The Russian Violin School
The Russian Violin School
The Russian Violin School
Foreword
Acknowledgments
Note on Transliteration
About the Companion Website
This first form of this book was published in Moscow in 1983 under the title
Pedagogicheskoe Nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy). Compiled by Yuri
Yankelevich’s sister, Elena Yankelevich, ten years after Yuri Yankelevich’s
passing, the Russian version of the book consisted of two methodological
texts by Yuri Yankelevich and various supplemental essays by Yankelevich’s
students and colleagues. The book was reedited and reprinted in Russian in
1993, 2002, and 2009. In 1999 the book was translated into French as Yuri
Yankelevitch et l'ecole russe du violon. The English translation of the book in
the present volume, The Russian Violin School: The Legacy of Yuri
Yankelevich, is based on the 2009 Russian edition and retains the two
methodological texts by Yuri Yankelevich and the original essays by Maya
Glezarova and Vladimir Grigoryev. In lieu of the other supplemental material
the companion website www.oup.com/us/therussianviolinschool to this book
provides updated biographical information on Yankelevich’s students,
selected video interviews, the original essay by Gregory Zhislin and further
resources. Aside from the introduction and unless otherwised specified, all
material is translated from the original Russian to English.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
With great gratitude to Yuri Yankelevich’s nieces, Irina and Nataliya Lifshits,
for making the publication of this edition possible and for continuing the
commitment of their mother, Elena Yankelevich, to preserving Yankelevich’s
legacy. I would also like to thank all of Yankelevich’s students and assistants
who have generously shared their experiences and assisted with the
supplementary materials, including Alexandre Brussilovsky (who released
the French translation), Dmitry Sitkovetsky, Mikhail Bezverkhni, Lydia
Dubrovskaya, Irina Medvedeva, Ilya Grubert, Lev Markiz, Vladimir
Landsman, Eugenia Chugaeva, and Maya Glezarova.
I thank my violin teacher Boris Roninson for introducing me to this branch
of the Russian Violin School, and my violin teacher for five years at Indiana
University, Nelli Shkolnikova. One of Yankelevich’s first students to receive
international recognition, Nelli Shkolnikova was extremely faithful to
Yankelevich’s principles, and it was she who many years ago first handed me
Yankelevich’s book with the dream that it would one day be available in
English. It is to her memory I dedicate this translation.
Translation is often a painstaking process, and I am extremely grateful to
my family, friends and colleagues for their advice and support. Thanks to
Andrew Maillet for preparing the musical examples, and to all the editors at
Oxford University Press and Newgen Knowledgeworks for steering this book
through completion. I thank my doctoral advisers Jane Palmquist and Joseph
Straus who supported this project since the beginning; and all the readers of
my drafts, including Mai Kawabata, Michael Appleman, Greg Erickson, and
my mother, Tatiana Putilina, without whose love this project would also not
be possible.
NOTE ON TRANSLITERATION
For the most part in this book, transliterated terms are spelled according to
the table provided by the US Board on Geographic Names, except for proper
names that are familiar to readers in other spellings. For ease in accessing
further research, the transliteration consistent with the Library of Congress is
also provided for names and titles in the bibliography (although for clarity,
double capitalization is omitted). English translations of Russian titles in the
notes and bibliography are the translator’s own.
ABOUT THE COMPANION WEBSITE
www.oup.com/us/therussianviolinschool
MASHA LANKOVSKY
Yuri Yankelevich was one of the preeminent Russian violin teachers of the
twentieth century. He taught at the Moscow Conservatory from 1936 to 1973
and produced an exceptional number of outstanding students, including over
forty prize-winners in international competitions. Yankelevich was keenly
interested in the methodology of violin playing and teaching and contributed
a significant number of musical editions and pedagogical texts (the latter are
translated in English in this volume for the first time).
As an heir to the rich traditions of violin playing in Russia, Yankelevich
was particularly influenced by the violin teachers Lev Tseitlin, Konstantin
Mostras, and Abraham Yampolsky who helped establish the Moscow Violin
School following the Russian Revolution of 1917. Because of limited
communication with the West during the Soviet years, the methodological
approach of these teachers has largely remained unknown outside Russia.
Despite the huge success of many Soviet violinists in international
competitions, few were allowed to travel freely outside of Russia and little
was known of the methodology behind their playing. This lack of information
has often led to vague and imprecise characterizations of what became known
as the “Russian Violin School” in the West. Yankelevich’s scholarly works
shed light on the pedagogy of the Moscow and Soviet Violin Schools and
reveal a modern analytical and individual approach, which incorporates
elements of psychology and physiology as well as detailed analysis of the
most efficient techniques, all in the service of artistry and individual
expression.
THE RUSSIAN VIOLIN SCHOOL
Yuri Yankelevich was born into the cultured family of a renowned Omsk
lawyer, who was also an amateur violist and a founder of the Omsk
Philharmonic Society. Yankelevich’s mother was an accomplished pianist,
and the young boy grew up with chamber music resounding in the home.
Yankelevich made quick progress studying the violin with Anisim Berlin, a
student of Auer’s who had moved to Omsk.7 In 1924 the family moved to
Leningrad (as St. Petersburg was named at the time), and Yankelevich
entered the Leningrad Conservatory as a student of Ioannes Nalbandyan.
Yankelevich soaked in the creative atmosphere at the conservatory, and he
recalled with fondness the deep impressions made by Alexander Glazunov’s
chamber music classes and meetings with other faculty, including Sergei
Korguyev. Outside the conservatory, Yankelevich was also influenced by the
rich artistic cultural life of 1920s Russia, especially by such luminaries of the
theater and opera as Ivan Yershov, Fyodor Chaliapin, and Alexander
Ostuzhev. The influence of opera on Yankelevich is not coincidental, for one
of the most characteristic traits not only of Yankelevich’s methodology but
indeed of the entire Russian Violin School is the idea that the violin should
“sing.”8 In 1928 Yankelevich began his graduate studies in Moscow with
Abraham Yampolsky, who was to have a profound effect on Yankelevich’s
future career. Yankelevich was appointed assistant concertmaster of the
Moscow Philharmonic in 1930, but gradually he felt himself more and more
drawn to pedagogy. Playing in itself did not provide him with complete
satisfaction, and as his colleagues would often approach him for tips or
fingerings he found his recommendations to be successful, inspiring him as a
pedagogue musician. Yankelevich recalls how, in following studies with
Yampolsky, “I began to be consciously drawn to pedagogy, to the cognitive
aspects of the theory and practice of violin playing… . From Yampolsky I
understood that violin playing is not a miracle, not ‘alchemy,’ but a science
and that besides inspiration there exist objective rules, which, when
combined with serious work, can accomplish a great deal.”9
In 1932 Yankelevich started teaching at the Central Music School and the
specialized high school (uchilische).10 In 1936 he became Yampolsky’s
assistant, and his life was then forever tied to the Moscow Conservatory.
After seventeen years as Yampolsky’s assistant, Yankelevich was given his
own class at the conservatory in 1953.
The first of Yankelevich’s students to achieve international recognition
was Nelli Shkolnikova, who won first prize at the Jacques Thibaud
competition in 1953. Nelli Shkolnikova started studying with Yankelevich
when she was still a child and when Yankelevich was still Yampolsky’s
assistant. The pedagogical talents of Yankelevich became widely recognized
as more and more outstanding students emerged from his studio. In the
coming years, forty of his students were awarded prizes at international
competitions, including Irina Bochkova, Victor Tretyakov, Vladimir
Spivakov, Gregory Zhislin, and many more (a list of Yankelevich’s students
is found in Appendix A).
In addition to teaching soloists, Yankelevich also nurtured generations of
chamber musicians, orchestral musicians, and teachers. Throughout his
career, Yankelevich taught close to two hundred students through his
pedagogical activities at the Moscow Conservatory, the Gnessin Institute, the
Moscow uchilische (specialized high school), and the Central Music School.
He also conducted methodological lectures and seminars in Moscow,
throughout the former Soviet Union, and in a limited number of visits abroad
(the latter included masterclasses and lectures in Japan, Germany, and
Czechoslovakia).11
The scope of Yankelevich’s pedagogical and methodological activities
reflected his desire to constantly share his knowledge. He never belonged to
that category of teachers who try to “guard” their professional secrets. He
worked very closely with his assistants, consulting with them and granting
them considerable autonomy, while still maintaining a cohesive and
programmed course of study for each individual student. Many of
Yankelevich’s assistants, including Maya Glezarova, Zinaida Gilels, Evgenia
Chugaeva, and Felix Andrievsky, worked closely with him for decades and
became recognized teachers in their own right. The pedagogical model of
utilizing assistants not only made it easier to develop a unified and
comprehensive methodology but also allowed a deep and thorough
exploration of each methodological element.
Yankelevich was extremely methodical in his own work, and his archives
contain more than thirty notebooks filled with his observations. He treated
pedagogy as serious and disciplined work. “I work like a slave all my life,”
he would say. “I leave the conservatory at 11 pm or later, and then I continue
working at home. It is important to not just listen to the student, but really to
work with them. A teacher who just hears the student play and simply
corrects the notes with ‘play like this here, and like this there. Now play this
again’—that is not work.”12 Indeed, at times there were some who criticized
him for being overly analytical and pedantic. Studying with Yankelevich (and
with some of his assistants) unquestionably required a certain discipline and
perseverance. And yet, as his own texts reflect, his rigor and work ethic arose
solely from his desire to serve the music and was free of ego. His humility
allowed his mind to be open, and he was constantly questioning and
investigating new ideas. Above all, he believed it is the teacher’s
responsibility to uncover and bring out the individual qualities in every single
student.
Yankelevich’s students were distinguished not only by their impeccable
quality of technique but also by their sophisticated and comprehensive
musical taste and understanding. Contrary to some assumptions about the
Russian Violin School, Yankelevich, just like Abraham Yampolsky before
him, saw technique solely as a tool for musical expression. He believed that
absolutely all elements of playing the violin must be directed toward a final
musical goal. “The performer should be in possession of all technical skills,
but at the same time he or she must first develop an understanding of music
and delve into the composer’s intent. Basically, this means that one not only
needs to possess the tools to speak, but most importantly one needs to know
what to say.”13
Yankelevich always took into consideration the psychological and
physiological aspects of playing the violin. This was reflected not only in his
approach to solving technical problems (e.g., conditioning appropriate motor
and nerve reflexes) and choosing repertoire (e.g., analyzing and taking into
account the student’s character) but also in his desire to cultivate the student’s
independence and individual personality. He was interested in teaching not
only how to play the violin but also how to think about playing the violin.
The following words of Abraham Yampolsky particularly resonated with
Yuri Yankelevich:
In practice, we [teachers] are obliged to spend a lot of time working on formal
perfectionism, working on intonation, technical aspects, etc… . In the process of
preparing a student we often overlook the isolated, exceptional moments in the
student’s performance. We don’t notice these sparks, since our attention is turned
to correcting all kinds of deficiencies. At the same time, we become accustomed
to hearing the same piece played dozens of times in the traditional way and
subconsciously cultivate a certain aural inertia. Any divergences from the general
norm give us the impression of something strange and illogical. If this occurs in
the student’s performance we immediately try to correct him or her, instead of
carefully listening and trying to discern that which is valuable and creative, and
may be embedded in the artistic intentions of the student.14
The two texts by Yuri Yankelevich translated in this book illustrate both his
extremely detailed analytical and methodological side as well as his flexible
and broad understanding of music as an active, living process. Details of his
methods and his psycho-physiological approach are thoroughly discussed in
chapter 3 by Maya Glezarova and chapter 4 by Vladimir Grigoryev. All these
texts not only offer invaluable practical advice on overcoming specific
difficulties but also illustrate the fascinating thought process of a great
pedagogical mind. Certain themes are consistently present. These include the
importance of understanding the violin as a singing and “vocal” instrument,
the interconnectivity between the parts and the whole (this applies to both
technical elements such as positioning and musical elements such as form
and style), the importance of creating an individually catered plan of study
for each student, a scientific analysis of every element of technique in order
that it may serve in a clearly envisioned musical context, and viewing each
problem not as an isolated matter but as part of an inseparable chain
connecting every stage and component of violin playing.
Yankelevich’s works stand alongside texts by Carl Flesch, Ivan Galamian,
and Leopold Mozart as some of the most insightful treatises in the history of
the instrument. In “Setting Up the Violin and Bow Hold,” instead of simply
prescribing rules on how the instrument should be positioned, Yankelevich
analyzes the different possibilities and places them in historical context,
discussing the pros and cons of various points of view. He then offers
practical suggestions that take into account the individual constitution of each
student and provide the optimal conditions to accomplish all the tasks the
music requires with the minimal amount of tension.
Similarly, in “Shifting Positions in Conjunction with the Musical Goals of
the Violinist,” Yankelevich analyzes the technique of shifting in historical
context and addresses the various suggestions and arguments encountered in
the methodological literature. In this extremely detailed and thorough work,
Yankelevich addresses every element of shifting, always in light of how the
technique may best serve the performer’s musical goals. Following a general
presentation of how we understand the positions, Yankelevich goes on to
analyze the role of the ear and reflexes, the elimination of tension, and the
optimal movements in all parts of the hand while shifting. He then classifies
different types of shifts and makes a detailed analysis of each type of shift.
For the basis of his analysis, Yankelevich conducted oscillographic readings
in the acoustic laboratory of the Moscow Conservatory of different shifts
performed by the leading violinists of the day. He also addresses shifts that
incorporate harmonics and open strings, shifts in double stops, and the
coordination between shifting and bowing. In conclusion he presents a
summary of general principles that may be used in performance and
pedagogical practice. This work originated as Yankelevich’s doctoral
dissertation in 1955. Following his defense, the head of his dissertation
committee, David Oistrakh, wrote the following: “This work will help many
of our teachers (especially those further away on the ‘periphery’) since there
are still many conflicting opinions in the field that sometimes even block the
development of violin methodology and pedagogy.”18
Maya Glezarova worked closely as Yankelevich’s assistant for over twenty
years. In chapter 3 she describes key elements of Yankelevich’s methodology
as well as providing examples of the long-range individual repertoire plans he
would create for his students. Vladimir Grigoryev, drawing on his
observations of Yankelevich’s seminars and lessons, provides a
comprehensive examination of Yankelevich’s pedagogy, addressing tone
production, exercises, intonation, vibrato, fingerings and bowings, repertoire,
and his general approach to methodology. (Additional resources and
commentaries from Yankelevich’s students may be found at the companion
website to this book.)
Forty years after Yankelevich’s passing, his work and his legacy remain
relevant largely because he was never dogmatic in his principles. He
encouraged an approach to learning that was as flexible as it was
comprehensive. Yankelevich’s legacy lives on through the playing and
teaching of his students and his “grand-students” all over the world. With the
present publication of his works in English, it is hoped that his legacy will
continue through to many more generations of violinists to come.
NOTES
1. When Leopold Auer was appointed as Henryk Wieniawski’s successor in St.
Petersburg, he continued the long tradition of prominent foreign musicians
working in Russia. One of the first violinist composers to settle in Russia was
Luigi Madonis (1690-1779), who was born in Venice but lived and worked in
St. Petersburg from 1733. Later, famous European violinists such as Pierre
Rode, Louis Spohr, Charles de Bériot, Henri Vieuxtemps, Heinrich Ernst,
Henry Schradieck, and Jan Hřimaly all spent some time working in Russia.
However, outside the Imperial Court and before the openings of the Moscow
and St. Petersburg Conservatories, violin playing was also widespread in circles
of aristocratic intelligentsia, artisans, and serfs in the peasant populations. The
most comprehensive text that addresses violin playing in Russia from its
origins through the 1860s is Israel Markovich Yampolsky’s Russkoe
Skripichnoye Iskusstvo (Russian Art of the Violin) published in 1951.
Yampolsky describes the often-competing worlds of peasant string traditions
and foreign guest violinists at the Imperial Court. This divide was bridged by
Russia’s first great virtuoso, Ivan Khandoshkin (1784–1804), who was born
into a family of serfs but whose musical talent allowed him to engage in a
career at the Imperial Court. Another influential Russian violinist was Aleksey
Lvov (1798–1870), who composed a number of violin pieces as well as a
pedagogical treatise with a set of twenty-four caprices.
2. The family name Yampolsky appears often among twentieth-century Russian
string players, and it is important not to confuse Israel and Abraham
Yampolsky. Abraham Yampolsky’s (the violin teacher referenced here) brother
was Mark Yampolsky (1879–1951), a cellist, fellow member of the Persimfans
Orchestra, and professor at the Moscow Conservatory. Mark’s son was Israel
Yampolsky (1905–1976), who studied the violin with his uncle, Abraham
Yampolsky. Israel Yampolsky later became a prominent musicologist and
wrote Russkoe Skripichnoe Iskusstvo (Russian Art of the Violin) and Principles
of Violin Fingering, among other publications. Of no relation to this family is
Philip Yampolsky (1874–1957), a student of and former assistant to Leopold
Auer. Interestingly enough, for a short while Philip Yampolsky taught
Abraham Yampolsky (in St. Petersburg) as well as Leonid Kogan (in
Dnepropetrovsk). Leonid Kogan later continued his studies in Moscow as a
student of Abraham Yampolsky. Unless otherwise noted, the single name
“Yampolsky” will hereafter refer only to Abraham Yampolsky.
3. Boris Schwarz, Great Masters of the Violin (New York: Simon & Schuster,
1983), 421.
4. Ibid.
5. “Persimfans” stood for Pervyi Simfonicheskii Ansambl’ (lit., “The First
Symphonic Orchestra Ensemble”).
6. These English titles are translated by the editor; the original Russian titles are
found in the bibliography.
7. Anisim Berlin (1896–1961) was also the grandfather of acclaimed cellist
Nathalia Gutman.
8. Yankelevich himself was in possession of a fine baritone voice. Not only would
singers sometimes attend his classes at the conservatory but also he was
sometimes invited to serve on the jury of competitions for singers.
9. Tatiana Gaidamovich, “Zhizn’ pedagoga v tvorchestve ego uchenikov” (The
Pedagogue’s Life through the Creative Work of His Students), in Yuri
Yankelevich, Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy), 4th ed.,
(Moscow: Muzyka, 2009), 326.
10. The Central Music School provided a ten-year course of study to students aged
seven through eighteen, combining intensive musical training with regular
schoolwork. The uchilische or music high school was a parallel five-year
music-intensive program for students aged fifteen through nineteen.
11. Impressions of his trips to Japan and the GDR are found in his essay “Na
muzykal’nyh seminarakh v IAponii i Gdr” (At Musical Seminars in Japan and
the German Democratic Republic), Masterstvo muzykanta-ispolnitelia (1972).
12. Gaidamovich, “Zhizn’ pedagoga v tvorchestve ego uchenikov,” 331.
13. Elena Yankelevich, “Professiia—pedagog” (Profession—Pedagogue), in
Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy), 4th ed., Yuri Yankelevich
(Moscow: Muzyka, 2009), 412.
14. Abraham Yampolsky, “O metode raboty s uchinikame” (On Methods of
Working with Students), in Voprosy skripichnogo ispolnitel’stva i pedagogiki,
sbornik statei (Matters of Violin Performance and Pedagogy, a Collection of
Articles), ed. S. Sapozhnikov (Moscow: Muzyka, 1968), 18.
15. Excerpt of comments recorded on tape during a lesson, found in Elena
Yankelevich, “Professiia—Pedagog” (Profession—Pedagogue), 411.
16. Tatiana Gaidamovich, ed., Muzykalnoe ispolnitelstvo i pedagogika (Musical
Performance and Pedagogy) (Moscow: Muzyka, 1991), 89.
17. Mark Lubotsky, “A. I. Iampolskii—Muzykant, Pedagog, Vospitatel” (A. I.
Yampolsky—Musician, Pedagogue, Mentor), Sovetskaya Muzyka 24, no. 11
(1960): 117–23.
18. Gaidamovich, “Zhizn’ pedagoga v tvorchestve ego uchenikov,” 330.
1
Setting up the Violin and Bow Hold
YURI YANKELEVICH
NOTES
1. The term “positioning” in this context is a translation of the Russian word
postanovka. Postanovka derives from the Russian verb postavit’ (to place, put,
arrange) and refers to the setup of the arms, hands, and body when establishing
the violin and bow hold. This term is translated throughout the text
interchangeably as “positioning” or “setup.”—Translator’s note.
2. The term “shoulder” is used in the anatomical sense, meaning the shoulder
bone (specifically the section of the arm from the shoulder to the elbow joint).
This term often leads to confusion, since even in specialized methodological
works the “shoulder” may refer to both the shoulder bone and the shoulder in
the everyday sense of the of the word. The shoulder bone referred to in this
context is the humerus bone.—Translator’s addition in italics.
