Tips Genius Explained
Tips Genius Explained
Tips Genius Explained
HCAMBRIDGE
~ UNIVERSITY PRESS
PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE
The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
I Introduction I
v
For Jordan
Preface
Vll
vm Preface
nation. First, he began to compose music when he was no more than four.
Second, by the time he was six or seven Mozart was such a brilliant per-
former on both harpsichord and violin that the young prodigy and his
older sister were able to travel around Europe demonstrating their talents
on money-making tours. Third, Mozart had an amazing memory for
music, and it was reported that at fourteen he wrote out the complete
score of a lengthy multi-part musical composition, Allegri's Miserere, after
hearing it performed on just a couple of occasions. 3 All three of these feats
are remarkable by any standards. They certainly appear quite mysterious.
It is hard to see how they can be explained without appealing to magic or
miracles. Perhaps he was born possessing some innate gift that made him
totally different from other children. It seems impossible to imagine any
other way to account for Mozart's dazzling childhood accomplishments
at composing, performing, and memorizing music.
Can psychological research help to provide alternative explanations?
Let's start by looking at the young Mozart's composing. He did indeed
begin creating music at an exceptionally young age. But by the standards
of mature composers, Mozart's early works are not outstanding. The ear-
liest pieces of all were probably written down by his father, and perhaps
improved in the process. Many of Wolfgang's childhood compositions,
such as the first seven of his concertos for piano and orchestra, are largely
arrangements of works by various other composers. 4 Of those concertos
that only contain music original to Mozart, the earliest that is now
regarded as a masterwork (No.9, K. 271) was not composed until he was
twenty-one: by that time Mozart had already been composing concertos
for ten years. Similarly, Mozart's first symphonies, written in the style of
J. S. Bach's son Johann Christian Bach, who helped and encouraged the
nine-year-old boy when they met in London in 1764-5, consist of move-
ments lasting no longer than four minutes and have been said to be
almost copies ofJ. C. Bach's.
So Mozart only started producing the distinctive music that we asso-
ciate with him after a lengthy period of training. The same is true of other
great composers. An investigation by John Hayes, who examined the
output of seventy-six well-known composers, established they all took a
long time to reach the peak of their capabilities. 5 With seventy-three of
the seventy-six, Hayes discovered that no major work was produced prior
to the tenth year of their composing career. (The three exceptions were
Shostakovich and Paganini, who each composed a substantial work after
only nine years, and Eric Satie: Trois Gymnopedies was written in his ninth
6 Hayes' method for deciding if a particular musical composition meets the criterion of
being a 'major' one was simple but ingenious. He looked in current catalogues for items
that are available in several recordings, the reason for insisting on the availability of more
than one version being to exclude immature compositions that could have been recorded
simply for their novelty value. 7 Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer (r993).
8 Sloboda, Davidson, Howe, & Moore (r996).
Introduction 5
despite the fact that the measures of practising available to researchers are
rough-and-ready ones, unreliable because they are largely retrospective,
and taking little or no account of either the quality or the appropriateness
of young people's practising activities. Practice and preparation are
equally vital in other fields of achievement. For instance, around ten years
of sustained training are needed for a chess player to reach international
levels, and it takes comparable periods of time to reach the highest stan-
dards in mathematics, the sciences, tennis, athletics, and a number of
other sports. As in music, although it is widely believed that certain gifted
individuals can excel without doing the lengthy practising that ordinary
people have to engage in, the evidence contradicts that view.
Returning to Mozart, are we now any the wiser about his precocious
performing skills? Nobody knows for certain how much time the young
Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart actually spent practising, but it is clear that
his father, Leopold Mozart, subjected him to an arduous and unusual
regime. From the child's earliest years much of the boy's time was
devoted to musical activities. There were few opportunities to play out-
doors or make friends with other children. Leopold Mozart, a capable
violinist and a highly ambitious music teacher, went to great lengths to
make his son into an outstanding musician, having had considerable
success at teaching Wolfgang's sister, Nannerl. The father was anxious to
display his children's abilities (and his own teaching skills) in the best pos-
sible light, and he was not above subtracting a year from their ages on the
posters advertising their public performances.
Let's assume that Mozart's father made his son practise for an average
of three hours a day from the age of three. In that event, by the time the
child was six (when he and his sister were first taken around Europe on
the musical tours in which they displayed their talents), Mozart would
already have practised for a total of around 3,500 hours. That is roughly as
much time as the typical young performer today takes to reach the stan-
dard of a good amateur player. In Mozart's day it was (as it still is) unusual
for a young instrumentalist to have already practised for more than 1,000
hours by the age of six. So if the young Mozart had experienced substan-
tially more training and practice than that, this would largely account for
his standard of performing being superior to anything his audience had
previously observed in a child of his age.
Lacking the knowledge we now have about the likely consequences of
prolonged practising, it would not have been at all surprising if spectators
watching the youthful Mozart's performances could not give a rational
explanation for the feats they were witnessing. They would have seen
nothing like them. But we, unlike Mozart's contemporaries, can perceive
that there was no real mystery involved. These days, it is by no means
6 Genius Explained
recall would not have been a handicap, because the increased remember-
ing that specialised knowledge makes possible transcends age differences.
Although adults do better than children at most tests of memory, the
reverse is true when the task involves information that children, but not
adults, can connect to their existing knowledge. For example, in a study in
which ten-year-olds who were good chess players were given a memory
task that required them to recall chess pieces arranged in legitimate posi-
tions, the children performed better than adult participants who were not
expert players. But items that were unconnected to the children's special
interest were recalled more accurately by the adults. 12
For all that, Mozart's memory feat still seems remarkable, and it was
remarkable. To a non-musical person, a memory feat like Mozart's seems
to involve recalling an immense sequence of separate notes. But imagine
the unusual everyday life of the young Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart. He
inhabited a world of music, hour after hour, day after day, in the company
of a father who was an expert teacher. By adolescence, the sheer amount
of Mozart's musical knowledge would have been enormous by most
people's standards. He would have recognised many familiar structures
and patterns, eliminating the need to recall each note separately. As a
result, compared with a non-musician Mozart would have perceived the
task very differently, with the information that needed to be remembered
being meaningful and interconnected. And although Allegri's Miserere is a
lengthy composition, it is one that happens to contain a great deal of rep-
etition. For a person as knowledgeable as Mozart, that would have light-
ened the burden of remembering. 13
We can now see that it is entirely possible that all three of Mozart's
remarkable early feats could, after all, have been achieved through the
operation of mental processes that were broadly the same as the ones that
give rise to the more modest skills and achievements of ordinary people. It
no longer appears inescapable that Mozart must have begun life with
some mysterious special gift of genius. Of course, what we have achieved
by unravelling the likely causes of certain of Mozart's early feats falls far
short of a full accounting for his creative achievements. I have not even
begun to sketch out the uniquely creative powers that enabled a master-
piece like Don Giovanni to be forged. But a start has been made, and it is a
12 Chi (I978).
13 A complicating factor is that our capacity to assess the magnitude of the memory feat is
constrained by the impossibility of knowing whether or not Mozart's recall of the music
really was as accurate as it has been assumed to have been. The evidence verif'ying
Mozart's accuracy at remembering rests on the statement of one singer, who had no
opportunity to assess the precise match between Mozart's version and the original score.
Minor discrepancies from the original would probably have gone undetected by Mozart's
audience.
8 Genius Explained
One way to make progress towards explaining the human attainments that
result in their creator being seen as a genius is to discover how a person
masters the knowledge and mental skills that make those accomplish-
ments possible. That is the approach taken in this book. The creative activ-
ities that are most directly involved in the construction of masterpieces will
not be neglected, but my primary aim is to trace the routes by which a few
outstanding individuals gained the capabilities their achievements have
depended upon. Charting individuals' early advances is, I think, a particu-
larly effective way to help reveal the origins of genius.
I am convinced that it is indeed possible to understand genius and its
causes. A major aim of the present book is to unearth the influences that
have helped make a few rare individuals capable of remarkable feats of
imagination and discovery. When that has been achieved, providing us
with some understanding of the contributing factors, the absurdity of
appealing to mystical forces will be evident. There is simply no need to
believe that mysteries or miracles are involved.
Our efforts to account for genius will run into numerous difficulties, of
course, if only because explaining how a young person becomes the adult
individual he or she turns out to be is never easy. But although it is pos-
sible that with those men and women whose lives and feats are the most
striking of all the barriers to understanding will be especially daunting,
and that the problems that have to be overcome in order to discover how
certain children grow up to be geniuses are vastly more challenging than
the ones involved in charting the progress towards maturity of an ordi-
nary boy or girl, there is no compelling evidence that this must be so. I am
not convinced that there is anything about the lives and achievements of
geniuses that is in principle less amenable to explanation than the lives
and achievements of other people. The children's writer Enid Blyton was
no genius, but explaining how she was able to turn out the thousands of
words she produced every single day is as much of a challenge as account-
ing for the accomplishments of authors who were far more creative. That
geniuses are special is undeniable, but the view that they are special for
reasons that are mysterious needs to be challenged.
Introduction 9
The explanatory powers innate gifts may appear to have, in the absence of
independent evidence of their existence, are similarly imaginary rather
than real.
Deciding whether or not there are solid grounds for believing that
innate gifts and talents do actually exist is a complex issue, and I explore it
Introduction 11
in Chapter 9· But unless their existence can be verified, all that is achieved
by invoking special inborn qualities as the cause of genius is to create the
kind of pseudo-explanation that attributes events to the presence of some
or other kind of magic ingredient.
A not uncommon view that is sometimes linked to the belief that genius
is a consequence of a person being endowed by nature with a special gift is
that it is only possible for someone to become a genius as a consequence
of being designed in advance to be one. That assertion is easily rebutted.
The reasons for questioning it are not unlike the arguments with which
Darwinian science has refuted the claim that the human species could
never have come into being except through some form of 'design from
above'. Darwin's theory contradicted that belief by demonstrating that it
was indeed possible for humans to be created as a consequence of evolu-
tionary processes, in the absence of any designer. Our species did not have
to be planned in advance.
Nor did the lives of individual geniuses. The processes that enable an
individual's capabilities to be acquired through learning and experience
are very different from the ones that enable new species to evolve.
However, the learning and training experiences that creative people
undergo obviate the necessity for their accomplishments to depend upon
being designed in advance just as convincingly as evolution makes design
from above unnecessary for the emergence of new species.
Before going any further, we should try to decide what a genius is.
Precisely what do we mean by the term? A straight answer to that seem-
ingly simple question is not at all easy to find. For better or worse, there is
no straightforward specification or definition of genius. Even listing the
defining attributes turns out to be impossible.
Why do these difficulties arise? The essential reason is that whilst
saying that someone is a genius appears to be a statement about the
person's qualities, it is actually not. What is really being achieved by
calling a person a genius is to acknowledge or recognise their achieve-
ments. The word 'genius' is ours, not theirs, and it is a kind of accolade
that has been bestowed upon certain individuals, usually not until well
after the person has died.
The term 'genius' has a long history, but until fairly recently the most
common use was not for describing a person but for identifying the sup-
posed reason for someone being capable of creative accomplishments. A
person's genius was seen as working in broadly the way that a poet's muse
was believed to function: genius was envisaged as a partly external spirit
that gave a helping hand. Not until the eighteenth century did the practice
of referring to a person as a genius become common. The modern
12 Genius Explained
meaning of the word comes partly from the Latin word genius which
stems from gens, meaning family, but also from the Latin ingenium, denot-
ing natural disposition or innate ability.
We can call a man a giant because he is very tall, but there is no single
attribute of a person that justifies saying that someone is a genius.
Describing a person as a genius is not like stating that he or she is tall, or
even intelligent or clever. The word is never introduced solely as a
description of an individual: it always denotes a recognition of outstand-
ing accomplishments. If you are unconvinced about that, try to think of
someone who is widely regarded as having been a genius but who never
produced highly valued creative work: I suspect that you will fail. There
have always been men and women who were exceptionally intelligent,
wise, artistic, sensitive, incisive and so on, but unless they have produced
major achievements, other people have not called them geniuses.
Whenever someone is widely regarded as having been a genius, we can be
sure that the person has made a contribution which is valued. If a baker is
someone who makes bread, a genius is a man or woman who produces
masterpieces or discoveries that greatly impress other people.
The difference between being immensely capable or creative and being
regarded as a genius is not totally unlike the difference between being
exceptionally brave and winning a medal for bravery. To win a medal, you
undoubtedly do need to be brave, but you have to be a little fortunate as
well. The bravery must have positive consequences, and it must be
observed by someone who is in a position to report it. Similarly, in order
for someone to be regarded as a genius, that person not only has to be
exceptionally able but also must achieve something that is appreciated by
others, and whether or not that happens will be partly outside the
person's control. As we shall see, success often goes not to the individual
who is most intelligent or capable in absolute terms, but to the man or
woman who happens to possess just those skills or qualities that are
needed in order to solve a particular problem at a particular moment in
history. So the accolade of genius is bestowed on a person for creating
something that others admire, rather than for being outstandingly clever.
By and large, creative individuals are more likely to be regarded as gen-
iuses if their achievements are not too recent: few of those who are widely
acknowledged to have been a genius died less than a hundred or so years
ago, Einstein being a notable exception. It also helps if the person's
different accomplishments are linked rather than being too diffuse. Sir
Richard Burton (I 82 I -90) was one of the most dazzling of all Victorians.
As well as translating the Arabian Nights into English, he led expeditions
of discovery, translated other poetry and folklore, mastered around thirty
languages, wrote poetry of his own, contributed to archaeology, ethnol-
Introduction 13
ogy, anthropology, and the study of swordsmanship, and also made dis-
coveries in botany, zoology and geology. Yet, largely because his achieve-
ments were so scattered, few have thought Burton to have been a genius,
for all his brilliance.
The fact that the word 'genius' is used more as an accolade than as a
description helps make it the useful term it is, but creates some difficulties
as well. One limitation is that introducing the term does not actually help
to account for a person's attainments. We should not be fooled into think-
ing that anything is being clarified by a statement such as 'She produced a
great novel because she was a genius'. All that is really being said here is
that the individual who wrote her great novel was a person acknowledged
to be capable of doing just that.
Another problem is that there is no objective procedure or hard-and-
fast criterion for categorising people as geniuses or non-geniuses. A
limited number of individuals are very widely regarded as having been
geniuses: Archimedes, Plato, Aristotle, Dante, Copernicus, Galileo,
Michelangelo, Newton, Darwin, Bach, Beethoven, Mozart, Shakespeare,
Rembrandt and Einstein would be placed in that category by most edu-
cated people in the English-speaking world, as might some others,
perhaps including Dickens, Schubert, George Eliot, Tolstoy,
Tchaikovsky, Balzac, van Gogh, and Flaubert. But what about Trollope,
Coleridge, Renoir, Monet, Manet, Degas, Turner, and Jane Austen? And
should we include Emily Bronte, Benjamin Franklin, Marie Curie,
Puccini, Verdi, Brunei, Charlotte Bronte, Elizabeth Gaskell, Edgar Allan
Poe, or James Joyce? As soon as we move on from a surprisingly small
number of creative people, most of whom have been dead for a long time,
agreement on who deserves to appear in a definitive list of geniuses
becomes impossible, even though there are certainly hundreds and pos-
sibly thousands of individuals for whom a serious claim can be made.
Charles Darwin's cousin Francis Galton tried to introduce a degree of
objectivity by referring to one in a million individuals as 'illustrious' and
one in four thousand as 'eminent', but in the absence of clear rules for
deciding how to select particular men and women, even that approach to
categorising outstanding people could never have worked.
To complicate things, reputations wax and wane. In common with
other circumstances in which accolades are bestowed, the matter of
whether or not a particular creative man or woman acquires the reputa-
tion of being a genius depends on factors outside that individual's
control. Chance can play a role. Had Albert Einstein or Michael Faraday
lived thirty years earlier or thirty years later than they did, the particular
skills and qualities they possessed might have had less impact.
Conversely, there are other scientists whose importance might well have
14 Genius Explained
What are geniuses like? What kinds of people are they? They are hugely
diverse, but a few characteristics are shared by virtually all of them. The
first is an intense curiosity and dedication to one's work. A second and
perhaps more surprising trait possessed by most geniuses is the capacity
to acquire a variety of different human qualities.
Geniuses are usually sure about what they want to do, single minded,
committed, and they have a firm sense of direction. They often work with
a ferocity and intensity, even when impeded by doubts and frustrations.
They also share a capacity for sustained diligence. Isaac Newton said that
he discovered the law of universal gravitation by thinking about it contin-
uously; Charles Darwin attributed much of his success to a capacity to
his way when he was twenty-two to take part in HMS Beagle's voyage hap-
pened only because Darwin had been noticed as a young man whose
judgement as well as knowledge outstripped his years. He was 'the very
man they are in search of', the Regius Professor of Botany at Cambridge
University told him. That Darwin could grasp that opportunity was only
possible because when his father proved awkwardly opposed Charles had
the wit to take the only course of action that could have induced the
parent to drop his veto. Later, it was because of Darwin's well-deserved
high reputation that when the theory of evolution finally appeared in 1859
it was sympathetically examined by his fellow scientists (rather than
encountering the instant rejection that had greeted other evolutionary
ideas) and quickly seen to be as sound as it was revolutionary.
Darwin was by no means unusual or unique in having to call upon a
variety of human qualities. Even Albert Einstein, although often seen as
an isolated thinker, leaned heavily upon his communication skills and his
capacity for friendship, and Thomas Edison would have achieved very
little were it not for his impressive organisational powers.
a great composer is that he was born with a special gift for music, the
chances are that the person will fail to discern alternative explanations. In
common with a young woman who, asked for directions to a neighbour-
ing town, told me 'You cannot get from here to [nearby] Helensburgh:
you'll have to start somewhere else,' those who are rigidly committed to
one explanation may have their minds opened up by being encouraged to
examine things from an alternative perspective.
It is helpful to think of a person's life as being like a kind of journey, one
that follows a particular route which is unique to that individual.
Biographical accounts make it possible to trace the temporal patterns of
events and consequences that take place as a person develops, and plot
the very different routes by which young people move through the time
that structures their lives. Once we gain a detailed knowledge of the
events of a person's childhood, it is likely that we will begin to discern how
and why the child gradually turned into the adult he or she eventually
became.
In tracing such a route and trying to identify the various experiences
and events that collectively make a child into an adult, an essential facet of
the person's development involves the expansion of their capabilities.
Everyone's expertise has to be acquired, and so do their likes and dislikes,
their interests and their preferences. That is just as true of geniuses as it is
of people whose accomplishments are unexceptional. Like the skills and
abilities of ordinary men and women, the more remarkable capacities of a
genius are gained more or less gradually. Especially rare or impressive
capacities build upon a foundation of more commonplace ones. When
the path can be charted towards the extraordinary attainments of, say, a
grandmaster at chess, or a concert pianist, it is usually found that the
person's itinerary through the earlier stages of expertise is broadly similar
to that of other people. The exceptional individual goes further, and may
move ahead faster, but always there is a route to be traced. There are no
gaps or inexplicable leaps. If there appears to be a gap, the chances are that
when we look closer we will discover that what is being identified is a
hiatus in our own knowledge, not a discontinuity in the person's progress.
The analogy between a person's early life and a journey or a voyage can
be misleading if pressed too far. The voyage metaphor may appear to
suggest that people forge ahead along a single track, with the implication
that the first step towards exposing the causes of genius is just a matter of
identifying a person's special capability and seeing how it was nurtured.
In reality, it is more accurate to envisage the trajectory of someone's life as
involving a number of linked but partly independent strands, all of which
contribute to the person's progress.
Tracing the events of someone's formative years involves getting close
Introduction 19
In the following chapters I shall trace the early lives of a number of gen-
iuses, attempting to discover how and why each individual became
capable of their remarkable accomplishments. Deciding which men and
women to concentrate upon could have been difficult, but two constraints
guided my choices and made selection easier. First, relatively detailed
20 Genius Explained
We enjoy being told about those geniuses who amaze us with feats that are
especially spellbinding. Without them it would be harder for people to
cling to the belief that geniuses are a special breed, akin to the magicians
and dragons and fabulous giants that populated the mythologies of past
generations. So we prefer geniuses to be sharply different from ordinary
people, and preferably a little eccentric. Einstein makes an ideal genius. It
is frustratingly hard to understand his discoveries, let alone imagine a
more conventional person emulating them. Mozart too has a special mys-
tique, fuelled by most people's inability to even imagine the possibility of
creating anything that could move us in the way his music does.
Darwin is different. Nobody doubts his theory's monumental power or
disputes its immense influence, but the principle of natural selection has
the disturbing quality of being easy to understand. At its core is a trans-
parently simple idea: in a species whose members are not identical, those
individuals that are the best adapted to their environments are the most
likely to procreate and pass on their inherited characteristics. That ele-
mentary but elegant principle accounts for the evolution of all species.
For some critics of Darwin the discovery of natural selection has too
much of the air of an accidental encounter with something that has been
waiting to be found. It is the kind of idea that, once articulated, seems to
be plain obvious as well as being right. Like the invention of the wheel, the
theory of evolution is an advance that left people asking themselves why
nobody had hit upon it earlier. As soon as Thomas Huxley learned of
Darwin's theory he wondered why he had been so stupid not to have
thought of it himself.
Detractors have found additional excuses for withholding admiration
from Darwin. Some have suggested that since artificial selection of
domestic animals had been an established fact of life for many genera-
tions before Darwin, only a small mental leap may have been needed in
order to arrive at the principle of natural selection. Other critics have
seized upon the sheer implausibility (as we see it today) of creationism -
the Genesis story that the world was created in 4004 BC - as a rival
23
24 Genius Explained
that situation did not alter until the end of the century, well after Darwin's
death, when Mendel's findings concerning the inheritance of genetic
characteristics became known. At the time Charles Darwin was grappling
with the theory of evolution, biologists knew practically nothing about the
passing-on of characteristics between generations. Today, it takes an
effort to comprehend just how little was known then about elementary
truths about inheritance that ten-year-olds now take for granted. Yet in
Darwin's lifetime nobody even knew what was actually transmitted from
one generation to another. Until the beginning of the nineteenth century
it had not even been verified that people inherited anything from their
mothers at all. It was widely believed that inheritance took place exclu-
sively via the semen of the male.
Because of these barriers to evolutionary thinking, enormous intellec-
tual effort was needed in order to arrive at the theory of evolution by
natural selection. So despite the simplicity of the principle that accounts
for evolutionary change, Darwin's achievement was far from being an
easy accomplishment.
There may be no such animal as a 'typical' genius, but most can be placed
reasonably comfortably within one or other of a number of categories, on
the basis of shared attributes. Darwin cannot. Most geniuses were
remarked upon as being precocious while still children. Darwin was not.
Many geniuses have had to struggle in order to make a living. Darwin
never had to. When we begin to investigate the events of Darwin's life that
enabled him to become capable of his great work, it quickly becomes
evident that his early circumstances were not at all like those of certain
other geniuses. Charles Darwin came from a wealthy family. There was
no lack of educational opportunities in his formative years. There was no
need for Darwin to keep striving in order to escape poverty and ignorance
and extend his horizons, since he was born privileged.
Because he was born to wealth, it was possible for Darwin to add to his
capabilities during the course of his early life in a manner that was meas-
ured and unhurried, even stately. A wealthy young person had the luxury
of being able to afford to waste time, make bad decisions, and vacillate,
before eventually getting started on a course of action that would lead in a
meaningful direction. Thus Leo Tolstoy, for example, did not have to
settle into writing until he had gambled away a fortune and arrived at
middle age. In Darwin's case, likewise, there was no necessity for an
arduous battle against the odds. And he did not lack the kinds of social
connections that could ease a young man's path. The adult Darwin was
fiercely committed to his work, and he was as diligent and determined as
anyone, but these qualities were never imposed upon him. Circumstances
The young Charles Darwin 27
did not require Darwin to be as forceful as some geniuses have had to be,
in order to stand any chance of getting ahead.
We know about numerous aspects of Darwin's life. Many letters to and
from him have been preserved. There are descriptions of him by relatives
and friends, and two versions of a brief autobiography. Reading the
Darwin correspondence is a joy, and it provides many glimpses of a
deeply sympathetic individual who cared for his family and friends and
whose warm feelings for them were strongly reciprocated. Despite that,
he was often highly anxious and not infrequently unhappy.
Enough information exists for it to be possible to trace in some detail
the course of Darwin's early years. We know about many of the events and
the people he encountered, and the books he read and the lectures he
attended. There are plenty of cues that help reveal how he experienced his
days and made use of the various opportunities that came his way as he
developed into a scientist. In Darwin's case, plotting the route of his
movement forward reveals a lengthy, very gradual, but steadily rising
course. We see a child who appears to be remarkably ordinary, lacking any
obvious talent or special gift, slowly extending his capabilities, little by
little. Eventually, a point is reached at which it becomes evident that,
without anyone noticing, the very ordinary boy has become an extraordi-
narily able young scientist, exceptionally well-positioned to take advan-
tage of any opportunity that presents itself.
