The Relationship Between Family Caregivi
The Relationship Between Family Caregivi
The Relationship Between Family Caregivi
Abstract
Caring for dependent relatives has become a normative challenge for families in the USA and
throughout the world. The study objective was to examine the relationship of family caregiving
responsibilities and the mental health and well-being of individuals, ages 18–24 years, referred to
as emerging young adults. It was hypothesized that young adult caregivers with past and present
responsibilities would report significantly more symptoms of depression and anxiety, have lower
self-esteem, and use less adaptive coping styles than non-caregiving peers. The sample consisted of
353 undergraduates (81 past caregivers, 76 current/past caregivers, and 196 non-caregivers).
Caregivers were also evaluated in terms of care recipients, duration of caregiving, tasks, and hours
of effort. Caregivers had significantly higher levels of symptoms of depression and anxiety than
non-caregivers. Research to clarify how caregiving interacts with other stressors in emerging
young adults and influences behavioral health should be a priority.
Introduction
Caring for dependent family members with medical disorders, disabilities, or frailty has become
a normative developmental challenge for families in the USA and throughout the world.1,2 There is
a substantial research literature about the caregiving process, the experience of adult caregivers and
Address correspondence to Donna Cohen, PhD, Department of Child and Family Studies, MHC 2406, College of
Behavioral and Community Sciences, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33612, USA. Phone: 813-974-4665; Email:
[email protected].
Jennifer Greene, MSPH, College of Education, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33620, USA. Phone: 813-449-
4011; Email: [email protected]
Constance Siskowski, RN, PhD, American Association of Caregiving Youth, Boca Raton, FL, USA. Phone: 516-391-
7401; Email: [email protected]
Peter Toyinbo, MBChB, PhD, James A. Haley Veterans Medical Center, HSR&D Center of Innovation on Disability and
Rehabilitation Research (CINDRR), Tampa, FL, USA. Phone: 813-554-7613; Email: [email protected]
)
Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research, 2016. 1–12. c 2016 National Council for Behavioral Health. DOI
10.1007/s11414-016-9526-7
Methods
The research design involved quantitative analyses of a convenience sample of university
undergraduates, ages 18–24 years, who responded to single mass e-mail sent to undergraduates by
the Office of the Registrar with a link to an anonymous online survey. The survey was conducted at
a large, urban, public university in the southern USA in 2009. The study was approved by the
university’s Institutional Review Board.
Participants
The sample consisted of 353 undergraduates in the following three groups.
Relationship Between Family Caregiving and Mental Health
(1) 81 students who were past, i.e., pre-college, caregivers only. Henceforth, this group will be
referred to as past caregivers.
(2) 76 students who had been caregivers in the past and were also current caregivers.
Henceforth, this group will be referred to as current/past caregivers.
(3) 196 students who reported never having been a caregiver. Henceforth, this group will be
referred to as non-caregivers.
Measures
The survey included the following variables for all respondents: demographic information,
identification of a caregiver role, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies of Depression Scale
(CES-D)18, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)26, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
(SES);27, and the Response to Stress Questionnaire (RSQ).28 Respondents who identified as
being and/or having been family caregivers were asked questions about four caregiving
variables: care recipients, duration of caregiving, tasks, and effort.
Demographic information The six items included age, gender, race/ethnicity, participation in
extracurricular activities and clubs, whether they had a part-time job(s), and if so, the number
of hours worked weekly.
Caregiver status Participants were asked to answer questions about current assistance
provided (Do you currently provide assistance to a person who needs special medical care
as a result of an injury, aging, illness, disability, or other health condition?) and past assistance
provided (Did you provide assistance in the past, i.e., before entering college, to a person who
needs special medical care as a result of an injury, aging, illness, disability, or other health
condition?). Participants answering BNo^ to both questions were defined as non-caregivers.
Those who endorsed only providing assistance in the past were defined as past caregivers.
Those who endorsed providing assistance currently and in the past were defined as current/past
caregivers. Seven participants (2% of sample) who reported only being caregivers since
entering college were included in this latter group.
Depression Depression was measured using the 20-item CES-D where respondents are asked
to think about how they felt or behaved in the past week. Items are presented on a scale from
zero to three, with zero indicating Brarely or none of the time (less than 1 day),^ one indicating
Bsome or a little of the time (1–2 days),^ two indicating Boccasionally or a moderate amount
of time (3–4 days),^ and three indicating Bmost or all of the time (5–7 days).^ Scores can
range from zero to 60, with higher scores indicating more depression symptomatology and a
score of 16 or higher indicating a clinical level of depression. The scale has high reliability,
with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.89.18 The scale had high reliability in the current sample, with a
Cronbach’s alpha of 0.92.
