UFCLECTURES2023
UFCLECTURES2023
UFCLECTURES2023
CONTINUE
FIRST Y E A R
- To introduce the students to the study of Structural Linguistics, with special reference to the
European and American Schools.
- To provide an overview of the techniques in language segmentation and to introduce the learner
to the study of morphology.
-To present an introduction to Phrase Structure Grammar and prepare the students for the study of
Transformational Generative Grammar.
- Finally, to familiarise students with the I.P.A. system of phonetic transcription in order to
introduce them to Generative Phonology and some distinctive feature theories.
2
Contents of the Course: S T R U C T U R A L L I N G U I S T I C S
Part One:
European Structural Linguistics 1.
FERDINAND DE SAUSSURE 1.1
Language, Langue, Parole 1.1.1
Synchronic / Diachronic Approaches to Language 1.1.2
Syntagmatic / Paradigmatic Analyses of language
The Linguistic Sign: Signifier / Signified 1.1.3
Place of Language in Human Fact: Semiology 1.1.4
Phonology 1.2
Different Views of the Phoneme 1.2.1
Three Classical Schools of Structural Linguistics 1.2.2
(The Prague School; The Copenhagen School; The American School)
Morphology 1.3
Language Types: Inflectional / Derivational 1.3.1
Affixes: Prefixes, Infixes, Suffixes 1.3.2
Agglutinative Affixes 1.3.3
American Structuralism: Bloomfield : I. C.A. 1.4
Part Two:
American Structuralism 2.
Harris' Approach to Structuralism 2.1
Transformational Generative Grammar 2.2
Context-free Rules 2.2.1
Context-sensitive Rules 2.2.2
The Transformational Component 2.3
Generative Phonology 2.4
Distinctive Feature Analysis 2.4.1
Phonological Rules 2.4.2
Underlying Representations 2.4.3
Ordered vs. Unordered Rules 2.4.4
3
Selected Bibliography:
Bolinger, D. (1968) Aspects of Language, Harcourt, Brace & World Inc., N.Y.
F. De Saussure (1959) Course in General Linguistics, Fontana / Collins, N.Y.
Hyman, L.M. (1975) Phonology: Theory and Analysis, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, N.Y.
Lyons, J. (1968) Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics, C.U.P
Robins, R.H. (1971) General Linguistics: An Introductory Survey, Longman, London, 2nd Ed.
REVIEW
In the Pre-19th Century, the study of language was intrinsically related to the study of philosophy,
archaeology aesthetics theology, etc.
The 19th century saw a growing tendency to focus on language as AN OBJECT OF STUDY IN
ITSELF. This approach led to the question 'What is Language?' A different methodology was
used for the investigation of language. In this new tendency, the linguist starts his research on the
basis of a consistently empirical approach regarding LINGUISTIC FACTS, and he derives his
evidence from them alone.
Early Beginnings: more and more languages were studied and examined.
Classification of languages in terms of mutual relationships. Linguistic forms were then
described, analysed and explained. No more questioning about when and where a form existed
and how it looked and was used. Linguistic science began to ask why the form had taken that
definite shape. In this respect, linguistics passed from a purely descriptive to an explanatory
science. The chief innovation of the beginning of the 19th century was that language was looked
upon as growing, moving and continually changing (dynamic).
4
LECTURE ONE
(08/04/2023)
Comparative Philology
1. Language-Families
- It is a well-known fact that different languages resemble one another in many ways, e.g. :
a) Vocabulary
b) Grammatical Structure
Vocabulary: English / German son : Sohn; mother : Mutter; brother : Bruder; seven : sieben;
have : habe, etc.
Grammatical Structure: These are SVO Languages.
- There are fewer instances of vocabulary similarities between English and Russian and very few
indeed between English and Turkish.
- The degree of resemblance in vocabulary is often supported by the degree of grammatical
resemblance.
- These facts are a matter of experience and observation, immediately obvious to anyone who sets
out to learn, or simply examines the language in question. These facts are explained by saying
that English and German are closely related; that English and Russian are more distantly related.
- In this context, the term relationship is being used to refer to a historical, or genetic relationship.
That is the two languages must have developed from some earlier single language. This is
otherwise expressed by saying that they belong to the same family of languages.
- Most of the languages of Europe and many languages of Asia belong to what is called the Indo-
European family.
- Many sub-families exist within this wider family:
a. Germanic: German, English, Dutch, Swedish, etc.
b. Slavonic: Russian, Polish, Czech, etc.
c. Romance: French, Italian, Spanish, etc.
d. Indo-Iranian: Sanskrit, Persian, etc.
e. Celtic: Gaelic (Irish and Scottish), Welsh, Breton, etc.
5
- Other major language-families include:
To have established the principles and methods used in setting up these, and other languages
families, and what is more important, to have developed a theory of language change and
linguistic relationship was the most significant achievement of nineteenth century linguistic
scholarship.
- The term comparative philology came to replace the less satisfactory ‘comparative grammar’
in the course of the 19th century.
- Although some scholars prefer to call it historical linguistics, the important thing to notice is
that philology has no connection with textual criticism or literary scholarship, which are quite
separate branches of study for which the term philology is also used.
2. Linguistics as a Science
It is usually said that the 19th century saw the birth of the scientific study of language in
the Western World.
- In fact, it was in the course of the 19th century that facts of language came to be carefully and
objectively investigated and then explained in terms of inductive hypotheses.
2.e Loan-words
It was believed that the degree of resemblance was greater than can reasonably be
attributed to chance. This resemblance was quite sufficient proof of relationship. Of course, this
takes no account of what is referred to technically, though perhaps misleadingly, as borrowing.
