Provided by South East Academic Libraries System (SEALS)
Provided by South East Academic Libraries System (SEALS)
Provided by South East Academic Libraries System (SEALS)
By
Reinhart Burger
December 2011
DECLARATION
I, Reinhart Gerrit Burger (student number: 209096014), hereby declare that the treatise for
Magister Artium in Counselling Psychology is my own work and that it has not previously
Official use:
of the candidate to the effect that it is his/her own work and that it has not previously
treatise/dissertation/thesis.
ii
Acknowledgements
The researcher would like to thank the following people, without whom this study would not
• My supervisor, Prof. Gregory Howcroft, and co-supervisors, Prof. Louise Stroud and
• My parents, for their emotional and financial support that allowed me to bring this
project to completion.
• My friends – you mean the world to me. I would particularly like to thank Genee
Pienaar, Juazel Pieterse, and Nicky Viviers who played, and continue to play, an
iii
Table of Contents
Declaration......................................................................................................................... i
Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................... ii
Summary ............................................................................................................................ x
iv
v
3.3. Academic skills disorders, mental retardation, and developmental delays ................. 46
vi
5.4. The relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-
5.5. The relationship between chronological age and human figure drawing raw scores .. 88
vii
Appendix
viii
List of Tables
Table 4: SPSS Output for Curve Estimation between Standardized Human Figure
ix
List of Figures
x
Summary
drawing (HFD) tests for developmental testing in the South African context. Due to their
characteristics such tests may be particularly suitable to screen for developmental delays in
the local South African context. The objectives of the research were to explore and describe
the standardized HFD scores and Griffiths Mental Development Scales – Extended Revised
exists between the standardized HFD scores and GQs; and to investigate the strength and
directionality of this relationship if it was found to exist in the sample. Relevant literature was
discussed and an integration of key constructs was provided. The research sample consisted
Mandela metropol, Eastern Cape, South Africa. The HFDs of the Draw-a-person items of the
GMDS-ER protocols were scored according to the scoring criteria of the Draw-a-Person
Intellectual Ability Test for Children, Adolescents, and Adults (DAP: IQ). Key findings
marked relationship existed between standardized HFD scores and GQs. The DAP: IQ might
significant medium positive correlation indicative of a small but definite relationship existed
between chronological age and HFD raw scores. In conclusion, the findings suggest that
HFDs follow a developmental progression and that HFD tests may be useful in
developmental testing.
1
Chapter 1
Introduction
1.1. Overview
This chapter provides an overview of the research. The motivation and rationale for
the research is presented. The history and uses of non-projective human figure drawing tests
Human figure drawings (HFDs) are often used to assess development since the
characteristics of such tests are particularly well suited to the South African context. This
study aims to explore whether HFDs provide a useful estimate of a child’s mental
developmental level. Contemporary non-projective use of HFDs is based on the principle that
an index of a child’s conceptual maturity, mental maturity and intellectual ability may be
obtained by the systematic analysis and scoring of HFDs. The history and uses of HFDs, as
will be reviewed in the following section, warrants the formulation of the hypothesis that
scores obtained from these drawings are somehow related to scores of mental development
obtained from a well-established, robust and detailed measure with proven validity and
reliability such as the Griffiths Mental Development Scales – Extended Revised (GMDS-ER)
The psychological use of human figure drawings (HFDs) can be divided into their use
2
representations of the human figure are used to gain insight into the personality and affect of
the person who made the drawing (Michaelides, 2005). The non-projective use of HFDs
(Koppitz, 1968), or intellectual ability (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). This study focuses on
the non-projective use of human figure drawings, with particular reference to their use in
developmental testing.
investigation for over a century. The value of human figure drawings (HFDs) was first
researchers found that the increasing number of details and progressively more realistic
proportions of the body parts are correlated with a child’s chronological age (Schuyten,
1904), and particularly with mental age (Rouma, 1913). At the end of the 20th century
commentary on children’s drawings was included in various baby biographies, articles and
books on child development (Jolley, 2010). These works typically described stage and age
since elaborated on this groundbreaking work to produce ever more refined systems for
obtaining and scoring children’s HFDs. This pioneering research allowed Florence
Goodenough to develop the first standardised test based on HFDs, known as the Draw-a-Man
3
awarded points based on the number of body parts drawn, the proportions of these parts, and
the way they were attached to the main figure (Cox, 1993).
In his revision of the Draw-a-Man Test, Harris (1963) challenged the historical
assumption that HFDs could be used to obtain estimates of intelligence. The terms
intellectual and conceptual maturity were introduced to convey the view that the test
measures the child’s actual rather than potential level of intellectual functioning (Cox, 1993).
In keeping with this more contemporary view, Koppitz (1968) based the scoring of her Draw-
also recognized by Reynolds and Hickman (2004), the developers of the most recent test
based on HFDs called the Draw-a-Person Intellectual Ability Test for Children, Adolescents,
Human figure drawing tests are becoming more valid with the incorporation of
quantitative global scoring systems, particularly when used for the measurement of
intellectual maturity (Sack, 2009). The DAP: IQ (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004) is the latest
human figure drawing test to employ such a scoring system. It also offers the largest
collection of normative data on human figure drawings (Sack, 2009). These characteristics
add to the reliability and validity of the measure and contributed to the decision to use an
adapted administration of the DAP: IQ to obtain human figure drawing scores in the present
study.
identification of children with developmental delays is essential to ensure that these children
achieve more favourable developmental outcomes. The earlier a child with developmental
difficulties participates in an intervention programme, the greater the gains will be (Rydz,
4
Shevell, Majnemer, & Oskoui, 2005). The failure to identify developmental delays and
provide appropriate intervention may also lead to more severe problems and secondary
handicaps (Lifter, 1992). To this end, assessment must aim to gather data that is
Psychometric measures serve a vital purpose in this regard. Cronbach (1990, p. 32)
defines a psychometric test as a “systematic procedure for observing a person’s behavior and
describing it with the aid of a numerical scale or categorical system”. Tests aim only to
provide an indication of an individual’s ability at a given point in time, whereas the term
assessment implies a broader scope including integration and evaluation of the information
obtained (Anastasi, 1988; Cronbach, 1990). Human figure drawings that employ systematic
tests.
1.4.2. Screening and diagnostic measures. Developmental measures fall into two
categories, namely screening measures and diagnostic measures (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001).
Screening measures are generally less comprehensive than diagnostic measures, providing
usually provide numerical scores and age equivalents for a child’s overall performance as
well as for specific domains measured (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). The DAP: IQ is evidently
simpler than the GMDS-ER insofar as the DAP: IQ only provides an indication of the broad
domain that Reynolds and Hickman (2004) call ‘intellectual ability’. Conversely, the GMDS-
development (Griffiths, 1954). In this sense, the DAP: IQ more closely resembles a screening
measure whereas the GMDS-ER fits into the diagnostic measure category.
5
The aim of screening is to identify children that require further in-depth assessment
(Widerstrom, Mowder & Sandall, 1997). Brooks-Gunn (1990) outlines the following
2. the design of such tests should allow for their use in post-natal clinics,
paediatricians’ offices, community health services and outpatient hospital clinics;
3. the test should be accessible to a broad range of practitioners and require only a
minimal amount of training;
4. the test should be time-efficient so as to better ensure that it is used within the
context of busy clinical practices;
6. such tests should keep the amount of false negatives to a minimum, since children
The DAP: IQ and HFD tests of intellectual ability in general meet these requirements,
with the possible exception of the seventh criterion, namely the minimization of false
negatives. Personal correspondence with professionals who have used such tests suggest that
they may overestimate intellectual ability, thereby incorrectly placing at-risk children in a
non-risk group. HFD tests may therefore play an important role in the screening of
developmental delays, although it may be necessary to use them in conjunction with other
Developmental Screening Test 2nd edition (Frankenburg et al., 1990); the Bayley Infant
Screening Test (Newborg et al., 1984), among others. The DAP: IQ is a popular measure in
South Africa and possesses characteristics that make it particularly attractive within the local
6
context. This measure may therefore be of great value in South Africa if it is confirmed to be
1.4.3. The DAP: IQ in the local South African context. The DAP: IQ remains
popular in South Africa. This popularity may be understood in the light of several
distinguishing features of this measure. The total time taken to administer, score, and
interpret the DAP: IQ is usually less than 10 to 12 minutes and the test may also be
administered in a group format (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). These characteristics make this
an appealing measure to use in the South African context where resources available for
A serious issue facing psychometry in South Africa is that many of the psychometric
measures in use are not appropriately standardized for use in the local context (Foxcroft,
Roodt & Abrahams, 2001). The difficulty associated with standardization and the
development of appropriate norms for use with the diverse South African population poses a
major challenge with regards to ethical test usage (Murphy, 2002). Davis (2006) established
that the published DAP: IQ norms for children aged 5 to 7 are appropriate for use in the local
South African context. This puts the DAP: IQ at a major advantage over other measures that
have not been appropriately normed for use in the local context. In this regard human figure
drawing tests may be valuable when other measures have not in any way been appropriately
standardized. The present study also contributes to a project to restandardize the GMDS-ER
that is currently being conducted and is therefore valuable in helping to address the issue of
psychological assessment in South Africa identified culture as the main issue of test
7
development and revision. In particular, a need for culture fair and culture reduced measures
Reynolds and Hickman (2004) describe the DAP: IQ as a culturally reduced measure.