Darwin was a complicated man, and discovering the ways in which he
steadily achieved those advances in his capabilities that prepared him for
his most momentous work is a less straightforward matter than charting
the routes via which others have pushed themselves ahead in their
different ways. There were always parts of himself that Darwin kept
hidden from others. He was not secretive except when he had good
reasons for hiding things, but he carefully guarded his privacy. Having
discovered as a child that keeping his own counsel was a good way to
protect himself from the intrusive demands of adults and older sisters, he
was reluctant to correct people when their perceptions of him were inac-
curate. In particular, during the crucial years immediately preceding the
voyage on HMS Beagle that was to establish his reputation as a mature
scientist, Darwin encouraged his relatives to persist in an increasingly
inaccurate view of him and regard him as being less determined, less
mature, and less committed to science than he really was, and consider-
ably more naive and indecisive.
Even with his sisters, whose love and affection he enjoyed and valued,
the good-natured Darwin was happy for them to continue regarding him
as their error-prone and somewhat ineffectual younger brother, well past
the point when it was clear to those who knew of his work that he was
28 Genius Explained
2 Note from John Maurice Herbert to Charles Darwin, early May 1831. P. 122 The corre-
spondence of Charles Darwin, U!lume I. Burkhardt & Smith (1985).
The young Charles Darwin 29
3 Letter from Charles Darwin to John Maurice Herbert, I3 September r828. Pp. 64-65,
The correspondence of Charles Darwin, Ullume I. Burkhardt & Smith (r985).
30 Genius Explained
tice, but the bulk of his large income came from mortgaged property: he
was a kind of one-man building society. The Darwins were gentry, like
other wealthy and well-connected families living in the country, although
lacking a large estate they could not be described as landed gentry in the
usual sense.
Unhappily, Susannah Darwin died when Charles was only eight, after a
painful illness. The adult Darwin had only a few memories of his mother.
In the years after her death Charles' older sisters had rarely talked about
her: it was too painful for them. Some biographers have suggested that an
outcome of this understandable if regrettable reaction was to prevent
Charles from properly mourning his mother and coming to terms with
the death, thereby contributing to the frequent physical maladies and
feelings of depression he experienced as an adult. The evidence is too
sketchy for that interpretion of events to be either confirmed or refuted.
Even before his mother's death, two of Charles' sisters, Caroline (born
in 1800) and Susan (born in 1803) were helping with his schooling and
that of his younger sister Catherine. The older sisters had a strong interest
in the new ideas about education that were being promoted by educa-
tional thinkers like Guizot and practitioners such as Pestalozzi. 4 Later
they set up a small infant school of their own. From many of their letters
to Charles that have been preserved, as well as numerous letters from
Charles to them, it is clear that Caroline and Susan always cared very
deeply about Charles and his younger sister Catherine (the youngest
child in the family, born eighteen months after Charles). There were
times in Charles' early childhood when he chafed at his older sisters'
attentions, and their letters display a certain well-intentioned fussiness,
which he sometimes found irritating. But Charles was always grateful to
Caroline and Susan for their kindliness in his childhood.
Darwin believed that his younger sister Catherine had been quicker
than him and more advanced for her age. Being the fifth of sixth children
is not an enviable position within a family, and Darwin recalled occa-
sional childhood incidents in which he behaved outrageously in order to
gain attention, and then felt foolish after getting into trouble and receiv-
ing the inevitable reprimand. But even if he sometimes wanted more
attention than he was getting, he never felt unloved. As Caroline recalled
as an old lady in her seventies, far from being a naughty or stupid boy, he
was 'particularly affectionate, tractable and sweet tempered, and my
father had the highest opinion of his understanding and intelligence'. 5
That was not the invented sentiment of an elderly person: much earlier,
4 For differing views concerning the effects on Charles Darwin of his mother's early death,
see the biographies by Bowlby (r990) and Browne (r995).
5 Quoted in Brent (r98r), p. 23
32 Genius Explained
she had told him in a touching letter written when he was a seventeen-
year-old student at Edinburgh University, 'I think when you & Catherine
were little children & I was always with you or thinking about you was the
happiest part of my life and I dare say always will be.' 6
Darwin first went to school early in 1817, when he was just eight. A rec-
ollection by a schoolmate suggests that Darwin's mother was taking a
close interest in his education, despite the fact that her health was seri-
ously affected by the illness that killed her later that year. The schoolmate,
who was later to be one of the many naturalists with whom Darwin corre-
sponded, said that Darwin, who had brought a plant from home for the
small garden of the school, told him that his mother 'had been teaching
him how by looking into the interior of a blossom he could ascertain the
name of the plant'. 7
A year later Darwin was moved to Shrewsbury School, a long-estab-
lished institution which under its headmaster, Dr Butler, was thought to
be among the best dozen schools in England at the time. Darwin was a
pupil there for seven years. Despite the fact that the school was very close
to Darwin's home, his father made the sensible decision that Charles
would board, and thereby avoided placing him in the awkward position of
being a day boy in a boarding school. This arrangement worked out well.
Unlike most of the other boarders Darwin often had time to make brief
visits home, and his early days at the school were made a little easier by
the fact that his brother Erasmus, four years older than Charles, was
already a pupil there.
Despite these advantages Darwin did not shine at Shrewsbury School.
His schoolwork was never more than average. He later became highly
skilled at shooting, but at school he never made an impression at any
sport. He was certainly not disliked: schoolfellows recalled him as kindly,
friendly, gentle and popular, and were intrigued by his knowledge of
natural history. But his formal school achievements were not in the least
distinguished.
In the early education of a modern scientist we would expect to find
some relationship between the individual's scientific progress and his suc-
cesses in other subjects. Nowadays, it would be surprising to find a young
scientist as enthusiastic and capable as Darwin making little impression at
school. But matters were arranged differently in Darwin's day, because
science as a school subject simply did not exist. It seems remarkable to us
that a hundred years after Newton's death, at a time when Faraday,
Ampere and others were producing the stream of discoveries that would
make electrical power a practical possibility, and many other scientists
knowledge. The butterflies and beetles that fascinated him at ten still fas-
cinated him at twenty, albeit for different reasons. He had gradually
become something of an amateur expert, but without ever having had to
make a sudden commitment to studying natural history as an academic
subject. His activities as a naturalist in training sometimes involved
fatigue and hardship, and yet his efforts would always have been directly
fuelled by his own purposes and interests. Unlike a young person from an
impoverished background, the young Darwin was lucky enough never to
be in a position in which the vague and distant goal of self-improvement
had to serve as the main incentive for persevering at hard studies in unfa-
vourable conditions.
More holidays in Wales followed, offering plenty of opportunities for
collecting. At eleven (in 1820), Charles had been on a riding tour with
his fifteen-year-old brother, Erasmus. The year after that, once more
with Erasmus and also on this occasion with two of his Wedgwood
cousins, he went on a more ambitious tour in which they covered 250
miles in ten days, going as far as Bangor. From there they went to see one
of the engineering wonders of the day, Thomas Telford's suspension
bridge that was being built across the Menai Straits to link the island of
Anglesea with the mainland. (It was completed in 1826, and was joined
in 1850 by a railway bridge that had been built by George Stephenson's
son Robert, who spent a brief period at Edinburgh University in 1822-3,
two years before Darwin arrived there.) In the following summer Darwin
enjoyed at least two further riding tours. He was now thirteen, and
beginning to find delight in beautiful scenery, a source of pleasure
throughout his life.
Charles' brother Erasmus was a companion on most of his childhood
holidays. Considering the four-year gap in their ages Erasmus was gener-
ous to Charles, lending him books and including him in the older
brother's activities. Most biographers have taken at face value a comment
by Charles that because their minds and tastes were very different he did
not think that he owed much to Erasmus intellectually. The two brothers
were indeed very different, but Erasmus had a much bigger influence on
Charles' development than has been recognised. When Charles was
sixteen he and Erasmus had a year together at Edinburgh University,
where they were both studying medicine; they lodged together, read the
same books, and they not only spent a good deal of time in each other's
company but shared a number of interests and enthusiasms. Even more
importantly for Darwin's early progress in science, when only thirteen, he
and Erasmus (who was then studying at Cambridge) set up a simple
chemistry laboratory in a toolshed in the Darwins' garden. Ostensibly,
Erasmus was in charge and giving the orders, with Charles a mere assist-
The young Charles Darwin 37
ant. But the actual day-to-day arrangements for setting everything up and
running the experiments were left entirely in Charles' hands.
A number of surviving letters from Erasmus to Charles make it clear
how this cooperation at a distance worked in practice. What strikes the
reader most forcefully is that Erasmus never doubted for one minute that
his thirteen-year-old brother was sufficiently competent and responsible
to make all the necessary arrangements. Some of these were highly
complex or demanded considerable initiative on Charles's part; others
required him to become involved in negotiations or search for abstruse
information. Charles was being trusted to get on with the job of imple-
menting some fairly elaborate plans. By putting Charles in a position in
which it was taken for granted that he was mature and capable enough to
undertake some complicated tasks, Erasmus was not only recognising his
younger brother's competence but also, perhaps unwittingly, doing much
to help Charles learn how to act independently. Erasmus encouraged
Charles to develop an ability to organise things on his own. For a boy who
was the fourth of five children, and whose siblings were too prone to cast
him in the role of the immature younger brother, the opportunity that he
was being given by Erasmus to take responsibility for the organisation and
running of a potentially dangerous laboratory would have been a godsend.
At the same time, Charles was also receiving a valuable chance to learn
about practical chemistry. Whatever Dr Butler might have thought, for
Darwin all this was 'the best part of my education at school' . 11
Because he was a sensible and agreeable young person, neither too con-
ceited nor too shy to be a good companion, Darwin usually got on well
with other people. That enabled him to take advantage of openings that
would have not been on offer to someone less personable. The young
Darwin was not a charmer - men were not dazzled by his conversation
and women did not swoon in his company- but people did seem to enjoy
being with him. Adults found him likeable. That was partly because he
was sensitive to other people's needs and prepared to listen to them, as
well as exhibiting a youthful enthusiasm that was already coupled with the
lively curiosity that was to stay with him until his death. Even his
Wedgwood uncle (named Josiah like his more famous father), who was
considered to be a taciturn and rather forbidding man, took a keen inter-
est in Charles' activities. Consequently, he was able to be of enormous
help to his nephew some years later, at a crucial point in Charles' life. It
was Uncle Josiah who, in 1831, persuaded Darwin's father to drop his
opposition to the idea of Charles taking part in the five-year voyage of
HMSBeagle.
Darwin learned to shoot (an activity which for ten years or so was one
of his main sources of pleasure) from another local landowner, William
Mostyn Owen, whose estate at Woodhouse, about twelve miles from
Shrewsbury, was regularly visited by members of the Darwin family. The
Darwin correspondence includes sixteen letters written to Charles by
Owen's high-spirited youngest daughter, Fanny, the first love of Darwin's
life. Some of these letters contain messages from William Owen himself,
saying how he is missing Charles or looking forward to his next visit.
Another adult to be impressed by Charles' lively curiosity and enthusiasm
was the historian Sir James Mackintosh, who Charles met when staying at
Maer, the Wedgwood family's estate. 'There is something in that man that
interests me', said Mackintosh, who was then writing his History of
England, but died a few years later, in 1832, before he could discover just
how percipient his remark had been. 12 And later, when Darwin was a uni-
versity student, at both Edinburgh and Cambridge, he encountered a
number of distinguished scientists who were sufficiently impressed by
him to take him seriously, and found him congenial enough to deserve
their companionship. As a consequence, he spent a good deal of time at
both universities in the company of active scientists who were the ideal
mentors for him: these men had an immense influence on Darwin's
future career.
The young Darwin was often away from home. As well as boarding at
school and going for holiday expeditions in the vacations, he often went to
stay with his Wedgwood cousins at Maer, close to Stoke-on-Trent in the
Potteries region towards the centre of England, about twenty miles from
Darwin's home, or at Woodhouse with the Owen family. Apart from the
various experiences travel provided and the opportunities to see relatives
and make new friends, spending time away from his own home had the
further desirable outcome for Darwin of getting him away from the some-
times oppressive company of his father.
Dr Robert Darwin was no ogre. He was considerate to his patients, fair
to his tenants and kind to his servants. He had suffered some tragic losses:
his mother had died when he was only four, his older brother died from
septicaemia as a medical student at Edinburgh after cutting his finger at a
post-mortem on a child, and he was devastated when his wife Susannah
died. He had an active and enquiring mind and numerous interests, and
became a Fellow of the Royal Society, an influential scientific institution
in Britain. He was in many respects an excellent parent, caring deeply
about all his children, and his decisions were usually wise. He was
devoted to Charles. As Caroline Darwin recorded, 'My father was very
fond of him & even when he was a little boy of 6 or 7, however bustled &
overtired, often had Charles with him when he was dressing, to teach him
some little thing such as the almanack - and Charles used to be so eager
to be down in time. Charles does not seem to have known half how much
my father loved him.' 13
But Dr Darwin was not an easy parent to live with. Tall and fat, with an
incongruously high-pitched voice, he was somewhat overbearing, with a
tendency to hold forth at great length. When not listening to his own
voice he liked to know what everyone else in the room was saying. A con-
sequence of this was that younger members of the family, who might have
preferred to relax and gossip over a quiet game of cards, were forced to
endure long evenings of stiff conversation. Family and guests complained
that the atmosphere in the Darwin house was not so free or spontaneous
as that at Maer, the Wedgwood home, or at Woodhouse, where their
friends the Owens lived.
In a wealthy family such as the Darwins' it was possible for all the chil-
dren to get away from time to time on visits to other houses, and Charles
also took advantage of the fact that in the 1820s it was easier for a son than
a daughter to find reasons for being absent from home. He was usually
able to maintain a cordial relationship with his father, but he soon dis-
cerned that doing that was easier at a distance. From the time he was
sixteen or so his periods of staying at home were usually brief.
Some biographers under the influence of Freud have been convinced
that Darwin's professed love and affection for his father concealed an
unconscious hatred. That seems unlikely. Warm feelings predominated,
although Darwin's view of his father was balanced and realistic rather
than idealised. Charles Darwin was perfectly aware that his father could
be an awkward customer and a bit of a bully, with plenty of faults, and had
on a few occasions been unjust towards himself. But Darwin also knew
his father as a kindly and deeply caring parent, a man of many enthu-
siasms and a source of amusing anecdotes. As an adult, Darwin chose to
make numerous visits to his father that could easily have been avoided. In
his Autobiography, written when he was getting old, Darwin writes about
his father with transparent good spirits. He rambles on and on, as one
story reminds him of another, dragging himself away from his pleasurable
memories of his parent only when the time to return to the narrative of his
own life is well overdue.
There were family resemblances and shared concerns within the
Darwin family, the most striking being the deep interest of both Charles
Darwin and his grandfather Erasmus Darwin in evolution and its possible
with and answer some questions about their activity at the time, for
example, what they were doing, where they were, whether they were alone
or in company, and so on. One of the questions asked was whether the
person was enjoying whatever it was that he or she was doing. Another
question asked how alert the person felt at the time when the bleeper
sounded.
When Csikszentmihalyi asked the adolescents how they felt about
activities other than studying, such as talking to friends or watching tele-
vision, their responses were generally positive, and differences in family
background had little effect on the answers. But there was a very different
pattern of responses if the bleeper sounded when an adolescent was stud-
ying. First, answers to the questions about enjoyment and alertness
tended to be negative, with participants usually reporting that they were
not enjoying studying, and not feeling alert or attentive. Second, on these
occasions the answers given were strongly affected by the participants'
family backgrounds. One group was very different from the others; that
was the group whose family backgrounds were both supportive and stim-
ulating. These young people, but not the others, were generally positive
about studying. They enjoyed it more than the others did, and when they
were engaged in study on their own they reported being much more
attentive and alert.
Essentially, certain of the young people observed by Csikzentmihalyi
were perceiving their studying activities very differently from the other
teenagers, and the differences in the way they experienced studying were
closely related to their home backgrounds. Perhaps this was because
young people whose families were both stimulating and supportive had
learned to get on with the job of working at a study activity and had
acquired the habit of doing so. Because they were happier about studying
they devoted more time to it and because they were more alert and
attentive they learned more. Those individuals who were not so well-
prepared for practice and study activities by their family backgrounds
may well have caught up later, but temporarily at least, they were at a real
disadvantage.
The findings of Csikszentmihalyi's investigations encourage us to
believe that Darwin's supportive and stimulating family background
would have made him better equipped than most boys of his age to study
on his own. And released from the detested school, Darwin did indeed
throw himself into his studies at Edinburgh. Settling in was not difficult,
and with all the family connections there were plenty of invitations to dine
in the evenings. Edinburgh was (and still is) an attractive city and an excit-
ing place to be. Darwin conscientiously attended a number of lecture
courses, making detailed notes, and back in the comfortable lodgings
44 Genius Explained
which he and Erasmus had found for themselves they devoured large
numbers of books, borrowing more volumes from the university library
than most students that year. Erasmus, when he first heard in the previous
winter about Charles' new plans, had urged that they should 'read like
horses', and that is just what they did. They studied books on medicine
and anatomy, and other scientific topics, and wrestled with Newton's
Optiks as well as lighter fare such as Boswell's Life of Samuel Johnson.
Diligent as Darwin was in his first year at Edinburgh, it was not long
before he found himself wondering whether he was really suited to medi-
cine. A number of bad experiences prompted his doubts, and despite his
initial enthusiasm, he could not hide from himself the fact that the lecture
courses bored him stiff. With the exception of a series of lectures on
chemistry given by Thomas Hope, whose teaching was universally
admired, none of the lectures Darwin attended gripped him. He was also
put off medicine by watching a couple of surgical operations. Operations,
which at that time were conducted without anaesthetics, were horrifying.
Screaming patients writhed against the straps holding them down as the
blood-soaked surgeons rushed to finish their work. It did not help that
one of those operations that Darwin witnessed was on a child. He found
the experience unbearable and could not stay to the end.
Two further discoveries fuelled Darwin's concern about whether he
had made the right decision in choosing medicine. First, he had become
aware (probably from talking to Erasmus) just how wealthy his family
was. If the thought that he would need to earn an income had influenced
his choice of profession, he now knew that doing that was not strictly nec-
essary. Second, Charles Darwin was finding his mind increasingly drawn
away from medicine and towards the scientific disciplines that border
natural history. The habit of observing, and then raising fundamental
questions, had never left him. He noted in his diary on 18 January 1826
that in the evening he had seen a hedge sparrow creeping into a hole in a
tree, and asked himself, 'Where do most birds roost in winter?'. On 9
February he recorded catching a sea mouse. He noticed that it tried to
coil itself into a ball when its mouth was touched. At this point his obser-
vations betray the fact that by now he has come a long way from being a
naive amateur. He remarks that one authority states that the sea mouse
has two feelers, whereas Linnaeus says it has four of them. Darwin had
never stopped being interested in collecting, and now that he was in a uni-
versity environment where he was exposed to the enthusiasms of amateur
natural historians and could see at first hand the work of serious scientists
who were systematically describing and classifying natural phenomena,
he could hardly resist having his own interests dragged back in that direc-
tion. Among the books he had borrowed from the library during the pre-
The young Charles Darwin 45
Without his elder brother to constrain him, Charles was always out and
about, meeting people, walking with friends, attending the meetings of
societies, and, when not otherwise engaged, frequently observing and col-
lecting, and dissecting his specimens. He was keen to learn how to stuff
birds, and for two months he took daily lessons in taxidermy from a freed
black slave, who entertained him with intriguing accounts oflife in exotic
places such as South American tropical rain-forests. The accounts of
daily activities that are found in Darwin's correspondence show that, at
least in the first half of his life, the popular image of him as a withdrawn
individual who avoided company and was often unwell is very wide of the
mark. Until he was well over thirty, Darwin was almost always vigorously
active and outgoing, and usually robustly healthy.
During that year, Darwin became involved in a number of societies at
Edinburgh. He went to meetings of the Wernerian Natural History
Society, where on one occasion he listened to the great American natural-
ist and artist James Audubon. He also heard a report, in the course of a
lecture by Robert Grant, of some of the first original discoveries made by
himself, the speaker's 'zealous young friend Mr Charles Darwin'. He was
especially active in another society, the Plinian Society, which attracted a
wide variety of students and others. Among its members were a number
of radically-minded individuals who challenged religious orthodoxy,
believed in democracy, and thought that the world was created by physi-
cal causes rather than the events recorded in the Bible. These people were
enthusiastic about evolutionary views. By then Darwin was already aware
of the possibility that species evolve rather than being suddenly created in
their final form, having read (probably during the previous summer) his
grandfather Erasmus's Zoomania. He was soon elected to the Plinian
Society's council, and became friendly with some of the most radical
members. These included William Browne, a lively heretic who was
active in anti-clerical politics, and William Greg, who held the daring
notion that the brains of lower animals were not fundamentally different
from those of humans, and challenged the dogma that morality came
from God, not nature.
All this questioning of the established order must have seemed heady
stuff to the seventeen-year-old Darwin, but his contact with the Plinian
Society gave him his first sharp warning that expressing radical or anti-
clerical views could be a dangerous activity. Despite the fact that religious
persecution had abated somewhat since Galileo's time, speaking too
wildly or too freely could still have dangerous consequences. The particu-
lar event Darwin witnessed was a minor one, involving censorship rather
than persecution, but it presaged more sinister acts of oppression that he
was to encounter later, at Cambridge University. All that happened was
The young Charles Darwin 47
that following a much debated talk by Browne, who argued for the mate-
rialist view that mind and matter are related (contradicting the traditional
Cartesian wisdom), with consciousness being an outcome of the activity
of the brain, some members of the society were so concerned about the
possible consequences of these views having been expressed that it was
decided to strike the record of Browne's proposition from the minutes. It
is not known who did this, or what were the precise reasons underlying
the decision to make the deletions, but for one reason or another that
repressive course of action was judged to be necessary.
Much of Darwin's time at Edinburgh that year was spent working in the
field as a practical natural historian, observing, collecting, and gradually
gaining more and more of the knowledge and skills that distinguish the
expert from the mere hobbyist. Darwin was tireless in his searches for
species. He walked along the coast, and sometimes persuaded the cap-
tains of trawlers dredging the ocean bottom to let him accompany them.
He was looking for sea-creatures, corals, sponges, polyps, leeches, sea-
pens and molluscs, and indeed almost any kind of marine life.
This vigorous activity coincided with his getting to know the prominent
zoologist Robert Grant, an expert on sponges who became one of the
most influential of Darwin's teachers. Grant was a happy choice as
Darwin's first real scientific mentor, because as well as being an excellent
scientist whose own research quickly engaged Darwin's interest, he was a
man whose broader views about the origins ofliving things were unusual
in being strongly inclined towards evolution. Grant, who had studied in
Paris, was an admirer of Lamarck, the great French naturalist (still living
in 1827) who was famous for having produced an influential evolutionary
theory. Lamarck's theory is based upon the assumption that acquired
characteristics can be passed down through inheritance. We now know
that this cannot happen, but in an era when no knowledge of genetics
existed Lamarck's key assumption was not readily disproved. (In the
Soviet Union a variety of Lamarckism was still being advocated by the
biologist T. Lysencko in the 1950s.) Robert Grant had also read and
admired the discussion of evolution in Erasmus Darwin's Zoonomia, an
experience that may have contributed to his willingness to be helpful to
Charles. Grant was an evolutionist and a freethinker at a time when few
scientists challenged the prevailing conservatism. His views would have
provoked Darwin to begin thinking seriously about the evolutionary pos-
sibilities that had been hinted at in Zoonomia.
By guiding Darwin's activities and directing them towards realistic
scientific goals, Grant helped to ensure that the energy of the young
enthusiast was not wasted. At the same time, Grant, whose own dedica-
tion to the practical task of finding specimens was unsparing, gave
48 Genius Explained
It was decided that Charles would study for a Bachelor of Arts degree
at Cambridge University. Cambridge, because of its links with the
Church, was the appropriate institution for a British student intending to
become ordinated. Darwin had left Edinburgh early in 1827, but to
prepare himself for his course at Cambridge he needed tuition in some of
the subjects at which he was weak, and he did not arrive at the university
until the beginning of 1828, around his nineteenth birthday. During the
three years he spent at Cambridge Darwin did enough work in non-
science subjects to pass his examinations, but as at Edinburgh, much of
his energy went into natural history.
At Cambridge, Darwin enjoyed many of the pleasures that appealed to
affluent and well-bred young men of his generation. He shot (and con-
sumed) many kinds of birds, hunted, rode, drank, and enjoyed the antics
of dining societies. Had it not been for the habitual self-discipline that he
had already acquired in his two years at Edinburgh it is quite possible that
these diversions would have taken over his life. But by now, thanks largely
to the experiences of those Edinburgh years, Darwin was sufficiently
mature to be able to organise his time properly and make sure that during
his three years at Cambridge he did not just enjoy himself with the friends
he made there, but succeeded at his examinations as well, and also found
plenty of opportunities for pursuing natural history.
