ESM in Critical Event Studies
ESM in Critical Event Studies
ESM in Critical Event Studies
Introduction
Borne of psychological investigation, experiential sampling methods
(ESM) began to come to prominence during the 1980s as a means to
investigate well-being (Schimmack, 2003). ey increased in promi-
nence, largely attributed to the work of Csikszentmihalyi and Larson
(1987) which highlighted research reliability and validity, and become
widely advocated by Csikszentmihalyi in a broad range of publications
(Csikszentmihalyi & Rathunde, 2014; de Vries & Csikszentmihalyi,
2006; Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel, 2007; Kubey & Csikszentmihalyi,
1990). ESM were therefore demonstrated to be able to measure peoples’
experiences; in situ, increasing eco-validity; in real time, minimising recall
bias and over an extended number of days. For this reason it was able
to contribute to the many experiential investigations including, a ect,
mood, behaviours and emotions of experience is signi cant and has been
widely used to investigate an expansive remit of experience(s) including
music in everyday life (Juslin, Liljeström, Väst äll, Barradas, & Silva,
2008), pre-competitive emotions in sport (Cerin, Szabo, & Williams,
2001) and identity (Burke & Franzoi, 1988).
p. 348). ese recorded inner experiences are then expanded upon dur-
ing in-depth interviews which take place within 48 hours of the initial
data collection period.
e application of both ESMs generally, as well as DESs, is broad
and has provided a large library of research into many di erent aspects
and experiences of life. eir use in events is very limited however,
with only one exploratory day-sampling approach (Riddle & Arnold,
2007) being used at a live music performance to provide any primary
research into this subject area (Wood & Moss, 2015). Furthermore,
they have as of yet not been used in a music festival setting. is is
despite being used to investigate other music experiences; for example,
everyday use ( Sloboda, O’Neil, & Ivaldi, 2001; Greasley & Lamont,
2011) and the importance of music in peoples’ lives (Krause, North,
& Hewitt, 2013; O’Neill, 2002). is research method is therefore
well positioned and with a large body of work to support it, as a
means to contribute to the growing investigations into festival experi-
ence. Moreover qualitative ESM/DESs can facilitate the development
of critical event studies by expanding psychological and sociological
investigations which are, at present, in their infancy and are primar-
ily quantitative in nature (Ballantyne & Packer, 2013; Laiho, 2004;
Packer & Ballantyne, 2010).
Critical Realism
Based upon the work of Bhaskar (1978), this research is underpinned
by a philosophical perspective that articulates that the tension between
a realist ontology and a relativist epistemology can provide a critical
contribution. at is to say that the postpositivist lens of natural sci-
ences and the interpretivist lens of social sciences can inform each other
to generate new truths. As Hartwig (2007) o ers, critical realism (CR)
is a social theory and a cognate practice that promotes truth and free-
dom by critiquing both science and other ways of knowing. Taking
this further, Dean, Joseph, and Norrie (2005), p. 7) contend that it
originates in transcendence theory of physical science and in doing so
bridged the gap between positivists and conventionalists.
Against positivisms, Bhaskar conceptualises natural lawfulness in terms
of powers or tendencies, rather than of regular recurrence of speci able
events. Against conventionalisms, he insists that natural things exist inde-
pendently of human theories about them.
It is the removal of lawlessness and thus determinism that places
science in a position to be understood as historico-cultural and pro-
vides CR with perspectivalism but that all perspectives are constrained
by the real nature of objects of study. It is this that grounds the phi-
losophy with an ontological realism and an epistemological relativism
(Collier, 1994 ). However, Bhaskar’s realism argues that while there
are intransitive things that exist independently of human activity,
the moment we study them, they become transitive and saturated
with human activity and knowledge. is means that we can have
true knowledge of real objects but that this knowledge is time and
space speci c and therefore can be replaced in the future. Moreover,
Bhaskar’s relativism is not an ‘anything goes’ (Dean et al., 2005 , p. 8)
perspective but one which admits the possibility and necessity of
truth. is, then, helps overcome the problems inherent within not
recognising how reality a ects our knowledge and thus, avoids the
epistemic fallacy (Gro , 2015 ).
258 J. Moss
False Beliefs
e nal aspect, or cornerstone of CRs contribution to this research,
to be discussed here, are its considerations of false beliefs. Freedom,
for a CR philosophy, is not the freedom written about in other critical
individualistic theories but that it comes with necessary constraints.
