Social Anxiety and The School Environment of Adolescents
Social Anxiety and The School Environment of Adolescents
Social Anxiety and The School Environment of Adolescents
Environment of Adolescents 7
Anke W. Blöte, Anne C. Miers, David A. Heyne,
and P. Michiel Westenberg
group of adolescents are very anxious about social situations and potential negative
evaluation from others (Gren-Landell et al. 2009). In these cases, a high level of
anxiety for social situations may interfere with their daily functioning. Giving an
oral presentation, answering questions in class, making friends, and participating in
cliques or gangs are all potentially stressful for them. In these situations, they may
be ridiculed, excluded, or even victimized. The socially anxious student may stop
socializing with classmates and friends, stop attending certain classes, or even
refuse to attend school altogether. This can have severe and detrimental conse-
quences for their cognitive and social development as well as their academic
achievements (Van Ameringen et al. 2003).
This chapter begins with a description of the way social anxiety manifests itself
in the school setting. More specifically, we review situations which are distressful
for and even avoided by socially anxious students. An extreme consequence of this
distress and avoidance, school refusal, is also discussed. In the second part of the
chapter, we describe the way in which socially anxious students’ behavior, and the
behavior of their peers or classmates, may contribute to the difficulty that socially
anxious youth experience during social interactions at school. In addition to these
behavioral and interpersonal factors, we pay attention to the negative social cogni-
tions of socially anxious students. We discuss whether, and to what extent, these
negative cognitions are biased or contain a kernel of truth (Norton and Hope 2001).
Finally, we present some considerations for intervention and future research.
What do we know about how adolescents with high levels of social anxiety experi-
ence social situations in school settings? Two studies focused on how socially anx-
ious and non-anxious adolescents differed in their fear and avoidance of
school-related social situations (Gren-Landell et al. 2009; Ranta et al. 2012). Two
other studies investigated fear and avoidance of social situations in adolescents with
clinical social anxiety levels only (Beidel et al. 2007; Rao et al. 2007), and three
investigated adolescents from the general population (Essau et al. 1999; Ranta et al.
2007; Wittchen et al. 1999). The following overview begins with the two studies
conducted with clinical samples.
In a sample of 150 children and adolescents with clinical social anxiety levels,
Rao et al. (2007) used the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children
(ADIS-C; Silverman and Albano 1996) to investigate distress and avoidance related
to 20 different social situations. More than 80 % of adolescents (13–17 years) diag-
nosed with Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) reported moderate-to-severe distress in
the school situations focused on “giving oral reports/presentations” and “asking
teacher a question.” These two situations were also frequently avoided. Respectively,
71.1 and 73.7 % of adolescents reported at least moderate avoidance. Fewer adoles-
cents reported at least moderate distress or avoidance in other school situations
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 153
focused on “taking tests” and “participating in gym class.” Across all situations
adolescents reported significantly greater fear and more avoidance than did chil-
dren. Specifically, a significantly higher percentage of adolescents than children
reported moderate-to-severe distress and at least moderate avoidance in the school
situations “asking teacher a question” and “writing on chalkboard.” Children and
adolescents did not differ in distress or avoidance for the situations “giving oral
reports/presentations” or “taking tests” (Rao et al. 2007). In the Beidel et al. (2007)
study, which also used the ADIS-C in a study of 63 adolescents (aged 13–16 years)
with social anxiety disorder, very similar results were reported. The school situa-
tions “oral reports or reading aloud” and “asking the teacher a question or asking for
help” were again two of the most distressing social situations reported by adoles-
cents. The percentages of adolescents endorsing at least moderate distress in those
two school situations were 90.5 and 87.3 %, respectively. Furthermore, these situa-
tions were also frequently avoided. Respectively, 65.1 and 69.8 % reported at least
moderate avoidance (Beidel et al. 2007). In all, these two studies conducted with
clinical populations show a very high prevalence of fear and avoidance of school-
related social situations.
Other studies have investigated the frequency of social fears and/or avoidance in
community samples of adolescents. Essau et al. (1999) investigated the frequency
of social fears in a large community sample of German adolescents aged 12–17 years.
The most commonly feared social situations were performance/test situations and
public speaking, reportedly feared by 31.1 and 19.7 % of the sample, respectively.
In a second, large sample of 14–24-year-olds, Wittchen et al. (1999) reported a
slightly lower prevalence, with 18.2 and 13.2 % of the sample fearing performance/
test situations and public speaking, respectively. Ranta et al. (2007) administered
the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN; Connor et al. 2000) in a large Finnish popula-
tion sample of adolescents aged between 12 and 16 years. Just over 12 % of the
sample reported extreme avoidance of speeches (Ranta et al. 2007). The situations
investigated in these three studies are not specific to the school setting, and the
findings are not related to adolescents’ general level of social anxiety. Hence, these
studies do not specifically inform about the impact of school-related social situa-
tions on socially anxious adolescents.
Gren-Landell et al. (2009) study of a large sample of Swedish adolescents
(12–14 years) attending secondary school investigated the fear of specific social
situations, including school situations, using a self-report measure. The school
situations included in the questionnaire were “speaking in front of the class,”
“raising your hand during a lesson,” “being together with others during breaks,”
and “eating together with others during lunch break.” In the total sample, “speaking
in front of the class” was the most feared situation, with 6.2 % endorsing marked
fear of this situation. Of the students who fulfilled DSM-IV criteria for SAD
(n = 93), 63.4 % reported marked fear in the situation “speaking in front of the
class”; this was the most common fear in this group. In contrast, only 3.6 % of the
students who did not fulfill SAD criteria (n = 2,035) reported marked fear in this
school situation. For the other three school situations, up to 15 % of adolescents in
the SAD group reported marked fear in contrast with 2.3 % of the non-SAD group
154 A.W. Blöte et al.
(Gren-Landell et al. 2009). In a second study, Ranta et al. (2012) compared a group
of adolescents (12–17 years) diagnosed with SAD (n = 22) with a group without
SAD (n = 299). Based on reports on the SPIN, 64 % of the SAD group reported
avoiding speeches at least “very much” versus 31 % of the non-SAD group.
Although the latter study did not focus on school situations specifically, the results
are consistent with those of Gren-Landell et al. (2009), inasmuch as more than
60 % of clinically socially anxious adolescents reported moderate fear or avoidance
of speech situations.