7
This, rather is a geographical or cultural contact, for words tend to travel across geographical and
linguistic boundaries together with the object or custom to which they refer.
- We have only to think of the vast numbers of words of Greek or Latin origin in the vocabularies
of modern European languages to see the point.
- The founders of comparative philology were well aware that words pass easily from one
language to another, but they had no sure way of distinguishing loan-words from the rest of the
vocabulary. For this reason, they tended to rely particularly upon grammatical resemblance as
evidence of linguistic relationship.
TABLE 1
g h k d t b p f Gothic
h c g t d t f b p Latin
k g d t ph b p Greek
h j d t bh b p Sanskrit
instances of words which illustrate these correspondences are: Go. fotus, L. pedis, Gk. podos,
Sanskrit padas (foot)
Go. taihum, L. decem (c=[k] in Latin), Gk. deka, Sanskrit dasa (ten).
- Grimm and his contemporaries saw that there were many exceptions to such generalisations.
They observed, for instance, that although the word for brother was completely ‘regular’ in the
development of the consonants (cf. Go. brothar: L. frater; b f, t), the word for father was
only partially so (cf. Go. fadar ; L. pater; f p but d t).
Some fifty years later, a strikingly different principle was proclaimed by a group of
scholars who saw themselves as revolutionaries (the Young Grammarians).The principle was
that the sound changes which we can observe in documented linguistic history proceed according
to fixed laws which suffer no disturbance save in accordance with other laws. Consequently,
amendments were made on the previous laws in order to account for ‘irregularities’ and
exceptions to the law.
The notion of 'evolution', although it was by no means a new concept, was one of the
dominant ideas of 19th century thought. It was an idea which was enthusiastically taken up by the
romantic movement in its reaction to the classical tradition.
- With the publication of Darwin's Origin of Species (1859) and the selection of the notion of
purpose or design, not only was evolutionary biology offered the possibility of adopting the
prevailing mechanistic, or positivist outlook of the natural sciences, but the whole idea of
evolution was thought to have been put on a sounder ‘scientific’ footing.
- The apparent success of the positivist outlook in biology tended to promote the search for laws
of evolution in all social sciences.
Contemporary linguistics, however, is no longer committed to a positivist conception of science
and as we shall see, it is no longer predominantly concerned with the evolution of languages.
- Recognition of the different levels of structure, or patterning in language carries us into the
third of our chronological periods in the history of linguistics - the period of 20th century
structural linguistics. (Excerpts from J. Lyon, 1968).
11
LECTURE TWO
MODERN LINGUISTICS
(UFC 2023)
The breakthrough with traditional grammar was the priority of the spoken language. The
traditional grammarians tended to assume that the spoken language is inferior to and in some
sense dependent upon the standard written language. In conscious opposition to this view, the
contemporary linguist maintains that the spoken language is primary and that writing is
essentially a means of representing speech in another medium.
- The relative antiquity of speech and writing is, however, of secondary importance. Far more
relevant to understanding the relation between speech and writing is the fact that all systems of
writing are demonstrably based upon units of spoken language. In the description of the spoken
language, the linguist generally finds that he must recognise basic units of three different kinds
‘sounds’, ‘syllables’, and ‘words’. All commonly used systems of writing take one or the other of
these units as basic.
1. Ferdinand de Saussure
Ferdinand de Saussure (1857-1913), a Swiss scholar, is undoubtedly the founder of
Modern Linguistics. His lectures (restructured from the notes of his students after his death) were
published in 1915 as Cours de linguistique générale.
- He was born one year after Sigmund Freud, the founder of Modern Psychology, and one year
before Emile Durkheim, the founder of Modern Sociology. This sets out to locate Saussure's work
in historical perspective and to treat the founding and renovation of these disciplines as a single
revolutionary gesture in the development of the social sciences.
- The notion of systems which should be the primary object of attention is crucial in each of these
thinkers: a language as a system in Saussure; the psyche as an interpersonal system in Freud; the
system of collective norms and beliefs for Durkheim.
- de Saussure formulated explicitly a new conception of language and new methods for the study
of languages.
- His greatest contribution is his being among the first to expose the MULTIFACETED nature
of language. He realised that language could be studied from more than one point of view of its
functions.
Language can be considered as:
1. A means of communication
2. A means of expressing ideas, etc.
13
- It can be analysed from the point of view of the conditions in which it exists and be considered
as a cultural and historical phenomenon.
- It can be analysed from the point of view of its internal structure and be considered as a system
of signs for encoding and decoding messages. Ferdinand de Saussure not only put forward this,
but he developed several concepts to bear his arguments.
- de Saussure differentiated between Extrinsic approach and Intrinsic approach to language:
a. Extrinsic Approach: Conditions in which language exists; i.e., language in relation to the
history of the people and civilisation where it is spoken. Also, in relation to politics, literature,
geographical spread, etc.
- De Saussure maintained that there was no necessary or direct relationship between the inner
composition of a language and the external conditions of its existence. He explained his position
by drawing an analogy with the game of chess: the fact that the game came to Europe from Persia
is an extrinsic fact and has absolutely no bearing on either the system or the rules of the game.
- The inner mechanism of a language can be studied and explained quite adequately without any
knowledge of its history.
- A productive study of the inner mechanism entails a clear distinction between the synchronic
aspect of a language or the axis of simultaneity, and the diachronic aspect or the axis of
successiveness. - Synchronic linguistics studies the inner composition of a language or its system.
- Diachronic linguistics studies the history of individual linguistic units.
c. Langue / Parole
Concept
Sound Image
Two principles:
Principle 1: The arbitrary nature of the linguistic sign: The bond between signifier and signified is
arbitrary.
Principle 2: The linear nature of the signifier.