13 Experience Sampling Methods in Critical Event Studies … 259
Phenomenology
Looking at this research through a critical realist lens, while also acknowl-
edging its basis in the exploration of experience and therefore within a social
science sphere of enquiry, the research takes an interpretivist, hermeneutic
perspective to this investigation. is again is a step away from the tradi-
tional events research perspective of reductive, positivistic view that aims for
widely generalisable ndings but instead, focuses on the idiographic, subjec-
tive details of the experience. In doing so, it aims to gather rich narratives of
260 J. Moss
the experience that illustrate deep insights into what music festival experi-
ence might be, not what positivistic research has reduced it to.
One may perform cause-e ect analyses with things and one may come up
with universal laws about the behaviour of forces. But one would have to
speak of the unfolding of motivational relations over time with humans and
of the role of meaning in determining the value of human life. Causes and
motivations cannot be lumped together; neither can forces and meanings.
Existential Phenomenology
Phenomenology o ers new and challenging perspectives to events
research and the research in this chapter uses Ashworth’s (2015) adap-
tion of existential phenomenology as its epistemological foundation. is
strand of phenomenology is built upon Husserl’s early twentieth-century
work (1925/1977, 1931/1960) which contests two phenomenological
fundamentals. e rst was to address the disparity between concepts
and experience. Husserl saw that the former was not grounded in the
latter and, therefore, there was a signi cant lack of clarity and appropri-
ateness between the two. is meant that phenomenology had to, as the
Husserlian slogan states, return to the things themselves, as experienced.
is perspective was supported by Husserl’s other theoretical foundation
which states that there is nothing more fundamental than what is experi-
enced and all investigation should use this as its starting point. erefore,
as Ashworth (2015, p. 11) states:
of things being more than the sum of their parts is again discussed.
In Husserl’s (1936/1970) book e Crisis of European Science and
Transcendental Phenomenology, he uses this phrase to unify existentialism
and phenomenology. is is reasoned by establishing experience as not a
matter of lawful response to variables but as a system of interconnected
relationships and meanings which he termed ‘lifeworld’. is can only be
researched from a rst-person perspective and one that acknowledges the
consciousness of the participants.
Lifeworld
In correspondence to Smith’s (1996) adaptation of Husserl’s fundamen-
tals, this research was undertaken with the phenomenological exploration
of the participant’s lifeworld in relation to their lived experiences at live
music festivals. It also pursued this investigation with Ashworth’s (2006)
assertion that these subjective lifeworlds have universal features that must
be considered when undertaking research from a phenomenological per-
spective. ese are as follows:
Selfhood: Concerned with social identity; ‘Identity is undeniably part
of sociality our identity links us to others and is provided by interaction
with others’ (p. 216).
Sociality: Concerned with how the experience a ects our relationships
with others; ‘Other people are a central a part of our lifeworld especially
because the evidence from them of our selfhood is so direct’ (p. 216).
Embodiment: Concerned with how the situation relates to feelings of
a person’s body and how we see vulnerabilities and physical strength.
Temporality: Concerned with how the meaning of time, duration, and
biography relates to the experience the person is in.
Spatiality: How people perceive the space that is laid out as part of the
experience and the variables of meaning the space can have; ‘Moreover,
this “geography” will not merely be a physical, but there will be social
norms and a host of other meanings associated with the place’ (p. 217).
Project: ‘How does the situation relate to the person’s ability to carry
out the activities they are committed to and which they regard as central
to their life?’ (p. 217); this includes emotions of pride and regret. is
project may be at the core of their lifeworld.
13 Experience Sampling Methods in Critical Event Studies … 263
Discourse: Concerned with the type of terms used to describe the situa-
tion; what are the social, educational, commercial, and ethical expressions.
Mood-as-atmosphere: ‘Moodedness’ (Ashworth, 2015, p. 13), or
being there as a state of mind.
So far, this chapter has discussed the DES method: its origins, develop-
ment, and how its use must be underpinned by a philosophical position
that is contrary to a large body of event research. is was detailed to
demonstrate that the research has a strong foundation and methodologi-
cal approach. e following section of this chapter discusses how DES
was implemented for primary data collection with the aim of acquiring a
phenomenological exploration of experience at live music events.