No test can claim to be culture free because psychological tests sample behaviour that is
affected by the context in which the person was raised (Jansen, 1991). A test may however be
described as ‘culture-fair’ when the test content is based on experiences that are common
across cultures (Baker, 2005). The DAP: IQ is based on the drawing of a human figure,
which is a frequently experienced object to children in all cultures and makes this a
particularly suitable object to include in a test based on drawing. There are however some
noteworthy exceptions to the reduced role of culture in the DAP: IQ that receive remarkably
little attention in the literature. Some religions, including Islam for example, altogether forbid
children from drawing human figures (Carmichael, 2006). Such an exception clearly has a
massive impact on the validity and ethics of using the test with certain populations, but it is
South Africa is a multiracial and multi-ethnic society. According to the most recent
census conducted in 2001, the population of South Africa consists of the following
demographic groups: Black African (79%), White (9.6%), Coloured (8.9%), and
Indian/Asian (2.5%) (Statistics South Africa, 2003). These figures may lead the reader to
assume greater cultural homogeny than is in fact the case. Acknowledgement of the diversity
of such groups reduces the risk of bias. This is also important when considering the
establishment of appropriate norms which is an on-going process in South Africa. The ‘Black
African’ group comprises Zulu (22.4%), Xhosa (17.5%), North Sotho (9.8%) Tswana (7.2%),
South Sotho (6.9%), Tsonga (4.2%),Venda (1.7%), and Ndebele (1.5%), while those from
relatively recent European descent including Afrikaners, English speakers from varying
backgrounds and Portuguese are grouped under ‘White’ (Institute for African Development,
8
spoken at home. The most recent census provides the following figures: IsiZulu (23.8%),
IsiXhosa (17.6%), Afrikaans (13.3%), Sepedi (9.4%), English (8.2%), Setswana (8.2%),
Sesotho (7.9%), Xitsonga (4.4%), SiSwati (2.7%), Tshivenda (2.3%), IsiNdelbele (1.6%), and
other (0.5%) (Statistics South Africa, 2003). While exceptions like the prohibition on
drawing human figures referred to above are important to investigate, the reduced role of
culture in the DAP: IQ, along with Abell, Wood, and Liebman’s (2001) assertion that this
measure may be used with non-reading or non-English speaking individuals, further explains
Drawing activities may be particularly helpful when working with shy or impulsive
children, children with speech and language difficulties, or those who speak a different
language from the practitioner as these children tend to respond well to such activities
(Klepsch & Logie, 1982). Another contributing factor is that the use of this measure in South
registered counsellors) and other disciplines (e.g., general medical practitioners and
A short screening test such as the DAP: IQ may be of remarkable value if it is found
widely used for this purpose and there is reason to believe that HFDs may be useful in the
correlated with ability in language and mathematics, as well as with overall achievement in
Grade 1. Such evidence suggests that human figure drawing tests can be meaningfully
applied in the screening for developmental delays. The value of this research lies in the
9
pressing need to establish whether HFDs such as the DAP: IQ serve this function of
The primary research objectives of this study are summarised in the following four
statements:
• To explore and describe the standardized human figure drawing scores of the sample.
human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients of the sample.
Chapter 1 serves as an introduction to this study. In this chapter, the context, problem
statement, motivation for the research, and primary objectives of the study are briefly
presented.
Chapter 2 presents conceptual models of mental development and forms the theoretical
framework of this study. Literature from the broad field of developmental research is
reviewed and applied to human figure drawing tests and the Griffiths Mental Development
Chapter 3 presents a discussion in which the relationship between key constructs of the
clarified.
10
Chapter 4 describes the research methodology and the design utilised in this study. The
the research design, participants, sampling procedure, measures used and data analysis
Chapter 5 provides a presentation and a discussion of the research results of this study. These
results are discussed in the context of the conceptual frameworks presented in earlier chapters
Chapter 6 presents the conclusions to the present study based on the research results, reviews
limitations, and provides suggestions for further research in this area. The chapter concludes
1.7. Conclusion
This chapter provided an overview of the research. The motivation and rationale for the
research was presented. The history and uses of non-projective human figure drawing tests
children’s human figure drawings and that human figure drawings have been used to measure
developmental assessment in the South African context was then discussed. It was shown that
specific characteristics of human figure drawing tests may make such tests particularly useful
tools to identify developmental delays in the South African context. This was followed by a
discussion of the research objectives. The chapter concluded with a delineation of the
chapters that are to follow. In the following chapter, the theoretical foundations of this study
11
Chapter 2
2.1. Introduction
The historical development of human figure drawing (HFD) tests and the recognition
that aspects of human figure drawings are related to a child’s chronological and/or mental age
as well as the developmental progression seen in these drawings was briefly discussed in
Chapter 1. In this chapter relevant literature from the broad field of developmental research is
reviewed and applied to human figure drawing tests and the Griffiths Mental Development
Scales – Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). Important definitions are first provided to orientate
theory of cognitive development (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969); Vygotsky’s (1978) sociocultural
theory; Bruner’s (1960, 1966, 1986, 1990, 2006a; 2006b) theory of cognitive development;
& Eliot, 1993; Bensur, Eliot, & Hedge, 1997; Case, 1985; Cherney et al., 2006; Chi, 1977;
Kail, 1992; Kail & Hall, 1994; Kail & Salthouse, 1994; Schneider & Pressley, 1997; Scott,
1981; Zgourides, 2000). Similarities and differences between these views and the
implications of these theoretical contributions to the present study are discussed throughout.
The chapter provides the reader with a framework in which to understand the relationship
between development in general and the developmental progression seen in HFDs. To this
end, Luquet’s (1927) theory of drawing development is integrated with the discussion on
development in general.
Development has been defined as the “orderly and relatively enduring changes over
12
social interaction, and in many other behaviours” (Newcombe, 1996, p.4). Ruth Griffiths
adopted an inclusive view of development, which she defined as “the processes and rates at
which growth and maturation of a child’s attributes and abilities takes place” (Luiz et al.,
2006a, p.1). Di Leo (1970, p.4) defined development as "differentiation and increase in
increase in size and weight". Di Leo (1970, p. 4) notes that "these two aspects of maturation
are intimately related and begin inseparably, with life". Both Griffiths’ and Di Leo’s broad
definitions are not particularly helpful for the purposes of operationalization. A final
expansion, and alteration of mental processes from birth until death including sensory and
motor perception and control, all types of memory, consciousness, attention, analyzing,
thought” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 115). The authors of the most recent human figure drawing
test of intellectual ability, the DAP: IQ, state that “as children mature cognitively and their
thought processes become more complex, their drawings of a person become more detailed,
(2004). Luquet’s theory was chosen because it provides a useful introduction to the
other researchers in the field (Jolley, 2010). Jean Piaget, the Swiss developmental
psychologist and philosopher, incorporated Luquet’s theory of drawing development into his
own theory of cognitive development (Piaget & Inhelder, 1956, 1969). This provides an
invaluable theoretical link between the concepts of representational development and mental
13
after which specific developmental theories are reviewed and discussed in relation to human
1. that it allows one to understand changes that are apparently universal in all
cultures;
3. that it allows for a greater appreciation of the way that the environmental context
4. the study of development also provides a knowledge base that may be used in the
While all the reasons for studying development listed above are important, the fourth
is the most directly relevant for the purposes of the current study. An understanding of
developmental norms. This knowledge is useful in applied psychology as it allows for the
screening of children with developmental delays who may then receive appropriate
child development. The focus is on particular aspects of these theories that are most relevant
14
Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) theory of cognitive development remains one of
the most well-known and influential theories of child development (Slee & Shute, 2003).
This section provides an overview of the contributions made by Piaget that are most relevant
to the present study. Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) theory is first contextualized within
the constructivist framework. The key concepts that Piaget used to explain intellectual
discussion on Piaget concludes with the four factors he believed to underlie cognitive
development.
Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) theory is often described as a constructivist theory.
produced by the mind in interaction with the environment. Mental processes are then used in
the understanding of new information and the recall of the past (Matsumoto, 2009).
Philosophically, it owes much to the work of Berkeley and Kant who underlined the
subjective nature of perception (Slee & Shute, 2003). This label is applicable to Piaget’s
theory since the role of the child in actively constructing the external world through acting on
it is emphasized (Gelcer & Scharwtzbein, 1989). Gelcer and Schwartzbein highlighted two
levels of abstraction.
15
mainstream theories do, but rather propose that people actively participate in observation and
that meaning is co-created through this process (Slee & Shute, 2003). The theories of
Vygotsky and Bruner that follow also contain strong elements of constructivism. It must be
noted that while Piaget acknowledged the child’s active role in his own development, he did
not discount the role of biological maturation and that he regarded development in this sense
agreement with other constructivists, Piaget pitches his theory against the nativist assumption
that children are born with knowledge or ideas about reality or that adults teach them how to
think (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). Piaget’s constructivist argument proposes that children’s
understanding of the world is actively constructed on the basis of their experiences (Shaffer
& Kipp, 2010). In this process they “experiment with objects they encounter; they make
connections or associations between events; and they are puzzled when their current
understandings (or schemes) fail to explain what they have experienced” (Shaffer & Kipp,
2010, p. 54). Two mechanisms, namely assimilation and accommodation, underlie this
process of active involvement in their own intellectual development. Children make sense of
new experiences by “incorporating them into existing schemes” (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010, p.
