
els require the use of knowledge and inferences (see Fletcher et al.,
1994, for a review).
An important part of comprehension is concept development
and knowledge of word meanings. Vocabulary knowledge has long
been known to be a major correlate of comprehension ability, as
measured by standardized tests (e.g., Davis, 1944, 1968). Research
has found that comprehension is diminished by lack of relevant
word knowledge (Anderson and Freebody, 1983; Kame’enui et al.,
1982; Marks et al., 1974). Mezynski (1983) and Stahl and Fairbanks
(1986) reviewed a series of studies that trained subjects for word/
concept development to improve comprehension scores and found
that, when certain conditions of instruction were met, the gain in
comprehension was attained.
Of course, some comprehension of passages is possible, even
when a few of the words are unknown to the reader (Anderson and
Freebody, 1983; Kame’enui et al., 1982). Reading itself can provide
one with meanings for unfamiliar words, although readers also fail
to learn much about most of the unfamiliar words they encounter
(Jenkins et al., 1984; Nagy et al., 1985; Shu et al., 1995; Stahl et al.,
1989).
Comprehension monitoring is the ability to accurately assess
one’s own comprehension (Baker and Anderson, 1982; Garner,
1980; Otero and Kintsch, 1992; Vosniadou et al., 1988). To study
this, an inconsistency is introduced into a short text, to see whether
the reader detects it either during recall or when explicitly ques-
tioned. A typical result is that some readers do and some do not
detect these inconsistencies, and those who do tend to be either older
readers (compared with younger readers) or more skilled (compared
with less skilled) readers. A less skilled reader may fail to detect the
contradictions in texts because they have misconceptions about high-
level reading goals (Myers and Paris, 1978). An alternate explana-
tion is that less skilled readers have difficulties with the component
processes of representing a text (i.e., word identification and basic
comprehension) and that this difficulty rather than an independent
failure to employ a monitoring strategy is the source of the problem.
Word Identification
The identification of printed words has long been treated as a
skill that is essential for novice readers, yet it remains important in
skilled adult reading as well and is a necessary (but not sufficient)
factor for comprehension. By “word identification,” we mean that
the reader can pronounce a word, not whether he or she knows what
it means.
For a skilled reader, the identification of a printed word begins
with a visual process that operates on the visual forms of letters that
make up a word. The visual process is constrained by the sensitivity
of the retina, such that visual forms are perceived sufficiently for
identification only within a relatively narrow region (the fovea).
Studies of eye movements suggest that readers can correctly perceive
only 5 to 10 letters to the right of the fixation point (McConkie and
Rayner, 1975; Rayner and Pollatsek, 1987). The effect of this limi-
tation is that readers’ eyes must come to rest (fixate) on many words.
Visual processes initiate word identification and immediately trig-
ger other processes that complete it, including, most importantly,
phonological decoding processes, which concern the correspondences
between printed letters and the sounds of the language, especially
phonemes, the small sound units within spoken and heard words.
The research on reading in alphabetic writing systems has developed
an important consensus that phonological decoding is a routine part
of skilled word identification. How the phonological and visual-
orthographic information gets combined for the identification of
individual words has been the focus of much research, fueled in
recent years by theoretical debates about how to conceptualize the
2Indeed, it is becoming clear that, even in nonalphabetic systems, simple word identifica-
tion brings about an activation of the phonology of the word form, even if the reader’s task is
to determine meaning (Perfetti and Zhang, 1995).
BEGINNING TO READ
well, all involving the common core of the reading on which they
begin work in the early grades.
Most 5-year-olds from supportive literacy backgrounds continue
to make rapid growth in literacy skills. Children who are, as Hiebert
(1994) puts it, dependent on schooling for literacy, or who have
spent four or more years without rich support for literacy, will tend
to show patterns more like younger children. However, when such
children are asked or enticed into doing tasks such as “reading your
own way” or “writing your own way,” they do respond in interpret-
able ways rather than showing no knowledge.
Children during this period will “read” from books that have
been read to them frequently, increasingly showing the intonation
and wording patterns of written language in their pretend readings
(Purcell-Gates, 1991). Initially, they act as if pictures are what one
looks at when reading aloud from familiar stories (Sulzby, 1985b,
1994). When watching an adult read silently, they may insist that
something be said for reading to take place (Ferreiro and Teberosky
(1982), but five-year-olds increasingly engage in intensive scrutiny of
the pictures in a page-by-page fashion, as if reading silently before
they begin to “read to” another aloud in an emergent fashion. Some
of these emergent readings will focus on pictures as the source of the
text, but increasing numbers will begin to attend to the print.