Anxiety Anxiety was measured using the 40-item STAI which consists of two 20-item
subscales: state anxiety and trait anxiety. Instructions for the state anxiety inventory are to
think about how the person feels right now, at this moment. For the trait anxiety inventory,
Self-esteem Self-esteem was measured using the 10-item Rosenberg SES. Items are presented on
a scale rated from zero to three, zero indicating Bstrongly disagree,^ one indicating Bdisagree,^ two
indicating Bagree,^ and three indicating Bstrongly agree.^ Scores can range from zero to 30, with
higher scores indicating higher self-esteem. The scale has high reliability with test-retest
correlations ranging from 0.82 to 0.88 and Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.77 to 0.88.30 Studies
have shown unidimensional and two-factor structures for the SES, self-competence and self-
liking.30 The scale had high reliability in the current sample, with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.91
Coping Participants completed six items from the RSQ. The complete 57-item RSQ measures
two broad domains of coping, voluntary vs. involuntary responses and engagement vs.
disengagement strategies, in response to age-appropriate social stressors. Within these two broad
dimensions, the RSQ has five factors (primary control, secondary control, disengagement coping,
involuntary engagement, and involuntary disengagement), and each factor is composed of different
types of coping responses. The RSQ has good reliability, ranging from 0.72 to 0.89,28 as well as
good convergent and divergent validity.31
In this study, three items were used to measures voluntary disengagement, a dimension of
maladaptive coping characterized by wishful thinking (i.e., wishing to be smarter, wishing the
problem would go away, and wishing someone would take the problem away). Three additional
items were used to measure voluntary engaged problem-solving, a dimension of adaptive coping
(i.e., think of ways to deal with or fix the problem, ask others for help or ideas, and try to fix the
situation). Items were rated on the use of a particular coping strategy from one to four, one
indicating Bnot at all,^ two indicating Ba little,^ three indicating Bsome,^ and four indicating Ba
lot.^ Scores can range from six to 24, with higher scores indicating higher frequency of use of
coping behaviors. In the current sample, the three wishful thinking items had moderate reliability,
with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.75, while the problem solving items had a lower reliability, with a
Cronbach’s alpha of 0.56.
Caregiving variables Respondents were asked to identify all care-recipients for whom they had
responsibilities, duration of caregiving, all caregiving tasks, and time spent providing care.
Duration of caregiving was assessed by asking participants to identify the age caregiving
responsibilities began (age 5 and younger, 6 to 10 years, 11 to 15 years, 16 to 20 years, or age 21 to
24 years). A checklist of caregiving tasks included six activities of daily living (ADLs: feeding,
bathing, dressing, toileting, assistance with walking, changing diapers) and eight instrumental
activities of daily living (IADLs: appointments, translating, emotional support, company, cleaning/
laundry, grocery shopping/preparing meals, medical tasks/medical equipment, and medication
management). Time spent was recorded as estimated average hours during a typical school day and
a typical weekend day.
Results
Characteristics of the sample
Participants in all three study groups were predominantly Caucasian females and were, on
average, 21 years old. Table 1 shows that the groups were similar for all sociodemographic
variables except one: there was a significant association between extracurricular engagement and
caregiver status (χ2 (2, N = 353) = 6.04, p G 0.05). Although greater than half of all three groups
reported outside activities, non-caregivers were more likely to engage in extracurricular activities
(67.9%) than past caregivers (58.0%) and current/past caregivers (52.6%). More than half of
participants in the three groups worked 20 or more hours weekly, but the differences were not
significant.
n % n % n %
Gender
Male 36 18.4 17 21.0 17 22.4 0.64 0.73
Female 160 81.6 64 79.0 59 77.6
Age
M (SD) 20.57 (1.65) 20.89 (1.59) 20.79 (1.64) 1.25d 0.29
Race/ethnicity
Caucasian 131 68.2 56 68.3 53 70.7 5.77 0.83
African-American 20 10.4 6 7.3 4 5.3
Latino 23 12.0 11 13.4 13 17.3
Asian 9 4.7 3 3.7 1 1.3
Multiracial 6 3.1 4 4.9 2 2.7
Other 3 1.6 2 2.4 2 2.7
Do you participate in extracurricular activities?
Yes 133 67.9 47 58.0 40 52.6 6.04 0.05
No 63 32.1 33 40.7 36 47.4
No response – – 1 1.2 – –
Do you have a part-time job?
Yes 114 58.2 41 50.6 48 63.2 2.60 0.27
No 82 41.8 40 49.4 28 36.8
If you have a part-time job, how many hours per week do you work?