- The linguistic sign is arbitrary; i.e. a particular combination of signifier and signified is an
arbitrary one. By arbitrary nature of the sign, de Saussure means that there is no natural or
inevitable link between signifier and signified. The very few cases where the linguistic signs may
be in some way motivated, i.e. made less arbitrary are onomatopoeia where the sound of the
signifier seems in some way imitative.
16
e. Substance / Form
Language is Form, not Substance , de Saussure said; i.e., what is essential for a linguistic unit is
not the substance of which it is made, but only the set of oppositions of which it is a member.
This set determines its significance or value.
- Form is independent of substance: deaf people, blind people, mute people can learn a language.
- Sound and meaning constitute substance of language, whereas the delimitation of the units
constitute form.
«Les phonèmes sont avant tout des entités oppositives, relatives et négatives. » (Cours, p.164).
A bottle of milk
P
A A glass of milk
R
A A glass of water
D
I A *glass of tea
G
M A cup of tea
A
T Two *cup of tea
I
C Two cups of milk
- His explicit distinction between the various objects of study in language provided a basis for
disciplines concerned with language to specialise and integrate as in the exact or natural sciences.
Thus, linguistics and ethnography have integrated into ethnolinguistics; the same is true for
sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics etc.
- Many different schools of linguistics can be distinguished at the present time, but they have all
been directly or indirectly influenced (in various degrees) by de Saussure's Cours.
17
LECTURE THREE
(UFC 2023)
OF STRUCTURAL LINGUISTICS
- Many different schools of linguistics can be distinguished. They have all been directly or
indirectly influenced (in various degrees) by de Saussure's Cours (1915).
- The three classical schools of structural linguistics which adopted de Saussure's theory of
language in one way or another are:
a) The difference between these three schools in respect to de Saussure's theory is primarily one
of emphasis on particular attitudes to language study.
b) All of them distinguish between Synchronic and Diachronic phenomena and they all
concentrate on the synchronic aspect of language.
c) The Prague school structuralists, however, stress more than the others on the relationship
between synchronic and diachronic phenomena.
d) All three share de Saussure's views on langue and parole. But the Prague school structuralists
de-emphasise the autonomy of langue while the American and Copenhagen structuralists use
other terms to designate these two concepts.
text = parole structure = langue The American School
text = parole scheme = langue The Copenhagen School
18
e) All three distinguish between parole units (i.e. sounds) and langue units (i.e. phonemes).
They consider the disciplines dealing with parole units (phonetics) non-linguistic, i.e. non-
functional on their own or outside a system (cf. [b] of Berber vs. [b] of English.).
f) All three are concerned above all with the interrelationships, oppositions or functions of the
elements in a given system. But the Prague school attaches importance to the study of the
substance of the elements (phonetics as well as semantics).
g) All three schools study in one way or the other not only syntagmatic but also paradigmatic
relations.
h) Their linguistic descriptions follow to a certain extent the same methodological principles:
In the Prague School definition of the phoneme, the latter is seen neither as a sound nor as
a group (family) of sounds, but rather as an abstraction, a theoretical concept (construct) on the
phonological level. Thus, the phoneme in Prague School terminology is a purely abstract
phenomenon.
Trubetzkoy (1939) developed a whole theory of distinctive features based on articulatory
properties to try to capture generalisations on how sounds combine and group themselves
naturally. His theory has been later on revised by Jackobson who was concerned more with the
19
acoustic properties of the sound than its articulatory properties. He himself developed his own
theory of distinctive features based on acoustic considerations (1952).
To show that the phoneme cannot be ‘a class of sounds’, the Prague School functionalists
analysed the behaviour of some sounds in particular languages. They then developed concepts
such as phonemic overlapping which has been discussed extensively by European phonologists
(André Martinet, 1947, Fisher Jorgersen, 1959). The discussion was raised concerning /t/ and /d/
in Danish to try to explain that a phoneme cannot be regarded as a class, group or family of
sounds. The case of the Danish /t/ and /d/ was discussed extensively by Jackobson, Fant & Halle
(1952)
Consider:
Word-initially, /t/ and /d/ are pronounced respectively [t] and [d] as in [tag] ‘roof’ and [dag] ‘day’
(examples discussed in class).
Word-finally, however, /t/ is pronounced [d] and /d/ is pronounced [] as in:
‘a hat’ [had] /hat/
‘hate’ [ha-] /had/
In Danish and in many other languages, a rule exists that weaken consonants when they occur at
the end of words. e.g.,
/habs/ [hæps] ‘a jail'
/kanz/ [kns] ‘a treasure’ (in Algerian Spoken Arabic)
So, in Danish, /d/ weakens word-finally (at the end of the word).
The result is that [d] initial as in [dag] ‘day’, must be assigned (attributed) to the phoneme /d/.
But [d] final as in [had] ‘a hat’ must be assigned to the phoneme /t/. Thus, one phone ([d]) is
assigned to one or two phonemes, depending on the phonetic context (environment) where it
occurs.
- We see that such a case contravenes (contradicts) the idea of the phoneme as a family of sounds.
We notice that though [d] is different from [t] in terms of sounds, it is nevertheless considered as
an allophone of /t/ in particular contexts in Danish.
- The other aspect of phonological analysis that the Prague School functionalists concentrated on
was Neutralisation and Archiphoneme.
Bernard Bloch (1941) makes a distinction between partial overlapping and complete overlapping:
«The intersection or overlapping of phonemes will be called partial if a given sound X occurring
20
under one set of phonetic conditions is assigned to phoneme A while the same X under a different
set of conditions is assigned to phoneme B; it will be called complete (overlapping) if successive
occurrences of X under the same conditions are sometimes assigned to A, sometimes to B." (pp.