3. Ion Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry (tekhnika levoi
ruki). Perevod s Nem. V. N. Rimskogo-Korsakova (The Formation of a Natural
System of Violin Playing (Left-Hand Technique), trans. V. N. Rimsky-
Korsakov (from the German) (Moscow: n.p., 1930); B. Mikhailovsky, Novii
put’ skripacha (New Directions for the Violinist) (Moscow: n.p., 1934). The
first name of B. Mikhailovsky is unknown as is further information about Ion
Voiku, who appears not related to the recognized Romanian violinist of the
same name Ion Voicu (1923–1997)—Translator’s note.
4. Joseph Joachim and Andreas Moser, Violinschule (Berlin: N. Simrock, 1905).
5. The term “music school” implies a beginning or preparatory school, as opposed
to a college or university.—Translator’s note.
6. Leopold Mozart, Osnovatel’noe skripichnoe uchilishche/perevod s nem
(Fundamental Violin Treatise) (translated from German) (Saint Petersburg,
n.p., 1804).
7. Bartolomeo Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit
(Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel, 1797).
8. Leopold Auer, Graded Course of Violin Playing: A Complete Outline of Violin
Study for Individual and Class Instruction (New York: Carl Fischer, 1926).
9. Joachim and Moser, Violinschule.
10. Gustave Koeckert, Rationelle Violintechnik (Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel,
1909).
11. Viktor Valter, Kak uchit’ igre na skripke (How to Teach Violin Playing), 3rd
ed. (St. Petersburg: n.p., 1910).
12. Boris Struve, Tipovye formy postanovki ruk u instrumentalistov: Smichkovaia
gruppa (Typical Forms of Positioning the Hands of Instrumentalists: Bowed
Instrument Group) (Moscow: n.p., 1932).
13. Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry (tekhnika levoi ruki).
Perevod s Nem. V. N. Rimskogo-Korsakova.
14. The term “pad” here is taken to also include the abundant variety of cushions
and shoulder rests that are placed between the violin and shoulder.
Yankelevich’s students used a metal shoulder rest covered in nylon that was
slanted toward the shoulder.—Translator’s note.
15. L. Nemirovsky, Mekhanicheskie i psikhologicheskie momenty v osnovnykh
priemakh skripichnoi tekhniki (Mechanical and Psychological Moments in
Violin Technique) (Moscow: n.p., 1915).
16. Leopold Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke (Violin Playing as I Teach It)
(Moscow: Muzyka, 1965); and Interpretatsiia proizvedenii skripichnoi klassiki
(Violin Masterworks and Their Interpretation) (Moscow: n.p., 1964).
17. The author is referring to the top section of the frog where it connects to the
bow stick.—Translator’s note.
18. The author is referring to the U-shaped hollow at the top of the frog.—
Translator’s note.
19. Struve, Tipovye formy postanovki ruk u instrumentalistov.
20. As an example one can refer to the famous violinist Joseph Szigeti, who had
such long arms that when playing at the tip, his forearm was perpendicular to
his shoulder (an angle most violinists encounter at the middle of the bow). This
explains why Szigeti never needed to lift his little finger; it never caused him
any difficulties.
21. Carl Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry (The Art of Violin Playing) (Moscow,
n.p., 1964).
2
Shifting Positions in Conjunction with the Musical
Goals of the Violinist
YURI YANKELEVICH
Example 2.1
Example 2.2
Example 2.3
These examples show that the systems we examined are not able to clearly
differentiate one position from another. On the one hand, various placements
of the hand may correspond to one position as in Example 2.2. On the other
hand, an identical placement of the hand may belong to different positions if
the notes are enharmonically substituted. This is obvious in Example 2.3
taken from Flesch’s treatise, in which Flesch poses the question—is the hand
in second or third position?9
Thus it becomes understandable why many pedagogues search for new
ways to divide the fingerboard. One such attempt is an interesting system
suggested by Israel Yampolsky.10 This system is based not on the diatonic
scale (which determines the positions according to whole and half-steps) but
on the chromatic scale. Yampolsky arrives at the completely justified
conclusion that, since “the chromatic alternation of a note by a half-step with
the same finger (such as the first or fourth which form the borders of the
position) inevitably creates a change in the hand’s placement, it is essential to
reexamine and define the current systems of dividing the fingerboard.”
Yampolsky writes:
Example 2.4
Example 2.5 Brahms: Violin Concerto, mvt. 1.
When the note lying in another position is reached with the help of an
extension, then the wrist and forearm may lean slightly in the direction of the
extended finger as in Example 2.16.
However, in this case the first finger stays in place. It is necessary to
emphasize that there is a big difference between this leaning movement of the
hand and that which takes place when shifting positions, since in the latter
case the entire arm moves, including the forearm, wrist, and fingers.
As illustrated earlier, it is possible to place the fingers in different positions
while retaining the same placement of the hand. In cases of enharmonic
substitution (where theoretically the designation of the position should
change), the fingers and the hand remain practically in the same place.
At the same time the opposite phenomenon may be observed: moving the
fingers a half-step could either be accompanied by an entire displacement of
the hand (forearm, wrist, and fingers) or may only involve sliding a finger in
the framework of the same position (as in a chromatic scale). Taking all this
into account, one may readily agree with Jean Delphin Alard’s idea that a
position be defined as that placement of the hand that, without changing,
permits all the required notes in a phrase or passage to be executed.15
David Oistrakh presents a particularly interesting point of view. He
suggests envisioning the positions as zones, with a zone encompassing all the
possible placements of the hand in one position—low, normal, and high. This
characterization unites both types of fingering, since the outer limits of the
zone would be defined by a fifth-wide position. Oistrakh did not associate the
fifth-wide position only with arpeggios; in practice he often used this position
in diatonic sequences by sliding one of the fingers a half-step.
Consequently, in practice it is the movement of the entire arm in carrying
out specific musical tasks that is important and not the numerical delineation
of positions. Konstantin Mostras expressed this aptly when he remarked that,
for a violinist who has mastered the instrument, it does not matter to which
positions the notes belong since playing is directly connected to the sonic
realization of musical thought. Many competent violinists experience
difficulties if asked to shift, for example, from the second finger in first
position on the E string to the fourth finger in ninth position. Yet, at the same
time this shift is easily accomplished if the positions are not named and only
the pitches are given.
We may conclude that all systems of dividing the fingerboard to this day
are entirely relative, and any search for new systems is counterproductive.
This is especially so considering that actual musical performance is never tied
to the necessity of classifying the positions, although the latter may serve a
purpose in the beginning stages of study.
Table 2.1. Separate components of movement in the left hand in different parts of the
fingerboard.
Lower part of the Upper part of the
fingerboard fingerboard
Leading element Forearm Wrist (hand)
Guided element Wrist (hand) and fingers Fingers
Supportive Shoulder (vertical movement) Shoulder (horizontal
element movement)
However, the forearm plays the leading role only in the first four positions.
When shifting in the upper part of the fingerboard, the movements of the
forearm are absent since the body of the violin stops the forearm from
moving closer to the torso. In this case the wrist must be very active, and its
flexibility accommodates the required changes of position.
It is simple to determine whether only the wrist is involved when shifting
in the upper part of the fingerboard by using the same experiment mentioned
earlier. Hold the violin in playing position with the elbow fixed on the table,
as explained in the first experiment, and place the first finger on the note E3
on the E string (seventh position) and shift to the note B3 on the same string
(ninth position). In this case we notice that the shift is accomplished easily.
However, if we then try to put the fourth finger down in the new position on
the note E4, we find it impossible. For the fourth finger to reach the
fingerboard it becomes necessary to lift the elbow from the table and free the
shoulder. The shoulder then makes the supportive motion inward (in the
direction of the right arm), allowing all the fingers to find their necessary
position on the string. The extent of this shoulder movement increases as the
distance of the shift widens. When moving from high positions to lower ones
in the same area (approximately fifth position), the shoulder makes an
opposite movement to the left.
Consequently, both the shoulder and the wrist are involved when changing
positions in the upper part of the fingerboard. As already noted, the forearm
almost doesn’t move in the upper part of the fingerboard, since it is restricted
by the body of the violin. The forearm moves only slightly in tandem with the
shoulder motion. The main movements in this case are those of the wrist.
Similar to shifts in the lower part of the fingerboard, the fingers simply
follow. The shoulder movement, ensuring normal finger action in the attained
position, is purely supportive. Thus, the shoulder is involved in both the
lower and upper parts of the fingerboard. In the first case it lifts and lowers,
and in the second case it moves to the right or the left.
The following chart illustrates the combination of the separate components
of movement in the left hand in different parts of the fingerboard (see Table
2.1).
We have already noted more than once that relaxed, efficient movements and
complete command of the motor apparatus are essential conditions for
playing the violin. In analyzing the conditions that determine the movements,
it is important to examine the violinist’s positioning or setup, since it
significantly influences the origin of these movements and therefore playing
in general. Great teachers always recognized the importance of a good setup.
Even Leopold Mozart wrote of violinists “whose playing leaves a poor
impression, because they limit themselves by their awkward hold of the
violin and bow.”47 At the same time pedagogical practice clearly shows that
determining the best setup is a difficult matter, leading to many
disagreements and errors.
The principal error is that the violinist’s positioning is sometimes treated as
something fixed and self-contained. The aforementioned works by Voiku and
Mikhailovsky provide examples of this. These authors examine positioning in
an abstract way, disconnected from the demands of playing, and thereby turn
it into a goal unto itself. Yet an efficient setup may be examined only in
direct correlation to those movements for which it was created. The only
factor determining the setup should be how it accommodates the necessary
movements to produce the desired sound.
At the same time, one must consider that in the process of finding the
optimal techniques, certain movements may cause the positioning to change
and readjust. This is why positioning should never be considered as static; it
is dynamic, just like all playing movements, and reflects the diversity of
musical problems.
Another serious mistake is to dogmatize specific forms of positioning. An
efficient setup should always take into account the individual characteristics
of the performer, who in turn must also adapt to ensure the most efficient
motor functions in any given circumstances. When we stress the importance
of the player’s individual characteristics in determining the positioning, it is
important to emphasize that these characteristics are not important in their
own right but only within the context of the musical goal, which they are
entirely subservient to.
Turning directly to the setup of the left hand, we must note that the manner
in which the instrument is held plays an essential role in ensuring free
movement. The free movement of the left hand is naturally limited if it also
needs to support the violin. This explains the many tendencies in violin
pedagogy to support the violin only with the chin and collarbone, allowing
the left arm to be entirely free. This technique is considerably widespread,
and found many supporters in the Czech and German schools and was also
recommended by the French school.48 For the German school, this technique
was a logical consequence of excessive finger pressure on the strings; since
the left hand was already restricted by excess pressure, it was required
(whenever possible) to be free from additionally supporting the instrument.
Another point of view insists that the violin should always be held with
two points of support—between the chin and collarbone and between the
thumb and index finger of the left hand. Nemirovsky was an ardent defender
of this approach.49 He wrote that the widespread practice of supporting the
violin with the help of the shoulder or a pad (which he rejected) was based
upon a “gross misunderstanding of the essence of the instrument” that it was
“a deformity, disrupting the natural structure and habits of the body.”50
At the same time, Nemirovsky correctly understood that without a
“constant point of contact” (i.e., between the chin and collarbone) it is
impossible to support the violin while shifting downward. For these cases he
recommended auxiliary movement in the thumb preceding the shift, thereby
creating an extra point of support.
Boris Struve had his own particular opinion on how the instrument should
be positioned.51 He conceded the possibility of holding the instrument both
ways, that is, with one or two points of support. He believed that the choice
should be based not on methodology but solely on the anatomical
particularities and constitution of the player. For those violinists with sloped
shoulders the rational positioning, according to Struve, is to hold the violin
with two points of support. For if the violin were to be supported only in the
chin area, the shoulder would lift significantly, and this in turn would cause
tension in the left hand. However, for those with naturally high shoulders,
Struve recommended holding the violin with one point of support.
Meanwhile, just like Nemirovsky, Struve believed special coordinated
movements in the thumb are necessary when shifting downward when the
violin is held with two points of support. However, in those cases when this
coordination is not enough, Struve did recommend using a pad.52 On the one
hand, the pad relieves the need to raise the left shoulder; and on the other, it
allows the violin to be held with one point of support, thereby avoiding the
extra movements in the thumb.
Without denying the importance of the constitutional particularities of the
body, this should not be the sole determining factor. In reality, the two
indicated ways of supporting the violin are closely intertwined. Every player
will concede that the violin is held more firmly under the chin when shifting
compared to when one is playing in a fixed position. In the latter case, some
violinists even lift their head from the violin, proving the existence of two
points of support at that instant. Those who heard Fritz Kreisler will recall
that he often lifted his head away from the violin.
These examples show that it is wrong to insist on one static positioning.
Similar to any other technique, the setup needs to be dynamic so it may
correspond to the variety of musical tasks. This is particularly evident when
shifting. The degree to which the violin is held with two points of support
changes, depending on how much pressure is exerted by the chin. This, in
turn, depends on the shift’s direction (up or down), on the freedom and
elasticity of the left-hand movement, and also on the degree of finger
pressure on the strings and the opposite pressure of the thumb.
Alexei Lvov was a proponent of securing the instrument primarily with the
chin and shoulder.53 Lvov recommended holding the violin “almost at a right
angle to the body,” and securing it with the chin firmly enough so that the left
hand could freely change position, barely holding the violin at all.
In practice, holding the violin tightly with the chin is often associated with
lifting the left shoulder.54 For example, this is the setup advocated by Valter
and Nemirovsky.55 The latter, as already noted, strongly advocated holding
the violin with the chin but without using a pad.
Raising the left shoulder is neither efficient in providing constant support
of the violin nor in temporary moments when the instrument needs to be
firmly fixed with the chin. Additionally, raising the left shoulder causes
tension in the muscles, which impacts free movement throughout the left arm
and hand. Struve also shared this opinion, although he believed that the
negative impact did not for some reason apply to those with high shoulders.
Yet, practice shows that tension occurs in both cases, although it may
manifest itself differently.
Mostras aptly notes that lifting the left shoulder changes the direction of
the movement of the forearm. Thus, if normally the direction of the forearm’s
movement corresponds to the direction of the fingerboard, when the shoulder
is raised then this movement strays from the fingerboard and requires special
adjustment.
Since many violinists recognize the problems caused by lifting the left
shoulder, various techniques of holding the instrument attempt to address
this. The more common method in contemporary practice is to use a cushion
or pad. We find this to be indisputably rational, since this entirely eliminates
the need to lift the shoulder and allows the left arm to move freely. Regarding
the cushion’s negative effect on the sound of the instrument (incidentally,
pressing the shoulder against the instrument without a pad also affects the
sound), this may be avoided depending on the cushion’s design.56 At the
same time it should be emphasized that using a pad does not mean the violin
needs to be “rigidly fixed” in one position, as Nemirovsky implies. The pad is
simply used to help free the left hand when it is necessary for the violin to be
supported with the chin without lifting the shoulder (such as in descending
passages, chromatic glissandi, big leaps, etc.).
Example 2.21
Consequently, the idea of transferring the thumb to the violin bout should
not be rejected in principle, since the difficulties that ensue are readily
overcome. Indeed, rejecting this technique would make playing in high
positions impossible for many violinists. It should be noted that sometimes
violinists with very small hands are unable to play in high positions even with
the thumb on the violin bout, and they are forced to transfer the thumb to the
side of the fingerboard.
In cases of an immediate shift from a very high to a very low position, the
thumb is released and returns to its normal position without touching the
violin neck. The technique is useful in situations similar to Example 2.23.
Sometimes we may observe the thumb released in a similar manner in
other parts of the fingerboard. However, since this is clearly the result of an
individual adaptation, it should not be recommended as a general rule.
As we have seen, the functions of the thumb are quite diverse and often
complex, and they require a lot of flexibility and agility. There is no doubt
that they closely depend on the general condition of the entire left hand and,
conversely, the latter in its turn is affected by the position and functions of
the thumb.
Maximum freedom of movement in the left hand is ensured not only by
correctly positioning the thumb but also by the positioning and character of
movement in the other fingers. The angle at which the fingers touch the
fingerboard is of primary importance. Naturally, to a certain extent this
depends on the individual particularities of the player. However, in all cases it
is more efficient to place the fingers at a slight angle in relation to the string.
The angle should only be slight, but the fingers should never be perpendicular
to the string. This ensures both free movement in the left hand and better tone
quality.
The slight angle helps the quality of sound by providing more contact
between the cushion of the fingertip and the string, and it also creates more
favorable conditions for vibrato. Furthermore, the angle facilitates the process
of sliding and extending the fingers and transferring the fingers to different
strings. Finally, an inclined position of the fingers provides one consolidated
positioning for playing both technical passages and cantilena (i.e., playing
with and without vibrato). We will also note that an inclined position of the
fingers is justified by corresponding more closely to their natural position.
The so-called steep position of the fingers is therefore not rational.
It is most efficient to place the fingers so that they touch the string at the
middle of the fingertip. In practice, we may sometimes find the fingers
positioned on the side of the fingertip. This leads to a series of undesirable
consequences, including poor tone quality and problems with vibrato; it may
also cause the fingers to slide off the string when shifting.
Another equally important factor in assuring free movement in the left
hand is the angle at which the fingers fall to the fingerboard, in respect to the
curvature of the latter. Mikhailovsky correctly analyzed this matter in his
method, noting that in all parts of the fingerboard the fingers should press the
string perpendicular to the tangent of the fingerboard.71 Effectively, in this
position the entire cushion of the fingertip, and not just one side, presses the
string. This helps the sound and also allows the finger to be more stable on
the string during shifts. If the fingers do not fall at an angle perpendicular to
the string, another common problem is that the fingers may pull the string
sidewise, in the direction of the E string.
The correct angle for the fingers to drop to the string depends on the
corresponding turn of the hand, which is connected to the corresponding
position of the elbow underneath the violin. The angle of the fingers on
different strings changes depending on the curvature of the fingerboard, and
consequently the corresponding positioning of the elbow also changes. The
elbow moves slightly to the left when playing on the higher strings, and
draws inward to the right when playing on the lower strings. The position of
the elbow also depends on the way the instrument is held; if the violin is held
flat, then the elbow moves further under the instrument to the right, and vice
versa. In the higher positions the elbow is required to move further under the
violin than in the lower positions. Consequently, the maximum degree that
the elbow draws inward to the right occurs when playing on the G string in
the higher positions, and the elbow moves furthest to the left when playing on
the E string in the lower positions.
Thus, it becomes clear that it is impossible to speak of a single fixed
position of the elbow. Moving the elbow is a necessary component of moving
the left hand. Voiku called these elbow movements, which allow a
comfortable positioning of the fingers on each string, “steering movements”;
this term became widespread and is still used in pedagogical practice today.72
A correct understanding of this “steering movement” is also found in other
methodological works, including Nemirovsky’s treatise (that appeared in
1915, ten years before Voiku’s) and publications by Lesman and others.73
We have now analyzed the components of left-hand activity that are
necessary to ensure complete freedom of movement along the fingerboard.
This analysis has attempted both to determine the particular positioning that
allows free movement in the left hand and to uncover those “inhibitory”
elements that could restrict it. On this basis we proceed in the following
section to analyze the actual character of left-hand movement along the
fingerboard, which is essential to mastering the technical and artistic sides of
violin playing.
Example 2.24
Example 2.25
Example 2.26a
Example 2.26b
Example 2.27a
Example 2.27b
Example 2.28a
Example 2.28b
On all bowed instruments, the horizontal movement of the left hand along the
fingerboard is almost always made by sliding a finger along the string
(exceptions are certain types of shifts examined in section 9). This slide
provides an uninterrupted connection to the instrument, helping the player
perceive distances on the fingerboard.
In the pedagogical literature we encounter the opinion proposed by Flesch
—that all shifts be divided into two categories, with some considered
“technical” and others considered “expressive” (Flesch’s terminology).83 And
these shifts should be played differently, with the term glissando referring
only to the “technical” shifts, and the term portamento referring to the
“expressive” shifts.
However, glissando is determined by musical context. While in some cases
it should be inaudible, in others it serves as a tool for special expression. For
this reason we consider it incorrect to principally divide shifts into being
either “expressive” or “purely technical.” While the character of sound in
what seems to be a purely “technical” passage may strive to be inaudible, it
should only be determined by how the passage needs to sound within the
musical context of the entire piece.
It is also necessary to note the similar methods of studying portamento and
glissando; both are based on mastering smooth and elastic movements while
shifting. By adhering to the principle that “shifts must be inaudible,” many
pedagogues prohibit students from smoothly connecting notes in the early
stages of study. This has the opposite effect, since students develop the habit
of moving suddenly and abruptly and end up making the shifts even more
obvious.
In analyzing portamento as an expressive device and attempting to
determine its principles, some violinists have attempted to systemize it.
Alard, for example, believed that shifts should be light and quick when
playing allegro and slower when playing adagio, thereby associating the
character of the shift with the length of the note.84 However, when playing
cantilena there are many examples of using both fast and slow shifts in the
same tempo, depending on the musical context.
