ZZ Ok Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004) .
ZZ Ok Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004) .
ZZ Ok Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004) .
net/publication/345115519
CITATIONS READS
122 208
3 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Roy F. Baumeister on 20 May 2023.
June P. Tangney
George Mason University
Roy F. Baumeister
Case Western Reserve University
Angie Luzio Boone
George Mason University
June P. Tangney and Angie Luzio Boone, Department of Psychology George Mason
University; Roy F. Baumeister, Department of Psychology, Case Western Reserve
University.
This research was supported by a research grant from the John Templeton
Foundation and by research grant #MH-57039 from the National Institutes of Health.
We thank Ronda Dearing for assistance with data analysis.
Address correspondence to June P. Tangney, Dept. of Psychology, George Mason
University, 4400 University Drive, Fairfax VA, 22030-4444.
Measurement of Self-Control
In order to investigate the possible benefits of self-control, it is
necessary to have a good trait measure of this construct. Existing
measures are few and old. In fact, the relative dearth of published
evidence on the benefits of self-control among adults may indicate
that researchers have not been satisfied or successful using the
existing scales. Recent advances in self-control theory (see Carver
& Scheier, 1981, 1998; also Baumeister et al., 1994; Muraven
& Baumeister, 2000) suggest the need for developing new scales
as opposed to relying on very old measures. For example,
Baumeister et al. (1994) identified four major domains of self-
control—controlling thoughts, emotions, impulses, and perfor-
mance—which would be important to include in an overall index
of self-control. Hence, a second aim of the present investigation was
Benefits of Self-Control 273
emphasized the ‘‘operate’’ phase of the loop, by which the self per-
forms operations that alter itself. Regulating the stream of thought
(e.g., forcing oneself to concentrate), altering moods or emotions,
restraining undesirable impulses, and achieving optimal perfor-
mance (e.g., by making oneself persist) all constitute important
instances of the self overriding its responses and altering its states or
behaviors. More generally, breaking habits, resisting temptation,
and keeping good self-discipline all reflect the ability of the self to
control itself, and we sought to build our scale around them.
Benefits of Self-Control
Self-control is widely regarded as a capacity to change and adapt the
self so as to produce a better, more optimal fit between self and
world (e.g., Rothbaum et al., 1982). Central to our concept of self-
control is the ability to override or change one’s inner responses, as
well as to interrupt undesired behavioral tendencies and refrain from
acting on them. From this perspective, self-control should con-
tribute to producing a broad range of positive outcomes in life. In
fact, empirical evidence indicates that people with high dispositional
self-control have better outcomes in various spheres. In two
independent studies, we sought to replicate and extend these prior
findings, taking advantage of two large ongoing investigations in
which multiple outcomes were being assessed.
Impulse Control
A second domain involves impulsive behaviors. Many university
students suffer from problems in impulse regulation, as has been
widely documented (see Baumeister et al., 1994, for review). In
particular, problems with regulating eating are prevalent, if not
epidemic, among female university students, whereas surveys of
male students suggest that many suffer from alcohol abuse problems
(e.g., Heatherton, 1993; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Johnston,
O’Malley, & Bachman, 1991; Williamson, 1990). Regulating intake
of food and drink is one of the most obvious and direct applications
of self-control, and so we predicted that people high in self-control
should exhibit fewer such problems.
Several studies have linked impulse control problems to deficits
in self-control. Storey (1999) concluded that poor self-regulation, as
assessed by the Barratt Impulsivity Scale, was an important pre-
dictor of heroin addiction. Wills, DuHamel, and Vaccaro (1995)
found that self-control, as assessed by a scale they derived from
a behavior rating scale by Kendall and Wilcox (1979), was an
Benefits of Self-Control 277
Adjustment
A third domain involves psychological adjustment. Many psycholo-
gical problems and disorders involve some degree of self-regulation
failure. The link between psychological symptoms and self-control
could be bidirectional. On one hand, difficulties with self-regulation
can set the stage for a range of psychological problems. Indeed,
problems with self-control are the hallmark of many disorders
detailed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Conversely, the
emotional distress associated with many of these problems can
impair self-control by preempting limited resources and by produ-
cing stressful outcomes that further burden the individual’s
regulatory capacity.
Of particular interest is the hypothesis that psychological
difficulties can result from either too little or too much control.