Print-focused emergent readings are significant in a number of
ways. Children may temporarily refuse to read, saying that it is the
print that is read and they do not know how to do that. Or they may
temporarily read by focusing solely on an isolated feature of reading,
such as sounding out real words or nonsense strings with signs of
great satisfaction, picking out isolated strings of sight vocabulary
words, or tracking the print while reciting text parts that do not
match the print. These reading behaviors appear to indicate a period
during which the child is bringing together to the text bits and pieces
of knowledge about how print works from other contexts, such as
play, writing, and environmental print (Sulzby, 1985b, 1994).
Children’s writing also takes great strides forward during this
period. Children appear to move across various forms of writing
even up to grade 1, using scribble, nonphonetic letter strings, and
drawing as forms of writing from which they subsequently read.
Beginning
Becoming Productive
Progress in Understanding
guage and written language may produce some difficulties for learn-
ing to comprehend what one reads, and limits on background knowl-
edge or a lean conceptual vocabulary can affect some text passages
and not others. It is not clear that limits on inferencing processes for
reading- and comprehension-monitoring strategies can be viewed as
independent of the powerful effect of knowledge—background and
word knowledge as well as knowledge of the features of written
language that are not in the child’s oral language repertoire.
Research on what young good comprehenders do is not as far
along as research on children’s word processing. Studies that con-
trast skilled and less skilled comprehenders have shown that skilled
comprehenders are better at decoding (e.g., Perfetti, 1985), have
superior global language comprehension (Smiley et al., 1977), and
have superior metacognitive skills (Paris and Myers, 1981). As
Stothard and Hulme (1996:95) note, though, many studies use mea-
sures of comprehension that “confound decoding and comprehen-
sion difficulties” and are less useful for identifying the crucial fea-
tures of skilled comprehension in children. Few studies have been
completely successful, however, in avoiding this confound. Some
studies have matched subjects on decoding measured in oral reading
by counting errors.
In a series of studies of 7- and 8-year-olds in English schools,
Yuill and Oakhill (1991) compared children matched for chrono-
logical age and for reading accuracy but who differed significantly in
reading comprehension on a standardized norm-referenced test that
measures the two aspects of reading separately. The skilled com-
prehenders (at or slightly above the level expected for their chrono-
logical age in comprehension) were notable for the work they did
with the words and sentences they encountered in texts. For ex-
ample, they understood pronoun references, made proper inferences
about the text from particular words, drew more global inferences
from elements of the text that were not adjacent, detected inconsis-
tencies in texts, applied background knowledge, and monitored their
comprehension.
Stothard and Hulme (1996) compared similarly identified skilled
and less-skilled comprehenders but included a comprehension age
match for the less skilled as well and found an additional feature:
poor word recognition skills will reveal, for some children, stop-
pages in other areas that create comprehension problems; more re-
search is called for on factors related to comprehension growth from
birth to age 8 that may produce problems as children read to learn in
elementary school.
The “fourth-grade slump” is a term used to describe a widely
encountered disappointment when examining scores of fourth grad-
ers in comparison with younger children (see Chall et al., 1990).
Whether looking at test scores or other performance indicators, there
is sometimes a decline in the rate of progress or a decrease in the
number of children achieving at good levels reported for fourth grad-
ers. It is not clear what the explanation is or even if there is a unitary
explanation. The most obvious but probably least likely explana-
tion would be that some children simply stop growing in reading at
fourth grade.
Two other explanations are more likely. One possibility is that
the slump is an artifact; that is, the tasks in school and the tasks in
assessment instruments may change so much between third and
fourth grade that it is not sensible to compare progress and success
on such different tasks and measures. It may be that the true next
stage of what is measured in third grade is not represented in the
fourth-grade data and that the true precedents for the fourth-grade
data are not represented in the third-grade data.
A second possibility is that it is not so much a fourth-grade
slump as a “primary-grade streak,” that is, that some children have
problems in the earlier years that are hidden while so much else is
being learned, in the same way that a tendency to make errors in the
outfield does not bother a ball club while the pitching staff is having
a streak of strikeouts. Previously “unimportant” reading difficulties
may appear for the first time in fourth grade when the children are
dealing more frequently, deeply, and widely with nonfiction materi-
als in a variety of school subjects and when these are represented in
assessment instruments. It may be that there had been less call for
certain knowledge and abilities until fourth grade and a failure to
thrive in those areas might not be noticed until then. It is, of course,
this latter possibility that is important for preventing reading diffi-
CONCLUSION
Table 2-2 shows a set of particular accomplishments that the
successful learner is likely to exhibit during the early school years.