66-67).
Complete overlapping:
- In English, /t/ and /d/ intervocalically, are both pronounced as an alveolar tap [r ]. Thus, for
many speakers of American English, ‘betting’ and ‘bedding’ are pronounced identically as
[bi].
-One can say that they are two distinct phonemes on the basis of minimal pairs like /bt/ vs. /bd/.
The sound [r] appears then as an allophone of both /t/ and /d/ in /bti/ and /bdi/.
What this means in terms of Prague School phonology is that an opposition has been neutralised
in intervocalic position (between two vowels). Thus, while the phonemes /t/ and /d/ contrast
word-initially as in /tin/ vs. /din/ and finally as in /bit/ vs. /bid/, they lose their contrasting force or
load when they occur between two vowels in American spoken English. Such an opposition (/t/
vs. /d/) is neutralisable in this position (i.e. intervocalically).
To explain this phenomenon, the Prague school linguists introduced the concept of
Archiphoneme. They write the archiphoneme in their phonological representations in upper case
letter (capital letter) to indicate that a neutralisation took place in this position. Thus, `betting' and
`bedding' will be represented on the phonological level either as /T/ or /D/ where the choice is
open for the selection of a capital /T/ or capital /D/ (but not both!).
In the case of Finnish or Chinese, the distinction on the content plane has no equivalent on the
expression plane. In other words, there is only one word used for both male and female.
- A distinction which exists only on one plane and does not commute with a distinction on the
other plane is regarded as a variant of the same linguistic unit.
The principle task of Glossamatics is ANALYSIS The immediate object of analysis is TEXT
What must be discovered is a SYSTEM
This hypothesis has remained a hypothesis because, so far, no definite experiments have been
achieved to confirm or reject its central thesis.
1) The object of linguistics is not sounds and meanings in themselves, but the combination
of certain sounds with certain meanings
2) He developed the principle of distinctive meaning
- He defined linguistic forms as forms in which certain sounds are patterned with certain
meanings.
For example, he defines the complex form as forms with «partial phonetic and semantic
resemblance to some other linguistic forms». That is, words, phrases, sentences. Simple forms, he
says, are forms with no such resemblance. That is to say, morphemes.
Syntax
Morphology
Phonology
Phonetics (notice that meaning is left aside)
- Working within this scholastic environment, L. Bloomfield set himself the task to define
linguistic forms. He defines them as forms in which sound and meaning combine in a co-
ordinated manner. Phonemes, then are not linguistic forms as they represent sounds but they have
no meaning by themselves.
a. Linguistic forms
Free Forms: can be used on their own or with other forms, e.g.,
Poor / John, Poor John
Bound forms: never used alone. These are morphemes than cannot stand on their own, e.g.,
{s} morpheme of plurality in English boy + {s} boys
Prefixes and Suffixes such as {un}, {ful}, {er} as in: un/grace/ful, dis/grace/ful, sing/er....
Infixes as the infix {-t-} in Arabic /iâtakaa/ (reflexive of /âakaa/ "to complain")
Simple Forms: simple morphemes like: the, a, it, man, woman, etc., prepositions like by, with..
Complex Forms: a combination of a free form with other forms, e.g., meaningless, happier,
helpful, poor John, poor John ran away, etc.
From these classifications, Bloomfield defined all the linguistic concepts he developed, especially
the concepts of Constituent, Class, and Construction. We shall study here the first concept only,
i.e. the concept of Constituent.
24
- The common part of any two complex forms, if it is a linguistic form, is a constituent or
component of these complex forms.
-Constituents subdivide into Immediate and ultimate (i.e. terminal).
The sentence «Poor John ran away» is composed of two complex linguistic forms:
Component Component
I.C. I.C.
So, the sentence «Poor John ran away» is composed of two (02) Immediate Constituents (I.C.)
and four Ultimate Constituents (U.C.). We shall see later on (Cf. Lecture Four) that {ran} can still
be divided into two ultimate constituents, namely {run} + {past}.
25
LECTURE FOUR
(UFC2023)
-Prior to Chomsky's Syntactic Structures (1957), many American linguists regarded the aim of
their discipline as being the classification of the elements of Human Languages. Linguistics was
to be a ‘verbal botany’. As Hockett (1942) says, «Linguistics is a classificatory science » (Quoted
from R.H. Robins, A Short History of Linguistics, Indiana University Press, 1967, p. 239).
- Sequences of word classes are gathered into sentences and classified into sentence types.
- The aim of this type of linguistic work was to provide the linguist with the necessary tools and
method, in fact a set of rigorous methods known as the discovery procedures to use to extract
from the corpus the various elements such as phonemes, morphemes, etc.
- Little place was given to the study of meaning and of the speaker's actual use of the language.
26
II. Structural Linguistics
- They insisted on objective methods of verification.
- Availability of specific techniques of discovery (i.e., I.C.A.).
- Refusal to allow any talk on meaning or on mental entities or unobservable features.
Sentences 1 and 2 above look as if they have the same grammatical structure. Despite this surface
structure similarity, their deep structures are different. In Sentence 1, John is direct object of the
verb "to please". In Sentence 2, John is subject of the verb "to please".
- Structural assumptions are inadequate to handle syntactical facts such as those which exist in
ambiguous sentences like (1) and (2) above. Ambiguity derives not from the words themselves
but from the syntactical structure itself (the syntagmatic relation that exists between words in a
linear order). Other examples of ambiguous sentences:
The shooting of the hunters is terrible.
I like her cooking.
Flying planes can be dangerous.
-Structural linguists had little or nothing to say about these cases. They simply ignored them.