54) – a process Piaget termed assimilation. Conversely, children may adapt their existing
cognitive schemes to allow the incorporation of new experiences - which Piaget called
refers to a state where “a balanced, harmonious relationship [exists] between one’s cognitive
structures and the environment” (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010, p. 54), whereas disequilibrium is
16
events” (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010, p. 54). A child will use accommodation (that is, modify his
existing schemes) when assimilation is not effective in the incorporation of new experiences
into current cognitive schemes (that is, when the facts contradict a child’s current
Piaget’s contributions is his description of the stages of cognitive development. Piaget’s four
stages are: (a) sensorimotor, (b) preoperational thought, (c) concrete operations, and (d)
formal operations. Inhelder (1962) described several characteristics of Piaget’s stage theory,
namely: Piaget’s theory describes stages in which mental operations form and become ever
more organized over time; these stages are hierarchical, with each stage building on
preceding stages; individuals show similarities when reaching each stage; and the stages
progress in a fixed direction from lower complexity to higher complexity. These stages are
2.3.2.1. Sensorimotor stage. In this first stage from birth to two years of age infants
learn through the use of their senses and develop motor control by doing, that is, by actively
exploring and interacting with their environment (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000).
Early causal reasoning develops during this stage, as infants progress from simply using their
referring to the realization that objects still exist even if they cannot be seen, is generally
developed around the age of nine months (Zgourides, 2000). The end of this stage is marked
by the development of symbolization: the process of using mental symbols and words as
mental representations that signify real objects (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). This is the
17
2.3.2.2. Preoperational thought. The stage of preoperational thought spans the ages
of two to seven years (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). This stage is of particular
importance as participants in the current study are in this developmental stage. There is an
increase in symbolic thought, namely thought that relies on the use of language and other
symbols (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). Children’s concepts during this stage
are still primitive, for example, they know the names of objects but cannot classify objects by
grouping them according to features (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). The
advances in language use that occur at this stage play an important role in drawing
development (Toomela, 2002). The ability to name objects helps a child to select which
aspects to include in his drawing, whereas self-directed speech helps the child to plan
Piaget’s description of the semiotic function that develops during the preoperational
period is of particular relevance to the present study (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). The semiotic
function refers to a new ability that allows children to represent things, including objects,
events, or conceptual schemes, by means of a signifier (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). Drawing is
an example of this ability to use signs and symbols to refer to something else (Sadock &
Sadock, 2007). The child’s proficiency in drawing a human figure is therefore dependent on
now operate on a new level by using concepts: “the mental representation of a thing or class
of things so that an individual can decide whether a specific stimulus is an instance of that
object or class of objects and act on the basis of that judgment” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 123).
The non-projective use of human figure drawings is based on the belief that human figure
drawings measure conceptual maturity (Harris, 1963). Harris (as cited in Cox, 1993) stated
that:
18
the child’s drawing of an object is an index of his concept of that object, and his
As Cherney, Seiwert, Dickey, and Flichtbeil (2006, p. 127) put it: “children’s
therefore be hypothesised that human figure drawings measure a child’s mental development
by providing an indication of the degree to which the child has developed the semiotic
function.
4 year olds refers to the initial stage during which a child scribbles without knowing that he
can use his drawings to represent reality (Luquet, 1927). During this scribbling stage the
child’s drawing is simply motor activity without any attempt to create a graphical
fits with the absence of the ability to use symbols as would be predicted by Piaget’s
According to Luquet (1927) a child will eventually notice a likeness between his own
scribbles and an aspect of the world, called fortuitous realism. This new knowledge that he
can represent life, subsequently typifies his future drawing development insofar as he
increasingly attempts to create representational drawings from the outset (Luquet, 1927).
Luquet’s (1927) second stage of drawing development which spans the ages of 4 to 7
years is known as the pre-schematic stage. A child at this stage attempts to represent objects
in his drawings, but his increasingly representational drawings are hampered by difficulties
with motor, cognitive, and graphic obstacles. Errors are frequent, including such errors as
19
incorrect use of line (due to poor motor control), omission of details (due to poor attention),
problems of position, orientation and proportion, and inaccurate relations between parts of the
drawing (Luquet, 1927). Intellectual realism develops towards the end of this stage (Luquet,
1927). At this stage a child aims to represent as many as possible of the essential details of a
subject in their characteristic shapes. Jolley (2010, p. 17) states that a child at this stage of
development draws “from what he or she knows about the topic, not from what he or she sees
from one viewpoint”. The child’s concept of what he is drawing is therefore crucially related
to the quality of his drawing output. With regard to human figure drawings, it is at this point
where the body appears (that is, not a tadpole drawing anymore), the amount of details
increase, and the relationship between the details becomes increasingly accurate. According
to Luquet (1927) children at this stage are not trying to represent a subject as seen from a
certain angle, but rather are relying on an internal model constituted of features the child
believes to be important to the subject at hand. These internal models become increasingly
The human figure drawings used in the present study were produced by children in
this age range. The growing ability to create representational drawings that develops during
this stage is consistent with Piaget’s and Inhelder’s (1969) proposal of the emergence of the
semiotic function and the rapid development of representational thought that occur during the
preoperational stage.
2.3.2.3. Concrete operations. The stage of concrete operations spans the ages of 7 to
11 years (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). This age range exceeds the upper age
limit of the present study, and as such is not as relevant to the present study as the preceding
stages. During this stage children develop a more complete understanding of causal
relationships, become more systematic in their thinking, and develop the ability to take on the
perspective of others (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). The emergence of concrete
20
operations marks “logical thought about specific objects or situations, which involves letting
terms” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 124). With this increase in logical thought, children attain
conservation, that is, they come to realise that if the shape of an object changes other
characteristics stay the same and it does not become a different object altogether (Sadock &
Sadock, 2007). A better understanding of the relation between things allow them to recognize
that one thing can take different forms or turn into something else and back again such as is
the case with water and ice (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). Children’s thinking is still concrete at
this stage, but the increase in abstract thought and emergence of conservation and
reversibility distinguish this stage from its predecessor (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides,
2000).
Luquet’s (1927) theory of drawing development refers to children between the ages of
7 and 9 years as being in the schematic stage. Visual realism emerges when the child at this
level of drawing development realises that although his drawings include the important
features of his subjects, they do not look realistic (Luquet, 1927). Such a child abandons
“separation, transparency, plan, and folding-out techniques, and instead they begin to get to
grips with the graphic techniques of visual realism that include occlusion, suppression of
details, and perspective” (Jolley, 2010, p. 18). The use of such techniques is more cognitively
demanding than earlier, simpler drawing styles. As such, a higher level of cognitive
development would be necessary for the successful progression to this stage of visual realism.
The child now attempts to draw visual models, rather than internal models. Cherney et al.
(2006) support the progression described by Luquet (1927), stating that the greater amount of
distinctive features included in children’s drawings with advancing age lends a quality of
21
2.3.2.4. Formal operations. Piaget’s final stage, that of formal operations, stretches
from the child’s 11th year past the end of adolescence (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). This stage is
characterized by ever more logical, systematic, and symbolic thought, that allow for the
2009; Sadock & Sadock, 2007). The most organized forms of cognition appear during this
stage (Matsumoto, 2009; Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). Hypothetico-deductive
reasoning involves the ability to form and test hypotheses (Matsumoto, 2009; Sadock &
Sadock, 2007). Individuals in this stage are also able to perform scientific-inductive
(Matumoto, 2009). The ages at which the different developmental stages appear are
approximate and not all individuals reach the stage of formal operational thought (Sadock &
Sadock, 2007).
the stages of intellectual development described above, Piaget also attempted to explain the
processes that account for this developmental progression. Piaget (as cited in Slee & Shute,
2003) outlined four factors that underlie cognitive development, namely (a) maturation; (b)
experience; (c) social transmission; and (d) equilibration. These four factors are now
discussed in turn.
begins before birth and extends long into life, allows for the unfolding of increasingly
2.3.3.2. Experience. Piaget divides experience into direct physical experience and
mathematical experience. The prior involves experience that originates directly through the
use of the five senses, whereas the latter involves logico-mathematical experience that arises
22
from the child acting on the world instead of from direct experience in itself (Slee & Shute,
2003).
2.3.3.3. Social transmission. Piaget regarded the child’s interaction with the physical
world to be the main impetus of development, but he also acknowledged the role of
interaction with other children and adults as motivator for development (Slee & Shute, 2003).
In such instances the child has to decentre in order to make sense of conflicting ideas (Slee &
Shute, 2003).
2.3.3.4. Equilibration. Through this most basic underlying factor the child obtains a
new balance between previously understood information and new experience (Slee & Shute,
The broad factors that Piaget believed to account for cognitive development illustrate
the fact that a multitude of influences impact on cognitive development. A discussion of all of
these influences would fill several books and is well beyond the scope of the present study.
The most directly relevant of these were therefore included in the discussion. Specific
theories of development focus more heavily on some of these factors than others, but the
reader is encouraged to bear in mind that a holistic view that takes into account as many as
possible of these influences will lead to the most thorough understanding of the complex
process of development. This concludes the section on Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969)
theory and the discussion now turns to Vygotsky’s (1978) sociocultural theory.
