Uba Booklet Fonologia de La Lengua Extranjera Ii 2023
Uba Booklet Fonologia de La Lengua Extranjera Ii 2023
Uba Booklet Fonologia de La Lengua Extranjera Ii 2023
Phonology II
Profesoras:
Cecilia Schumacher
Nancy Suárez
-2023-
1
INTONATION
A brief introduction
The voice pitch varies continuously. We don´t speak in a monotone. The pitch variation extends
over single phonemes, sequences of phonemes and whole utterances. So Intonation refers to the
patterns that can be found within this pitch variation.
O´Connor: The pitch patterns of spoken English, the speech tunes or melodies, the musical
features of English.
“It is not what you say, but the way you say it” (not the choice of words, but the manner in
which the words are used. The way of saying something may depend on gesture, facial
expression and voice quality, but usually most significant factor is intonation (e-mail)
• Characteristics of intonation:
2
1. Tonality: the first matter a speaker has to decide is the division of the spoken material into
chunks. There will be an intonation pattern associated with each chunk. These chunks are
known as Intonation Phrases or IP. Each IP has its own intonation pattern or “tune”.
IP= word group= tone group= intonation group= tone unit
2. Tonicity: speakers use intonation to highlight some words as important for the meaning
they wish to convey. To highlight an important word we accent it, i.e., we accent its
stressed syllable, i.e., we add pitch prominence (= a change in pitch, or the beginning of a
pitch movement. (tonicity= nucleus placement)
3. Tone: what kind of pitch movement
• PITCH: Three variables of pitch:
1. Direction of the pitch movement (i.e. whether the pitch falls, rises, falls and then rises,
etc.
2. The degree of pitch movement: does the pitch move a lot or a little. How much does the
pitch fall or rise
3. The placement of this pitch movement within the speaker´s own voice range
• Choice of words can be fairly conscious, but intonation seems much less conscious.
What happens when a non-native speaker uses inappropriate intonation? :obscuration of meaning,
perception of a quite different message. Native speaker can make allowance for grammar
mistakes, mispronunciations, choice of vocabulary, but intonation is different.
• Why is it more difficult to teach intonation? Partly because we are not in control of a
practical, workable, system through which we can make intonation comprehensible to
ourselves or to our learners. But then this may be due to the nature of intonation: less
perceptible, less tangible than other areas of language.
Anatomy of an IP:
EX: '
Tonic syllable= The Tonic = The nucleus: the most important accent in the IP. It is the place
where the pitch changes or the pitch movement for the nuclear tone begins. It carries the main
pitch change. A tone unit must contain a tonic syllable, otherwise it is incomplete
The tonic syllable is often the LLI in the tone unit, owing to the tendency in English for new
information or the focus of information to be placed at the end of a sentence.
3
The tonic syllable may be placed earlier in the tone group, where there is a word of greater
importance to the message
Occasionally the tonic syllable may be a non-lexical word, as when some contrast is made
Onset: the first accent before the nucleus IMPORTANT: is the pitch of the onset syllable
significant? Low bounce vs Take Off
Head: the part extending from the onset to the last syllable before the nucleus
Tail: The part of the IP that follows the nucleus. It contains non-prominent syllables. It may contain
a word stress.
Attitudinal: usually isolates certain intonation contours. Cannot isolate phrase from larger context.
• Tone unit represents focus of information. Where is usually the focus of information in
English? Content words vs function words.
• LLI rule: when the IP contains information which is new only we generally place the
nucleus on the LAST LEXICAL ITEM
Functions of intonation
4
Rhythm: English is a stressed- time language: the stressed syllables tend to recur at regular
intervals regardless of the number of unstressed syllables in between.
According to the principle of stress timing these lines each occupy approximately the same
amount of time. The more unstressed syllables there are the quicker you have to say them in
order to fit them into the beat. In other words the time taken to speak each utterance depends
on the number of stresses and not on the number of syllables. This means that maintaining
regular stress depends on maintaining irregular syllable length. This is in contrast to the
phenomenon of syllable timings where the time taken to speak an utterance depends roughly
on how many syllables there are. English, Dutch and German are examples of languages said
to be stress timed. Spanish, Japanese and French are said to be syllable timed.
English does not use tone lexically. English is not a tone language, but English does use tone
for intonation. English makes use of tone intonationally, but not lexically. In fact the intonation
system of English constitutes the most important and complex part of English prosody. By
combining different pitch levels we express a range of intonational meanings: breaking the
utterance into chunks, perhaps distinguishing between clause types (such as statements vs.
questions) focussing on some parts of the utterance and not on others, indicating which part of
our message is background information and which is foreground, signalling our attitude to what
we are saying.
Sources:
5
SUPRASEGMENTAL FEATURES
Suprasegmental (also called prosodic) means “superimposed on the basic sound segments of
language”, so suprasegmental features are phenomena which are spread over more than one
sound segment. Intonation , for example, is the melodic pattern which extends over a whole
utterance, and stress, less obviously, is a property of a whole syllable.
Stress is the degree of force with which a sound or syllable is uttered. In popular usage, it is
associated with a general notion of emphasis; it gives PROMINENCE to some syllables, and
hence words, and in combination with intonation helps to avoid monotony and make speech more
interesting for the hearer. The strong stresses of an utterance provide it with its rhythm (rhythm:
perceived regularity of prominent units in speech).
Stress is not the only factor involved in producing the auditory effect of prominence. The
concomitants of stress are one or more of the following:
a) Loudness
b) Greater length
c) Higher pitch
d) Difference in quality and inherent sonority
After stress, the effect of prominence is achieved first by pitch, and then by length; loudness and
quality have less effect.
Source: Mott (2005) English Phonetics and Phonology for Spanish Speakers
6
English Intonation - An introduction. J. C. Wells
Chapter 1
Intonation: the melody of speech. The study of pitch variation of the voice and how speakers use
this pitch variation to convey linguistic and pragmatic meaning.
It also involves the study of the rhythm of speech, and the study of how the interplay of accented,
stressed and unstressed syllables functions as a framework onto which the intonation patterns are
attached.
Prosodic features → pitch, loudness and speed: they combine together to make the rhythm of
speech.
Tone languages: English makes use of tone intonationally, but not lexically
The three Ts: three linguistic intonation systems: speakers of English face three types of decisions
as they speak
1) Tonality (chunking): division of the spoken material into IPs, word groups, tone groups,
intonation groups
2) Tonicity: nucleus placement: to highlight an important word, we highlight it: we accent its
stressed syllable, that is to say, we add pitch prominence to the rhythmic prominence that a
stressed syllable bears.
7
Nucleus: the most important accent in the IP. It indicates the end of the focused part of the
material. It is the place where pitch movement for the nuclear tone begins.
Functions of intonation:
Rhythm: English is a stressed- time language: the stressed syllables tend to recur at regular
intervals
Is English a Tone Language? English does not use tone lexically. English is not a tone
language, but English does use tone for intonation. English makes use of tone intonationally,
but not lexically. In fact the intonation system of English constitutes the most important and
complex part of English prosody. By combining different pitch levels we express a range of
intonational meanings: breaking the utterance into chunks, perhaps distinguishing between
clause types (such as statements vs. questions) focussing on some parts of the utterance and
not on others, indicating which part of our message is background information and which is
foreground, signalling our attitude to what we are saying.
Chapter 2: Tone
It is useful to apply the notion of a default tone: the default tone is the most frequent, the expected
tone for an IP
8
Fall for: statements, exclamations, wh- Non Fall for: Yes- No questions
questions and commands
Statements: a) Fall: what we say is potentially complete. It is expressed with confidence, definitely
and unreservedly. It signals finality.
Fall: endpoint always low. After a falling nucleus, the tail is always low. There is very often a step
up in pitch as we reach the beginning of the nuclear fall.
9
10
Rise: if there is a tail, the rising pitch movement does not happen wholly on the nuclear syllable.
The rise is spread over the nuclear syllable and all the following syllables. Last syllable: highest
pitched.
Fall-rise: falling part takes place on the nuclear syllable or between that syllable and the next and
the rise begins on the last stressed syllable. It may express the following:
11
12
Declarative questions: grammatically like statements. Identified as questions only by their
intonation, or by the pragmatics of the situation where they are used.
Usually said with the rise (You´ll be coming to dinner?), a fall-rise ( You didn´t go and tell
him?) or even a fall (So, she´ll be free by \six then?).
13
Yes-no and elliptical answers: the answer to a yes-no question is usually not a complete
statement. Rather, it is just yes or no (or an equivalent). Quite often we support the yes or no
by an elliptical verb phrase. Or we may just use the elliptical verb phrase on its own. The tones
for these answers may be any of the tones that can be used in full statements: fall, rise, fall-
rise:
14
15
Questions
Wh-questions: default tone is a fall: definitive fall. The speaker knows and tells.
A wh-question, however, can also be said with a rise or, (less commonly with a fall-rise). This
is the encouraging rise. This has the effect of making it more gentle, kindly, encouraging,
sympathetic or deferential, as opposed to the businesslike fall.
16
17
Yes-No questions: the default tone is the rise.
Requests are usually said with a rise too: 'Will you send him the letter?
A fall (“insistent fall”) is also possible: this makes the question more insistent, more businesslike,
more serious, more threatening perhaps: ex: I´ll 'ask you once more. 'Did you take the money?
(“insistent fall”). This “insistent fall is often used in guessing games:
18
Tag questions with reverse polarity
→ expecting the other person to agree: “insistent fall” (appeal for agreement). In some cases the
falling tone has the force of an exclamation. Exclamations always have a fall.
→When attached to a command, a tag often comes in the tail rather than having its own intonation
phrase:
→ If the tag after a command does have its own IP, the tone is usually an encouraging rise, giving
a softening effect:
→After a command, a tag with a fall sounds very insistent (not all speakers find this construction
intonationally well-formed): 'Answer the phone, │ will you?
Constant-polarity tags (= Tench: “copy tag”): main clause: positive, tag: positive.
This kind of tag implies a sudden realization of the significance of the proposition:
If there is no separate tonality for the copy tag, there is added a note of irritation:
19
20
Independent elliptical questions → default tone: yes-no rise. Function: keep the
conversation going:
Ex. A: I 'really like it here. B: Do you? (I was afraid you wouldn´t)
21
Checking → interjections such as ∕OK? Or ∕Right? : usually said with a rise
Pardon question rise: ∕What? ∕Sorry? ∕Pardon? (with this question you can query about
the entire previous utterance or just one element in it.