As soon as he arrived at Cambridge Darwin became friendly with a
second cousin, William Darwin Fox, who shared his passion for insects
and was enthusiastically building a collection of beetles. Being a few years
older than Darwin, Fox left Cambridge earlier, but they maintained their
contacts through frequent letters. From Darwin's surviving letters to Fox,
which are peppered with jokey remarks, it is clear that he gained tremen-
dous enjoyment from entomology (the study of insects). The content of
Darwin's letters to Fox and many of his friends jumps unselfconsciously
from the personal to the scientific and back again. Here there is a sharp
contrast with the correspondence of some other great scientists of the
time, such as Michael Faraday. Faraday kept his personal and profes-
sional lives separate, but Darwin's letters often contain a delightful mix of
warm messages of friendship and scholarly information or requests. For
instance, one letter to Fox, written from home in the summer of 1828,
begins with the lament 'I am dying by inches, from not having any body to
talk to about insects', quickly switches to technical information accompa-
nied by three well executed sketches of beetles, then jumps to the
comment 'I am constantly saying, "I do wish Fox was here"' and repeats a
previous invitation, adding that 'My Father desired me to say, that he
should be at anytime most happy to see you'. The letter next moves to a
technical description for another paragraph, mentioning that Darwin has
50 Genius Explained
seen 'the Cocc: bipunctata (or dispar) 4 or 5 in actu coitus with a black
one with 4 red marks', then asks Fox's pardon 'for sending such a very
selfish letter', disarmingly requests Fox to 'remember I am your pupil',
asks about Fox's plans for the summer, and concludes with plans for their
next meeting and queries about family matters. 15
Correspondence with a friend often continued for many years. In
William Darwin Fox's case, although he and Darwin rarely met after Fox
left Cambridge, the two were still writing to each other in the 1870s.
When Fox died in 1880 Darwin was too unwell to attend his cousin's
funeral, but he poignantly remarked that he could still picture his face and
imagine his voice as clearly as if he were present in the room.
Something else that is evident from the contents of Darwin's letters to
his cousin, which often concerned birds, beetles, and other species they
were interested in, is that Charles Darwin at this time usually knew what
he was talking about. Fox must have been taken aback at first to discover
that by the time Darwin arrived at Cambridge, this young man who had
only just reached his nineteenth birthday was already something of an
expert and almost certainly better-informed than Fox himself, who as
well as being older than Darwin was devoted to natural history and quite
an ambitious collector. Darwin himself was building up a substantial col-
lection, and was prepared to devote considerable time to it, and money
too when that was necessary. As in his second year at Edinburgh, he was
constantly going on long walks and expeditions, searching for new and
rare specimens. He was furious to discover that a supplier whom he was
paying to provide beetles was letting another collector have the first pick,
and reported to his cousin, 'accordingly, we have made our final adieus,
my part in the affecting scene consisted in telling him he was a d---d
rascal, & signifying I should kick him down the stairs if he ever appeared
in my rooms again'. 16
As we have seen, Darwin never had difficulty finding mature scientists
who were willing to take him seriously. In Darwin's correspondence
during his three years at Cambridge there are increasing signs that his
skills were admired by his friends, who already included a number of
competent scientists. By the time he began his lifelong friendship in early
1830 with Professor John Henslow, the most influential of Darwin's
mentors, it was clear that Henslow already regarded him as a very promis-
ing young biologist.
John Henslow, Professor of Botany at Cambridge University, is rightly
15 Letter from Charles Darwin to William Darwin Fox, I2 June I828, p. 56, The
Correspondence of Charles Darwin, vol. I, p. s6. Burkhardt & Smith (I98s).
16 Letter from Charles Darwin to William Darwin Fox, April I, I829. The Correspondence of
Charles Darwin, Vol. I correspondence vol. I, 8r. Burkhardt & Smith (I985).
The young Charles Darwin 51
17 The correspondence of Charles Darwin, U!l. 9, p. 133, Burkhardt & Smith (1994).
52 Genius Explained
and the 'serious' scientists back in Europe. When many months went by
without any ofHenslow's letters reaching him (an understandable conse-
quence of the difficulty of communicating by post with a small sailing
craft circumnavigating the globe) he initially became more and more
anxious that he was failing at his duties. Only after Henslow's supportive
and encouraging letters finally reached him did Darwin start to become
sufficiently assured to start thinking of himself as a theoretical scientist in
his own right. 20
It would be wrong to assume that had it not been for the Beagle Darwin
would have become just another country parson who dabbled in natural
history. One can only speculate on what might have happened, but by the
time the opportunity to join the voyage arrived Darwin was set to make a
definite impact, albeit one that might otherwise have been less theoretical
and not nearly so important to science. Even without the Beagle, Darwin's
eventual reputation might well have been on a par with the well-regarded
natural scientists of the day, such as Henslow or Hooker.
It is not even true that had it not been for the Beagle Darwin would have
been unable to travel in other continents. At Edinburgh, he talked at
length to the freed slave who taught him taxidermy and who had accom-
panied the naturalist Charles Waterton on his travels in South America.
Darwin was also hugely impressed by Humboldt's account of that conti-
nent. He copied out Humboldt's descriptions of Tenerife, with its lush
vegetation and volcanic scenery, and enthusiastically read them aloud to
his friends. Like a number of nineteenth-century naturalists who were
able to go on extensive voyages, including Charles Lyell, Thomas Huxley,
Alfred Wallace, Henry Bates and Joseph Hooker, Darwin was eager for
the experiences that such a voyage could provide, and wealthy enough to
be relatively unconcerned about the cost. In the summer of 1831 Darwin
threw himself into making plans for a scientific voyage to Tenerife. The
intention was that Darwin would be accompanied by Henslow and
several other naturalists. To Darwin's dismay, by early August it was
apparent that the plan would have to be postponed, but there was no
doubt about the seriousness of Darwin's intentions. What this episode
demonstrates is that even if the Beagle opportunity had not arisen,
Darwin would have devoted considerable efforts to finding a way to go on
some other scientific voyage to the tropics.
The question has to be asked: Why, when the invitation to accompany
the Beagle first arrived towards the end of 1831, was his father so firmly
opposed to the project? If Charles Darwin really was a seriously commit-
ted and well-prepared young naturalist by then, why was Dr Robert less
20 Sulloway (1985).
54 Genius Explained
than enthusiastic? As we have seen, for all his faults Dr Robert had usually
displayed considerable sensitivity and judgement on matters connected
with Charles's future. So why not now? The answer to that question is
interesting because it throws light on aspects of Darwin's character that
contributed to his genius.
There was not just one reason for Dr Robert's initial opposition to the
Beagle plan. Even today, despite the telephones, faxes, cheap travel and
electronic mail that make long-distance travel a far less daunting prospect
than it used to be, a parent can find it alarming to be suddenly confronted
with the prospect of a twenty-two-year-old son disappearing for a period
of years to the other side of the world. That would appear especially wor-
rying if the father had recently been very ill (as Dr Darwin had, in the pre-
vious November) and the son (as in Darwin's case) had never left Britain
except for a short trip to Paris in the company of an older sister. On those
grounds alone, there is nothing outrageously insensitive about Dr
Robert's initial negativity. But there was another important reason,
namely that neither Dr Robert nor the other members of Darwin's imme-
diate family quite appreciated how accomplished and serious and com-
mitted a scientist Charles had become. As we have seen, they had
persisted with a view of him that was somewhat out-of-date. They had
experienced few signs of the ferocious determination with which he could
pursue his interests. They were still seeing him as the younger son, able
enough, but somewhat immature and aimless, and still in need of the
guidance of his family, whether to correct his spelling errors or to keep
him from spending most of his time shooting, hunting, and drinking with
his friends. To them the prospective voyage of the Beagle might well have
seemed too much like a dangerous jaunt, perhaps not entirely respect-
able, and certainly a hazardous prospect for the vulnerable and impres-
sionable (in their eyes) young Charles.
Not being fully aware of his growing reputation as a serious naturalist,
Charles's father and his sisters could not at first perceive that, dangerous
and uncertain as the prospective voyage undoubtedly was, it was also a
marvellous scientific opportunity. Nor did they appreciate the extent to
which Charles was being honoured by being chosen. They were not aware
that the invitation represented an acknowledgement of the remarkable
promise that influential scientists had discerned in this particular young
man.
In the event, it only took a briefletter from Dr Robert's brother-in-law
to make Darwin's father understand the true situation, and see that his
reaction had been too hasty and rapidly change his mind. In this, as in
other matters, the much-maligned Dr Robert was neither foolish nor
inflexible. But why was it that his family's perception of Charles's pros-
The young Charles Darwin 55
at all, does not detract from the fact the years on the Beagle were
immensely influential ones. As well as the rich diet of experiences it
exposed him to, the voyage gave Darwin time for prolonged and uncon-
strained ruminations. His careful reading of Charles Lyell's newly-pub-
lished Principles of Geology, which established beyond reasonable doubt
that the physical world in its present form came into being as a conse-
quence of gradual change, would have removed any lingering belief in the
Genesis story of the earth's beginnings. Darwin, who had already been
encouraged to speculate about evolution and its possible causes, was able
to see that if Genesis was so utterly mistaken about the causes of changes
in the physical landscape, it could hardly be right about the origins of
flora and fauna.
The achievements of Darwin's later career were to make heavy
demands upon his determination and his courage, as well as involving
immense intellectual struggles, but by then he had already acquired a
degree of commitment to his work that kept him wedded to the scientific
problems that faced him. The fact that following the return to Britain of
HMS Beagle towards the end of 1836 he received much praise and recog-
nition for what he had achieved during the voyage cannot have been
unhelpful. Certainly, the years immediately following his return were a
period in which he was furiously active as a scientist. Even then he was
distracted by a variety of false leads and unfruitful ideas that delayed his
reaching the conclusions that formed the nub of evolutionary theory. It
was in 1838, when Darwin was twenty-seven and two years after his
return, that it became clear to him that natural selection was the key to
evolution, the insight which created a profound revolution in human
knowledge when it was eventually published in 1859.
3 The long ascent of George Stephenson
Until the 1830s, travelling conditions in England had improved little since
the Norman invasion of 1066, although recent improvements in road sur-
faces allowed the very fastest coaches from London to reach Exeter, 200
miles to the west, in 16 hours and Manchester, even further in the north,
in 26 hours. Then, quite suddenly, the railways arrived. People rushed to
take advantage of the new trains, quickly jettisoning their fears about this
alarming way of being moved around at amazing velocities. Men and
women who a few years earlier would have hooted with laughter at the
very idea of humans being transported at much above 10 mph were regu-
larly travelling at four times that speed. Charles Darwin's 1838 diary finds
him grumbling like a present-day commuter about late trains and missed
connections. The British statesman W. E. Gladstone, another frequent
traveller by train, confided to his diary a stream of similar complaints
about waiting for late connections and enduring smoky carriages.
Railway travel created exciting new possibilities for those who could
afford it. On the afternoon ofhis wedding in Shrewsbury in January 1839
Charles Darwin and his bride caught a train to London, arriving the same
evening. By the early 1840s there were already almost 2,000 miles of
railway lines in Britain, and even Queen Victoria enthused about rail. In
1849 a new line through the northern wilds of East Yorkshire allowed
Charlotte Bronte (whose brother Branwell had worked for a railway
company) to take her ailing youngest sister Anne for what was intended to
be a holiday in the seaside resort of Scarborough. Sadly and unexpect-
edly, Anne died there within a few days. On a dark evening in 1851, a
despairing George Eliot enacted a scene that would not have been out of
place in Brief Encounter. She begged the man she loved to explain his feel-
ings as they waited for her train. The train 'whirled her away very sad,' he
recorded, just after he had warned her 'that I felt great affection for her
but that I loved E. [his mistress] and S. [his wife] also'. The future author
of Middlemarch could not foresee that she would soon be setting up house
with a different married lover, but she did perceive that with railways now
commonplace, the world of her childhood -Jane Austen's unchanging
England -had been left behind.
58
The long ascent of George Stephenson 59
patent for cast-iron rails. It had been agreed that Stephenson's rails would
be used for the new railway, bringing him a profit worth more than his
yearly salary. So when he was asked to comment on the idea of introduc-
ing a different kind of rail, made of wrought iron and costing over twice
the price of his own, an unenthusiastic response could have been antici-
pated. But Stephenson insisted on using the new 'malleable' wrought-
iron rails, despite the financial loss to himself. He stressed their advantage
of being unbreakable, unlike rails made of cast-iron, and he persuaded his
employers that the reduction in repairs and delays would justify the extra
expense.
George Stephenson's early experiences were far from being ones that
would normally be associated with the acquisition of exceptional exper-
tise, they did nevertheless give him some uncommon chances to gain
some of the special capabilities that would have helped a young person to
become an engineer. And Stephenson's everyday experiences also pre-
sented him with special opportunities to know about recent technological
advances that were beginning to make it possible for steam power, in
combination with metal rails, to be exploited for moving coal and other
materials within collieries.
So perhaps the young George Stephenson's prospects were not so
entirely bleak as at first glance they seem to have been. During the years
when he was edging towards adulthood, a perceptive worker like
Stephenson might have perceived that steam locomotion was starting to
be a practical possibility. And as he added to his practical skills, he would
have become aware that he already possessed some of the knowledge and
some of the capabilities that could help to make that possibility into a
reality.
Almost all we know about George Stephenson's early years and much of
our knowledge of his later life comes from the Life of George Stephenson
by Samuel Smiles, who began work on the biography soon after
Stephenson's death in 1848. It was published in 1857, two years before
Smiles' better known Self Help appeared, and was a widely admired best-
seller. George Eliot was among those who praised it.
The Life of George Stephenson provides a vivid and colourful picture of
Stephenson's life. Samuel Smiles was particularly well-equipped to write
about Stephenson, being the secretary of a railway company as well as an
experienced journalist. He had seen Stephenson and had heard him lec-
turing, and he was given aid and encouragement by George Stephenson's
already distinguished engineer son, Robert. To collect information,
Smiles repeatedly visited the region where Stephenson had grown up. He
located a number of old people who had known the great engineer and
were happy to provide recollections of his childhood. Smiles, whose job
working for a railway company had led to him becoming closely involved
with many aspects of railway construction, put his own experience to
good use in writing about the problems that Stephenson had encoun-
tered.
Smiles was a fervent admirer of George Stephenson. The engineer's
lack of interest in religion would have troubled his more pious biogra-
pher, but that aside, Stephenson epitomised most of the qualities that
Smiles valued: courage, thrift, optimism, diligence, and above all perse-
verance. Fortunately, Smiles was a superb storyteller. The disarming
The long ascent of George Stephenson 63
3 Smiles (r88r) p. v-vi. (This and subsequent quotations from Samuel Smiles' The life of
George Stephenson are taken from the r88r Centenary Edition of this biography, which
was originally published in r857).
4 See, for example, Davies (r975), p. r67; Rolt (rg6o), p. ro.
64 Genius Explained
festered into bitter disputes. But warts and all, George Stephenson was a
most remarkable individual, a true hero from the working class, and a
self-made titan.
5 Spoto (1983).
The long ascent of George Stephenson 65
make it clear that some of them were highly ingenious. By this time he was
undoubtedly giving plenty of close attention to machines and carefully
observing their workings. 'Much to the marvel of the pitmen', 6 Smiles
reported, George and a friend constructed an elaborate apparatus in the
form of a miniature winding machine, linked to a model engine made
from clay from a local bog and hemlock branches that served as imagi-
nary steam pipes. The apparatus simulated the activity of sending tubs of
coal, which the boys modelled with hollowed-out corks linked by twine,
up and down a mine, using a structure constructed from pieces of waste
wood found in the nearby carpenters' shop.
Stephenson's activities as he approached adulthood provide firm evi-
dence of a willingness to work hard and throw himself into the kinds of
pursuits that would have extended his capabilities enough for him to have
aimed at becoming more than an unskilled pit workman. As a child he
worked at a variety of jobs, leading plough-horses, hoeing turnips,
working as a colliery 'corf-bitter' employed to clear stones and dross from
the coal, and looking after the horse-powered 'gin' or winding-wheel
which lifted coal out of the mine. At the age of thirteen he was, in the rec-
ollection of one of Smiles' elderly informants, 'a grit growing lad, with
bare legs an' feet' who was 'very quick-witted and full of fun and tricks:
indeed, there was nothing under the sun but he tried to imitate'. 7
Stephenson's earliest ambition was to become an engineman, respon-
sible for the daily operation of a steam engine, and he made his first big
step in that direction at the age of fourteen, when he was appointed as an
assistant fireman, to be paid one shilling (a twentieth of a pound) per day.
That job did not last long, because the pit closed down - as often hap-
pened at the time - but by fifteen he was a full fireman, although it was
another two years before his income reached twelve shillings a week and
he was making a man's wage. After a further move he became the
'plugman', or engineman, that he had aspired to be, employed to keep a
pump engine working and remedy minor defects. This was a relatively
skilled job, especially for someone of his age. It needed more knowledge
than was required for his father's post as a fireman, and was also better
paid.
By this time George Stephenson was not only displaying an intense
interest in engines, but also gaining a degree of expertise. This is the point
in Stephenson's life when we see the first glimmer of a possibility that the
young man might eventually become a proper engineer, and perhaps an
innovative one, despite his lack of formal education. As well as closely
observing the engines he worked on, he extended, in more sophisticated
developments to steam engines that had been made by the Scottish engi-
neer James Watt and others, his inability to read made the information
that he was so anxious to acquire practically inaccessible to him. Having
never been to school, even basic arithmetic was beyond him, and he
would have been unable to decipher the diagrams and plans that are
essential to the work of an engineer. Another obstacle was his lack of any
theoretical knowledge of the physical sciences.
It is hard to imagine how his life and prospects must have seemed to the
young George Stephenson at that time, but the possibilities he would
have been able to envisage for himself would inevitably have been
restricted by the circumstances of his life as a poorly-paid illiterate
workman living in an isolated part of England. He would have encoun-
tered only a few individuals who had been as far as London. He would
have possessed limited knowledge of the possible consequences of being
educated or of the means by which self-education might be acquired. Far
from being in a position to envisage the possibility ofbecoming a qualified
engineer himself, he would have had little access to the kind of informa-
tion that he would have needed in order to know what being an engineer
actually involved. A person who does not have opportunities to even
become aware of what can be achieved is condemned to a restricted exis-
tence. For an illiterate young worker living at the end of the eighteenth
century to be capable of seeing beyond the limits of his daily environment
would have required a rare combination of curiosity and imagination, in
addition to the energy and resourcefulness George Stephenson possessed
in abundance.
In one respect Stephenson was fortunate. He was lucky enough to gain
his engineering skills at a time when the practical knowledge that he could
acquire in the course of his everyday life as a colliery worker was crucial,
and his lack of the formal and theoretical kinds of knowledge that are only
accessible to literate people was a less crippling handicap than it would
have been only a few years later. Even so, illiteracy was a serious obstacle.
Stephenson's growing interest in engines was making him increasingly
aware of how much he needed the knowledge that his lack of education
had denied him. So, at an age when many young people today would
already be at university, Stephenson set out to give himself the beginnings
of an elementary education.
He learned to read and write by attending lessons, travelling three
nights each week to a neighbouring village after his long working day had
ended. Soon afterwards he started to learn arithmetic as well. Stephenson
was immensely determined, as well as being strongly motivated to gain
these skills. A contemporary who began at the same time recalled to
The long ascent of George Stephenson 69
Smiles that George Stephenson quickly moved ahead of him and 'took to
figures so wonderful', probably because he attacked his studies with such
enthusiasm. Smiles' informant recalled that,
George's secret was his perseverance. He worked out the sums in his by-hours,
improving every minute of his spare time by the engine-fire, there studying the
arithmetical problems set him upon his slate by the master. In the evenings he
took to Robertson the sums which he had 'worked', and the new ones were 'set'
for him to study out the following day. Thus his progress was rapid ... 12
At around this time Stephenson also extended his practical qualifications,
learning the difficult skill of 'braking' the engines that transported miners
and coal wagons to and from the surface. That job required a combina-
tion of steadiness, alertness and precision. Any error could easily damage
the wagons being drawn out of the pit or even endanger the pitmen's lives.
Stephenson worked as a brakesman for several years. During this period
he added to his wage of around one pound per week with small sums
earned by another skill he had managed by then to acquire, mending (and
subsequently making) shoes for the local people. He worked late into the
night at improving his reading, writing and arithmetic. His very first
efforts at inventing date from this period, when he tried but failed to
produce a brake that would automatically reverse an engine. With the
long working hours that were customary at the time it would have been a
struggle for him to squeeze all his activities into the time available to him.
But somehow he did, and he even found time to get married, in
November 1802. By then George Stephenson was twenty-one.
He continued studying hard in his spare moments, 'paving the way to
being something more than a manual labourer', 13 and he 'set himself to
study the principles of mechanics, and to master the laws by which his
engine worked'. 14 Smiles depicts Stephenson at this period of his life
spending his winter evenings sitting by the side of his young wife and
occupied in studying mechanical subjects or modelling experimental
machines, when not mending or making shoes, or constructing shoe-
lasts, yet another activity which earned a few extra pence. As well as that,
he started mending clocks, and found that people in his village were
equally happy to trust him with their timepieces and their footwear. Like
many mechanically-minded individuals before him and since, he set out
to make a perpetual motion machine. Like all the others, he did not
succeed.
Smiles exaggerates at times, and Stephenson cannot have been always
quite so disciplined and determined as the figure Smiles portrays. But the
account of Stephenson's everyday life in his early twenties must be largely
12 Smiles (I88I), p. II. 13 Smiles (I88I), p. IS. 14 Smiles (I88I), p. IS.
70 Genius Explained
correct, because only someone who could make an enormous and sus-
tained effort to learn would have been capable of making the steady
progress Stephenson had to achieve in order to transform himself from an
illiterate labourer into an engineer. The French novelist Balzac wrote,
perhaps ironically, of making himself into a genius: George Stephenson
literally did just that.
In 1803 George's only child was born, Robert Stephenson. Tragically,
Robert's mother died before he was two. The record of George
Stephenson's life around this unhappy time is incomplete, but between
1802 and 1805 he changed jobs at least three times, moving to Montrose
in Scotland, where he was employed to superintend the operation of an
engine made by the innovative firm of Boulton and Watt. He impressed
his employers with his ingenuity, on one occasion saving them a good deal
of money by finding an effective way to prevent sand getting into the
water that was drawn into the engine, clogging it up. But his troubles were
not over. Returning home from Scotland, he discovered that his father
had been blinded in a serious pit accident, and had no income to live on.
George's sparse savings went in paying his father's debts. Shortly after-
wards, at a time when taxes were rising and poverty was increasing as
unemployment rose and incomes fell, large numbers of young men were
being called into the military services to quell the unrest, and Stephenson
was among those ordered to join up. The only way a man could avoid this
was by paying for a substitute to serve in his place: Stephenson was able
to do this, but he had to borrow the money. At this time he was under-
standably pessimistic about the future, and seriously considered joining
his sister Ann and her husband who were emigrating to America. Only
the fact that he could not afford the transatlantic fare stopped him doing
so.
That grim period of Stephenson's life came to an end by about 1808.
He was now twenty-seven, and once again working in the Newcastle
region as a brakesman. It was a job with less responsibility than the post
he had held in Scotland, and less pay, but the George Stephenson of this
period was a confident and forceful young man, well aware of his abilities
and not shy of displaying his knowledge. He was continuing his educa-
tion, still conscientiously working on exercises in arithmetic during spare
moments, and sending his slate to be marked when he was too busy to get
to the teacher himself. Sometimes he used the side of a coal wagon as a
blackboard to work on.
Stephenson learned much from the man who taught him during this
period, John Wigham, a keen reader and a lively talker who enjoyed dis-
cussing ideas with his pupils. Wigham helped Stephenson to learn how to
draw plans and sections, and together they worked their way through a
The long ascent of George Stephenson 71
on his way to "the preaching" in the Methodist chapel') and asked for his
help.
'Well, George,' said 'Dodds, 'they tell me that you think you can put the engine at
the High Pit to rights.' 'Yes, sir,' said George, 'I think I could.' 'If that's the case, I'll
give you a fair trial, and you must set to work immediately. We are clean drowned
out, and cannot get a step further. The engineers hereabouts are all bet (sic); and
if you really succeed in accomplishing what they cannot do, you may depend upon
it I will make you a man for life' 16
And of course Stephenson succeeded. Insisting on employing only men
picked by himself, and aware of the ill-feeling of the workers regularly
employed on the engine towards a young pit brakesman who was claim-
ing to know more than they did about their own engine and to be able to
remedy defects 'which the most skilled men of their craft including the
engineer of the colliery, had failed to do', he proceeded to take the engine
to pieces. He raised the water cistern that serviced the engine, enlarged
one of the inlets, modified various valves, and increased the pressure. This
was all done in about three days. By this time heads were shaking, with
even Dodds declaring 'Why, she was better as she was; now, she will
knock the house down' (that is, self-destruct from excessive vibration).
But the engine was started up, and by the same night the water in the pit
was lower than it had ever been. Two days later all the surplus water had
been removed. The pit workers could finally be 'sent to the bottom' (of
the mine) after a year of profitless inactivity.
This was the big break for which Stephenson had prepared himself,
and Dodds made good his promise to make him 'a man for life'.
