Byers 1996
Byers 1996
Byers 1996
To cite this article: E. Sandra Byers PhD (1996): How Well Does the Traditional Sexual Script
Explain Sexual Coercion?, Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 8:1-2, 7-25
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.
Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-
licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly
forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any
representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The
accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently
verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of
the use of this material.
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
How Well Does the Traditional Sexual Script
Explain Sexual Coercion?
Review of a Program of Research
E. Sandra Byers, PhD
dude that the traditional sexual script has proven useful as a frame-
work for understanding sexual coercion in heterosexual dating
relationships. However, our research calls some aspects of this
theory into question. Some modification to this theory is needed.
[Arri.de copies available from 77ae Ha worih Documen[ Delivery Service:
1-800-342-9678.1
between 22% and 83% (depending on the wording of the items) of the
women surveyed report having been coerced into engaging in some form
of unwanted sexual activity at some time in the past (see Craig, 1990, for a
review of prevalence studies). A smaller, but substantial, percentage of
men (between 20% and 42%) acknowledge having engaged in sexual
activity by using sexual coercion (Craig,1990). Most of these incidents
occurred within the context of dating or other non-stranger peer relation-
ships.
Several theorists, most notably feminist theorists, have hypothesized
that socialization practices with respect to traditional gender roles, and
corresponding cultural attitudes, cause sexual coercion (Brownmiller,
1975; Burt, 1980; Clark & Lewis, 1977). Although not always labeled in
this way, these theorists posit that the "traditional sexual script" supports
and condones male sexual coercion against women and that this sexual
script remains the normative dating script in North American society.
These theories stand in contrast to theories that place the causes of sexual
coercion in specific characteristics of offenders that are not seen to be
normative, such as offenders' psychopatho~ogy,coercive dispositions, or
hypererotic orientation (Craig, 1990;Kanin, 1985).
In this article, I first review the aspects of the traditional sexual script
that have been theorized to promote and maintain sexual coercion. Then 1
present the results of a program of research I conducted in conjunction
with colleagues and former graduate students that is designed to test the
validity of this theory for understanding coercive sexual interactions be-
tween dating partners.
what, where, when, how, and why of sexual behavior (DeLamater, 1987;
Gagnon, 1990; Gagnon & Simon, 1973; McComick, 1987; Rosen &
Leiblum, 1988). They also depict the sequence in which behaviors are
expected to occur within sexual intcractions. Individuals may improvise
and express their own personajities and preferences while still adhering to
the guidelines prescribed by the script.
Sexual scripts are teamed through socialization. The sexual script that
is most pervasive in North America has been termed the traditionai sexual
script (TSS), and it contains very different expectations for men’s and
women’s behavior and attitudes in sexual situations. It is these prescribed
gender differences which are hypothesized to link adherence to the TSS
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
and sexual coercion (cf. Brownmillet, 1975; Byers, 1990; Clark & Lewis,
1977; Crooks & Baur, 1993; Korman & Leslie, 1982; LaPtante, McCor-
mick & Btannigan, 1980; Peplau, Rubin & Hill, 1977). In the following
list, I have summarized the gender differences in expected behaviors and
attitudes within the TSS that have been theorized to be particularly impor-
tant to the occurrence of sexual coercion. As the TSS prescribes that
sexual relationships be heterosexual, the following discussion describes
only male-female interactions.
(1) The TSS depicts men as “aversexed” and women as “undersexed.”
As such, men are described as having strong sexual needs, being obsessed
with sex, being highly motivated to engage in sexual activity, and willing
to exploit or pursue any sexual opportunity made available by a woman.
Women, on the other hand, are depicted as having few sexual needs, being
sexually reluctant, seeing sex as a means of procuring love or commit-
ment, being slow to arouse, and being dificult to satisfy sexually.
(2) Women’s perceived worth and status are seen as being decreased by
sexual experience whereas men’s worth and status are seen as being en-
hanced. That is, for men, sexual experience is perceived by society and by
their peers as reflecting positive characteristicssuch as masculinity, viril-
ity, and attractiveness. Fpr women, on the other hand, sexual experience is
attributed to undesirable characteristics such as nonselectivity, promiscu-
ity, emotional disturbance, and lack of values.