Bekker makes a similar attempt by classifying portamento into three
categories depending on how audible it is: nonexistent, slight, and strong.85
He also distinguishes the styles of portamento (“lyrical” or “heroic”),
depending on the fingering that is used. This is discussed in more detail
below. It is perfectly clear that classifying portamento in this way, taking into
consideration only how audible it is and ignoring the quality and character of
its sound, is purely mechanical. It similarly does not make sense to connect
the sonic character of the portamento to the fingering used in the shift. There
are many examples where a shift that Becker labels as “lyrical” can sound
energetic or passionate, such as in Lev Tseitlin’s interpretation of a Bach aria.
Or a “heroic” shift might sound lyrical when played gently piano.
The problem in trying to establish such rules is that it is impossible to
systemize the diversity of artistic processes in general and portamento in
particular. The diversity is explained by the multitude of artistic problems;
the varying musical content; the style of the piece; and the performer’s
understanding of the character and nuances of sound, phrasing, the
correlation of various elements in the musical fabric, and so on.
Naturally, only a performer technically in command of shifting can
produce the subtlety that different portamenti require. And although the ear is
the determining factor in evaluating the quality of the connecting notes,
auditory perception on its own cannot always tell what purely technical
process of connecting the notes will work best in each case.
To determine general guidelines for shifting, we conducted a study of the
connections between notes in both the context of a musical composition and
randomly sampled, in addition to a comparison of both categories. The study
was made with the help of an oscillograph, and the results are presented in
the following section.86
In presenting this material, we divided shifts according to how they are
executed. This subdivision is based on the classification presented in
Davidov’s cello treatise in which the principal types of shifts are categorized
as follows:87 (1) shifts with the same finger; (2) shifts where an open string
separates the positions; (3) shifts where the first note in the new position is
played with a higher finger than the last note of the former position; and (4)
shifts where the first note in the new position is played with a lower finger
than the last note of the old position.88 Davidov not only categorizes the
shifts but also provides examples of each type of shift, including those
between notes lying on different strings and shifts that incorporate harmonics.
Consequently, we propose the following classification of shifts on the
violin:
Besides these types of shifts, there are also shifts made without a connecting
slide (e.g., shifts via an open string, shifts accomplished by extending or
contracting the fingers, and shifts that incorporate harmonics). Furthermore,
there is also a special kind of shifting made with a glissando in scalar (usually
chromatic) passages.
The first four types of shifts were analyzed in detail with the help of an
oscillograph. This study produced a large and interesting amount of material
that clarifies the way shifts are executed. The diagrams in the following
section are based on a transcription of the oscillograph readings.
Each type of shift has its own specific characteristics. In order to determine
these characteristics in the most detailed and objective manner possible, we
made a series of oscillographic recordings of Type 1, 2, 3, and 4 shifts played
by the eminent violinists David Oistrakh, Yakov Rabinovich, and Dmitri
Tsyganov. The results are presented below in the form of graphs for easier
analysis.
In all graphs, the vertical axis represents the pitch (with each division
corresponding to a semitone) and the horizontal axis delineates time (with
each division corresponding to a determined fraction of a second, varying
according to each graph). Each graph also shows the overall time it took to
produce the given shift, which is especially important in determining its
character.
Type 1 Shifts
For these types of shifts, the oscillograph study first analyzed the shift from
E1 to G1 without any musical context. As the graph illustrates, the shift
played by Tsyganov begins slowly and ends with significant acceleration in
the sliding finger (Graph 2.1). An analogous phenomenon is observed in the
return shift from G1 to E1 as the hand moves back down the fingerboard.
Both the shifts played by Oistrakh (Graph 2.2) and Tsyganov (Graph 2.3) are
distinguished by the same characteristic—a slow beginning followed by a
slide that accelerates to a greater or lesser extent.
Graph 2.1 I division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.09 seconds.
Graph 2.2 1 division = 0.01 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.23 seconds.
Graph 2.3 1 division = 0.014 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.124 seconds.
Graph 2.5 1 division = 0.04 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.3 seconds.
Graph 2.6 1 division = 0.04 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.37 seconds.
Graph 2.7 1 division = 0.015 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.135 seconds.
Graph 2.8 1 division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.18 seconds.
The notes were connected in a similar fashion (this time at the interval of a
fifth) in the following shift from Tchaikovsky’s Serenade Melancolique
(Graph 2.7 = Tsyganov, Graph 2.8 = Rabinovich, Graph 2.9 = Oistrakh).
Graph 2.9 1 division = 0.014 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.157 seconds.
Graph 2.10 1 division = 0.023 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.095 seconds.
Graph 2.11 1 division = 0.02 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.29 seconds.
A similar shift, but this time with a slightly different portamento and
smoother movement in the bow arm, makes the sound slightly coquettish and
more grazioso (see Example 2.31).
It is rare to find Type 1 shifts that also involve transferring the finger to
another string, for usually in these cases another fingering is used to ensure a
better sound. When these shifts do occur, Lesman indicated that the moving
finger should transfer to the new string at the same time as the bow.89
However, Tseitlin’s technique may be considered more appropriate. Tseitlin
suggested preemptively preparing the fifth, that is, placing the finger
immediately on two strings. When the change to a new string coincides with
a change in bow, then another method is also possible. If the glissando occurs
on the initial bow stroke, then the finger slides along the string of the first
note; whereas if the glissando occurs on the subsequent bow stroke, then the
finger slides along the string of the second note. If the shift requires the finger
to transfer to a new string in conjunction with activity in the other fingers
(e.g., in double stops or chords), then the only possibility is to slide the finger
along the initial string and then make the transfer.
The special lyrical quality of shifts made with one finger is explained by
the close resemblance to vocal portamento (something to also strive for in
other shifts where possible). This also explains why Type 1 shifts are widely
used to give additional expression to the sound. Similar to any other
technique, the manner in which these types of shifts are used largely depends
on the individual style of the player. Thus Tseitlin, whose playing was broad,
powerful, and full, tended to employ a slower portamento, making the shifts
sound rich and deep as in Example 2.32.
Example 2.33 Kreisler: Caprice Viennoise.
Example 2.36a
When Kreisler plays his Caprice Viennoise, he gives this type of shift a
slightly tremulous and sensuous quality that intrinsically reflects his
interpretation (see Example 2.33).
Oistrakh often used this type of shift and would give it a completely
different, lyrical color. It was characteristic for Oistrakh to make a
descending glissando with the first finger (Examples 2.34 and 2.35).
Type 2 Shifts
Before moving to an analysis of Type 2 shifts, we should note that the vast
majority of specialized texts, such as those by Alard, Voldan, Voiku,
Jockisch, Koeckert, Mikhailovsky, Radmall, Flesch, and many others,
confirm the necessity of using so-called auxiliary or intermediary notes in
these types of shifts.90 “Intermediary notes” refer to those notes that
correspond to where the initial finger (i.e., the finger making the slide) would
lie in the new position (see Examples 2.36a–d).
Example 2.36b
Example 2.36c
Example 2.36d
Graph 2.12 1 division = 0.013 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.142 seconds.
The oscillographic analysis of these types of shifts clearly shows the actual
borders of the finger movement and the degree to which they correspond to
the intermediary notes. Graph 2.12 represents the shift E1–B1 played by
Oistrakh. As the curve indicates, the connecting finger only arrives at the
note F , whereas the corresponding intermediary note for this type of shift is
supposed to be G1. Consequently, in this example the sliding finger falls a
half-step short of the intermediary note.
Graph 2.13 1 division = 0.02 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.089 seconds.
Graph 2.14 1 division = 0.013 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.13 seconds.
A slightly larger gap in this respect is observed when Tsyganov plays the
same shift (Graph 2.13).
A similar break occurs when Tsyganov plays another shift of the same
interval C2–G2 (Graph 2.14). As the curve indicates, the connecting finger
only makes it to the note D2 (indeed even a quarter-tone below), even though
the intermediary note should theoretically be E2. In this example the gap is
wider than a whole step.
At the same time we should note that in other shifts of the same distance,
the sliding finger does indeed reach the corresponding intermediate note. This
can be seen in Graphs 2.15 (Oistrakh) and 2.16 (Rabinovich), which
represent the shift F1–B from Tchaikovsky’s Serenade Melancolique.
Graph 2.15 1 division = 0.023 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.0117 seconds.
Graph 2.16 1 division = 0.042 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.42 seconds.
In another example, looking at the shift G 1–D 2 in Rakov’s violin
concerto, we see that Oistrakh (Graph 2.17) does not reach the intermediary
note (missing it by a whole step), while Tsyganov does reach the
intermediary note (Graph 2.18).
Graph 2.17 1 division = 0.016 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.096 seconds.
Graph 2.18 1 division = 0.026 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.133 seconds.
If the disparity is not always observed in small shifts (though most often it
is) it becomes perfectly obvious when the distance of the shifts increases, as
shown in the following examples. Thus, in shifts at the interval of a seventh,
the discrepancy is the equivalent of a whole tone, and sometimes it is even
larger, as seen in Graphs 2.19, 2.20, and 2.21.
Graph 2.19 1 division = 0,013 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.173 seconds.
Graph 2.20 1 division = 0.025 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.3 seconds.
Graph 2.21 1 division = 0.013 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.106 seconds.
Graph 2.22 1 division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.17 seconds.
Example 2.37
Example 2.38
Example 2.39
If the shift is made with the help of an intermediary note, then the finger
must press the string and slide precisely to the intermediary note before the
new finger drops. With this method, even if the intermediary note is inaudible
because of the well-timed placement of the subsequent finger, one still hears
the end of the connecting slide and the consequent break between the notes.
The reason intermediary notes are recommended by many teachers
(particularly Lesman) and supported in the literature is because they seem to
simplify all types of shifts, in essence consolidating them into shifts made
with one finger. We find it extremely helpful to compare Type 1 and Type 2
shifts, but it is necessary to turn the student’s attention not so much to
similarities in the movement of the sliding finger (in this case, the index
finger) as to differences.
When Type 2 shifts occur between different strings (Example 2.37), they
are played the same way (i.e., the finger connecting the notes has the same
character as described earlier, and the only difference is that the new finger
drops onto a different string). The same is true for ascending Type 2 shifts
that arrive at a harmonic (Example 2.38). The difference in Type 2 shifts that
descend from a harmonic (Example 2.39) is that the connecting finger presses
the string the moment it begins to slide (not earlier). Afterward, the regular
character of the slide is retained and the finger pressure gradually releases.
Example 2.40
Type 3 Shifts
However, the oscillograph studies show that this shift never begins on an
“intermediary” note. This is seen in an excerpt from the Glazunov concerto
(performed by Rabinovich, Oistrakh, and Tsyganov) and in an excerpt from
Concerto No. 5 by Vieuxtemps (performed by the same violinists). In all
cases without exception, this slide began to a greater or lesser extent near the
second note, becoming, as Mostras would say, a kind of “ramp” leading into
the note. Thus, for example, in Rabinovich’s version the slide begins only at
a half-tone from the second note, and not at the distance of a major third that
would correspond to the intermediary note (Graph 2.25).
The same is observed when Oistrakh (Graph 2.26) and Tsyganov (Graph
2.27) play this shift.
Graph 2.27 1 division = 0.034 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.25 seconds.
Graph 2.28 1 division = 0.034 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.29 seconds.
Graph 2.29 1 division = 0.02 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.133 seconds.
Flesch did note the possibility of making Type 3 shifts without auxiliary
notes, although he referred to them as “free portamento-fantasies”
representing a “purely individual means of expression.” For example, Flesch
considered a slide that started as close as possible to the second note to be a
personal trait of the violinist Jacques Thibaud. Yet, as the examples above
show, the discrepancy between the intermediary note and the actual start of
the portamento is not just a characteristic of one performer.
In other cases, the finger may even begin sliding before the intermediary
note. For instance, let us examine Example 2.41.
For this shift, Tseitlin recommends bringing the third finger closer to the
first, and letting the third finger make the slide along the entire fingerboard
from start to finish. In this type of shift, the accelerating speed and increase of
pressure in the bow plays an important role. The use of this technique
depends on the musical context, which in Example 2.41 requires a lot of
brilliance and emotional excitement to emphasize the transition to the new
theme.
Comparing the previous examples of the Glazunov and Vieuxtemps
concerti, it is easy to see that all performers make the portamento longer in
the Vieuxtemps than in the Glazunov. This again proves that the fundamental
factor that determines the difference is the different character of the music—
more expressive and sentimental in the Vieuxtemps and more strict and noble
in the Glazunov. On this basis we may conclude that the nature of movement
in Type 3 shifts should not be determined by intermediary notes, but by the
specific sound required by the character of the music.
Example 2.42
Example 2.43
In Type 3 shifts the pressure of the sliding finger also changes. However,
in contrast to Type 2 shifts, the pressure increases in approaching the new
note. When Type 3 shifts occur across strings, the sliding finger makes the
connection on the new string.
Type 4 Shifts
In the specialized literature there are two points of view regarding ascending
Type 4 shifts. Both of these opinions involve intermediary notes, but they
differ in how they are used. According to the first point of view, which dates
back to the classical German treatises of David, Joachim, and others, the shift
is made using a so-called upper intermediary note (Example 2.42). As
Joachim notes in his treatise, in these cases the last finger on the string before
the shift slides to the position in which the second note lies.
The other point of view is based on the so-called lower intermediary note
(Example 2.43). This idea was developed by Flesch and was propagated in
the later methods of Ševčík, Voldan, and others.
Although both of these points of view attempt to assure a smooth
connection between notes, neither opinion is really justified. For the musical
and expressive purpose of this shift is to make the connection between notes
as uninterrupted as possible. Therefore any glissando that occurs outside the
framework of these notes, as occurs with both upper and lower intermediary
notes, is inappropriate.
An oscillographic analysis of Type 4 shifts shows that in practice neither
upper nor lower intermediary notes are used. An analysis was made of two
separate shifts: F1–C2 (Graphs 2.31 and 2.32; performers Tsyganov and
Rabinovich) and D2–A2 (Graph 2.33; Oistrakh). As the graphs illustrate,
neither the upper nor the lower auxiliary notes were used in any of the
examples, for if auxiliary notes were used then the curve would extend
beyond the limits of the connecting notes in both directions.
Graph 2.31 1 division = 0.023 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.25 seconds.
Graph 2.32 1 division = 0.019 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.228 seconds.
Graph 2.33 1 division = 0.025 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.15 seconds.
Graph 2.34 1 division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.12 seconds.
Graph 2.35 1 division = 0.022 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.132 seconds.
Graph 2.36 1 division = 0.022 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.198 seconds.
The same result was observed when the shifts were made within a specific
musical context. The following three oscillographic readings represent a shift
from the Glazunov concerto, played by Rabinovich (Graph 2.34), Oistrakh
(Graph 2.35), and Tsyganov (Graph 2.36). None of these performers used an
intermediary note when making the shift.
The curve representing the slide is either completely uninterrupted or close
to being uninterrupted. As already noted, an uninterrupted line is
characteristic of Type 1 shifts with one finger. Since in Type 4 shifts the
initial note is played with one finger and the consequent note with another, it
becomes obvious that one finger replaces the other during the slide itself. The
closer this substitution coincides with the general movement of the hand, the
closer does the curve resemble an uninterrupted line. Thus, looking at Graph
2.32 one can conclude that in this case the general hand movement was
slightly slower, and the new finger repeated some of the path covered by the
initial finger (albeit only fractionally by a quarter-tone). In Graph 2.35 the
opposite is observed: the movement of the hand is slightly accelerated,
causing the new finger (the first) to be carried further along, instead of
capturing the glissando right where the old finger (the second) ended. This
explains the slight break in the graph. However, we should remember that
these kinds of breaks are so short and insignificant that they are only
noticeable on such a sensitive instrument as the oscillograph.
Graph 2.37 1 division = 0.016 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.096 seconds.
Graph 2.38 1 division = 0.017 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.153 seconds.
One might suppose that in more difficult circumstances, when the shift is
made not with adjacent but with outer fingers (such as the first and fourth),
auxiliary notes are indeed necessary. This matter was also subjected to an
oscillographic analysis (Graphs 2.37–2.43). No intermediary notes were used
in the shifts G1–B1 (Graph 2.37; Rabinovich) and D2–A2 (Graph 2.38;
Oistrakh) made with the third and first fingers. The same result was observed
when Rabinovich played the shift A1–D2 (Graph 2.39) with the outer fingers
(i.e., shifting from the fourth to the first). Moreover, as the graphs indicate,
the connecting slide did not go higher or lower than the indicated notes.
Therefore, it is clear that in practice intermediary notes are not used to play
these types of shifts, no matter what fingers are used.
Graph 2.39 1 division = 0.017 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.229 seconds.
Graph 2.40 1 division = 0.022 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.37 seconds.
Graph 2.41 1 division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.09 seconds.
Graph 2.42 1 division = 0.019 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.28 seconds.
Graph 2.43 1 division = 0.03 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.21 seconds.
Example 2.44
Example 2.45
Although learning Type 4 shifts is more difficult than other types, once
they are assimilated they do not pose any additional problems. It helps to
begin learning these shifts with adjacent fingers. The “mechanism” of
substituting one finger with another can be studied with the help of the
following exercises in Example 2.44. These exercises help clarify the essence
of this technique, which is also important when correcting previously
developed bad habits.
The principles of making Type 4 shifts remain the same even when
adjacent fingers are not used. Generally only the movement of the hand
accelerates, since the new finger must be transferred further because of the
greater distance between the fingers. It should be remembered that, in these
shifts the pressure of the new finger increases while the pressure of the
previous finger releases. In this respect the movement of the initial finger is
similar to the movement it makes sliding to a harmonic (see Example 2.45).
In practice, when Type 4 shifts are played with nonadjacent fingers, the
outer fingers often draw close together before the shift as the inner fingers
slightly lift. This technique is recommended in some specialized methods
including Lesman’s work and Davidov’s treatise for cellists.94 However, we
find this technique should be considered more of an individual adaptation.
Also, as the tempo increases, the degree to which the fingers draw together
decreases. S. Kozolupov and L. Ginsberg make a similar point when
analyzing this technique for cellists.95 In a fast tempo this technique
inevitably restricts movement in the hand and can make the position unstable,
thereby causing problems in intonation.
In descending shifts, the initial finger makes the slide, and the subsequent
finger immediately drops to the necessary spot on the fingerboard. Using an
intermediary note in this case means the connecting glissando must go
beyond the limits of the connected notes, as in Example 2.46.
Example 2.46
Graph 2.44 1 division = 0.025 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.1 seconds.
Graph 2.45 1 division = 0.016 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.16 seconds.
Graph 2.46 1 division = 0.025 seconds. Total duration of the shift = 0.3 seconds.
Example 2.47
Example 2.48 Goedicke: Etude.
Example 2.53a–d
Example 2.54
Example 2.55
So far we have examined Type 4 shifts where both notes lie on one string.
However, Type 4 shifts often occur between different strings. In these cases
four variations are possible: in Examples 2.53a–d, versions a and b illustrate
movement up the fingerboard; and versions c and d illustrate movement
down the fingerboard.
Naturally, these examples may also be played differently (see Examples
2.54 and 2.55).
Yet, although these shifts may be avoided by using an alternate fingering,
an analysis of the literature shows that these shifts are still nonetheless
sometimes indicated.
Thus, Examples 2.56 and 2.57 illustrate shifts that correspond to Example
2.53a.
Example 2.63
All the types of shifts we examined may be further complicated under certain
conditions. One example is when the shifts cover a great distance (i.e., so-
called leaps that create well-known problems in intonation). Two other
reasons, common in pedagogical practice, often make the problems in leaps
even worse. The first reason is when the student constantly fears playing out
of tune. This creates a lack of confidence in moving the left hand, and
consequently the student flattens or “falls short” of the arrival note. The
second reason is tied to the fear of playing out of rhythm. This causes the
shift to be made too quickly, almost convulsively, and this also creates
problems for intonation.
In these cases we recommend focusing special attention on the fluidity and
tranquility of the actual movement. This helps to make the shift properly,
thereby improving the intonation and quality of sound. When working on
making a shift smooth, it is important to keep in mind the principle of
beginning the shift slowly and then accelerating. Pedagogical practice has
shown this method helps overcome the difficulties associated with large
leaps. The player perceives the beginning of the leap to be associated not
with the initial note, but with the moment the movement accelerates. Thus,
the distance between the notes appears to diminish, providing greater
confidence in accomplishing this kind of shift. Moreover, the smooth
movement of the left hand then does not disrupt the smooth movement of the
bow, which is especially important when making the leap in legato.
There are many different opinions on how best to execute large leaps. For
example, some believe that in making a leap from a lower finger to a higher
finger, the higher finger should help by also sliding (as in a Type 3 shift). On
the other hand, others believe that only the initial finger should be used. We
find the answer to depend on the player’s individual adaption. However, in
all cases, the solution must proceed from the degree to which either method
produces the sound closest to that required in the musical excerpt.