Stephenson's immediate reward was a gift of ten pounds, about £soo or
750 US dollars in the currency of the 1990s and hardly a generous sum in
relation to the huge savings his intervention had produced. But more
importantly, Dodds gave him a job and promised to keep him in mind for
promotion when something better turned up. On the accidental death of
the engine-wright at Killingworth in 1810, Stephenson was appointed to
the post, at a salary of one hundred pounds per year. That was by no
means a huge salary, but Stephenson was able to add to it by taking on
extra assignments with the blessing of his employers, a substantial mine-
owning company known as the 'Grand Allies'. He was given a horse to
ride between the different mines. The pit workman had finally become
the engineer.
In his post as engine-wright at Killingworth, many of Stephenson's
responsibilities involved finding improved ways to transport coal and
other commodities. From around this time he started to work on tasks
that equipped him with the capabilities that would enable him to explore
the possibility of developing engines that propelled themselves. One of his
first duties at Killingworth was to erect a (stationary) winding engine to
remove coal from a mine. It pulled wagons via a rope that was wound
around a revolving drum. At around this time he also designed a 'self-
acting' transport system for coal, in which empty wagons moved up to the
railhead by the power created by full ones moving down an incline. He
also found ways of using the surplus power from the underground
engines that pumped water to the surface, for hauling coal from deeper
parts of the mine. By introducing economies of this kind, as well as using
stationary engines to move coal trucks, Stephenson was able to reduce the
number of horses required in one pit from 100 to only 15 or 16. The mine
owners were quick to see the advantages of doing this, and they put him in
control of the colliery machinery in their other pits.
By now Stephenson was starting to think seriously about the possibility
of introducing locomotive engines. He would have heard rumours of
various attempts to produce engines that reliably moved themselves along
metal rails, rather than merely pulling vehicles from a stationary point.
Steam locomotives of a kind had already been invented. As early as 1804
one was being operated by an engineer from Cornwall in the south-west
of England, Robert Trevithick, but it was not economical. Soon after that
a locomotive to Trevithick's design was constructed at Gateshead, near to
Stephenson's home in the north-east. It was intended to be used at
Wylam, Stephenson's birthplace, but it never worked properly. By 1811 a
number of inventors were trying to develop practical locomotives,
although many problems still needed to be overcome. A reasonably reli-
able locomotive had been made in Leeds by John Blenkinsop, with
toothed wheels that moved along a racked line. In the long run this did
not prove to be an economical arrangement for moving goods or passen-
gers along flat surfaces, but Blenkinsop's engine was a commercial
success and went on working for several years.
The colliery owner at Wylam, John Blackett, was sufficiently encour-
aged by the success of Blenkinsop's engine to have a second locomotive
made, despite the failure of the Trevithick engine he had ordered. The
new one, which was built at Gateshead, was designed in a way that com-
bined elements of the Trevithick and Blenkinsop locomotives. This
engine weighed six tons and had a single cylinder and a fly-wheel. It was
described to Samuel Smiles by the engineer who supervised its construc-
tion Gonathon Foster) as 'a strange machine, with lots of pumps, cog-
wheels, and plugs, requiring constant attention while at work'. 17 On
still wanting the man who should accomplish for the locomotive what James
Watt had done for the steam-engine, and combine in a complete form the best
points in the separate plans of others, embodying with them such original inven-
tions and adaptations of his own as to entitle him to the merit of inventing the
working locomotive ... This was the great work upon which George Stephenson
now entered. 19
By this time in his life Stephenson had finally succeeded in becoming
prepared to make a real contribution to the improvement of steam loco-
motion. Someone knowing only what Stephenson had already achieved
by then, and also knowing about the nature of his duties and the facilities
that were available to him, might easily have guessed that he would subse-
quently have earned himself at least a footnote in the history of steam
locomotion.
It had taken Stephenson a long time to reach the point of being ready to
make a major impact as an inventor. His progress had been slow and
effortful. By the summer of 1813 he was already thirty-two. Before reach-
ing the equivalent age Schubert had died, leaving behind him a remark-
able body of great music, Charles Darwin had pieced together (but not
made public) the theory of evolution by natural selection, and Charles
Dickens had already become the celebrated author, having written
Pickwick Papers when he was twenty-four. By and large, the preparatory
years of individuals who produce major accomplishments correspond
with the years of childhood and adolescence, but that is not true in the
case of George Stephenson. Even the most basic skills of literacy and
numeracy were only acquired by him when he was already an adult. And
yet even that handicap did not stop him eventually becoming an engineer-
ing genius.
Looking back at George Stephenson's life prior to 1813, and asking how
and why he had managed to reach the point at which he was finally in a
position to begin to establish a reputation as an innovative engineer, we
can now be sure that there were no miracles involved: there is nothing
totally inexplicable about George Stephenson's progress. Undoubtedly,
the journey through life that had eventually led him to that stage was an
unusually arduous one: he had to overcome obstacles that were more
numerous and more serious than most would-be inventors have encoun-
tered. But our reasonably detailed examination of the route by he which
eventually arrived at the stage of being ready to make a big contribution
has shown that there were no sudden or incomprehensible advances in his
capacities.
At the funeral of Charles Darwin in 1882 it was noticed that the dark
clothes of the mourners in Westminster Abbey were brightened by
coloured beams of light from a window-panel commemorating George
Stephenson and his son Robert. Shine on!
4 Michael Faraday
1 Wolpert (1992).
84
Michael Faraday 85
the ways in which they treated one another, Faraday- as Riebau noticed-
concentrated on the physical world: 'he went an early walk in the
Morning Visiting always some Works of Art or searching for some
Mineral or Vegitable curiosity - Holloway water Works Highgate
Archway, Middlesex Water Works - Strand Bridge - Junction Water
Works etc. etc ... his mind ever engaged.' 5
As we have seen, the adult Michael Faraday was deeply grateful to
Riebau for the opportunities he had been given to read books. In the ear-
liest years of his apprenticeship Faraday's reading was wide rather than
deep, the choice of material often being decided by whatever happened to
come to his attention in his work as a binder of books. He read the
Arabian Nights. He devoured novels, probably including Fanny Burney's
Evelina: 6 there exists a copy of that bound by Faraday himself. Following
Riebau's advice, Faraday copied down observations that struck him as
insightful and reproduced illustrations that impressed him. He was
always curious about new and interesting things.
Little is known about the way in which Michael Faraday began to gain
an elementary knowledge of science. All that is certain is that between the
ages of fourteen and eighteen he had plenty of opportunities to read and
learn, that he took advantage of them, and that by eighteen his interests
were largely directed towards the sciences. Perhaps the growth of his
enthusiasm about science was fairly gradual: until he was sixteen or so he
may simply have been excited by the unfamiliar and exotic world of
knowledge which books opened up for him. But by the time he was eight-
een his thirst for knowledge had become decidedly strong, and at around
this period, probably in 1810, he was attracted by a number of items he
encountered in Encyclopaedia Britannica. He told a friend that his mind
had been turned towards science by an article on electricity written by
James Tytler, which he came across when binding that encyclopaedia.
By then Faraday had changed from being a boy who was curious and
energetic like many young people, to something rarer: an intense, excep-
tionally diligent and keenly committed would-be scholar. Somehow or
other, Faraday had made himself into a reasonably well-informed young
man with a serious interest in the sciences. He had already mastered a
number of books on chemistry. It is possible that he had begun to form
some vague career ambitions in which learning and scholarship played a
part. By the end of 1810 he was a decidedly studious and disciplined indi-
vidual, and already possessed a remarkable capacity- reminiscent of the
young Isaac Newton- to keep persevering until difficult learning chal-
lenges were mastered.
9 Isaac Watts, The Improvement of The Mind: or a Supplement to the Art of Logic: (containing a
variety of remarks and rules for the attainment and communication of useful knowledge
in religion, in the sciences, and in common life. To which is added, Discourse on the
Education of Children and ll>uth: two parts, complete in one volume). Printed by J.
Abraham, Clement's Lane, Lombard Street, r80I. 10 Watts (r8or), pp. 6-7.
11 Watts (r8or), pp. 34-5.
94 Genius Explained
Reading The Improvement of the Mind would also have made Faraday
more aware of the necessity to undertake a number of different activities
that all contribute to learning. Reading is essential, Watts agrees, but
reading on its own is insufficient, and the student must think about the
material he has read. Study is vital, but it is not enough to simply remem-
ber the material. Careful thought is important, but it is only effective in
conjunction with information that is accurate and reasonably detailed.
Attendance at lectures is valuable, but in order to get the most from them
the student needs to take notes during the lecture and go over them and
revise them afterwards. The acquisition of new information is also vital,
but it is equally necessary to be able to distinguish between words and
things. A student should never be contented with learning lists of words
and phrases, without properly understanding them, 'less your laboured
improvements only amass a heap of unintelligible phrases, and you feed
upon husks instead of kernels' . 12
Watts' writing manages to be erudite and practical at the same time, and
Faraday's confidence in himself as a scholar would almost certainly have
increased as a consequence of his having access to it. The book offers con-
vincingly down-to-earth advice about the procedures and activities that
result in effective learning. That would have helped Faraday to gain a
broad view of the possible route by which he could achieve his aim of edu-
cating himself. Prior to encountering The Improvement of the Mind he had
probably formed some idea of what he wanted to achieve as a learner, but
without a proper knowledge ofhow to do that he would not have been at all
certain that his rather vague goals were realistic. Watts' book showed
Faraday how he could move ahead. It gave him a clear picture of what was
possible. Armed with it, Michael Faraday would have gained some idea of
what had to be done in order to make further progress. His initially vague
aspirations would have started to seem like practical possibilities, ones that
could be achieved by making sensible plans and diligently following them.
Fortified by what he had learned about the process of becoming a
scholar from Watts' guide, and helped by the calm and supportive back-
ground of his own home life and the stimulating environment of his
apprenticeship, Michael Faraday now threw himself into the pursuit of a
scientific education. By around the time he became eighteen in 1809
Faraday had already made himself into a reasonably well-informed young
man who had a serious interest in the sciences and had already mastered a
number of books on chemistry. By the end of the following year he was a
studious and disciplined individual, capable of persevering until difficult
learning challenges were mastered.
It is true that geniuses are often far from ordinary in a number of ways.
The sheer force of their enthusiasms is often extraordinary, as is their
single-minded devotion to particular interests. Some geniuses, like
Newton, might conceivably be regarded as having been egocentric to an
extent that justifies calling their actions 'pathological'. Simply because
geniuses are exceptional, it is perhaps inevitable that some of the descrip-
tive terms others apply to them are ones that are often associated with
abnormalities that take pathological forms, creating in people's minds a
link that may be illusory. In the particular case of Faraday we can note
that although it is certainly possible to describe his efforts as being obses-
sive, that word could just as fittingly be applied to the majority of excep-
tionally creative individuals. Furthermore, the act oflabelling a person as
obsessive does little to help reveal the actual causes or motives underlying
his or her activities.
The rate of Faraday's progress towards changing himself into a well-
informed amateur scientist was undeniably impressive, but perhaps no
more so than might have been expected in an unusually well motivated
young learner who was capable of persevering at the intense regime of
learning and study activities that Faraday set himself. In that respect
Faraday was not unlike Stephenson. The immense enthusiasm for learn-
ing that is seen in both these men, amounting almost to a hunger for it, is
to some extent characteristic of intellectually curious individuals who are
conscious of having missed out on schooling and anxious to catch up.
As with Charles Darwin and George Stephenson, when Michael
Faraday's steady advance towards becoming an exceptionally capable
individual is carefully charted, we conspicuously fail to encounter any
obvious gaps or sudden leaps: each new capability or fresh understanding
can be seen to build upon ones that have already been gained. There is no
point at which Faraday inexplicably magnified his capabilities or sud-
denly expanded them. In making that observation I do not claim that any
person sharing Faraday's intense motivation and devotion towards learn-
ing would necessarily have made equivalent gains. Yet there is clearly no
need to assume that Faraday could not have made the progress he did
unless he possessed some inherently special powers of learning. Nor are
there any convincing reasons for believing that Michael Faraday was
innately more clever than most other people. Outside his particular areas
of interest he was not noticeably well-informed: he never seemed to know
much about the crucial political events of his lifetime, for example. And
far from regarding himself as being unusually intelligent, he was always
conscious of what he perceived to be limitations, particularly in regard to
remembering. He seems to have been convinced that his memory was
defective. He may have been right about that, although it is possible that
98 Genius Explained
the lapses which distressed him were no more than the absent-minded-
ness of someone whose deep preoccupation with his own particular inter-
ests restricted the amount of attention that could be directed to the
external practicalities of everyday life.
As in the case of George Stephenson, what is most impressive of all
about Faraday's achievements as a student is not the size of the learning
gains he made relative to his efforts, but the sheer magnitude of the
efforts. So if we are to look for qualities in Michael Faraday that resist an
explanation based on his unusual experiences and mental activities, it is
more likely that they will involve his exceptional determination and
capacity for sustained concentration on problems than his learning or
thinking capacities as such. But as we have seen, there is every reason to
believe that no special explanation is required. His remarkable powers of
determination and concentration were nurtured by his experiences, and
fuelled by an understandably acute desire to learn.
At least up to the stage ofhis life when he reached the age of twenty-one
in 1812, there is no evidence at all to suggest that the contributing causes
of Faraday's capabilities were fundamentally different to the various
influences that help determine ordinary people's abilities. Faraday was
remarkable and extraordinary, of course, but the influences that resulted
in him gaining exceptional capabilities do not appear to have been funda-
mentally different from the ones that have enabled other people to
acquire more ordinary levels of competence. Nothing that has been
observed in the course of Faraday's life up to this point provides any
grounds for believing that his progress was assisted by exceptional mental
processes, or by special inherent qualities that are exclusive to geniuses.
No firm indications exist of innate gifts or talents playing a role. Nor is
there any evidence that Faraday possessed fundamentally enhanced
learning powers or especially speedy cognitive processing capacity, or
totally inexplicable powers of creativity.
Aided by the advice he encountered in The Improvement of the Mind,
Faraday's efforts to educate himself from 1810 until the completion ofhis
apprenticeship in 1812 took the form of an organised campaign. The
regime he followed would have ensured that someone as strongly moti-
vated as him, and as able to tolerate long periods of hard effort, could
hardly have failed to acquire a sound grounding in what was known about
chemistry and physics at the beginning of the nineteenth century. He was
always very aware of his lack of formal education, and during those years
he almost certainly overestimated the benefits that other young people
gained from the schooling he had missed. At that time he believed that
only by Herculean efforts on his part could the lost ground be made up.
Only later in life did he start to appreciate that an education which is
Michael Faraday 99
largely directed by the learner's own choices and voluntary decisions has
advantages as well as limitations, and in certain respects can be the best
kind of education there is.
from having a wealthy sponsor) for John Stuart Mill. Some geniuses have
received help from a number of different individuals. Charles Darwin, for
instance, was given considerable assistance by his sisters (who taught him
as a child), his father, his brother, and by a variety of university scientists,
notably Robert Grant at Edinburgh and John Henslow at Cambridge.
Faraday's powerful mentor was Humphry Davy, the celebrated
chemist. Davy was not the first person to be helpful to Faraday, of course.
Riebau had done much to encourage him in the early years, and others,
such as Tatum, aided him at a later stage. But Davy's patronage was espe-
cially significant, because he commanded a position right at the apex of
British science.
There was a large element of luck in Faraday's gaining a post with
Davy, although it was not a matter of luck alone. Faraday had already
been fortunate in his choice of employer when he began his apprentice-
ship. Perhaps even at that time Faraday or his father might have perceived
that Riebau would prove unusually kind and encouraging, although it is
more likely that nobody had anticipated just how favourable the circum-
stances ofhis apprenticeship would turn out to be. With Faraday's second
major stroke ofluck, he was taken on by Humphry Davy as an assistant at
the Royal Institution. Davy's highly original research in chemistry and his
reputation as a popular lecturer had made him one of the most fashion-
able British scientists of the time, and undoubtedly the most glamorous.
Faraday knew just how valuable a chance he had been given. He had
done all he could to bring that about, having brought himself to Davy's
attention earlier. He had also approached Sir Joseph Banks, the wealthy
and powerful naturalist who in his twenties had travelled around the
world with Captain James Cook in the Endeavour. But Banks was now
almost seventy, and not prepared to exert himself on behalf of a young
man he had never heard of. However, largely by chance, Faraday's efforts
to gain Davy's help did succeed. At a moment that might have been pre-
cisely calculated to cater for Faraday's aspirations, the irresponsible beha-
viour of Davy's laboratory assistant prompted his employer to look for a
replacement. It quickly became obvious that the eager young Michael
Faraday was the ideal candidate.
Faraday had known about Davy's work well before 1812. Two years
earlier he had read a book entitled Conversations on Chemistry by Mrs Jane
Marcet, in which she gave a clear account of the views that Davy was pro-
pounding in the highly acclaimed series of lectures he delivered at the
recently founded Royal Institution. Their original purpose had been to
disseminate knowledge to the artisan class, but the audience soon came to
be dominated by young ladies who flocked to see this brilliant and attrac-
tive young scientist, and tickets were hard to obtain. Faraday was given
102 Genius Explained
17 Zuckerman (r977).
Michael Faraday 103
As Harriet Zuckerman has shown in her research into the careers of Nobel
prizewinning scientists, a typical prizewinner will have attended one of a
small number of prestigious universities and gained advantages from
working with high-ranking scientists, will have had access to good facilities,
and will also have become known to other active scientists as a person who
is associated with a highly regarded institution. 18 At the Royal Institution
Faraday would have enjoyed similar advantages, even though his relation-
ship with Davy was always formal and not particularly close, and strictly
confined to scientific activities. like Faraday, Davy, who was only thirteen
years older than him, had known poverty in his early life, but the two men
could not have been more different. Where Faraday was modest and
unworldly, Davy was ambitious and urbane, with an eye for worldly
success.
Someone as vain and egocentric as Davy was might appear to have
been a less than ideal candidate for the role of mentor. Unsurprisingly, the
relationship between the two men was clouded at times. A biographer
who had known Faraday for the last thirty years of his life perceived that,
'Davy was hurt by his own success. He had very little self-control, and but
little method and order' . 19 Faraday himself, his first biographer reported,
'has been known to say that the greatest of all his advantages was that he
had a model to teach him what he should avoid'. 20 Yet Davy's sheer
enthusiasm for science was infectious, and Faraday would have been
inspired as well as intrigued by the great chemist's eagerness to talk about
his current interests to anyone who would listen, and think aloud about
the problems he was engaged on, as is evident from the reports of Davy's
thoughts and speculations that pepper the journal made by Faraday in the
eighteen-month period he spent accompanying his employer on the
European mainland. In that respect at least, Davy, for all his faults, would
have been a better mentor for a young scientist than Faraday ever could
have been, despite his being an excellent lecturer and a more generous
and less selfish individual than Davy. Unlike the extroverted Davy,
Faraday found it impossible to think aloud and would rarely discuss his
own ideas with other people until they were fully formed. He was a very
private man. Faraday was not unfriendly and did not lack a sense of
humour, but there were firm limits on his everyday sociability. There was
a spirit of exclusivity in the Sandemanian sect which had such an impor-
tant place in his life, and this made Sandemanians seem antisocial to out-
siders. Even within the sect socialising was largely restricted to Sundays.
These constraints do not seem to have impeded Faraday's develop-
ment as a scientist. Sundays apart, his scientific interests filled most of his
Newton's response to the question of how he was able to achieve his pro-
digious accomplishments, 'by working on it continually' is valid for virtu-
ally all great innovators in the arts or sciences. As I have already
remarked, every creative individual has to find a way to form some kind of
protective shell that cuts out mundane disturbances and makes lengthy
contemplation possible. In Faraday's case, that was achieved by his
adopting a persona that kept him partly detached from other people and
their concerns. He was totally uninterested in political issues, his 400 sur-
viving letters barely mentioning the turmoils and crises that for many
people were burning issues. Modest and genial, he was rarely unkind, but
he put sharp limits on his involvement with others. For all his serenity and
goodness, the sense of a sharply defined personality who is forceful and
effective in his dealings with the outside world is less evident in accounts
of Faraday than it is in contemporary reports of other great innovators.
Although his humility and benevolence were often remarked upon and he
was conscientious in meeting his obligations- making young relatives and
other visitors welcome in his laboratory and enthusiastically entering into
their games, going to enormous lengths to produce lectures that were as
clear and effective as he could make them, putting special efforts into the
science lectures for children which he introduced at the Royal Institution
- he was in many respects a solitary scholar. He was happy to know that
his lectures were enjoyed by the many people who attended them, and his
audiences included literary figures like Dickens as well as scientists such
as Darwin. But he usually avoided having students working under his
supervision. He admitted that when he and an assistant were working in
his laboratory, many hours might pass without a word passing between
them. So far as was possible, he worked unaided, and as he undertook the
routine work on an experiment, his mind would be totally engaged in his
thoughts. As one of his biographers put it, Faraday's dialogue was with
nature. 21
establishing a base camp close to the summit we can perceive that the
whole ascent is possible after all. Similarly, the discovery that Faraday's
earlier development into an accomplished scientist required no miracles,
makes it seem likely that his later achievements too, for all their extraordi-
nariness, can be accounted for along broadly similar lines. Once again,
the facts do not point to a need for inherently special powers that are
unique to geniuses. The challenge of explaining Faraday's genius con-
fronts us with problems rather than mysteries.
Even in comparison with the very few other highly promising young sci-
entists who were engaged in research at the time and might have been
regarded as Faraday's potential competitors, Michael Faraday at twenty-
five had some distinct advantages. His position at the Royal Institution
made him as well placed as anyone to conduct experiments and to learn
about new discoveries and developments. It has also been suggested that
even the fact that - partly as a consequence of his being self-educated -
his mathematical competence was restricted may have worked to his
advantage. That is indeed possible, because the more mathematical
approach that he might otherwise have favoured could have imposed
unhelpful constraints at that particular stage of scientific discovery. 24
Michael Faraday's extreme diligence was a further strong asset, and so
too were the practical skills, acquired in his apprenticeship, that made
him such an expert at building scientific apparatus. Finally, Faraday's
religious beliefs, which inclined him to perceive his work as part of an
enterprise that helped to reveal the secrets of a divinely created universe,
may have been especially congruent to the conceptualisation of physical
phenomena (in terms of fields and forces) that was central to the scientific
progress he made.
Michael Faraday conducted thousands of experiments and made many
important discoveries. His reputation as a major scientist was sealed
when he discovered electromagnetic induction, just a year after George
Stephenson's place among the great inventors had been secured by the
opening of the Liverpool and Manchester Railway. Coincidentally, in the
month of Faraday's most earth-shaking discovery Charles Darwin was
making the decision that led to him embarking on the voyage of HMS
Beagle, setting in train the series of creative thoughts that eventually
resulted in yet another momentous achievement.
24 Williams (1965).
5 Manufacturing genius
108
Manufacturing genius 109
ness. Some of these people- who include parents ofYehudi Menuhin, the
architect Frank Lloyd Wright, and the economist Maynard Keynes
(whose mother worried about him working his brain too hard even before
he reached the age of two 1) -have gone to immense lengths to give their
child special opportunities. Despite that, some of these parents have been
inclined to see their own actions as being ones that merely nurtured an
existing natural talent, rather than active interventions designed to create
special abilities.
Certain other parents who have become heavily involved in activities
designed to help a child to gain exceptional abilities have been members
of cultural groups in which it has been customary for the parents to
decide upon a child's vocation. Family traditions have played a part here.
These parents have often made a start at a very early stage in their child's
life, and there may have been little or no provision for the child's own
desires to influence the training activities. The early musical experiences
of the famous Korean cellist Yo Yo Ma reflect a modern form of this kind
of background. The childhoods of Wolfgang Mozart and his sister,
N annerl, exemplify earlier versions of these practices.
All these parents have been alike in going to lengths that many would
think extreme in order to invest in their children's futures. The motives
have varied, although in many cases the parents have been men or women
who believed themselves to have been prevented from making the best of
their own careers. Some were immigrants who could not fulfil their ambi-
tions because they were unable to restart their professional career in a
new country. Others have been disadvantaged men or women deter-
mined to give their children opportunities that were denied to themselves.
Often these people have had time on their hands, or they have possessed
more surplus energy than would be present in individuals with demand-
ing careers of their own. A notable exception is James Mill, the father of
John Stuart Mill. As we shall discover, James Mill put enormous efforts
into giving his son a remarkable early education at the same time as being
engaged in a massive intellectual project of his own.
How successful have these efforts to produce superior children been?