27
III. Structuralism vs. Generative Grammar
- The inability of structural methods to account for such syntactic facts eventually led Chomsky to
challenge not only the methods but the goals and indeed the definition of the subject matter of
linguistics given by the structuralists.
- Instead of a taxonomic goal of classifying elements by performing sets of operations on a corpus
of utterances, Chomsky argued that the goal of linguistic description should be to construct a
theory that would account for the infinite number of sentences of a natural language. Such a
theory came to be called "Generative Grammar" because its aim was to construct a device that
would generate (i.e. produce) all and only the possible sentences of a given language.
- In Syntactic Structures (1957), Chomsky adopts an I.C. Analysis of sentences in order to gather
information concerning the constituent structure of sentences. Instead of cutting sentences into
their various Immediate Constituents (I.C.), then into their Ultimate Constituents (U.C.) until no
further cutting is possible without altering (or losing) the identity of individual ultimate
constituents, he rather analysed this division into I.C. and U.C. of the sentence for a better
understanding of the structure involved.
- His approach to sentence structure analysis as opposed to earlier techniques is that he does not
just draw a diagram to illustrate the structure of a particular sentence, but he formalises the
analytical divisions into a system of ordered rules by deriving each decision from some previous
one, like this:
1. S NP + VP
2. VP Verb + NP etc.
- Chomsky starts his analysis of a sentence from the surface (or apparent) structure and goes
down via a number of derivations to the deep (abstract, non-apparent) structure of the sentence.
To do this, he uses rules of the type: X Y / Z
- All the elements situated on the left of the arrow (represented by X in the rule above) constitute
the Structural Analysis (S.A.) of the rule in question. Y represents the Structural Change (S.C.) of
the rule. The oblique bar (/) is called the context (or environment) bar, and Z represents the
context itself (or environment where the rule applies).
- By a series of ordered rules (some of which are obligatory, i.e. must be applied, and some are
optional, i.e. may or may not be applied), Chomsky derives the deep structure of a given surface
sentence. Since one rule is fed by the previous one, following a process of input --> output, in our
case above, Rule 1 serves as input of Rule 2 etc., provided the environment (Z) is satisfied.
Thus, the grammar will generate strings of elements which ultimately will give the deep structure
of a sentence. Chomsky (1957) refers to this type of grammar as ‘Phrase Structure Grammar’
(PSG).
- In this grammar, Chomsky develops a method which both orders the analytical decisions and
which formally relates them to each other by deriving each decision from some previous one, like
this:
29
The arrow is an instruction to re-write (or replace) the element(s) of the SA part of the rule
into a string of elements on the right of the arrow representing the SC part of the rule.
- On the basis of the Phrase Structure Rules above, we can now build a Phrase Marker (or tree
NP VP
T N Verb NP
- With a very limited set of rules and a very limited number of lexical entries (vocabulary), many
sentences can be generated (many other combinations have been deliberately left out). This
indicates that the grammar has some generative power (or capacity). Increase the vocabulary in
the lexicon and you get an increased number of sentences accordingly.
- This was in fact the first component of a generative grammar as expounded in Syntactic
Structures (1957). It consists of rules which take an initial element S (standing for sentence) and
assigns to this S a particular Phrase Structure Marker (or tree diagram). The rules of the first
30
component or Base Component produce strings of elements which represent the underlying
structure of a simple sentence (kernel sentence).
- According to Syntactic Structures (1957), the kernel sentence is any sentence which results from
the application of Phrase Structure Rules (or PSrules) only. That is, the simple active declarative
sentence, which in our case would be any of ‘'The boy saw the girl’, ‘The girls kissed a boy’, etc.
- PSrules generate a kernel string of the type:
T + N + Verb + T + N
which in our case would generate a kernel sentence like ‘The girl kissed a boy’. The result of the
application of PSrules is an underlying string of the type: the + boy + verb + a + girl to which
optional transformations have not applied yet.
- Such a model, cannot of course handle all the cases of the language, as it has only given us so
far derivations for kernel sentences, i.e. simple active declarative sentences. Moreover, we shall
see that such a grammar (Phrase Structure Grammar) is neither powerful enough nor adequate in
the treatment of sentence structure in a language.
NP1 VP
Verb NP2
N Adj. Inf.
John is easy to please
John is eager to please
- Another case of the weakness of PSGrammar relates to Active/Passive sentences. Here again,
PSG gives us two different phrase markers for syntactically related sentences.
Thus, surface similarities like 'I like her cooking' conceal underlying differences, i.e. I like when
she is cooking / I like what she is cooking, for which PSGrammar gives us only one tree diagram.
Similarly, surface differences like 'The boy will read the book / The book will be read by the boy'
conceal underlying similarities in that these two sentences are derived from one tree diagram. Yet,
PSGrammar gives us two unrelated tree diagrams for these related sentences (Active/Passive are
derived from one single deep structure). We notice in sentences like 'The boy saw the girl / The
girl was seen by the boy' the word order is different, with addition of elements in the passive form
(was seen, by, etc.). However, these two sentences carry the same meaning and are derived from
the same deep structure. PSGrammar alone gives us no way of picturing this underlying
similarity. It would give us two unrelated descriptions of these related sentences.
- To account for weaknesses of this type, and others in PSG, Chomsky claims that the grammar
requires additional rules of another kind. These rules he calls Transformational Rules (or T-rules)
which transform phrase markers into other phrase markers by moving elements around, adding or
deleting elements in the string.
- In the Base Component (or Phrase Structure Component) we have rules of the form X Y .
The result is a string of formatives which may or may not be in a correct order.
- In the Transformational Component we have rules that will add, delete or change the order of
formatives in the terminal string produced by the PS-rules. Some of the transformations will be
obligatory, others will be optional.