The impact of school-related social situations on adolescents with high levels of
social anxiety is well illustrated by findings from the Social Anxiety and Normal
Development (SAND) study (Westenberg et al. 2009). This study made use of a
sample of adolescents with a wide range of social anxiety levels. The adolescents
completed a questionnaire that asked how distressed they felt in 20 different social
situations and how frequently they tried to avoid these situations (Sumter et al.
2009). The questionnaire was based on the social phobia module from the ADIS-C
(Silverman and Albano 1996). The questionnaire includes situations that are spe-
cific to the school context such as answering questions in class, giving a speech in
class, and asking the teacher a question or asking for help. The distress and avoid-
ance questions are answered on a 9-point scale. For the present chapter, we com-
pared the socially anxious and non-anxious adolescents, as described below.
A group of high and low socially anxious adolescents was selected (12–18 years),
using scores on the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca and
Lopez 1998). Forty high socially anxious adolescents were selected based on their
scoring in the top 20 % of their gender-specific distribution. The high socially anx-
ious group had a mean SAS-A score of 56.73, which is above the clinical cutoff
recommended by La Greca (1998). Subsequently, an equal number of adolescents
were selected with low (but not the lowest) social anxiety scores, SAS-A scores
between 10 and 32 % according to the gender-specific distribution. The percentage
of adolescents in each social anxiety group (high and low) endorsing a low, moder-
ate, and high degree of distress and avoidance is presented in Table 7.1. The table
presents distress and avoidance results for all 20 social situations included in the
questionnaire, but for the purposes of this chapter, we now focus our discussion on
the social situations that are relevant to the school context.
With regard to experienced distress, significant differences between high and low
socially anxious adolescents can be seen, particularly in the following school-
related social situations: “answering questions in class,” “giving a speech in class,”
“reading aloud in class,” “writing on the board,” and “taking tests.” For the majority
of these five school situations, none of those in the low socially anxious group
endorsed high distress, compared to between 6 and 25 % of those in the high socially
anxious group. As well as experiencing more distress in school-related social situa-
tions, high socially anxious adolescents were more likely to try to avoid these situ-
ations relative to low socially anxious adolescents. For the classroom situations
“answering questions,” “giving a speech,” “reading aloud,” and “writing on the
board,” between 20 and 27 % of high socially anxious adolescents indicated high
avoidance, compared to 7 % or fewer of low socially anxious adolescents.
7
Table 7.1 Distress and avoidance per ADIS situation in high (HSA) and low (LSA) socially anxious participants aged 12 years and older
Distress % Avoidance %
Mann- Mann-
ADIS situation Low (0–2) Moderate (3–5) High (6–8) Whitney U Low (0–2) Moderate (3–5) High (6–8) Whitney U
LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA
1. Answering questions in 95.5 72.7 2.3 20.5 2.3 6.8 751.0* 81.8 52.3 15.9 27.3 2.3 20.5 656.5*
class
2. Giving a speech in class 79.5 47.7 20.5 27.3 0 25.0 610.5** 84.1 54.5 9.1 18.2 6.8 27.3 670.0*
3. Reading aloud in class 95.5 59.1 4.5 29.5 0 11.4 611.0** 86.4 63.6 11.4 11.4 2.3 25.0 723.0*
4. Asking the teacher a 95.5 77.3 4.5 18.2 0 4.5 790.0 86.4 70.5 11.4 13.6 2.3 15.9 799.5
question or for help
5. Taking tests 88.6 65.9 11.4 18.2 0 15.9 730.5* 90.9 84.1 6.8 4.5 2.3 11.4 895.5
6. Writing on the board 100 70.5 0 15.9 0 13.6 682.0** 90.9 63.6 6.8 13.6 2.3 22.7 692.0*
7. Working/playing with a 100 86.4 0 13.6 0 0 836.0 95.5 81.8 4.5 15.9 0 2.3 835.0
group of kids
8. Gym class 95.5 79.5 2.3 15.9 2.3 4.5 816.5 88.6 86.4 6.8 9.1 4.5 4.5 947.0
9. Walking in the hallways 97.7 77.3 2.3 20.5 0 2.3 769.5* 100 86.4 0 9.1 0 4.5 836.0
10. Starting or joining in on 97.7 86.4 2.3 9.1 0 4.5 857.0 97.7 77.3 2.3 13.6 0 9.1 768.0*
a conversation
Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents
11. Using school or public 90.9 79.5 9.1 13.6 0 6.8 852.0 75.0 61.4 11.4 20.5 13.6 18.2 843.0
bathrooms
12. Eating in front of others 95.5 70.5 4.5 22.7 0 6.8 723.0* 97.7 84.1 2.3 6.8 0 9.1 834.0
13. Meetings such as scouts 97.7 84.1 0 11.4 2.3 4.5 838.5 93.2 81.8 6.8 13.6 0 4.5 855.0
or team
14. Answering or talking on 93.2 77.3 6.8 15.9 0 6.8 809.5 86.4 63.6 13.6 25.0 0 11.4 733.0*
the telephone
155
Table 7.1 (continued)
156
Distress % Avoidance %
Mann- Mann-
ADIS situation Low (0–2) Moderate (3–5) High (6–8) Whitney U Low (0–2) Moderate (3–5) High (6–8) Whitney U
LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA LSA HSA
15. Musical or athletic 90.9 59.1 9.1 20.5 0 20.5 642.0** 90.9 68.2 6.8 15.9 2.3 15.9 741.0*
performances
16. Inviting a friend to get 95.5 75.0 4.5 18.2 0 6.8 767.0* 93.2 77.3 6.8 15.9 0 6.8 809.5
together
17. Speaking to an adult 97.7 79.5 2.3 13.6 0 6.8 790.5* 97.7 72.7 2.3 20.5 0 6.8 724.5*
18. Talking to persons you 100 47.7 0 31.8 0 20.5 462.0** 97.7 54.5 2.3 18.2 0 27.3 544.0**
don’t know well
19. Attending parties 93.2 79.5 4.5 11.4 2.3 9.1 834.5 90.9 84.1 6.8 9.1 2.3 6.8 899.5
20. Having picture taken 97.7 75.0 2.3 20.5 0 4.5 747.0* 95.5 77.3 2.3 15.9 2.3 6.8 794.0
**p < .001; *p < .01; (1-tailed). P values in bold < .0025 (Bonferroni correction). HSA = 44 (23 boys, 21 girls), LSA = 44 (20 boys, 24 girls). HSA-LSA
differences tested with Mann-Whitney U
A.W. Blöte et al.