To assess the effectiveness of the parents' activities it is necessary to pose
two distinct questions. First, to what extent has the intention to equip a
child with special knowledge or skills been achieved? Second, how
effective have the parents been at ensuring that their child also gained the
broader kinds of competence that a person needs to have in order to enjoy
a productive and fulfilling life? These wider capacities include various
practical attainments and social capabilities that make a young person
1 Skidelsky (1983).
110 Genius Explained
young man would also be healthy, strong, active and happy. As the proud
parent reported to his readers 'in this, as everybody knows, I have suc-
ceeded'.3
Karl Witte was impressed by young children's curiosity and enthu-
siasm to learn, and he was acutely conscious that opportunities to teach
them are often wasted. If only a parent would seize these chances, Witte
observed, 'What an immeasurable amount a child will learn in six, eight
or ten years, that is, in 3,650 days, in 36,500 hours, reckoning the day at
ten hours, if every conversation with him or in his presence teaches him
something!' 4
The perspicacity of that observation has been amply confirmed by
twentieth-century research findings. In one recent investigation, for
instance, it was observed that three-year-olds from different social classes
differed considerably in the scope of their spoken vocabularies. Searching
for an explanation, the researchers discovered enormous variations in the
children's actual experiences of language. Even by the age of three, those
children who came from professional families had already heard more
than thirty million words directed specifically towards them. In sharp
contrast, children from working-class families had heard around twenty
million words, and children in families living on welfare had heard only
ten million words, on average. 5 This demonstration of the huge variability
in children's early experiences, and the likely consequences, underlines
Karl Witte's good sense in drawing attention to the numerous opportu-
nities for learning that are present in the early years of a child's life. It also
illuminates the depressing extent to which such opportunities are com-
monly wasted, even today.
Pastor Witte was convinced that despite the prevailing view which held
(then as now) that a person's achievements largely depend upon inborn
aptitudes and talents, in reality the child's opportunities to learn during
the early years formed a more crucial influence. Like conscientious
parents today, Witte and his wife went to great pains to fill their boy's
waking hours with as rich and as varied a range of experiences as they
could provide for him. They were ahead of their time in having an intui-
tive understanding of the fact that acquiring a good grasp of language
greatly amplifies a child's mental powers. They saw that language 'makes
the child intelligent at an early time, for it puts his attention and his
several mental powers continuously in action. He is obliged always to
search, distinguish, compare, prefer, report, choose, and in short he must
work, that is, think.' 6 Knowing that, they made sure that their boy
3 Howe (rggo), p. 230. 4 Witte (r975), p. 86. 5 Hart & Risley (r995).
6 Witte (r975), p. 75·
114 Genius Explained
learned many things in the arms of his mother and in my own, such as one rarely
thinks of imparting to children. He learned to know and name all the objects in
ten different rooms, the rooms themselves, the staircase, the yard, the garden, the
stable, the well, the barn, - everything from the greatest to the smallest, was fre-
quently shown and clearly and plainly names to him, and he was encourage to
name the objects as plainly as possible. 7
For a growing child, the Witte parents were formidably careful guides.
They took care to make sure that their son was exposed to all kinds of
objects and events, and they drew his attention to the most informative
and interesting aspects of the things they showed him. They let him see
watermills and windmills, owls and bats, as well as concerts and operas,
and even lions and elephants. Always they took pains to explain things
carefully, in ways that a young child could understand. They made sure
that he noticed whatever was important, and encouraged him to respond.
Consequently the child 'became accustomed to what he had seen and
heard, and he himself addressed us, enquired, reported, retorted, etc'. 8
And he was stimulated to understand the objects and events he witnessed
not 'by merely staring at them, as children generally know them, but thor-
oughly'.9
When he was translating Witte's book, Leo Wiener must have been
struck by the Wittes' unusual sensitivity to their child's feelings and
wishes. He could not have failed to notice the importance they placed on
making sure that the child was always motivated to engage in the learning
activities they provided for him. With reading, for example, the parents
went to some lengths to be sure that, before any kind of instruction was
given, the child definitely wanted to read. Pastor Witte also drew attention
to the necessity to keep things informal when encouraging a young child
to learn. On one occasion he discovered that the child had been put off
learning to read because his mother had been teaching in too formal and
heavy-handed a manner. Afterwards, the father was especially careful to
delay teaching new reading skills until the boy had clearly demonstrated
that he was eager to acquire them.
From Norbert Wiener's account of his early years it is clear that
although his father had been greatly influenced by Witte's book, Leo
Wiener's somewhat domineering personality made it impossible for him
to match either the Wittes' relaxed informality or their genuine sensitivity
to the feelings and wishes of their son. Leo Wiener was not perceived by
his son as an ideal teacher, and in some respects his faults exceeded those
of Billy Sidis's father. He was often insensitive and overbearing. As the
adult Norbert Wiener recalled, 'He tended to impose his amusements
and preferences on those about him without fully realizing that many of
them might have come to a fuller participation in a life together with him
if this participation had not been so obviously enforced.no
That tendency to impose on others seems to have been a hallmark of
Leo Wiener's personality. It was especially evident in his teaching
methods. According to his son, Leo Wiener's lessons were neither gentle
nor relaxed. Norbert Wiener recalled how, 'He would begin the discus-
sion in an easy, conversational tone. This lasted exactly until I made the
first mathematical mistake. Then the gentle and loving father was
replaced by an avenger of the blood. The first warning he gave me of my
unconscious delinquency was a very sharp and aspirated "What!" >II
Yet his father's excesses did not stop Norbert Wiener from making a
success of his life. Unlike Billy Sidis, Norbert Wiener did survive his
childhood and adolescence more or less intact, even if the process of
growing up was not all easy for him. But he found it painfully difficult to
break away from his parents in early adulthood and become an indepen-
dent person with a life of his own.
There was more than one reason for the fact that Norbert Wiener even-
tually managed to break away from his family and get started on a creative
career, whereas Billy Sidis stayed defeated by the miseries of his child-
hood and adolescent years. Examining the crucial differences between
the two is helpful here, shedding light on a number of the positive and
negative influences that are likely to emerge when parents allow their
child's upbringing to be influenced by the idea of trying to manufacture a
genius. One important difference was that Wiener, unlike Sidis, could
readily see his parents' positive sides. These compensated for and perhaps
outweighed the negative ones. Norbert Wiener perceived that for all his
faults his father was something of a hero, always admirable, sometimes
warm and loving, constantly enthusiastic. As he put it, 'my father was a
romanticist ... His righteousness partook of the elemant of elan, of
triumph, of glorious and effective effort, of drinking deep of life and the
emotions thereof. For me, a boy just starting life, this made him in many
ways a noble and uplifting figure, a poet at heart .... my taskmaster was at
the same time my hero ... nz
Also, Leo Wiener's help and support could always be relied upon. That
was still true even when Norbert had reached the advanced stage of pre-
paring for his doctoral exams at Harvard. Norbert Wiener particularly
dreaded the oral component of the examinations, but he could still count
on his father's assistance, and 'Every morning he went for a walk with me
to keep up my physical condition and to reinforce my courage .... He
education, describing in some detail how the father carried through his
intentions. The task he set himself was, as the son recognised, a monu-
mental one, to which James Mill directed 'an amount oflabour, care, and
perseverance rarely, if ever, employed for a similar purpose, in endeavour-
ing to give, according to his own conception, the highest order of intellec-
tual education'. 22 The achievement was made all the more impressive by
the fact that James Mill was a major scholar and thinker in his own right,
and at the same time as he was making his son into one of the dominant
thinkers of his century he was also turning out some very substantial
achievements of his own. But despite John Stuart Mill's vast admiration
for his father, he was not so reticent about criticising him as Norbert
Wiener implied, and the son's respect for the parent's immense strengths
is balanced with a willingness to describe in considerable detail those of
his father's actions that he found oppressive.
Second, the Autobiography is remarkable by virtue of being a descrip-
tion of an education that in important respects was brilliantly successful.
As the father intended, his child did become a man who unquestionably
belongs within the category of geniuses. John Stuart Mill was a great
social reformer. Mill's insights on economics, politics and philosophy
helped to create modern democracy and combat poverty and injustice.
His ideas continue to influence modern thinking about social issues. The
Autobiography is indisputably an account of the development of a great
thinker.
Third, Mill himself was very conscious of the fact that the intellectual
qualities he possessed had to a large extent been deliberately instilled in
him by his father. 'Manufactured' was his own choice of word for this
state of affairs. In this respect Mill was totally different from other gen-
iuses of his era, such as Stephenson and Faraday, who were largely self
taught. He was equally unlike Darwin, who became a genius almost
despite the efforts of his teachers, who would have thought his choice of
natural history as a vocation quite absurd. There is no other genius for
whom the term 'manufactured' is quite as fitting as it is for Mill.
The shock of discovering in his early twenties that acquaintances
regarded him as being a manufactured man, capable only of reproducing
opinions stamped on him by others, led John Stuart Mill to change some
of his ideas rather abruptly. Even so, he could never forget that he was the
son of a parent who had set out to manufacture a genius. He was only too
aware that his successes and failures bore the marks of the effective if
heavy-handed education his father had personally implemented in order
to realise the intention of bringing up a child who would be, as James Mill
22 Mill (I97I), p. 5·
Manufacturing genius 121
had said to his friend and patron Jeremy Bentham, 'a successor worthy of
both ofus'. 23
The manner in which James Mill proceeded to implement that inten-
tion is described in John Stuart Mill's account, in his Autobiography, of his
extraordinary upbringing. John Stuart Mill was a scrupulously honest
man, and in most respects his narrative is as accurate and as fair as the
limitations of human memory allow. It is occasionally misleading,
however. The absence of information about his mother has encouraged
readers to infer that she lacked significance in Mill's early life to a degree
that is belied by family correspondence and visitors' observations. Also,
the lack of references to his eight brothers and sisters leaves the reader
with the impression that he was a more solitary child than could have
been possible in a houseful of children who displayed 'a plentiful lack of
manners, and as much impertinence, sometimes called impudence, as
any children need to have'. 24 The depiction of a constrained family life is
partly belied by a friend's observation that the Mill parents did not
prevent John's younger brother James from trundling his hoop round the
great hall of Ford Abbey, Jeremy Bentham's mansion, where the Mill
family enjoyed a number of summers. 25
John Stuart Mill was born in 1806, a year after the marriage of his
parents and just under three years before Charles Darwin's birth. His
father, James Mill, had been a prodigy himself. James Mill had been born
in Scotland, the son of a shoemaker, and by the time he was seven he had
drawn attention to himself as a clever child. His mother had high expecta-
tions for him, and made sure that unlike other children in humble rural
families he was spared all household chores and encouraged to spend his
time on his studies. On leaving school he came to the attention of a local
landowning family, the Stuarts (after whom John Stuart Mill was given
his middle name), who were looking for a suitable tutor for their daugh-
ter. The patronage of the Stuart family made it possible for James Mill to
attend Edinburgh University. But he found it hard to make a living in
Scotland, and in 1802 he moved to London, at the age of twenty-nine. He
thrived there, and by the time of his marriage three years later he was
23 Quoted in Mill (1971), p. xi. 24 Packe (1954), pp. 33-4·
25 The reasons for Mill's Autobiography giving the misleading impression that his father was
the only member of the family to have influence upon him may have stemmed from his
guiltily uncomfortable feelings, following a falling out at the time of his marriage to
Harriet Taylor, with whom he had already spent some years of - sexless - intimacy,
despite her already having a husband. Mill's way of keeping at bay the tensions this
uncomfortable domestic situation aroused in him was to see Harriet as the perfect
woman, against whom any hint of a slight was evidence of unjustified hostility. He
behaved uncharacteristically badly towards his sisters and his mother, apparently because
they did not quite succeed at solving the delicate problem of responding to the news of his
marriage in a way that he found acceptable.
122 Genius Explained
editing a newspaper and earning at least four or five times as much as the
fathers of George Stephenson and Michael Faraday were ever paid. Soon
after John Stuart Mill was born, James Mill decided to devote most of his
time to writing a history of India. That enormous project necessitated
financial sacrifices and was to drag on for almost twelve difficult years,
which roughly coincided with the period in which James devoted a major
portion of his considerable energies to the education of his eldest son.
Eventually, ten substantial volumes were completed, establishing James
Mill's reputation and bringing him a series of influential jobs and
financial security.
We must be careful not to misinterpret James Mill's reasons for shoul-
dering the onerous task of making his son into one the most powerful
thinkers of his century. He did not see himself as taking on some kind of
challenge or test. Nor did he regard the task as an experiment to discover
if it was actually possible for a parent to create a son who was capable of
major intellectual achievements. He rarely betrayed doubts about the
practicality ofhis intentions. James Mill's massive self-confidence left him
no reason for questioning his belief that what he set out to do was achiev-
able, so long as he put sufficient effort into it. As we have already seen, the
immense amount of work he invested in the task of educating his eldest
child was motivated by the desire to produce a son capable of carrying on
his own labours. Undeniably, taking that stance demanded a degree of
conceit or arrogance, in addition to a certain lack of sensitivity to the
child's own needs. And yet it has to be said in James Mill's defence that
building on his own enormously valuable intellectual work was a job
worth doing.
The young John Stuart Mill was certainly a child prodigy, but unlike
many prodigies he was never encouraged to see himself in that light. For
better or worse, his father was meagre with praise and constantly
reminded the boy of his imperfections. James Mill deliberately avoided
giving his son the impression that he was more able than other children of
his age. Consequently, 'From his intercourse with me I could derive none
but a very humble opinion of myself ... if I thought anything about
myself, it was that I was rather backward in my studies, since I always
found myself so, in comparison with what my father expected from me.' 26
As we shall see, there were negative as well as positive outcomes of the
approach taken by James Mill. Constant criticism is never beneficial for a
young person. But at least John Stuart Mill was spared the intrusive press
attention that can damage an immature child.
Since James Mill knew that he could hardly prevent the boy becoming
aware for himself that others of his age did not possess knowledge and
mental skills matching his, he was careful to persuade his son that he
should take no credit for any superiority he noticed in himself. He told
the boy that any advantages that he came to possess were solely the result
of the superior education he had received. Whether or not James Mill was
entirely correct about that is open to question, of course - a matter that
will be discussed in Chapter 9· But it is interesting to observe that this
very same view was emphatically held by all the other parents - those of
Witte, Wiener and Sidis - who have featured most prominently in the
present chapter.
In the Autobiography, John Stuart Mill reports that he had no recollec-
tion of his life before starting to learn Greek, but that he had been told
that instruction began when he was three years old. He remembered
having to memorise lists of common Greek words written on cards pre-
pared by his father: the Greek words were paired with the English equiva-
lents. Grammar was introduced later. It was not until he was seven or
eight that his father began teaching him Latin. His ignorance of that lan-
guage added considerably to his father's burden in teaching him Greek,
the reason being that there was no lexicon that enabled Greek words to be
translated directly into English, or vice versa. It was therefore necessary to
translate via Latin. Consequently, when John Stuart was set to making
translations (starting with Aesop's Fables and Xenophon's Anabasis), he
had to ask his father to provide him with the meaning of each and every
Greek word that he did not already know. So James Mill would have been
constantly interrupted. Nevertheless, 'This incessant interruption he,
one of the most impatient of men, submitted to, and wrote under that
interruption several volumes of his History and all else that he had to
write during those years.' 27 One cannot help wondering how often it
crossed James Mill's mind that life might have been easier had he decided
to teach his son a little Latin before starting on Greek.
Until he was eight, the more formal aspects ofJohn Stuart Mill's educa-
tion were restricted to Greek and arithmetic. He disliked arithmetic
intensely. He spent much of his time reading, and each morning, as he
accompanied his father on a walk before breakfast, he gave an account of
what he had read on the previous day, prompted by notes made on slips of
paper while he read. Perhaps surprisingly, the young Mill seems to have
quite enjoyed this activity of reporting back to his habitually impatient
parent. He says in the Autobiography that he remembers it as having been
a voluntary exercise rather than a prescribed one. Its pleasurableness
doubtless reflected the fact that the books he read were mostly ones that
27 Mill (I97I), p. 6.
124 Genius Explained
unlike the ones that Michael Faraday had been encouraged to engage in
by the author of The Improvement of the Mind. As the years went by the
training became more onerous and intensive until he reached the age of
fourteen and went to live in France for a year. His education continued
after that, of course, but subsequently he was not so closely supervised by
his father.
By the time he came to start writing the Autobiography at the age of
forty-seven, John Stuart Mill felt ready to look back on his extraordinary
education and make a careful appraisal of its strengths and its weaknesses.
There were, he perceived, many aspects of James Mill's teaching that
deserved praise. He was particularly glad that his father had taken pains
to make sure that he was familiar with logic, which benefited his thinking
greatly. And he could see that the father's approach was not infrequently
restrained and sensitive. For example, when John Stuart Mill as a young
child took it into his head to engage in the activity of what he called
'writing histories' his father avoided criticising these doubtless naive
efforts, being content to encourage his son 'in this useful amusement,
though, as I think judiciously, he never asked to see what I wrote; so that I
did not feel that in writing it I was accountable to any one, had the chilling
sensation of being under a critical eye'. 31
A particularly important positive aspect of James Mill's approach was
that it avoided rote learning and drill. At all times, what was learned had
to be genuinely meaningful to the child. In John Stuart Mill's own words,
Most boys or youths who have had much knowledge drilled into them have their
mental capacities not strengthened, but overlaid by it. They are crammed with
mere facts, and with the opinions or phrases of other people ... so often grow up
to be mere parroters of what they have learnt, incapable of using their minds
except in the furrows traced for them. Mine, however, was not an education of
cram. My father never permitted anything which I learnt, to degenerate into a
mere exercise of memory. He strove to make the understanding not only go along
with every step of the teaching, but if possible, precede it. Anything which could
be found out by thinking, I never was told, until I had exhausted my efforts to find
it out for myself. 32
James Mill also had the good sense to make sure that his son did not
become arrogant. He was very careful to protect him from situations in
which people would comment on his exceptional abilities or compare him
favourably with other boys of his age. In this the parent was almost too
successful, and John Stuart Mill became convinced that he was in no
respect inherently special, and even rather backward in his studies. He
was also sure that he possessed no capabilities that could not have been
acquired by any ordinary child who was given the training he had
received. 33
So James Mill's teaching had a number of good features. But it had
some glaringly negative aspects as well. For a start, the instruction Mill
provided for his son was not invariably as careful or as thorough as it
might have been, and even when it was not he tended to blame the child
for his failures. With mathematics, for example, James Mill could not
spare the time to teach his son properly, leaving him to learn almost
entirely from books. Yet he made his son aware of incurring parental dis-
pleasure by his inability to solve difficult problems 'for which he did not
see that I had not the necessary previous knowledge'. 34
There were further manifestations of this inability or unwillingness to
perceive that the boy's defects were the result of inadequate teaching
rather than some basic fault. For instance, the child found it hard to read
Greek aloud, and experienced particular difficulties in regard to arriving
at correct voice inflexions, or modulations. This irritated the father, who
sharply criticised each error. But according to his son, although James
told his son what he should have done he never took the more sensible
step of actually showing him how to do it. In these and other ways James
Mill failed to provide help which was sufficiently practical and concrete to
match the needs of a child. The defect here, according to the son, was of
'trusting too much to the intelligibleness of the abstract, when not
embodied in the concrete'. 35
James Mill also failed to teach his son to be tactful or diplomatic. John
Stuart Mill observed in the Autobiography that despite the fact he was not
an arrogant child, and never had an inflated opinion of his own powers,
adults often found him 'greatly and disagreeably self-conceited'. 36 The
reason for this perception, he suggested, was that he was disputatious,
and did not hesitate to contradict people, a habit that he believed to have
been acquired as a consequence of being encouraged to discuss matters
beyond his age with adults, without having been taught to respect or defer
to older people. He thought that the probable reason for his father failing
to correct 'this ill breeding and impertinence' was that he was simply
unaware of it, because in his father's presence the son 'was always too
much in awe of him to be otherwise than extremely subdued and quiet in
his presence'. 37
The awe in which John Stuart Mill held his father was a sympton of the
most crucial defect of all in the educational regime that James Mill
devised. That defect is encapsulated in the brief statement, in a discarded
draft of the autobiography, that 'mine was not an education oflove but of
33 Mill (I97I), p. 20. 34 Mill (I97I), p. 9· 35 Mill (I97I), p. I7.
36 Mill (I97I), p. 2I. 37 Mill (I97I), p. 2I.
Manufacturing genius 127
130
Einstein and the prodigies 131
star. But even Bidder needed all of thirteen minutes to do the enormous
mental calculating task of multiplying 257,689>435 by 356,875,649.
At this time Bidder had received very little formal education. He had
attended a small local school, but his biographer describes him as having
played truant much of the time, 3 and according to one report he could
still not read and write when he was eight. 4 It is significant that Bidder
started to become interested in arithmetic before he had learned to read,
because (although he would not have known it at the time) the most
effective methods for doing quick mental calculations are very different
from the pencil-and-paper calculating techniques that schools teach. Had
he learned arithmetic at school before he became interested in mental cal-
culating, George Bidder's school learning would have impeded his
progress as a mental calculator rather than helping him. As Bidder
himself explained, there is a fundamental difference between mental cal-
culating and calculations based on pencil and paper. In the former, but
not in the latter, it is vital to keep the amount of information that needs to
be stored (in memory) while the calculating is proceeding, to an absolute
mm1mum.
Bidder used the example of a multiplication problem to demonstrate
how mental arithmetic works. Someone is asked to multiply 279 times
373· He begins by multiplying 200 X 300, which comes to 6o,ooo. Next
200 X 70 (= J4,ooo) is added to that, making 74,000. Now the previous
total (6o,ooo) can be forgotten, and at any future moment as the calcula-
tion proceeds it is only necessary to keep in memory the most recent total.
Next, to the current total of 74,000 the calculator adds, successively,
200 X 3 (= 6oo), then 70 X 300 (= 21,000), 70 X 70 (= 4,900), 70 X 3
(=210), 9X300 (=2,700), 9X70 (=630), and, lastly 9X3 (=27).
Finally, after proceeding through each of these (relatively easy) steps, one
at a time, the calculator arrives at the final total, 104,067.
This is a more cumbersome technique for multiplying large numbers
than the ones taught in school. There would be no point in using it when
it is possible to store (in writing) the separate results of the various stages
of a calculation task. But the methods taught by schoolteachers would
present insuperable problems for someone who had to rely on mental cal-
culation alone. That is because of the sheer number of digits that the
person would have to store in memory in order to retain the results of the
intermediate steps in the calculation. Yet with the strategy described
above, all that needs to be retained in memory whilst the computation is
proceeding is a single running total.
A key implication of this fundamental difference between mental cal-
Bidder's case apart, there have been few factual parallels to the romanti-
cised view of child prodigies portrayed in novels like Body and Soul.
Nevertheless, it is not hard to locate historical instances of attention being
paid to young people from poor origins as a consequence of their unusual
capabilities, leading to some kind of sponsorship that enabled the individ-
ual to have access to formal education. Michael Faraday's mentor
Humphry Davy, the son of a woodcarver, benefited in this way. So did the
great navigator James Cook, and also John Stuart Mill's father, James
Mill. In the absence of some kind of sponsor or mentor, the chances of a
child born into poverty getting ahead were much reduced. In the rare case
of George Stephenson, the fact that he became a great engineer without
enjoying any of these advantages makes his achievements all the more
remarkable.
It is much more common for the identification of special abilities to
follow rather than precede the making available of favourable opportu-
nities and a fair amount of assistance. The majority of prodigies have
grown up in relatively affluent families. Many have had parents who took
a close interest in their child's early education. Even substantial libraries
were at hand in the family homes of a number of those distinguished
people who were prodigies as children, including the mathematician
Leibnitz, Jeremy Bentham the economic thinker, and the historian
Thomas Macaulay. A few prodigies, including the illustrious German
writer Goethe and the great mathematician Pascal, as well as John Stuart
Mill, have had parents who were impressive scholars themselves.
A 'rags to riches' element is present in a number of autobiographical
narratives written by child prodigies, and this has helped to spread the
idea that poverty is a common feature of their early lives. The true picture
is somewhat different, however, even if it is true that, as George
Stephenson's case demonstrates, early poverty and a lack of formal edu-
cation have not always prevented a young person from eventually achiev-
ing eminence. It is easy to be misled by the not-uncommon tendency of
successful people, including a number of former prodigies, to magnify
their achievements by exaggerating the poverty or misery of their origins.
According to George Bernard Shaw's accounts of his childhood, for
instance, his early life was dominated by penuriousness and the incon-
stancy of a rejecting mother. In reality, that playwright enjoyed the advan-
tages of a stimulating and lively home, which provided all kinds of
opportunities that were beyond the reach of most children. And even
when an autobiographical portrayal of childhood deprivation is largely
accurate, often that is not the whole story. For example, the author H. G.
Einstein and the prodigies 135
Wells recalled that his parents were poor and unhappily married, and not
well educated. He recollected spending his childhood years in a squalid
house that was damp and bug-infested, and in which all the carpets were
frayed and worn and the furniture old and discoloured. All that is entirely
true, but Wells' account does less than full justice to a number of mitigat-
ing factors. His father, who played cricket for Kent, was something of a
local celebrity, and also a keen reader and a thoughtful man who went to
great lengths to broaden his knowledge. Wells' mother, too, was a not
entirely ordinary woman. She kept a daily diary, and, unusually for a
working class wife in the 186os, she made a big effort to give her young
son a good start in life. She taught him to count, and when he was five she
pasted up large letters from the alphabet in the kitchen. Between them,
Wells' parents made sure that he was always supplied with books, paper,
and pencils. It was largely because of the encouragement they gave him
that by the time H.G. Wells was seven he was already, like his father, an
enthusiastic reader.