We should also emphasize that the use of intermediary notes when making
large leaps to the high positions creates excess tension in the hand and is
consequently inadvisable. This is clear in Example 2.69.
Example 2.69 Paganini: Witches’s Dance.
When making leaps to the high positions, it is important to note that the
shape of the instrument requires the hand to be prepared in advance in order
to make a unified movement. The feeling of this unified movement should be
so thoroughly assimilated that it is able to almost anticipate the movement
itself. As Tseitlin notes, this makes it much easier to execute the leap
accurately. According to Mostras, one reason for problematic intonation in
large leaps is when vibrato is used during the shift, since this obscures a clear
perception of distance.97
To insure accurate intonation in large leaps, we recommend special
exercises that involve leaps of varying distances and their subsequent
comparison (the latter is particularly important). These exercises enable the
correct calculation of distance on the fingerboard (developing a “good eye,”
as Yampolsky would say), and they also develop confidence in making large
leaps. The exercises may be structured according to the following principles.
1. Leaps from the same note to different positions. There are two versions
of this exercise that may also be combined.
(a) Shifts with the same finger, but to different notes separated by
various intervals all the way down to a half-step (these exercises
especially help sharpen the sense of distance on the fingerboard).
(b) Shifts with different fingers, but to the same note.
2. Leaps from different positions to the same note (for example, to a
harmonic).
3. Leaps from different notes to different notes.
Even before making the leap one must clearly imagine the sound of the
desired note and pre-hear it. This way, the movement of the hand realizes a
sonic relationship that has already been perceived. Only in this manner, and
not through mechanical repetition, can the necessary aural-motor connections
be created that allow command of the fingerboard (“knowing the
fingerboard,” as Oistrakh would say).
Shifts that involve notes separated by rests are a special case. These shifts
often make it difficult to play the new note in tune since sometimes it is
played as if it were an independent note instead of being associated with the
previous one. This happens when contact is lost with the fingerboard during
the rest. For example, this is a common problem in the following excerpt
from the Tchaikovsky concerto, where the difficulty lies in playing the note
E3 in tune (see Example 2.70).
Example 2.72
Another example is found in Ernst’s Othello where many performers do
not always accurately land on the natural harmonic E (in this case the rest is
not indicated, although the nature of the variation requires it) (see Example
2.71).
To avoid these types of difficulties it is necessary to retain uninterrupted
contact with the fingerboard during the pauses. This is why Yampolsky
recommended the following method of playing in Example 2.70: the first
finger, on the note F3, does not leave the string, and during the rest shifts one
step lower to E3 (see Example 2.72).
In Example 2.71, one may recommend not playing the harmonic randomly
from above but instead making a Type 2 or Type 3 shift during the rest. The
choice of shift can be left to the player.
This analysis of different methods of shifting allows us to critically
evaluate their main elements and also determine the correct ways they should
be studied. Returning again to the use of intermediary notes, we should note
that this method could be useful in the very early stages of study. It allows
the student to develop a sense of distances on the fingerboard and helps to
organize the correct placement of the fingers in the new position. However, it
is important to remember that the use of intermediary notes should be as
short-lived as possible so that the conditioned reflexes are not fixed.
Otherwise, this turns into a bad habit that restricts further development.
Example 2.74a
Example 2.74b
It is useful to begin studying these types of shifts first. In the early stages of
study, shifts via an open string are the easiest since the fingers are not
involved, and only the hand needs to move along the fingerboard. However,
there are other hidden dangers in these types of shifts. Specifically, these
shifts often cause the player to lose the perception of distance on the
fingerboard. To remedy this, the open string may be temporarily eliminated,
reestablishing the uninterrupted connection of the fingers with the string and
thereby helping to develop and reinforce the correct perception of distance.
For example, the exercise in Examples 2.74a–b may be helpful in studying
the excerpt in Example 2.73.
Example 2.77
Example 2.78
Example 2.80
Example 2.82
The second method is based on contracting, rather than extending, the
fingers. In a similar way the finger reaches the required note, then the hand
follows and assumes a normal position. The type of shift is illustrated in
Example 2.80.
This is also a way to make a Type 4 shift when moving to a lower note
when the hand is ascending, or to a higher note when the hand is descending
(see Example 2.81).
Since these types of shifts serve a particular aesthetic purpose and are
consequently limited in use, teachers usually do not spend a lot of time on
them. However, exercises for developing an extended, normal, and contracted
position of the hand are helpful for developing general elasticity and
flexibility in the hand. An example of such a useful exercise is to play a
descending scale in broken thirds using Baillot’s fingering (Example 2.82).100
However, one should not consider this fingering to be used in performance
practice as Emil Kross recommends.101
Example 2.83
Example 2.84
Shifts from Natural Harmonics
In Example 2.86, the fourth finger does not press the string at all, and
moves directly from one harmonic to another, only gently touching the string.
Chromatic Glissando
Example 2.87
Example 2.88
Flesch correctly noted that the most common reason for poor coordination
is that the general arm movement is too slow.103 He recommends the
following method to correct this: The student should first play the chromatic
scale as an uninterrupted glissando, around twelve times in a row, keeping it
rhythmical. After this movement of the forearm is assimilated, a “vibrato-
like” movement in the wrist is added. This creates an even vertical movement
in half-steps and, after sufficient practice, leads to mastery of a clean
chromatic glissando. Although this exercise seems to be structured correctly,
it contains a serious flaw. In attempting to initially establish an even,
rhythmical arm movement, this method does not take into consideration the
fact that the distances between adjacent intervals are different in the lower
and upper regions of the fingerboard. Therefore, the descending movement of
the arm cannot be even, but should constantly accelerate (naturally, this
accelerando movement must always be developed in conjunction with the
ear). Consequently, it isn’t possible to use Flesch’s recommended preparatory
exercise of making an uninterrupted glissando since it does not allow one to
establish the required degree of acceleration.
In this respect, Abraham Yampolsky’s method of mastering the chromatic
glissando may be considered more efficient. For example, when working on a
passage from Wieniawski’s Violin Concerto No. 2 (Example 2.87),
Yampolsky recommends the following exercises. The first exercise (Example
2.88) sketches out the general movement of the arm, taking into account the
acceleration as it approaches the lower positions. The second exercise
(Example 2.89) retains the main elements of the general arm movement and
fills in the intervals with a pulsating finger movement in half-steps.
Example 2.89
If the student has difficulties making the pulsating half-steps with the
finger, or when the chromatic glissando is first introduced, the student may
be advised to practice small sections of the scale (staying within the intervals
of a fourth, fifth, or octave). These can be played in different rhythms
(triplets, quadruplets, etc.) in both ascending and descending directions.
These exercises are especially useful when working on glissandi in diatonic
sequences, which are encountered less frequently. Diatonic sequences are
more difficult because of the need to move the hand at irregular intervals, but
in all other respects they follow the same principles laid out for chromatic
sequences.
It is important to note the specific role the thumb plays in descending
chromatic glissandi over large distances. The thumb may be involved in two
ways. Firstly, the thumb can be a point of support for the wrist as the latter
moves from the higher positions to where the thumb is positioned, and then
the thumb continues to move in tandem with the whole arm. Alternately, the
thumb can move together with the arm from the very beginning, sometimes
even losing contact with the violin neck. The choice of method usually
depends on individual adaptations, although we find the second method to be
more efficient.
In most cases the descending chromatic glissando is made with the third
finger. However, it is also a useful exercise to play it with other fingers. For
example, playing it with the first and fourth fingers helps develop this
technique for octaves, and using the second and third fingers helps prepare
the chromatic scale in sixths.
While usually the chromatic scale is played in a descending direction, it
should also be studied ascending. This helps in preparing the chromatic
glissando in double stops, which, as we know, occurs in both directions. We
should also add that the glissando in sixths is usually played with the second
and third fingers, the glissando in thirds with the first and third fingers, and
the glissando in octaves with both the first and fourth fingers and the first and
third fingers.
When playing the chromatic glissando legato, it is important to pay special
attention to even and smooth movement in the right arm. When the chromatic
glissando is played with different bowings (such as détaché, spiccato,
staccato, or ricochet), the nature of the left-hand movement changes
completely. In these cases, there is no need for a gradual, interrupted
movement in the left hand; it simply makes a continuous smooth movement
while the right hand provides the necessary pulsation. In these cases
coordination is especially important between the progressive accelerating
movement in the left hand (or decelerating movement when ascending) and
the bow changes that determine the steps of the chromatic scale.
Shifts made with double stops are based on the types of shifts we have
already examined. The difference here is that two shifts take place on two
strings. To systemize our analysis, we find it convenient to group the double
stops in the following categories: (1) octaves and tenths; (2) thirds and
fingered octaves; and (3) sixths and fourths.
OCTAVES
Movement in octaves consists of two Type 1 shifts made simultaneously on
two strings. The main problem of octaves is the difficulty in connecting the
shifts smoothly, which causes the shifts to be sudden and abrupt. This jerky
quality arises from the desire to give the sound more definition. However,
usually it only creates a harsh tone, leads to poor intonation, and makes the
octaves sound heavy and stiff. Unfortunately, many treatises even
recommend playing octaves by “jumping.” In reality, the only efficient way
to play octaves is to move the hand as lightly and smoothly as possible, in the
same way described earlier for Type 1 shifts. Starting the shift gently ensures
a smooth general movement in the arm, while the direction toward the end of
each shift gives the sound the required definition. At the same time it is
important to make sure the fingers do not excessively press the string so that
the general progression of the hand is not restricted.
Example 2.91
Since octaves are usually played with the first and fourth fingers, the
question often arises of what to do with the second and third fingers. Many
methods recommend keeping the second and third fingers on the string, since
this seemingly makes the first and fourth fingers more secure, and therefore
helps intonation. Other treatises recommend that only the third finger remain
on the string, and other methods recommend taking off both the second and
third fingers. In our opinion the latter is the more rational. Leaving all the
fingers on the string (even just the third finger) binds the entire hand. This
makes vibrato impossible in cantilena, reduces agility in faster tempos, and
complicates playing in the higher positions because of the smaller distances.
According to Mostras, the fourth finger becomes the point of support when
octaves ascend, and the first finger is the point of support when the octaves
descend. Because the distances diminish in the upper part of the fingerboard,
it is very important to note that the first and fourth fingers must draw
considerably closer when playing octaves. This complicates the general
progression of the hand and requires additional coordination. How, then,
should we study octaves to most efficiently develop these coordinated
elements? The specialized literature offers two different points of view.
The first suggests starting to learn octaves by connecting two or three notes
of the scale, and then gradually increasing the range to an octave. Sometimes
it may be recommended to begin with the chromatic scale, since this keeps
the intervals consistent and seemingly facilitates shifting. In this method, the
main focus is on learning the gradual progression of octaves.
The second, more efficient point of view is based on studying the change
in distance between the first and fourth fingers in different parts of the
fingerboard. Flesch, for example, recommends the exercise in Example 2.91.
Example 2.92
Example 2.93 Sibor: Technique of Double Stops.
Example 2.94
Example 2.95
Example 2.96
The following example shows both versions of this method: Example
2.94a illustrates the fourth finger making a Type 2 shift; and in Example
2.94b the shift is made through the open string.
Shifting in octaves from one pair of strings to another without using the
open strings is especially difficult, since both fingers need to simultaneously
move to a new position and change strings. The specialized literature
provides almost no advice on how to make these shifts, leading Flesch to dub
them “leaps into the unknown.” However, Voiku does provide the following
suggestion for making these types of shifts (see Example 2.95).104
According to Voiku, for the shift to sound even and clear, the fingers
should transfer to the first position and subsequently slide to the E (i.e., to
the D and A strings), without losing contact with the strings. However, this
method is not very efficient since it based on simultaneously sliding both
fingers, which tenses the hand and affects the sound. This method only brings
the hand to the new position without being able to avoid the simultaneous
transfer of two fingers to different strings, which creates extraneous sounds.
We consider Abraham Yampolsky’s method to be more successful. His
technique is based on the fact that at some point in shifting from one pair of
strings to another, the bow lies only on the common (middle) string (Example
2.96). This is the precise moment when the connecting Type 2 shift is made
to the new position, which is immediately followed by a transfer of the initial
finger (Example 2.97).
Example 2.97
Example 2.98a
Example 2.98b
Example 2.99
TENTHS
The main difficulty of tenths is created by the extension in the fingers,
especially in the lower part of the fingerboard. Just like octaves, tenths are
connected with Type 1 shifts made simultaneously on two strings. The jerky,
abrupt shifts that are intrinsically problematic to octaves become even more
common and significantly accentuated when playing tenths. This is due to
both the large stretch in the fingers and the excessive pressure of the fingers
on the fingerboard caused by the desire to keep the fingers in their extended
state. In this case (just as in octaves), the hand should try to remain free and
relaxed, which also facilitates vibrato in phrases where the tenths need to
sound more melodious and expressive. Meanwhile, it is especially important
to note that the correct shape of the hand may significantly relieve excessive
pressure and tension. Effectively, in extending the fingers, it is much easier to
draw the first finger back than it is to stretch the fourth finger forward.105
This is because of both the anatomical structure of the hand and the relative
weakness of the little finger. Therefore, the hand should be positioned
primarily by adjusting to the position of the little finger, taking into account
the placement of the thumb. And yet, we often find the opposite occurs; when
starting to learn tenths, students reach up with the fourth finger instead of
drawing back the first finger. If this is not corrected in due time, the student
will usually always be afraid of playing tenths.
The exercise in Example 2.99 facilitates the study of tenths.
This exercise starts in the upper positions, where the distance between the
intervals is smaller. This relieves excessive tension and makes the extension
easier. As the hand moves to the lower positions, the distance between the
intervals is increased gradually, making it easier to make the subsequent
extensions without extra tension. At the same time, the progression in the
lower voice helps the ear to hear the interval correctly and play it in tune.
This is an important point, since students often begin learning tenths without
a clear perception of how they should sound. And finally, this exercise
correctly positions the hand, since it requires the first finger to extend down
instead of stretching the fourth finger up.
Example 2.100
Example 2.101a
Example 2.101b
Another significant difficulty that is specific to tenths and not encountered
in octaves is that the fingers often need to move to different intervals in
diatonic sequences. And this is made even more difficult by the fact that even
if both fingers move the same interval, the distances they cover are not the
same. This is especially obvious when both voices move a whole step and is
explained by the great distance between the fingers.
The student must be entirely aware of all these factors when playing
diatonic sequences and never lose sight of the varying distances between the
fingers in the upper and lower positions. With this in mind, we recommend
the exercises in Example 2.100 for work on intonation, which start by
connecting any two degrees of the scale and then subsequently widening the
interval.
Auer suggested working on intonation in tenths by playing with the bow
alternatively only on the upper or lower string.106 Mostras presents an
interesting opinion by suggesting that, in connecting tenths, the guiding
finger should be the one that covers the greater distance.107 Thus, in example
Example 2.101a, the fourth finger leads; and in Example 2.101b, the first
finger leads.
Example 2.102
Example 2.103a Rachmaninov: Romance.
THIRDS
It is possible to use all the main types of shifts when playing in thirds. Shifts
that retain the same pair of fingers (Type 1 shifts) may be subdivided into
two subcategories. In the first subcategory the fingers practically do not
change shape as they move from one major third to another major third, or
from a minor third to another minor third (Examples 2.103a and 2.103b). In
the second subcategory, the fingers significantly change their placement as
they shift from a major third to a minor third or vice versa (see Examples
2.104a–d).
Example 2.112
Example 2.113a
Example 2.113b
Koeckert notes that it is even better to make this shift with “two
intermediary double-stop notes” (Example 2.112).
We already noted the nonutility of this method of sliding both fingers in
our previous analysis of octaves.
Shifts across strings in thirds with the same pair of fingers (similar to Type
1 shifts) are rarely employed. However, we still find it necessary to indicate
the best way of playing them, which corresponds to the method of shifting in
octaves without using the open string. In Examples 2.113a–b, the shift starts
at the moment the bow is only on the A string and continues with the first
finger transferring to the note A2.
Example 2.114a
Example 2.114b
Example 2.114c
Example 2.115 Rakov: Violin Concerto, mvt. 2.
The method of shifting in thirds from one pair of strings to another is based
on utilizing the common string (analogous to shifting in octaves as already
described). The shift is made with the finger that lies on the common string
precisely when the bow is solely on that string. These shifts usually resemble
Type 2 or Type 4 shifts. This method is illustrated in Example 2.114a, where
the second finger makes the slide while the bow is on the D string. Similar
shifts are illustrated in Examples 2.114b and 2.114c.
The expressive quality of Type 3 shifts in thirds requires both fingers to
slide, as Example 2.115.
It is evident that the slide should not use intermediary notes, and generally
it should follow the principles we described for this type of shift in one voice.
The right hand plays an important role when shifting from one pair of
strings to another. The moment when the bow lies solely on the common
string should not disrupt the impression of two uninterrupted voices.
Therefore, the bow needs to remain for as little time as possible on the one
string and transfer softly and smoothly, avoiding any kind of emphasis.
In those cases when there is no common string, the principle of shifting
remains the same. The main difference is that the slide is made with the
finger that is closest to the two arrival strings.
Thirds are among the most commonly used double stops. Often employed
in cantilena, they are, together with sixths, the most rewardingly expressive
intervals for violinists. This is because the fingers lie closer together than in
other intervals (such as octaves and tenths), and this significantly facilitates
the vibrato that is so important in cantilena. Also, of all double stops, thirds
are the easiest to use to demonstrate brilliant virtuosity. This is evident in the
numerous virtuosic passages, and even entire pieces, written only in thirds.
Therefore, mastering thirds is essential to violin playing.
The most common problem in playing thirds is excessive pressure of the
fingers on the string. This causes abrupt shifts that lack the necessary
flexibility and disrupt the flow of one unified movement. We have already
noted how this affects violin playing in general, but when playing double
stops, and thirds in particular, these problems are even more pronounced.
Mastering shifts in thirds (or any other double stops) should be based on
properly mastering simple shifts, since the same principles generally apply to
double stops. For example, preliminary exercises in playing scales with two
fingers (with the first and second and with the third and fourth) are very
useful. Once regular shifts have been mastered, the more difficult conditions
of double stops (i.e., moving both pairs of fingers), which require better
coordination and more exertion in the muscles, should not affect the skills
that are already in place. This should be always taken into account in
pedagogical practice so that exercises for shifting in thirds are structured
correctly.
Unfortunately, many irrational suggestions in the specialized literature
essentially block the development of the basic elements of shifting. For
example, in David’s Violinschule it is suggested to play the preparatory
exercises for scales in thirds with a martelé stroke (probably because the rest
is supposed to facilitate the preparation of the following fingers).110
However, this exercise prohibits the possibility of controlling the actual
quality of the shift itself, which then naturally blocks the development of the
necessary skills.
Flesch breaks down playing in thirds into the following three elements: (1)
dropping the fingers in one position; (2) shifting between positions on the
same pair of strings; and (3) shifting from one pair of strings to another.111
He recommends studying each of these elements separately, spending one
month on each type of movement. This creates a standardized system that
doesn’t take into consideration the individual particularities or the prior
preparation of each student, and can lead to undesirable results. In separating
the elements of playing thirds, Flesch’s system directs the student’s attention
away from developing one unified movement, for even ideal mastery of
separate elements does not necessary result in ideal mastery of the combined
elements. Also, it is not necessary to apply the preparatory exercises equally
to all three elements, but only to the weaker components. Thus, if for any
reason one of the components in the movement is problematic, it should be
corrected with the corresponding exercise. But even then, the exercise needs
to be integrated into the unified movement so that the player realizes how this
element fits into the entire process.
Example 2.116
Example 2.117
The choice of the correct fingering is very important in ensuring free and
swift sequences in thirds, especially when the thirds are played in the lower
registers and involve shifting between different pairs of strings. In the higher
registers, where the shifts usually occur on the same pair of strings, the
problem generally lies in making the shift coincide with the strong rhythmic
pulse.
Some violinists are able to play thirds very freely and lightly in fast tempos
in the upper positions. This is largely explained by the possibility of not
having to lift the first and third fingers from the string, especially when
ascending. However, this is impossible in the lower positions where the
alternating up and down hand movements are in themselves quite difficult.
To further complicate matters, these shifts from one pair of strings to another
create difficulties for the right hand that affect the sound quality.
Many violin methods suggest fingerings based on contrasting tonalities
that may or may not make use of open strings. When open strings are used,
two types of fingerings are recommended: shifting through third position
(Example 2.116) or shifting through second position (Example 2.117)
Example 2.118
Example 2.119
In our opinion the first fingering is not rational, especially when ascending.