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 157
School Refusal
Bools et al. 1990). Truancy, on the other hand, is said to occur when a young person
is absent from school and the parents do not know about the child’s absence
(Kearney 2002) or whereabouts (Berg et al. 1985). Moreover, truancy is often asso-
ciated with severe antisocial behaviors (Vaughn et al. 2013), and it is not commonly
associated with anxiety (Heyne et al. in press). Following, we review literature indi-
cating the extent to which the serious problem of school refusal may be associated
with social factors, especially social anxiety. We also review the negative impact
that social factors seem to have on the short- and longer-term well-being of school-
refusing youth.
In an early study of 29 youth (M age = 14.9 years) diagnosed with DSM-III-R
social phobia, Strauss and Last (1993) found that the most common fear exhibited
was “a fear of school” (p. 146). Indeed, almost two-thirds (64 %) of the socially
phobic youth exhibited a fear of school. It is worth bearing in mind, however, that a
fear of school is not synonymous with the presence of school refusal as defined by
Berg and colleagues (Berg 1997, 2002; Berg et al. 1969; Bools et al. 1990). We turn
now to studies which were based on samples of school refusers selected according
to the comprehensive criteria of Berg and colleagues.
Place et al. (2002) interviewed the families of 17 school refusers (aged
12–15 years). These youth had difficulty with peer relationships, a sense of isolation
(most did not belong to a friendship group when they started secondary school), and
were bullied and teased at school. It is likely that social anxiety contributed to or
stemmed from the social difficulties experienced by these school-refusing youth.
Heyne et al. (1998) assessed self-efficacy among 135 children and adolescents
(5–15 years; M age = 11.4 years) referred for school refusal. Across the sample,
youth’s perception of their ability to cope was lowest for the social-related situation
of answering peers’ questions about absences from school and highest for the non-
social situation of doing schoolwork. The low level of self-efficacy for the social-
related situation may be associated with social anxiety, in line with Rudy et al.
(2014) finding that social self-efficacy uniquely contributed to levels of social anxi-
ety among youth. In Buitelaar et al. (1994) follow-up study of 25 school-refusing
adolescents (M age at referral = 14.8 years), the majority were found to have “unsat-
isfactory or insufficient social relationships” prior to referral (p. 251). Further, one
of the most common diagnoses at initial contact was avoidant disorder of childhood
or adolescence (AD), which overlaps extensively with SAD (APA 1994). In another
study of school-refusing adolescents (11–17 years; M = 14.6 years), SAD was a pri-
mary or secondary diagnosis among approximately two-thirds of the sample (65 %;
Heyne et al. 2011).
Two additional studies reportedly investigated school refusal even though Berg
and colleagues’ school refusal criteria were not used to recruit subjects. Bernstein
et al. (2001) studied 41 adolescents (M age = 15.8 years) who attended school less
than 80 % of the time and were diagnosed with both an anxiety disorder and a
depressive disorder. About two-thirds (67 %) of these adolescents had a diagnosis
of SAD. In the Beidas et al. (2010) study of children and adolescents (7–16 years;
M = 11.0 years) with “denial to attend school or difficulty remaining in school,” it
was found that more youth presented with a principal diagnosis of SAD (n = 11)
160 A.W. Blöte et al.
1
The authors used the term “school phobia,” and adolescents were deemed to have school phobia
if their score on the school phobia subscale of the self-report Screen for Child Anxiety Related
Emotional Disorders (SCARED) was ≥3.
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 161
(bi)directionality of the relationship between social anxiety and school refusal and
to determine which factors moderate and mediate the relationship.
The relationship between social anxiety and school refusal appears to be age
related. Kearney and Albano (2004) examined the function of youths’ school
refusal, drawing on questionnaire responses from 143 youth (5–17 years; M = 11.6)
and their parents. Youth who refused school to escape from aversive social and/or
evaluative situations were typically older, whereas younger school refusers were
more likely to endorse other reasons for refusing to attend. More specifically, the
mean age of youth seeking to escape aversive social and/or evaluative situations was
14.4 years (SD = 1.8), whereas youth seeking to avoid stimuli that provoke negative
affectivity were 11.8 years on average (SD = 2.7), youth refusing school for atten-
tion were 9.2 years on average (SD = 2.8), and youth pursuing tangible reinforce-
ment outside of school were 12.9 years on average (SD = 2.8). Although this study
did not measure social anxiety per se, the older youth’s motivation to escape aver-
sive social and/or evaluative situations is clearly related to social anxiety. Last and
Strauss (1990) reported that, at intake, school refusers with SAD were older than
school refusers with separation anxiety disorder.
The higher prevalence of social anxiety among older school refusers, as reported
by Last and Strauss (1990), may simply be a reflection of the higher prevalence of
SAD among older youth relative to younger youth (e.g., Costello et al. 2003). It
might also be explained by the increasingly complex and demanding nature of the
secondary school environment relative to the primary school environment. Galloway
(1985) argued that, as the child grows older, school attendance problems are increas-
ingly under the influence of school-based factors. At secondary school the student
is confronted with a larger and more complex social environment involving multiple
teachers, moving between classes, and needing to function more autonomously
(Steinberg 2005, cited in Holmbeck et al. 2012). These aspects of secondary school-
ing, combined with the increasing importance of the peer context during adoles-
cence, may lead some vulnerable youth to become overwhelmed and to escape to
the security of the home environment.
There is accumulating evidence that social anxiety is associated with poor
response to treatment for school refusal. In Bernstein and colleagues’ (2001) 1-year
follow-up of school-refusing adolescents treated with cognitive-behavioral therapy
(CBT) plus imipramine or CBT plus placebo, the retention rate of disorders was
higher for SAD (50 % retention) and AD (50 % retention) relative to other disorders.