0–9 18 15.8 5 12.2 4 8.3 8.88 0.35
10–19 38 33.3 13 31.7 17 35.4
20–29 35 30.7 13 31.7 17 35.4
30–40 21 18.4 7 17.1 5 10.4
More than 40 2 1.8 3 7.3 5 10.4
a
n = 196
b
n = 81
c
n = 76
d
One-way ANOVA
Tukey post hoc tests revealed that current/past caregivers had significantly higher scores on the
CES-D (M = 18.80, SD = 12.14) compared to the non-caregiver group (M = 13.81, SD = 10.56).
Moreover, the effect size of this difference was 0.45, a medium size effect. Higher percentages of
past as well as current/past caregivers (43.9 and 46.1%) had clinically significant CES-D scores
(916) as compared to 29.1% of the non-caregiving group.
An ANOVA showed that caregiver status had a significant overall effect on state anxiety as
measured by the STAI, F (2, 347) = 5.563, p = 0.004. Tukey post hoc tests revealed that current/
past caregivers (M = 45.85, SD = 13.68) had significantly higher scores on the STAI state anxiety
scale compared to the non-caregiver group (M = 39.92, SD = 13.34), and the effect size was
medium (d = 0.44). Tukey post hoc tests revealed that current/past caregivers (M = 44.90, SD =
13.51) had significantly higher scores on the STAI trait anxiety scale compared to the non-
Relationship Between Family Caregiving and Mental Health
Table 2
Frequencies of caregiving activities by caregiver status
Type of activity Past caregivers Current/past caregivers χ2 p
(n = 81) (n = 76)
n % n %
caregiver group (M = 40.32, SD = 13.07). The overall effect (small effect size of 0.35) of caregiver
status on trait anxiety was not significant at the conservative alpha level of 0.01, F (2, 342) = 3.363,
p = 0.036.
Outcome M SD M SD M SD d p d p
Depression** 13.81 10.56 15.44 10.52 18.80 12.14 0.16 0.49 0.45 0.00
State anxiety** 39.92 13.34 42.06 12.11 45.85 13.68 0.17 0.43 0.44 0.00
Trait anxiety* 40.32 13.07 42.15 12.23 44.90 13.51 0.14 0.54 0.35 0.03
Self-esteem 22.75 5.95 22.81 5.54 21.86 6.31 0.01 0.10 0.15 0.53
Coping: overall* 15.53 2.55 16.31 2.77 16.13 2.70 0.30 0.07 0.23 0.23
Coping: problem solving
I try to think of different ways to deal with 3.34 0.85 3.60 0.65 3.46 0.83 0.33 0.04 0.14 0.51
or change the problem or fix the situation.*
I ask other people for help or for ideas about 2.90 0.97 2.96 0.91 2.82 1.02 0.06 0.86 0.08 0.82
how to make the problem better.
I do something to try to fix the problem or take 3.43 0.77 3.53 0.74 3.44 0.75 0.13 0.58 0.01 0.10
action to change things.
Problem solving coping subtotal 9.67 1.90 10.10 1.65 9.74 1.93 0.24 0.19 0.04 0.96
Coping: wishful thinking
I wish that I were stronger, smarter, or more 2.07 1.06 2.15 1.06 2.35 1.10 0.08 0.86 0.26 0.15
popular so that things would be different.
I deal with the problem by wishing it would just 1.83 0.97 1.99 1.05 1.89 0.92 0.16 0.46 0.06 0.92
go away, that everything would work itself out.
I just wish someone would come and get me out 1.94 1.00 2.07 1.02 2.15 1.09 0.13 0.60 0.21 0.30
of the mess.
Wishful thinking coping subtotal 5.85 2.47 6.21 2.51 6.39 2.62 0.15 0.52 0.22 0.26
d= Cohen’s d
*p G 0.05, **p G 0.01
and 0.23, respectively, were obtained. Specific adaptive and maladaptive coping skills endorsed by
each group are presented in Table 3.
Discussion
The results of this study confirm the scant literature showing that young adults expend
considerable time and effort caring for dependent family members, and although it is not a
randomized trial, it is the first to use a control group of non-caregiving peers to show an association
between providing care and mental health and well-being.14,33 This age group not only has
significant caregiving responsibilities today but they are also the emerging caregiver cohort of
tomorrow.7 Dellman-Jenkins and Brittain34 identified at least three related family dynamics
contributing to the increasing involvement of this age group: (1) role strain/stress and decreasing
availability of the middle-aged generation to be caregivers, (2) growth of the oldest cohorts and
longer-lived family generations, and (3) a strong filial attachment and sense of responsibility to
provide needed care. Baus and colleagues15 emphasized that caregiving will be a twenty-first
century challenge for college students who will be increasingly involved because of societal aging
trends, especially the tendency of women, ages 20–24 years, to postpone childbearing, and
pressures to assume some of the responsibilities once provided by the burdened middle-aged
sandwich generation.