This section discusses the theory of the Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky. Important
aspects of his theory such as cultural mediation, the centrality of language and social
interaction in cognitive development, and his concepts of elementary and higher mental
23
functions are discussed. His concept of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) is also
discussed and applied to the two measures used in the present study. The section concludes
with a description of key differences between the theories of Piaget and Vygotsky.
development by affording a central role to society and culture as forces that shape cognitive
experimentation on the world, Vygotsky (1978) contended that social interaction forms the
interweaving of biological development of the human body and the appropriation of the
cultural/ideal/material heritage which exists in the present to coordinate people with each
other and the physical world”. This statement does justice to Vygotsky’s emphasis on the
primacy of social interaction in development, but does not neglect the essential contribution
incorporate each new perspective into his or her understanding of child development as these
are discussed. While consolidating such a broad body of work is admittedly no easy task,
such a creative synthesis will allow the reader a more thorough understanding of child
development that goes beyond the limits of dogmatic adherence to any one theory.
individual to adapt to his environment, Vygotsky argues from the opposite direction: that
cognitive development begins in social interaction and that these social functions are then
internalized and converted into mental functions (Driscoll, 2000). Vygotsky (1978) stated
that:
24
every function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, between people
equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of ideas. All
the higher functions originate as actual relationships between individuals. (p. 57)
2.4.1. Elementary and higher mental functions. Vygotsky (1978) drew a distinction
between elementary and higher mental functions. Elementary mental functions are innate and
include such abilities as attention, sensation, perception and memory (Vygotsky, 1978).
Higher mental functions, on the other hand, refer to the creation and use of abilities such as
Vygotsky (1978) proposed that culture transforms the elementary mental functions to give
rise to more sophisticated higher mental functions. These higher mental functions are the
tools of the culture in which the individual lives. He believed that higher mental functions
originate externally in social occurrences and that they are only later integrated into a
person’s thinking by means of language (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). In this way society determines
what is to be learned and how it is to be learned, thus society is the main influence behind
This shows that Vygotsky (1978) viewed social interaction not only as one important
factor in cognitive development, but as the central force behind cognitive development.
Vygotsky’s (1978) central tenet is therefore that learning occurs as a result of social
interaction that transforms elementary functions into higher-level functions, thus allowing the
development of more complex thought through the use of signs and symbols.
and higher mental functions. He suggested that elementary mental functions are innate while
higher mental functions are socially mediated. It would be incorrect to understand this as
25
meaning that higher mental functions do not also rely on biological maturation. It is known
that any learning corresponds to physiological changes. The point to keep in mind here is that
such phenomena as learning may be investigated at different levels of abstraction and that
investigators may choose to focus on neuronal pathways, metabolic activity in the brain,
willed behaviour, or on any of a number of contexts from the micro to the macro-sociological
context, to name but a few influences on learning. These different perspectives do not
necessarily negate one another and together allow for a better understanding of development.
Vygotsky viewed language as the most important cultural tool that allows the
development of thought (Slee & Shute, 2003). Vygotsky also observed qualitatively different
developmental stages (Slee & Shute, 2003). He highlighted the importance of speech and
thought coming together at around the age of two years, which forms the basis of future
conceptual development, and believed that proper abstract thought appears in adolescence,
but that the more concrete thought of previous stages remains present (Slee & Shute, 2003).
Vygotsky’s ideas in this regard are very similar to those of Piaget discussed earlier. Although
both theorists proposed developmental stages, Piaget’s stage theory is more widely
recognition of the role of social interaction and language in cognitive development (Slee &
Shute, 2003). Language is a symbolic system that relies on representational thought. Human
figure drawings also rely on representational thought and may be of value to gauge the
child’s ability to think in terms of abstract symbols that refer to things and his level of
offers valuable insight into children’s drawings. He proposed that children in early stages of
drawing development draw from memory, that is, from what they know instead of from what
they see. He noted that such children would not only ignore perceptual information of the
26
object in front of them, but that they in fact contradict such information (Vygotsky, 1978).
This observation supports Luquet’s (1927) assertion that drawing development progresses
from intellectual realism to visual realism. It also underlines the importance of memory in the
creation of graphic representations as well as the fact that internal concepts are essential for
(1986, p. 149) wrote: concept formation “is a complex and genuine act of thought that cannot
be taught by drilling, but can be accomplished only when the children’s mental development
itself has reached the requisite level”. Here again, mental development appears to be closely
of language in drawing. Vygotsky (as cited in Toomela, 2002) suggested that a direct
relationship between stimulus and response is presupposed in elementary functions but that
this direct link can be semiotically mediated by signs such as verbal language. In this way
“language selects and reorganises information and directs attention to significant aspects of a
situation” (Toomela, 2002, p. 235). Toomela further argues that words are tools that allow the
child to choose which aspects of the object to represent in his drawing. Toomela uses the
example that if a child only has a word (or concept) of a dog, he can differentiate between
dogs and other animals, but only when he learns words such as ‘collie’ and ‘poodle’ the child
becomes able to appreciate the subtle differences between these objects and represent them in
his drawing. Vygotsky’s notion of language as a cultural tool that originates in the social
context and allows for cognitive development when internalized is therefore very useful in
proposition that culture transforms elementary mental functions into higher mental functions.
As the child learns words such as ‘dog’, ‘collie’ and ‘poodle’ through social interaction and
27
internalizes them by means of language, his developing conceptual maturity allows for ever
The link between drawing and language suggested by Toomela (2002) corresponds to
Vygosky’s (1978) description of drawing as graphic speech which is based on verbal speech
and precedes the development of written language. This discussion further illustrates the
of the semiotic function discussed earlier in relation to Piaget’s preoperational stage. As Ahn
(2007) notes, Piaget and Vygotsky both regard drawings as reflections of children’s
representational abilities. One may understand representational thought as resulting from the
argued, or from the development of higher mental functions as a result of the internalization
hypothesized that children’s human figure drawings offer an indication of their development
2.4.3. The zone of proximal development (ZPD). Another famous concept that
originated with Vygotsky is the zone of proximal development (ZPD). The ZPD refers to the
Vygotsky (1978) suggested that at any point in development there are things that an
individual can do without help, things that he or she can only do when helped by another, and
things that he or she still cannot do even with the help of others. The ZPD is the gap between
an individual’s actual developmental level – those things he can do without assistance, and
the level of potential – the things an individual can do with guidance or support by others that
are more proficient in the task at hand (Vygotsky, 1978). In the following section the concept
of the zone of proximal development is applied to the two measures used in the present study.
28
performance. Several measures are taken toward this end. Throughout the administration of
the measure, the child is encouraged to perform as well as he possibly can. The test
administrator will first determine a basal, this being a set of six consecutively passed items
(Luiz et al., 2006a). Items are then administered in order of progressing difficulty until the
child fails six consecutive items and a ceiling is thus established (Luiz et al., 2006a). The
basal helps to ensure that the child is credited for items early in the test that he can reasonably
be expected to have mastered granted his developmental level. The ceiling provides him the
opportunity to demonstrate his abilities in items that he has only recently mastered or is
beginning to master.
The items that make up each particular child’s basal and the preceding items can be
These items correspond to tasks the child has already mastered and can perform without
assistance. As test items become more difficult, the child progresses toward the higher end of
the zone of proximal development until he finally reaches his ceiling. Since the test
administrator may usually only encourage the child and is generally not allowed to actually
help him, the upper end of the child’s performance on the GMDS-ER is more an indication of
tasks the child can perform without assistance than tasks he can perform with some assistance
or that he cannot master even if helped by others. The child’s performance on the GMDS-ER
therefore does not necessarily reflect the upper end of the child’s zone of proximal
development.
The GMDS-ER does however include items such as “Washes own hands and face,
with some assistance” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 35). These items measure the middle range of
Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development. The administration instructions for some items on
the GMDS-ER, such as “Builds a tower of 8+ bricks” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 52), require that
29
the test administrator demonstrate the required task to the child before the child attempts it. In
this item, for example, the examiner may help the child by pointing out where the bricks
should be placed. Some items on the GMDS-ER allow the child two chances to pass the task,
while others such as “4-squares board: 50 seconds” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 69) provides the
opportunity for the child to improve his performance with a second attempt. In this timed
task, for example, the best of the two scores is recorded, thus allowing for the child to
demonstrate his best performance. In cases where the examiner may help the child, the test
items measure the middle range of Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development. Failing these
items reflects the upper limits of the zone of proximal development, namely those tasks that
With regards to tests of HFD’s the standardized administration of the DAP: IQ does
not permit the administrator to encourage the child or to provide him with guidance or
assistance while he is completing his human figure drawing. This task therefore measures the
lower range of the zone of proximal development, namely tasks the child can complete
without assistance. This does not however imply that this test measures only the most
developed abilities of the child, thereby underestimating his developmental level. Norm
tables are used to obtain standardized test scores and as such the child is positioned relative to
the performance of his peers. This overcomes the apparent problem posed by using a task
where successful completion would indicate mastery in the lower range of the zone of
proximal development.