Another unfounded common belief is that the typical prodigy's special
abilities suddenly appear without any assistance at all. This view has
sometimes been promoted through the reports of parents who have
wanted to convince others that their child's accomplishments were a sign
that the child had been chosen to be the recipient of special God-given
powers. Such parents have tended to portray themselves as having made
no active contribution to their child's abilities, and simply looking on
admiringly as they saw their child's marvellous capabilities magically
unfolding.
It is conceivable that a few of these accounts may be reliable, but in a
substantial proportion of them the professed passivity of the parents is
clearly belied by the fact that their descriptions contain detailed informa-
tion about the child's achievements, which could never have been
obtained without a big investment of time and considerable planning. For
instance, the parents of one child prodigy claimed to have resisted any
temptation to help their child to learn, let alone actively teach her, and yet
they nevertheless kept a meticulous record of her progress, noting, for
instance, that her speaking vocabulary at sixteen months was 229 words,
and that at five years of age she introduced 6,837 words over a six-month
period, all of which had been listed and classified into the different parts
of speech. With another set of parents, who insisted that their daughter
learned to read entirely unaided and claimed that they only realised this
when they discovered her reading Heidi at the age of four, it turned out
that they too kept elaborate records of the child's accomplishments, such
as the precise letters she had learned at various ages, the time at which she
136 Genius Explained
first mastered the alphabet, her counting skills, and the colours she recog-
nised. 5 It is hard to believe that parents who have devoted as much time as
these people did to making detailed records of their children's progress
could possibly have avoided becoming actively involved in the children's
early learning.
7 Lehmann (r997).
138 Genius Explained
An account of Einstein's early life can provide some helpful insights into
possible relationships between being a prodigy in childhood and being an
adult genius. Ostensibly, of course, Albert Einstein seems a distinctly odd
choice for an example of a child prodigy. Nobody disputes his genius, but
the accepted view is that he was far from being a prodigy. He was, we are
told, a backward child who was born with an oddly shaped skull, making
him a late speaker and a poor student. He is said to have been a trouble-
maker at his high school (which he left prematurely) and a pupil who
gained low grades, failed examinations, and was particularly weak at lan-
guages. Unable to get the kind of job he sought on completing his educa-
tion, Einstein was obliged, we are told, to take a menial post in a patents
office. Matters were not helped by his father being a bankrupt. The
Einstein family, members of a persecuted Jewish minority, had to leave
their home in Germany and take up residence in Italy, where they
suffered from being alien immigrants who lacked even the basic security
conferred by citizenship. Against this unpromising background, it seems
hardly surprising that Einstein's sudden bursting into prominence as a
scientist of unique brilliance and originality has been regarded as
magical. Here is genius at its most mysterious. He must, it appears, have
been born to be a genius. No alternative explanation appears possible.
None of the above statements about Einstein is totally unfounded.
Nonetheless, and contrary to what is widely believed, the young Einstein
undoubtedly was a child prodigy, albeit a largely unrecognised one.
Numerous observations of the progress he made while still a child illus-
trate his prodigiousness. From very early days the young Albert Einstein,
born in 1879, made a distinctly favourable impression on others. Just a
few months after his second birthday his maternal grandmother was
writing to a relative that Albert, whom she described as sweet as well as
good, was already creating amusing ideas. Her letter is one of a number of
items of evidence obtained at the time of Einstein's early childhood that
firmly contradict the much repeated claim that his language development
was retarded. Another is an anecdote that can be precisely dated to the
time when he was aged two years and eight months. This describes him
reacting to being told on the occasion of the birth of his baby sister that he
now had a new playmate, by asking where were the wheels on this new
toy. The child's confusion is unexceptional, but the language develop-
ment of a two-year-old capable of articulating such a question cannot
have been impeded.
Like many intelligent children, Albert Einstein was sometimes reluc-
tant to talk, and a maidservant once called him stupid because she
observed he had a way of repeating everything twice. The most likely real
Einstein and the prodigies 139
temperament and interests. There were eight hours of Latin every week,
and four of Greek from the fourth year onwards. This left little time for
other subjects, and so there were only three mathematics classes per
week, and only two science and geography classes. Physics was not taught
at all until the seventh year. Fortunately for Einstein, he had made consid-
erable progress in those subjects by private study in his spare time, reach-
ing levels of attainment well beyond the school's requirements.
By the age of eleven or so Albert Einstein was reading about science
and philosophy in books that were beyond the understanding of most
children. He was already contemplating the conflicting claims of science
and religion, and had become convinced that much that he had read in
the Bible could not be true. At the same age he became enchanted by
mathematics. On encountering Pythagoras' theorem he determined to
prove it. He succeeded, but only after three weeks of the kind of strenuous
and unremitting contemplation that (although Einstein would not have
known it at the time) was a characteristic mental activity of his great pre-
decessor Isaac Newton. In common with Newton and a number of other
outstanding thinkers (including Galileo and Bertrand Russell) 8 Einstein
became particularly strongly attracted to the certainty and purity of
Euclid's geometry. Before the age of twelve he had quickly worked his way
through a geometry textbook and made a serious start on the study of
advanced mathematics. Such was his progress that the family friend who
had first encouraged his interest was soon finding it impossible to keep up
with the child.
Science and mathematics were not the only difficult subjects Einstein
began to master in his childhood. At the age of thirteen he studied - and
appears to have enjoyed and comprehended - Kant's notoriously daunt-
ing Critique of Pure Reason. A classmate from this period later recalled how
impressive a conversationalist the boy had already become. His main
interests were intellectual ones, although he could be a mischievous prac-
tical joker at school. He was also acquiring a love of music. His mother, a
capable pianist, had arranged for him to have lessons from the age of six.
For years the child made very little progress, but at thirteen he suddenly
acquired a passion for Mozart's sonatas, and leaped ahead, discovering
that 'love is a better teacher than a sense of duty - at least for me'. 9 His
much-admired expertise at playing the violin gave him enjoyment
throughout his life.
Clearly, the young Albert Einstein was indeed a child prodigy. His
accomplishments by the age of twelve were already far beyond the
average, especially in science and mathematics. Yet as is true of the other
Albert Einstein's father got into difficulties at times, but for substantial
periods he flourished and prospered.
At the time of Albert's birth, his parents lived in a comfortable apart-
ment in the city ofUlm, in southern Germany. They moved to Munich a
year later. It was a good marriage, and the child's home background was
harmonious as well as being unusually supportive. Einstein's mother and
father were educated people who took their parental responsibilities seri-
ously, making sure, for instance, that their eldest child always finished his
school homework. Both of them respected their Jewish origins but took
little notice of Jewish religion or ritual. They did not attend a synagogue,
and no specifically Jewish rites or customs were followed at home, and nor
were Jewish cooking rituals obeyed. Their reading, in which the works of
authors such as Schiller and Heine were prominent, mirrored that of
other cultured Germans of the time.
In short, Einstein's early family background was one that provided
large measures of mental stimulation for the growing child, and also gave
him the support and structure that would have enabled a child to flourish.
So both of those attributes of a home background that Csikszentmihalyi's
investigations have shown to be especially crucial for promoting mental
expertise and competence were present in abundance in the Einstein
home. For a growing scientist, his was an exceptionally privileged child-
hood. The advantages he was given could never have guaranteed that his
progress would be exceptional, let alone as prodigious as it turned out to
be, but they undoubtedly did contribute to his intellectual development.
Albert's uncle Jakob, his father's younger brother, was a qualified engi-
neer as well as an ambitious industrialist who at times enjoyed very con-
siderable success. At the time of Albert's birth Jakob Einstein was running
a firm that provided gas and water installations. In 1880 he was joined by
Albert's father, and the business was substantially enlarged by the pur-
chase of a company that made gas boilers. The brothers extended their
activities to the new field of electrical engineering, and by 1882 they were
participating in an international electro-technical exhibition that was
held in Munich, where they exhibited dynamos, arc lamps, light bulbs,
and a complete telephone system. As this side of the business expanded
and developed, the gas and water installation operations were faded
out and the boiler making works were abandoned. There was a further
expansion in 1885, when the brothers opened a new factory specialising in
electrical engineering. At its peak the firm employed two hundred
workers.
Unfortunately, the brothers were to overreach themselves. Their busi-
ness got into difficulties and eventually had to be liquidated. But the
company's failure could not alter the fact that the young Einstein had
Einstein and the prodigies 143
were not covered in any of the teaching provided at Zurich. For example,
only through working on his own was he able to master certain advances
that were crucial to his own special interests, in particular the theoretical
discoveries of physicists such as Michael Faraday's successor James Clerk
Maxwell, and Ludwig Boltzmann. But despite his independent streak,
Einstein managed to do extremely well at some ofhis examinations, partic-
ularly in the initial years. His exam performance in the fourth year was less
impressive, because by then his efforts to extend his mastery of mathemati-
cal physics were being largely directed by his own particular interests.
Nevertheless, on graduating Einstein was confident of getting a univer-
sity position, and he was surprised and depressed when he failed to do so.
A number of factors contributed to his lack of success. Sheer bad luck and
poor timing were two. Anti-semitism may have been another reason,
although there is no firm evidence of that. A more powerful cause of
failure was that during his student years, whilst Einstein undoubtedly did
make a strong impression on those professors who were subsequently in a
position to offer him employment, a number of them had found his inde-
pendent and non-subservient approach to his studies intensely annoying.
He had ruined his chances of getting employed by one of the professors
who was needing an assistant, by neglecting enough of the practical work
he was obliged to undertake to earn himself a reproof for 'lack of applica-
tion' . 11 Another professor who might have been able to find a job for him
had been antagonised by Einstein's lack of respect and by the rather
mediocre essay he had submitted. Yet another potential employer among
the Zurich professors was probably put off by the unapologetic tone of
Einstein's confession that he had failed to attend any of the man's semi-
nars. The frustrated Einstein was forced into taking a temporary teaching
position that provided an income for a few months. He became con-
vinced, almost certainly wrongly, that one of his professors was working
against him by giving him poor references.
Finally, however, in June 1902 Einstein did manage to find a job. It was
not a university position but a post in the Swiss Patent Office in Bern. He
had first heard about this opportunity early in 1901 from another student
who was a close friend, and whose father recommended Einstein for the
post.
A job in a patent office may appear to have represented a distinctly less
attractive proposition for Einstein than the university position he had
hoped for. In reality, however, as Einstein quickly appreciated, it was an
excellent opportunity. For a start, the post was permanent, offering better
pay and more security than a university assistantship, and good chances
13 Sosniak (r985).
150 Genius Explained
However hard parents try, they can never have complete control over
their children's experiences. That is partly because a person's experiences
are always partly determined by the individual's own biological endow-
ment. Consequently, with the possible exception of those rare instances
in which the children are identical twins whose parents strive to treat
them in precisely the same manner, there are always going to be substan-
tial differences between children in the way in which they actually experi-
ence those everyday events and activities that help determine their
interests and abilities.
Imagine, for example, being the elder or younger daughter of parents
who try hard to be entirely fair and even-handed in their approach to their
offspring. These parents conscientiously act towards one child in exactly
the same manner as they did towards their other child at the same age. 14
There comes a time, however, when the older sister notices that the
younger sister is being treated differently to her. Perhaps the parents are
less strict towards the younger sister, or expect less of her. Conversely, the
younger sister begins to notice that her sibling is being given more respon-
sibility, or is allowed to stay up later. In short, the older sister is experienc-
ing the life of an older sister, and the younger sister is experiencing family
life very differently. Over a period of time there will be many situations that
the two children experience in contrasting ways, with consequences that
will ensure that however similar the two girls' environments are they will
eventually become women who differ from one another in a variety of ways.
By combining each of the two states at any one of these three stages
with each state at the other stages, we arrive at eight possible categories of
people, as follows:
1 Men and women who did not have stimulating and supportive early
upbringings, were not child prodigies and did not produce any crea-
tive achievements as adults.
2 Individuals who did have a stimulating and supportive upbringing,
but did not become prodigies and never made any creative achieve-
ments as adults.
3 People who had a stimulating and supportive early upbringing, were
prodigies in childhood, but who, in maturity, did not make any
exceptional achievements.
4 Men and women who enjoyed a stimulating and supportive early
upbringing, were child prodigies, and did produce major adult
achievements.
5 Those who did have a stimulating and supportive early upbringing,
but were not child prodigies, and yet did have impressive achieve-
ments in adulthood.
6 People who did not have a stimulating and supportive early upbring-
ing, but were prodigies in childhood, and, as adults, made substan-
tial achievements.
7 Men and women who did not have a stimulating and supportive
early upbringing, but were nevertheless prodigies in childhood, and,
as adults, did not produce any creative achievements.
8 Individuals who did not have a stimulating and supportive early
upbringing, and were not prodigies in childhood, but who, as adults,
nevertheless produced creative achievements.
remind us that having a good early start does not guarantee outstanding
excellence in adulthood. Billy Sidis belongs in it, although he is untypical
both in being relatively well-known and in having had to endure an adult
life that was particularly unhappy. There are numerous reasons why not
every exceptionally promising young person will have a fulfilling or pro-
ductive adult life, or be capable of creative achievements. One is that there
is no guarantee that a child prodigy will develop all the qualities that are
needed in order to make exceptional mature accomplishments possible.
A musician, for instance, may be technically outstanding but lack certain
of the emotional or intellectual resources that make a performance dis-
tinctive. A mathematician may possess a mind furnished with exceptional
skills and knowledge but not have the drive necessary to sustain the
arduous intellectual effort that is required in order to make progress in
the face of difficulties. What is more, as we have seen, in some circum-
stances influences that accelerate a young person's progress sufficiently to
make it appropriate for the individual to be regarded as a prodigy may
also work to reduce rather than increase that individual's likelihood of
becoming capable of creative adult accomplishments. For example,
parents who are anxious for their children to excel at particular skills can
all-too-easily deprive them of experiences that help children to become
sufficiently independent and self-motivated to make the best of their
capabilities.
Knowledge about membership of all the other categories bears on
important questions concerning relationships between genius and the
circumstances that precede it. Perhaps surprisingly, with none of these
five categories is it totally impossible to identify exemplars. It appears,
then, that there may be few if any absolutely essential background factors,
in the absence of which it would be quite impossible for someone to
aspire to being a genius. Less surprisingly, a substantial proportion of
those people who are acclaimed as creative geniuses belong within the
fourth category, and that is true of a number of Nobel prizewinners
(Zuckerman, 1977). Mozart is one name that comes to mind. Others are
Yehudi Menuhin, the violinist, and among the other individuals who we
have encountered, John Stuart Mill and Norbert Wiener are both
members of this category.
Belonging to the fifth category are people who did enjoy a stimulating
and supportive early upbringing, but who were not child prodigies, and
yet did produce creative adult achievements. Although there are certain
areas of achievement in which it is very unlikely that someone will become
exceptionally capable unless considerable progress has already been made
by the end of childhood, that is not true of all fields of endeavour. Leo
Tolstoy, the novelist, and William James, the philosopher and psychologist
154 Genius Explained
brother of Henry James, are two examples of people who gave few early
indications of the exceptional creativity they were to display as adults.
Charles Darwin is another person who belongs in this category. All three
of these individuals came from wealthy families. Wealth makes it possible
for individuals to make false starts and delay committing themselves to a
career without incurring serious penalties, as Tolstoy was able to.
The final three categories include people who did not have early
upbringings that were particularly supportive or stimulating. In the sixth
group are people who, despite that, were prodigies in childhood and, as
adults, did make creative achievements. The lack of a good start in life is
clearly a disadvantage for a young person, and in many cases it has ruled
out the possibility of becoming a highly creative adult. Nevertheless, it is
possible to identify a few individuals who were prodigies and who did
produce major achievements despite having had no special advantages in
their early years. George Bidder definitely fits into this category, as does
James Mill, and Michael Faraday arguably belongs within it. He
undoubtedly produced major creative achievements, and he was certainly
a bright and precocious child, even if it would be stretching things a little
to assert that he was a child prodigy. Another instance of a person who
was a prodigy and also a genius, despite not having enjoyed an especially
stimulating early background, was J. M. W. Turner, perhaps England's
greatest artist.
To belong in the seventh category a person is required not to have had a
stimulating and supportive early upbringing, but nevertheless to have
become a prodigy in childhood, but without going on to produce creative
achievements in maturity. This is one category for which members might
be expected to be very rare if not non-existent, and it is certainly not easy
to identify individuals who clearly belong within it. All the same, a few can
be found. One such person was a talented young American mental calcu-
lator, who as a boy entered into a competition with George Bidder. His
name was Zerah Colburn, and he was born in Vermont, and his encoun-
ter with Bidder took place when Bidder was aged twelve and Colburn
probably fourteen. 15 Like George Bidder, Zerah Colburn began attract-
ing attention to himself in early childhood, beginning at the age of six
when his father heard him repeating multiplication tables. Colburn, like
Bidder, travelled round giving public demonstrations for money. Colburn
seems to have been even more precocious than Bidder as a young child. It
was reported that at the age of six he could solve problems such as squar-
ing 1>449, multiplying 12,225 times 1,223, and discovering how many
seconds there are in 2000 years.
15 Howe (rggo).
Einstein and the prodigies 155
Broadly speaking, child prodigies are young people who for one reason or
another seem to have made an unusually good start in life. So far as the
likelihood of a person becoming capable of mature creative accomplish-
ments is concerned, the fact that the person has been a prodigy in child-
hood is important, not because it points to some inherent special gift or
talent, but simply because it indicates that unusually fast progress has
already been made. Exceptional early progress does not make exceptional
adult achievements inevitable, and yet in many cases it does help make
them possible. That is especially true in those areas of achievement in
which mastery depends upon lengthy and concentrated training.
In fields in which the links between early learning and mature achieve-
ments are less straightforward, the advantages of being a prodigy may be
fewer. Among novelists, for example, the young Trollope was a day-
dreamer, the youthful Charles Dickens was an observant young person
who was keenly alert to the world he perceived, and the Bronte children
created shared imaginary worlds that depended upon the close relation-
ships which existed within a family consisting of intelligent and well-
informed individuals. It is true to say that each of these writers had
childhood experiences that contributed to their eventually becoming
156 Genius Explained
major novelists. However, for them there would have been relatively little
added advantage to be gained from their acquiring in childhood the kind
of highly specialised expertise that would have led to them being seen as
child prodigies.
The fact that it is not essential to have been a prodigy in order to
become capable of creative mature accomplishments does not mean that
there are no limits to what can be achieved in the absence of a good early
start. Stephenson could make himself into a great engineer only because
many of the skills an engineer needed in his time were practical ones that
he could practise in his everyday life. Modern-day equivalents of George
Stephenson are fairly rare, although the ranks of exceptional jazz-players
and other non-classical musicians have included some individuals with
little formal education in music. But as we perceived earlier, even in
Stephenson's era someone with his total lack of school education could
never have become a great scientist like Faraday or Darwin, because
science cannot be learned from everyday experience. A career like
Faraday's is hard to imagine today, with schools providing basic science
education and young scientists having to gain a very substantial body of
knowledge and skills in order to become prepared for making original
contributions. Charles Darwin is a particularly a well-known example of a
person who was a genius, but without having seemed at all remarkable as
a child, but his career was only possible because his childhood activities
did provide him with an invaluable fund of knowledge and skills that he
was able to build upon later in life.
7 The expertise of great writers
157
158 Genius Explained
other makers of creative achievements, put enormous efforts into the task
of acquiring exceptional expertise.
As any recreational tennis player or bridge enthusiast or amateur musi-
cian knows, it is possible to spend enormous amounts of time practising
an activity without becoming dramatically better at it. If practising is
essential for acquiring high levels of skill, it certainly does not guarantee
it. Researchers investigating expertise have tried to isolate what it is that is
different about the kinds of training and practice activities that do
produce high levels of expertise, compared with the kinds of practising
that do no more than maintain an amateur's modest level of mastery.
The kind of practice that is necessary in order to acquire exceptional
expertise has to be considerably more intensive and much more system-
atic. Also, the particular practice activities that are engaged in need to be
much more closely related to the specific improvements that are being
aimed at. For example, where an amateur tennis player wanting to
improve a weakness at backhand volleying might simply play more games
and look for opportunities to use backhand volleying shots, a professional
would be more likely to have training sessions in which the coach made
sure that there were hundreds of opportunities to practise the particular
actions that required attention. This kind of formal and deliberate prac-
tising is liable to be less inherently motivating than the performing activ-
ities that people prefer to engage in. 1
Creative writers, just like those people whose creativity takes other forms,
take a long time to master their craft. A reader of someone's 'first novel'
would be unwise to infer that the book in the hand represents its author's
earliest attempt at serious fiction. A first published novel is almost never
its originator's first substantial writing project. All successful authors have
been lifelong readers, and the majority gained the habit of committing
their thoughts to paper at an early age. It is not uncommon for distin-
guished writers to have made their first try at writing a book well before
the end of childhood. John Stuart Mill did that, as did John Ruskin and
H. G. Wells, among others.
Inevitably, these juvenile efforts are often naive, crude and uneven.
Nevertheless, as well as displaying their authors' often remarkable preco-
city they are significant in other ways. First, the manufacture of a whole
book by a child signals a devotion to the difficult enterprise of writing that
augurs well for an author's future progress. At the very least it demon-
strates a willingness to persist at arduous intellectual pursuits for lengthy
periods of time. Second, the fact that a child is actually capable of writing
a book indicates that the young writer has already made a good start at the
training at writing that progress builds upon.
The early literary lives of the Bronte family exemplify the capacity for
juvenile efforts at writing and the circumstances giving rise to them to
influence as well as portend literary careers. Each of the four younger
Bronte siblings, whose mother had died when the eldest, Charlotte, was
only five, became seriously involved in writing as a recreational activity at
around the age of ten. Soon after the deaths in 1825 of the two oldest
sisters, Maria (born 1814) and Elizabeth (born 1815), the older of the four
surviving children, Charlotte (born 1816) and Branwell (born 1817)
started to produce a series of tales about imagined worlds. The earliest
compositions were little more than extensions of childish play, bearing all
the usual hallmarks of childish writing. They were poorly spelled, largely
unpunctuated, and closely modelled upon the stories and newspaper arti-
cles that the children had most recently been reading. Remarkably,
however, a firm habit of writing about imaginary worlds became estab-
lished, and it persisted for a decade, making major invasions into the
growing authors' time. Over the years the initially childish efforts at
writing became more and more sophisticated and adventurous, and they
increasingly took verse as well as prose form. A degree of continuity was
maintained, however, in the content as well as the form of the narratives,
and certain of the story characters that first made their appearance in the
early 1830s were still being written about at the end of that decade. For all
of the Brontes, their childhood literary activities were immensely impor-
tant influences on their later capabilities and accomplishments, providing
frequent and regular opportunities to practise and extend the writing
skills that all authors depend upon.
The imaginary worlds had their beginnings around 1826, when
Charlotte and Emily, who shared a bed, invented simple unwritten plays,
not unlike those created in many children's imaginary play. The very first
of the Brontes' plays took most of the characters from toys, especially
their brother Branwell's toy soldiers. The earliest surviving play that was
written down, by Branwell, is set in Lorraine and concerns the imaginary
intrigues and battles between would-be rulers, in the course of which the
imagined events include a rebellion and a siege. As the Brontes' biogra-
pher Juliet Barker notes,Z most of the essential elements of their juvenile
writings were already in place at that time, including political rivalries,
battles, and rebellions that are played out within fantasy kingdoms.
Numerous sources were drawn upon. A particularly important inspira-
tion was Blackwood's Magazine, a monthly journal containing a wide
2 Barker (1994), p. 152. My discussion of the Brontes' early writing activities has drawn
heavily upon this excellent book, The Brontifs, by Juliet Barker.
160 Genius Explained
came much later, following many years during which they constantly gave
themselves practice in writing, and experimented at it, their efforts
fuelled by the obvious delight that came from manufacturing stories
about imaginary worlds.
Understandably, the children were drawn to what they found exotic
and dramatic. Toy soldiers newly added to Branwell's collection were
quickly shared out among his siblings, given names, and provided with
invented backgrounds. One box of soldiers quickly became the 'Young
Men', a brave band of twelve young Englishmen who had landed in an
African kingdom after an exciting journey that involved dangerous adven-
tures. Once in Africa these heroic young men encountered further
dangers, which included 'an Immense and terrible monster his head
touched the clouds was encircled with a red and fiery Halo his nostrils
flashed forth flames'. 4 From time to time a character would be trans-
formed into an entirely different individual. For instance, a soldier that
Emily initially named Gravey suddenly acquired the name of a real
person, the Arctic explorer William Perry, whose adventures had recently
been described in Blackwood's Magazine. But Emily's older sister
Charlotte often refused to permit such transformations. After naming
one of her soldiers after her current hero, the Duke of Wellington, she
insisted on keeping him in the cast throughout a number of plays, until
she eventually permitted him to be superseded by his sons.