The pathogenic nature of self-control failure is fairly obvious. The
DSM-IV has an entire cluster of diagnoses that fall under the
umbrella of ‘‘Impulse Control Disorders,’’ and many other disorders
are essentially defined by problems in the regulation of thought,
affect and/or behavior (e.g., panic and other anxiety disorders,
antisocial personality disorder, anger management problems).
Psychological problems purported to stem from an excess of self-
control are less obvious, but they have been hypothesized to be
important too. Most notably, notions of overcontrol pervade clinical
conceptualizations of both obsessive-compulsive disorder and
278 Tangney et al.
Interpersonal Relationships
A fourth domain concerns interpersonal relationships. High self-
control should make people better, more desirable relationship
partners and could contribute to relationship success in a variety of
ways. Self-control could contribute directly to harmonious interac-
tions, such as when people refrain from saying hurtful things on
impulse. It can also contribute indirectly, such as by enabling people
to resist temptations to become involved with alternative partners.
Poor self-control may lead to angry outbursts and even aggressive
behavior, as well as difficulty moving beyond interpersonal slights to
forgive others.
There is a good deal of evidence suggesting that children with
good self-control get along better with others. A longitudinal study
by Eisenberg et al. (1997) confirmed that good self-regulation,
reported by parents and teachers, at early ages predicts better social
functioning up through age 10. Maszk, Eisenberg, and Guthrie
(1999) found that teacher ratings of children’s self-control (ages 4-6)
predicted subsequent social status such that children who had better
self-control went on to become more popular. Fabes et al. (1999)
found that good effortful control, reported by parents and teachers,
(interacting with situational factors) predicted more socially com-
petent responding among preschool children. Moreover, the long-
itudinal studies by Mischel et al. (1988) and Shoda et al. (l990), cited
above, found that effective capacity to delay gratification at age 4
predicted better interpersonal relationships in early adulthood.
There is also a growing body of research confirming that poor
self-control leads to aggression and antisocial behavior. Much of
this work was stimulated by a landmark book by Gottfredson and
Hirschi (1990), who proposed that low self-control is a major cause
of criminal and violent activity. In subsequent empirical tests, this
theory has held up well, insofar as criminals and other rule breakers
typically exhibit deficits or lapses in self-control assessed with
280 Tangney et al.
Moral Emotions
The moral emotions constituted another domain potentially relevant
to self-control. Shame and guilt have been linked to a variety of
interpersonal and personal outcomes. On balance, guilt appears to
be the more adaptive response to sin and failure. People who experi-
ence guilt about their bad behaviors tend to be motivated in a con-
structive, future-oriented direction—confessing, apologizing, or in
some way undoing the harm that was done (Tangney, 1991, 1995b;
Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow, 1996). In contrast, research has
consistently shown that shame brings with it a panoply of psycho-
logical and social hidden costs.
We were unable to find any previous studies linking self-control
to shame and guilt. However, given the evidence that shame often
provokes irrational anger and other impulsive attempts to defend
the self (e.g., externalization of blame, efforts to hide or escape
shame-inducing situations), we anticipated a link between poor self-
control and proneness to shame. In contrast, we anticipated a posi-
tive relationship between proneness to ‘‘shame-free’’ guilt and high
self-control.
RESULTS
Properties of Self-Control Scale
As shown in Table 1, internal consistency estimates of reliability
were high. Alphas for the Total Self-Control Scale were .89 in both
Studies 1 and 2. Similarly, the Brief SCS was highly reliable
(alpha 5 .83 and .85 in Studies 1 and 2, respectively). Thus, the scale
appears to have adequate internal reliability.
In addition, to establish test-retest reliability of the new Self-
Control Scale, 233 participants in Study 2 completed the scale
a second time in Session 3, conducted roughly three weeks later.
288 Tangney et al.
Test-retest reliability was .89 for the Total SCS score and .87 for the
Brief SCS.
One possible concern is the degree to which the Self-Control Scale
correlates with Social Desirability. As shown in Table 2, the
correlations between self-control and social desirability (as assessed
by the Marlowe-Crowne and BIDR) ranged from .54 to .60. This
raises the possibility that any observed effects involving self-control
may be due to social desirability biases. Therefore, we repeated all
the main analyses with social desirability partialed out.
Impulse Control
The second prediction was that self-control would contribute to
success at impulse regulation, so that people scoring low on self-con-
trol would report a higher rate or incidence of dysfunctional, impu-
lsive behaviors.