A different kind of pardon question is the please- repeat wh-question: it involves changing the
focused element into a question word. Tone: always a rise
She took a tonga. She took what? (no fronting of the question word)
Echo question: uses some or all of the same words as used by the previous speaker,
but with a rise. This may be a simple request for repetition or clarification, or it may also express
surprise and amazement at what the other speaker has said:
22
Other sentence types
Warnings may be said with a fall-rise (if said with a fall tone, these would indeed be not as much
warnings as straight-forward commands): ex: Watch out.
* The implication here is something like this: 'Do as I say, │or 'something bad happens.
23
Like negative statements, negative commands often have a fall-rise without necessarily implying a
warning: ex: 'Don´t forget the salt.
Said with a rise, the sentence may sound soothing and kind (we use this tone when speaking to
children, for example. To adults, it can sound patronizing): 'Come to Daddy.
24
Interjections and greetings → default tone: fall:
Many short interjections can be said with an encouraging rise, inviting the other person to speak or
to continue speaking: (answering the phone) Hu llo.
A: Oh Mary. B: Yes.
The interjection oops, whoops (uttered when you have just made a mistake or dropped something)
seems always to have a rise and mid level.
Ex: Here is your change. Thank you (to express genuine gratitude, it is necessary to use a fall)
In calling someone by name, we normally use a rise or fall-rise if trying to get their attention. A fall,
on the other hand, is a straightforward greeting (or, of course, an exclamation)
For most greetings, both falls and rises are perfectly possible and acceptable. A definitive fall is
more formal, an encouraging rise is more personal:
• A vocative after hello or hi usually has its own rising tone. Hello may be shift-stressed so
that the accent falls on the first syllable. Ex: \Hello ∕John
Whereas Hello may have any tone, hi can only have a fall
Farewell: goodbye and its equivalents have a rise , expressing good will. The same applies when
a TV presenter signs off . To get rid of an unwelcome guest, we would use a fall: 'Good bye
The informal See you tends to have a fall-rise rather than a rise: See you.
25
Sequencing of tones: leading and trailing tones
Many phrases or clauses do not stand alone, but are attached to some other element. They are not
complete in themselves, but are dependent on some other (independent) structure. If a dependent
element precedes the main element, we say it is leading. If it follows the main element, we say it is
trailing. In either case, the unmarked tone for a dependent element is a fall-rise or a rise. With a
leading dependent element, this non-fall is usually a fall-rise.
Alternatively, a leading dependent element may have a rise (including the possibility of a mid-level
tone):
26
Ex: 'After lunch, │ we could 'call on Mary.
Fall-rise (less commonly rise) + fall → for the order: dependent (leading) –independent elements
Fall + rise (less commonly fall-rise) → for the order independent – dependent (trailing)
27
Topic and comment:
Topic: (subject or theme): typically said with a non-falling tone (dependent fall-rise or rise)
Comment (the thing we say about the subject or topic, a “rheme”: fall
1. Your 'passport will be ready to morrow. (straightforward, typical of rapid, routine style of
speech)
2. Your passport / will be ready to morrow. (slower, more deliberate form of delivery)
3. Your passport / will be ready to morrow (like version 2 + the fall-rise adds emphasis to the
topic as against the comment. It can also be a signal of contrast
The relationship between non-falling topic and falling comment is preserved if we change the
sentence structure so as to reverse the order of the two parts:
28
29
Open and closed lists
You can have coffee or tea. (the speakers offers two drinks)
You can have coffee or tea. (with a rise on “tea”, the speaker implies there are more options.
The list is open.
→ It is also possible to avoid giving each item its own IP: openness of a list can be signalled simply
by not having reached the nucleus:
Alternative questions → sets of two or more yes-no questions linked by “or”. They are treated as
lists:
Adverbials → if placed at the beginning, they usually have a leading non-final fall-rise (or rise). If
placed at the end, they tend to have a trailing rise.
Some reinforce the sense of the main clause (“Of course, for ever”) : reinforcing fall:
Ex: Do you 'think I ough to say something? Of course, │ you must pro test.
31
Fall plus Rise (O`Connor & Arnold: High Dive)
• This combination of Fall and Rise is particularly common where the first nucleus goes on a
word referring to a mental state or on an intensifying word:
• Also found in cases where the second nucleus falls on information that is new though fairly
predictable:
• The part with the fall contains the most important idea, while the part with the rise contains
an idea of secondary importance
• Commands said with a fall-plus-rise pattern are pleading requests, rather than orders that
are expected to be obeyed:
a) fall-rise b) fall-plus-rise:
↓ ↓
Implication no implications or reservations. Straightforward definitive fall
for the major focus, followed by a dependent rise for minor focus
a) A: I´ve got some chocolate here B: I like chocolate (but I´m on a diet)
b) A: I´ve got some chocolate here B: Oh good. │I like │ chocolate . 'Pass it over.
32
33
Chapter 3
Basic principles
Nucleus = nuclear accent → syllable that bears the nuclear tone: We accent a syllable by giving
it a prominent change in pitch, or movement in pitch, or the start of a pitch movement. An accented
syllable is always also rhythmically stressed, i.e. it has a rhythmic beat. Pragmatically, we accent a
word by accenting its stressed syllable (or at least one of them if it has more than one). This
indicates the importance or relevance of the word for what we are saying.
Lexical stress: part of the basic pattern of a word´s pronunciation, as shown in dictionaries.
In an IP there may be other accents in addition to the nuclear accent. If so, the nucleus is the last
accent in the IP. Any other accents come earlier in the IP and are “prenuclear”. The first is known
as the onset.
To make a word the nucleus of an IP, we put a nuclear tone on (or starting on) its lexically stressed
syllable. To produce an English intonation pattern correctly it is essential, therefore, to know which
syllable in each word bears the stress.
Content words and function words. Generally speaking, we accent content words but no function
words. Hence the nucleus is typically placed on the last content word. To refine our tonicity rule
so as to allow for compounds, we need to change “on the last content word” to an expression
covering both simple words and compound words: lexical item
Default tonicity (neutral, unmarked) The nucleus typically falls on the Last Lexical Item (LLI
Rule)
We deaccent (=remove potential accents from) old information. If all the information is new, then
we can accent all the lexical items. So the nucleus is placed on the last lexical item.
34
Old information:
• repeated words: A: How about a gin and tonic? B: Oh I´d prefer a vodka and tonic
• synonyms: A: shall we wash the clothes? B: Oh I hate doing the laundry
• hypernyms (words of broader meanings): A: do you like ∕football? B: Yes, I like sports
(“sports” is a hypernym of football and so treated as old information)
• hyponyms count as new information: A: Do you like ∕sports? B. Yes, I 'like football
• “ minutes”, “days”, “o´clock”, “woman”, “dollars”, “hours” → words that are not deaccented
35
Prospective and implied givenness:
Prospective givenness: important idea: accentuation and tonicity depend on the speaker´s
mental planning. Words that are about to be repeated are often deaccented:
but
Broad (we bring everything into focus) and narrow focus (we can focus selectively on one part of
the message)
Focus domain: the part of the IP that is placed in focus. The nucleus tells us where the focus
domain ends. Broad focus means that the focus domain is the whole IP. Everything in the IP is
brought into focus. We use broad focus, for example, in answer to the question What happened?
Contrastive focus (narrow focus): the nuclear accent draws attention to a contrast the speaker
is making. Any following material within the same IP is unaccented and forms part of the tail of the
IP.
Ex: You 'may have started the essay, │but 'have you finished it? (explicit contrast)
I 'don´t know what you are complaining about. ( implicit contrast: the hearer is left to infer
the other part of the message)
Sometimes a pattern of contrastive focus is lexicalized. (ex: \monosyllable, \high jump, \long jump).
London Underground lines have lexicalized contrastive focus: the Central Line, the Northern Line
Or
36
Pronouns and demonstratives
Example
It’s me.
A: Who’s there? B: I am
Follow me.
Look at this!
He is a friend of mine
• The complement of the verb to be regularly receives the nucleus, even if it is a pronoun.
Ex: : A: Who´s that? B: It´s me. / A: Who´ll be on next? B: It´ll be you, I think.
• English has a number of idioms involving fixed tonicity: fossilized idiomatic expressions said
with a particular intonation:
Ex: 'Good for you! (genuine congratulation)
• In clause-final position the possessive pronouns (mine, yours, etc.) tend inherently to
convey new information, and attract the nucleus ( ex: 'give me hers, │ and 'I´ll take his.)
(ATTENTION: ex: I´ve been talking to a friend of mine)
• Reflexive pronouns →True reflexive (Subject and object have the same referent. The
object of a verb or after a preposition is not accented because it is not usually contrastive.
Ex: She is very pleased with herself)
→ Contrastive: ex: Do you like the dress? I 'made it my self (when
contrastive, the pronoun attracts the nucleus)
• Reciprocal pronouns each other and one another: not contrastive, and therefore not
accented.
Sometimes the speaker wants to emphasize the polarity( = the quality of being either positive or
negative) of a verb or its tense. In both cases this may cause the nucleus to go on an auxiliary or
modal verb
38
a) Marked negative: ex: A: I think they´ll just surrender. B: They won´t
surrender.
b) Marked positive: ex: A: You are not involved. B: Oh but I am involved.
c) Polarity with tense: ex: A: Are you a vegetarian? B: I used to be a
vegetarian, but...
.
Yes-no question: The nucleus goes on the word yes or no (or a synonymous adverb) if they are the
only answers. If they are followed by a short sentence fragment involving a verb, the nucleus falls
on the operator (=auxiliary or modal verb):
Different options:
B: Definitely
B: 'Not really.
B: Yes, │ I have.
B: Yes, I have.
B: No, I haven´t.
A: Is she coming with us? B: I´m afraid she isn´t → Accent on contracted form
Prepositions
A sentence such as “How are you?” consists of function words only (the verb be behaves like an
auxiliary – generally deaccented) . There are no lexical items. Yet the nucleus has to go
somewhere:
► If a direct or indirect wh-question has the pattern wh-word- be- pronoun, then the nucleus goes
on the verb “be” itself: ex: ' Tell me how you are. 'Who is it? 'Tell me who it is. This need
not involve narrow or contrastive focus of any kind.
►If a speaker answers the question “How are you?” by repeating the same words back, there is
normally a change of tonicity. The answer has contrastive focus on “you”, therefore “you”attracts
the nucleus.
►If the verb “be” consists of more than one word (e.g. has been, will be), the nucleus goes on the
second of them. The same applies in the corresponding indirect questions
►If the word following “be” in a wh-question of this type is a demonstrative rather than a pronoun,
then the nucleus tends to go on the demonstrative:
►Intonational idioms:
Words such as “too, as well, either, anyway, anyhow” and synonyms attract the nucleus despite
being function words:
►Empty words (such as: things, people) do not attract the nucleus
40
►Pro-forms: “one” used as a pronoun: not accented. With plural or mass noun, the pro-form
corresponding to “one” is “some” or “any” . When used in this way, they are not accented either:
Some and any, Numerals, One, So, Do, There (not accented when used a a pro-form: “China? I´ve
always wanted to go there)
One is usually accented in the expressions the one, the right, wrong, first, last, only one, which
one.