Shifting the relatively large distance to third position, just for the sake of one
note, inevitably restricts mobility in the passage. Even when perfectly
executed, this method still seems busy. This fingering is possible when
descending, but only when making one shift and not a series of shifts, which
are especially awkward when playing staccato. We should also add that this
fingering disrupts correct voice leading, which, as Flesch notes, becomes
especially obvious in cantilena (see Example 2.118).112
The other suggested fingering that moves through second position
(Example 2.117) is more rational. The shorter distance between shifts allows
the passage to be played smoothly and lightly in quick tempos, especially
when the shifts coincide with the strong beats.
However, we do not always find it necessary to use open strings when
tonalities allow it. In some cases it can be better to use a fingering that avoids
the open string, even if the distance of the shift is increased (see Example
2.119). 113
It would be awkward to play this scale using open strings, since the shifts
would coincide with weak beats rather than strong beats and also occur from
a major third to a minor third and vice versa.
When tonalities don’t permit the use of open strings, two fingerings are
possible: moving through the even-numbered positions (i.e., half position to
second position, second position to fourth position) or moving through the
odd-numbered positions (first to third, third to fifth).114 At the same time the
following must be taken into consideration: On the one hand in the higher
positions (e.g., from third to fifth), the hand needs to travel less distance, but
at the same time the higher positions on the A and D strings sound less
brilliant. However, the choice of fingering should still be based on whether
the shifts coincide with the strong rhythmic beats. Both these types of
fingerings may also be used in progressions in thirds when the tonalities do
permit open strings.
Example 2.121
In certain cases when thirds are played in the lower positions, a fingering
may be used that incorporates extensions and eliminates the need to shift.
This fingering is very useful as an exercise to strengthen the fingers and
develop dexterity and agility. This is how Sergei Korguyev presents it in his
work Exercises in Double Stops.115 However, the practical uses of this
fingering are limited.
FINGERED OCTAVES
Fingered octaves are used for more clarity and brilliance in octave passages.
It is also clear that many figurations such as trills, mordents, and so on can
only be played with the help of fingered octaves (see Example 2.120).
Fingered octaves are also used when octaves are played in cantilena and a
glissando must be entirely avoided.
The technique of fingered octaves is very similar to that used in playing
thirds, with the only difference being the reverse placement of the fingers
(i.e., in fingered octaves the first and second fingers move along the lower
string, and the third and fourth fingers move along the higher string).
Therefore all shifts in fingered octaves on the same pair of strings adhere to
the principles for Type 1, Type 2, Type 3, and Type 4 shifts in thirds.
Some methods, including Voiku’s, recommend sliding two fingers on two
strings, as in Example 2.121.
However, this technique that we already noted to be ineffective for thirds is
even less advisable for fingered octaves, since it is particularly difficult to
slide the fingers on two strings while they are extended.
The extension of the fingers in fingered octaves makes all the difficulties
of mastering this technique even more complicated. The stretch, the tension it
creates, and the instinctive desire to maintain the extended position of the
fingers by pressing harder on the string all contribute to limiting the free
movement of the hand between positions. The stretch also affects the
smoothness of the shift. Therefore, when working on fingered octaves it is
absolutely necessary to make sure that the effort in stretching the hand does
not create extra finger pressure on the string and that all the conditions for
ensuring smooth shifts are respected.
Example 2.122
Example 2.123
Example 2.124
Example 2.125
Example 2.126
Example 2.127
The most common fingering for scale passages in fingered octaves can be
seen in Example 2.125.
We should note that in some cases it is also possible to play fingered
octaves using the open string and shift through second position, similar to
what was already described for thirds. This is the fingering Georgy Dulov
recommends for playing ascending scales (Example 2.126).117
At first this seems to be more difficult, primarily because of the need to
transfer the second finger across the string. But there is enough time to do
this, since the transfer happens when the bow lies only on the common string
(although it is very important to correctly determine the moment the finger
transfers). The more serious problem with this fingering is that the half-steps
often occur between adjacent fingers (i.e., between the first and second or
third and fourth). This creates significant difficulties, which is why this
technique is usually avoided in practice.
Kayum Baiburov has an interesting suggestion of shifting at the half-step
in such cases. This is possible not only with the first and third fingers but also
with the second and fourth fingers (see Example 2.127). The advantage of
this fingering, which becomes evident after a certain amount of practice, is
that the distance of the shift is smaller and this allows greater smoothness and
mobility.
Example 2.128
SIXTHS
The special characteristic of these double stops is that the fingers have to
transfer from one string to another. However, it is also possible to shift in
sixths without transferring the fingers. Let us first examine Type 1 shifts.
These may be divided into two subcategories, similar to the analogous shifts
in thirds. In the first subcategory the fingers maintain the same placement (for
example, shifting from a minor sixth to another minor sixth, or a major sixth
to a major sixth). In the second subcategory the fingers change their
placement (from a minor sixth to a major sixth and vice versa). Since Type 2
shifts in sixths are more difficult for intonation, we find it logical to begin
studying shifts in sixths using Type 1 shifts (just as we did with thirds). This
way the fingers do not change, and it is possible to start by connecting those
degrees of the scale that retain the same interval.
Another example of when the fingers don’t need to cross strings while
shifting is when one pair of fingers replaces another, such as when shifting
from the first and second fingers to the third and fourth (or vice versa) or
shifting from the first finger and open string to another pair of fingers. Those
shifts in sixths that don’t involve transferring the fingers across the strings
may be made using Type 1, Type 2, or Type 3 shifts and they follow the
same principles established for these shifts in one voice.
Transferring the fingers across the strings creates significant technical
difficulties that affect the sound (by creating extraneous noises). For this
reason, in both the pedagogical literature and in practice, we encounter many
attempts to avoid this transfer by using alternate fingerings. The first option is
to play all the shifts in sixths, as well as sixths in one position, by sliding the
same pair of fingers (Type 1 shifts). Another option that avoids transferring
the fingers is to always alternate the first and second fingers with the third
and fourth fingers.118 And finally, by combining these two fingerings it
becomes possible to play long melodic excerpts in sixths almost without
transferring the fingers across strings (see Example 2.129).
Example 2.131
When playing scale passages in sixths that are similar to those shown in
Example 2.130, it is preferable to use the first fingering that is based on
sliding the same pair of fingers.
In this case it is not effective to use a combined fingering (as in Example
2.129) because the numerous alternating movements of the hand up and
down the fingerboard will disrupt the fluidity and cause insecure intonation.
Aside from the two fingerings already indicated, there is also a third
fingering in which the sixths are not played with adjacent fingers, more
reminiscent of fingered octaves (see Example 2.131).
Example 2.133
Example 2.134a
Example 2.134b
Example 2.135
Example 2.136a
When we analyze the function of the second finger in this case, we find it
consists of two elements: the second finger begins the shift, but then it also
needs to transfer to the lower note (A1) in the second sixth. Therefore, the
movement of the second finger needs to correctly coordinate both these
elements. The first part of the shift follows the principles of a Type 2 shift,
with the finger gradually releasing until it completely lifts away from the
string. This is precisely the moment when the finger can easily transfer to the
adjacent string. Sometimes, to avoid extraneous sounds, the bow may
momentarily rest on one string.
Type 3 shifts that transfer the fingers are made the same way, respecting
the general principles for these types of shifts. However, in this case the
finger transfer occurs slightly earlier than in Type 2 shifts (see Example
2.135).
In Type 4 shifts, such as in Examples 2.136a–b, the second finger that
starts to slide on the D string is pushed out by the first finger that follows.
The moment the second finger cedes its place to the first finger, instead of
lifting it transfers to the adjacent string and continues its movement by
pushing the third finger out. Thus, this shift really consists of two
simultaneous Type 4 shifts, which is evident not only in the fingering but also
in the manner of shifting. This is easily illustrated by drawing the bow only
on the upper or lower strings.
Example 2.136b
Example 2.137
Example 2.138
Shifts in sixths from one pair of strings to another make use of the instant
when the bow lies only on the common string. When the shift occurs
precisely at this moment, there is no need to cross-transfer the fingers. The
method of sliding both fingers in these cases on two strings is entirely
illogical.
Shifts can be avoided in the lower registers by using open strings when the
tonality permits (see Example 2.137).
In tonalities that don’t permit the open string, and in the upper registers,
the fingering can be structured as in Example 2.138.
In these cases Korguyev also presents a fingering that merits attention (see
Example 2.139).120
Example 2.140
This fingering manages to reduce the general number of shifts and finger
transfers because of the descending shifts to the third and fourth fingers.
Using the half and second positions in the lower register creates the same
advantages as for thirds in analogous conditions, and likewise reduces the
number of shifts.
FOURTHS
In terms of execution, these double stops are related to sixths in all respects.
Fourths differ from sixths by the reverse placement of the fingers in the same
way fingered octaves differ from thirds (see Example 2.140).
This difference determines some aspects of how they are played, similar to
the relationship already described between thirds and fingered octaves.
However, fourths are closer to sixths than fingered octaves are to thirds, since
fingered octaves also involve extensions that affect how they are played. We
should note that it is very rare to encounter progressions in fourths in their
pure form in the violin literature. However, from a pedagogical point of view
these double stops are useful for developing good intonation as well as agile
and supple fingers.
When analyzing the activity of the left hand, we must always consider its role
in the general process of violin playing. In this respect, an important element
is coordination between the left and right hands, which requires special
attention in our study.
As practice shows, shifts are often accompanied by a change in the
intensity of the bow pressure or a disruption of the bow’s steady movement
along the string. It is very common for both beginners and those violinists
with entrenched bad habits to release the bow pressure as they shift. This is
particularly noticeable in long-distance shifts in which the bow almost seems
entirely to lift away from the strings. This phenomenon is also observed in
smaller shifts that follow each other in immediate succession (e.g., in scale
passages) and is the most common problem for the right hand while shifting.
The opposite problem is also encountered—when shifts are accompanied by
extra pressure that causes an unintentional crescendo. Naturally, we are not
speaking of those cases where a portamento is specifically hidden or
emphasized by the right hand for musical purposes.
Other problems that affect the right hand while shifting include slowing
down the bow (which usually coincides with releasing the pressure) or
speeding up the bow (which is less common). Additionally, other
complications present themselves that illustrate the interdependence of both
hands. For example, when shifting from the higher to lower positions,
sometimes the right hand begins to follow the movement of the left hand and
draws the bow away from the bridge toward the fingerboard. Although to a
certain extent this movement is natural to help the tone quality, it is often so
excessive that the bow seems to slide onto the fingerboard, as Mostras notes
in his work Intonation on the Violin.121
A good example of the interdependence of the hands is when most students
first attempt to play a chromatic glissando with one finger. Their right hand
often imitates the pulsating movement of the left hand and plays something
similar to staccato. We should also note that the right hand might have an
equally disruptive effect on the left hand. For example, changing the bow
pressure on the string usually immediately affects the pressure of the fingers
in the left hand. This often makes fixing this widespread problem even more
difficult. The danger here is that releasing the finger pressure then causes a
release in bow pressure, which naturally affects the sound. But when the
teacher asks the student to play with a fuller bow stroke, the fingers again
may press too hard on the string.
The influence of the right hand is similarly evident when the same passage
is played first with one bowing and then with another. The different
movements in the right hand, as a rule, alter the movements in the left hand
and affect the execution of the passage. For instance, in Example 2.141, the
passage from Saint-Saens’ Rondo Capriccioso may come out perfectly when
played détaché but not work at all in spiccato, because of problems in
shifting.
Example 2.142
Flesch gives an interesting example of how the right hand influences the
left. He notes that when a passage ends with a harmonic, the last note often
doesn’t sound because the fourth finger reflexively copies the movement of
the bow (which lifts from the string at the end) and thereby does not quite
reach the final note (see Example 2.142).122
We also need to emphasize that problems in coordination depend not only
on the difficulty of the movements themselves but also on the degree to
which the student is prepared for these movements. Expecting the student to
prematurely execute a technique that he or she is not prepared for usually
leads to poor results. For example, asking the student to make the shift
inaudible before he or she is prepared usually causes the student to release the
bow from the string or slow it down. Consequently the student will always
associate the shift with a disrupted movement in the right hand, creating a
bad habit (which can be very dangerous if the teacher does not observe it in
time). It is therefore important to strictly follow a sequential method that
progresses from simpler to more complicated tasks, gradually adding
movements that are more and more difficult. In this respect it is important to
note the order in which different shifts are studied. For example, it would be
wrong to begin by learning Type 4 shifts, which are technically more
difficult. And yet, this mistake is made all too often, especially in the older
treatises.
An extremely important pedagogical task is to develop good coordination
between the hands. Unfortunately, all too often the teacher is confronted with
another, rather thankless task of correcting problems in coordination that
developed because the first stages of study were structured incorrectly. For
this reason it is essential to prevent the possibility of developing bad habits.
However, it is also important to remember that in the pedagogical process it
is not enough to just develop a certain number of coordinated relationships,
for it is impossible to foresee all the coordinated interactions that might be
encountered in the student’s musical future. The teacher must work on
developing the student’s facility for quickly assimilating new tasks, which
allows any type of coordination to be integrated with ease. For this reason it
is necessary to focus on developing quick reactions in the nervous processes
right from the beginning, especially if these reactions appear to be slow.
Example 2.145
Example 2.146
Because Type 2 and Type 3 shifts differ in their musical and expressive
properties, the nature of the music determines which shift to choose. Because
a Type 2 shift is characterized by a portamento that seems to naturally flow
from the previous note, it would therefore be wrong to prematurely change
the bow. Similarly, it is not logical to separate the portamento from the new
note in a Type 3 shift, since this shift was musically chosen precisely because
the portamento flows into the new note.
In this light, let us look at bow changes for other types of shifts. In Type 1
shifts, Voldan and Ševčík, among others, recommend making the slide with
the new bow stroke.129 Other violinists, such as Koeckert and Israel
Yampolsky, believe the slide belongs to the first stroke and not the second.130
In our opinion, being confined to just one of these methods limits the
expressive possibilities, since modifying the moment the bow changes
significantly affects the sound. One version emphasizes the beginning of the
slide (because the portamento is linked to the first note) and the other
emphasizes the end of the shift (because the portamento is linked to the
second note) (see Example 2.145).
The appropriate use of both these methods broadens the expressive
possibilities open to the performer, similar to choosing between a Type 2 or
Type 3 shift.
The question of what to do in a Type 4 shift should be resolved the same
way (see Example 2.146).
In the first measure of Example 2.146, the portamento is tied to the first
bow stroke, emphasizing the beginning of the shift and the slide of the initial
finger. In the second measure of Example 2.146, the portamento is tied to the
second bow stroke, emphasizing the end of the shift and the slide of the
subsequent finger. Thus the expressive potential of this shift becomes greater
than using a single method, as some treatises recommend. The choice of
method must always stem from which expressive quality the particular shift
requires.
Example 2.147a
Example 2.147b
Example 2.148
Example 2.151a
Example 2.152
The coordination between the hands that the music requires is established
through the interrelation and interdependence of the chosen bowings and
fingerings. This relationship is primarily determined by what most accurately
conveys the character of the music, which is why this relationship is dynamic
and constantly changing. For this reason, we cannot speak of a permanent
relationship between certain bowings and fingerings. And this is why we
cannot entirely agree with Moser’s opinion regarding the fingering for the
following excerpt from the finale of Viotti’s Violin Concerto No. 23 (see
Example 2.152).133
Moser provides two possible fingerings and writes that if the passage were
to be played with a different bowing (all détaché or all legato in one bow),
then there would be no problem with the upper fingering. But the indicated
bowings cause the shifts to fall awkwardly (creating acoustic-aesthetic
problems), which is why Moser suggests using the lower fingering where the
change of position coincides with a change in bow. Thus Moser seems to
canonize a very specific relationship between bowings and fingerings. Yet,
such a permanent relationship may in other cases completely contradict the
musical essence of what is being played. In Moser’s example, the relationship
is based on the principle of shifting while changing the bow. This largely
helps to cover up the sound of the shift itself. Unquestionably, this principle
has positive attributes, but only when the passage needs to sound very clear
and brilliant in a quick tempo. Applying this to other musical material that is
more lyrical would have an adverse effect. Thus, there is a good reason that
Abraham Yampolsky suggests the opposite fingering in an analogous passage
in Bach’s Chaconne. This creates a more expressive and lyrical sound in this
particular case (see Example 2.153).
Example 2.153
Because shifts occur at the expense of the length of the previous note, in
sequences that contain notes of different lengths, it is logical to choose a
fingering in which the shift takes place after the long note rather than after the
shorter note (see Examples 2.154 and 2.155).
The opposite scenario causes the shift to be made hastily and tensely,
thereby affecting the sound and depriving the music of the required lightness
and elegance.
All the above clearly confirms the importance of developing good
coordination between both hands in order to establish unity and correctly
convey the musical character of the piece. This places an important
responsibility on the teacher, whose challenge is to create the best conditions
for developing these kinds of coordination.
This study has examined different aspects of the problem of shifting in the
context of musical interpretation. Our analysis allows us to establish a series
of general principles that can be used in performance and pedagogical
practice. For clarity and convenience, we will list them here as separate
points.
1. Work on clean intonation while shifting should be based on developing
skills that cultivate a feeling for distance.
2. A shift is made with a unified movement in the arm and hand, not with
the isolated movement of just one component such as the wrist,
forearm, or shoulder. However, depending on the circumstances, the
leading element may be either the forearm or the wrist, with the other
parts of the arm following or making auxiliary movements.
3. In the beginning stages of study, horizontal movements of the hand used
in shifting or preparation for shifting should be studied as early as
possible. This method aims to overcome the inborn grasping reflex that
may otherwise restrict free movement in the left hand while shifting.
This method also allows the early development of coordination between
the main movements of the left hand, that is, the vertical movement in
the fingers and the horizontal movements of the hand and arm.
4. Every technique and procedure turns out to be dynamic and modifiable,
depending on the musical context. This includes how the violin is held
(with one or two points of support), the position of the left elbow, the
position of the thumb, and the pressure of the fingers on the string.
5. The development of any playing movement should be based on
perceiving and consolidating a feeling for the character of the
movement in association with the corresponding auditory perception.
6. Most shifts are characterized by a relatively slow beginning with a
subsequent acceleration. Both the individual particularities of the
performer and the musical context may, to a certain degree, alter the
moment when the acceleration starts and how it develops, although they
essentially do not change the nature of the movement itself. This
method of shifting establishes smoothness, flexibility, facility in
changing positions, a lyrical sound, and a light and agile technique.
7. It is not necessary to use so-called intermediary notes while shifting.
Instead of following a prescribed method of utilizing intermediary
notes, the procedures for shifting should be determined by the specific
musical context. However, we should point out that intermediary notes
may be helpful for learning to shift in the very early stages.
8. Shifts in double stops generally adhere to the same principles
established for simple shifts.
9. A good setup for the violinist should be determined by its organic
relationship to the movements required while playing. If this
relationship is disrupted, the setup becomes dogmatic and removed
from the active processes of playing. This kind of incorrect positioning
can restrict the player’s subsequent development.
10. The creation and development of playing movements must occur in
constant relation to auditory perceptions, instead of being based on
abstract “correct” systems that have no relation to sound.
11. The character of the sound, and consequently the character of the
movements, must always be determined by the character of the music.
NOTES
1. Carlo Tessarini, Nouvelle methode pour apprendre par theorie dans un mois de
tems à jouer du violon divisée en trois classes, avec des leçons à deux violons
par gradation (Paris: n.p., 1750). The following references and notes, unless
otherwise mentioned, are those supplied by Yuri Yankelevich in the original
text; English titles for Russian texts are supplied by the translator.
2. Lionel de la Laurenci, L’école francaise de violon de Lulli à Viotti. v. III (Paris:
n.p., 1909).
3. Leopold Mozart, Osnovatel’noe skripichnoe uchilishche/perevod s nem
(Fundamental Violin Treatise, translated from German) (St. Petersburg: n.p.,
1804).
4. Bartolomeo Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit
(Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel, 1797).
5. Louis Spohr, Violinschule (Wien: T. Haslinger, 1832).
6. Joseph Joachim and Andreas Moser, Violinschule (Berlin: N. Simrock, 1905).
7. Leopold Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke (Violin Playing as I Teach It)
(Moscow: Muzyka, 1965).
8. Karl Davidov, Shkola dlia violoncheli. Ridaktsia i dopolneniia S. M.
Kozolupova i L. S. Ginzburga (School of Cello Playing), ed. and rev. S.M.
Kozolupov and L. S. Ginzburg (Moscow: n.p., 1947).
9. Carl Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry (The Art of Violin Playing) (Moscow:
n.p., 1964).
10. Israel Yampolsky, Osnovy skripchnoi applikatury (Principles of Violin
Fingering) (Moscow: Gosudarstvenoye muzykal’noe izdatelstvo, 1955).
11. Ibid., 54—55.
12. In the old schools of Beriot and Spohr, it was likewise assumed that each
position corresponded to its own tonality; the tonality was based on the second
rather than the first finger on the G string.
13. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
14. Otakar Ševčík, Shkola skripichnoi tekhniki/perevod s cheshskogo (School of
Violin Technique, translated from Czech) (Moscow: n.p., 1929).
15. Jean Delphin Alard, Polnaia shkola dlia skripki, priniataia dlia rukovodstva v
parizhskoi konservatorii (Complete Violin Method in Use at the Paris
Conservatory, translated from French to Russian by A. Sokolova) (Moscow:
n.p., 1909).
16. Nikolai Garbuzov (1880–1955) was a Russian musical acoustician and
theoretician who proposed a theory of hearing centered around “zones,” which
takes into account the influence of such factors as pitch, loudness, timbre, and
rhythmic relations.—Translator’s note.
17. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
18. Konstantin Mostras, Intonatsiia na skripke (Intonation on the Violin), 2nd ed.
(Moscow: Gosudarstvenoe muzykalnoe izdatelstvo, 1968).
19. Leopold August Sass, Neue Schule für Geiger (Leipzig: Steingräber Verlag,
1920).
20. Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit.
21. Siegfried Eberhardt, Absolute Treffsicherheit auf der Violine (Berlin: Fürstner,
1912).
22. Gustave Koeckert, Rationelle Violintechnik (Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel,
1909).
23. P. Radmall, “Change of Position,” The Strad 689 (1947).
24. L. Nemirovsky, Mekhanicheskie i psikhologicheskie momenty v osnovnykh
priemakh skripichnoi tekhniki (Mechanical and Psychological Moments in the
Fundamental Skills of Violin Technique) (Moscow: n.p., 1915).
25. Leopold Auer, Graded Course of Violin Playing: A Complete Outline of Violin
Study for Individual and Class Instruction (New York: Carl Fischer, 1926).
26. Bedřich Voldan, Nová škola poloh: (analogická soustava skupinová) (Praha:
Neubert, 1924); Jan Mařák and Viktor Nopp, Housle, dějiny výroje houslí,
houslařstvi a hry houslove. Methodika. 3. vydáni, dopiněné a do doby
přitomné, upravil Viktor Nopp (Praha: Hudební matice umělecké besedy,
1944).
27. It should be noted that by free movement, we mean a minimal expenditure of
neuromuscular energy (with no restrictive elements) that the given task
requires.
28. Voldan, Nová škola poloh.
29. Ion Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry (tekhnika levoi
ruki). Perevod s Nem. V. N. Rimskogo-Korsakova (The Formation of a Natural
System of Violin Playing [Left-Hand Technique], translated from the German
by V. N. Rimsky-Korsakov) (Moscow: n.p., 1930). Further information on the
author of this work is unknown and the text appears not related to the
recognized Romanian violinist of the same name Ion Voicu (1923–1997)—
Translator’s note.
30. Iosef Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke (School of Violin Playing) (Leningrad:
n.p., 1924).
31. This is readily seen with beginners, who become tired and need a rest even
when holding the violin only for five minutes.
32. B. Mikhailovsky, Novii put’ skripacha (New Direction for the Violinist)
(Moscow: n.p., 1934).
33. In this treatise the term “first” joint refers not to the bottom, as is customary
today, but to the top joint; Rode-Baillot-Kreutzer, Skripichnaia shkola (Violin
School), 2nd ed. (St. Petersburg: n.p., 1829).
34. Pierre Marie François de Sales Baillot, L’art du violon: nouvelle méthode
(Mayence: n.p., 1834).
35. Alard, Polnaia shkola dlia skripki; Hubert Leonard, Ecole Leonard pour le
violon (Paris: Richault et Cie, 1877); Jules Charles Pennequin, Nouvelle
méthode de violon, théorique et pratique: en deux parties (Paris: Enoch, 1900).
36. Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit; Auer, Moia
shkola igry na skripke; Joachim and Moser, Violinschule; Edmund Singer and
Max Seifriz, Grosse theoretisch-praktische Violinschule (Berlin: Cottaschen
Buchhandlung, 1887); Heinrich Ernst Kayser, Neueste Methode des
Violinspiels, op. 32 Th. 3 (Leipzig: Cranz, 1900); R. Gofman, Bol’shaia i
podrobnaia shkola tekhniki igry na skripke, v progressivnom sistematicheskom
raspolozhenii ot pervyh shagov obucheniia do visshego ssovershenstvovania.
Perevod s Fr. (Large and Detailed School of Violin Technique, in Progressive
Systematic Format from the First Steps to the Highest Mastery, translated from
French) (Moscow: n.p., 1959).
37. Andreas Moser, Methodik des Violinspiels (Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel, 1920).
38. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849–1936) was a renowned Russian physiologist,
psychologist, and physicist largely known for his work on conditioned reflexes.
—Translator’s note.
39. Kayser, Neueste Methode des Violinspiels, op. 32 Th. 3; Reinhold Jockisch,
Katechismus der Violine und des Violinspiels (Leipzig: J. J. Weber, 1900); V.
Val’ter, Kak uchit’ igre na skripke (How to Teach Violin Playing), 3rd ed. (St.
Petersburg: n.p., 1910).
40. Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke; and Lesman, Puti razvitiya skripacha
(Directions of the Violinist’s Development) (Leningrad: n.p.,1934), 23.
41. Hugo Bekker, Tekhnika i estetika igry na violoncheli (perevod s nem.)
(Technique and Aesthetics of Cello Playing, translated from German))
(Moscow: n.p., 1977).
42. Konstantin Mostras, “Lektsii po metodike igry i prepodavaniia na skripke”
(Lectures on Methodology of Playing and Teaching the Violin). Manuscript.
43. Jockisch, Katechismus der Violine und des Violinspiels.
44. As always, the sound naturally depends on simultaneous movement in the left
and right arms. Isolating them in this example is only in the interest of
analyzing in greater detail the particular aspects of playing that are the principal
subjects of our study.
45. Koeckert, Rationelle Violintechnik.
46. Val’ter, Kak uchit’ igre na skripke.
47. Mozart, Osnovatel’noe skripichnoe uchilishche/perevod s nem.
48. Voldan, Nová škola poloh; Pennequin, Nouvelle méthode de violon.
49. Nemirovskii, Mekhanicheskie i psikhologicheskie momenty v osnovnykh
priemakh skripichnoi tekhniki.
50. Ibid., 97.
51. B. Struve, Tipovye formy postanovki ruk u instrumentalistov. Smichkovaia
gruppa (Typical Forms of Positioning the Hands of Instrumentalists: Bowed
Instrument Group) (Moscow: n.p., 1932).
52. A literal translation of Yankelevich’s term is “cushion” but the term “pad” in
this and later instances is used to refer to the variety of cushions and shoulder
rests that are placed between the violin and the shoulder.—Translator’s note.
53. Alexei Lvov, Sovety nachinaeshemu igrat’ na skripke (Advice to a Beginner on
Playing the Violin) (St. Petersburg: n.p., 1859).
54. In this case we are not speaking of the shoulder in the anatomical sense of the
word (i.e., the humerus bone), but of the shoulder joint that includes the
clavicle and spatula.
55. Val’ter, Kak uchit’ igre na skripke.
56. Instead of a pad, Yankelevich’s students used a metal shoulder rest covered in
nylon that was slanted toward the shoulder. Unlike a cushion or pad, the
shoulder rest didn’t touch the back of the violin and didn’t affect the sound.—
Original editor’s note.
57. Abraham Yampolsky, “O metode raboty s uchinikami” (On Methods of
Working with studentS), in S. Sapozhnikov, ed., Voprosy skripichnogo
ispolnitel’stva i pedagogiki, sbornik statei (Matters of Violin Performance and
Pedagogy, a Collection of Articles) (Moscow: Muzyka, 1968).
58. Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke.
59. Mozart, Osnovatel’noe skripichnoe uchilishche/perevod s nem.
60. Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit.
61. Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke.
62. Singer and Seifriz, Grosse theoretisch-praktische Violinschule.
63. Joachim and Moser, Violinschule.
64. In this respect we agree with Struve’s view of the role that the saddle joint
plays in positioning the thumb in relation to the other fingers of the right hand
on the bow; Struve, Tipovye formy postanovki ruk u instrumentalistov.
65. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
66. Goby Eberhardt and Gustav Saenger, My System for Practising the Violin and
Piano Based upon Psycho-Physiological Principles (New York: Fischer, 1906).
67. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
68. Nemirovskii, Mekhanicheskie i psikhologicheskie momenty v osnovnykh
priemakh skripichnoi tekhniki.
69. Narcisse Augustin Lefort, Méthode complète de violon (Paris: E. Leduc, P.
Bertrand et Cie, 1910).
70. Campagnoli, Neue Methode der fortschreitenden Fingerfertigkeit; Spohr,
Violinschule.
71. Mikhailovsky, Novii put’ skripacha.
72. Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry.
73. Nemirovsky, Mekhanicheskie i psikhologicheskie momenty v osnovnykh
priemakh skripichnoi tekhniki.
74. Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke; Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
75. Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke, 5:15.
76. Ivan Nazarov, “Psikhofizicheskii metod dostizheniia i sovershenstvovaniia
muzykal’noi tekhniki,” (Psychophysical Method of Attaining and Perfecting
Musical Technique), diss., Leningrad, 1946.
77. Joachim and Moser, Violinschule.
78. Mikhailovsky, Novii put’ skripacha.
79. Kayser, Neueste Methode des Violinspiels, op. 32 Th. 3.
80. Valter, Kak uchit’ igre na skripke.
81. See n. 38.
82. Mostras, Intonatsiia na skripke.
83. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
84. Alard, Polnaia shkola dlia skripki.
85. Bekker, Tekhnika i estetika igry na violoncheli.
86. The transcripts of the oscillographs were done by O. E. Sakhaltueva, an
employee of the acoustic laboratory of the Moscow Tchaikovsky Conservatory.
87. Davidov, Shkola dlia violoncheli.
88. By “higher” and “lower” fingers, Davidov means their placement on the
fingerboard where the second finger, for example, is higher in relation to the
first finger, the third is higher than the first and second fingers, the first finger
is lower than the second, third, and fourth fingers, etc.
89. Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke.
90. Alard, Polnaia shkola dlia skripki; Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy
skripichnoi igry; Jockisch, Katechismus der Violine und des Violinspiels;
Koeckert, Rationelle Violintechnik; Mikhailovsky, Novii put’ skripacha;
Radmall, “Change of Position”; Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
91. According to Israel Yampolsky’s expression; Yampolsky, Osnovy skripchnoi
applikatury, 108.
92. Spohr, Violinschule.
93. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
94. Lesman, Puti razvitiya skripacha; Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke; Davidov,
Shkola dlia violoncheli.
95. Addendum to Davidov, Shkola dlia violoncheli, 35.
96. Spohr, Violinschule, 71; and Alard, Polnaia shkola dlia skripki.
97. Mostras, Intonatsiia na skripke.
98. Ferdinand David and Waldemar Meyer, Violinschule (Leipzig: Steingräber,
1900).
99. Yampolsky, Osnovy skripchnoi applikatury.
100. Baillot, L’art du violon.
101. Rudolph Kreutzer and Emil Kross, 42 Etüden oder Capricen (Mainz: Schott,
1884).
102. Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke.
103. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
104. Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry.
105. This applies not only to tenths, but also to all other instances when the fingers
are extended (fingered octaves, etc.)
106. Auer, Moia shkola igry na skripke.
107. Mostras, Intonatsiia na skripke.
108. Carl Guhr, Paganini’s Kunst die Violine zu spielen (Berlin: B. Schott’s Söhne,
1929).
109. Voiku, Postroenie estestvennoi sistemy skripichnoi igry; Koeckert, Rationelle
Violintechnik.
110. David and Meyer, Violinschule.
111. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
112. Ibid.
113. There is no indicated fingering in the previous editions, which is taken to be a
typographical error. The fingering Yankelevich implies is assumed to alternate
between the first and third positions when ascending without using the open
strings.—Translator’s note.
114. We should stress the necessity of studying scales (not just thirds, but also
fingered octaves and sixths) in the even-numbered positions, something often
overlooked in the course of study.
115. Sergei Korguev, Uprazhneniia v dvoinykh notakh (Exercises in Double Stops)
(Moscow: n.p., 1949).
116. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
117. Georgy Dulov, Sistematicheskii kurs gamm dlia skripki (Systematic Course of
Violin Scales) (Moscow: n.p., 1924).
118. We should note that when this fingering is used in scale passages, the moment
the third and fourth fingers drop, they seem to push away the first and second
fingers. For, as Mostras notes, it is inefficient to keep all four fingers on the
string simultaneously.
119. Boris Sibor, Skripichnaya technika dvoinih not (Violin Technique of Double
Stops) (Moscow: n.p., 1928); Korguev, Uprazhneniia v dvoinykh notakh.
120. Korguev, Uprazhneniia v dvoinykh notakh.
121. Mostras, Intonatsiia na skripke, 15.
122. Flesch, Iskusstvo skripichnoi igry.
123. Yampolsky, “O metode raboty s uchinikame.”
124. Lesman, Puti razvitiya skripacha.
125. Joachim and Moser, Violinschule.
126. Ševčík, Shkola skripichnoi tekhniki/perevod s cheshskogo; Voldan, Nová škola
poloh; Lesman, Ob igre na skripke; Lesman, Puti razvitiya skripacha; and
Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke.
127. Mikhailovsky, Novii put’ skripacha, 69.
128. Yampolsky, Osnovy skripchnoi applikatury.
129. Voldan, Nová škola poloh; Ševčík, Shkola skripichnoi tekhniki/perevod s
cheshskogo.
130. Koeckert, Rationelle Violintechnik; Yampolsky, Osnovy skripchnoi
applikatury.
131. Carl Flesch, Etuden-Sammlung für Violine (Copenhagen: Wilhelm Hansen,
1921).
132. Lesman, Shkola igry na skripke.
133. Moser, Methodik des Violinspiels.
Yuri Yankelevich with his father, 1909, and as a young boy.
MAYA GLEZAROVA
Pay attention to the evenness of tone, its quality, the change of bow, and
the gradation of crescendo and diminuendo.
e) Combined détaché and legato (whole bow, upper and lower halves)
Pay attention to the singing quality of the stroke and the active beginning
of each note.
Exercise 4: Exercises for the wrist and fingers. Using Kreutzer Etude No.
11.4
a) fingers + wrist
• Connect at the frog on different strings
• In the middle of the bow
• Starting alternatively and
b) wrist
• legato on two strings
• détaché on two strings at the tip and combination of détaché
and legato at the tip
c) Détaché crossing strings. Using Kreutzer’s Etude No. 1.
• spiccato at the frog in duplets or triplets
• spiccato at the frog skipping over strings (using Kreutzer No. 6).5
When working with young children, Yankelevich would never talk down
to them. He always spoke seriously and never lowered his standards.
In comparing the sound of the violin to the human voice, Yankelevich
sought more than simply a beautiful tone—he sought a tone capable of
expressing specific musical content.
How mistaken were those who referred to Yankelevich’s detailed work on
various skills as “overly technical.” Every scale and etude studied was later
put to the service of music in order to help uncover the artistic meaning of the
composition.
The highest technical mastery is when the artist can so easily overcome the
difficulties that present themselves that his or her thoughts and creativity may be
entirely directed to the musical aspect of performance. Only then does the
listener understand the music not by “how difficult it is” but through the essence
of its content.
YEAR 2
O. Rieding Concerto
A. Komarovsky La Course, or Racing
N. Baklanova Sonatine, Concertino, and Allegro
A. Yanshinov Concertino
F. Seitz Concerto No. 1
A.Vivaldi Concerto in A minor
N. Rubenstein Spinning Wheel
E. Jenkinson Dance
L. Alard Nocturne and Serenade
C. Dancla Variations
YEAR 3
G. Hollender Concerto
J. B. Accolay Concerto
G. Viotti Concerto No. 23
C. Bohm Perpetual Motion
A. Yanshinov Spinning Wheel
G. Pergolesi Aria
A. Spendiarov Lullaby
J. Aubert Presto
C. de Bériot Variations No. 1
F. Schubert The Bumblebee
S. Prokofiev Gavotte
J. Haydn Minuet of the Bull
A. Khachaturian Andantino
YEAR 4
J. Mazas Etudes
C. de Bériot Concerto No. 9 (complete)
G. F. Handel Sonata No. 6
R. Glière Romance “By the Brook”
J. S. Bach Concerto in A minor
J. Fiocco Allegro
J. S. Bach Sicilienne
YEAR 5
G. Viotti Concerto No. 22 (complete)
D. Kabalevsky Concerto (movement 1)
J. Matteson Aria
F. Kreisler (Francoeur) Sicilienne and Rigaudon
G. F. Handel Sonata No. 2
H. Vieuxtemps Fantasie et Appassionata
H. Wieniawski Concerto No. 2 (movement 1)
H. Vieuxtemps Concerto No. 2 (movement 1)
P. Sarasate Fantasy on themes from Gounod’s “Faust”
F. Kreisler Grave (in the style of Bach)
F. Ries Perpetual Motion
A. Aleksandrov Aria
L. Daquin The Cuckoo
F. Kreisler (Pugnani) Preludium and Allegro
F. Chopin (Auer) Nocturne
H. Wieniawski Concerto No. 2 (movements 2 and 3)
M. Bruch Concerto No. 1
H. Vieuxtemps Concerto No. 5
L. Boccherini Allegretto
Vieuxtemps Rondino
Certain pieces, such as the concerti and variations by de Bériot, the pieces
and concerti of Vieuxtemps, Moto Perpetuo by Bohm and Ries, and Spohr’s
Concerto No. 9, were thoroughly polished and developed skills such as
different bow strokes and command of the fingerboard to a very high
technical level. At the same time these pieces helped develop the student’s
weaker side—stimulating panache and variety, a lyrical cantilena,
exaggerated dynamics.
NOTES
1. Konstantin Ushinsky (1824–1871) was an esteemed Russian writer and teacher
who developed influential theories on pedagogy.—Translator’s note
2. This and subsequent quotes of Yankelevich were noted by the author during
lessons and conversations.
3. The numbering of the Kreutzer Etudes may vary according to the edition. In
this case Kreutzer Etude No. 1 refers to the Etude in sixteenth notes in C Major,
often listed as No. 2 in current editions.—Translator’s note
4. Etude in sixteenth notes in A Major,often listed as No. 13. See n. 3.
5. Etude in eighth notes in D Major, often listed as No. 7. See n. 3.
6. Maya Glezarova does not provide more specific information on these works,
and unless commonly known otherwise, the English translation of the Russian
title is provided.—Translator’s note
4
Yankelevich’s Methodological System
VLADIMIR GRIGORYEV
(1) Knowing the student, along with his or her characteristics and
capabilities, and finding the correct, individual approach to his or her
development.
(2) Choosing musical and technical repertoire for individual work with the
student.
(3) Providing a psycho-physiological basis for rational violin technique
and systems that lead to professional mastery.
(4) Offering specific methodological directions for concrete problems such
as various expressive devices, bowings, organization of lesson plans,
and so on.
(5) Analyzing problems related to performance, along with showing the
student how to prepare for performances and directing the entire
pedagogical process towards this eventual goal.
And then humorously he added, “In practice I divide students into two
groups: the first I teach, and the second I expel.”
Yankelevich had a special approach to each individual student. He
believed that a comprehensive knowledge of psychology is essential for the
modern pedagogue.
I am often asked about the function of psychology in actual practice, and I
always reply that that it is indispensable. While all people differ psychologically
and physiologically, there are nonetheless certain types who exhibit similar traits.
For example, there are those musicians who are easily excited, but not very
emotionally focused. These students readily progress “outwardly” but not so
easily “inwardly.” It is the teacher’s task to study each student carefully, to
determine all the strong and weak sides, the potential for development, as well as
the limits or “obstacles” along the way. Only then is it possible to build an
effective plan for educating the student. Meanwhile, it is still important to
develop the student’s most individual characteristics.
Yankelevich never believed in deceiving a student with false praise, with the
idea of “stimulating the student.” He maintained that the student must only be
praised when worthy of it and criticized when deserved.
I am against falsely encouraging the student, since this does not give the student
an objective assessment of his or her playing. Approaching the student with
intelligence and tact means being able to instill confidence in the student.
When Yankelevich was preparing a student for a competition, he wouldn’t let
anything go unnoticed and was unforgiving down to the smallest details. But
right before the competition itself, he would change tactics and mainly
emphasize the student’s merits. This gave the student confidence and a sense
of tranquility on stage.
Yankelevich firmly dismissed the division of teachers into those who
taught beginners and “master teachers” (as Carl Flesch did, for example).
Yankelevich believed teachers at the “higher level” to be “disassociated from
how the violinist’s skills are established, from the real ‘kitchen’ where talent
is developed. Conversely, those who only teach beginners, including some
teachers at the Central Music School, do not see the long-term perspective.