The regulation of eating is one important form of impulse
control. Participants in Study 1 completed the Eating Disorders
Inventory. As Table 2 shows, better self-control was associated with
fewer problems regulating eating. Self-control was negatively
correlated with most EDI subscales, including drive for thinness,
bulimia, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness, interpersonal distrust,
Benefits of Self-Control 289
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Reliabilities of Study Measures
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
Self-Control Scale
Total Self Control Study 1 44–168 114.47 18.81 0.89
36 36–180
Study 2 50–154 102.66 18.19 0.89
Brief Self-Control Study 1 15–63 39.22 8.58 0.83
13 13–65
Scale Study 2 17–62 39.85 8.61 0.85
Marlowe Crowne Study 1 35–63 48.51 4.93 0.74
33 33–66
Social Desirability Study 2 77–189 127.38 20.09 0.80
BIDR
Impression Study 2 20 0–20 0–16 5.80 3.56 0.75
Management
Eating Disorder
Inventory
Drive for Thinness Study 1 7 0–21 7–21 4.67 5.49 0.87
Bulimia Study 1 7 0–21 0–17 1.73 2.89 0.74
Body Dissatisfaction Study 1 9 0–27 0–27 9.03 7.52 0.91
Ineffectiveness Study 1 10 0–30 0–30 3.05 4.31 0.86
Perfectionism Study 1 6 0–18 0–17 5.93 4.03 0.71
Interpersonal Study 1 7 0–21 0–21 3.33 3.55 0.79
Distrust
Interoceptive Study 1 10 0–30 0–25 3.41 4.44 0.81
Awareness
Maturity Fears Study 1 8 0–24 0–24 4.53 4.50 0.82
Michigan Alcohol Study 1 13 0–13 0–9 0.00 19.74 0.88
Screening Test—S
Symptom Checklist 90
Somatization Study 1 12 12–60 12–53 23.54 8.82 0.89
Obsessive Study 1 10 10–50 10–50 22.58 7.81 0.87
Compulsive
Interpersonal Study 1 9 9–45 9–42 18.62 7.39 0.88
Sensitivity
Depression Study 1 12 12–60 12–54 25.87 9.89 0.91
Anxiety Study 1 10 10–50 10–45 18.85 7.43 0.88
Hostility-Anger Study 1 6 6–30 6–25 11.58 4.49 0.79
(Continued)
290 Tangney et al.
Table 1 (cont.)
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
MCMI-III
Clinical Personality
Patterns
Schizoid Study 2 16 0–23 0–17 4.43 3.75 0.69
Avoidant Study 2 16 0–24 0–23 4.65 4.86 0.81
Depressive Study 2 15 0–23 0–22 5.23 5.28 0.85
Dependent Study 2 16 0–24 0–22 6.93 5.00 0.76
Histrionic Study 2 17 0–24 3–24 17.09 5.33 0.80
Narcissistic Study 2 24 0–32 0–27 15.45 4.52 0.67
Antisocial Study 2 17 0–24 0–18 6.48 4.16 0.69
Sadist (Aggressive) Study 2 20 0–27 0–21 7.33 5.10 0.77
Compulsive Study 2 17 0–25 2–25 13.69 4.80 0.67
Negativistic Study 2 16 0–26 0–22 7.63 5.19 0.76
(Passive-Aggressive)
Masochistic Study 2 15 0–22 0–19 3.27 4.05 0.81
(Self-Defeating)
Severe Personality
Pathology
Schizotypal Study 2 16 0–25 0–20 4.38 4.30 0.77
Borderline Study 2 16 0–25 0–22 6.10 4.99 0.77
Paranoid Study 2 17 0–26 0–22 5.42 4.80 0.77
Clinical Syndromes
Anxiety Study 2 14 0–20 0–17 4.49 3.85 0.74
Somatoform Study 2 12 0–17 0–12 3.81 3.36 0.72
Bipolar: Manic Study 2 13 0–18 0–17 7.21 3.59 0.68
Dysthymia Study 2 14 0–20 0–19 3.73 4.29 0.82
Alcohol DependenceStudy 2 15 0–21 0–16 3.72 2.92 0.66
Drug Dependence Study 2 14 0–20 0–17 3.59 2.86 0.68
Post-Traumatic Study 2 16 0–21 0–18 3.79 4.04 0.82
Stress Disorder
(Continued)
Benefits of Self-Control 291
Table 1 (cont.)