Vocatives
☺ A vocative at the beginning of an utterance is accented, and normally has its own IP, thus
becoming nuclear. (it can be said with a fall or a fall-rise)
☺ We also accent a vocative when we want to indicate who we are talking to, perhaps when there
are other people within earshot: 'Hi, Peter!
☺ Final vocatives are generally not accented but attached to the preceding IP as (part of) the tail.
Even if a final vocative appears to include new information directed towards the known addressee,
it remains unaccented (or it may be uttered as a separate IP in low key (e.g. “you´ve missed it, you
fool”). We can, however, say them with a rise, to sound convergent.
When reporting clauses follow quoted words, they are usually out of focus. The nucleus goes on
the appropriate item among the quoted words, and the reporting clause forms a tail to the IP. There
is often a rhythmic break between the quoted words and the reporting clause.
Ex: 'How are you doing? , he asked. (rhythmic break + silent beat)
Reporting clauses bring about a problem of analysis. Rhythmically, the reporting clause may
indeed be separated from the preceding reported matter, so that it seems to be like a separate IP.
But tonally if we were to treat reporting a clause as a separate IP, we would have to say that the IP
was anomalous in having no nuclear tone. The reporting clause is intonationally tail-like: low after
a nuclear fall on “from” or continuing the rise after a nuclear rise after a nuclear rise or fall-rise on
“from”. Longer reporting clauses may need to be broken up into more than one IP. Any additional
nuclei copy the same nuclear tone as the tone on the quoted material. Exceptionally, where the
41
reporting clause is immediately followed by further material, it may have its own IP, usually with a
rise to indicate non- finality
Although adverbs in general are usually accented, adverbs and adverbial phrases of TIME and
PLACE are often not accented (=unfocused) when at the end of an IP, even if they contain new
information. They therefore form part of the tail. This does not apply in sentences where the sense
of the verb would be incomplete without the final adverbial. Such adverbials are typically in focus,
and therefore bear the nucleus. (ex: Put it on the table)
Alternatively, in statements, final adverbs and adverbials of time and place may bear the nucleus in
a separate IP, typically making with the preceding IP a fall-plus-rise pattern:
Naturally, there are also many cases in which final adverbs and adverbial phrases of time and
place are important to the message, and are therefore brought into focus and receive a nuclear
accent:
Ex: He´s got a tat 'too on his arm, │ 'not on his leg.
Adverbs of manner do tend to bear the nucleus if they are at the end of the clause.
There are several types of adverbs and adverbial phrases that- contrary to the general rule- do not
get accented when at the end of a clause. They remain out of focus. Like adverbs of place and
manner, they go in the tail, with the nucleus on some earlier word. We can divide them into two
lists:
42
Or thereabouts Or so, even
enough
In general the adverb “indeed” is accented (however , if used in a short response question with a
fall, to show surprise or annoyance, it is not accented : ex: “Quentin´s won a prize. Has he
indeed?
The word “again” is accented if used in its basic sense of “one more time”, since it is contrastive. It
is not accented when it means “back to a previous state”:
'This is how you close it │and ' this is how you open it again.
Etcetera and its synonymous (“approximatives”) (and so, and so forth, and whatnot, and stuff, and
things, and the like, and such like) are usually kept out of focus.
Phrasal verbs
Verb plus adverbial particle → verb + a particle, which may be an adverb. General rule: phrasal
verbs are lexically double-stressed. (exceptions: e.g.: 'pour down: It was 'really pouring down.)
Verb plus prepositional particle → verb + a particle which is clearly a preposition. Mostly
lexically single-stressed: primary stress on the verb. This is something I (ex: Are these the books I
∕sent for?)
►Certain constructions leave the preposition stranded. Typical cases are passivization, relative
clauses and wh-questions. The preposition then goes in the tail, although phonetically it retains its
strong form:
(exceptions: prepositions of more than one syllable tend to be stressed: look after, do without,
bump into)
43
►There are phrasal verbs that include both an adverbial particle and a preposition, e.g: go along
with, look down on. These are double stressed: go a long with
Adverb or preposition?
Adverbs and adverbial particles are usually accented, prepositions and prepositional particles are
not.
English has several words that can function both as prepositions and as adverbs (in, on , by). They
are typically accented when they are used as adverbs but not when used as prepositions. If they
are at the end of the clause, adverbs attract the nucleus, but prepositions repel it.
Ex: a) She sat in a comfortable chair. (preposition) vs She took in the information. (adverb)
1. constituents in each of the sentences: [She sat] [in a comfortable chair] vs [She took in]
[the information]
2. Replacement of the lexical NP by a pronoun: She sat in it. Vs She took it in.
3. Single-stressed vs double-stressed in final position: 'What did she sit in? Vs 'How much
did she take in?
Separated particles= extraposed particle
In the case of a lexical object and a separate particle, the nucleus goes by default on the object
(Take your shoes off). However, if the object is a pronoun (i.e. not lexical) the nucleus goes on the
adverbial particle in the regular way (Take them off). This also applies if the object is lexically filled
but is already given, and therefore out of focus.
The adverb may well not be accented after a lexically filled subject in sentences such as:
But inevitably accented after a pronoun: (talking about the television) 'Is it on?
Constructions and idiomatic expressions in which final verbs and adjectives tend to be deaccented:
44
Verb at the end of a sentence or clause. A final verb is usually deaccented and the nucleus goes
on the preceding noun
The same deaccenting applies to the final adjective in sentences such as: Ex: We are going to
get the table ready, and to the “up to” in: “I wonder what Eleanor is up to”.
HAVE or GET + noun + verb: nuclear accent on the noun (except when the NP is a pronoun or
empty word, so the nucleus goes on the verb
But
Event Sentences
These are sentences describing an event, misfortune, appearance or disappearance, where the
verb is intransitive. The nucleus tends to be located on the subject, provided it is lexically filled,
even if the verb contains apparently new information. Descriptions of the weather count as event
sentences. So do statements relating to unpleasant bodily sensations (My arm´s hurting). Possible
explanation: verb: predictable form the context.
a) 'Dogs must be carried. (if you have a dog with you, you must carry it)
Sometimes the echoed word, although repeated, nevertheless clearly conveys new information:
45
You say your name´s Smith? Yes/ Smith.
The first speaker asks the second for information. Supplying that information involves repeating a
word just used by the first speaker. Thus the same word is reused by the second speaker and the
information it conveys is new. So it has to be brought into focus.
►Reusing your own words: We can also repeat ourselves for emphasis giving the same
information more than once and presenting it afresh each time, focusing on it anew:
Idiomatic expressions in which repeated words get accented: more and more, hours and hours,
again and again, face to face, from day to day
Chapter 4
►Essentially the intonation structure reflects the grammatical structure. An intonation break (= the
boundary between two successive intonation phrases) generally corresponds to a syntactic
(=grammatical) boundary. We regularly place an intonation break between successive phrases,
and occasionally between successive words. We can even break a word
►In the case of lists, all the items tend to be treated equally: either none of them is followed by an
intonation break or they all are.(ex: the flags are 'red, white and blue. Vs. The 'flags are red, │
white │ and blue.
►Tonality in speech plays a role similar to the role of punctuation in writing. Intonation breaks
often correspond to punctuation marks. However, the two do not always go in parallel. There are
many cases where a punctuation mark is used, but an intonation break is optional or even unlikely.
46
In particular little words such as well, yes, no, oh at the beginning of a sentence, although set off by
a comma in writing, are not usually followed by an intonation break in speech:
►Some words such as then, though, even, you know, are preceded by a comma in writing but
have no intonation break in speech: ex. We´ll 'see you on Tuesday, then.
Each IP presents one piece of information. The speaker has to break the message up into chunks
of information – into IPs and has considerable freedom of choice to do so. Typically, an IP lasts for
between one and two seconds. The chunks also reflect the speaker´s decisions about focus. Each
IP covers a single focus domain (culminating in the nucleus) and the associated out-of-focus
material
Tonality varies considerably according to the style of speech. In some styles IPs may be very
short, with nearly every accent being nuclear.
Basic rules:
• There is normally an intonation break at every sentence boundary. This is a major break (//)
more important than the intonation break within a sentence.
• Each clause tends to be said as a separate IP: So if a sentence consists of several clauses,
there will usually be an intonation break at each clause boundary.
• If a new sentence involves a change of grammatical subject, the subject (particularly if it is
not a pronoun) tends to have its own IP (ex. We´ll ar 'rive at about ten. The children │
can 'come along later.
• There is usually an intonation break between coordinate clauses. If however the subject of
coordinate clauses is ellipted, there is usually no intonation break after the first verb,
providing the subject is unchanged:
47
I washed │ and ironed the clothes. (no ellipted object)
• Simple structures like the following count as a single clause, and are usually said as a
single IP:
I want to apologise. I think he is wrong. He said he was sorry. I hope you haven´t forgotten
►Vocatives are treated differently depending on where they come. In initial position, they tend to
form a separate IP. When not initial, they are usually attached to what precedes, forming part of
the tail of the IP.
►Imprecations (= calling on God) can form a separate utterance. But if part of a larger utterance,
they usually have their own IP when initial, but otherwise are attached to the preceding IP:
Adverbials
Their behaviour varies depending on their relationship to the rest of the clause.
►Ordinary adverbs and adverbials – those that modify the verb or an adjective – are typically not
given their own IP (ex: she quickly picked up the pencil). However, adverbials at the beginning of
the clause are usually followed by an intonation break and thus form a separate IP:
►In the middle of a clause, an adverbial is often a kind of parenthesis. It may have its own IP, with
separate IPs before and after:
48
Ex: She 'dances beautifully.
►Sentence adverbials (adjuncts) that modify the whole clause or sentence, typically have their
own IP. They are set off from the surrounding material by an intonation break
►A noun phrase (NP) is particularly likely to have its own IP if it is heavy (= long, consisting of
several words). A separate IP for the subject of a clause is more frequently found in reading aloud
and in formal speeches to an audience than in everyday conversation:
►The grammatical subject is particularly likely to have its own IP if it is different from the subject of
the preceding sentence or clause.
►Making the subject NP into a separate IP has the advantage of allowing the speaker to place
contrastive focus on it by locating a nucleus there. Even very light (=short) NPs can be made into
separate IPs to allow this:
The children │ say they don´t like her. │ But I │think she´s wonderful.