(3) The TSS casts men as the initiators in sexual situations and women
as the recipients of the initiations. Thus, because of their supposed large
sexual appetites, men are expected to initiate and vigorously pursue dates
with women, all sexual interactions, and increasingly intimate sexual activ-
ities within any given sexual interaction. Women are expected to adopt a
passive, defensive stance in order to protect their perceived worth. They
are expected to be prepared for and to respond cautiously to these initia-
tions.
10 SEXUAL COERCION IN DATiNG RELATIONSH/PS
(4) Because women are depicted as not “really” interested in sex and as
having their worth decreased by being “too” sexually available or seem-
ing “too” interested in sex, the TSS prescribes that women are expected to
place limits on the level of sexuai activity in which they engage with their
male partner. Thus, women are expected to counteract, force€idly, men’s
continual pressure to increase sexual intimacy. Even when they are inter-
ested in engaging in sexual activity, women are expected to offer at least
initial token resistance to the man’s advances. It should be noted that
forceful resistance is inconsistent with women’s gender role scripts, which
dictate that women should be passive, submissive, and unassertive, On the
other hand, men are expected to try to remove women’s restrictions to
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
enhance their own worth and meet their own sexual needs. As such, the
TSS legitimizes men’s use of a variety of coercive and noncoercive influ-
ence strategies in attempting to overcome the woman’s reluctance to en-
gage in the sexual activity. This expectation is similar to behavioral ex-
pectations within men’s gender role script, which prescribes that men be
active and aggressive. Use of these strategies is further justified by wide-
spread belief in women’s use of token refusal. As such, men who accept
the woman’s refusal and stop their sexual advances may be perceived as
not sufficiently masculine to gain sexual access (Muehlenhard, 1988;
Muehlenhard & Hollabaugh, 1988).
Two additional aspects of the TSS have been described as contributing
to sexual coercion but are not limited to sexual situations.
( 5 ) The dating and gender role scripts dictate that women’s worth is
enhanced by being in a romantic relationship. Thus, within the TSS, a
woman is expected to restrict sexual access but to do so in a way that does
not cause the man to decrease his romantic interest in her. To accomplish
this, she should not be too assertive or aggressive in refusing sexual
advances, she must appear sexy, and she must convey (through willing
participation in low levels of sexuai activity) that satisfying sex will occur
in the future if the man remains in the relationship.
(6) Women are expected to be emotional, sensitive, and nurturing in
interpersonal relationships, whereas men are expected to be unemotional,
relatively insensitive, and self-focused. The prescription for women to be
nurturing and consider the other person’s need before their own is in
conflict with the aspect of the TSS that calls for women to restrict access to
her sexuality. The latter would require that the woman place her needs and
wants before those of her male partner. The prescription that men be
unemotional and put their own needs first suggests that, in pursuing their
sexual goals, men need not take into account the woman’s feelings or
reluctance to engage in the sexual activity.
E. Sandra Byers I1
whether the TSS is the normative sexual script in North American dating
relationships. Other questions that need to be addressed are: How accurate
are the gender role stereotypes described in the TSS? To what extent are
aspects of the TSS related to the use and experience of sexual coercion?
We addressed these broad questions in a series of studies that are reviewed
in the following sections.
than the women did. However, initiations by the women were not rare,
albeit only about half as frequent as initiations by the men in the study. In
fact, the majority of participants reported one or more sexual initiations by
the woman during a two-week period. Of interest, initiations by women
were more likely to occur within a steady dating relationship. Thus, the
sexual script that designates men as the initiators appears to apply more
strongly in casual relationships. Other researchers have found that men’s
overt sexual advances are usually preceded by women’s signals communi-
cating sexual interest and that women are more likely than men to begin
sexual encounters (Moore, 1985; Moore & Butler, 1989; Perper, 1989;
Perper & Weis, 1987). Moreover, in contrast to predictions based on the
TSS, after controlling for the number of initiations, the women were no
more likely to refuse a sexual initiation by their partner than were the men.
Only a minority of initiations by the men or the women (20%) resulted in a
refusal by the partner, calling into question the view that most sexual
interactions between men and women are adversarial. The results also
indicate that in the typical sexual interaction, and particularly within
steady dating relationships, women are not serving a restrictive function,
and men do not feel obliged to accept every available sexual opportunity.
In so far as the TSS and its stereotypes regarding initiation and restriction
of sexual activity are used to explain sexual coercion, the explanation is
likely to be inadequate, incomplete, or inaccurate.