For example, they do not envision how the techniques, bowings, and so forth
that they teach will be used in, say, the Brahms concerto. This kind of rift is
detrimental to both groups.”
In this regard, he also made another point. Teachers of beginners often
tend to hurry, trying to do more than they can and should. They assign
repertoire that is too difficult in an attempt to quickly “move” the student.
I am in favor of a rather slow pace when beginning the violin. The technical
skills need to be acquired gradually, otherwise quality suffers and the hands
become tense. Not everyone understands this. For example, one of my students,
eleven-year-old Pavel Kogan, was performing Kreisler’s Preludium and Allegro
in Leningrad. Some reproached me that at his age he should already be playing
the Mendelssohn concerto. I replied that while it would be difficult for me to
argue with everyone now, let us all see what he will be playing five years down
the road!
Regarding the first point of support, between the jaw and the collarbone,
Yankelevich noted the efficiency of using a pad or shoulder rest (with
preference for the latter). He believed that without a shoulder rest or pad, it is
usually very difficult to support the violin when the left hand is moving. The
shoulder rest facilitates the movements of the left hand along the fingerboard
and relieves the need to lift the shoulder, which causes tension in the hands:
I observed one of my students who managed to even tense his leg when playing
extensions! This is why I start students with a shoulder rest (a cushion may
dampen the sound). If someone who is already in command of a solid technique
wants to play without one (especially in the case of a short neck or high
shoulders) I don’t mind, although I always monitor that the shoulder doesn’t lift
or turn.
The second point of support is in the left hand. Here the difficulty is
twofold for the violin needs to be both held and played on at the same time.
Ideally, this should become one unified process.
The main problem in holding the violin is that the neck of the violin is squeezed,
which restricts and interferes with free movement. It is necessary to develop the
psychological sensation that the thumb does not seem to support the violin. This
can relax it to an effective degree and, incidentally, also helps the vibrato. For the
thumb plays an entirely supportive role and its position depends solely on the
other fingers. If the other fingers lie correctly on the strings, then the thumb
settles into its natural position. Most importantly, the thumb should not get in the
way.
In determining the role of the wrist, elbow, and forearm, he believed that
the most important factor is to provide optimal conditions for accurately and
reliably grouping the fingers above the string and placing them correctly. All
components of the arm need to be coordinated in making the preparatory and
anticipatory movements. He said, “The position of the wrist is especially
important, since it should be a natural extension of the forearm.” These
conditions are essential for ensuring technical freedom and developing the
correct sensations of movement.
According to Yankelevich, the correct setup should stem from a basic
positioning of the wrist and fingers in relation to the fingerboard.
Beginners often have problems with the fourth finger—the pinkie. It is difficult
for them to play a simple tetrachord because of the fourth finger. In this case
many teachers recommend extending it. However, it is better to draw back the
index finger than to stretch forward the little finger. One should begin by
adjusting the hand to the third and fourth fingers—this is the correct position to
start from.
Beginners were recommended to use the following setup of the left hand in
first position. The wrist would be situated slightly higher than the first
position, somewhere between the first and second position, and then it would
draw backward to accommodate the first two fingers. This type of setup not
only makes the little finger more comfortable but also releases tension in the
wrist.
This setup also ensures that the wrist does not bend too far in the opposite
direction, which is sometimes observed in practice. This is also the setup
advocated by Auer. The slight bend in the wrist, which naturally should not
be exaggerated, helps in developing a good vibrato and keeps the fingers
grouped correctly above the string. Yankelevich said, “The fingers must be
grouped at the interval of a fourth, and retain that placement so they may
drop when needed to the corresponding note in any given position. This is
especially important in fast passagework.”
Yankelevich believed one of the essential problems of violin playing,
affecting both technical agility and tone quality, is the appropriate degree of
finger pressure on the string. He noted that many violin methods and treatises
recommend not only applying a lot of pressure to the string but also
developing strength in the fingers, even “hammering” the fingers on the
fingerboard. He found it paradoxical to recommend that the fingers press
strongly while at the same time suggesting that the thumb should touch the
neck very gently. Strong finger pressure leads to “clenching the fingerboard,
since every action has an equal reaction. This instinctive clutching reflex,
which has been problematic throughout the history of violin playing, needs to
be eliminated. Instead, a professional reflex that is based on the optimal
pressure of the fingers needs to be developed.”
Yankelevich believed the clutching reflex to be reinforced by spending too
much time in first position when the beginner’s hand is “fixed” in a static
position. Later, when learning to shift, the student is surprised to discover
“that the hand must move along the fingerboard. Not only are the student’s
psycho-physiological perceptions disrupted, but a whole series of difficulties
ensue. Firstly, the student, who has already become used to holding the violin
with the left hand, finds it now needs to release in order to move along the
fingerboard. Then it also turns out the violin needs to be supported at the
shoulder, and the student is not used to that. Consequently, the student is
confronted with an entirely new set of questions and problems because of the
need to move the hand along the fingerboard. I believe it is important to find
a different setup at the beginning so these types of problems do not arise.”
Yankelevich considered it incorrect to simply avoid the clutching reflex by
entirely changing the setup. He was critical of Campagnoli’s suggestion to
bring the left elbow to the middle of the chest so that the violin neck can rest
on the thumb: “Ease in one spot here unavoidably creates tension in other
places. The elbow needs to be brought out as little as possible and remain as
close as it can to its natural position.” He rejected Mikhailovsky’s theory of
holding the violin neck in the hollow between the thumb and index finger. He
was also skeptical of Voiku’s recommendation to position the violin on the
base of the index finger, since in this case one unnatural position replaces
another and the setup restricts the development of the professional techniques
that are required later.
He pointed out that Voiku was incorrect in considering the thumb to be
“problematic” and believed he underestimated its role. At the same time he
agreed with Voiku in the need to bring the left elbow back. “It is better to
draw the elbow back than to bring it forward, for the latter is the more
unnatural position.”
To determine the correct amount of finger pressure, Yankelevich proposed
the following solution:
A string that is not pressed firmly enough creates a fuzzy and distorted sound. By
gradually increasing the pressure, we arrive at a sound that is clean and has
quality. Here it is important to stop. Further pressure causes the sound to get
worse, the finger technique suffers, and tension arises that leads to jerky shifts
and a tight sound. Tension is always the result of excess energy.
TONE PRODUCTION
Yankelevich also noted that when the bow is drawn entirely flat along the
string, the sound becomes hoarse and the tone quality suffers.
Yankelevich often lamented that many modern violinists have lost the
culture of playing cantabile. This is why some players avoid long legato and
frequently change the bow. “In my opinion this is a mistake. One cannot
deprive the violin of its most valuable asset—its lyricism. For of all the
instruments, it has the greatest ability to sing.” He believed a singing tone to
be associated, in part, with the ability to draw the bow slowly. He
recommended a series of exercises to help develop the so-called everlasting
bow. He advised starting these exercises in piano, drawing the bow for eight
quarter-notes per direction and gradually increasing to twenty quarter-notes.
“Some violinists are able to keep the bow moving for one minute. However,
it is important to produce a sound that has quality and not just engage in
gymnastics.”
After achieving a quality piano, one can move on to playing forte.
Yankelevich recalled Auer’s important observation that a large sound
requires mastery and skill. He believed that the strength of the sound depends
on the correct pressure of the bow on the string. To achieve a rich, full,
unforced tone the hand must retain the same elasticity and freedom developed
when playing piano. Therefore the bow must be held freely without the
fingers ever tensing. The index and middle fingers transfer the minimal
amount of necessary pressure from the shoulder. In fortissimo, the bow must
be held extremely lightly with the fingers, as if it were just a pencil, in order
to feel the string’s response. Here it is important to also note Flesch’s
observation on sensing the distance to the bridge. At the frog there is more
support from the little finger, while at the tip of the bow there is more
pressure on the index finger. However, this feeling of pressure in the hand is
purely internal, something similar to how pianists play “from the shoulder.”
Most importantly, one needs to maintain the equilibrium between density and
power in the sound.
The next step is to change the dynamics from forte to piano and vice versa
and then intensify and release the sound in the middle .
Aside from leading to a mastery of cantilena, these exercises develop a “big
breath” and the skills of messa di voce, making the sound supple and vocal.
These exercises are excellent material for working on unnoticeable bow changes.
Although they are very useful, for some reason they are not so common in
practice. This is surprising, since it is the right hand that is responsible for the
artistic component by creating emotional expression and musical meaning. When
working on tone quality it is important the student forms an idea of a “good”
sound and knows what to aim for. The violinist should be able to hear and
analyze not only his or her own sound but the sound of others as well. The
violinist should never be self-content, and from the very beginning the teacher
should instill the desire to produce the best possible sound.
Very often players make a kind of “banal accent,” giving a long note an
accent at the beginning similar to playing martelé. Yankelevich considered
this to be “a classical antiquated mistake, which many turn into dogma.” He
explained:
They recommend placing the bow on the string, “pinching” it and then
continuing the sound with a marked stop. However, in practice the effect does
not work and extraneous sounds occur. An energetic accent happens not from
“pinching” the note but solely from the energetic movement of the bow using
equal pressure. Martelé is developed from détaché, in which pressure and
movement occur at the same time. The pressure must be constant in all parts of
the bow, otherwise the string will not resonate.
Yankelevich noted that the common effect of the bow shaking when
pressed strongly to the string is explained by the performer “not sensing the
string and feeling its resilience,” and also because the hand does not release
its tension at the tip of the bow.
Strong accents are especially difficult. Yankelevich used the beginning of
the second movement of Handel’s Sonata No. 4 as an example:
This requires lightning-like precision between placing the bow on the string and
moving it. When executed correctly the accent should use half the bow! The
difficulty lies in making the accent from the air, to be in command of the bow
and to be able to start the note both from the string and from above the string.
The hand must make a circular movement above the string, which changes
depending on whether the accent is on a down-bow or an up-bow. This type of
exercise also helps develop freedom and energy in the right hand.
He noted that basic muscle training is still useful for violinists, though
“naturally, not the same as for a ballet dancer.”
The instrumentalist acquires many skills that need to be maintained, and in this
respect scales can be very useful. But it is important to initially work out the
skills separately, in isolation, and then bring them together in a scale. This is
much more productive. The scale should not be turned into a goal itself.
The last stage is the highest. It reflects an inner understanding of sound and
movement. This stage is similar to the so-called autogenic training used in
psychology. Yankelevich always insisted on developing the student’s inner
perceptions.
By just looking at the score it is possible and necessary to learn to imagine the
music and “pre-feel” the movements. This is very important. For example, when
preparing a long program for a concert or competition, when it is not possible
(and not recommended) to play excessively, it is better to sit on a bench in a park
and imagine the music and pre-feel the movements. It is also possible to learn a
new piece this way, with only the eyes and without the instrument.
INTONATION
“The process of playing in tune is both complicated and, at the same time,
relatively simple,” Yankelevich would say.
Mostras wrote a lot about how problems in positioning and technique affect
intonation. But this is not what is most important. Looking at the issue in its
entirety, to play the violin in tune it is first necessary to possess a sufficiently
musical ear. Secondly, the ear needs to be developed and be active. Thirdly, the
ear needs to be coordinated with the physical movements, that is, a “reflex for
distance” needs to be developed in relation to aural perceptions. And fourthly, the
technique of the movement needs to be correct.
VIBRATO
Yankelevich liked to joke that, “it is harder to ruin vibrato than to establish
it.” He noted that, from a psychological perspective, vibrato is one of the
most complicated and difficult problems in violin playing. He explained this
firstly by the fact that the speed and small amplitude of a vibrating movement
make analysis and external control very difficult. And secondly, the player’s
vibrato turns into a subconscious habit, making it very difficult to
comprehend. Serious obstacles arise in controlling the vibrato and musically
modifying its parameters. It becomes difficult for the student not only to
change the vibrato movement but also to stop vibrating in those places he or
she is used to.
According to Yankelevich, the specific nature of vibrato necessitates a
special approach in the early stages of developing it. All the conditions, both
technical and musical, must be in place so that vibrato emerges naturally,
without constraint. “What is meant by ‘naturally'?” Yankelevich would ask.
It means that the teacher must foresee the difficulties that the violinist’s left hand
will encounter and envision the condition of the hand, the positioning of the
fingers, and so on. If the hand is relaxed and the fingers are flexible there should
be no obstacles to vibrato emerging, since to a certain extent this is an automatic
and natural process. When the student tries to imitate the teacher or other
students who can already vibrate, then the hand will naturally be able to vibrate
as long as it is not restricted. Consequently, special exercises are unnecessary.
Examining the different types of vibrato, beginning with the two main
types—the wrist and the arm vibrato—Yankelevich favored the wrist vibrato
from both an artistic and a technical point of view.4 “I am more inclined to
the wrist vibrato, for the arm vibrato entails a large expense of muscular
energy. A constant arm vibrato may sometimes even affect the player’s
technical development. The wrist vibrato allows more possibilities for
variation than the arm vibrato, which is generally more standardized and
uniform.”
At the same time he explained that it is relatively rare to encounter both
types of vibrato in their pure state; usually they are combined, or transition
from one type to another. This is preferable and produces quite an artistic
effect. “Some violinists use one form of vibrato in the higher positions and
another in the lower positions. Heifetz employed both types of vibrato.
Oistrakh’s vibrato with his index finger was even circular in nature.” The
transition from one type of vibrato to another has to do primarily with the
degree of “cohesion” in the joints. In an arm vibrato, the wrist and shoulder
are fixed, and in the wrist vibrato the shoulder and forearm are fixed.
From a technical perspective, he believed the initial impulse to be more
important than the hand’s movement:
The impulse may come from the wrist, the elbow, or a mixture of the two. But it
is always the finger that transfers the impulse to the string. Therefore it is
essential that the joints in the finger are free and the hand is flexible and relaxed.
The finger cannot press too firmly to the string, since this restricts the freedom of
the oscillating movement. The feeling of flexibility and suppleness needs to also
occur throughout the other parts of the arm, for the smallest bit of tension
anywhere is immediately reflected in the vibrato.
The first group was already discussed earlier in the section on tone
production, so let us only address the matter of legato. Yankelevich would
always stress that legato is not simply what happens when the bow glides
over the string while the left hand plays several notes. “Legato is also a bow
stroke,” he would say, “and as a stroke it has its own beginning, middle, and
end.” In his opinion the main difficulty of legato consists of uniting the
lyrical character of the sound in general with the lyrical character and
roundness of each individual note. Thus, one cannot just rely on the right
hand since “a lyrical legato requires both hands to be active.”
When evaluating a violinist’s playing, Yankelevich would first pay
attention to tone production and the expressivity of legato:
We have, to a great degree, lost the art of this stroke. A smooth legato is a color.
Cantilena, a long melodic line—these are the violin’s strengths. The charm of
legato must not disappear.
SELECTING REPERTOIRE
At a certain stage Yankelevich believed one can and should assign many
classical works. The included Martini’s Gavotte, Lully’s Gavotte, Haydn’s
Minuet, and so on as well as older Italian works of Corelli, Tartini, Vivaldi,
Geminiani, and Torelli. But these should be assigned “only when the student
is ready for them and can play them properly from an artistic point of view.
In order to arrive at a true understanding of both classical and contemporary
music, the student’s instrumental mastery needs to be developed by assigning
pieces appropriate to the student’s age and taking into account the student’s
intellectual and emotional understanding.”
In the more advanced classes, Yankelevich found the works of Ernst to be
especially beneficial for technique. “In terms of motor skills, they help more
than works by Paganini.” Yankelevich recommended first learning Ernst’s
Othello Fantasy and Hungarian Airs and then his concerto. In response to
comments that Ernst’s compositions are musically lacking, he always replied,
“There is no bad music, only those unable to find the music in these works.”
He recalled the words of Auer who told one student who resisted playing
Ernst’s Othello: “You will keep playing this piece until you are able to make
good music out of it!”
In the more advanced classes the repertoire was different, and included
Mozart, Beethoven (except for the concerto), and Bach. The latter can be
studied “once the student has a certain degree of skill and is able to perceive
the depth of these works,” Yankelevich said. “It is most important that the
teacher is very exigent in maintaining high artistic standards. The more
complicated contemporary and classical works need to be approached
gradually. Only by mastering the right tools for expression and by becoming
musically, and no less importantly, generally educated, do the doors open for
the violinist.” He would affirm that “two concerti by Vieuxtemps and two
sonatas by Mozart are enough to develop both technique and musicality.”
But just choosing the correct repertoire is not enough, for every drop must
be “extracted” from the piece so it has the maximum benefit. This is why the
teacher must know precisely what may be “extracted” from any given piece,
as well as from the student playing the piece. The Kreutzer etudes are a good
example. They may be used “to improve the quality of bow strokes,
intonation, tone, tempo, and so on. In terms of improving technique Kreutzer
gives just as much as Dont, and possibly even more in terms of quality. The
Kreutzer etudes, followed by Rode and Dont, provide an essential technical
foundation. They must be covered very thoroughly, always maintaining a
high level of quality. Later, all the other etudes may be played such as
Fiorillo, Dancla, and so forth. For developing right-hand technique (the wrist,
various bowings), one can take two or three etudes by Gavigné, as well as
certain etudes of Schradieck and Rovelli.” In the beginning classes he
recommended etudes by Kayser and Mazas (especially for those needing
extra musical development), followed by the “baby” Dont etudes.
Yankelevich generally attached a lot of importance to etudes, but he did
not see them as a goal unto themselves. He believed that the more etudes are
covered, the more pieces need to be studied in order to maintain a balance
between technical and musical development. It is also important to know the
extent to which etudes should be perfected and what is a good “average”
number of etudes to be covered. Yankelevich believed that one of the main
problems in pedagogy, aside from overwork, inefficiency on the part of the
teacher, and laziness on the part of the student, is that too few etudes and
pieces are studied in general.
The student’s technique is established through etudes. However, the etudes need
to be learned correctly and, most importantly, with quality. The difference
between a good and bad violinist seems obvious—the good one plays better, the
bad one plays poorly. Continuing this train of thought means a good violinist
creates a beautiful tone, makes good shifts, and so on. Consequently, good
technique, good sound, and good bowings need to be cultivated and developed.
What material to use for this—pieces? Well, in this case everything must be
learned anew, for each piece is unique. Instead, pieces should make use of
technical skills that have already been established and are best acquired through
etudes. A student cannot progress normally without covering at least two or three
etudes per month, with different goals set for each one. This comes to about
twenty-five to thirty etudes per year. Therefore, regular practice should always
include at least two etudes, with two or, even better, three lessons devoted to
each etude.
Yankelevich noted that, “if the student maintains this rhythm of learning, he
or she will be able to assimilate repertoire quickly. This helps develop the
professional skills of being alert and being able to quickly orient oneself in
new repertoire. These are essential qualities for a violinist, for without them
even the best technique cannot be used to its maximum potential.”
The criteria for perfecting a piece or etude are also important. This
includes being able to play from memory, in a concert setting, up to tempo.
“Attempting to bring each piece to absolute perfection can slow the student’s
progress,” Yankelevich said. “It is important to achieve the main goal or
concrete purpose of the etude or composition. For example, Ries’s Moto
Perpetuo is targeted at developing sautillé and coordinating the bow with the
fingers. Once the piece is played in tempo with good intonation, good tone,
and basic phrasing, the goal may be considered accomplished.” He advised
setting aside the more difficult etudes and pieces and returning to these works
later to polish them for performances. The others do not need to be so
thoroughly perfected. He also temporarily “forgave” musical deficiencies if
the student did not yet understand the depth of the piece, although he would
always come back to the piece later since he liked to complete everything he
started.
Yankelevich would always point out the importance of taking into account
the personality and desires of the student.
Some students are easily able to perfect pieces without losing interest, whereas
others quickly “wilt” and lose motivation. The first may be kept on the same
repertoire, while the others need the repertoire to be frequently updated, although
still in line with the same goal. The teacher can, and should to a certain degree,
take into consideration the student’s requests for repertoire, but only if they do
not conflict with the general plan that the teacher must always have in mind.
Yankelevich also did not neglect to continue working with former students
after they started concertizing: “Any performer is in need of friendly advice.
It is better that this advice comes from a qualified pedagogue, who has
already analyzed him or her better than anyone else.” In giving advice to
concertizing violinists, Yankelevich said: “One should be able to play
everything and master every style. It is a mistake that so many players
consider virtuoso pieces by Paganini, Vieuxtemps, Wieniawski, and Kreisler
to be ‘superficial.’ These pieces not only provide a means of perfecting violin
mastery but also the opportunity to showcase it.”
He observed many competitions in which a violinist was able to perform
the classics with quite good taste and understanding, but turned out to be
helpless in the virtuoso-romantic works, which are an obligatory part of any
competition.