In further analysis of data from the original cohort, Layne et al. (2003) found that the
presence of AD was a significant predictor of poorer school attendance at posttreat-
ment.2 There was no treatment group by diagnosis (AD/no AD) interaction effect,
suggesting that the additional use of pharmacotherapy does not change the impact of
social avoidance on the outcome of CBT for school refusal. Heyne et al. (2011)
reported that school attendance 2 months following treatment was lower for school-
refusing adolescents who still met criteria for SAD (18 % of school-time attended)
relative to those who had no disorder or a disorder other than SAD following
2
Social anxiety disorder was not analyzed as a predictor of treatment outcome.
162 A.W. Blöte et al.
Socially anxious adolescents may encounter all sorts of negative outcomes from
their interactions with classmates. The range of negative behaviors that can be expe-
rienced in the classroom varies from neglect, and lack of acceptance and support at
one end to rejection and direct teasing and physical aggression at the other (see
Kingery et al. 2010 for a review). The relation between social anxiety and these
forms of victimization appears to be bidirectional (for a review, please see Garcia-
Lopez et al. 2011). A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies showed that internaliz-
ing problems, including withdrawal and anxiety, predicted an increase in peer
victimization and peer victimization, in turn, predicted an increase in internalizing
problems (Reijntjes et al. 2010). Socially anxious students seem to be trapped in a
vicious cycle of social anxiety and victimization (Ollendick and Hirshfeld-Becker
2002; Siegel et al. 2009). This conclusion is based on studies using self-report
measures (e.g., La Greca and Harrison 2005; Ranta et al. 2009; Storch and
Masia-Warner 2004) as well as ratings from classmates in sociometric studies
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 163
(e.g., Erath et al. 2007; Inderbitzen et al. 1997). Furthermore, observations in the
school and classroom have substantiated the negative treatment of socially anxious
students (Blöte et al. 2007, 2010; Spence et al. 1999).
Some features of the social environment may to some extent protect socially
anxious youth against the harmful effects of negative peer treatment. For example,
the number and quality of friendships, being a member of a “crowd” (be it a high-
status or low-status crowd), and being liked by at least some peers have all been
found to predict lower levels of social anxiety (La Greca and Harrison 2005; London
et al. 2007) over time. However, there is no evidence that friendship and crowd
membership variables play a mediating or moderating role in the link between vic-
timization and social anxiety (La Greca and Harrison 2005). These variables pre-
dicted lower levels of social anxiety independent of the degree of victimization.
In the following section, we explore why socially anxious adolescents are treated
in a negative way by classmates. We firstly address factors in socially anxious stu-
dents themselves and then factors in the peer group.
perhaps because their anxiety is expressed in their body posture, facial expression,
voice, way of verbally communicating, and physical appearance? We also consider
how much time it takes before these students are perceived as socially anxious and
are treated in a negative way. We therefore discuss studies related to (a) the first
impression that socially anxious adolescents make on peers who are not acquainted
with them; (b) research on emerging social relations in new peer groups; and (c)
socially anxious adolescents’ ongoing relations in groups with familiar peers.
First Impressions
Recently, a few studies investigated the impression that socially anxious adolescents
make on unfamiliar peers. A study by Miers et al. (2010) addressed peer perceptions
of the social performance of socially anxious youth. Participants in this study were
adolescents aged between 13 and 17 years, divided into a high and a low socially
anxious group based on their anxiety scores on the SAS-A (La Greca and Lopez
1998). These participants gave a speech in front of a prerecorded audience. Video-
recorded 2-min fragments of the speeches were observed by whole school classes of
unfamiliar peers who individually rated the fragments. Peer observers rated speaker
behavior on speech content, facial expressions, posture/body movement, and way of
speaking (voice, language). Results showed that high socially anxious speakers
were judged as performing more poorly on all four behavioral aspects. After con-
trolling for self-reported depressive symptoms, the differences between the high-
and low-anxiety groups were still significant, except for facial expression. So, peer
responses to the speakers’ behavior could mainly be ascribed to speakers’ social
anxiety and not their depressed mood. Furthermore, the four aspects of perceived
speaker behavior were strongly related, and the authors concluded that because no
particular behavior made a unique contribution to predicting which group speakers
belonged to (i.e., high or low anxious), there may be a general lack of social skills
in socially anxious speakers. An alternative conclusion reached by the authors was
that socially anxious speakers exhibit certain behavior that elicits a negative halo
effect (a general impression determining the judgment of individual qualities of a
person, Thorndike 1920) in other people’s judgments.
Another study further supports the proposition that socially anxious adolescents
make a general negative impression on unfamiliar peers, and it also presents evi-
dence regarding mediators in the social anxiety – rejection link. Blöte et al. (2015)
divided adolescents (13–17 years) into a high socially anxious group and a low
socially anxious group. The video-recorded speeches of these adolescents were
observed by whole classes of unfamiliar same-aged peers. The peers rated the
speeches with respect to the social performance of the speaker and the speaker’s
likeability (rejection). Each peer rated the speakers on only one of the four behav-
ioral aspects in order to prevent carryover effects that might have caused the high
intercorrelations in the Miers et al. (2010) study. Trained adult observers also rated
the social performance using the Performance Questionnaire (PQ; Cartwright-
Hatton et al. 2005; Miers et al. 2009). The physical attractiveness of the speakers
was rated by different peers (i.e., different to those rating social performance and
likeability) using photographs (screenshots) of the speakers taken before they
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 165
started their speech. Results showed that the ratings of the four different aspects of
social performance were once again strongly related. That is, the result replicated
that of the Miers et al. (2010) study, even when different peers rated different aspects
of social performance. The main findings of the study were that socially anxious
adolescents were relatively less liked compared to their non-anxious counterparts
and that physical attractiveness and social performance equally mediated the link
between social anxiety and peer liking. However, the mediation effect was partial,
as the direct link between social anxiety and peer liking was still significant after
including the mediators in the model. The authors argued that there may be one or
more characteristics other than physical appearance and social performance that
lead peers to reject socially anxious adolescents who are unfamiliar to them.
A study by Verduin and Kendall (2008) suggests that overt nervousness may be
one of these other mediators in the link between social anxiety and likeability. The
study investigated the effect of anxiety on peer liking in children and young adoles-
cents (9–13 years). Same-aged unfamiliar peers rated video-recorded speeches of
youth with different anxiety disorders, among them a group with social phobia and
a control group without any anxiety disorder. Peers rated the state anxiety and like-
ability of the speakers. Results suggested that peer-rated state anxiety partially
mediated the link between social phobia and peer liking. The authors argued that
there must be other characteristics in addition to overt nervousness, such as depres-
sion or poor social skill, that make socially phobic youth less attractive to unfamiliar
others. This conclusion and the conclusion from the Blöte et al. (2015) study sug-
gest that at least three variables mediate the link between social anxiety and peer
liking in first impressions, namely, overt nervousness, social performance, and
physical appearance.