On the other hand in the standardized administration of the GMDS-ER the examiner
initiates the ‘Draws a person’ item or task by saying to the child: “I want you to draw a man”
(Luiz et. al., 2006a, p. 53). The instructions further state “encourage the child to draw the best
possible person” (Luiz et. al., 2006a, p. 53). The degree to which the examiner is allowed to
assist the child in this task is not clear from the standardized instructions, and the
30
administration of this item does therefore not conform to the DAP: IQ standardized
instructions. The most likely interpretation of the GMDS-ER ‘Draws a person’ instructions is
that the examiner must be supportive, but not directive. Instructing the child to include
features such as eyes or arms would in most cases mean that the child would pass later
‘Draws a person’ items that he might not pass without these instructions. Such instructions
would therefore defeat the purpose of these items. It may therefore be concluded that
‘encourage’ does not mean to give specific advice to the child. The human figure drawings
obtained from the GMDS-ER protocols therefore provide an indication of the child’s best
effort to produce a human figure drawing, based on his concept of a person. It follows that
although the GMDS-ER ‘Draws a person’ instructions differ from the DAP: IQ instructions,
2.4.4. Differences between the theories of Vygotsky and Piaget. Vygotsky’s (1978)
social cognitive theory of development departs from Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969)
theory in several respects. Firstly, Vygotsky affords a central role to culture and social
interaction in the process of cognitive development, whereas Piaget’s theory tends to depict
the child as a more autonomous agent that realizes his own development through interaction
with his environment. Vygotsky’s theory also breaks away from the defined milestones set
out in Piaget’s stage theory and instead promotes the view that there always remains some
ongoing process without a final destination (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). The discussion now turns to
The work of the American psychologist and educator Jerome Bruner will now be
discussed. Key assumptions of Bruner’s theory are first presented. This is followed by a more
31
detailed discussion of his theory, focussing on such aspects as the role of culture and
representation; the spiral curriculum; and cognitive readiness. Throughout the discussion,
these contributions are compared to the ideas of Piaget and Vygotsky presented in the
The first key assumption of Bruner’s (1960, 1966, 1986, 1990, 2006a; 2006b) theory
is that meaning is actively constructed from the world and there is no objective reality that
exists independently of human mental activity and symbolic language (Bruner, 1986). The
implication of this metaphysical position is evident in the observation that the same event can
hold different meanings to different individuals (Slee & Shute, 2003). Secondly, he argued
that development can never occur independently of an individual’s cultural and historical
context (Bruner, 1986). The third assumption inherent in his work is that children play an
active role in constructing their world, but do not do so in isolation (Slee & Shute, 2003).
Instead, it is through social interaction that the child “acquires a framework for interpreting
experience, and learns how to negotiate meaning in a manner congruent with the
requirements of a culture” (Bruner & Haste, 1987, p. 1) and that in this way, “culture gives
shape to our thoughts” (Bruner, 2006b, p. 4). Bruner’s ideas can be seen to occupy a position
between the Piagetian emphasis of the child’s active role in his own development and the
instruction. From this perspective language is seen as a prerequisite of thought (Slee & Shute,
32
that form part of development, namely: enactive, iconic and symbolic. These modes of
statement that their development “is in that order, each depending upon the previous one for
its development, yet all of them remaining more or less intact throughout life.”
Piaget’s sensorimotor period (Slee & Shute, 2003). Bruner (1966, p. 17) agrees with Piaget
(1954) that during this stage things are “lived rather than thought”. The non-representational
nature of scribbling in children’s early drawings reflects the fact that early drawing is based
2.5.1.2. Iconic representation. The second stage, known as the stage of iconic
representation, involves representing the world through a mental image (Bruner, 1966).
According to Bruner (1966, p. 21), this stage begins when “a child is finally able to represent
the world to himself by an image or spatial schema that is relatively independent of action”.
Such a mental image is a cognitive representation that represents a body of knowledge (Slee
& Shute, 2003). Iconic representation corresponds to the earlier period of Piaget’s
preoperational stage. The emergence of the semiotic function - namely the ability to represent
things by means of a signifier - which occurs later in this Piagetian stage marks the transition
from an iconic mode of representation to the next stage: that of symbolic representation.
symbolic representation, are able to use symbols to represent their experience of the world.
This type of representation is more developed than that found during iconic representation
because symbols such as words can be used that do not resemble their referent, whereas
iconic representations such as pictures are more directly related to their referents (Slee &
33
Shute, 2003). Bruner (1966) believed that children’s growing ability to use language is
participants in the present study would have relied on enactive and iconic representation and
some would have reached symbolic representation. It should be noted that although the task
of drawing a human figure at first seems to rely on iconic representation insofar as the figure
discussed in the section on Vygotsky, the symbolic system of language serves a mediating
function between stimulus and response and helps the child to plan and organize his activity.
A child who is able to use symbolic representation may therefore be expected to provide a
more detailed, complex, and organized human figure drawing than one who relies solely on
Bruner’s theory was strongly influenced by the work of Piaget and Vygotsky (Slee &
Shute, 2003). Bruner (1986, 1990) agrees with Piaget’s concept of cognitive development as
occurring in progressive stages, with each successive stage building on earlier stages. He also
Piaget. Bruner believes that children develop a representational system and that learning
occurs by comparing new stimuli with existing mental structures (Lutz & Huitt, 2004).
from this ongoing comparison (Driscoll, 2000; Lutz & Huitt, 2004). This aspect of Bruner’s
Piaget.
is a central concept in the present study. Bruner and colleagues (Bruner, Oliver & Greenfield,
34
sophisticated representations of the world rather than by the gradual acquisition of separately
identifiable skills that do not necessarily occur in a sequence as the process unfolds”.
Lutz and Huitt (2004) contend that Bruner’s theories may also be linked to
information processing theories in that he believes development to rely on the elaboration and
increased sophistication of mental structures that occurs with interaction and experience. His
theory also places importance on the role of culture in development. In this sense his beliefs
are similar to that of Vygotsky (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). Vygotsky’s influence on Bruner’s views
may be seen in his assertion that children are social beings whose competences “are
interwoven with the competences of others” (Bruner, 1986, p. 11). Bruner emphasized the
role of culture in development, stating that cognitive growth is shaped as much “from the
outside in as the inside out” (Bruner, 1966, p. 13). Furthermore, he highlighted that humans
are well suited to adapt to their environment by social means instead of morphological means
(Bruner, 1986).
2.5.2. The spiral curriculum. Bruner also introduced the concept of a ‘spiral
curriculum’. This term refers to fitting the instructional method to students’ developmental
level and then revisiting this material in more complex forms as the individual’s cognitive
abilities develop (Bruner, 1960). Referring to the spiral curriculum, Bruner (1966, p. 44)
stated that “any idea or problem or body of knowledge can be presented in a form simple
enough so that any particular learner can understand it in a recognizable form” and that “a
curriculum as it develops should revisit these basic ideas repeatedly, building upon them until
the learner has grasped the full formal apparatus that goes with them” (Bruner, 1960, p. 13).
Lutz and Huitt (2004) propose that the principal difference between Bruner (1986)
and Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) theories center around the notion of cognitive
readiness. Piaget argued that learning and development are determined by an individual’s age
35
and level of biological maturation, whereas Bruner argued that selected aspects of any
material can be taught to any child (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). This claim is moderated, however,
by adding that it will probably be necessary that this content and material receive additional
attention as the individual attains more knowledge and capacity (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). These
two viewpoints are therefore not necessarily mutually exclusive: while learning may occur if
instruction is tailored to take the individual’s developmental level into account, as Bruner
argued, Piaget’s view cannot be discounted since the individual’s capacity to deal with
increasingly complex material increases as cognitive and biological structures mature and
between neuronal myelination and stage progression as discussed later in this study.
A striking difference between Piaget and Bruner’s theories lies in the notion of
progression. Piaget’s (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) theory is clearly a stage theory, with
however, that Piaget made provision for overlap between stages, and that the stage element of
development. He argued against the Piagetian idea that cognitive development unfolds in a
uniform manner that is unaffected by cultural or social differences. Instead, he held that
(Bruner as cited in Driscoll 2000, p. 236). Lutz and Huitt (2004) make a strong point by
arguing that if the tools of a culture are different, categorization and representation would
also be different. This would in turn mean that the skills and types of knowledge required at
different ages would not be the same as in other cultures. This brings into dispute the validity
of the notion of invariant developmental stages as proposed by some stage theorists (Lutz &
Huitt, 2004). Empirical research substantiates Bruner’s claim that exact developmental stages
36
do not occur and that development does not progress uniformly regardless of cultural and
societal differences (Renner et al., 1976). This is important for the present study, as it means
that the items on the GMDS-ER and the act of drawing a human figure are not independent of
culture. Children who have not been exposed to bicycles may have difficulty with item
‘AIV.13 Rides a bicycle (two-wheeler)’, for example, and children who have not been taught
producing a human figure drawing than those who have had practice in the medium.