If a violinist is a true artist, he or she must be versatile and, just like an actor, be
capable of transforming from drama to vaudeville! The interpretation of a subtle
and charming short piece by Kreisler requires its own particular approach. There
may only be three or four phrases, but each note, each turn, assumes a paramount
importance. Today, the art of playing short pieces is being forgotten, and yet
nothing opens a violinist more than playing these kinds of works. They not only
condition musicality but also develop the ability to express more profound and
powerful sentiments.
WORKING ON REPERTOIRE
The second stage is to analyze the piece, including its content, style, and
genre. This stage should begin when the piece is initially being discovered,
but not by playing it on the violin as is usually done. Yankelevich suggested
listening to a good recording, but never more than twice to avoid any risk of
copying it. Even better is to play the piece at the piano, or for more advanced
musicians, to visually read the score and analyze its elements.
If the student has not yet played the piece with instrument in hand, then there are
no customary habits and the student can perceive the piece as music and not just
as another technical exercise. Thus, the composer’s intentions become clearer as
does the character of the piece, which is important in guiding a good musician.
This also makes it easier to imagine how the composer heard his or her own
piece. For the character and emotional structure of the piece, the colors and
nuances, are beyond the limits of transcription, since all that the composer heard
and understood cannot be expressed solely in notes. And yet, one may find a
student already playing a piece from memory without being fully aware of the
composer’s indications.
1. Create an internal idea of the tempo that optimally suits the musical
excerpt, taking into account the genre, style, and so on.
2. Verify the tempo with the instrument, and establish the metronome
marking.
3. Play something else, then return to the excerpt and compare the tempo
with the metronome marking. Do this a few times, trying to accurately
match the tempo.
4. Try to play the entire piece through and maintain the tempo, if possible
with a recording device. Go back and check the tempos, taking into
consideration the entire form.
Yankelevich found this method helps to learn how to accurately return to the
tempo relatively quickly.
Another important means of creating musical meaning is phrasing. In his
opinion, work on expressive phrasing should begin only when the style and
general unified contour of the piece is perfectly clear. At the same time,
purely intellectual work is not always enough since intuition plays a huge
role in phrasing. This is what differentiates the “artist” violinist from the
“artisan” violinist. Intellect alone is not enough, for talent arises from
intuition. Therefore it is necessary to combine intuition with understanding.
In the final stage of unifying a piece, it becomes necessary to polish and
correct one’s own conception. Yankelevich advised returning to listen to a
variety of recordings but with a critical ear. “It is necessary to determine the
differences in the interpretations, why the performers play differently, what
they are trying to say, and how this is tied to their particular way of playing.
This is how one’s own understanding is enriched, through clearly
understanding the interpretations of other performers.”
And lastly, Yankelevich noted the psychological components of
performing on stage and engaging the audience. He compared the finished
piece to a painting:
A small painting may be perfectly appropriate for a small room, but in a public
space only a poster will do. In order to interest and captivate the audience, it is
important to have a clear idea of the impression one desires to create. Playing
that is fast and technical does not work well in the concert hall; it blurs together
and becomes trivial. The stage requires expansive and expressive playing,
exaggerated accents, a powerful sound, and articulate technical passagework.
Therefore, when playing with piano, it is necessary to make everything more
pronounced and play on a larger scale, but at a slightly slower tempo so that
everything may be controlled. This ensures a feeling of calmness on the stage,
whereas fast playing creates instability.
If the piece is too difficult for the student this will inevitably be reflected in
the grade. However, this is a sign for the teacher that this piece should never
have been assigned in the first place. “This kind of approach increases the
teacher’s responsibility in choosing the student’s repertoire,” Yankelevich
said.
Another criteria in grading should be the frequency of the student’s
performances over the course of the year and the efforts made to perform and
assimilate repertoire. He believed that the yearly grade should take into
consideration all the year’s performances and be lowered if there are too few
of them.
Regarding specific grades, he made the following observations: “a grade of
excellence (‘A’) requires good rhythm, intonation, tone quality,
musicianship, and adherence to the score.7 The absence of any of these
elements must lower the grade, and an ‘A’ must be given only to a good
musician and instrumentalist.” Requiring “artistry” in a performance,
although desirable, is beyond the scope of beginners and should only apply to
exceptional children. However, the standards in this respect should be raised
at the conservatory level.
Meanwhile, the meaning of a grade of “B” or “C” needs to be elevated.
“Often teachers will give a ‘C’ when the student really deserves a ‘D,’ and a
‘B’ is sometimes misconstrued as a complete misfortune. This all results in
‘A’s being handed out a dime a dozen.”
For Yankelevich, performing on stage was a fundamental component to
becoming an artist.
There are talented violinists who appear to be capable of playing brilliantly at the
highest level, but who do not end up fully developing their abilities. For, at a
certain professional stage, the artist’s development is determined by factors that,
to a certain degree, lie beyond the confines of the classroom. In this respect
performing in public is very important, since the stage provides something that
no teacher in a classroom can. If the teacher always treats the student as a
student, he or she will forever remain a student. I often reproach teachers, myself
included, for being too narrow in our judgments. We need to give our students
more trust and creative freedom.
CONCLUSION
In his book Didactica Magna, John Amos Comenius wrote that the
educational process should be “brief, pleasant, and thorough.” Yankelevich
liked to repeat this often, and it became something of a motto for his own
work. For him, “brevity” consisted of finding the optimal path for the
student’s formation and eliminating anything superfluous in the pedagogical
process. All his actions were to the point, never losing sight of the long-term
goal. However, the long-term goal that was so characteristic of his pedagogy
did not just consist of creating a prospective plan for the student. He believed
in foreseeing the student’s entire development, so that the tactical goals did
not eclipse the long-term strategic goals that were much more important.
Yankelevich was always waiting (and waiting impatiently) for the hidden
capabilities of the student to emerge. He believed these to “emerge more
readily in an atmosphere of mutual understanding and friendship that dispels
any inhibitions.” With incredible tact and perseverance he managed to create
a wonderfully creative atmosphere in his classroom, which naturally brought
out the better musical and personal qualities in his students.
Yankelevich proceeded from the very correct and contemporary
understanding of the two-sided nature of the pedagogical process. The
teacher not only acts as a leader by giving suggestions and sharing his or her
experience but also serves as a kind of “objective mirror” for the student.
This way the teacher provides the student with essential objective information
on the state of the student’s apparatus and whether or not it functions to
realize the student’s musical goals. This constant and painstakingly corrective
process of working helps to explain the quick and natural mastery of the
instrument that distinguished so many of Yankelevich’s top students.
Yankelevich clearly understood what fosters a creative atmosphere in the
studio, and he was able to establish the ideal conditions for his concise
remarks to resonate. He believed that vivid and aphoristic comments are
remembered better than narrow instructions that can block the student’s
imagination, and which he always avoided.
Yankelevich always strove to “optimize” the student’s mental state, but
never to the point of overworking it. This applied to both lessons and
performances. He understood that optimal conditions without exertion make
the student more receptive so that the teacher’s comments are absorbed
creatively rather than mechanically. This helps the student to utilize his or her
reserves, take initiative, and establish an individual plan of study, which, in
interacting with the teacher’s plan, creates the ideal fusion that forges the
student’s individual personality. Yankelevich insisted on achieving an
“inverse relationship” between the teacher and the student (something that is
often declared but rarely achieved). This meant not only mutual
understanding and a good “reaction” to comments, but also the emergence of
the creative “I” in the student, which in turn eventually enriches the teacher.
This is probably why Yankelevich wanted his class to have a spectrum of
talent, remarking that the more students differ, the more they influence each
other. “The greater the diversity of talent in the studio, the more successful
the development of all the students.”
Yankelevich considered the “pleasure” of the learning process for the
teacher to lie in gaining new and interesting experiences by nurturing
interesting students. For the student it lies in generating interest not just in the
final goal but also in the creative learning process itself. He believed any
curtailing of the student’s creative capabilities inevitably causes boredom.
That is why he believed in and used the method of experimentation:
“Achieving a difficult goal that initially seemed impossible is a motivating
stimulus that helps the student progress faster. The student starts to believe in
him- or herself and trust the teacher.”
Yankelevich helped his students to remove any obstacles in their way; he
eliminated tension, made their playing more dynamic, and accelerated the
process of learning new repertoire. He always saw a connection between the
amount of material covered in the lessons (without compromising quality),
between “the stream of music that flows through the student” and the
development of the student’s talent and abilities. “This is how to nurture an
artist, for this develops memory, sight-reading, and the ability to quickly
master new repertoire.”
And finally, the thoroughness of his teaching was evident through his
constant research into contemporary practices and his compilation of the
experience of the leading pedagogues. He was able to discover and determine
all the elements of the playing process, be they mechanical-acoustic,
physiological, psychological, or aesthetic-artistic. He not only determined
them but also linked them together by understanding their interaction, and
then used this insight in his practice.
It was this multifaceted vision, this profound knowledge of the pedagogical
process, which caused Yankelevich to abandon formulaic systems in favor of
wider synthesis. This helped him to be extremely accurate and precise in his
corrections, even in the most difficult cases. He enabled students by giving
them complete information on their own abilities and future paths, which
endowed them with a high degree of self-control. He developed remarkable
cohesion of all the links in the pedagogical process and the methodological
principles he recommended. This cohesion stemmed from his accurate
understanding of the parts and the whole, both their differentiation and their
hierarchy. In any part Yankelevich could see the whole, and in the whole he
saw the individual components. This explains the solid technical foundation
of his students and also his ability to delegate work between the teacher and
the assistants. Yankelevich’s assistants were always allowed a lot of
independence in solving specific problems, which were based on the main
goals set forth by the teacher.
In conclusion, it should be added that Yankelevich continued serious work
with his students even after they became concertizing artists. He developed a
transitional program to move them from student to independent creative
artist. His experience with all aspects of violin pedagogy, from the beginning
child to the recognized artist, allowed him to see all that was new—all that
was replacing the old traditions, methods, and dogmas.
Many teachers are guilty of resisting all that is unusual and different from their
customary established ideas. This kind of inert thinking does not allow them to
see what is new. Their students continue to play the theme of Bach’s Chaconne
entirely in chords, they don’t want to take a look at the urtext score and discover
Mozart’s original bowings … they stubbornly cling to the past.
Yankelevich was an innovator, and his work was at the heart of the battle
to modernize violin teaching and methodology. Through his relentless,
gargantuan efforts he contributed enormously to the Russian violin school
and developed valuable traditions that will nurture more than one generation
of violinists to come.
NOTES
1. As already noted in the Introduction, it is important not to confuse Abraham
Yampolsky with his nephew, Israel Yampolsky (the author of Principles of
Violin Fingering). Throughout this text the single name “Yampolsky” will refer
only to Abraham Yampolsky.—Translator’s note.
2. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849–1936) was a renowned Russian physiologist,
psychologist, and physicist largely known for his work on conditioned reflexes.
—Translator’s note.
3. Nikolai Garbuzov, Zonnaia priroda zvukovysotnogo slukha (Natural Zones of
High-Frequency Hearing) (Moscow: Izd-vo Akademii nauk SSSR, 1948).
4. “Wrist” vibrato is also known as “hand” vibrato.—Translator’s note.
5. Etudes in different editions of the Kreutzer etudes are numbered differently. In
this case, Kreutzer No. 1 refers to the etude in sixteenth notes in C major (often
listed as Kreutzer No. 2) and Kreutzer No. 11(often listed as No. 13) refers to
the etude in sixteenth notes in A major.—Translator’s note.
6. See n. 5.
7. In the original text Grigoryev designates the grades according to the Russian
numerical system, with 5 being the highest and 1 the lowest.—Translator’s
note.
APPENDIX A
Below is the list of Yankelevich’s students taken from the latest Russian
edition of Pedagogicheskoe Nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy).1 Further
biographical information on Yankelevich’s students, including select
interviews and essays, are available on the companion website to this book
www.oup.com/us/therussianviolinschool.
Ruben Aharonyan
Levon Ambartsumian
Felix Andrievsky
Boris Belkin
Mikhail Bezverkhni
Irina Bochkova
Alexandre Brussilovsky
Eugenia Chugaeva
Lydia Dubrovskaya
Arcady Futer
Boris Garlitsky
Alexander Gelfat
Tatiana Grindenko
Ilya Grubert
Lina Guberman
Vladimir Ivanov
Pavel Kogan
Mikhail Kopelman
Bogodar Kotorovych
Vera Kramarova
Vladimir Landsman
Lev Markiz
Albert Markov
Irina Medvedeva
Anatoliy Melnikov
Gayane Pogosova
Anna Rosnovskaya-Leikina
Zare Saakyants
Sergey Sapozhnikov
Dora Schwarzberg
Nelli Shkolnikova
Lidia Shutko
Dmitry Sitkovetsky
Valery Slutsky
Evgeny Smirnov
Vladimir Spivakov
Sigvard Stenberg
Victor Tretyakov
Valeria Vilker-Kushment
Gregory Zhislin
Valery Zvonov
NOTE
1. Yuri Yankelevich, Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy), 4th ed.
(Moscow: Muzyka, 2009).
APPENDIX B
Problema zvuchaniia v skripichnoi igre: Analiz raboty Karla Flesha (The Problem of
Tone in Violin Playing: An Analysis of the Work of Carl Flesch). Manuscript,
1932.1
Pravaia ruka skripacha i rabota nad shtrikhami (The Right Arm of the Violinist and
Work on Bow-Strokes). Manuscript, 1940.
Tekhnika levoi ruki skripacha (Left-Hand Technique of the Violinist). Manuscript,
1940.
O vibratsii (On Vibrato). Manuscript, 1940.
Nekotorye cherty metoda prof. Iampol’skogo (Certain Aspects of the Method of Prof.
Yampolsky). Manuscript, 1951.
Ostsilograficheskii analiz smen pozitsii (An Oscillographic Analysis of Shifting
Positions). Manuscript, 1952.
Smeny pozitsii, priyomy ikh vypolneniia i vospitanie sootvetsvuiushchikh navykov
(Shifting: Means of Execution and the Cultivation of Corresponding Skills).
Manuscript, 1952.
O metodah ovladeniia pozitsiiami (On Methods of Mastering the Positions).
Manuscript, 1955.
Smeny pozitsii, v sviazi s zadachami khudozhestvennogo ispolneniia na skripke; opyt
obobshcheniia nekotorykh polozhenii sovetskoi skripichnoi shkoly (Shifting
Positions in Conjunction with the Musical Goals of the Violinist; Experience
Reflecting Certain Views of the Soviet Violin School). Doctoral dissertation, 1955.
Voprosy pervonachalnoi postanovki ruk skripacha (Matters of Initially Positioning the
Violinist’s Hands and Arms). Manuscript, 1956.
O metodakh A. I. Iampol’skogo v formirovanii skripacha muzykanta (On Yampolsky’s
Methods in Developing a Musician-Violinist). Manuscript, 1957.
Ob intonatsii (On Intonation). Lecture at the Moscow Conservatory, October 29.
Manuscript, 1958.
Lecture at the conference of the orchestral faculty at the Moscow Conservatory
November 28th, 1958. Manuscript.
Lecture at the 3rd conference of the association of Russian pedagogue-musicians
March 25th, 1959. Stenogram.
Lecture at the conference dedicated to the technical development of the violinist
Manuscript, 1960.
Cycle of lectures at the Moscow Conservatory andGnessinInstitute: On Vibrato; On
Notation; System of Intervals; Temperament. Manuscript.
“Smena Pozitsii” v sbornike Ocherki po metodike obucheniia igre na skripke
(“Changes of Position” in Essays on Violin Pedagogy) Moscow, 1960.
“O pervonachalnoi postanovke skripacha” v sbornike Voprosi skripichnogo
ispolnitelstva i pedagogiki (“On the initial positioning of the violinist” in Matters of
Violin Performance and Pedagogy) Moscow, 1968.
Konkurs, problemy i opyt (Competitions; problems and experience) Sovetskaya
kultura, No 5, 1970.
Na muzikalnykh seminarakh v Yaponii I GDR (At musical seminars in Japan and the
German Democratic Republic) Masterstvo muzikanta-ispolnitelya, Moscow, 1972.
NOTE
1. This is a reproduction of the list published in Yuri Yankelevich,
Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy), 4th ed. (Moscow: Muzyka,
2009).
APPENDIX C
Published Compositions
Unpublished Compositions
NOTE
1. This is a reproduction of the list published in Yuri Yankelevich,
Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical Legacy), 4th ed. (Moscow: Muzyka,
2009); complete reference information for compositions is sometimes missing.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
NOTE
1. The references provided here are largely based on the list compiled in
Yankelevich, Yuri (Iankelevich, Iurii). Pedagogicheskoe nasledie (Pedagogical
Legacy). 4th ed. Moscow: Muzyka, 2009. In certain cases complete
bibliographic information is unavailable.
INDEX
Bach, Johann Sebastian, 24, 73, 105–106, 153–154, 173, 187, 210–211, 218, 220, 232
Baiburov, Kayum, 136
Bekker, Hugo, 25, 50, 73
Bériot, Charles de, 44, 57, 156n12, 169, 173, 218–219
Berlin, Anisim, 5, 11n7
bow
angle of, 24, 189–190, 196–198, 215
change in direction of, 25–26, 147–149, 153, 164, 197, 214
coordination with left hand, 143–154, 216, 221
drawing of, 24–26
holding of, 20–24, 167, 190–191 (see also Russian bow hold; positioning)
movement of, in relation to shifting, 70, 80, 93, 103, 107–108, 122, 129, 130–131,
136, 140–154
placement in relation to bridge, 20, 115, 143, 189, 196–197 (see also sounding
point)
practicing without using, 203
pressure of, 51, 142, 143, 144, 147, 150, 152, 195–199
speed of, 143, 144, 150
See also bowings
bow strokes. See bowings
bowings
in Bach, 211
affecting chromatic scale, 119–120
détaché, 23, 145, 152, 163–165, 199, 202, 211–215
general work on and exercises for, 163–165, 169–170, 211–216
affecting left hand, 143
legato 39, 70, 108, 150, 197, 212
martelé, 152, 165, 199, 213–214
ricochet, 152, 165, 215
sautillé, 215, 221
spiccato, 144, 152, 165, 211, 213, 214–215
staccato, 133, 152, 165, 214
See also chords
Joachim, Joseph, 14, 15, 30, 44, 48, 57, 70, 94, 135, 147
Jockisch, Reinhold, 51, 60, 81
Karbulka, Josef, 2
Kayser, Heinrich Ernst, 48, 50, 70, 220
Khandoshkin, Ivan, 10n1
Koeckert, Gustave, 15, 38, 52, 57, 81, 128, 129, 148
Kogan, Leonid, 4, 11n2, 18, 19, 22
Korguyev, Sergei, 2, 5, 134, 139, 141, 170
Kresiler, Fritz, 24, 55, 81, 185, 222
Kreutzer, Rudolph
treatise of, 47,
etudes, means of studying, 5, 164–165, 170, 176n3, 212–213, 216, 220
Marak, Jan, 42
martélé. See bowings
mental control, 5, 179, 203–204, 228, 231. See also psycho-physiological approach
Mikhailovsky, B., 14, 47, 53, 62, 70, 81, 147, 194
Młynarski, Emil, 2
Moscow Conservatory, 1–4, 162, 175, 177–178
Moscow Violin School. See Russian Violin School
Mostras, Konstantin
as representative of Moscow violin school and influence on Yankelevich, 2, 4–5
on coordination of hands, 143
exercise for developing preparatory bow movement, 25
exercise for integrated movement in passage work, 67–68
on finger pressure, 51
on guiding finger in octaves, 120
on guiding finger in tenths, 125
on lifting the shoulder, 55
on open string disrupting passagework, 112
on pre-hearing and pre-feeling, 72
on role of reflexes, 35–37
on use of vibrato in large leaps, 109
Mozart, Leopold, treatise of, 15, 29, 53, 57
Mozart, Wolfgang Amadeus, 211, 218–219, 232
musical expression. See interpretation and imagination, musical development of
Nalbandyan, Ioannes, 2, 5
Nazarov, Ivan, exercises for coordinated movement, 66
Nemirovsky, L., 18, 38, 54, 55, 56, 58, 63, 147
Nopp, Viktor, 42
Yampolsky, Abraham
as representative of Moscow school and influence on Yuri Yankelevich, 2, 4–8,
177, 179, 180, 182
on bow pressure, 191
on chords, 215
edition of Kreutzer etudes, 165, 170
exercise for coordination in passagework, 67–68
exercise for initial attack of note, 213
exercise for developing quick motor reflexes, 146
fingering for passage in Chaconne, 153–154
manner of holding bow, 21
on mental control, 203
method for mastering chromatic glissando, 117
method for shifting across strings in octaves, 122
on performance preparation, 228
use of psychology in pedagogy, 183, 217
questionnaire of, 217
on retaining finger on string, 110
on staccato, 214
on supplementary thumb movements, 56
creating supplementary variations, 200, 224
Yampolsky, Israel, 10, 11n2, 31–32, 113, 147, 148
Yampolsky, clarification of family name, 11n2