There is also evidence that high socially anxious adolescents are perceived as
“different” by same-aged unfamiliar peers. Blöte et al. (2012) asked the entire class-
rooms to rate the video-recorded speeches of high and low socially anxious adoles-
cents from the Miers et al. (2010) study. Results showed that high socially anxious
speakers were perceived as less similar to the peer raters themselves compared to
low socially anxious speakers and that similarity perceptions mediated the link
between social anxiety and peer rejection. Notably, not only low, but also high,
socially anxious peer raters perceived high socially anxious speakers as different.
As suggested by the authors, the similarity rating may have been influenced by
wishful thinking, with peers not wishing to be like the high anxious speakers. The
perception of dissimilarity may be part of the general negative halo in the judgments
of both high and low socially anxious peers.
A recent study helps to shed further light on the question of why high socially
anxious adolescents are less effective in social situations relative to nonsocially
anxious adolescents. Blöte et al. (in press) aimed to identify explicit differences in
behavior between high and low socially anxious youth. The study used naive observ-
ers to describe conspicuous features in the video-recorded speech behavior of
socially anxious speakers (9–16 years). Based on these descriptions, the Speech
Performance Observation Scale for Youth (SPOSY) was developed, including sub-
scales related to expressiveness, confidence, and agitation. Expressiveness as
166 A.W. Blöte et al.
measured with the SPOSY comprises behavioral features such as looking friendly,
having a good intonation, using adequate gestures, and showing facial expressions.
It also comprises directing attention to the public. It was found that expressiveness
and confidence are important features distinguishing between high and low socially
anxious youth. Low expressiveness in combination with a lack of confidence may
explain the negative peer responses towards socially anxious youth (Blöte et al. in
press; Van Beek et al. 2006).
Emerging Relationships
Very few studies have addressed the relation between social anxiety and peer rejec-
tion/victimization in the first weeks or months of newly formed groups. One study
by Vernberg et al. (1992) investigated peer relations of relocated adolescents (12–
14 years) followed over a period of 8 months. Victimization measures were exclu-
sion and direct aggression. It was found that the level of adolescents’ social anxiety
did not predict self-reported exclusion or direct aggression experiences in the new
social group, but it did negatively affect the development of friendships.
A study by Gazelle et al. (2005) examined the effect of anxious solitude on social
interactions with familiar and unfamiliar same-aged peers in young preadolescent
girls (9-year-olds). Anxious solitary girls and a control group participated in two
playgroups, one made up of familiar female classmates and the other made up of
unfamiliar girls. In general, anxious solitary girls were perceived by playmates as
less socially competent and were less liked and more victimized by playmates com-
pared to the controls. Notably, these effects were larger in the playgroup of familiar
classmates relative to the so-called unfamiliar playgroup. Anxious solitary girls
showed more whining, complaining, and repetitive behavior in the familiar play-
group than in the unfamiliar playgroup, and the familiar playgroup reacted more
negatively towards them than the unfamiliar playgroup. So, the girls were better off
in the new group of unfamiliar peers than in the group of familiar classmates. This
does not mean that it is generally better for socially anxious youth to be placed in a
new social environment. Gazelle et al. (2005) argue that relatively positive peer
behavior in unfamiliar groups may become more negative with time. Once a social
group starts to agree on the low social status of the anxious solitary youth, exclusion
and victimization may start.
Established Relationships
A number of studies have focused on the social interactions of socially anxious
students in their school environment. Spence et al. (1999) conducted school-based
observations of socially phobic children and adolescents (7–14 years) and found
that youth with social phobia were less socially skilled compared to a control group.
They were also treated less positively by school peers. Relative to the control group,
the socially phobic youth initiated fewer social interactions and had less interactions
with their schoolmates. The socially phobic youth were less socially competent,
according to parents’ perceptions and the perceptions of the youth themselves. The
authors argued that the poor social skills of socially phobic youth may result in
negative peer responses.
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 167
Overt anxiety
First impressions
Physical attractiveness
Emerging relationships
Social performance
Established relationships
Self-worth
Crowd boys
Friendships
membership
Self-worth girls
Fig. 7.1 Overview of results of studies reviewed in this chapter regarding the association between
social anxiety and rejection/victimization. Solid lines tested pathways; Dotted lines hypothesized
pathways
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 171
conclude from the reviewed studies that some aspects in the behavior of socially
anxious students are more important than others in eliciting negative peer judg-
ments. The studies seem to suggest that a general negative halo effect (Alden and
Taylor 2004; Miers et al. 2010) is at work in peer judgments of high socially anxious
adolescents. Future studies are needed to ascertain whether this immediate negative
impression is triggered by one or more specific characteristics, which is then carried
over to other aspects of the person judged. High socially anxious adolescents may
show their anxiety in their behavior and facial/body expression (possibly lacking
expressiveness), and peers may not like this, consequently extending their negative
opinion to the whole person (Blöte et al. 2015). The finding that socially anxious
students are negatively judged and disliked very quickly, in the first minutes of their
social performance, is quite disconcerting in view of the possible consequences of
this response. Negative self-perceptions of socially anxious students about their
social performance seem to be immediately confirmed by peers. This in turn will
have consequences for their behavior in subsequent peer interactions.
In light of the negative first impression socially anxious students make on peers, it
is not surprising that they encounter negative social outcomes when entering unfa-
miliar social groups. It is not yet clear which behaviors specifically elicit the negative
peer responses in emerging social relationships (just as it was not clear in first impres-
sions), although the Gazelle et al. (2005) study suggests that in girls, “annoying”
behaviors like whining and complaining might (partly) mediate the link between
anxious solitude and victimization. Furthermore, this study suggested that placing a
victimized socially anxious student in a new social group will have some positive
effect at first, when the social status of this student is not yet determined by the group.
However, this effect is only temporary and victimization may increase over time.
In established relationships, socially anxious students’ poor social skills may
(partly) explain why they are treated in a negative way by schoolmates (Blöte et al.