This list is neither exhaustive nor incontestable, but it does capture
many highlights of the course of reading acquisition that have been
revealed through several decades of research. Needless to say, the
timing of these accomplishments will to some extent depend on the
particular curriculum provided by a school. For example, in many
areas of the country, the kindergarten year is not mandatory and
little formal reading instruction is provided until the start of first
grade. The summary sketch provided by the table of the typical
accomplishments related to reading over the first years of a child’s
schooling presupposes, of course, appropriate familial support and
access to effective educational resources. At the same time, there are
enormous individual differences in children’s progression from play-
ing with refrigerator letters to reading independently, and many path-
ways that can be followed successfully.
Ideally, the child comes to reading instruction with well-devel-
oped language abilities, a foundation for reading acquisition, and
varied experiences with emergent literacy. The achievement of real
reading requires knowledge of the phonological structures of lan-
guage and how the written units connect with the spoken units.
Phonological sensitivity at the subword level is important in this
achievement. Very early, children who turn out to be successful in
learning to read use phonological connection to letters, including
letter names, to establish context-dependent phonological connec-
tions, which allow productive reading. An important mechanism for
this is phonological recoding, which helps the child acquire high-
quality word representations. Gains in fluency (automaticity) come
with increased experience, as does increased lexical knowledge that
supports word identification.
Briefly put, we can say that children need simultaneous access to
some knowledge of letter-sound relationships, some sight vocabu-
Kindergarten Accomplishments
• Knows the parts of a book and their functions.
• Begins to track print when listening to a familiar text being read or
when rereading own writing.
• “Reads” familiar texts emergently, i.e., not necessarily verbatim from
the print alone.
• Recognizes and can name all uppercase and lowercase letters.
• Understands that the sequence of letters in a written word represents
the sequence of sounds (phonemes) in a spoken word (alphabetic
principle).
• Learns many, thought not all, one-to-one letter sound correspondences.
• Recognizes some words by sight, including a few very common ones (a,
the, I, my, you, is, are).
• Uses new vocabulary and grammatical constructions in own speech.
• Makes appropriate switches from oral to written language situations.
• Notices when simple sentences fail to make sense.
• Connects information and events in texts to life and life to text
experiences.
• Retells, reenacts, or dramatizes stories or parts of stories.
• Listens attentively to books teacher reads to class.
• Can name some book titles and authors.
• Demonstrates familiarity with a number of types or genres of text (e.g.,
storybooks, expository texts, poems, newspapers, and everyday print
such as signs, notices, labels).
• Correctly answers questions about stories read aloud.
• Makes predictions based on illustrations or portions of stories.
• Demonstrates understanding that spoken words consist of a sequences
of phonemes.
• Given spoken sets like “dan, dan, den” can identify the first two as
being the same and the third as different.
• Given spoken sets like “dak, pat, zen” can identify the first two as
sharing a same sound.
• Given spoken segments can merge them into a meaningful target word.
• Given a spoken word can produce another word that rhymes with it.
• Independently writes many uppercase and lowercase letters.
• Uses phonemic awareness and letter knowledge to spell independently
(invented or creative spelling).
• Writes (unconventionally) to express own meaning.
• Builds a repertoire of some conventionally spelled words.
• Shows awareness of distinction between “kid writing” and
conventional orthography.
• Writes own name (first and last) and the first names of some friends or
classmates.
• Can write most letters and some words when they are dictated.
First-Grade Accomplishments
• Makes a transition from emergent to “real” reading.
• Reads aloud with accuracy and comprehension any text that is
appropriately designed for the first half of grade 1.
• Accurately decodes orthographically regular, one-syllable words and
nonsense words (e.g., sit, zot), using print-sound mappings to sound
out unknown words.
• Uses letter-sound correspondence knowledge to sound out unknown
words when reading text.
• Recognizes common, irregularly spelled words by sight (have, said,
where, two).
• Has a reading vocabulary of 300 to 500 words, sight words and easily
sounded out words.
• Monitors own reading and self-corrects when an incorrectly identified
word does not fit with cues provided by the letters in the word or the
context surrounding the word.
• Reads and comprehends both fiction and nonfiction that is
appropriately designed for grade level.
• Shows evidence of expanding language repertory, including increasing
appropriate use of standard more formal language registers.
• Creates own written texts for others to read.
• Notices when difficulties are encountered in understanding text.
• Reads and understands simple written instructions.
• Predicts and justifies what will happen next in stories.
• Discusses prior knowledge of topics in expository texts.
• Discusses how, why, and what-if questions in sharing nonfiction texts.
• Describes new information gained from texts in own words.
• Distinguishes whether simple sentences are incomplete or fail to make
sense; notices when simple texts fail to make sense.
• Can answer simple written comprehension questions based on material
read.
• Can count the number of syllables in a word.
• Can blend or segment the phonemes of most one-syllable words.
• Spells correctly three- and four-letter short vowel words.
• Composes fairly readable first drafts using appropriate parts of the
writing process (some attention to planning, drafting, rereading for
meaning, and some self-correction).