I gave the book you asked about │to the girl at the checkout.
Topics
►The first element in a clause is typically the topic (or “theme”), while the remainder is the
comment (or “rheme”). This first element is most often the grammatical subject. We can signal its
status as topic (a) in various syntactic ways, (b) by choosing an appropriate tone and (c) by giving
a separate IP
►We can also topicalize other clause elements. A topicalized object or complement is usually
followed by an intonation break:
His rudeness │ I shall ignore . │But his actions │I cannot for get.
►In cleft sentences one constituent of the sentence is fronted and introduced by it is (or it was,
etc). This topicalized constituent must include focused material and therefore and intonation
nucleus. If there further focused material to follow, then there must be an intonation break:
49
Ex: It was Ve ronica that I chose.
►Pseudo cleft sentences involve “what”. There is usually an intonation break between the two
halves of the construction:
►A defining relative clause does not have its own separate IP. A non-defining relative clause
does.
There may well be an intonation break after a defining relative clause when the NP is heavy.
►The distinction between defining and non-defining applies to certain other constructions as well:
Look at that house near the bus stop (=not the other houses)
Parallel structures
►The items in a list are sometimes separated by intonation breaks, sometimes not. This applies
more generally to parallel grammatical structures, including (a) parallel words or phrases, whether
or not coordinated, and (b) strings of letters or numbers.
►Some coordinated structures are potentially ambiguous, and can optionally be disambiguated by
the insertion of an intonation break. (ex: old men and women).
►With a string of letters or numbers there are equally two possibilities. If we think they will be
familiar to the hearer, or if there is no need to be particularly explicit, we run them together in the
same IP. If we think they may be unfamiliar to the hearer or need to be made especially clear, we
can make the message easier for the hearer to process by placing an intonation break after each
item.
B: S, E, I, Z, E.
Tag questions
►Tag questions tend to have their own IP. This applies particularly to reverse-polarity tags
(=checking tags).
►A tag is sometimes incorporated as a tail into the IP of the main clause, if it has a rising tone.
This does not happen with reverse-polarity tags after a fall:
►In constant polarity tags (=copy tags) it is quite usual for the tag not to have its own IP. If a
constant-polarity tag does have its own IP, the tone must be a rise.
Chapter 5
Prenuclear patterns
►If there are any accents before the nucleus, the first such accent (=the onset) constitutes the
beginning of the head of the intonation pattern.
►The syllables (if any) before the first accented syllable are called the prehead.
The reason we recognize prehead, head and nucleus as separate parts of the IP is that the
speaker makes separate tonal choices at each of these points. The choice of prehead tone (high or
low), the choice of head tone (high or low), and the choice of nuclear tone (rise, fall. Fall-rise) are
all independent of one another. There is, however, no such choice in the tail. The pitch
characteristics of the tail are determines by the choice of nuclear tone.
Simple heads
High falling [ ]
Low level [ ]
Low rising [ ]
Complex heads
51
►Complex high level head: successive accented syllables form a series of level steps, each one
lower pitched than the preceding. Any unaccented syllables are at the same pitch height as the
accented syllable they follow. For this reason this is sometimes known as a “stepping head”: You
'really must 'make up your mind.
►In a complex falling head, there is a series of falls, one from each accented syllable, and each
one starting at a slightly lower pitch than the preceding one. Each fall is spread over the accented
syllable and any unaccented syllables that may follow. The complex falling head is sometimes
known as a “sliding head”: You really must make up your mind.
►In a complex low rising head, there is a series of rises, one from each accented syllable. Each
may start at a slightly higher pitch than the preceding one; or each may start again at the same low
pitch. Each rise is spread over the accented syllable and any unaccented syllables that may follow.
The complex low rising head is sometimes known as the “climbing head”: You really must
make up your mind.
Preheads
►Preheads usually consist of nothing but lexically unstressed syllables. Sometimes, however, they
may include a syllable that is lexically stressed but that the speaker chooses not to accent- i.e. in a
word that the speaker chooses not to accent. Such syllables usually carry a rhythmic stress (beat).
Varieties of fall
►Three particular varieties of fall: a high fall, a low fall and a rise-fall. A rise-fall involves a complex
pitch movement, starting with a rise from a mid pitch to a high pitch and then a fall from high to low,
finishing on a low pitch. The most prominent part is the initial rise. If there is a tail, the rise-fall pitch
movement is spread over the nuclear syllable and the first or first two syllables of the tail.
All falling nuclear tones finish low; the final tendency in the tail after a falling nuclear tone is always
low level
52
►The difference of tone meaning between the high fall and low fall is the degree of emotional
involvement. The high fall implies greater interest on the part of the speaker, greater excitement,
greater passion, more involvement. The low fall implies relative lack of interest, less excitement, a
dispassionate attitude, less involvement. The higher the starting point of a simple fall, the greater
the degree of emotional involvement; the lower the starting point, the less the emotional
involvement.
►Rise-fall: 2 meanings a) the speaker is impressed. This tone meaning is found with statements,
exclamations and yes-no question, but not with wh-questions or commands. This meaning is
sometimes reinforced by using a breathy voice. The result can sound gossipy (ex: A: She 'came
top of the class. B: Did she just!
With commands, the rise-fall suggests that the speaker refuses responsibility, refuses to be
involved (ex: A: What colour shall I choose? B: Please your self.)
Varieties of fall-rise
►High fall-rise (tone meaning similar to the high rise. It is used with questions (particularly echo
questions), where it adds animation or intensity and has the effect of making the question more
specific. It can suggest astonishment:
Varieties of rise
►High rise: checking, pardon questions, echo questions. Also tone for uptalk statements.
►Wide rise: combines the special characteristics of the low rise and the high rise, since it has a
rising pitch movement that starts from a low pitch and moves to a high pitch. It is associated with
the non-solidarity of indignant or truculent disagreement
53
In the case of yes-no question, the differences between these possibilities are subtle. A high rise
signals informality; a low rise signals polite interest (at least British English; Americans may
perceive it as patronizing); a wide rise adds a note of surprise:
Both the low rise and the high rise can be used as leading dependent tones.
The difference in tone meaning between these four tone varieties is not great. Sometimes
speakers just seem to ring the changes between them to avoid repetition. The low rise is perhaps
more formal, more oratorical; the high rise is more casual; and the mid level has no special tone
meaning except non- finality: The fall-rise may factor in its usual meaning of implication or contrast.
American English differs from British English in making little or no use of the low rise as leading
dependent tone – one of the reasons, perhaps, that British English may strike Americans as stuffy
and formal.
►With an independent rise, the choice of prenuclear pattern may affect the tone meaning. In a
statement or command, a high head or prehead with a low rise creates a soothing and reassuring
effect: ex: 'Don´t worry.
►A low level head before a rise, on the other hand, may sound defensive, grudging or generally
non-supportive: ex: A: The top´s fallen off. B: I don´t suppose it matters.
►With yes-no questions, a high head plus low rise is the usual one in RP and similar kinds of
British English. To Americans it sounds formal. A low prenuclear pattern plus high rise suggest
informality.
Non-nuclear accenting
54
►Any lexically stressed syllables that do not receive an accent may bear a rhythmic stress.
►Downgrading principle (deaccent): it is sometimes known as the rule of three, since it weakens
the middle accent or stress of three. It applies in strings of letter names or numbers:
A, B, C
►With longer strings of potential accents, the speaker has considerable freedom over which ones
to downgrade. But the onset accent and the nuclear accent are always unaffected.
Ex: She´s 'aged six teen. Vs. There were 'sixteen people there.
Unimportant words at the beginning: there are various words and phrases that are regularly left
unaccentednat the beginning of an utterance. That is to say, they form part of the prehead. This
applies, obviously, to function words such as articles, pronouns, conjunctions and modal or
auxiliary verbs:
• interrogative wh- words (relative wh-words, however are not usually accented: “When
planning a meal, first you…”)
• Demonstratives are accented to draw attention to a new topic (the demonstrative “that” is
readily accented, but the conjunction “that” is not)
• As an adverb of place, initial “there” is accented. Ex: 'There he sat. As a pronoun
(“existential there”) it is not.
55
• In yes-no questions, accentuation of an initial auxiliary or modal verb is optional.
• The modals “ought, used, need, dare” are usually accented even in statements.
• An initial contracted negative verb is almost always accented, so too is the word “not”.
• We often accent auxiliaries and modals if by doing so we avoid an awkwardly long prehead
(They are going to be late for work)
• Other modals that are usually accented include “may, might, could” in most cases. “Must” is
accented to express an inference, but usually not when it expresses obligation
• Pronouns are accented not only when contrastive, but also when coordinated to signal a
change of grammatical subject or object.
• When you is used as an overt subject of an imperative, it is implicitly contrastive and
therefore accented.
• The words “even” and “only” are accented when they point forward to the item that bears
the nucleus (when used as a synonym of “but”, “only” is not accented.
• Prepositions and subordinating conjunctions (if, when, although) can go either way. If they
have considerable semantic content, or are polysyllabic, then they may well be accented;
otherwise they are not accented.
• When prepositions or other grammatical items are coordinated they are usually accented
(although the second accent may then be downgraded)
Further considerations
Stylization: As well as the intonation patterns described so far, English also has a few stylized
patterns. These patterns are used rather rarely, and their pitch and rhythmic characteristics differ
from ordinary patterns. They are used for short utterenaces in circumstances that are routine and
predictable, typically to repeat something you have said many times before.
The stylized high-mid pattern consists of a high level pitch followed by a mid level pitch: Jim my!
56
Discourse Intonation and Language Teaching - David Brazil, Malcolm Coulhard and
Catherine Johns
• Pitch (David Crystal: the attribute of auditory sensation in terms of which a sound may be
ordered on a scale from “low” to “high”. Physiologically, pitch is primarily dependent on the
rate of vibration of the vocal cords within the larynx. As a perceptual term, it relates to the
listeners´ judgements as to whether a sound is “high” or “low”.
Any vibrating object emits a sound or note whose perceived pitch can be for all practical purposes
be regarded as directly related to the frequency of the vibrations; for instance, vocal cords, piano
wires or anything vibrating 262 times or cycles a second will produce the sound we hear as middle
C. The variation in pitch of the voice is achieved by tightening and slackening the muscles to alter
both the length and the thickness of the vocal cords and thereby the frequency at which they
vibrate Men´s vocal cords are heavier and appreciably longer than women´s and thus vibrate on
average more slowly and produce a range of pitches roughly an octave lower
• Stress: degree of force used in producing a syllable. From the speaker´s point of view, it is
the pushing out of more air from the lungs. The listener perceives this as an increase in
loudness. Stressed syllables tend to be longer- a fact additionally emphasized in English by
the tendency to change the vowel quality of unstressed syllables towards schwa.