Men j. compliance wirh women k rel;ls~ls.Based on the argument that
the TSS is the normative sexual script, we predicted that men typically
would wish to engage in a higher level of sexual activity than would
women (that is, that adversarial sexual relationships are normative). We
also predicted that, given a situation in which the man wanis to engage in a
higher level of sexua1 activity than the woman does, men typically wouId
use some form of verbal or physical coercion to try to gain sexual access
(i.e., use of coercion by men to overcome resistance is normative). To test
E. Sandra Byers I3
these predictions, Kim Lewis and I asked unmarried college men and
women to self-monitor their dating and sexual experiences over four
weeks (Byers & Lewis, 1988). Respondents recorded whether they had
been on a date, whether any level of sexual activity had occurred on that
date, and whether the man had desired to engage in a higher level of sexual
activity than did the woman. If respondents experienced a disagreement,
they recorded the words and actions used to communicate about the differ-
ing levels of desired sexual intimacy. Descriptions of the man’s behavior
following the woman’s refusal of his sexual advance were coded in terms
of the degree ofthe man’s compliance or noncompliance with the woman’s
refusal.
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
male sexual coercion in dating relationships that led, in part, feminists and
others to conclude that sexual interactions between men and women typi-
cally are adversarial and contain coercive elements. However, our research
has failed to support some aspects of the TSS as being nonnative or typical
of sexual interactions in dating relationships. Contrary to predictions
based on the TSS,we did not find that, in these sexual interactions, men
typically attempt to extend the sexual boundaries, women typically try to
serve a restrictive function, or men g p i d l y attempt to coerce the sexually
reluctant woman to engage in the disputed sexual activity. Rather, we
found that men and women typically agree about the desired level of
sexual activity. When a disagreement does arise, it is most typical for men
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
The TSS casts men as the coercers and women as the coerced in sexual
situations. Early researchers adopted this assumption and only investi-
gated male sexual coercion of women. In keeping with recent research
using U.S. college students (Muehlenhard ?i Cook, 1988; Struckman-
Johnson, 1988), we have found evidence that a substantial percentage of
Canadian male college students report having experienced sexual coercion
by a woman (O’Sullivan, 1991; Finkelman, 1992). However, in both stud-
ies, a larger percentage of women than men reported having been coerced
to engage in unwanted sexual activity within a one-year period. For exam-
ple, Larry Finkelman,Lucia O’Sullivan, and I found that 17% of male
respondents compared to 36% of the female respondents reported having
had an unwanted sex experience because of sexual coercion by their het-
erosexual dating partner during one year of university (Finkelman, 1992).
Of particular interest with regard to critiquing the validity of the TSS, in
16 SEXUAL COERCION IN DA TlNG RELATIONSHIPS
both studies some women reported having used sexual coercion against a
reluctant male partner.
In keeping with the TSS, our early research on disagreement situations
only examined situations in which the man desired to engage in a higher
level of sexual activity than did the woman (Byers & Lewis, 1988). How-
ever, in a recent study, O’Sullivan and I examined situations in which the
woman desired to engage in a higher level of sexual activity than did her
male dating partner (O’Sullivan & Byers, 1993). We also extended past
research by examining the range of influence strategies used in disagrec-
ment situations by the “ardent” partner, not just the negative or coercive
strategics that have been studied in the past. We found that 56% of partici-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
suggested. However, although men and women are not always able to live
up to their sexual ideal, in sexual situations these roles may be converging.
Summay. The gender roles described in the TSS are accurate in de-
scribing behavior that is more characteristic of men or of women. Thus,
men morc often than women take the role of initiator, use coercion, and
engage in other instrumental behaviors. Women are more expressive than
arc men. However, there is also cvidence of considerable overlap in men’s
and women’s roles in sexual situations, as well as of convergence in what
is perceived as ideal behavior for men and women. In emphasizing gender
differences, the TSS may not suficiently describe gender similarities
(Hyde, 1985, 1994).