Sample
e number of participants in this research was nine using a typical
case approach. e sample size, appropriate to this research, based on
Englander’s (2012) work is to acquire a number of participants that are
experienced in the phenomenon under investigation. e focus, therefore,
264 J. Moss
Recruitment
Participant Incentives
e participants were already attending the music festival and had pur-
chased their tickets independently of the research and thus, they had
already expressed their own desire to experience this festival. Due to the
length of time the participants would be involved in the research and as
a means to avoid issues of low morale, motivation, and dropout (Juslin
et al., 2008), the participants were incentivised with the o er of food pay-
ment over the duration of the festival. In addition, the participants were
also provided with external battery packs to ensure their smartphones
would last the duration of the music festival without having access to
an AC mains supply. is was important because while not having a
functioning mobile phone may not have impacted upon their personal
experience of the festival, it would have led to the participants dropping
out of the research.
Research Site
Equipment
e participants all had use of their own smartphone; these were used so
that the participant burden was minimised and they could also be used
when there was no Wi-Fi/internet availability. ey were also provided
266 J. Moss
with an external battery so that they could ensure their phones were
charged during the entire research period. ey were all asked to down-
load the Supernote app (available for both iPhone and Android models)
into their smartphone.
e improvements in technology and the lowered cost have signi cantly
developed from pen, paper, and pagers, to palm-top personal organisers, to
mobile phones, and now the latest research using ESM can do so with a
smartphone application. is is supported by Raento, Oulasvirta, and Eagle
(2009) who discuss the two main arguments for smartphone use in research
is that rstly their ‘ exible control’ and secondly their ‘cost e ciency’
(p. 429). Furthermore, they recognise the smartphone’s role in improving
ecological validity. Firstly because they are an ‘integrated and nonintrusive
part of both the individuals’ as well as social life,’ so their ‘Access’ is strong
and secondly, ‘those phenomena accessible to smartphones can be studied
without the researchers being present,’ so they are capable of ‘unobtrusive
data collection’ (p. 429). ese factors, combined with real-time self-doc-
umentation provide greater control in ESM studies (Raento et al., 2009).
Randall, Rickard, and Vella-Brodrick (2014) used the MuPsych application
(app) to study music-based emotional regulation. is app has demon-
strated high ecological validity (Randall & Rickard, 2013) but personal
biases and expectations are still present in this type of approach due to the
reliance on self-reporting (Bylsma & Rottenburg, 2010). In relation to this
research, the app is not suitable for data collection due to its quantitative
design and while there are qualitative apps available as illustrated in https://
faculty.unlv.edu/hurlburt/desinfo.pdf, there were no means to download it,
at least in the U.K. at the time of the primary research.
Pre-brie ng
e key part of this preparation was ensuring the participants were suf-
ciently well informed so that they were able to e ectively engage and
undertake their role as co-researcher. e purpose of the pre-brie ng
meeting was to enable this. In addition this type of meeting helps develop
trust in DES research (Fuller-Tyszkiewicz et al., 2013; Scollon & Kim-
Prieto, 2003) as well as in-depth interviews (Englander, 2012).
13 Experience Sampling Methods in Critical Event Studies … 267
During the meetings, each participant was told that the research would
cover a 5-day period beginning on ursday 14 August at 12:00 midday
and ending on Monday 18 August at 12:00 midday. During this time,
they would receive noti cations on a random basis via SMS messages.
e message would state one word, ‘Capture’, and upon receiving it, the
participants would be expected to record what they were experiencing
at the moment they received the message. e smartphone application
Supernote was used and enabled them to capture their experiences using
words, photographs, or voice recording as well as logging the time and
date at which it was received. How to use this app was also part of the
pre-brie ng. It was explained to them that they should feel empowered
to ‘capture’ whatever they were experiencing at that time and that there
was no ‘right or wrong’ response to the noti cation. e signals, while
randomly organised, had been planned by the researcher. It was designed
for the participants to receive them at unpredictable moments because as
Hulbert and Heavey (2001) argue from their own research using DES,
it means:
e bleep catches experiences in ight and most subjects report that
the disruption is small and that it is possible to capture at least a substan-
tial portion of the ongoing experience.
It was then explained to the participants that following the 5-day
DES period, a one-to-one interview would take place with them within
the next 48 hours and that it would last about an hour. e location
for this was chosen by them as somewhere they felt relaxed. ey were
informed about their anonymity and the con dentiality and security
of their data. ey were also assured that this research had been subject
to, and had passed, all She eld Hallam University’s criteria for ethical
research. eir right to withdraw at any time during the research col-
lection and analysis process without the information that they provided
being used was also made clear to them. ey were also provided with
a written copy of this information, a site map, and emergency contact
information.
e DES was implemented as outlined but while the data collected
was invaluable to the overall research, it provided only a starting point for
the collection of the deeper phenomenological information. e inter-
view technique that was used is discussed next.