- In the Morphophonemic Component we have rules which re-write the morphemic representation
into a proper string of phonemes. These rules are also of the type XY, e.g. take + S --> /teiks/,
take + Past /tuk/.
33
VII. Some Rule Applications in TGG for English
Rules
(1) S NP + VP
(2) VP Verb + NP
NPsing
(3) NP
NPplur.
In Rule (10), Aff. stands for any of {Past, S, , ing}, and v stand for any {V, M, have, be}.
(11) Negative Transformation (Optional)
T.Not X1 - X2 - X3 X1 - X2 + not - X3
In Rule (11), T.Not should apply before T.Aff. (Rule ordering).
Rule (11) applies to the strings
NP - C - V ...
NP - C + M ... NP - C + have ... NP - C + be...
(12) do Affix
# Aff. do + Aff.
In Rule (12), do. Aff applies after C (Rule ordering).
(13) T.Question (Optional) T.Q. X1 - X2 - X3 X2 - X1 - X3 Rule (13) applies to the strings
NP - C - V ...
NP - C + M ... NP - C + have ... NP - C + be ...
We require that T.Question applies after C and before T.Affix" (Syntactic Structures, p. 63).
35
LECTURE FIVE
(UFC2023)
DISTINCTIVE FEATURE THEORY
The phoneme was first defined as a «class of sounds», «a family of sounds» (cf. the
physical reality of the phoneme), as a psychological entity (cf. the psychological reality of the
phoneme) or as a phonological construct (cf. the phonological reality of the phoneme). It was
thus considered as a segment or a unit of phonology that is used to discuss phonological
contrasts. However, such an approach becomes problematic when one wants to study the
phonological system of a language as a whole in order to capture all the phonological relations
underlying the phonemic behaviour of these units (the phonemes). Therefore, for a contrast such
as /p/, /b/ in English, we concentrate on the features included in these segments rather than on the
segments themselves. This gives you better insights into the phonological behaviour of the
phonemes as oppositional units. In this respect, only the features which are distinctive are taken
into account. The features which are redundant (cf. aspiration in English] are left aside. That is
why, distinctive features are proposed to capture only phonological contrasts. Phonetic contrasts,
such as [ph] / [p] in the English words [phin] and [spin] are therefore not necessary within a
theory of distinctive features.
1
. Jackobson, R.: "Observations sur le classement phonologique des consonnes", 3rd International Congress of
Phonetic Sciences at Ghent, 1938).
36
On the contrary, their distinctive feature (Qualité différentielle) allows not only understand why
they are different but also how they are different from the other phonemes of the same system.
Let’s consider the following minimal pairs from English: bin / pin nabbing / napping ; tab / tap. If
we were to stop our analysis at this level, nothing is said about how the distinctive features of
these phonemes operate within the phonological system of English.
On the other hand, if we look at them in terms of the prime features, we look at them in terms of
what distinguishes them from the remaining oppositions of the same system. Thus, we’ll get the
following feature matrices:
A. /b/ B. /p/
+ consonantal + consonantal
- vocalic - vocalic
+ labial + labial
+ stop + stop
+ oral + oral
+ voice - voice
The feature matrices A and B above represent the phonemes /b/ and /p/ of English respectively.
Feature matrix A. tells us that /b/ is a consonant ([+consonantal] or [+cns] for short), that it is not
a vowel (or that it cannot take the position of a vowel in the word) ([-vocalic] or [-voc] for short),
that it is labial, abbreviated to [+lab], that it is oral [+oral], and that it is voiced [+vce]. /p/ has the
same features except for voicing as it is characterised as [-vce].
Consider the following example where the phonemes /p/ and /b/ of English , e.g. Voiced /
Voiceless; Oral / Nasal, High / Low, etc.
something more general than four segments changing into four other segments picked at random.
And the function of phonological rules is to capture this generality. In particular, the four
phonemes /s, z, t, d/ have in common that they are alveolar consonants. Their phonetic
realisations have in common that they are alveo-palatal consonants. Thus, we get :
37
Alveolar Alveopalatal / Palatal or written formally as :
s
z z / __ j
t t
d d
If the phonemes becoming [ , , t, d] had been /s, k, b, r/, no general phonetic feature such
as alveolar could have been stated. Similarly, if /s, z, t, d/ have become [, g, p, l ], no general
Chomsky and Halle, in their voluminous book The Sound Pattern of English (SPE, 1968)
react to such feature specifications: while we can group segments in terms of Consonantal vs.
Vocalic (in a binary manner), such specifications do not allow us to group three classes as
opposed to the fourth. In fact, the most natural grouping (cf. /s, z, t, d /) of these four major
classes may be between true consonants, liquids, and glides on the one hand, and vowels on the
other. Therefore, in their feature system (SPE, 1968) Chomsky and Halle propose the feature
[+syllabic] or [+syll] for short which replaces the feature Vocalic. Then, [+syllabic] covers all the
segments that constitute a syllabic peak (nucleus) and syllabic liquids as the l of [sdl], and
syllabic nasals as the n of [sdn]. All remaining segments are said to be [-syllabic]. The table
below illustrates an SPE (1968) classification.
TABLE 1
C V L G N L N
cns + - + - + + +
syll - + - - - + +
son - + + + + + +
nas - - - - + - +
(Source: L.M. Hyman, Phonology: Theory and Analysis, Holt, Rinehart and Winston,1975: 44).
40
Back means the retracting the body of the tongue from the neutral position; non-back sounds are
produced without such a retraction from the neutral position, e.g. [k, g, u, o, a] as opposed to [p,
t, i, e].
Low means the lowering of the body of the tongue below the level it occupies in the neutral
position; non-low sounds are produced without such a lowering of the body of the tongue, e.g.