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
that men who reported having engaged in both consenting and noncon-
senting sex tended to hold more traditional views of women’s roles as well
as to subscribe more strongly to coercion-supportive beliefs (Byers & Eno,
1991; Grenier & Byers, 1990). Similarly, in the role-play study of men’s
responses to women’s refusals of their sexual advances, Wilson and I
found that men who had more liberal attitudes toward women’s roles were
more compliant with the woman’s refusals (Byers &Wilson, 1985). These
findings are in keeping with the results of several other studies (Koss,
Leonard, Beezley & Oros, 1985; Muehlenhard & Linton, 1987; Rapaport &
Burkhart, 1984). Also in keeping with predictions based on the TSS,more
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
their erotophobia (Fisher, Byme, White, & Kelley, 1988). Thus, they may
act according to their beliefs that force is an expected and accepted means
of resolving discrepancies in the desired level of sexual intimacy. These
characteristics are similar to Kanin’s { 1985) description of hypererotic
date rapists who hold an exaggerated view of the importance of engaging
in extensive sexual activity and conquest and who also believe that their
peers would support their aggressive behavior.
Taken together, these results suggest that the TSS contributes to sexual
coercion in that both coercion-supportive and traditional gender role be-
liefs, as well as active dating, were independently associated with coercive
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
sexual behavior. However, the data also suggest, in keeping with the
results of the self-monitoring study of sexual disagreement situations, that
use of physical force is not part of the typical sexual script for dating men
(Byers & Lewis, 1988). In addition, in at least somc instances, factors that
are not part of depictions of the TSS apparently are related to men’s use of
physical force.
Pwdicting wumen k experience of sexual coercion. We have conducted
a number of studies to examine women’s behavior in situations in which
there is a discrepancy in the level of sexual intimacy desired by women
and by their male partners. We did so to test predictions based on the TSS
and not to suggest that women should be held responsible, in any way, for
their own victimization.
The TSS prescribes roles for women in sexual situations that might put
them at risk for experiencing sexual coercion or, at least, reduce their
effectiveness in responding to unwanted sexual advances. For example,
the TSS prescribes that women behave unassertively with men, particular-
ly if they want to keep the man’s romantic interest in them, and that, to
avoid being devalued, they should agree only to engage in lower levels of
sexual intimacy. Thus, women would be expected to be more definite in
their refusals of higher level sexual activities than of lower level sexual
activities and in their rehsals of advances by men in whom they had less
romantic interest. Barbara Gilcs and I tested these predictions using role-
play scenarios (Byers et al., 1987). Each scenario described the man mak-
ing one of three sexual advances, indicated the woman’s level of romantic
interest in her date, and instructed the female participants that they did not
want to engage in that activity, The women then role-played how they
would communicate their nonconsent to their date. In keeping with predic-
tions based on the TSS, the verbal refusals of the women were less definite
in the scenarios in which they were described as having greater romantic
interest in their date. The women were also less verbally definite in the
E. Sandra Byers 21
at greater risk, more definite refusals will not necessarily protect women
from experiencing sexual coercion. Three of the men who engaged in the
extended role-play became less compliant and three other men ignored both
of the women’s refusals and continued their advances even after the second
refusal (Byers, 1988). The remaining 26% of the men did not become
either more or less compliant in response to the second refusal. Moreover,
3 1% of the men indicated that they would initiate the rehsed advance
again the same evening, suggesting that they believed that the woman’s
refusals were insincere. Needless to say, even if a woman’s less assertive
behavior puts her at greater risk to experience male sexual coercion, she is
still not in any way responsible or to’blame for her own victimization.
CONCLUSION
For example, they may adopt femate sexual initiation, male restriction,
and female coercion. Our research calls into question the assertion that the
TSS is the normative script for dating interactions. Rather, it may be one of
a number of common and traditional scripts. Moreover, our research has
demonstrated that the sharp distinctions between appropriate behavior for
men and women in sexual dating situations are missing. Men’s and
women’s roles in sexual interactions overlap considembly, particularly in
established relationships.
Even though notions of appropriate behavior for men and women in
sexual situations may be evolving, the concept of the TSS and its role in
sexual coercion should not be abandoned. The TSS has proven valuable
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
REFERENCES
Bem, S. ( I 974). The measurement of psychological androgeny. Journal of Con-
sulfing and Clinical Psycholugy, 42, 155-1 62.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Burt, M. R. (1980). CuItural myths and support for rape. Journal ofPersonality
and Social Psychology, 38,2 17-230.
Byers, E. S. (1988). Effects of sexual arousal on men and women’s behavior in
sexual disagreement situations. The Journal of Sex Research, 25,235-254.
Byers, E. S. ( I 990, November). Research on the traditional ~ I Z U Qscript.
~ Sympo-
sium presented at the meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Sex,
Minneapolis.