• Uses invented spelling/phonics-based knowledge to spell independently,
when necessary.
• Shows spelling consciousness or sensitivity to conventional spelling.
• Uses basic punctuation and capitalization.
• Produces a variety of types of compositions (e.g., stories, descriptions,
journal entries), showing appropriate relationships between printed
text, illustrations, and other graphics.
• Engages in a variety of literary activities voluntarily (e.g., choosing
books and stories to read, writing a note to a friend).
Second-Grade Accomplishments
• Reads and comprehends both fiction and nonfiction that is
appropriately designed for grade level.
• Accurately decodes orthographically regular multisyllable words and
nonsense words (e.g., capital, Kalamazoo).
• Uses knowledge of print-sound mappings to sound out unknown
words.
• Accurately reads many irregularly spelled words and such spelling
patterns as diphthongs, special vowel spellings, and common word
endings.
• Reads and comprehends both fiction and nonfiction that is
appropriately designed for grade level.
• Shows evidence of expanding language repertory, including increasing
use of more formal language registers.
• Reads voluntarily for interest and own purposes.
• Rereads sentences when meaning is not clear.
• Interprets information from diagrams, charts, and graphs.
• Recalls facts and details of texts.
• Reads nonfiction materials for answers to specific questions or for
specific purposes.
• Takes part in creative responses to texts such as dramatizations, oral
presentations, fantasy play, etc.
• Discusses similarities in characters and events across stories.
• Connects and compares information across nonfiction selections.
• Poses possible answers to how, why, and what-if questions.
• Correctly spells previously studied words and spelling patterns in own
writing.
• Represents the complete sound of a word when spelling independently.
• Shows sensitivity to using formal language patterns in place of oral
language patterns at appropriate spots in own writing (e.g.,
decontextualizing sentences, conventions for quoted speech, literary
language forms, proper verb forms).
• Makes reasonable judgments about what to include in written
products.
• Productively discusses ways to clarify and refine writing of own and
others.
• With assistance, adds use of conferencing, revision, and editing
processes to clarify and refine own writing to the steps of the expected
parts of the writing process.
• Given organizational help, writes informative well-structured reports.
˙ • Attends to spelling, mechanics, and presentation for final products.
• Produces a variety of types of compositions (e.g., stories, reports,
correspondence).
Third-Grade Accomplishments
• Reads aloud with fluency and comprehension any text that is appro-
priately designed for grade level.
• Uses letter-sound correspondence knowledge and structural analysis to
decode words.
• Reads and comprehends both fiction and nonfiction that is
appropriately designed for grade level.
• Reads longer fictional selections and chapter books independently.
• Takes part in creative responses to texts such as dramatizations, oral
presentations, fantasy play, etc.
• Can point to or clearly identify specific words or wordings that are
causing comprehension difficulties.
• Summarizes major points from fiction and nonfiction texts.
• In interpreting fiction, discusses underlying theme or message.
• Asks how, why, and what-if questions in interpreting nonfiction texts.
• In interpreting nonfiction, distinguishes cause and effect, fact and
opinion, main idea and supporting details.
• Uses information and reasoning to examine bases of hypotheses and
opinions.
• Infers word meanings from taught roots, prefixes, and suffixes.
• Correctly spells previously studied words and spelling patterns in own
writing.
• Begins to incorporate literacy words and language patterns in own
writing (e.g., elaborates descriptions, uses figurative wording).
• With some guidance, uses all aspects of the writing process in
producing own compositions and reports.
• Combines information from multiple sources in writing reports.
• With assistance, suggests and implements editing and revision to clarify
and refine own writing.
• Presents and discusses own writing with other students and responds
helpfully to other students’ compositions.
• Independently reviews work for spelling, mechanics, and presentation.
• Produces a variety of written works (e.g., literature responses, reports,
“published” books, semantic maps) in a variety of formats, including
multimedia forms.
INTRODUCTION TO READING 85
PART II
Who has reading difficulties and what are the factors present in
early childhood that predict failure and success in reading? Part II
addresses these questions.
Large numbers of school-age children, including children from
all social classes, have significant difficulties in learning to read. To
clarify this statement, we outline a number of conceptual issues in
identifying and measuring reading difficulties in young children.
Categorical and dimensional approaches to estimating reading diffi-
culties are presented, as are prevalence figures.
In a study on preventing reading difficulties, however, it is not
enough to assess actual reading difficulties. Ideally, we want to
know which children or groups of children will have problems learn-
ing to read when they are in school and given reading instruction.
Effective preventions are necessary for children to receive in their
preschool years, in some cases even starting in infancy—for example,
for children with hearing impairments. Thus, there is a need to
know what factors predict success and failure in learning to read.
We consider predictors that are:
85