The TONE GROUP (= Tone Unit): the phonological unit set up to handle the structure of
tunes. It can be usefully analysed into four separate components:
57
The Nucleus is the label widely given to the syllable on which the major pitch movement
begins. The tail carries the continuation and completion of the pitch movement begun in the
nucleus.
The head of the tone unit consists of the stretch of utterance from the first stressed syllable,
the onset, up to but not including the nuclear syllable. The prehead comprises all other
unstressed syllables.
The approach outlined in this book: the description of intonation is seen as one aspect of
the description of interaction. Intonation choices carry information about the structure of the
interaction, the relationship between and the discourse function of individual utterances, the
interactional “given-ness” and “newness” of information and the state of convergence and
divergence of the participants.
Discourse Intonation
Transactional Interactional
↕ ↕
Tone: the major pitch movement, the distinctive pitch level of a syllable.
58
Some parts of what the speaker says merely make reference to features which he takes to
be already present in the interpenetrating worlds of speaker and hearer (referring tone).
Others add new information to the message (proclaiming tone). Decisions about what
information to proclaim and what to refer to are a speaker´s constant concern and are
made in the light of the moment by moment assessment of the state of the play. Tone
choice depends solely on the speaker´s assessment and not on any real world truth.
Key:
There are three factors which affect the pitch range exploited by a particular speaker on a
particular occasion:
a) Idiosyncratic: the nature of an individual´s vocal cords determines the absolute limits of
his pitch range within which he exploits a much narrower range for speaking.
However much the speaker widens or narrows the pitch range, he will still exploit the
same small number of pitch contrasts to convey linguistic meaning. English speakers
only exploit linguistically three pitch contrasts: high, mid and low. There are no absolute
values for high, mid and low key, even for a particular speaker; in fact a given high key
tone unit may well be lower than an earlier mid key tone unit. It is the pitch level of one
crucial syllable, relative to the height of the same crucial syllable in the preceding tone
unit, which is significant.
For each and every tone group the speaker must select high, mid or low key.
►By choosing high key, the speaker marks the matter of the tone unit as contrastive:
High BOGnor
Low
In addition to the selected key, the speaker must also mark the tone unit as proclaiming
or referring;that is to say as new information or information that belongs to common
ground.
59
a) to mark an equivalent relationship which he assumes or asserts exists, in a given
conversational context, between items in successive tone units:
High eVENtually
Here the speaker chooses low key to mark the matter of the two tone units as
existentially equivalent- low key conveys the information that the speaker has only one
set of neighbours, that “the Robinsons” and “our neighbours” are, in this context,
synonymous.
High
Low ILL
One statement is implied by the other – the two are existentially equivalent for the
speaker at this time
High WIMbledon
Low
High
Mid B: // p no PLAY // p //
High
Low
*** Analyse this sentence said with high key and low key .
The choice of tone can also carry the more general social meanings of convergence
/divergence, or solidarity/separateness. By choosing proclaiming tone, the speaker places himself
outside the area of convergence: he is heard to be reserving his position in some general way or
60
perhaps staking a proprietory claim to the view expressed in the ensuing discourse, or simply
emphasizing the likely lack of agreement on a point:
They are variants of the referring and proclaiming tones respectively. In all situations there are
social rules which determine who speaks when and to whom, and in many situations speaking
rights are distributed differentially. The term dominant is proposed in a technical sense to indicate
the person who has the greater freedom in making linguistic choices, and it is argued that the
freedom to make choices in the r/r+ system is only available to a dominat speaker, or, as ever, to
one who is using the system to claim dominance.
Ex: (patient to doctor) p WELL // r i´ve come to SEE you // p with the RASH // r i´ve GOT on my
CHIN //…
// r+ i´ve come to SEE you // p with the RASH // r+ i´ve GOT on my CHIN //…
By choosing p tone, the speaker presents the information as new to the hearer.
// p it´s RAINing//
By choosing p+ tone, the speaker signal that he is simultaneously adding information to the
common ground but also to his own store of knowledge. In this case, the information is doubly
new: both to the hearer and to the speaker.
Below are a number of pairs of questions and after each pair is a transcribed item.
To which of the two questions is the transcribed item the more probable answer? Then alter the
responses to make them fit the other question in each pair, by simply changing tones, from ╲ to v ,
or from v to ╲ .
a) (1) Which book are you going to read? (2) Who´s going to read this book?
61
b) (1) When do you eat? (2) What will you do after the meeting?
c) (1) What have you got planned for tomorrow? (2) When do you do your shopping?
d) (1) Is Tom the younger of the brothers? (2) Is John older than Tom?
e) (1) Are both John´s parents teachers? (2) Is it John´s father who works at the hospital?
f) (1) Is that the way to the station? (2) Is the post office down that street?
62
The Intonation Systems of English by Paul Tench
Intonation: the linguistic use of pitch in utterances (by saying “linguistic” we hope to avoid
reference to other cases of pitch such as singing, and to the subjective , aesthetic evaluations
about how “nice” and “pleasant” and accent´s intonation is.
Intonation has two dimensions → linguistic: concerns the message itself: how many pieces of
information there are; what information is new; whether the message is complete or incomplete;
whether the speaker is telling you something or asking, or whether the speaker is turning to a new
topic or finishing off an old one.
If a language uses pitch variation to differentiate between words, we call that language a tone
language.
English is not a tone language. Although the differences of pitch can fall on a single word in
English, it is a distinctly different matter from the lexical tone of a tone language.
Tonality: the system by which a stretch of spoken text is segmented into a series of discrete units
of intonation which correspond to the speaker´s perception of pieces (or “chunks”) of information.
Tonicity: the system by which an individual, discrete unit of information is shown to have a
prominent word which indicates the focus of information.
Tone: the system of contrasting pitch movements in each unit of intonation, which, among other
roles, identifies the status of the information, e.g. major, minor or incomplete.
►The description of the intonation in terms of systems shows that intonation is as systematic as
other parts of the phonology. And because it is systematic, it can be presented in terms of
differences in meaning and that in turn implies that intonation can be taught and learnt.
►Each language has its own system of intonation. It seems inevitable that each language will
manifest tonality, tonicity and tones, but the way they do so will vary considerably.
63
Functions of intonation:
3. Attitudinal function → considered the primary function of intonation in the older , more
traditional studies of intonation.
4. Syntactic → relates intonation to the syntax of clauses (compare: she washed /and brushed her
hair. Vs She washed and brushed her hair. He asked himself vs. He asked himself)
5. Textual → this function concerns longer structures than single units of intonation and individual
pieces of information. An intonation unit and a piece of information do not usually appear in
isolation. The topic of the message is one factor that binds the intonation together; another is the
grammatical systems of reference and conjunction that show that clauses and sentences belong
together; and a third factor is intonation (phonological paragraphing)
Tonality:
Tonality is the system in intonation that divides spoken discourse into its separate individual
intonation units. The whole text is composed of many intonation units, each bearing a single piece
of information and representing the speaker´s management of the information of the whole
message. Each intonation unit contains one piece of information- as the speaker perceives it.
Neutral tonality: very useful starting point: one-to-one correspondence between clause, intonation
phrase and unit of information. (two main reasons for individual diversity: one is the imagined
speed of delivery: the slower the pace, the shorter the unit; and the second is people´s perception
of the organization of information)
64
Marked tonality: intonation units do not coincide with whole clauses: two or more clauses fit into a
single intonation unit (He did. I saw it), or two or more intonation units are needed to cover a single
clause.
I´m going to town this morning. (one clause, one IP) or I´m going to town │ this morning. (one
clause, two IPs)
The train arriving at platform two │is the ten twenty from Swansea to London Paddington.
►Lists: a special kind of long clause. Each item may be contained in a separate unit.
We want red, white and blue flags, please (neutral tonality since the intonation unit contains the
whole clause)
►Marked theme: the theme – the starting-point of the message is not the subject (neutral theme)
►Adjuncts:
Adjuncts that affect tonality include linking adjuncts like: however, nevertheless, perhaps, of
course, unfortunately, etc.
►Tags→ checking tags (two clauses, two intonation units: neutral tonality)
→ copy tags (may not necessarily require their own , separate IP; in that case: marked
tonality.
65
They lost against Newport of all teams │ didn´t they. (degree of uncertainty)
They lost against Newport of all teams │ did they. (sudden realization of the significance of the
information)
They lost against Newport of all teams did they. (with a touch of irritation)
♦Apposition →compare: *Tom Jones │the singer │ comes form South Wales.
♦Verb phrases → He came to hear about it. (involves a complex verbal phrase as an idiom
meaning “it happened by chance that...), but because it constitutes only one verbal phrase, it is the
predicate of only one clause, hence only one intonation unit
♦Negative domain:
♦Report clauses:
He said he would come. (it is best to regard the two parts as a single clause consisting of the main
verb of “reporting” and a clause as the direct object complementing the “reporting” verb. A single
intonation unit covers the whole sentence
66
Note the difference:
Tell him │to save time.(main clause followed by a dependent “purpose clause”)
Clause complements:
• there is a perceivable pitch change at some point following a tonic syllable; either a
stepping up after a falling tone, or a stepping down after a rise; if the pitch of the tonic
syllable was level, then either a stepping up or stepping down would signal the start of a
new intonation unit
• There is either a very slight pause or a change of pace in the flow of syllables; syllables at
the end of a unit tend to be relatively slower, but syllables at the beginning of a unit have a
tendency to speed up
Tonicity
The tonic syllable is that syllable in a given intonation unit which is made most prominent by a
combination of pitch, volume and length. The function of the tonic is to form the focus of
information: to express what the speaker decides to make the main point or burden of the message
(Halliday). The tonic represents the focus of each unit of information
Neutral tonicity → tonic syllable within the last lexical item in the intonation unit
Narrow focus →the focus is on only part of the information in the intonation unit
67
Marked tonicity → takes two forms:
►Event sentences
• Certain verbs require adjuncts of place as their complements; verbs like put, send require a
locative adjunct to complete their sense. Thus a distinction need to be kept between
locatives as essential complements to verbs – they have got to be there- and locatives that
add circumstantial information
Compare:
• Adjuncts of time and place: they do not normally take the tonic if they belong to closed
systems like here, there; now, then; yesterday, today, tomorrow unless a contrast is
intended.
• Another class of adjuncts that acts in a similar “non-tonic” way is the “comment adjuncts”
(though, of course, you know, at least).
• Other adjuncts, like adjuncts of manner, are lexical, i.e. they belong to open sets, and if
they occur in final position are likely to take the tonic.