268 J. Moss
the salient dimensions’ of experiences (p. 151). For this research, the
interview design was structured around the responses to the signals sent
during the rst part of the DES. ere were, however, no predetermined
questions and participants are able to discuss each entry in as much detail
as they wished. It is this type of interview that allows the interviewees to
focus on their experience and what they see as important in relation to
it. Furthermore, this degree of ‘structure’ will help maintain the partici-
pant’s focus and thus increase the chance of generating useful data which
can be criticism, or concern at least, for this type of interview technique
(Roulston, 2010).
From Mittelstaedt’s (2001) comparative study of research methods it
was found that 95 % of the sample found that the capturing aspects of
experience using experience sampling to be very accurate because there
was very little memory loss or bias. Furthermore 57 % found that the
in-depth interview was the most accurate picture of their experience.
is provides strong support for combining these two approaches by
using a descriptive sampling method followed by the in-depth interview.
Smith and Osborn (2007) support this by recognising this approach as
the foundation of interpretative phenomenological analysis. is inter-
view technique and the analytical process have been used to successfully
explore the experiences of people in a number of health- and medical-
related situations (Brocki & Wearden, 2006). Since then, it has been used
to investigate a wide range of socio-psychological phenomena includ-
ing identity, well-being, and parenting and occupational health (Clarke,
2009; Smith, 2015). e potential richness of the data from using this
type of interview technique, while also providing strong, credible, and
reliable because of the analytical framework, demonstrates its suitability
and applicability to this investigation into the experiences of live music
festivals. is perspective is supported by Reid, Flowers, and Karkin
(2005), p. 23) who conclude their review of the research approach by
stating that it is becoming increasingly diverse in its application with
research:
Going beyond traditional health psychology journals, and into social
psychology, nursing studies, religious and existential studies, music, art
and occupational therapy, and the ‘traditional’ journals of the medi-
cal profession…[It] is particularly suited to researching in ‘unexplored
270 J. Moss
Conclusion
e chapter was written to illustrate a new direction for event research.
Using the psychological research tool of DES can generate new insights
into events and festivals. e chapter discussed, however, not simply a
new method of data collection but also a new research philosophy. is
new philosophical lens sees not only events di erently, but in addi-
tion, the very focus and reason for doing research. It is a critical socio-
psychological perspective that can develop theory, permit new voices to
be heard, and experiences to be explored. It is this type of methodological
consideration and advancement, supported by those in social sciences,
which will drive events studies forward.
References
Aitchison, C. C. (2006). e critical and the cultural: Explaining the divergent
paths of leisure studies and tourism studies. Leisure Studies, 25(4), 417–422.
doi:10.1080/02614360600896411.
Andrews, L., Bennett, R. R., & Drennan, J. (2011). Capturing a ective experi-
ences using the SMS Experience Sampling (SMS-ES) method. International
Journal of Market Research, 53(4), 1–27. doi:10.2501/ijmr-53-4-479-506.
13 Experience Sampling Methods in Critical Event Studies … 271
Smith, J. A. (2015). Subject areas and references. Retrieved June 22, 2015 from
http://www.ipa.bbk.ac.uk/references.
Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative phenomenological
analysis: eory, method and research. United Kingdom: Sage Publications.
Smith, J. A., Harre, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (1995). Introduction. In J. A.
Smith, R. Harre, & L. Van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking psychology
(pp. 1–9). London: Sage Publications.
Smith, J. A. & Osborn, M. (2007). Pain as an assault on the self: An interpreta-
tive phenomenological analysis of the psychological impact of chronic benign
low back pain. Psychology & Health, 22(5), 517–534. doi:10.1080/
14768320600941756.
Smith, J. A. & Osborn, M. (2015). Interpretative phenomenological analysis. In
J. A. Smith (Ed.), Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods
(3rd ed.pp. 25–52). United Kingdom: Sage Publications.
Smyth, J. M. (2003). Ecological momentary assessment research in behavioral
medicine. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4(1), 35–52. doi:10.1023/a:
1023657221954.
Van Manen, M. (1997). Researching lived experience: Human science for an action
sensitive pedagogy. London, Ontario: Althouse.
Wood, E., Kenyon, A., & Moss, J. (2012). Researching Gen Y in their natural
habitat: Experience and emotion, music and drink. Academy of Marketing
Annual Conference.
Wood, E. H. & Moss, J. (2015). Capturing emotions: Experience sampling at
live music events. Arts and the Market, 5(1), 45–72. doi:10.1108/
am-02-2013-0002.