and Bruner is widely acknowledged. Experience, especially schooling, and contextual factors
included in intelligence tests (Jansen & Greenop, 2008; Nel, 2000). Sperber and Hirschfeld
(1999, p. cxxvi) claim that it is generally accepted “that cultural factors enable, constrain, and
channel the development of certain cognitive outcomes” and that while “some cultural
insofar as it opens readers to question the existence of one objective reality. While the
implications afforded by this perspective can be discussed at length, it is sufficient for the
1. The construct of ‘normal development’ is socially constructed and the same term
2. Culture plays a critical role in determining what is learnt and how this is learnt,
37
The stage progression that Piaget and Bruner described in connection with cognitive
development and Luquet described in children’s drawings of the human figure is partly
dependent on the biological maturation of the brain and nervous system. Such maturation
allows children to form “increasingly complex cognitive schemes that help them to construct
better understandings of what they have experienced” (Piaget as cited in Shaffer & Kipp,
2010, p. 55). This illustrates the interplay between biological growth and children’s active
role in their own development. Over time, if all goes well, this process allows “curious, active
children, who are always forming new schemes and reorganizing their knowledge” to
“progress far enough to think about old issues in entirely new ways; that is, they pass from
one stage of cognitive development to the next higher stage” (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010, p. 54).
development. The following section provides an overview of some of the most important
It has been hypothesized that there is an increase in processing speed (Kail &
Salthouse, 1994), working memory (Chi, 1977), and “increased dimensional complexity of
development (Cherney et al., 2006). Memory performance and cognitive development may
(Schneider & Pressley, 1997). Many of the cognitive processes measured by tests of mental
38
understand, use, and produce symbols” (Cherney et al., 2006, p. 137) underlies the capacity
from the referent, manipulating and transforming information, as well as other skills
(Cherney et al., 2006). Children are able to create increasingly complex drawings as they
grow older and this growing representational complexity is associated with increases in
working memory capacity (Bensur, Eliot, & Hedge 1997; Cherney et al. 2006; Scott, 1981).
Case (1985) suggested that increased cognitive capacity (that is, working memory) allows
children to differentiate between representations with greater ease. This allows for the
representations such as clothed persons with detailed facial features (Cherney et al., 2006).
Increased attention is another mechanism that accounts for the developmental nature of
human figure drawings as seen in Luquet’s (1927) suggestion that the child’s increased
The increased complexity with development that occurs in children’s drawings where
1. the child’s ability to pay attention to objects’ appearance irrespective of the child’s
2. the child’s capacity for recalling figural configurations and holding them in
Such biological maturational and cognitive mechanisms may account for the
39
the GMDS-ER and the items of intelligence tests. The neo-Piagetian theorist Pascual-Leone
(2000, p. 843), for example, proposes that “[t]he concept of mental-processing capacity […]
growth and thus developmental stages”. This author further contends that developmental
stages do not occur as a result of changes in the structure of children’s logic (as Piaget argued
in his work The Psychology of Intelligence), but rather that “stages index the endogenous
843). This “mental-attention capacity appears as a set of innate resources growing in power
with chronological age until adolescence” (Pascual-Leone, 2000, p.844) and is more narrowly
defined than working memory since it excludes situational learning. The development of
mental-attention capacity (which allows the processing of complexity) and learning both
child’s learning potential, which in turn allows for the emergence of Piagetian stages.
Kail (1992) suggests that the increase in the speed with which the mature brain and
nervous system can process information accounts for the important role of biological
maturation in cognitive growth. This gradual improvement that occurs with maturation
explains age-related improvement in diverse tasks. Zgourides (2000) observes that the
attainment of concrete operations, Piaget’s third stage discussed earlier, around the age of 7 is
dependent on the maturation of the brain and nervous systems at this age and that the increase
in neural connections allows children to progress to more advanced ways of thinking. The
importance of this fundamental mechanism is illustrated by Kail and Hall’s (1994) assertion
that age has a direct effect on global processing speed; global processing speed then has a
direct effect on naming speed, and naming speed in turn influences performance on word
recognition tasks. This increase in processing speed that occurs with age may be expected to
40
manifest most prominently in timed tasks. HFD tests and most items on the GMDS-ER are
not timed. It may be hypothesized that this improvement spurs cognitive development by
allowing children to process information more effectively thereby speeding up the processes
The research discussed above serves to illustrate the growing acceptance within the
provides a better fit with reality than either absolute position taken alone. Nativism, in
contrast to constructivism, refers to the idea that intellectual abilities and thought patterns are
inborn (Matsumoto, 2009). Luquet (1927) appreciated the role that increased attention and
the child’s growing ability to hold important aspects of the subject in mind plays in the
recent developments that link cognitive factors such as attention and working memory to
cognitive mechanisms such as those suggested by Pascual-Leone’s work (1970; 1978; 1980)
may therefore account for developmental change in human figure drawings as well as
2.7. Conclusion
This chapter provided a review of literature from the broad field of developmental
psychology and discussed the relevance of these contributions to human figure drawing tests
and the GMDS-ER. Important definitions were provided to orientate the reader and the
importance of studying child development was discussed. This was followed by a discussion
41
tests and the GMDS-ER. Luquet’s theory of drawing development was integrated into the
discussion as it provided a valuable theoretical link between Piaget’s theory and the study of
human figure drawings. The following chapter provides a discussion of the key constructs
relevant to the present study, namely intelligence, conceptual maturity and mental
development.
42
Chapter 3
3.1. Introduction
It has been suggested that “any description of child development must include in the
discussion a review of the concept of intelligence” (Stewart, 2005, p. 97). A discussion of the
relationship between the key constructs of the present study is included because it adds depth
to the theoretical foundations of the study, assists in the contextualization of the key
constructs, and provides further motivation for the research objective of exploring the
relationship between human figure drawing test scores and Griffiths Mental Development
Scales – Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) general quotients. This chapter clarifies the
particular, the psychometric approach to intelligence is described and related to human figure
drawing tests and the GMDS-ER. The concepts of academic skills disorders, learning
disorders, and developmental delays are then discussed and it is proposed that human figure
psychometric evidence of the overlap between scores on human figure drawing tests,
intelligence tests, and developmental tests. The chapter concludes with a discussion of a
The study of children’s drawings has been closely linked to the psychometric study of
intelligence from the outset (Bekhit, Thomas & Jolley, 2005). Authors such as Burt (1921)
incorporated drawing into general IQ tests, while Bekhit, Thomas and Jolley (2005) give the
Draw-a-Man test (Goodenough, 1926) the accolade of being the first dedicated IQ test. It was
Harris (1963) who reintroduced the notion of development into human figure drawing tests
by stating that his revision of Goodenough’s test was a measure of conceptual maturity. The
43
scope of the current study does not permit a comprehensive discussion of the nature of
intelligence, but the concept of intelligence is now discussed in light of the substantial
theories. Moreover, such concepts are often linked exclusively to particular theories or
it is into this category that the GMDS-ER falls (Jakins, 2009). The psychometric approach
was also chosen because it is currently the dominant perspective in the study of intelligence,
has generated the largest amount of research, received the most attention, and is the main
approach used in practical settings (Neisser et al., 1996). Intelligence as understood from the
psychometric perspective is broadly defined as “how well one scores on an intelligence test”
(Berg, 2000, p. 120). In the psychometric approach the nature of intelligence is examined by
Factors that do not cluster together or that follow different developmental trajectories are
believed to indicate distinct abilities (Berg, 2000). Berg focuses on three such distinct
information and think in flexible ways. Fluid intelligence also includes the ability to under-
stand and make conceptual relations” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 208). Fluid intelligence decreases
during adulthood (Matsumoto, 2009). Psychometric measures tap fluid intelligence through
tasks that require an individual to adapt to a novel situation and tasks in which earlier
experience is of little benefit (Berg, 2000). As noted earlier, Harris (1963) believed HFDs to
44
measure conceptual maturity, which he defined as “the ability to perceive and discriminate
similarities and differences, the ability to abstract these, and the ability to generalize or
classify objects correctly” (Harris as cited in Cox, 1993, p. 70). The ability to think in abstract
terms and to work with concepts that is an important aspect of fluid intelligence is therefore
dependent on conceptual maturity. Based on this connection, one might hypothesize that
human figure drawings measure fluid intelligence by gaining an indication of the level of
conceptual maturity that underlies this ability. The earlier section covering Piaget’s
human figure drawings and conceptual maturity therefore not only helps one to understand
the mechanism by which such tests measure intelligence, but also supports the hypothesis that
exposure (Matsumoto, 2009). Psychometric measures use items that draw on problem solving
The ability to think in abstract terms and manipulate concepts in novel situations form part of
fluid intelligence, but the knowledge base that relies on the storage of representations in long-
everyday functioning, although the extent to which this ability reflects crystallized and fluid
intelligence is still under debate (Berg, 2000). The concept is most often used in relation to
45
best conceptualized in terms of three distinct factors, namely Fluid Intelligence, Crystalized
Intelligence, and Everyday Intelligence as discussed above. Hunt (2005, p. 6) asserts that “the
one-factor versus multifactor debate has very largely been settled”. He justifies this statement
by arguing that Carrol (1993) demonstrated that a three-layer model provides the best fit to
the psychometric data. Hunt (2005) goes on to state that Carrol’s model is very similar to
Cattell (1971) and Horn’s (Horn, 1985; Horn & Noll, 1994) model. The latter model is
comprised of three factors, namely (a) Fluid Intelligence, (b) Crystallized Intelligence; and
(c) Spatial-Visual Intelligence (Hunt, 2005). This model differs from the one proposed by
Berg (2000) discussed above. While both models acknowledge fluid intelligence and
crystallized intelligence, Berg includes everyday intelligence whereas the Cattell-Horn model
replaces this ability with visual-spatial intelligence, defined as “the ability to reason spatially”
(Hunt, 2005, p. 6). The American Psychological Association (APA) established a task force
to consolidate research on the nature of intelligence and released a report stating that the most
intelligence (g) at the apex (Neisser et al., 1996).The point here is to show that although some
consensus has been reached, psychometric conceptions of intelligence differ and do not
(1927) g factor. This general factor believed to underlie performance on different cognitive
tasks was discovered through the statistical technique of factor analysis. Although g is well
established within the psychometric approach it remains a murky concept. As Neisser et al.