[,,h, o, a] as opposed to [ p, b, t, s, i, e, u]. It must be noted that the features High, Back and
Low are used for both vowels and consonants according to SPE (1968).
Coronal = sounds produced with the blade of the tongue raised from the neutral position; non-
coronal sounds are produced with the blade of the tongue in the neutral position. Thus, labial and
dental consonants are plus Anterior [+ant], while all other consonants are [-ant]. Dentals,
alveolars, and alveo-palatals are plus coronal [+ cor], while all other consonants are [-cor]. The
features anterior and coronal do not apply for vowels.
41
MORPHOLOGY
Like most linguistic terms, Morphology is 19th century in origin. Morphology «is simply a
term for that branch of linguistics which is concerned with the 'forms of words' in different uses
and constructions.»
- This level of analysis is momentarily out of fashion in linguistic theory. Few scholars in
linguistics have devoted books to it in recent years. The neglect of Morphology in linguistic
inquiry is not total. This is mainly true of the past 15 to 20 years. For example, M. Joos' Readings
in Linguistics (1957) is exclusively concerned with morphological issues; Z.S. Harris' Methods in
Structural Linguistics (1951) is also primarily concerned with Morphology. Nevertheless, interest
in Morphology has undoubtedly slackened since the mid 50's. The possible reasons being:
- The 1930's were for structural linguistics a decade devoted to phonology above all.
- The 1940's and early 50's saw an apparent progress in Morphology.
- The 1960's represent a decade where syntax in particular took place of pride. The main
inspiration has come from Chomsky's Syntactic Structures (1957).
II. Definition
Morphology is «the branch of grammar which studies the structure or forms of words,
primarily through the use of the morpheme construct. It is traditionally distinguished from
syntax, which deals with the rules governing the combination of words in sentences.» D. Crystal:
A first Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics, p. 232, A. Deutsch Ltd. (1980).
-Like the phoneme being a theoretical construct used in phonology, the morpheme represents the
theoretical construct used in morphology.
43
III. Formative, Morpheme and Word
- Elements entering the formation of words are usually called formatives, e.g. {s} of arms, birds,
etc. These are irreducible grammatical elements which enter into the construction of larger
linguistic units, such as words and sentences. The term formative has come to be used especially
in generative grammar, as an alternative to the term morpheme, for the terminal elements in a
surface structure representation.
- The Morpheme is "the minimal distinctive unit of Grammar, and the central concern of
morphology". It represents the minimal grammatical unit. The word is demonstrably not the
minimal grammatical unit. The most casual comparison of such word forms as cats, dogs, and
horses with cat, dog, horse, respectively, reveals the divisibility of the former set into two
grammatically significant elements, i.e. cat-, dog-, horse-, and -s (or in
phonetic transcription [s], [z], [iz]). The regular formatives of English plural nouns [s], [z] and
[iz] of the morpheme {s} are called allomorphs (or morpheme variants)
. They are phonologically predictable. That is: s / Voiceless___ ; z / Voiced___ and iz / s__ .
- «The retention of the word as a basic, though not the minimal, unit of grammatical description
allows the retention of the traditional division of the grammar of most languages into
Morphology the study of the structure of words, and Syntax, the study of the grammatical
structure of sentences as built up of words.» ( R. H. Robins, General Linguistics. An Introductory
Survey, Longman 1964 p. 181. The word is in fact a unit of expression which has universal
intuitive recognition by native-speakers, both in spoken and written language.
- A derivational formation usually produces a form that is substantially the same for grammatical
purposes as the original form (root-form) or the simpler and more basic underlying form; e.g.
manhood, neighbourhood, defensive, aggressive, manageress are derivational formatives in
English. For example, man and manhood belong to the same class, i.e. they are both nouns.
44
- Morphologically complex words in which individual grammatical categories may be fairly
easily assigned to morphemes stung together serially (or in a linear manner) in the structure of the
word form exemplify the process of agglutination. The word formation illegalities /i-li:g-µl-iti-z/,
is an example from English of agglutinative word structures. Turkish, Sudanese and Japanese are
among the typically agglutinative languages of the world, with Turkish considered as the purest
example of agglutinative languages.
- A stem may consist solely of a single root morpheme (i.e. a 'simple' stem, as in man), or of two
morphemes (i.e. a 'compound' stem, as in blackboard), or of a root morpheme plus a
derivational affix (i.e. a 'complex' stem as in manly, unmanly, manliness.). In Arabic, stems are
clearly distinguished from roots. For example, to the root <ktb> 'idea of writing' corresponds the
stem /kataba/. That is, in Arabic, the stem is the result of the insertion of vowels among the
radicals of a root.
45
The diagram below represents an overall picture of the morphological hierarchy in word
formation.
Root
Stem
Word
Morpheme
derivational Inflectional
Consider the following table where words from English are divided into their constituent
morphemes:
Prefix Stem one Stem two Der.suff. Der. suff. Infl. suff.
de segregate -- -- -- ed
-- defen(d) -- s(e) ive --
-- tooth ache -- -- s
-- manage -- (e)r ess --
46
LECTURE SEVEN
(UFC 2023)
INTRODUCTION TO PHONOLOGY
/p/ [ph] / ## __
/p/ [p] / ## s __
- There are two series of voiceless stops in Thai: an aspirated series and an unaspirated series.
The Thai words [phaa] "to split" and [paa] "forest" illustrate the same difference between
aspirated and unaspirated 'p' in the English words pin and spin. However, if the comparison were
to stop at the observation that English and Thai share a common inventory of aspirated and
unaspirated stops, an important phonological distinction would be missed.