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale
Self-Esteem Study 1 14–50 38.06 6.66 0.88
10 10–50
Study 2 18–50 39.54 6.86 0.89
Stability of Study 1 5–25 14.36 3.65 0.77
5 5–25
Self-Esteem Study 2 6–25 15.71 4.02 0.79
Mini-Marker
Extra version Study 2 8 12–56 38.09 9.25 0.86
Agreeableness Study 2 8 15–56 46.06 6.63 0.84
Conscientiousness Study 2 8 8–56 19–56 41.00 7.57 0.82
Emotional Study 2 8 8–56 35.00 8.65 0.81
Stability
Openness to Study 2 8 21–56 42.14 6.77 0.78
Experience
Multidimensional
Perfectionsm Scale
Self oriented Study 1 30–99 66.59 15.09 0.86
15 7–105
perfectionism Study 2 21–105 68.07 16.09 0.89
Other oriented Study 1 19–85 58.07 11.06 0.71
15 7–105
perfectionsim Study 2 24–90 56.29 10.89 0.72
Socially oriented Study 1 21–85 53.72 12.97 0.81
15 7–105
perfectionsim Study 2 19–94 54.26 14.01 0.85
Family Environment
Scale
Family CohesivenessStudy 1 9 9–36 12–36 27.29 5.07 0.82
Family Conflict Study 1 9 9–36 9–34 20.26 5.38 0.82
(Continued)
292 Tangney et al.
Table 1 (cont.)
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
Close Relationship
Questionnaire
Avoidant Study 1 1–7 3.37 1.95 –
1 1–7
Study 2 1–7 2.94 1.98
Anxious- Study 1 1–7 3.49 1.9 –
1 1–7
Ambivalent Study 2 1–7 2.83 1.87
Secure Study 1 1–7 4.37 1.84 –
1 1–7
Study 2 1–7 4.78 1.82
Empathy- Interpersonal
Reactivity Index
Perspective Taking Study 1 7–35 24.35 4.77 0.74
7 7–35
Study 2 9–35 24.36 4.88 0.75
Empathic Concern Study 1 15–35 28.03 4.15 0.70
7 7–35
Study 2 11–35 28.62 4.46 0.77
Personal Distress Study 1 12–47 29.27 6.13 0.76
10 10–50
Study 2 14–46 29.46 5.62 0.68
Anger Response
Inventory
Total Anger Study 1 47–112 89.54 10.94 0.85
23 23–115
Arousal Study 2 40–111 85.47 13.64 0.92
Intentions
Constructive Study 1 34–115 93.82 13.59 0.88
23 23–115
Intentions Study 2 34–115 87.02 17.60 0.93
Malevolent Study 1 27–111 69.81 17.64 0.92
23 23–115
Intentions Study 2 23–110 65.04 20.12 0.95
Fractious Study 1 25–115 75.10 19.84 0.93
23 23–115
Study 2 25–113 70.75 20.55 0.95
Maladaptive Responses
Direct Physical Study 1 7–28 10.97 3.84 0.70
7 7–35
Aggression Study 2 7–28 11.55 4.59 0.79
Direct Verbal Study 1 8–39 18.20 6.15 0.71
8 8–40
Aggression Study 2 8–36 16.70 6.39 0.79
(Continued)
Benefits of Self-Control 293
Table 1 (cont.)