To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study reporting that young adult caregivers, especially
those who had been caregivers both before and during college, were not only significantly more
depressed and anxious than their non-caregiving peers but also that the levels of symptomatology
in many of them were clinically significant. The comparatively higher indicators of emotional
distress in the caregiving groups suggest that the burden of caregiving coupled with university
pressures and other factors likely makes young adult students more vulnerable to psychiatric
distress. The psychosocial, behavioral, and environmental factors contributing to depression and
anxiety in young adult student caregivers should be a research priority. This group may be at risk
for continued poor mental health and difficulties in ongoing and future educational and
occupational pursuits, as well as overall success in life. It is possible that behavioral health
problems were influenced by the medical conditions and comorbidities affecting respective care-
recipients’ needs as well as available support systems. However, these variables were not included
in the survey.
No significant relationship between caregiver status and self-esteem was demonstrated in this
sample. Self-esteem, a measure of psychological adjustment, has been reported to be lower in some
studies of depressed populations.35 Since self-esteem is defined as an overall evaluation of one’s
competence and is considered to be a rather stable personality trait, it may be that participants in
this study, regardless of caregiving challenges, felt good about themselves and their overall
capabilities as individuals and university students.
It is important to emphasize that the frame of reference this sample used to make their responses
to the coping questions is not known because the RSQ test instructions are to answer each item in
response to the introductory statement, BWhen I have a problem.^ The findings that past and
current/past caregivers employed adaptive coping more often than (though not significantly more
often than) non-caregivers are in contrast to at least one study using the instrument. Pakenham and
colleagues,25 who studied a mixed sample of youth and young adults ages 10–25 years, reported
that caregivers were more likely than non-caregivers to use wishful thinking, a maladaptive coping
style, and less likely to use problem solving coping, an adaptive coping style.
The findings of this study of undergraduates indicate that past and current/past caregivers used
both wishful thinking and problem solving coping styles more often than (though not significantly
more often than) their non-caregiving peers. This may reflect that they are trying to find different
ways to deal with their problems, perhaps using wishful thinking to deal with their emotional
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
1. Bethesda, MD. National Alliance for Caregiving/AARP Public Policy Institute. Caregiving in the US: 2015 Report, 2015. http://
www.aarp.org/content/dam/aarp/ppi/2015/caregiving-in-the-united-states-2015-report-revised.pdf
2. Shahly V, Chatterji S, Gruber MJ, et al. Cross-national differences in the prevalence and correlates of burden among older family
caregivers in the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Psychological Medicine, 2013, 43: 865–879.
3. Cohen D, Eisdorfer C. An integrated textbook of geriatric mental health. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 2011.
4. Gitlin, LN, Schulz R. Family caregiving of older adults. In TR Prohaska, LA Anderson, RH Binstock (Eds.). Public health for an aging
society. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 2012.
5. Roth DL, Fredman L, Haley WE. Informal caregiving and its impact on health: a reappraisal from population-based studies. The
Gerontologist, 2015, 55: 309–319.
6. Schulz R. Current issues in informal caregiving research: prevalence, health effects, and intervention strategies. In H Lavretsky, M
Sajatovic, CF Reynolds III (Eds.). Late-life mood disorders, New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2013, pp. 236-253.
7. Bonnie RJ, Stroud C, Breiner H. Investing in the health and well-being of young adults. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2014.
8. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist 2000; 55(5): 469.
9. Arnett JJ, Tanner JL (Eds). Emerging adults in America: coming of age in the 21st century. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2006.
10. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: the winding road from the late teens through the twenties. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2014.
11. Tanner J, Yabiku S. Conclusion: the economics of young adulthood—one future or two. In: A Booth, AC Crouter, MJ Shanahan (Eds). Transitions
to adulthood in a changing economy: no work, no family, no future, Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group, 1999, pp. 254-268.
12. Becker S, Dearden C. Carers. Research Matters 2004; 18: 11-18.
13. Becker F, Becker S. Young adult carers in the UK: experiences, needs and services for carers aged 16-24. Essex, UK: The Princess Royal
Trust for Carers, 2008.
14. Levine C, Hunt GG, Halper D, et al. Young adult caregivers: a first look at an unstudied population. American Journal of Public Health
2005; 95(11): 2071-2075.