• Vocatives, final glosses (ex: He shouldn´t have done it the fool) or direct speech markers
can form an extra intonation unit. The effect is to give a little more prominence to the item
concerned.
• Some adjuncts can have two different functions, and the different functions follow different
tonicity patterns. An adjunct like “normally” or “frankly” can be used as an adjunct of
68
manner, and if it comes at the end of its clause, as in the following examples, it would tend
to be accompanied by the tonic:
He drives normally (=in a normal manner)
Tonicity by default
Neutral tonicity indicates broad focus where all the information in the unit is new, but it may
indicate narrow focus if that which is new information is found at the end of the unit. Marked tonicity
indicates new information that is either grammatical or not final
• In the cases of echoes and repetitions, the intonation unit only consists of given
information, but the units are treated as if they contain the same information structure of given and
new as the original (ex: A: is this your handbag? B: Sorry? A: is this your handbag?
• Insists (A: Why have you invited the Robinsons? B: But I haven´t invited them.) The focus
in the insists usually correlates with the given information in the original , because it is the given
information in the original that is the basis of A´s presupposition.
Compare:
He asked himself
He asked himself
Shoot John
Shoot John
69
Tone
Tone refers to pitch movements, and that observation which has frequently been commented on –
‘that it is not what they said, but the way they said it’ – relies primarily upon the pitch movements.
Tone has been defined hitherto as the contrastive pitch movement on the tonic syllable; so, We
make a distinction between primary and secondary tones. Primary tones are the basic contrastive
pitch movements on the tonic, i.e. whether the pitch of the voice moves up (rises), or moves down
(falls), or combines a movement of down and then up (fall-rises). Secondary tones are the finer
distinctions of the primary tones, i.e. the degree to which the pitch of the voice rises, falls or
combines a fall and a rise – whether there is, for example, a rise to a high pitch. or a mid pitch, or a
fall from a mid pitch or a high pitch, etc. Secondary tones also cover the pitch movements in the
pre-tonic segment (the head and the pre-head). (Pitch movements in the tail are not considered
separately from the pitch on the tonic because they are an extension to the tone itself.) The
primary tones of English, i.e. the fall, the rise and the fall-rise. The primary tones of English, i.e. the
fall, the rise and the fall-rise, function, like tonality and tonicity, in the organization of information,
but they also feature, like the other systems, in a second function. Wl Whereas tonality and tonicity
produce contrasts in grammar, the tone system produces contrasts in the communicative, or
illocutionary function; that is. They help to indicate differences between telling and asking, between
commanding and requesting, between congratulating and wishing, and a whole host of similar
functions that language is used to fulfil.
The secondary tones do not function, as such, in the organization of information – or, for that
matter, in either grammar or the communicative functions. Their role is in the expression of
attitudes.
Pitch is a relative matter. The actual range of pitch of most adults is lower than that of children; and
again the actual range of pitch of men is, on average, lower than that of women.
Falls: Neutral Fall (from mid-high to low): ‘plain statement’. It is the pitch you would expect if no
other meaning or connotation was intended. It is indicated by a downward-pointing line before the
syllable concerned.
There are ‘primary’ functions associated with the fall, but ‘secondary’ (High Fall or Low Fall) –
attitudinal – functions associated with the alternative degrees of fall.
Rises: the Neutral Rise (from Low to mid or mid-high) sounds a little like a query, but it can also
indicate something like ‘This is not all I want to say; I’m going to continue’( “incomplete
information”)
70
The High Rise is commonly associated with a stronger sense of querying, suggesting surprise or
even disbelief
The Low Rise (from low to mid-low) suggests a non-committal, or even grumbling attitude.
Fall-Rise
A common sequence of tones in a pair of intonation units is a rise in the first, to indicate
incompleteness, followed by a fall in the second, to indicate completeness
Another common sequence of tones in a pair of intonation is the reverse: a fall in the first and a
rise in the second. In this case, the rise cannot mean incomplete, because it does in fact come at
the end. This sequence is what Halliday termed major and minor information. The main piece of
information is contained in the first unit, and the second unit contains an extra piece of information.
So we cannot label the Fall, therefore, as necessarily indicating complete information, since that is
not the case in the example above.
Mid-level → only occurs in a non-final unit of intonation; and it is used as an alternative to the true
rise to indicate incomplete information.
A rise, the, indicates either incomplete or minor information, depending on whether it precedes or
follows the fall.
A final adjunct can either be contained in the intonation unit of the clause preceding it, or it can
have its own unit. In the latter case, the adjunct will typically have a rise, as minor information:
71
Difference between a single fall-rise tone and a fall-plus-rise sequence:
• If the pitch movement is contained in a single syllable, it is a single tone, not a sequence of
two.
• If the pitch movement has a rise on a final unstressed syllable, it is the single fall-rise,
because the rise in the fall-plus-rise sequence would have to co-occur with a tonic.
• The rise component in the single fall-rise ends at a higher level.
• The fall-rise tone relates to one piece of information in one intonation unit, whereas the fall-
plus-rise sequence relates to two pieces of information in two intonation units.
• The fall-plus-rise sequence lacks the implicational meaning of the single fall-rise
• When a fall-rise tone precedes a fall in a close sequence of two units, it comes as a
contrast with the ordinary rise. Compare:
In the kitchen / you´ll find a surprise. (the rise leads on to major information)
In the kitchen / you´ll find a sur prise. (the fall-rise highlights the theme)
Use of fall-rise in final position: there is a “but” about it (Halliday) : reservation, contrast,
personal opinion offered for consideration, and concession
Analyse:
I didn´t come │ because he told me. Vs. I didn´t come because he told me.
They don´t admit any students. Vs. They don´t admit any students.
The tone system is used in English not only for the indication of information status but for
another purpose, the indication of communicative functions. By ‘communicative function’ we
mean the purpose for a given utterance intended by the speaker. For instance, is the speaker
72
telling me something or asking me? Are they commanding me or requesting me to do
something? The tone system is a guide, in a general sense, to such communicative functions.
Can you tell when a rise, for instance, is indicating information status and when it is indicating a
communicative function? The phonetic answer is easy in theory, but sometimes difficult in
practice: a rise is operating in its informational capacity when it belongs to an intonation unit
that is dependent upon another. When indicating either incomplete or minor information, its
intonation unit is tied very closely – often with no pause – to another unit; such a unit acts as a
kind of satellite to the other. The same is true of the non final fall-rise indicating the highlighting
of a theme; it is closely attached to another unit without pause. However, when a rise is
operating in its communicative capacity, it is operating in an independent intonation unit, which
is often separated from preceding and following units by a pause or even silence, or, of course,
by a change of speaker. Furthermore, the rise for information status is usually confined to
neutral rise, from low to mid; whereas the rise for communicative functions not only uses a
neutral rise – and in that respect is wholly indistinguishable from its use in information status –
it does have the potential for high and low variations to add an attitudinal dimension to the
questions (or whatever).
►Falling tones are associated not with satellite, dependent, units of intonation, but with the
more central, nuclear, independent, units. This befits its designation of bearing major
information. This particular status of the fall is bolstered by the observation that usually
between half and two-thirds of all tones are falls.
The fall is associated with the speaker knowing something, telling something and in the case of
interjections, expressing their own feelings: the speaker´s knowledge, authority and feelings
dominate.
►The rise is associated with the speaker not knowing and therefore having to ask.
►A fall indicates the speaker´s certainty or dominance in respect of knowledge, authority and
feelings
► a rise indicates the speaker´s uncertainty or deference to the knowledge of the person
addressed.
Here is no special tone for each separate communicative function; the resources of intonation
are simply not sufficient for so many different functions. The tone system simply indicates the
speaker’s status vis-à -vis the hearer: either as dominant or deferent.
73
All the communicative functions can be grouped into three kinds: relating to knowledge in
respect to a) information, reality and belief; b) authority in respect to influencing other people´s
action (suasion); and c) social interaction. We will now consider each of these three groups of
communicative functions and show how the tone system operates to indicate dominance and
deference.
Following functions with a falling tone: statements, answers, explanations, reports, agreement,
acknowledgement, descriptions, suppositions, hypotheses, deductions. They all presuppose
the speaker´s dominance in knowing and telling. Other communicative functions that display
the speaker´s knowledge and therefore are accompanied by a fall are denials, affirmations adn
of course, disagreements.
The functions with an accompanying rise are yes/no questions, appeals and requests, which
are functions in which the speaker acknowledges knowledge, and authority, in the addressee.
Concession: is a kind of deference to somebody else´s proposition: rising tone: It´s possible
(You are conceding to your addressee that the proposition he made might be valid.
Challenges → also regularly take a rising tone (generally to a high level) (Your father´s
travelling to Timbuk tu)
Echo questions → rise: A: You´re going to meet Jack. B: I´m going to meet him?
Denials → fall (have I ever let you down: I´ve never let you down)
Leads-in → fall (preliminary utterance to telling a joke: Have you heard the joke about the two
poli ticians?)
Repeat question → fall: A: Do you want a cup of tea? B: Pardon? A: Do you want a cup of
tea?
74
b) Suasion: authority in respect to influencing people´s actions.
→ fall: indicates speaker´s authority: Turn the radio off.
→ rise: indicates deference to the other person´s authority or decision: 'Turn the
radio off.
Advice and recommendations → fall (because they contain a degree of authority): You should
take a little break
Compare: 'How about a cup of coffee? (recommendation); 'How about a cup of coffee?
(invitation)
Appeals → rise: A: I´ll have to throw this coffee away. B: You don´t have to do that.
c) Social exchanges
The third category of communicative functions is social exchanges. These kinds of
communication do not involve either the provision or elicitation of information, nor are they
primarily intended to influence other people’s action; they are simply intended to establish and
maintain relationships between people; for instance, greeting and bidding farewell,
introductions, attracting attention, apologizing, sympathizing, wishing, thanking, and so on.
Once again, some of these functions seem naturally to require a fall, others a rise, and yet
others either.
Farewells → typically accompanied by a rising tone: 'Good bye. 'See you a gain. (with a
fall, it will sound very much like a dismissal in which the speaker´s feelings dominate)
75
Good wishes → rise: 'Happy birthday. 'Have a nice time.
Praise, appreciation, approval and disapproval → fall: That´s great. It´s a lovely thought.
You 'shouldn´t have gone to so much trouble
→fall: sounds more like summons, in which a speaker´s feelings or sense of authority
dominate.
76
Focus Minus Expressions
Focus Minus Expressions: group of words, phrases and expressions that will not normally
attract accent because they are semantically predictable, old information or do not contribute
significant information to the context.