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
Direct Symbolic Study 1 7–30 13.85 4.84 0.65
7 7–35
Aggression Study 2 7–32 13.90 5.74 0.79
Indirect Harm Study1 11–40 21.15 5.98 0.70
11 11–55
Study 2 11–45 21.60 6.84 0.78
Malediction Study 1 10–47 24.48 6.55 0.67
10 10–50
Study 2 10–46 24.02 7.44 0.76
Displaced Physical Study 1 7–23 8.83 2.76 0.69
7 7–35
Aggression Study 2 7–25 9.75 4.22 0.85
Displaced Verbal Study 1 7–26 11.56 3.98 0.66
7 7–35
Aggression Study 2 7–25 11.34 4.29 0.79
Displaced Study 1 8–26 11.68 4.20 0.73
8 8–40
Aggression-Object Study 2 8–40 12.24 5.82 0.88
Self-Directed Study 1 9–41 22.45 6.54 0.72
9 9–45
Aggression Study 2 9–40 21.82 6.40 0.73
Anger Held In Study 1 10–44 25.90 6.74 0.70
10 10–50
Study 2 10–43 24.22 7.28 0.79
Adaptive Responses
Communicate Study1 20–55 42.92 6.98 0.76
11 11–55
with Target Study 2 19–55 41.15 7.60 0.80
Constructive Study 1 26–49 38.67 4.41 0.39
10 10–50
Action Study 2 22–50 38.06 5.01 0.55
Escapist-Diffusing
Behaviors
Diffusion of Anger Study 1 10–35 23.02 4.79 0.57
7 7–35
Study 2 10–35 22.94 4.66 0.57
Minimization Study 1 8–36 21.55 4.73 0.46
8 8–40
Study 2 10–36 21.88 4.69 0.75
Removal Study 1 7–34 19.50 4.47 0.49
7 7–35
Study 2 9–30 19.52 4.33 0.49
Doing Nothing Study 1 10–39 24.66 5.12 0.49
9 9–45
Study 2 11–40 26.11 5.29 0.51
(Continued)
294 Tangney et al.
Table 1 (cont.)
# of Possible Observed
Scale items Range Range Mean SD Alpha
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Marlowe Crowne Study 1 .56nnn .54nnn – – – –
Study 2 .60nnn .59nnn – – – –
BIDR Study 2 .57nnn .55nnn – – – –
Grade Point Average Study 1 .39nnn .39nnn .32nnn .32nnn – –
Study 2 .19nn .15n .23nn .18 .16n .11
Eating Disorder Inventory
Drive for Thinness Study 1 ! .34nnn ! .37nnn ! .31nnn ! .35nnn – –
Bulimia Study 1 ! .36nnn ! .35nnn ! .30nnn ! .28nnn – –
Body Dissatisfaction Study 1 ! .37nnn ! .40nnn ! .33nnn ! .34nnn – –
Ineffectiveness Study 1 ! .41nnn ! .44nnn ! .35nnn ! .39nnn – –
Perfectionism Study 1 .06 .08 .01 .04 – –
Interpersonal Distrust Study 1 ! .22nnn ! .25nnn ! .14nn ! .21nnn – –
Interoceptive Awareness Study 1 ! .48nnn ! .51nnn ! .43nnn ! .47nnn – –
Maturity Fears Study 1 ! .13n ! .16n ! .22nnn ! .23nnn – –
Michigan Alcohol Screening Test
Alcoholism Study 1 ! .31nnn ! .32nnn ! .26nn ! .26nn – –
Study 1 n 5 200 –346, except for analysis involving MAST and GPA n 5 140 –157.
Study 2 n 5 200 –255, except for analysis involving MC, n 5 98–105
n
po.05. nnpo.01. nnnpo.001.
296 Tangney et al.
Controlling for
Bivariate Correlations Marlowe-Crowne Controlling for BIDR
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
SCL-90
Somatization Study 1 ! .39nnn ! .35nnn ! .31nnn ! .26nnn – –
Obsessive-Compulsive Study 1 ! .42nnn ! .41nnn ! .33nnn ! .32nnn – –
Interpersonal Sensitivity Study 1 ! .36nnn ! .33nnn ! .27nn ! .24nn – –
Depression Study 1 ! .41nnn ! .38nnn ! .34nnn ! .32nnn – –
Anxiety Study 1 ! .36nnn ! .34nnn ! .33nnn ! .30nnn – –
Hostility-Anger Study 1 ! .40nnn ! .34nnn ! .27nn ! .22nn – –
Phobic Anxiety Study 1 ! .30nnn ! .25nn ! .25nn ! .19n – –
Paranoid Ideation Study 1 ! .29nnn ! .25nnn ! .22nn ! .17n – –
Psychotisicm Study 1 ! .33nnn ! .28nnn ! .25nn ! .20n – –
MCMI-III
Clinical Personality Patterns
Schizoid Study 2 ! .18nn ! .16n ! .07 ! .10 ! .08 ! .05
Avoidant Study 2 ! .27nnn ! .25nnn ! .17 ! .17 ! .17 ! .15n
(Continued)
Table 3 (cont.)