- In English, the two kinds of ‘p’ are found in different environments. The fact that one p is
aspirated and the other is not is predictable چfrom the place it falls within the word. Thus,
given the environments,
## _____ in ## s ____ in
where ## marks the beginning of a word. Since aspiration can be predicted from the
environment of the voiceless stop in a word, aspiration is said to be redundant in English.
The difference between English and Thai is that aspiration is not redundant in Thai. When two
words such as [phaa] and [paa] differ only by one sound, they are said to constitute a minimal
pair. In this case, aspirated and unaspirated ‘p’ in Thai are said to be distinctive units in the
sound system of this language and that aspiration is distinctive in Thai.
«In phonology we are concerned with the distinctive vs. redundant function of speech sounds... »
Hyman L. Phonology: Theory and Analysis, Holt, Rhinehart and Winston. N.Y., 1975.
48
- A difference in inventory between two languages has not only phonetic consequences, but also
phonological consequences. As an illustration, consider the case of English and Berber.
- In the labial series, English has four oral consonants, while Berber has only two:
English Berber
Voiceless stop : p
Voiced stop : b b
Voiceless Fricative : f f
Voiced Fricative : v
- Berber does not have a [p] or a [v], whereas English does. In English, in order to
distinguish [f] from all other consonants, it is necessary to say that it is:
1. voiceless
2. labial
3. fricative
We must specify it is voiceless to distinguish it from [v] which is voiced. We must specify it is
labial to distinguish it from [s] which is alveolar. Finally, we must specify it as fricative to
distinguish it from [p].
- In Berber, on the other hand, only two features are needed. In order to specify [f] in Berber it
is necessary to say that it is:
1. fricative
2. labial
In the first case we need not add that it if fricative, because we know that if a Berber consonant
is voiceless and labial, it can only be [f]. It cannot be [p] since this sound does not exist in the
language.
Similarly in the second case, we need not add that it is voiceless, because we know that
if a Berber consonant is fricative and labial it can only be [f]. It cannot be [v] since this
sound does not exist in the language.
Thus, in English each of these phonetic features is distinctive for all these sounds, whereas in
Berber there is some redundancy.
49
4. Phonological Rules
Phonological rules (or P-rules) convert phonological representations into phonetic
ones. For example, /la fn :tr/ ‘the window’ is converted to [lafn:tr] in French by a
phonological rule which can be written as follows:
/VC__
The rule states that the schwa may be deleted (that is becomes or zero) when the preceding
consonant is in turn preceded by a vowel.
Other phonological rules in English:
We miss you [wi mi (j)u]
In careful speech, speakers may pronounce [wi mis ju], but the more rapid the pace, the more
likely that forms such as the above will be heard. The following optional rules is therefore
needed:
s
z
t t / __ j
d d
The rule states that /s, z, t, d/ are palatalised to [, , t, d] respectively before a palatal glide /j/
50
LECTURE EIGHT
(UFC 2023)
GENERATIVE PHONOLOGY
- Chomsky & Halle's set of features proposed in The Sound Pattern of English (SPE, 1968)
represent the standard repertoire of features usually found in generative descriptions of
languages. However, this does not mean that they are accepted by all phonologists.
Vowels + - +
Glides - - +
Liquids - + +
Nasals - + +
Obstruents - + -
[+syllabic] (abbreviated to [+syll]) designates those sounds that form the nucleus of the
syllable. In all languages, vowels constitute the nucleus of the syllable and are hence
[+syllabic], while glides, liquids, nasals and obstruents are normally [-syll]. However, in many
languages liquids and nasals may form the syllable nucleus. In such cases, the syllabic
consonants are usually predictable by rule from non-syllabic underlying (or deep
representations. For example, in words such as middle, button represented phonetically here as
['midl] ['btn], the syllabic [l, n] are equated in phonological terms with /l, n/ respectively.
Therefore, their phonological representation should normally be /'mid l/ and /'bt n/.
51
The feature [+consonantal] (or [+cns] for short) groups together liquids, nasals and obstruents
and opposes these sounds to vowels and glides which are [-cns].
Many aspects of phonological structure are sensitive to the contrast provided by the feature
sonorant ([+son]).Obstruents are a natural domain for oppositions in voicing, while underlying
(phonological) contrasts between voiced versus voiceless sonorants are much rarer.
i e µ a o u
high + - - - - +
low - - + + - -
back - - - + + +
p t k q
anterior + + - - -
coronal - + - - -
high - - + - -
low - - - - +
back - - + + +
52
- Sounds produced with a constriction in front of the alveopalatal region are [+anterior]
([+ant]),all other consonants being [-ant] The main function of [+ant] being to differentiate
labials and dentals from other consonants.
-Coronal sounds involve raising the blade of the tongue above its neutral position. The feature
[+cor] groups dentals and alveopalatals together as opposed to other consonants.
SPE (1968) defines velar, uvular and pharyngeal consonants in terms of movements of the body
of the tongue from the neutral reference point (roughly where the vowel in bed is produced) by
the feature specifications high, low, and back.
Continuent - - +
Delayed release - + +
- Stops and affricates are produced with a stricture that blocks the air flow coming from the lungs.
They are therefore [-cnt].
- Fricatives allow air to flow through the stricture and are thus [+cnt].
- The feature [+del rel] distinguishes fricatives and affricates from stops. In affricates, the release
of the air is gradual whereas stops block the air coming from the lungs then release it abruptly.
1. +syll
-cns [-long] / ___##
+long
(where ## represent a strong word boundary)
2. +syll
-cns [-long] / ___CC $
+long (where $ represents a syllable boundary)
- In colloquial Arabic, vowels are inserted to break up clusters of three initial consonants.
e.g. /n + ktab/ [nktb] ‘I write’