1. Cohesion markers: They comprise expressions which make explicit the logical relation
between the proposition they are appended to and its context.
a. Additive: really, actually, as a matter of fact, in fact…
b. Concessive: though, if possible…
c. Contrastive: on the other hand, for a change…
d. Inferential: then, so…then, of course…
e. Reinforcing: of course, thanks, if necessary, for example…
2. Hearer-appeal markers: This class comprises items that are used by the speaker to appeal to
the hearer. Most of them serve to enhance the solidarity the speaker
intends to establish with the hearer, some are ‘challenging’ and as
such have a distancing effect.
a. Vocatives: (in final position)…Bob, …woman, …you two…
b. Softeners: I mean, I’m afraid, you know, please, mind you…
c. Tags (copy tags, and tags of reverse polarity in which the speaker is sure about
what he is saying)
3. Approximatives: They comprise expressions that indicate the approximate nature of the
proposition they are appended to. E.g.: …or something, …or two,…
in a way …and so forth…
4. Time-space markers: They establish a link between the proposition they are appended to and
the outside world. There is some previous reference (tenses, pronouns
etc). E.g.: now, this morning, yesterday, anywhere, here, there…
77
1
A. Match the focus minus expressions in the examples on the left to the type of expressions
on the right.
B. Work with tonality and tonicity in the 10 examples.
C. Read out the examples.
Example Type
78
FOCUS: identify the different nuclei according to the context
What´s happened?
Did your mother make a cheese cake? My mother made a chocolate cake
How much money did your wife spend on clothes? My wife spent $2,000 on clothes
79
COMPOUNDS: BRIEF OUTLINE:
Single-stressed compounds
1. Doer of the action: taxi driver, book seller, tin opener, hair dryer, goal keeper, food processor,
fortune teller, dressmaker
2. Kind of: current account, headache, teaspoon, hand bag, bookshop, credit card, post office,
question tag, punctuation mark
3. adj + noun: long established compound: high school, blackboard, grandfather, dark room
4. ing (GERUND): boarding school, running shoes, sleeping bag, steering wheel.
5. Noun + ing: Noun is the object of the verb: bookkeeping, time consuming, role playing, time
saving
1. Place or time: country house, winter holidays, kitchen sink, evening meal. (cf: Christmas
Day, Christmas Eve BUT Christmas present /card)
2. 2nd N made of the first: apple sauce, fruit salad, lemon pie, olive oil (exceptions: juice, cake,
bread, paper, milk, water)
3. 2 Nouns 2 referents: baby boy, woman doctor, twin sister, student teacher, woman driver)
4. Ing (PRESENT PARTICIPLE): running water, developing countries. working class
5. Proper names, Place names or names of institutions: Hyde Park, Times Square, Park
Avenue , River Thames, King´s College (exceptions: street, gate, high school, secondary
school, Pizza Hut)
6. Titles: James I, William the Conqueror.
7. Colours; navy blue, bottle green
8. Acronyms: USA, FBI
80
COMPOUNDS (notes compiled from Word and Sentence Stress
OrtizLira
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
Exceptions to LLI (compiled from Stress in the Speech Stream)
94
95
96
97
98
99
Exception to LLI Rule (notes complied from Word and Sentence Stress – ortiz Lira)
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
LISTENING PRACTICE (compiled from English pronunciation in Use – Intermediate – Mark
Hancock)
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
LISTENING PRACTICE (compiled from English pronunciation in Use – Martin Hewings
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
DRILLS
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
TEXTS AND DIALOGUES FOR READING PRACTICE
(Dialogues compiled from:Bradford, B. (2005) Intonation in context. Cambridge: CUP and Streamline
Departures)
1) Doctor So wanso
Doctor So wanso is the Secretary General of the U 'nited Nations. He´s one of the 'busiest
men in the world. He´s just a>rrived at New Delhi Airport now. The Indian Prime
Minister is meeting him. Later they´ll talk about Asian problems.
Yesterday he was in Moscow. He visited the Kremlin and had 'lunch with Soviet leaders.
During lunch they dis'cussed international politics.
To morrow he´ll 'fly to Nai robi. He´ll meet the President of Kenya and other 'African
leaders. He´ll be there for 'twelve hours.
Theday after to morrow he´ll be in London. He´ll meet the 'British Prime >Minister and they´ll
'talk about European eco nomic problems
Next week he´ll be 'back at the United Nations in New York. Next Monday he´ll speak to
the General A ssembly about his 'world tour. 'Then he´ll need a short holiday.
2) An Interview
Robin Knight, the television re/porter, is /interviewing the Duchess of Wessex for the
D: No, of course I don’t get up at that time. I have breakfast in /bed, and I read ‘The Times’.
D: I get up at ten.
R: …and /then?
R: Thank you, Duchess … you certainly have a /busy and /interesting life! #
3) Mrs Newell
Mrs. /Newell has gone to see the doctor and is discussing her /problem with him.
D: I see. /Here?
Mrs. N: Well – the usual /honey and hot /lemon. And /then I bought some cough
syrup.
D: Did it /help?
168
4) Jack Marsden
Jack /Marsden has arranged to /see his bank manager because he wants to >borrow
Jack: That’s /right. You >see, I want to raise enough /money for a de/posit on a
smallflat.
Jack: Yes. I’ve /seen about that. You >see, I’ve got a secure /job with a good salary.
B.M: Is the flat for yourself? Will you be living there alone?
Jack: Yes. For the /moment anyway. Why? Does that make any difference to the /loan?
Jack: Do you need to know anything /else? I’ve brought my /contract with details of my
/salary.
B.M: Good. Yes, fine. And have you any se/curities? Shares in any /companies?
5) Samantha
Lisa: That was Samantha on the phone. /Honestly, I don’t know how she does it.
L: Nothing, really. That’s what’s amazing. But /somebody has >sent her a dozen roses.
J: A dozen /what?
L: A dozen roses.
169
L: Yes. I know what you’re going to /say.
J: Poor /guy? What do you mean? He doesn’t sound very poor to /me if he can afford
a dozen roses.
L: No, I mean I feel /sorry for him. He’s in love with /her – yes. But /she’s not in love
with him.
L: She doesn’t even know who it is – and she says she doesn’t really mind. She always
>manages …#
6) Holidays
Mike: \Sue, | \there you are. || 'Have you got >time | for a little /chat? |
Mike: >Well, | you re'member Miˎguel | —who ˎstayed with us | last ˎsummer? |
Sue: \/No, | but I re'member you \telling me a/bout him. || He was 'very keen on \sightseeing, |
/wasn't he? |
Mike: 'That's ˎright. || >Well, | I ⬊got a \/letter from him | this \morning | and he's in\/vited me | to
spend a \/holiday | with his \family this year. || They've got a \/villa | in I\biza | >and | they've got a
\boat. || You 'know how \crazy I am about boats. |
170
Mike: It's ˎCelia. || You >see, | we've 'both been saving up like \mad | to go on a 'trip to\gether this
year. |
Mike: 'That's \right. || She'd be \so upset. || She's been 'doing all \sorts | of \jobs in her free time. ||
Sue: ˎLook, Mike. || \/First, | you must tell \/Celia | about your invi\tation. || She'll a⬊ppreciate your
\/problem. || But ⬊why don't you tell Mi\/guel | about the ⬈plans you had already made with \Celia.
John: Hey, look. | There`s Tony. |He looks a bit fed up. | ˅Tony.| Hey,| Tony.
171
Tony: Oh, | ¯hello, John.| >Pat!| I thought you were in Paris.
John: How are you Tony? |And how`s your precious sports car?
Tony: Don’t talk to me about it.| I wish l’d never seen it!
Tony: Oh,| yes, | I ˅was.| It was just what I’d dreamed of.| A bit old, | but in marvellous
con˅dition.| >And |it was very cheap.
John: EɅxactly.| I reɅmember. | I told you | at the time| I was a bit su˅spicious.| But you didn’t
listen.
Pat: You were lucky, Tony. | ˅Last time I saw you,| you said it was so reliable.
Tony: >Well, | yes. |It seemed reliable.| For a month or so. | But since ˅then | it’s been at the
˅garage| more than on the road.
Tony: At the Central Garage.| I’m hoping to pick it >up | first thing tomorrow.
Tony: ˅Actually, |they’re not. |But they’re not efficient,| either!| It was sup˅posed | to be ready
on Monday.
Pat: ˅Monday! |But tomorrow’s Thursday. | ¯Come on, | ¯cheer up, Tony.| you’ll have it for
the wee˅kend.
Tony: Not neces˅sarily. | Every time I ˅go |or ˅ring |they’ve found something else that needs
doing.| ˅Last weekend | was bad enough without it. |But ˅this weekend |is really important.
172
9) A tour guide in Prague is explaining to tourists what they can see on their trip down the Vitava
River. Here is part of what she says:
On your ˅right | we’re passing the ˈbeautiful National \Theatre, | built in the mid eighteen
˅hundreds | and one of the most imˈportant Czech cultural insti \tutions. | If you want to see
opera or ˅ballet in Prague, | ˈthat’s the place to \go. |We’re ˅now passing |under one of the
best-known bridges in the ˅world, |the \Charles Bridge. | It was built in the thirteenth ˅century |
and has seventy-five ˅statues |along its \sides. | Over on the ˅left, | up on the ˅hill, | is
ˈPrague \Castle, | the home of ˈCzech ˃kings |throughout the \ages | and ˈnow the seat of the
\President |of the Czech re \public. | You can ˅also |see the ˅spires |of Saint Vitus Ca \thedral,
| where most of the ˈCzech kings are \buried. |To your ˃right now |is the Ru \dolfinum, | a
\concert hall.
READING PRACTICE
B: I went to an English summer school a year ago. It was at the Gold School of English.
A: Was it good?
B: Yes, very good. There were grammar lessons and regular progress tests. And we also played
games. That was great.
B. Guessing games, for example. A simple game is the Bag Game. One group has a big bag, and
the other group guesses what’s in the bag. It’s a good game at the beginning, to get to know each
other.
Yes. It wasn’t a big group. I’ve forgotten exactly how many. We got on very well together.
My worst exam moment happened when I was caught cheating by my mum after a history
exam. I really liked history classes, but I didn’t have a very good memory . So on the morning of
the exam I wrote loads of important facts and figures on the insides of my shirt cuffs. I made sure
that I got to the exam room really early so I could sit at the back. I managed to answer quite a few
173
questions using the stuff I’d written on my shirt. I was terrified that I was going to get caught, but
luckily the teacher never noticed what I was doing. Stupidly though, when I got home I was so
happy the exam had finished that I just got changed out of my school clothes and left them on my
bed. Anyway, while I was playing football with my friends in the park, my mum came to get my dirty
clothes so that she could do some washing. She found the shirt and immediately realised what I’d
done – she was absolutely furious, of course, and stopped my pocket money for three months. It
taught me a lesson though, and I’ve never cheated at anything since.