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Mini Marker
Extraversion Study 2 .09 .11 .05 .09 .07 .09
Agreeableness Study 2 .29nnn .29nnn ! .05 ! .01 .12 .12
Conscientiousness Study 2 .54nnn .48nnn .49nnn .43nnn .47nnn .39
Emotional Stability Study 2 .50nnn .42nnn .42nnn .29nn .39nnn .30
Openness to Experience Study 2 .04 .05 ! .02 .01 ! .06 ! .04
Brief Perfectionsim Scale Study 2 .15 .10 .07 .01 .14 .09
Multidimensional Perfectionsim Scale
Self oriented perfectionism Study 1 .15 .15 .18n .17n – –
Study 2 .23nnn .20nn .20n .13 .12 .10
Other oriented perfectionsim Study 1 .13 .18n .15 .20n – –
Study 2 .12 .12 .15 .14 .17nn .16n
Socially oriented perfectionsim Study 1 ! .31nnn ! .29nnn ! .21nn ! .20n – –
Study 2 ! .26nnn ! .25nnn ! .19 ! .21 ! .20nn ! .19nn
Study 1 n 5 146.
Study 2 n 5 229–233, except for analysis involving MC and Self-Perfectionism, n 5 101–105.
n
po.05. nnpo.01. nnnpo.001.
Benefits of Self-Control 301
Interpersonal Relations
We hypothesized that self-control would be associated with positive
interpersonal relations. Theoretically, the link between self-control
and interpersonal adjustment should be bi-directional. For example,
in the context of the family, good family relations should improve
the capacity for self-regulation (as compared to living in a
dysfunctional, conflict-ridden family). Conversely, a strong capacity
for self-control should enhance one’s ability to get along well with
others, leading to better family dynamics and relationships.
As predicted, participants who reported a positive family
environment in their family of origin had higher self-control,
compared to their peers from more dysfunctional families. Table 5
shows that self-control was positively correlated with family
cohesion and negatively correlated with family conflict. We did
not have the opportunity to replicate across samples. (The measure
of family environment was included in Study 1 only.) But the effects
were consistently significant when considering both Total and Brief
SCS scores, and when controlling for social desirability.
Attachment style was also related to self-control (see Table 5). In
both studies, a secure attachment style was positively correlated with
the capacity for self-control (as measured by both Total and Brief
SCS scores), consistent with the view that self-control strengthens
and is strengthened by good, stable relationships. In contrast,
avoidant and anxious-ambivalent attachment styles were negatively
correlated with self-control. In Study 2, the findings were robust
with respect to social desirability—as assessed by both the Marlowe-
Crowne and BIDR. In Study 1, however, the relationship of self-
control to avoidant and secure attachment did not hold when
controlling for Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scores.
Table 5
Relationship of Self-Control to Family Cohesion and Conflict, Attachment, and Empathy
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Family Environment Scale
Family Cohesion Study 1 .38nnn .35nnn .27nn .24nn – –
Family Conflict Study 1 ! .37nnn ! .32nnn ! .24nn ! .18n – –
Attachment
Avoidant Study 1 ! .12n ! .12n ! .04 ! .05 – –
Study 2 ! .20nn ! .22nn ! .18 ! .26nn ! .18nn ! .21nnn
Anxious- Ambivalent Study 1 ! .25nnn ! .23nnn ! .18nn ! .15nn – –
Study 2 ! .24nnn ! .20nn ! .18 ! .13 ! .20nnn ! .15n
Secure Study 1 .16nn .15nn .06 .07 – –
Study 2 .26nnn .25nnn .29nn .35nnn .22nnn .22nnn
Empathy
Perspective Taking Study 1 .16nn .14n ! .02 ! .04 – –
Study 2 .27nnn .25nnn .10 .16 .11 .09
Empathic Concern Study 1 ! .03 ! .02 ! .22nnn ! .22nnn – –
Study 2 .19nn .21nnn ! .14 ! .03 .09 .11
Personal Distress Study 1 ! .28nnn ! .29nnn ! .32nnn ! .33nnn – –
Study 2 ! .18nn ! .13n ! .27nn ! .21n ! .19nn ! .13n
Study 1 n 5 337–350, except for analysis involving FES, n 5 150–153.
Study 2 n 5 248–250 , except for analysis involving MC, n 5 99–100.
n
po.05. nnpo.01. nnnpo001.