Byers, E. S., & Eno, R. (1 991). Predicting men’s sexual coercion and aggression
from attitudes, dating history, and sexual response. Journal of Psychology dt
Human Sauali@, 4,5569.
Byers, E. S.,Giles, €3. L., & Price, D. L. (1987).Definiteness and effectiveness of
women’s responses to unwanted sexual advances: A laboratory investigation.
Basic and Applied Social Psychoiogv,8,32 1-338.
Byers, E. S.,& Lewis, K. (1 988).Dating couples’ disagreements over the desired
level of sexual activity. 7’he J ~ u n t a olf Sex Research, 24, 15-29.
Byers, E. S., & Wilson, P. (1985). Accuracy of women’s expectations regarding
men’s responses to refusals of sexual advances in dating situations. Interna-
tional Journal of Women’sStudies, 4, 376387.
Clark, L.,& Lewis, D. (1977).Rape: The price ofcoercive s a u d i v . Toronto: The
Woman’s Press.
Craig, M. E. (1990). Coercive sexuality in dating relationships: A situational
model. Clinical Psychology Review, 10,395-423.
24 SEXUAL COERClON IN DATING RELATIONSHIPS
Crooks, R., & Baur, K. (1993). Our sexuality (5th ed.). Redwood City, CA:
Benjamin/Cummings.
DeLamater, J. (1987). Gender differences in sexual scenarios. In K. Kelley (Ed.),
Females, males and sexualiy: Theories and research (pp. 127-1 39). Albany,
N Y SUNY Press.
Finkelman, L. (1992). Report of the survey of unwanted sexual experiences
among students of UNB-F and STU. Unpublished manuscript, University of
New Bnmswick, Fredericton, NB.
Fisher, W. A., Byrne, D., White, L. A., & Kelley, K. (1988). Erotophobia-eroto-
philia as a dimension of personality. The Journnl of Sex Research, 25, 123- 15 I.
Gagnon, J. H. (1990). The explicit and implicit use of the scripting perspective in
sex research. In J. Bancroft (Ed.), Annual review of sex research, I , 1-43.
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 04:44 30 July 2012
Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (1973). Sexual conducr: The social sources of human
smualify,Hinsdale, IL: Dryden Press.
Grenier, G., & Byers, E. S.(1990, November), Patterns of coercive sexual behav-
iour in men. In E. S. Byers (Chair), Research on the rraditional sexual script.
Symposium presented at the meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of
Sex, Minneapolis.
Hiller, D., & Philliber, W. (1985). Internal consistency of the Bem Sex Role
Inventory. Social Psychology Quarrerly, 48,373-380.
Hyde, J. S. ( 1 994). Understanding human sexuality (5th ed.). New York: McGraw
Hill.
Hyde, J. S . (1985). Harlhe human experience: The psychology ofwomen (3rd
ed.). Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath.
Kanin, E. J. ( I 985). Date rapists: Differential sexual socialization and relative
deprivation. Archives ojSexual Behavior, 1 4 , 2 19-231,
Korman, S. K., & Leslie, G. R. (1982). The relationship of feminist ideology and
date expense sharing to perceptions of sexual aggression in dating. The Jour-
nal ofsex Research, 18, 1 14- 129.
Koss, M. P., Leonard, K.I., Beezley, D. A., & Oros, C. J. (1985). Nonstranger
sexual aggression: A discriminant analysis of the psychological characteristics
of undetected offenders. Sex Roles, 12,98 1-992.
LaPlante, M. M., McCormick, N., & Brannigan, G. G. (1980). Living the sexual
script: College students' views of influence in sexual encounters. The Journal
of Sex Research, 16,338-355.
Lawrance, K., Taylor, D., & Bycrs, E. S.(1990, November). Variations in global,
sexual, and ideal-sexual gender role descriptions. In E. S. Byers (Chair), Re-
search on the tradirional sexual script. Symposium presented at the meeting of
the Society for the Scientific Study of Sex, Minneapolis.
Lisak, D.,& Roth, S. (1988). Motivational factors in nonincarcerated sexually
aggressive men. Journal of Personalily and Social Psychology, 55, 795-801.
McCormick, N. B. (1987). Sexual scripts: Social and therapeutic implications.
Sexual and Marital Therapy, 2,3-27.
Moore, M. M. (1985). Nonverbal courtship patterns in women. Ethology and
Sociobiology, 6, 237-247.
E. Sandra Byers 25