B: There are three hats together in the window, madam. Do you want the one with the feathers?
A: No, not that one either. The one over there. The leather one.
B: Ah! The leather one. Now This is another leather hat, madam. It´s better than the one in the
window. It´s a smoother leather.
A: But I´d rather have the one in the window. It goes with my clothes.
B: Certainly, madam, but we don´t take anything out of the window until three o´clock on Thursday
D) My Husband`s gone!
174
Janet: His manager.
Jill: I wonder how long the affair will last. She´s much older than him, I guess.
Jill: She is much older, but she is richer. And more famous, you know.
B: OK, leave it here and we’ll take care of it. What’s the trouble?
A: There are various things. Some are serious, some are not so serious. The gear box is really
bad. It won’t go into top gear.
A: The steering wheel is stiff. And the radio aerial doesn’t work.
B: You can’t get spare parts. So if one part wears out, you have to have a new aerial.
A: I see. And would you repair the spare wheel? The air comes out.
Tom: Oh. I haven't been there either, and I need some money. It's four o'clock and the banks have
closed, you know.
Tom: I'll ask him then. By the way, here are the reports I wrote. The files about the murder at
Oxford Street. Remember?
175
Bill: I do remember them. Thank you, Tom. Are you leaving now?
Bill. Goodbye.
It was Saturday morning when I woke up to the sound of the alarm ringing in my ears. I quickly
jumped out of bed, remembering that we were going off to Wales in less than an hour’s time.
I ran upstairs to the spare bedroom to wake my sister up. I shook her vigorously and shouted,
“Come on, Madeleine! Get up or we’ll miss the train!” As I rushed into the bathroom I heard her
mumbling, “What train?” She had obviously forgotten what we had planned the night before. I
shouted at the top of my voice, “The train to Wales! We are supposed to be catching a train at
eight thirty to go home to see Mum. Remember?” It all went quiet for a while until she realized she
wasn’t dreaming. She almost flew out of the bedroom and started to panic as usual. “What time is it
now? Why didn’t you wake me earlier? I must have a cup of coffee first, otherwise I’ll never make
176
ACCENTUATION OF POLYSYLLABIC WORDS
Rules:
2. The Rule of Alternation: in an English word major stresses should not fall on adjacent
syllables (Carr, 2013). This tendency of alternating stress in order to avoid a succession of a
number of weak syllables is also responsible for some alternative pronunciations in contemporary
English, which favour, for example conˈtroversy over ˈcontroversy
3. The Downgrading Principle or Stress Shift : It is the act of moving (or shifting) the primary
stress to a previous syllable. Deaccenting potential stresses is characteristic of English rhythm.
This principle is sometimes called the Rule of Three, since it weakens the middle stress of three
successive stresses, even if the middle stress in the main one; for example, ˌafterˈnoon vs
ˌafternoon ˈtea.
4. The Derivational principle: there is a tendency to place a secondary stress on the syllable
which had the primary stress in the deriving Word. For example: ˈcharacter → ˌcharateˈristic.
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
Programa de la Materia
Autoridades
Directora: Abog. y Trad. Públ. Beatriz Rodríguez
Secretario: Abog. y Trad. Públ. Santiago Murias
Año: 2023
1. Fundamentación:
La enseñanza de la fonología es esencial para la formación y el desarrollo del futuro traductor ya que cumple
la doble función de presentar marcos teóricos para el estudio del nivel de la expresión oral del idioma inglés
y de contribuir a una adecuada utilización de las características suprasegmentales en la producción oral de
dichos profesionales. La inserción de fonología en el plan de estudios es además fundamental ya que brinda
las herramientas necesarias para poder mejorar la comprensión y producción del idioma, competencias
esenciales que debe desarrollar el traductor en el proceso de interpretación.
Siguiendo el enfoque general del plan de estudios de la institución, el conocimiento de la lengua extranjera
será la única garantía para una verdadera aproximación en profundidad y detalle al texto a traducir. Dicho
conocimiento permitirá que el alumno logre una expresión correcta y fluida tanto en forma escrita como oral.
La Fonología aparece entonces como el complemento necesario para el manejo de la lengua fuente y se
constituye en instrumento fundamental para lograr un desempeño adecuado en las materias de Interpretación
que los alumnos tienen en la carrera. En estos casos, la fonología los ayuda a decodificar los mensajes con
mayor precisión, no sólo a nivel segmental, sino también suprasegmental. La importancia de la fonología en
el plan de estudios trasciende sus objetivos específicos, ya que la transmisión de un mensaje se logra con
185
efectividad no sólo a través de un discurso coherente y un correcto uso de la gramática del idioma, sino
también a través de un reconocimiento y producción de los sonidos considerados correctos o standard, y una
adecuada utilización de las características suprasegmentales en la producción oral.
Los continuos cambios en la sociedad del siglo XXI urgen a una capacitación permanente por parte de un
profesional. La fonología como parte del plan de formación del futuro traductor debe acompañar en la toma
de conciencia acerca de la necesidad de una actualización y formación continua y debe promover el
entrenamiento para que los profesionales sean capaces de enfrentar los cambios que la sociedad requiere.
Para ello es esencial que exista una activa participación por parte de los alumnos en la materia y un
compromiso constante hacia su propio crecimiento personal y profesional. El profesional debe ser una
persona que posee un nivel cultural elevado y se mantiene informado.
2. Objetivos generales:
• conozcan las teorías actuales sobre los estudios de la entonación en general y en particular sobre los rasgos
suprasegmentales del idioma inglés.
• integren y profundicen los contenidos y las habilidades adquiridas en Fonología de la Lengua Extranjera I.
3. Objetivos específicos:
Que a través del curso de Fonología de la Lengua Extranjera II el alumno sea capaz de:
* manipular las dificultades del idioma en el plano fonológico y expresarse en el idioma extranjero
utilizando las características segmentales y supresegmentales del sistema fonológico del inglés.
* reconocer y saber explicar, utilizando la terminología adecuada, las opciones prosódicas de acento y
entonación desde las distintas perspectivas teóricas.
* mejorar y controlar sus sonidos sin perder la fluidez que requiere una producción natural.
186
* reconocer y representar los símbolos suprasegmentales, así como de entender y explicar los principios que
rigen su implementación y la estructura rítmica del inglés.
* comparar el sistema fonológico del castellano con el sistema fonológico del inglés.
* integrar los conocimientos, ejercitación y técnicas adquiridas en la materia, al resto de los campos del área
de lengua extranjera.
* lograr herramientas utilizables en los procesos de auto corrección y de corrección de errores en la actuación
profesional futura.
* valorar tanto sus aciertos como sus errores, ya que estos últimos forman una parte fundamental para el
proceso de aprendizaje.
* desarrollar una actitud responsable frente al trabajo, la puntualidad en clase, la entrega en tiempo y forma
de trabajos prácticos.
4. Contenidos mínimos:
Los rasgos suprasegmentales del idioma Inglés a nivel del discurso. Conceptos de Tonalidad, Tonicidad,
Altura Tonal y Tono. La palabra. Acentuación de palabras simples y compuestas. Nociones básicas de
algunos acentos del idioma inglés.
5. Contenidos específicos:
Unidad 3: Unidad tonal. Criterios en la división en unidades tonales. El tamaño de la unidad tonal. La
implicancia de la gramática en la unidad tonal. Tonalidad neutral y marcada. Vocativos, adverbios, frases
nominales largas. Tema y rema. Topicalización.
187
Unidad 4: Sistemas de notación. Unidad tónica. Estructura y límites. Núcleo de entonación. Principios
básicos. Foco. Información dada y nueva. LLI rule y sus excepciones. El acento en contexto.
Unidad 5: Tonos del inglés. Comparación entre inglés británico y americano. Tonalidad descendente.
Tonalidad ascendente. Tonalidad descendente-ascendente. Tonalidad ascendente-descendente. Tonalidad
sostenida. Secuencias tonales. Aspecto discursivo de la entonación. Negociación del significado. Concepto
de convergencia y divergencia. (cuatro clases)
6. Metodología:
Con la intención de acompañar a los alumnos en el proceso de apropiación activa de las características
prosódicas adecuadas a la tarea que desempeñarán en su futuro profesional se harán actividades de
sensibilización de los alumnos a los tonos, tonalidad y tonicidad del inglés. Junto con el trabajo de
percepción de las características prosódicas relevantes, se sistematizarán las mismas y se realizarán
actividades de aplicación. Se fomentará la reflexión y análisis sobre los distintos conceptos y las
dificultades de los alumnos estimulándolos a realizar el monitoreo de su propia producción.
Los alumnos se expondrán a bibliografía original de diferentes autores con el propósito de generar una
actitud crítica frente a material técnico de alta calidad académica.
Se trabajará con extractos de videos que ya han sido seleccionados y preparados para que los alumnos no
sólo perciban los elementos lingüísticos, sino también los elementos paralingüísticos y extralingüísticos. Se
alentará, en la medida que el tiempo lo permita, a que los alumnos busquen videos relevantes para su
discusión en la clase.
Se hará un continuo análisis de errores y sus posibles causas y se realizará en forma continua una
comparación entre el sistema fonológico español y el inglés, necesaria para su carrera.
La producción oral y el monitoreo de los sonidos y entonación por parte de los profesores y del propio
alumno tendrá un lugar fundamental en la clase, donde se procurará que las actividades se desarrollen en un
clima ameno, de respeto, compromiso y solidaridad.
188
7. Exámenes libres:
Los alumnos que se presenten para rendir examen con carácter de libres deberán rendir un examen oral, el
cual será de las mismas caracterísitcas que en el caso de los alumnos regulares.
La asistencia a clase es un requisito fundamental para la aprobación de la materia, ya que la misma posee un
carácter eminentemente práctico. Los conocimientos de los alumnos serán evaluados constantemente por los
profesores de la cátedra. El carácter de la materia requiere un seguimiento exhaustivo y pormenorizado de la
producción oral de cada alumno en particular.
9. Bibliografía
Bibliografía obligatoria:
* Brazil, D., Coulthard, M., Johns, C. (1981) Discourse Intonation and Language Teaching. Cambridge:
CUP
* Cruttenden, A. (2001) Gimson´s Pronunciation of English, 6th edition. London: Arnold International
Students´ Edition
* O´Connor, J.D., Arnold, G. F. (1980) Intonation of Colloquial English. London: Longman Group Ltd.
189
Bibliografía complementaria:
* Ortiz Lira, H., Finch, D. (1982) A Course in English Phonetics for Spanish Speakers. London:
Heinemann.
190