Benefits of Self-Control 303
Controlling for
Bivariate Correlations Mariowe-Crowne Controlling for BIDR
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Anger Response Inventory
Total Anger Arousal Study 1 ! .22nnn ! .21nnn ! .13n ! .14n – –
Study 2 ! .12 ! .09 ! .07 ! .05 ! .05 ! .02
Intentions
Constructive Intentions Study 1 ! .02 ! .04 .03 .00 – –
Study 2 ! .08 ! .09 ! .05 ! .12 ! .04 ! .06
Malevolent Intentions Study I ! .29nnn ! .29nnn ! .19nnn ! .19nnn – –
Study 2 ! .20nn ! .19nn .03 .00 .00 .01
Fractious Study1 ! .31nnn ! .33nnn ! .23nnn ! .26nnn – –
Study 2 ! .24nnn ! .21nnn ! .11 ! .11 ! .06 ! .03
Maladaptive Responses
Direct Physical Aggression Study 1 ! .26nnn .24nnn ! .21nnn ! .19nnn – –
Study 2 ! .18nn ! .15n ! .02 .06 .02 .04
Direct Verbal Aggression Study 1 ! .32nnn ! .23nnn ! .21nnn ! .17nnn – –
Study 2 ! .26nnn ! .23nnn ! .10 ! .03 ! .07 ! .04
Direct Symbolic Aggression Study 1 ! .34nnn ! .29nnn ! .25nnn ! .20nnn – –
Study 2 ! .25nnn ! .24nnn ! .13 ! .14nn ! .02 ! .02
Indirect Harm Study 1 ! .31nnn ! .29nnn ! .25nnn ! .23nnn – –
Study 2 ! .28nnn ! .25nnn ! .10 ! .08 ! .08 ! .06
Malediction Study 1 ! .24nnn ! .21nnn ! .16nn ! .14n – !
Study 2 ! .33nnn ! .31nnn ! .07 ! .07 ! .16n ! .15n
Displaced Physical Aggression Study 1 ! .18nn ! .16nn ! .16nn ! .14nn – –
Study 2 ! .16n ! .13 ! .02 .04 .06 .10
Displaced Verbal Aggression Study 1 ! .19nnn ! .19nnn ! .10 ! .12n – –
Study 2 ! .27nnn ! .23nnn ! .13 ! .08 ! .04 .00
Displaced Aggression (Object) Study 1 ! .25nnn ! .25nnn ! .17nn ! .17nnn – –
Study 2 ! .13n ! .12 ! .03 ! .01 .06 .06
Self-Directed Aggression Study 1 ! .17nn ! .19nnn ! .20nnn ! .23nnn – –
Study 2 ! .17nn ! .16n ! .08 ! .10 ! .02 ! .01
Anger Held In Study 1 ! .35nnn ! .37nnn ! .25nnn ! .28nnn – –
Study 2 ! .30nnn ! .30nnn ! .13 ! .22n ! .13 ! .14n
Adaptive Behaviors
Communicate w/ Target Study 1 .23nnn .22nnn .12n .08 – –
Study 2 .13n .14n ! .10 ! .14 .03 .04
Constructive Action Study 1 ! .02 ! .03 ! .01 ! .03 – –
Study 2 ! .02 ! .02 ! .06 ! .08 ! .06 ! .06
(Continued)
Table 6 (cont.)
Controlling for
Bivariate Correlations Mariowe-Crowne Controlling for BIDR
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Escapist-Diffusing Behaviors
Diffusion of Anger Study 1 ! .09 ! .10 ! .13n ! .15nn – –
Study 2 ! .02 ! .06 ! .09 ! .18 ! .05 ! .09
Minimization Study l .01 ! .02 ! .08 ! .11n ! !
Study 2 ! .03 ! .05 .03 ! .03 .05 .02
Removal Study 1 ! .01 ! .04 ! .05 ! .08 ! !
Study 2 .01 .03 ! .01 .00 .08 .10
Doing Nothing Study l ! .06 ! .07 ! .17nnn ! .18nnn – –
Study 2 ! .01 .00 .01 .04 .00 .01
Cognitive Reappraisals
Target’s Role Study 1 .01 .00 ! .08 ! .09 – –
Study 2 .11 .11 .05 .03 .06 .05
Self’s Role Study 1 ! .18nnn ! .17nnn ! .21 ! .20nnn ! –
Study 2 ! .04 ! .03 ! .15 ! .18 .00 .02
Long-Term Consequences
For Self Study 1 .17nnn .19nnn .12n .14nn – –
Study 2 .16n .15n ! .10 .10 .05 .04
For Target Study l .17nnn .20nnn .13n .16nn – –
Study 2 .13 .11 ! .11 ! .13 .01 .00
For Relationship Study l .23nnn .24nnn .15nn .16nn – –
Study 2 .16n .14n ! .15 ! .15 .03 .02
Total Study l .21nnn .23nnn .14nn .17nn ! !