The character of Perry was not the only borrowing from Blackwood's,
which had also supplied the African setting. The Brontes had discovered
in Blackwood's Magazine a lengthy review of a book describing an expedi-
tionary mission from the west coast of Africa into the interior. The stories
about the Young Men lifted a number of place names and names of kings
from that report. The imaginary land took up a large chunk of western
Africa, and included a number of features that were real, including the
Gambia and Niger rivers, and some that were based on real places, such
as a city at the mouth of the Gambia that was renamed Verdopilis by
Branwell and Verreopolis by Charlotte, as well as others that were entirely
fictitious. The young authors decided to divide the region into a confed-
eracy of states, each of which was ruled by a soldier belonging to one of
the children. Other soldiers ruled over islands situated off the coast. The
land as a whole became known as Glasstown. Later, following the
destruction of its main city at a later stage in the development of the
stories, it was to be renamed Angria. Within Glasstown were to be found
a rich range of people, places, events, and buildings, drawn partly from
the children's own experiences and partly from things they had read
magazine, the Glasstown poet 'Young Soult' was actually based on a real
person, Marshal Soult, a commander under Napoleon Buonaparte. But
as was to happen not infrequently in the Brontes' later juvenile writings, a
character invented by one sibling might be mercilessly abused by another.
Young Soult, for example, whose inventor, Branwell, regarded him with
some affection, is fiercely attacked in one of Charlotte's pseudonym's pro-
ductions, where he is amusingly but sharply caricatured as a pompous
poetaster named 'Henry Rhymer'.
Magic and mystery are frequent ingredients in the earlier stories. There
were a number of borrowings from The Arabian Nights in Charlotte's
tales, and she also adapted legends and tales mysteries that had appeared
in Blackwood's Magazine. Fabulous and exotic locations were favoured,
even in connection with characters based on real men and women. For
instance, Charlotte situated her Duke of Wellington in a white marble
palace among olive trees, palms, and myrtles, and other characters were
placed in settings filled with gold and diamonds. The playful author was
not too solemn to nudge her readers with an occasional comment on her
own facility at creating incongruous mixtures of fantasy and reality. She
broke off at one point from a description of the magnificence of a lavishly
decorated emerald dome to point out to any reader who might not have
already noticed it, that 'you are gazing on the production of a mighty
imagination'. 5
It might have been expected that as the children matured their imagi-
nary worlds would lose some of their allure. But that did not happen until
well into adulthood. There was simply no need to entirely abandon the
societies that had been invented with the first Glasstown stories, because
within the imagined worlds there was ample scope for developments
catering for the newer interests and preoccupations of authors who were
no longer children. Some of the later creations were complex literary
achievements, often scholarly as well as creative, sometimes in verse form
rather than prose. The two youngest sisters, Emily (born 1818) and Anne
(born 1820) contributed more frequently as time progressed, although
the majority of the surviving booklets are by Charlotte and Branwell.
Political elements became more prominent. Detailed histories were sup-
plied. Characters were more effectively delineated and developed, with
one of Branwell's creations, 'Alexander Rogue', an evil person who had
appeared in a number of stories (including some by Charlotte, who calls
Rogue 'deceitful, bloody and cruel' as well as 'skilled in all the sleight-of-
hand blackleg tricks of the gaming table' 6) taking centre stage in the first
of the books to depart from the miniature form of the earlier ones. The
reader of Sir Walter Scott's romances and obsessed with Byron - as were
numerous young women at the time - was writing a lengthy poem in
which one of the heroines laments her aristocratic lover, a marquis, who
has abandoned her for another woman. The latter affair was also
described in another story, this time in prose. Love and romance now
became the dominant topic of Charlotte's fiction. During this period all
the men she writes about are dashing, tall and attractive, and all her hero-
ines beautiful. They are also invariably aristocratic, even if their high birth
is sometimes a secret, hidden from all until the final pages. The romantic
ingredients of the stories are frequently combined with melodrama.
Passionate but unprincipled dark-eyed beauties are besotted with amoral
male characters whose scornful sneers betray their wicked natures. But
there are also more profound and thoughtful passages, in which serious
issues are discussed. Many elements of the Bronte sisters' published
novels can be detected in their later juvenilia.
The imaginary worlds continued to engage Charlotte's attention,
serving at times as a refuge and a solace, well into the stage of her life at
which she was starting to make a serious effort to earn a living from
writing. By this time she was learning to use some new techniques, experi-
menting in her Angrian stories with forms that she subsequently drew
upon in her published novels. For example, in an Angrian story written in
1838 she first used the device of narrating a tale through a series ofletters
between the characters. 7 At around the same time she was also beginning
to write about women who were more real and ordinary than the exoti-
cally beautiful creations that populate most of the earlier Glass town and
Angria stories. She was starting to depict females with more of her own
characteristics, plainer and more lifelike young women who were not
unlike the realistic heroine ofJane Eyre.
The long-lasting juvenile experiments in which Branwell and his three
sisters created and explored their secret worlds hugely influenced the
development of their imaginations and the growth of their writing skills.
The activities that produced their voluminous early writing equipped
each of the Brontes with the technical expertise a creative author needs.
Their habit of producing works quickly, without spending time revising
their prose, helped to make their writing fluent. The fact that their pro-
ductions were hidden from potentially censorious adult eyes ensured that
the young Brontes were able to explore and extend their literary powers in
a kind of protected haven, which allowed their confidence as writers to
grow as they matured, unconstrained by thoughts of what outsiders might
think. It is virtually unknown for a single family comprising four children
to produce three eminent female authors plus a talented brother who too,
but for ill-fortune, might well have been capable of impressive literary
achievements. It is hardly conceivable that each of a group of three sisters
could have created major novels had it not been for the fact that writing
activities played such a prominent part in their early lives.
Charlotte herself was fully aware of the benefits she gained from her
early writing. All the same, as Juliet Barker observes, 8 some of the habits
that Charlotte picked up in writing about Glass town and Angria had to be
discarded in order for her to become capable of the more realistic fiction
of her mature novels. Her juvenile writing is typically exotic, florid, and
extravagant. It is dramatic and often highly imaginative, lacking the sim-
plicity and down-to-earth characterisation that was needed in order to
produce a novel like Jane Eyre. Barker argues that it was difficult for
Charlotte to shake off 'her old bad habits of Gothic exaggeration'. 9 She
suggests that Charlotte's incomplete success at doing that mars some of
her work, including her first serious novel, The Professor, which was com-
pleted in 1846 when she was just thirty. It was rejected by a number of
publishers and did not appear in print until after her death.
Emily and Anne had less to unlearn, because there was not so marked a
conflict in either style or content between their juvenile writings and their
mature work. Juliet Barker notes that the world of Wuthering Heights is in a
number of respects similar to the world of Gonda!. Both draw on the
descriptive passages of Walter Scott as well as moorland life in Yorkshire
amid the landscape Emily could see from her own home. 10 Heathcliff has
a forerunner in a Gonda! character who first appeared ten years before
the writing of Wuthering Heights, a mysterious doomed outlaw whose sole
redeeming feature, like Heathcliff's, is his love for a beautiful woman.
Themes revolving around passionate adult love between individuals who
have grown up together and then been separated or cast into exile are
encountered in Gonda! tales and poems. Views about death that are
explored in Wuthering Heights are presaged in Gonda! poetry. Barker
points out that the drunken debauchery and casual cruelty that shocked
readers of Wuthering Heights are common elements in the Gonda! tales.
The decision of the neglected wife in Anne Bronte's The Tenant of Wildfell
Hall to nurse her debauched husband in his days of decline echoes a
Gonda! story in which Zenobia, the long-suffering wife of the unfaithful
Northangerland (previously the demagogue and adventurer Alexander
Rogue) makes the identical choice.
ventionality and perhaps also in the emotional intensity of its hold over
the minds of the young people who created the secret worlds of
Glasstown, Angria, and Gonda!. But at least as a source of experiences
that have nurtured the acquisition of skills and capabilities, other young
people's early circumstances have been just as significant. One conven-
tional and not unrealistic image of a successful person's early life, which
depicts a diligent and studious but otherwise unexceptional child making
the best of excellent early educational opportunities, is more frequently
personified in scientists- such as Marie Curie, for example- than in crea-
tive writers, as is evident from Harriet Zuckerman's study of the lives of
Nobel prizewinners. 11 Nevertheless, there are some writers whose early
progress is superior to the average but not altogether extraordinary, and
marked more by careful application to learning than by remarkable pre-
cocity. The author of Middlemarch, arguably the greatest novel in the
English language, was such a person in her younger days, although in
common with Curie she was to become notably less conventional in the
arrangements she made for her adult life.
Mary Anne Evans (later Mary Ann and eventually Marian: the name
'George Eliot' was only a nom-de-plume) grew up in the English mid-
lands. Born in 1819, three months after the infamous Peterloo Massacre-
a bloody suppression of peaceful demonstrators against taxes on corn
which came to epitomise what was most repressive about traditional
British society- she was a contemporary of the Bronte sisters. Like them,
she read widely as a child, enjoying the prose of Walter Scott and the verse
of Robert Southey, as they did, and later, the writings of Lord Byron. Like
them she attended a boarding school. Like them, she lacked close bonds
with a mother. In her case this was not a consequence of death but an
outcome of her mother's conspicuous failure to establish a warm relation-
ship with her. In common with Charlotte Bronte, Evans was acutely con-
scious of her lack of conventional beauty. Like the Brontes she was
religious in childhood and was affected by the religious controversies of
the time. Like them, as she approached adulthood she was oppressively
aware of her lack of options for the future. There were few obvious
choices available to her, and none of them would have seemed attractive.
They included being a governess, or a teacher, or perhaps a spinster
devoting her time to the care of an aged parent. In common with the
Brontes and (to an even greater extent) with John Stuart Mill, in her first
years as an adult she was strongly aware that her education had equipped
her with intellectual capacities that she was not at all certain how best to
utilise.
In other respects Mary Anne Evans was not at all like any of the Bronte
11 Zuckerman (r977).
168 Genius Explained
sisters. In sharp contrast to them, until she was a mature woman she did
not even consider the possibility of devoting large amounts of her time to
writing fiction. At the age of twenty Charlotte Bronte was already making
enquiries into the possibility of making a living through writing, and
asking for advice from Robert Southey, then the Poet Laureate. In
Evans's case it was not until she was already an experienced writer that
she began to think seriously about producing novels. What little early
fiction she wrote was largely realistic, and it contained nothing compar-
able to the strongly fantastic elements that are evident in the juvenilia of
all the Brontes. Their juvenile writings are often very funny, but there is
remarkably little humour in Evans' early efforts: at that time she was
invariably serious. Unlike the Brontes she seems to have been habitually
earnest as well as diligent, concerned with self-improvement and anxious
to transform and reshape herself through her growing intellectual
powers. As soon as she could, she removed herself both physically and
psychologically from the bourgeois world of her childhood, quite unlike
the Brontes, none of whom even considered abandoning their Yorkshire
roots. As a young person she was priggish, puritanical and moralistic.
Her biographer Frederick Karl asserts that she read more and knew
more and thought more than anyone in the mid-Victorian period. 12 That
is not an accomplishment that any of the Brontes would have aspired to
rival.
These differences ensured that the nature of Mary Anne Evans's train-
ing for her career in writing was strikingly different from the Brontes'.
And yet it was similar in one vital respect: like theirs, her preparation was
extensive, thorough, and long-lasting. The seriousness of her commit-
ment to becoming an especially knowledgeable woman was already
beginning to be apparent by the age of ten or eleven. At that time there
was nothing ostensibly prodigious about her development: as Frederick
Karl remarks 13 she did not sparkle or display quicksilver intelligence.
What she did already display to an unusual extent were qualities of gravity
and seriousness. Karl suggests that even then she was already looking
beyond childhood and anxious to move into an adult life in which she
imagined herselfbecoming more independent and free of the restrictions
that children find tiresome. Her reported response at a children's party,
when asked why she seemed unhappy, 'I don't like to play with children. I
like to talk to grown-up people.' 14 points to a profound irritation with the
state of childhood.
Her formal education was impressively good. Mary Anne's father, an
upwardly mobile and highly successful man who was employed as agent
on a large aristocratic estate, went to some pains to ensure that his daugh-
ter was given better educational opportunities than were customary for a
girl at the time. He was unusually attentive to her in the early years of her
childhood, often taking her with him as he made his rounds of the proper-
ties that he administered.
At the age of eight she was sent to a boarding school. The school was
not in itself remarkable, but it enabled Evans to become acquainted with
an intense young teacher named Maria Lewis, who was to be an impor-
tant mentor for the future novelist, and someone whose own interests and
attitudes ensured that Mary Anne would be exposed to various influences
that were outward-looking and intellectual, in contrast with the narrowly
provincial values that reigned unquestioned within her own family. She
was also now exposed to evangelical Christianity, bringing her into a form
of religion that was more fervent and more emotional than the traditional
kind of worship she was used to. Hence the effects of Lewis's attentions
on her young charge were thoroughly subversive, inclining her to question
the expectations of a family whose way of life she was already finding
irksome. As Karl points out, Robert Evans would have been horrified had
he known what was going on. Eventually, Mary Anne would outgrow the
tutelage of Maria Lewis and discard the religious beliefs that were so
important for her mentor. However, throughout a fifteen-year period
Maria Lewis was a significant intellectual influence. She played a key role
in enabling the growing child to extend her capabilities and become a
woman who had outgrown her conservative early background and devel-
oped into a clever and well-informed young person with a mind of her
own, and someone whose interests and values were not at all narrow or
provincial.
By ten or eleven, there was already a fierce intensity in the way in which
she absorbed knowledge from books, to the benefit of her own fast-
expanding mental powers. She was already reading Scott's novels. She
devoured poetry, and within a year or two would acquire an abiding
passion for Milton, whose serious and disciplined approach was to be a
lasting influence on her own work. Her father perceived that she was out-
growing her first school, and Mary Anne was moved to an above-average
educational establishment where for the first time she was able to receive
effective instruction in French, as well as in the traditional school subjects
such as history, arithmetic, and English. Characteristically, at the end of
her first year she won the school prize in French. The prize was a copy of
Pascal's Pensees, hardly an obvious choice for a child not yet thirteen, but
one that she nevertheless took to and seems to have rapidly mastered. She
also learned music. As Einstein was to do some generations later, Mary
Anne Evans became a good instrumentalist. The great novelist's expertise
170 Genius Explained
must have known that remarkably few other people would have possessed
the capacity to equal her feat.
entirely imaginary. One could hardly imagine the painfully shy Emily
Bronte pausing as she walked through a local market, as the self-assured
young Dickens did, to buy a bag of cherries and pop them, one at a time,
into the mouth of a dirty little child who was being carried on the shoul-
der of a coal-heaver. 15 But there are some similarities, of course. Like the
novels of the Brontes and George Eliot, the fiction of Dickens draws
extensively upon his own memories of childhood, and it does so in a
manner that is more direct, and certainly more obvious, than could be
envisaged in the creative achievements of, say, a composer or a scientist.
And Dickens' achievements, like those of the other novelists, drew upon
skills and capabilities that had been gradually and painstakingly acquired.
Like the others, Dickens gave himself a thorough training, and worked
very hard at becoming a writer.
As a preparation for a future novelist, Dickens' childhood had been rich
in two kinds of experiences. First, there was a strong family tradition of
story telling, and he had plenty of opportunities to absorb the knack of
putting together narratives that kept an audience in suspense. Second, at
an early age he gained the habit of reading regularly, a habit which lengthy
bouts of illness helped to instil. Yet there was little in Dickens' childhood
that prepared him for writing as such. As a boy he was certainly alert, sen-
sitive, and unusually observant, and especially after his months in the
blacking factory and his father's period of imprisonment as a debtor he
was sharply aware of the value of striving for qualifications and the
material security they could bring. But in comparison with other future
authors such as Mary Anne Evans and the Brontes he had far less actual
experience at expressing himself in written language, even though he had
been known to send occasional reports to newspapers while he was still at
school.
Strangely enough it was his father, whose way of life Charles Dickens
regarded in many respects as something to avoid rather than emulate,
who inspired his first serious training as a writer. After retiring from the
Navy Post Office and at the age of forty-one, John Dickens surprisingly
and uncharacteristically took it upon himself to master shorthand, in
order to increase his chances of getting himself a post in journalism. At
fifteen, already working as a law clerk, Charles took it into his head that if
he too learned shorthand he could become a reporter and earn much
more than he was currently getting, and it also struck him that a job in a
newspaper could be considerably livelier than his own position in a law
firm was turning out to be.
He found that learning shorthand was a difficult challenge, and one
that he did not enjoy at all, but he persisted at it doggedly and before
reaching the age of seventeen was ready and prepared to work as a
reporter. From this time onwards, working initially as a freelance legal
court reporter and later in a series of increasingly demanding journalistic
posts, his way of life encouraged him to concentrate on extending his
skills at communicating in writing. He was anxious to improve on his edu-
cation, demonstrating his devotion to self-improvement by applying at
the earliest possible time- his eighteenth birthday- for a ticket of admis-
sion to the British Museum Reading Room. He used that ticket produc-
tively, recalling in later years that his hours of study there had been some
of the most valuable ofhis life.
From his court reporting job he soon moved to a better position as a
parliamentary reporter for a periodical. This gave him more opportu-
nities to shine as well as enabling him to learn how Britain was governed
at the time. He took on occasional other assignments, succeeding to the
extent that by the age of nineteen he was sometimes earning as much as
twenty-five guineas in a week, which compared remarkably well with the
six shillings a week that he had been paid for working in the blacking shop.
At twenty-five he moved to a secure post with the Morning Chronicle,
where his job involved reporting on political meetings and elections in the
provinces. There was a good deal of travel, making for a busy and exciting
life that suited an energetic young man. He found numerous opportu-
nities to add to his income and his writing skills by undertaking additional
assignments, and by this time he had also begun to write short stories. At
the age of twenty-one he sent one of these to the Monthly Magazine. It was
accepted and quickly published. 16 He was asked for more, and they were
soon pouring out of him. By the summer of 1834 he was writing under the
pseudonym 'Boz' and making himself a reputation, and fame and wealth
were just around the corner.
Even those major Victorian authors whose autobiographical state-
ments have suggested otherwise put plenty of effort into gaining training
and experience at writing. Elizabeth Gaskell was constantly reading and
writing from an early age, keeping journals, making notes, recording
stories and conversations. Anthony Trollope, despite his self-depiction of
having been a dunce at his school, Harrow, won an essay prize in English
and kept a continuous journal for a ten-year period starting at the age of
fifteen. The fact that he had a literary mother would have encouraged him
to perceive the possible value of expertise at writing. He did acknowledge
the contribution to his development as an author of his early writing
activities, which he said 'habituated me to the rapid use of pen and ink,
176
Inventing and discovering 177
5 Gruber (rg8r).
180 Genius Explained
supposed, especially (but not only) in the sciences. Third, I point out that
even when highly creative people work alone they are usually building
upon earlier accomplishments by themselves and their contemporaries,
rather than starting completely from scratch. Fourth, I draw attention to
the fact that, geniuses, like everyone else, habitually see the creative activ-
ities they engage in as being difficult and arduous. Together, these obser-
vations contribute firm backing for the conclusion that the problem-
solving activities involved in the most remarkable creative achievements
and accomplishments are not fundamentally distinct from those that con-
tribute to the everyday accomplishments of people whose expertise would
not be regarded as being extraordinary.
On giants' shoulders
That the shoulders of one's predecessors can provide an artistic or intel-
lectual voyager with an advantageous viewpoint has become something of
a cliche, but it is a telling observation all the same. Many factors can affect
the way in which a creatively active individual makes use of earlier
thinkers' contributions. Reverence for previous discoverers' efforts- as is
evident in the remark by the twelfth-century writer, John of Salisbury,
that we are like dwarfs who see further than the giants of the past, but only
because we are raised up by their superior size - can be judicious and
healthy, so long as it does not stand in the way of change and innovation.
Conversely, a degree of disrespect or even contempt for the accomplish-
ments of earlier generations- as is illustrated in the remark about Newton
that he did not just stand upon the shoulders of giants but stamped on
them 6 - need not be disadvantageous provided that it does not lead to
crucial insights being ignored.
The giants' shoulders metaphor is a fitting one, and it is not easy to
think of counter-examples. It is doubtful whether there are any geniuses
who have not greatly profited from the efforts of their predecessors. There
have been remarkably few cases of innovators whose contributions were
unanticipated and original to the extent that it has proved impossible to
detect contributing influences. Significantly, such instances are not only
rare but almost always refer to individuals in connection with whom our
knowledge of the actual background circumstances is very sparse, such as
Archimedes. Even the most original discoveries and inventions have had
crucial antecedents. Turing's Universal Machine, a fair candidate to be
considered as the first 'real' computer, drew inspiration from Babbage's
nineteenth-century design for a computer, and Babbage in turn, built on
previous developments.
6 Bragg (1998), p. 96.
Inventing and discovering 181
8 Weisberg (r993).
184 Genius Explained
ers turned their efforts to flight included Samuel Langley, who began by
testing a series of model planes in order to help discover the optimal
dimensions. In 1896 a steam-powered model he had constructed reached
a height of 100 feet. Yet another able pioneer was Octave Chanute, an
engineer like Lilienthal. In common with Lilienthal, Chanute experi-
mented with gliders, and one of his planes stayed aloft for ten-second
periods, travelling about 250 feet.
All three of these inventors produced published accounts of their
endeavours, and these were carefully studied by Wilbur and Orville
Wright during the period when they were beginning to develop their own
flying machines. So the Wright brothers were definitely not starting from
nowhere, and their ideas did not generate in any kind of vacuum. And far
from being either uneducated or unprepared, the Wrights had grown up
in a home environment that was rich in opportunities to learn about
machines. Their first introduction to powered flight had taken place when
the older brother, Wilbur, was no more than eleven, at which time the
boys were given a rubber-powered toy helicopter. By the time they
became seriously interested in the possibility of trying to construct an
aeroplane, they were already trained mechanics who understood trigo-
nometry and algebra and had had some success at developing new tech-
niques for manufacturing bicycles. Their subsequent work involved the
efficient exploitation of their carefully acquired knowledge and expertise,
over a lengthy period of time. They worked on a series of problems,
always drawing upon any relevant data that other inventors had made
available, and they proceeded in a highly organised manner. Their strat-
egy involved making detailed plans and setting themselves intermediate
targets that formed stepping stones leading towards their eventual goal.
Of course there were dramatic moments, of which the most spectacular
took place in December 1903, when one of the Wright brothers' flying
machines took flight and stayed in the air for almost a minute, in which it
travelled not quite half a mile. Yet the invention of powered flight was
characterised less by sudden breakthroughs or spontaneous insights than
by rigorous planning, careful analysis, and much trial-and-error experi-
mentation. The Wright brothers' eventual success only appears sudden
or unexpected to those who are unaware of the efforts that preceded it.
The single moment at which powered flight can be said to have been
invented is just as elusive as the precise time at which steam engines
arrived.
Creativity in the arts is not entirely different. Sudden insights and 'aha'
experiences figure prominently in many accounts of artistic advances,
and a number of writers and other artists have sincerely believed that their
own work was created from thoughts and ideas that have suddenly pre-
Inventing and discovering 185
sented themselves, perhaps after swimming around for some time in the
unconscious mind. Coleridge's account of the writing of Kubla Khan,
according to which, on awakening from a dream-filled sleep, 'he appeared
to himself to have a distinct recollection of the whole, and taking his pen,
ink, and paper, instantly and eagerly wrote down the lines that are here
preserved' 9 is especially dramatic, although it is but one of a number of
similar accounts by literary authors. Dostoyevski made similar claims,
and insisted that 'a creative work comes suddenly, as a complete whole,
finished and ready, out of the soul of a poet' 10 • But whenever it has been
possible to find evidence about the real circumstances in which creative
achievements have been made, it turns out that they are not at all like that.
Coleridge's story, which reports events supposed to have taken place
almost twenty years earlier, is contradicted by evidence that he deliber-
ately drew upon various sources. Some of these suggested the poem's
images; others provided actual phrases that appear in the poem as pub-
lished. Contrary to his statement that he immediately wrote down the
poem in its final form, Coleridge made a preliminary version which he
subsequently altered. In the case of Dostoyevski, his claim that creative
works appear suddenly in a finished completed form is firmly refuted by
an analysis of the notebooks he filled when undertaking his work. 11 These
reveal, for instance, that when he was getting ready to write The Idiot he
worked on no less than eight plans for the first part of the novel, that he
speculated at length about possible ways in which the narrative might
develop, that he thought about the ways in which other authors had
resolved problems similar to the ones he was confronting, that his charac-
ters drew upon various models known to him, and that his themes and
narratives frequently made use of events he had experienced or read
about.
Musicians and painters are just as likely as writers to engage in substan-
tial preparatory work and to repeatedly revise their initial attempts. For
instance, Beethoven's Ninth Symphony drew heavily upon reworked ver-
sions of compositions that had been made during the preceding ten years
or so: Beethoven's notebooks demonstrate that large amounts of time and
energy were devoted to planning, preparing, developing and elaborating
the work. Even with Mozart, who is often said to have written out compo-
sitions from scratch, in their final form, the records firmly contradict this,
providing plenty of evidence of corrections and alterations, and aban-
doned false starts.