2010). Nervous behavior that appears to be a trigger for dislike/rejection at first
sight (Verduin and Kendall 2008) does not seem to have this effect among class-
mates who have known each other for some time (Blöte et al. 2007, 2010). In that
situation the social status of individual students has been agreed on and unpopular
students are disliked by classmates regardless of whether or not they are nervous
during a social performance. However, this explanation needs the necessary caution
because of method differences between studies, in particular differences in age and
diagnostic status of the participants and in the age of the observers. Verduin and
Kendall (2008) studied preadolescents and young adolescents with social phobia
and used peer-rated nervousness, whereas Blöte et al. (2007) and (2010) studied a
normative sample of older adolescents whose nervousness was judged by an adult
observer. Future studies will be needed in order to better understand the role of overt
nervousness in peer rejection/victimization.
In view of differences in peer interactions between boys and girls (Rose and
Rudolph 2006); it seems likely that the relation between social anxiety and victim-
ization is different between the sexes. Some studies indeed found evidence for the
moderating effect of gender (Ranta et al. 2013; Siegel et al. 2009; Tillfors et al.
2012). At the same time, it is difficult to draw conclusions from these studies
because of the dissimilar findings. They present an ambiguous picture of the effect
172 A.W. Blöte et al.
Cognitive Factors
The previous sections show that there is quite substantial evidence for negative
treatment of socially anxious youth by their age peers, both familiar and unfamiliar,
as compared to non-anxious youth. That is, socially anxious youth are more likely
to be rejected, neglected, and victimized. In contrast, the cognitive theories of SAD
(Clark and Wells 1995; Rapee and Heimberg 1997) place more emphasis on the
negative perceptions socially anxious individuals have of social situations and their
own performance in these situations. Several empirical studies show that socially
anxious youth indeed have negative social perceptions. Compared to non-anxious
youth, they interpret ambiguous social cues in a negative way (e.g., Blöte et al.
2014; Miers et al. 2008; Vassilopoulos and Banerjee 2008) and engage in negative
interpretations or evaluations of their own behavior in social situations (Alfano et al.
2006; Inderbitzen-Nolan et al. 2007; Miers et al. 2009, 2011; Ranta et al. 2014).
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 173
One of the main questions to arise out of the cognitive approach is whether the
negative perceptions of socially anxious individuals are based on a “kernel of truth”
(Norton and Hope 2001) or are biased relative to an objective source of information.
Cognitive theories imply that social experiences and feedback from other persons in
social situations influence the development of negative perceptions of one’s own
social performance. Given the evidence reviewed in section “Social interactions
with classmates” that socially anxious youth experience more negative social inter-
actions at school, this would suggest that negative social perceptions in socially
anxious youth are, at least to some degree, warranted. Indirect evidence for the
kernel-of-truth hypothesis was provided in a study on the social perceptions of high
socially anxious students and their classmates (8th–10th graders; aged 13–18 years).
All pupils in a class were asked to imagine an oral presentation situation in their
class where they or their classmates give a presentation (Blöte and Westenberg
2007). They then filled in a questionnaire how they saw themselves treated in such
a situation and how hypothetical classmates showing either fearful or relaxed behav-
ior were treated. The socially anxious students reported that they felt negatively
treated by classmates when giving a speech. These perceptions seemed warranted as
their classmates also perceived that the behavior towards the hypothetical socially
anxious (fearful) students in their class was more negative compared to class behav-
ior towards students who were nonsocially anxious (i.e., relaxed) (Blöte and
Westenberg 2007).
Direct evidence for the kernel-of-truth hypothesis could be provided by a direct
comparison between self-evaluations of high and low socially anxious youth with
evaluations of peer observers. If high socially anxious youth would evaluate them-
selves as poorer with respect to social performance than their low-anxious counter-
parts while the peer observers would not perceive a difference between these groups,
then we could speak of biased self-perceptions (Miers et al. 2009). Alternatively, if
the peer observers would also perceive a difference between high- and low-anxious
youth, the negative perceptions would be warranted, supporting the “kernel-of-
truth” notion.
We are not aware of a study that has compared the self-evaluations of socially
anxious youth with evaluations of peer observers. However, some studies have
investigated whether socially anxious youth have negatively biased perceptions of
their social performance using the performance evaluations of adult observers as a
standard. In terms of appearing nervous during a social performance situation, stud-
ies consistently show that socially anxious youth perceive themselves as looking
more nervous than do adult observers of these socially anxious youth (e.g.,
Cartwright-Hatton et al. 2005; Inderbitzen-Nolan et al. 2007; Miers et al. 2009).
Thus, socially anxious youths’ negative perceptions of their nervous behavior seem
to be unwarranted.
When comparing self- to observer evaluations of social skills, a negative bias
may not apply to all socially anxious youth. Miers et al. (2009) investigated high
and low socially anxious youth’s (aged 9–17 years) evaluations of social perfor-
mance during a short speech. The speeches were recorded and shown to indepen-
dent adult observers who evaluated the performance using the same questionnaire
174 A.W. Blöte et al.
as the participants. When analyzing the whole high socially anxious group, Miers
et al. (2009) found that participants and observers were in agreement about the
poorer social skills of high socially anxious youth. This suggested that the percep-
tions of high socially anxious youth were warranted. However, the authors also split
the high socially anxious group into two groups using the observers’ performance
evaluations: those with a good speech performance and those with a poor speech
performance. This analysis showed that the negative self-perceptions were justified
only for the socially anxious youth who had a poor speech performance. In contrast,
for the socially anxious youth whose performance was judged as good, their nega-
tive perceptions were unwarranted. Hence, the “kernel-of-truth” hypothesis seems
to apply only to socially anxious youth who are indeed less socially skilled (as
determined by adult observers).
In sum, with regard to the way they are treated by peers, the negative cognitions
of socially anxious adolescents seem partly warranted and partly colored by internal
processes. Socially anxious adolescents rightly expect negative responses from oth-
ers. Based on these expectations, they interpret neutral responses in a negative way.
As far as adolescents’ self-perceptions of social performance are concerned, they
are “incorrect” in thinking that they make a nervous impression (more nervous than
others), and some of them are right in evaluating their own social performance as
poor. Socially anxious adolescents who perform well may have negatively biased
self-perceptions, whereas poor performing socially anxious adolescents may be cor-
rect in judging their performance as poor.