(1996) put it in their report for the American Psychological Association’s taskforce on
intelligence:
46
one common view today envisages something like a hierarchy of factors with g at the
apex, but there is no full agreement on what g actually measures: it has been described
measure of neural processing speed (Reed & Jensen, 1992). (p. 78)
The general quotient (GQ) of the Griffiths scales was developed by Ruth Griffiths
(1954, 1970, 1984) in accordance with the notion of an underlying general factor (Stewart,
2005). Research supports the view that the GMDS-ER measures a single underlying factor
(Luiz, Foxcroft, & Stewart, 2001a; Munro, 1968). HFD tests of intellectual ability are
similarly based on the belief that performance on a certain task, such as drawing a human
figure, will provide an estimate of overall intelligence (or g). The GMDS-ER is based on the
developing the scales she had to “cast a wide net to include a large number of different
of its manifestations” (Griffiths, 1954, p.31). This clearly demonstrates that although the
GMDS-ER is a developmental measure, and not an intelligence test, Ruth Griffiths believed
it to measure intelligence. The reader is reminded of the striking similarity between the
Griffiths believed her scales of mental development measure intelligence, perhaps it would
not be surprising to find that HFD tests purported to measure intellectual ability provide an
Academic skills disorders, also referred to as learning disorders, exist when “an
47
reading, mathematics, or writing, is substantially below what would be expected for the age,
schooling, and level of intelligence for that individual” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 4). Learning
disorders differ from mental retardation as the achievement deficit in learning disorder is not
the result of a lack of intelligence (Matsumoto, 2009). The DSM-IV-TR groups these
disorders into four diagnostic categories, namely: reading disorder, mathematics disorder,
disorder of written expression, and learning disorder not otherwise specified (American
children with learning disorders usually find it exceptionally difficult to keep up with their
classmates by their third year (Sadock & Sadock, 2007). Learning disorders not only cause
psychological distress to an afflicted child, but may also lead to “demoralization, low self-
esteem, chronic frustration, and poor peer relationships” (Sadock & Sadock, 2007, p.1158).
Early diagnosis of such difficulties will allow such children to benefit from appropriate
intervention, thus reducing the risk of negative outcomes. Human figure drawing tests that
that expected from his HFD score. In this case the discrepancy between general intelligence
as measured by the HFD test and academic performance would warrant further assessment to
‘developmental delay’. This definition again illustrates the close association between the
concepts of intelligence and mental development. The term ‘developmental delay’ is often
used when referring to a deficit in mental developmental level, whereas the term ‘mental
48
retardation’ is more closely associated with the concept of intelligence, but both refer to the
same phenomenon. The DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) groups individuals with mental retardation
into four categories according to their IQ scores. These are (a) mild mental retardation (IQ
level of 50-55 to about 70); (b) moderate mental retardation (IQ level of 35-40 to 50-55); (c)
severe mental retardation (IQ level of 20-25 to 35-40) and (d) profound mental retardation
(IQ level below 20 or 25). Mild mental retardation may in some cases represent the low end
of the normal curve of intelligence, whereas more pronounced delays result from brain
developmental characteristics. Those with mild mental retardation frequently show small
developmental delays in the preschool years but are normally only identified when academic
or behavioural problems appear in the early elementary years (Mash & Wolfe, 2005). Such
individuals generally engage readily with peers, but may have moderate delays in expressive
language during their preschool years with no or only slight sensorimotor impairment (Mash
& Wolfe, 2005). Children with moderate mental retardation are normally identified due to
failure in achieving early developmental milestones in their preschool years (Mash & Wolfe,
2005). At school age, such children typically rely on single words and phrases to
communicate and their self-care and motor skills are comparable to that of a two to three year
old child without developmental delays (Mash & Wolfe, 2005). By adolescence, these
individuals regularly have poor relationships with peers as a result of trouble recognizing
social conventions such as appropriate dress and humour (Mash & Wolfe, 2005). Children
with severe mental retardation are usually identified at an early age due to substantial
developmental delays and visible physical features or abnormalities (Mash & Wolfe, 2005).
These children’s development is markedly delayed as seen in them reaching milestones such
as walking, standing, and toilet training much later than their peers (Mash & Wolfe, 2005).
49
Organic causes such as genetic defects normally underlie this form of mental retardation
(Mash & Wolfe, 2005). Individuals falling in the final category of profound mental
abnormalities such as asymmetrical faces (Mash & Wolfe, 2005). They show serious
impairment in sensorimotor functioning (for example, their responsiveness by the age of four
is similar to that of a one year old without developmental delay) and they can only reach a
rudimentary level of adaptive functioning such as communication skills, eating and self-care
Human figure drawing tests may play a most significant role in the identification of
individuals with mild mental retardation, since this form is often not easily identified at a
young age, and the majority of children with developmental delays (85%) fall into this
tests of intellectual ability, individual intelligence tests and developmental tests. Cox (1993)
presented an overview of various studies investigating the relationship between scores on the
Draw-a-Man test and tests of intelligence. Harris (1963) found correlations ranging from .2 to
ranging from .699 to .863; Yepsen (as cited in Cox, 1993) obtained a correlation of .6;
Williams (as cited in Cox, 1993) reported a correlation of .65; and McElwee (as cited in Cox,
1993) reported a correlation of .72 between the Draw-a-Man test and the Stanford-Binet
(Terman, 1916) test. Furthermore, Kline (1993) concluded that Harris’ revision of the Draw-
a-Man test provides a reasonable measure of general intelligence; while Reynolds and
50
scores and the three IQ scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Third Edition
with both the WISC-R and the Stanford-Binet scale (Abell, Von Briesen, & Watz, 1996;
Bensur & Eliot, 1993). Doubt however exists about whether these correlations are high
enough to suggest that such tests provide safe measurements of intelligence (Light & Barnes,
1995). Bekhit, Thomas and Jolley (2005, p. 208) disagree with the test developers’ insistence
that artistic ability does not influence the test results, arguing that “intelligent children may
not draw well, and conversely, some children who are artistically gifted may have a low IQ
score”. These authors further note that the human figure drawing test cannot predict the IQ of
children beyond 10 years of age, since children older than 10 years usually do not include
additional details in their drawings. These results, in general, indicate a clear relationship
between the scores of human figure drawing tests and those of individually administered
intelligence tests, but also show that these scores are not identical. Significant positive
correlations found between these measures may however suggest that they tap similar
constructs.
A comparison between the GMDS-ER and the Termin-Merril Scale (a revision of the
Stanford-Binet) yielded correlations varying from r=.79 to r=.81 (Griffiths, 1984). Another
study conducted in South Africa by Luiz and Heimes (1994) found high positive correlations
between the GQ of the Original Griffiths Scales and the General Intelligence Quotient of the
Junior South African Intelligence Scale (JSAIS). This evidence may indicate that the GMDS-
ER taps constructs similar to those measured by these two intelligence tests. Further
Neisser and colleagues’ (1996) finding that when adapted to measure individual differences
the tasks Piaget developed to gauge a child’s developmental level show a reasonable
51
indication of the direction of causality (Field, 2005). This means that an inference regarding
which variable causes the other to change cannot be made purely on the basis of statistical
evidence provided by correlation coefficients. Correlations also do not exclude the possibility
of unknown variables being responsible for the observed relationships. This is known as the
third-variable problem, or tertium quid which states that “[i]n any bivariate correlation
causality between two variables cannot be assumed because there may be another measured
or unmeasured variable affecting the results” (Field, 2005, p. 128). These psychometric
findings can therefore not be used as definitive proof of conceptual overlap, although it is
argued here that these empirical observations lend support to the hypothesis of conceptual
overlap when rationally combined with relevant theoretical considerations. Bearing in mind
that the purpose of this study is to investigate the usefulness of a HFD tests in the
any in fact exist) or the effects of unknown intervening variables are of little consequence. If
consistent correlations exist between these variables then any one variable may reasonably be
used as an indicator of the other. The usefulness of such an indication is dependent on the
strength of the correlation. The investigation into the possible existence of such a correlation
between HFD test scores and mental development and its strength (if a correlation exists) is
Piaget (1950, p. 8), referring to intelligence, stated that “its origins are
biological adaptation itself”. From this early stage then, intelligence and development is
intimately related. Earlier chapters have discussed the important role that conceptual
52
intelligence. In particular, it has been shown that such conceptual development is crucial for
an individual to progress through the developmental stages as outlined by Piaget and Bruner,
as well as for the acquisition of language. Britton as cited in Nystrand (1977, p.40) said that
“the ability to speak and to reason are … both …dependent upon the ability to generate
symbols, the ability to create representations of actuality”. Another ability- the ability to
produce a drawing of the human figure, is also dependent on representational thought. In this
way, an individual’s drawing of the human figure may provide an indication of both
intelligence and development by gauging the level of the representational ability that
underlies these processes. Considerable overlap exists between mental development and
intelligence on both conceptual and pragmatic levels. These concepts are related toeach other
On a conceptual level this overlap is illustrated by the simple fact that HFD tests
(such as the DAP: IQ), tests of mental development (such as the GMDS-ER), and intelligence
tests (such as the WISC-IV) all aim to measure an individual’s performance on selected test
tasks and to then relate this performance to other individuals from a selected cohort. To
illustrate, the DAP: IQ obtains a raw score providing an absolute indication of performance
on the test. With normal development, children’s absolute performance increases with age.