Just as scientists have exploited the advances made by others, writers,
artists and musicians have habitually borrowed themes and ideas from
9 Quoted in Perkins (r98r), p. ro. 10 Quoted in Weisberg (r993), p. 23r.
11 Weisberg (r993), p.230.
186 Genius Explained
17 Weisberg (r993).
9 Born to be a genius?
Everyone has heard it said of somebody or other that he (or she) was born
to be a genius. Can such an assertion ever be correct? A simple 'yes or no'
answer has to be negative, because sophisticated inborn capabilities
simply cannot exist. Outside mythology, nobody begins life having pro-
clivities that can guarantee the emergence of high abilities.
That does not necessarily mean that the idea of being born to be a
genius must be entirely false. People are not born identical, and some of
the ways in which they differ at birth can have consequences that affect
the course of their whole lives. One widely accepted view is that certain
individuals begin life possessing innate gifts or talents that predispose a
person towards exceptional attainments in a particular area of ability.
Another common belief is that a person's intelligence level, which has a
major role in determining the likelihood of substantial achievements, is
largely fixed at birth. This chapter examines some of the evidence that has
a bearing on the possible involvement of innately-determined influences
on variability, among the numerous contributing forces that combine to
enable certain individuals to become exceptionally capable.
All human individuals are affected in many ways by the particular com-
bination of genetic resources they inherit. That the influences of genetic
differences between people can extend to the manner in which lives are
experienced is easily verified. Just watch the contrasting ways in which
people at a party react to the entrance of a spectacularly beautiful individ-
ual and to a man or woman of ordinary appearance. Those differing
responses will certainly affect the individuals who elicit them. Indeed, the
manner in which others react to people can have an impact on many of
their experiences. One beautiful woman has her education enriched as a
consequence of influential people being drawn to her company; another
fails to make the most of her opportunities because of repeated experi-
ences of getting her wishes without having to make an effort. An ordinary-
looking man loses out because the teacher who might have been able to
help him prefers to spend time with other pupils. Another plain man
eventually thrives because his failure to gain attention fuels his determi-
188
Born to be a genius? 189
In the minds of many people it is a clear and simple fact, not to be ques-
tioned, that certain men and women have been born with innate talents
that make them capable of high attainments. I call that viewpoint 'the
talent account'. Someone who subscribes to it takes it for granted that
geniuses, in common with many others who achieve unusually high levels
of expertise, do so at least partly as an outcome of being born possessing a
special mental capacity of some kind.
The talent account is widely accepted and rarely challenged. It is not
hard to understand why. It agrees with our commonsense impressions. It
is consistent with numerous everyday observations. There is no denying
190 Genius Explained
that from an early age young people do differ in their patterns of ability.
One child does well at arithmetic but appears incapable of learning to
play a musical instrument. Another youngster is hopeless at both music
and arithmetic but has a flair for new languages. Even within the same
family there may be striking differences between siblings: one daughter
takes to the task of! earning to play the piano with apparent ease, while her
older sister struggles to master a few elementary pieces. In some cases
these differences between individuals appear to be present from very early
in life. Often it is evident that differences in ability cannot be explained in
terms of children's differing experiences of formal training. And some-
times children make contrasting amounts of progress even when they are
equally keen to do well.
In short, young men and women differ in their capabilities even when
there seems to be an absence of those causes of variability that arise from
differing opportunities to gain skills and knowledge. Not surprisingly, the
apparent lack of alternative explanations for such differences has led
many to conclude that there must exist inherent differences between indi-
viduals in their potential to excel, with some children but not others pos-
sessing innate gifts or talents. So the talent account continues to be
accepted, even in the absence of any positive evidence in support of it,
because it appears to provide an explanation of a kind for differences that
are otherwise inexplicable. Intuitively, it seems right.
But is the talent account actually correct? Its agreement with common-
sense beliefs, combined with the seeming lack of alternative reasons for
differences between people in their levels of expertise, makes it a plausible
theory. Yet there is a big gap between a plausible explanation and a proven
one, and in the case of the talent account there are compelling grounds
for questioning it. 1 One serious weakness is the fact that for most people
who accept the talent account their only justification for doing so is the
apparent lack of alternatives. An explanatory theory requires positive evi-
dence if it is to be convincing, and the fact that people have failed to
discern alternative causes of variations in progress hardly amounts to a
compelling case for the view that other causes do not exist. Ifl am anxious
to persuade others of the correctness of my personal explanation of par-
ticular events, my observation that I have not been able to come up with a
better explanation is unlikely to be perceived as reinforcing my claims.
In some cases alternative possible causes of variability become evident
just as soon as a serious effort is made to look for them. In the case of
musical skills, for instance, even when two children do not differ at all in
the amount of formal instruction they have been given, it takes little effort
1 A detailed examination of the evidence relating to the talent account is provided by
Howe, Davidson and Sloboda (r998).
Born to be a genius? 191
to realise that there may well have been large (if unnoticed) differences in
the extent to which they have enjoyed musical experiences and formed
preferences that would have affected musical development by sensitising
a child to certain patterns of sounds.
Does it greatly matter whether the talent account is true or false? It
matters immensely, not only because efforts to explain creative achieve-
ments can never succeed if they depend upon faulty assumptions about
the origins of a person's unusual capabilities, but also because important
practical issues are involved. The fact that the talent account is widely
believed in has consequences that affect the lives of numerous young
people. Within certain fields of expertise, such as music, unquestioning
acceptance of the talent account is almost invariably accompanied by the
belief that excellence is only attainable by those children who are innately
talented. A frequent result of teachers and other influential adults having
this combination of beliefs is that when scarce educational resources or
opportunities are being allocated they are likely to be directed exclusively
towards those young people who are thought to possess a special talent.
Young children who are believed to lack innate talents are denied
resources that are vital in order for a child to gain any chance of succeed-
ing.
If the talent account was shown to be correct, it might be argued that a
selection process that is based upon it makes sense, because it directs
limited resources towards those individuals who are most capable of
taking advantage of them. But if the talent account is wrong, and innate
talents are fictional rather than real, a policy of denying facilities to young
people because they are deemed not to possess such talents is clearly
wasteful and unjust. It could still be argued that those children who are
selected as being talented are the ones who are most likely to succeed
anyway, since their above-average early progress still may be a good pre-
dictor of eventual success even if the inference that such progress points
to an innate talent being present is wrong. It makes sense, in other words,
to have a selection policy that favours young people who have already
done well. Even so, a policy of totally denying learning facilities to any
child who (because he or she has not yet made unusual progress) is
thought to lack a vital innate talent can hardly be justified unless there are
convincing reasons for assuming that such talents do indeed exist.
There is no item of evidence that single-handedly confirms or refutes
the talent account, but various kinds of information have a bearing on the
issue. A number of findings have been seen as offering support. First, for
instance, there is some evidence that appears to show that skills appear
inexplicably early in a few children. Second, some other findings seem to
point to the possible existence of special inborn capacities in a small
192 Genius Explained
Even if the talent account is wrong, it is still possible that there are other
ways in which inborn differences between people may have predictable
influences on the likelihood of individuals becoming capable of major
creative attainments. Inherent differences in general intelligence could be
crucial here. That possibility seems to gain credence from the fact that it
is often assumed that a person's intelligence level is largely fixed at birth,
and from the widespread belief that intelligence has a major role in
making substantial achievements possible.
Both of these assumptions are questionable, however. The belief that a
person's intelligence is largely fixed has been forcefully promoted by
experts on intelligence testing, but there has never been convincing sup-
porting evidence for it. That is not to deny that there are some findings
that are consistent with the idea of a fixed intelligence. For instance, it has
been demonstrated that individuals' IQ (Intelligence Quotient) test
scores tend to be stable, as is evident from the observation that if someone
is tested on successive years, the person's two scores are usually similar.
However, the observation that in most cases a person's score does not
greatly change does not justify our inference that it cannot change. To
claim that the presence of stability proves change to be impossible is
rather like saying that the fact that most people live in the same house
from one year to the next and keep the same telephone number means
that these are unalterable. But it is easy to see that when there are good
reasons for altering either of them, change can and does occur. Similarly,
when there exist substantial reasons for intelligence levels changing, they
too do alter.
The idea that a person's intelligence is largely unchangeable is also
contradicted by evidence from each of a variety of different sources. 9 For
example, the case of some early intervention programmes that were intro-
duced under the 'Head Start' initiative in the United States. In 1969 the
psychologist Arthur Jensen made a widely-cited claim that these pro-
grammes had failed to achieve the intended outcomes. However, he
neglected to point out that many of those programmes had been meagrely
financed and had lasted no longer than two months on a part-time basis.
For a typical child participant the total duration of time involved would
often have been appreciably less than a hundred hours. That amount of
time may appear fairly substantial, but it is actually quite puny in compar-
ison with the periods of time needed to make major changes in young
people's acquired capabilities, even ones that are much narrower than
those assessed in intelligence tests. As we have already observed, around
3,000 hours of concentrated training and practice is required in order for
a higly motivated young person to reach the standard of performance at a
musical instrument such as the pianoforte that would be expected in a
good amateur player. Achieving professional standards of expertise
requires considerably longer, around 10,000 hours. And comparable
periods of time are necessary to achieve high levels of expertise in other
areas of attainment, such as chess, various sports, and foreign languages.
Moreover, in those kinds of capabilities at which children from varying
backgrounds are found to differ, it is usually found that there have been
massive differences between the children in the extent to which they have
enjoyed positive everyday experiences. Recall, for instance, the findings of
the study by Betty Hart and Todd Risley that examined the possible
reasons for the differences between three-year-olds from different social
classes in the size of their spoken vocabularies. 10 It was discovered that
even by that age those children who came from professional families had
already heard more than thirty million words directed towards them. In
contrast, the children from working-class families had only heard around
about twenty million words, and for the children from families on welfare
the comparable figure was around ten million words.
In short, behind the observed variability in language performance were
literally enormous variations in the children's actual experiences of lan-
guage. In the light of the evidence that differences of that kind of magni-
tude in children's experiences lie behind differences in their capabilities,
the investments oftime that have been devoted to early intervention pro-
grammes have been so tiny as to make it seem highly unlikely that they
would have large effects. In the circumstances it is remarkable that in
many cases the effects of such interventions have nevertheless been sub-
stantial. There is sufficient evidence of large alterations taking place in
11 Howe (r997).
200 Genius Explained
scores, equally accurate predictions could have been made about their
attainments in later life.
The belief that certain individuals can be said to have been born to be
geniuses is not one that is supported by firm evidence, and the innate gifts
or talents that are commonly believed to be possessed by a minority of
individuals who are thereby imbued with a capacity to excel in particular
areas of expertise are probably mythical rather than real. The idea that an
inherent quality of intelligence plays a role in determining an individual
becoming a genius appears to be equally groundless. And yet, as we have
seen, people's experiences are undoubtedly influenced by the particular
combinations of genes they inherit. If that is true, is it not inevitable that
someone's inherited genes strongly affect the likelihood of that person
becoming capable of exceptional achievements? And in that event, might
it not be possible at some stage in the future to 'read' the genetic informa-
tion that is present in the organs of a newborn baby, and make accurate
predictions about the child's future attainments? We cannot be certain,
but in my judgement the answer to both those questions is likely to be
negative, and I think that the reasons why many people believe otherwise
are rooted in misconceptions about the manner in which genetic
influences actually contribute to human variability.
When pondering about genetic inheritance, which involves a number
of complex issues that are not fully understood, we naturally lean upon
metaphors that we hope will help link the abstract complexities of genetic
science to the more familiar territory of our own existing knowledge. But
although metaphors can indeed be helpful, at least up to a point, they can
also mislead, and some of those that people have introduced in relation to
genetic causation have been definitely misleading. Genes are often
regarded as providing the function of 'blueprints', or 'instructions', or
'recipes' which do their work by telling human cells how to construct
individual human beings. Unfortunately, although none of these charac-
terisations of the functioning of genetic information is totally incorrect,
each considerably oversimplifies the true state of affairs. That is not the
way it works.
What is particularly deceptive about the above metaphors is their
implication that genetic materials invariably exert their influence by
initiating a fixed causal chain and, in consequence, largely determining
various outcomes. The reality is more complicated. Genetic resources
certainly make vital contributions, but they can do so at various stages in
an organism's development. The choices that an organism makes con-
cerning if, how, and when there will be a contributing input of a particular
item of genetic material, and concerning precisely what the form of that
Born to be a genius? 201
input will be, depend upon numerous other factors that affect the organ-
ism's development, including various environmental inputs that are
mediated by the organism's experiences. In humans, to complicate things
even more, the manner in which genes contribute to psychological func-
tioning is considerably less direct than is implied by any of the above
metaphors. In reality, rather than thinking of a gene exerting some
influence that directly affects a complex psychological characteristic, it
would be more accurate to imagine the relatively immediate effect of
genetic inputs being to affect, say, the production of some or other
hormone. Genes are sequences of DNA base pairs, and their direct effects
take the form of contributions being made to the structure of proteins.
That is achieved by affecting the structure of amino-acid sequences.
Depending on the particular circumstances, those relatively immediate
effects may help trigger off other influences, whose effects may influence
occurrences at successively more distant future stages. Eventually, follow-
ing what may be a lengthy chain of physical activities, there may be conse-
quences that take the form of psychological acts or events. Note, however,
that because at each of a number of subsequent stages the actual
influence of a particular item of genetic information will also depend on
various other factors, the long-term influence of a particular item of
genetic information will be largely unpredictable. So even when it is indis-
putable that genetically based influences upon human variability have
been among the contributing influences determining the rate and direc-
tion of an individual person's progress, it would be wrong to conclude
that any traits or activities that have been affected by those genetic inputs
have been straightforwardly determined by them.
Hence the simplistic notions of genetic causation that come to mind
whenever one sees newspaper accounts of the search for 'the gene for
intelligence, or 'the' gene for any other complex psychological attribute,
are, to say the least, misleading. They reflect a model of genetic causation
that is far too simple to represent what actually takes place, and is not
based upon anything approaching a realistic understanding of how
genetic differences between individuals actually contribute to people's
differing psychological lives. There are no direct one-to-one relationships
between genes and psychological characteristics, and the popular idea
that there are genes 'for' complex traits is simply wrong.
And yet, however misleading they may be, statements about 'the' gene
for this or that will continue to be made, partly for the simple reason that
they are easy to understand. This misuse is to some extent encouraged by
occasional newspaper reports of investigations in which certain gene
abnormalities are found to have predictable effects. Yet it needs to be reg-
istered that these reports almost invariably refer to circumstances in
202 Genius Explained
14 Gould (r984).
204 Genius Explained
would normally dictate that steps would be taken to ensure that some
contact between the twins could be maintained. In the majority of cases
they would be brought up within the same family. Just imagine the kinds
of circumstances that were sufficiently grave and desperate to make it
unavoidable that identical twins were not only separated at the time of
birth but were also brought up in circumstances that permitted no
contact between the twins to be maintained. It is almost inconceivable
that in circumstances as dire as those it would nevertheless be possible to
undertake all the activities that would be necessary in order to keep accu-
rate records of what had transpired. And yet in order for a properly con-
trolled research study to be undertaken it would be essential for accurate
and detailed records to be available.
These difficulties combine to make it extremely difficult to locate prop-
erly documented cases of identical twins who have been separated at birth
in numbers that are sufficient to conduct properly designed research
investigations. Nevertheless, some studies of identical twins reared
together and apart have been reported, and evidence has been collected
from around a hundred such individuals. On close examination, however,
it transpires that these reports are not actually comparisons between
twins reared together and ones separated at birth. In fact they are com-
parisons between twins who have been reared entirely together and ones
reared partly apart, and the latter category includes pairs of twins who
have spent as much as four years together, typically the earliest and most
crucial formative years. Also, the reports lack detailed information on
crucial variables such as the exact dates at which the twins were first sep-
arated. In view of the rare and difficult circumstances surrounding the
early separation of identical twins these defects may be inevitable. But
they are crucial defects all the same, and because of them it may never be
possible to arrive at firm answers to questions about the inheritance of
intelligence on the basis of the findings of twin studies.
It is currently becoming less necessary to depend on twin studies in
order to acquire information about the possible effects on mature capa-
bilities of differences between people in their genetic materials. Direct
mapping of genetic information will make it possible for a fuller and more
detailed picture to emerge, and it may well prove possible to identify
certain genetic features whose presence adds to the likelihood of their
possessor acquiring impressive mental capabilities. But it will never be
possible to identify individuals who, by virtue of their genes, are born to
be geniuses. One-to-one relationships between genetic differences and
ability differences are ruled out by the complexity and indirectness of
genetic contributions to human activities.
It tends to be assumed that if there do exist qualities that make some
Born to be a genius? 205
people more capable than others and have an inherited component, those
qualities are ones that are closely related to a person's cognitive attributes
such as cleverness or creativity. However, it is just as likely that those -
conceivably largely inherited- human qualities that make the largest con-
tributions towards setting geniuses apart from other people are ones of
temperament and personality rather than being narrowly intellectual
ones. The finding that assessments of a child's capacity to resist distrac-
tions and avoid impulsive actions are better predictors of later success
than measures of early intelligence is consistent with that view. As we have
seen, the particular qualities that contemporaries most frequently
remarked upon in geniuses such as Newton and Mozart were broadly
temperamental. Doggedness, persistence, the capacity for fierce and sus-
tained concentration, as well as intense curiosity, are the attributes that
others have noticed, and geniuses themselves have concurred with that
emphasis. A number of geniuses, including Darwin and Einstein, have
disclaimed having superior inherent intelligence, but no genius has ever
denied either possessing or relying upon a capacity for diligence or a
healthy curiosity.
One of the reasons for people being reluctant to let go of the idea that gen-
iuses are a race apart, distinct from everyone else by virtue of their inher-
ent qualities as well as their marvellous accomplishments, is the fear that
geniuses will be diminished if we remove the magic and mystery sur-
rounding them. I do not share that view. On the contrary, it is not until we
understand that they are made from the same flesh and bones as the rest
of us that we start to appreciate just how wonderfully remarkable these
men and women really are. They show us what humankind is capable of.
And it is only when we acknowledge that geniuses are not totally unlike
other people that our minds open up to all that we can learn from them.
Appendix: Personalia
206
Appendix: Personalia 207
Whitney, Eli (1765-1825) American inventor, best known for his cotton
'gin' which greatly improved the efficiency of the cotton industry in the
United States.
Wiener, Norbert (1894-1964) American mathematician, and an out-
standing prodigy in childhood. He contributed towards the early devel-
opment of computers.
Wright, Wilbur (1867-1912) and Orville (1871-1948) The American
pioneers of powered flight.
References
212
References 213
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993) The role of deliberate
practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100,
363-406.
POising, A. (1997) Albert Einstein, translated by Ewald Osers. New York: Viking.
Fowler, W (1981) Case studies of cognitive precocity: the role of exogenous and
endogenous stimulation in early mental development. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 2, 319-67.
Gardner, H. (1997) Extraordinary minds: portraits of exceptional individuals and an
examination of our extraordinariness. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Glendinning, V. (1992) Trollope. London: Hutchinson.
Gould, S. J. (1984) The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton.
Gruber, H. E. (1981) Darwin on man: a psychological study of scientific creativity,
second edition. University of Chicago Press.
Hamilton, J. (1997) Turner: a life. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995) Meaningful differences in everyday parenting and intel-
lectual development in young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Hayes, J. R. (1981) The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: The Franklin
Institute Press.
Hibbert, C. (1983) The making of Charles Dickens. Harmondsworth: Penguin
Books.
Howe, M. J. A. (1990) The origins of exceptional abilities. Oxford: Blackwell.
Howe, M. J. A. (1997) IQ in question: the truth about intelligence. London: Sage.
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W, & Sloboda, J. A. (1998) Innate talents: reality or
myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21,399-442.
Howe, M. J. A., & Godfrey, J. (1977) Student note-taking as an aid to learning.
Exeter University Teaching Services.
Jones, B. (1870) The life and letters of Faraday, UJlume I. London: Longmans,
Green,&Co.
Kagan, J. (1989) Temperamental contributions to social behaviour. American
Psychologist, 44, 668-74.
Karl, F. (1995) George Eliot: a biography. London: HarperCollins.
Knight, D. M. (1985) Davy and Faraday: fathers and sons, pp. 32-49 in D.
Gooding and F. A. J. L. James (Eds.), Faraday Rediscovered: Essays on the Life
and Wbrk of Michael Faraday, 179I-r867. New York: Stockton Press.
Lehmann, A. C. (1997) The acquisition of expertise in music, in Deliege, 1., and
Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.), Perception and Cognition of Music. Hillsdale, NJ:
Elbaum.
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1998) The historical development of domains
of expertise: performance standards and innovations in music. In A. Steptoe
(Ed.), Genius and the mind: studies of creativity and temperament in the historical
record. Oxford University Press.
Lewontin, R. (1982) Human diversity. New York: Freeman.
Mazlish, B. (1975) James and John Stuart Mill. London: Hutchinson.
Mill, J. S. (1971) Autobiography, (edited by Jack Stillinger). Oxford University
Press. [Originally published in 1873.]
Morris, P. E., Gruneberg, M. M., Sykes, R.N., & Merrick, A. (1981) Football
knowledge and the acquisition of new results. British Journal of Psychology,
76,415-25.
214 References
216
Index 217
Lewontin, R., 202 IS, I7, 23, 29, s6, 6I, 82, 109, I37, I4o,
Life of George Stephenson, 62-3 I48, I 53, I 57, I85, 205, 209; early
Life ofJesus, I7I precocity, 4-6
Lilienthal, Otto, I83-4 Murray, P., I
Lincoln, Abraham, 20 music, 5, 6-7, I8, I94-5
Linnaeus, Carolus, 44 musical training, 4-6, I94-5
Liszt, Franz, 20, I37
Liverpool and Manchester Railway, So-I, Natural History and Antiquities of Selborne,
I07 35,48
Locke, Joseph, 20 natural selection, 23-24, 56-7 see also
Longfellow, Henry, 20 evolution; theory of evolution
Lunar Society, 30 Newcomen, Thomas, 7I, I76, I78, 209
Lyell, Charles, 20, 53, 58, I70, 208 Newsome, D., 24
Lysenko, Trofim, 4 7 Newton, Isaac, I3, I4-I5, 32, 44, 55, 65-6,
74, 88, 97, IOO, I40, I45, I78, I8I, I86,
Ma, Yo-Yo, I09 205, 209; doggedness of IS, I67, I86
Macaulay, Thomas, 20, I34 Nobel prizewinners, I02-3
Mackintosh,James,38 Norris, C., I
Mal thus, Thomas, I78, 208 Nyiregyh:izi, Erwin, I47-8
Manet, Edouard, I3, 20
Martineau, Harriet, 20 Ohm, Georg Simon, 2I
Marx, Karl, 20 Origin of Species, On the, I 6
Matthew, Patrick, 24 Owen, Fanny, 38
Maxwell, James Clerk, 20, I46, 208 Owen, William Mostyn, 38
Mazlish, B., I27
Melville, Herman, 20 Paganini, Niccolo, 3, 209
memory skills, 6-7, 34; chess, 7; musical, 3, parental encouragement, 108-29, I35,
6-7 I37-43
Mendel, Gregor, I4, 26, 206 Pascal, Blaise, I34, I69, 209
Mendelssohn, Felix, 20, I37 Pauling, Lionel, I82
mental calculations, 46, I3I-3, I 54 Pease, Edward, 8I
mental disciplines, 33-4 Pensees (Pascal), I 69
mental struggle, I86-7 perfect pitch, I93
mentors, 38,47-8, so-I, IOO-I Perkins, D. N., I85
Menuhin, Yehudi, 109, I49, I 53, 209 persistance and perseverance, IS, 205; see
Merrick, A., 6 also doggedness
Michelangelo, I3 Picasso, Pablo, 56
Middlemarch, I67, I70 Pinker, S., 202
Mill, James, 100, I08-9, II9-27, I34, I49, pitch perception, I93
209 Plato, I3
Mill, John Stuart, 20, 2I, IOI, I08-9, Plinian Society, 46, 48
II9-29,I34,I53,I58,I67,I72,209; Plomin, R., ISI
appraisal ofJames Mill's teaching, I25-7; Poe, Edgar Allan, I3, 20
early education, I23-6; personality and Polgar sisters, 108
personal difficulties, I26-9 precocity, 3-7, 9, I92 see also prodigies
Millais, John Everett, 20 Priestley, Joseph, 30
Miserere (Allegri), 3, 7 Principia Mathematica, I 86
model building, 65-6 Principles of Geology, 57
Monet, Claude, I3 practising, 4-5, 64-5, 83, I 58, I95, I98
Moore, D. G., 4, I95 prodigies, 3-7, 29, 108-29, I30-55;
Moore, J., 24 advantages, I48; links with genius, I36,
Morris, P. E., 6 I4 7-55; misconceptions, I34-5; musical,
Morris, William, 20 3-7, I37; parents' involvement in
Mozart, Leopold, 3, 5-7, I49, 209 education, I34-6, I49; problems
Mozart, Nannerl, 5, I09 experienced, I48; problems of definition,
Mozart, Wolfgang Amadeus, I, 2-7, 9, I3, I36-7, I47
220 Index