It seems important that future studies make a direct comparison between self-
evaluations of performance, on the one hand, and performance evaluations by peers
(using the same measure) on the other hand. In this way, we can better investigate
whether the negative self-perceptions are based on a kernel of truth or are biased.
The school environment, and more specifically the formal classroom situation,
clearly presents a real challenge to socially anxious adolescents. A number of these
adolescents experience considerable distress at school and try to avoid distressing
classroom situations. In some cases, socially anxious adolescents may show a more
severe form of avoidance by refusing to go to school altogether. Moreover, studies
indicate that school refusers with AD or SAD have poorer school attendance and
that SAD among school-refusing adolescents is often treatment resistant. In all, this
chapter sketches a rather bleak picture of the school experiences of socially anxious
students.
What makes successful interventions for socially anxious students particularly
difficult is that these students not only think that they perform poorly in social
situations but that some of them actually are less socially skilled, less confident,
and less expressive and consequently elicit negative responses from peers. At first
glance, peers see them as different and reject them. Later on, when social
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 175
Future Studies
References
Albano AM (1995) Treatment of social anxiety in adolescents. Cogn Behav Pract 2:271–298
Alden LE, Taylor CT (2004) Interpersonal processes in social phobia. Clin Psychol Rev
24:857–882
Alfano CA, Beidel DC, Turner SM (2006) Cognitive correlates of social phobia among children
and adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34:182–194. doi:10.1007/s10802-005-9012-9
Alfano CA, Pina AA, Villalta IK, Beidel DC, Ammerman RT, Crosby LE (2009) Mediators and
moderators of outcome in the behavioral treatment of childhood social phobia. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48:945–953
Avant T, Gazelle H, Faldowski R (2011) Classroom emotional climate as a moderator of anxious
solitary children’s longitudinal risk for peer exclusion: a child × environment model. Dev
Psychol 47:1711
Baker JR, Hudson JL (2013) Friendship quality predicts treatment outcome in children with
anxiety disorders. Behav Res Ther 51:31–36
Beidas RS, Crawley SA, Mychailyszyn MP, Comer JS, Kendall PC (2010) Cognitive-behavioral
treatment of anxious youth with comorbid school refusal: clinical presentation and treatment
response. Psihologijske teme 19:255–271
Beidel DC, Turner SM, Young BJ, Ammerman RT, Sallee FR, Crosby L (2007) Psychopathology
of adolescent social phobia. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 29:46–53
Berg I (1997) School refusal and truancy. Arch Dis Child 76:90–91
Berg I (2002) School avoidance, school phobia, and truancy. In: Lewis M (ed) Child and adoles-
cent psychiatry: a comprehensive textbook, 3rd edn. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Sydney,
pp 1260–1266
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 177
Erath SA, Flanagan KS, Bierman KL (2007) Social anxiety and peer relations in early adoles-
cence: behavioral and cognitive factors. J Abnorm Child Psychol 35:405–416
Erath SA, Tu KM, El-Sheikh M (2012) Socially anxious and peer-victimized preadolescents:
“doubly primed” for distress? J Abnorm Child Psychol 40:837–848
Essau CA, Conradt J, Petermann F (1999) Frequency and comorbidity of social phobia and social
fears in adolescents. Behav Res Ther 37:831–843
Galloway D (1985) Schools and persistent absentees. Pergamon Press, Oxford
Garcia-Lopez LJ (2013) Tratando…trastorno de ansiedad social [Treating…social anxiety
disorder]. Piramide, Madrid in the References for that chapter.
Garcia-Lopez LJ, Irurtia MJ, Caballo VE, Díaz-Castela MM (2011) Ansiedad social y abuso
psicológico [Social anxiety and psychological abuse]. Behav Psycholy 19:223–236
Gazelle H, Putallaz M, Li Y, Grimes CL, Kupersmidt JB, Coie JD (2005) Anxious solitude across
contexts: girls’ interactions with familiar and unfamiliar peers. Child Dev 76:227–246
Gren-Landell M, Tillfors M, Furmark T, Bohlin G, Andersson G, Svedin CG (2009) Social phobia
in Swedish adolescents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 44:1–7
Grills AE, Ollendick TH (2002) Peer victimization, global self-worth, and anxiety in middle
school children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 31:59–68
Hanby MSR, Fales J, Nangle DW, Serwik AK, Hedrich UJ (2012) Social anxiety as a predictor of
dating aggression. J Interpers Violence 27:1867–1888. doi:10.1177/0886260511431438
Heyne D (2006) School rfeusal. In: Fisher JE, O’Donohue WT (eds) Practitioner’s guide to
evidence-based psychotherapy. Springer, New York, pp 600-619
Heyne D, King N, Tonge B, Rollings S, Pritchard M, Young D, Myerson N (1998) The self-
efficacy questionnaire for school situations: development and psychometric evaluation. Behav
Chang 15:31–40
Heyne D, Sauter FM, Van Widenfelt BM, Vermeiren R, Westenberg PM (2011) School refusal and
anxiety in adolescence: non-randomized trial of a developmentally sensitive cognitive
behavioral therapy. J Anxiety Disord 25:870–878
Heyne D, Sauter FM, Maynard BR (in press) Moderators and mediators of treatments for youth
with school refusal or truancy. In: Maric M, Prins PJM, Ollendick TH (eds) Mediators and
moderators of youth treatment outcomes. Oxford University Press, New York
Hodson KJ, McManus FV, Clark DM, Doll H (2008) Can Clark and Wells’ (1995) cognitive model
of social phobia be applied to young people? Behav Cogn Psychother 36:449–461
Holmbeck GN, Devine KA, Wasserman R, Schellinger K, Tuminello E (2012) Guides from
developmental psychology for therapy with adolescents. In: Kendall PC (ed) Child and
adolescent therapy: cognitive-behavioral procedures. Guilford, New York, pp 429–470
Inderbitzen HM, Walters KS, Bukowski AL (1997) The role of social anxiety in adolescent peer
relations: differences among sociometric status groups and rejected subgroups. J Clin Child
Psychol 26:338–348
Inderbitzen-Nolan HM, Anderson ER, Johnson HS (2007) Subjective versus objective behavioral
ratings following two analogue tasks: a comparison of socially phobic and non-anxious
adolescents. J Anxiety Disord 21:76–90
Ingul JM, Nordahl HM (2013) Anxiety as a risk factor for school absenteeism: what differentiates
anxious school attenders from non-attenders? Ann Gen Psychiatry 12:25
Kearney CA (2002) Identifying the function of school refusal behavior: a revision of the School
Refusal Assessment Scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 24:235–245
Kearney CA, Albano AM (2004) The functional profiles of school refusal behavior diagnostic
aspects. Behav Modif 28:147–161
Kingery JN, Erdley CA, Marshall KC, Whitaker KG, Reuter TR (2010) Peer experiences of
anxious and socially withdrawn youth: an integrative review of the developmental and clinical
literature. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 13:91–128. doi:10.1007/s10567-009-0063-2
La Greca AM (1998) Social anxiety scales for children and adolescents: manual and instructions
for the SASC, SASC–R, SAS–A (adolescents), and parent versions of the scales. University of
Miami, Department of Psychology, Miami
7 Social Anxiety and the School Environment of Adolescents 179
La Greca AM, Harrison HM (2005) Adolescent peer relations, friendships, and romantic relation-
ships: do they predict social anxiety and depression? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 34:49–61.
doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_5
La Greca AM, Lopez N (1998) Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and
friendships. J Abnorm Child Psychol 26:83–94
Last CG, Strauss CC (1990) School refusal in anxiety-disordered children and adolescents. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29:31–35
Layne AE, Bernstein GA, Egan EA, Kushner MG (2003) Predictors of treatment response in
anxious-depressed adolescents with school refusal. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
42:319–326
London B, Downey G, Bonica C, Paltin I (2007) Social causes and consequences of rejection
sensitivity. J Res Adolesc 17:481–506. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00531.x
Lyon AR (2010) Confirmatory factor analysis of the School Refusal Assessment Scale-Revised in
an African American community sample. J Psychoeduc Assess 28:511–523
McShane G, Walter G, Rey JM (2004) Functional outcome of adolescents with school refusal. Clin
Child Psychol Psychiatry 9:53–60
Miers AC, Blöte AW, Bögels SM, Westenberg PM (2008) Interpretation bias and social anxiety in
adolescents. J Anxiety Disord 22:1462–1471. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.02.010
Miers AC, Blöte AW, Bokhorst CL, Westenberg PM (2009) Negative self-evaluations and the
relation to performance level in socially anxious children and adolescents. Behav Res Ther
47:1043–1049. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.017
Miers AC, Blöte AW, Westenberg PM (2010) Peer perceptions of social skills in socially
anxious and nonanxious adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38:33–41. doi:10.1007/
s10802-009-9345-x
Miers AC, Blöte AW, Westenberg PM (2011) Negative social cognitions in socially anxious youth:
distorted reality or a kernel of truth? J Child Fam Stud 20:214–223. doi:10.1007/
s10826-010-9423-2
Nair MKC, Russell PSS, Subramaniam VS, Nazeema S, Chembagam N, Russell S, Charles H
(2013) School phobia and anxiety disorders among adolescents in a rural community popula-
tion in India. Indian J Pediatr 80:171–174
Nakamura M, Kondo I, Kubota A, Furukawa I, Suzuki T, Nakamura H, Hayakawa N, Ojima T,
Aoki N (2010) School-attendance problems, subjective symptoms and lifestyle factors: the
Shizuoka health and lifestyle survey of students [Nihon koshu eisei zasshi]. Jpn J Public Health
57:881–890
Norton PJ, Hope DA (2001) Kernels of truth or distorted perceptions: self and observer ratings of
social anxiety and performance. Behav Ther 32:765–786
Ollendick TH, Hirshfeld-Becker DR (2002) The developmental psychopathology of social anxiety
disorder. Biol Psychiatry 51:44–58. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01305-1
Place M, Hulsmeier J, Davis S, Taylor E (2000) School refusal: a changing problem which requires
a change of approach? Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 5:345–355
Place M, Hulsmeier J, Davis S, Taylor E (2002) The coping mechanisms of children with school
refusal. J Res Spec 1–10
Ranta K, Kaltiala-Heino R, Koivisto AM, Tuomisto MT, Pelkonen M, Marttunen M (2007) Age
and gender differences in social anxiety symptoms during adolescence: the Social Phobia
Inventory (SPIN) as a measure. Psychiatry Res 153:261–270, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
psychres.2006.12.006
Ranta K, Kaltiala-Heino R, Pelkonen M, Marttunen M (2009) Associations between peer victim-
ization, self-reported depression and social phobia among adolescents: the role of comorbidity.
J Adolesc 32:77–93, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.11.005
Ranta K, Kaltiala-Heino R, Rantanen P, Marttunen M (2012) The Mini-Social Phobia Inventory: psy-
chometric properties in an adolescent general population sample. Compr Psychiatry 53:630–637
Ranta K, Kaltiala-Heino R, Fröjd S, Marttunen M (2013) Peer victimization and social phobia: a
follow-up study among adolescents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 48:533–544
180 A.W. Blöte et al.
Verduin TL, Kendall PC (2008) Peer perceptions and liking of children with anxiety disorders.
J Abnorm Child Psychol 36:459–469
Vernberg EM, Abwender DA, Ewell KK, Beery SH (1992) Social anxiety and peer relationships
in early adolescence: a prospective analysis. J Clin Child Psychol 21:189–196
Weeks M, Coplan RJ, Kingsbury A (2009) The correlates and consequences of early appearing
social anxiety in young children. J Anxiety Disord 23:965–972
Westenberg PM, Drewes MJ, Goedhart AW, Siebelink BM, Treffers PDA (2004) A developmental
analysis of self‐reported fears in late childhood through mid‐adolescence: social‐evaluative
fears on the rise? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:481–495
Westenberg PM, Bokhorst CL, Miers AC, Sumter SR, Kallen VL, van Pelt J, Blote AW (2009)
A prepared speech in front of a pre-recorded audience: subjective, physiological, and
neuroendocrine responses to the Leiden Public Speaking Task. Biol Psychol 82:116–124.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.005
Wittchen HU, Stein MB, Kessler RC (1999) Social fears and social phobia in a community sample
of adolescents and young adults: prevalence, risk factors and co-morbidity. Psychol Med
29:309–323. doi:10.1017/s0033291798008174