Study 2 .16n .14n ! .12 ! .13 .03 .02
Study 1 n 5 342–351.
Study 2 n 5 224–227, except for analysis involving MC, n 5 99.
n
po.05. nnpo01. nnnpo.001.
308 Tangney et al.
Moral Emotions
Last, we considered the implications of self-control for people’s
reaction to their own transgressions. In the course of daily life, in
spite of their best efforts at self-control, people inevitably sin and
transgress, at least on occasion. An important component of
interpersonal adjustment is the manner in which people manage
their failures and transgressions. As shown in Table 8 across both
studies and when considering both the Total and Brief SCS scores,
people high in self-control exhibited an adaptive moral emotional
Controlling for
Bivariate Correlations Marlowe-Crowne Controlling for BIDR
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
Controlling for
Bivariate Correlations Martowe-Crowne Controlling for BIDR
Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self- Full Self- Brief Self-
Control Control Control Control Control Control
Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure
TOSCA
Shame-Proneness Study 1 ! .33nnn ! .35nnn ! .30nnn ! .32nnn – –
Study 2 ! .26nnn ! .22nnn ! .01 .01 ! .19nn ! .14n
Guilt-Proneness Study 1 .13n .13n ! .01 ! .01 – –
Study 2 .30nnn .27nnn .26nn .13 .13n .09
Externalization Study 1 ! .13n ! .14nn ! .11n ! .13n – –
Study 2 ! .23nnn ! .23nnn ! .10 ! .13 ! .09 ! .09
Study 1 n 5 342.
Study 2 n 5 249–252, except for analysis involving MC, n 5 100.
n
po.05. nnpo.01. nnnpo.001.
Benefits of Self-Control 311
DISCUSSION
The two goals of this paper were to provide evidence for the psycho-
logical benefits and advantages of self-control and, in order to accom-
plish that, to develop a trait scale to assess individual differences in
self-control. The results suggest that our scale performs well as a
trait measure and that self-control is indeed linked to a broad range
of positive outcomes. We shall discuss these two separately.
Benefits of Self-Control
A main purpose of this work was to test the hypothesis that self-
control would be correlated with a range of positive, desirable
outcomes. The present data provide strong and extensive support
for this view.
First, people with high self-control had better grades, as
compared with people low in self-control. Advocates of self-disci-
pline have long speculated that it will produce better performance.
For example, people with poor self-control may procrastinate on
tasks, which often leads to poorer performance and lower grades
(Tice & Baumeister, 1997). Although the current data are correla-
tional, precluding strong causal conclusions, the results are consis-
tent with the view that high self-control fosters strong academic
performance.
Second, people with high self-control showed fewer impulse
control problems, including binge eating and alcohol abuse. Third,
they showed better psychological adjustment, as assessed by a self-
report measure of psychopathological symptoms including somati-
zation, obsessive-compulsive patterns, depression, anxiety, hostile
312 Tangney et al.
Costs of Self-Control
Not all theoretical views about self-control have emphasized positive
outcomes. In particular, theories about overcontrol have held that
high levels of self-control contribute to pathologies such as
obsession and compulsion.
The present results offer no support for the view that high levels of
self-control are bad. All our findings suggest linear effects such that
more self-control is better. Analyses designed to test for curvilinearity
failed consistently to find any evidence that scores at both extremes
314 Tangney et al.
Measurement of Self-Control
Given that self-control has such broad implications for adjustment,
it is surprising that there are not many theoretically informed,
reliable, and valid measures of self-control. Apart from their sub-
stantive implications, results from the current study provide strong
support for the reliability and validity of the Self-Control Scale—a
relatively brief, easily administered paper-and-pencil measure. Its
internal consistency was good, especially for the full scale but also
for the subscales. Retest reliability over a one-to-three-week period
was also satisfactorily high. Moreover, the brief, 13-item version of
the SCS performed nearly as well as the full-length version.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
316 Tangney et al.
Using the scale provided, please indicate how much each of the following statements reflects how you typically are.
n
Items included in the Brief Self Control measure
(R) Reversed Items