This is then transformed into a standardized score that places the individual’s performance in
relation to a representative chronological age cohort. The standard score is in turn used to
calculate an IQ score, representing the ratio between the test subject’s performance and the
average performance of said representative age cohort. A higher IQ score therefore indicates
that the subject’s development is relatively advanced, specifically the subject progressed
quicker than expected for his age. With a lower score the inverse is true. In a similar vein, the
GMDS-ER obtains an absolute indication of the test subject’s performance (items passed),
which is then divided by the subject’s chronological age and multiplied by 100. The resultant
53
general quotient (GQ) is, again, an expression of the subject’s performance relative to the
expected performance of the representative age cohort. A higher ratio indicates the subject is
relatively well developed and a lower ratio (beneath 100) may be indicative of a
developmental delay. In a similar vein, the first intelligence test was developed by Alfred
Binet in France to identify children with delayed mental development so that they could be
provided with special education (Berk, 1997). The Intelligence Quotient (IQ) was developed
for this purpose. It expressed the child’s performance relative to his peers as the ratio between
his actual performance (Mental Age, which is often abbreviated to MA) and his chronological
age (CA). A ratio IQ is therefore expressed as MA / CA X 100. The concept of mental age,
created by Binet and Simon, is based on the belief that the same developmental pattern is
found in all children, but that the rate at which they develop differs. A person of any age that
performs as well as a nine year old child is therefore said to have a mental age of nine
(Matsumoto, 2009). Mental age is thus defined as the “[l]evel of intellectual development as
measured through a range of cognitive tasks and through comparison with chronological age
peers. […] Mental age can be expressed as the age at which that level of development is
typically attained” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 21). This method was however of limited use in
estimating the intelligence of adults because raw scores begin to level off from the age of 16
(Eysenck, 1994). The deviation IQ was therefore invented to replace the ratio IQ. The
deviation IQ utilises a raw score representing the subject’s actual performance and places this
with a median of 100 and standard deviation of 15 or 16, depending on the specific test
(Matsumoto, 2009). It follows from the preceding discussion that although these three types
his or her chronological age cohort, the underlying principle is essentially the same in all
three cases.
54
3.6. Conclusion
This chapter discussed and clarified the relationship between the key constructs in the
present study, namely those of conceptual maturity, intelligence and mental development.
The psychometric approach to intelligence was described and brought into relation with
human figure drawing tests and the GMDS-ER. Academic skills disorders, mental
retardation, and developmental delays were then discussed and related to each other. It was
proposed that human figure drawing tests may prove to be useful in the assessment of such
conditions. Psychometric evidence was then presented to support the notion that an overlap
exists between scores on human figure drawing tests, intelligence tests, and developmental
tests. The chapter concluded with a discussion of a priori evidence of the relationship
between the key constructs of the present study. The following chapter presents the research
55
Chapter 4
Research Methodology
4.1. Introduction
In this chapter the reader is presented with the methodological considerations made in
the study. Information is presented on the research design, participants, sampling procedure,
measures used, data analysis techniques employed toward achieving the aims of the study,
quantitative research paradigm refers to“a formal, objective, systematic process in which
numerical data are utilised to obtain information about the world" (Burns & Grove as cited by
Cormack, 1991, p. 140).As the term suggests, the exploratory-descriptive research design
employed in the present study combines features of exploratory and descriptive research
designs. Exploratory research is recommended when there is little existing research on the
subject of study (Rubin & Babbie, 2001). Exploratory research is defined as “studies of a
field which seek to discover interesting patterns and facts but without preformed hypotheses”
(Matsumoto, 2009, p. 198). The research question, namely whether human figure drawings
can be used to screen for developmental delays informed the exploratory research aim of
investigating whether a linear relationship exists between children’s human figure drawing
scores and their general mental development. The present study includes an extensive
literature review that examined constructs central to the subject of investigation. A literature
review such as is included in this study is one method of exploratory research (Selltiz,
Jahoda, Deutsch, Cook, 1965). A review of the existing literature revealed a lack of
56
theoretical work that integrates the concepts of intelligence and development. Insofar as the
limited scope of the study allowed, perspectives from different fields within the broader
Exploratory research is generally conducted for one or more of the following reasons,
(1) to satisfy the researcher’s curiosity and desire for better understanding,
The research question arose from the researcher’s observation that although human
figure drawing tests display a developmental progression, the non-projective use of such tests
is largely limited to estimates of an individual’s intellectual ability. It was also clear that
inferences about children’s mental development are made on the basis of such tests, which
raised the need to determine whether human figure drawing tests are appropriate for this
purpose. A review of pertinent literature did not yield a satisfactory integration of the
important constructs that come together in the present study, namely: intelligence,
development, intellectual ability, and conceptual maturity. Despite the limited scope of the
present study, considerable effort was made to clarify these constructs and to demonstrate
57
some of the ways in which they are connected. With regards to the aims of exploratory
research listed above, the study achieved meaningful results despite the relatively small
sample size and unsophisticated methods of data collection. These results are encouraging
and support the feasibility of more extensive studies to investigate the use of human figure
drawings as tools for developmental screening. The study and research process produced
In addition to the exploratory nature of the research discussed above, the research
design is also descriptive insofar as it was a correlation study aimed at investigating the
relationship between human figure drawing scores and general mental development. A
descriptive research design refers to “empirical research which seeks to describe, categorize,
and count usually in naturalistic settings rather than to control situations to test specific
relationship exists between human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients
(an exploratory aim), the study aimed to describe the strength and directionality of such a
relationship if one was found to exist. Explanatory studies, the third category of social
events (Babbie & Mouton, 2006). The present study did not investigate causality and as such
did not enter into the domain of explanatory research. A discussion of the participants and
58
Shaughnessy, 2001). With probability sampling each individual in the population has the
same chance of being included in the sample, whereas with non-probability sampling the
chance of a specific individual being selected from the population is not known (Zechmeister
et al., 2001). Due to the limited scope of the current study a type of non-probability sampling
called convenience sampling was the most feasible method of data collection. Non-
where the aim is to generate hypotheses for further investigation (Babbie & Mouton, 2006). It
is the most common sampling method used in psychology (Matsumoto, 2009). Convenience
sampling involves the selection of subjects based on their availability without concern of
and cost-effective than probability sampling (Cozby, 2004). This method is contrasted to
random selection from the population so that each member of the population has a known
probability of being chosen” (Matsumoto, 2009, p. 401). The decision to use convenience
sampling was informed by the limited scope of the current study as well as the need to obtain
the maximum amount of data points from the available sampling pool, namely completed
GMDS-ER protocols drawn from a University Psychology Clinic in the Eastern Cape of
South Africa that met the inclusion requirements of the study. This approach had the
approval from the Eastern Cape Department of Education that would have been necessary to
In this type of sampling participants are not systematically selected which places
limits on the confidence that they are representative of the population (Creswell, 2002;
sampling provides valuable information for answering questions and testing hypotheses
59
(Creswell, 2002). The principle drawback of this sampling method is that researchers must
“exercise great caution in generalizing from [collected] data” (Babbie & Mouton, 2006, p.
166). When interpreting the results the question of how well the sample represents the
broader population should be taken into account. In practical terms, it should be considered
how well the children who completed the GMDS-ER protocols collected from the specific
Psychology Clinic represent the broader population of five to seven year old children in
South Africa to which the results will be generalized. Probability sampling is not necessary
when research aims to investigate the relationship between variables rather than to generalize
the results (Spata, 2003). As the current study is of an exploratory-descriptive nature the
value of findings do not lie in generalizing beyond the sampling frame, but in the usefulness
of these findings to inform further research into the phenomena under study.
characteristic that the researcher is interested in studying” (Roscoe, 1969, p. 155). The
population that the current sample relates to is therefore all South African children between
the ages of 5 and 7 years. The sampling frame “refers to the set of all cases from which the
sample will actually be selected” (Mouton, 1996, p. 135). The sampling frame of the current
study is all children between the ages of 5 and 7 years that have undergone psychological
assessment utilizing the GMDS-ER at the specific University Psychology Clinic in the
The final sample size was 30 participants, and not 60 as originally proposed. Of the
30 participants, 21 were male and 9 were female. This smaller sample size was due to a lower
than anticipated number of suitable participants that met the inclusion criteria. Another factor
is that a preliminary estimate of the obtainable sample size was based on a survey that
included versions of the Griffiths Scales prior to the updated and current GMDS-ER. In the
interest of uniformity, protocols based on earlier versions of the Griffiths Scales were not
60
included in the final sample. The large effect size obtained allowed for the detection of a
significant correlation at the .01 alpha level. According to Cohen (1992) a correlation with
the standard α-level of .05 and the recommended power of .8 would require 783 participants
to detect a small effect size (r = .1), 85 participants to detect a medium effect size (r = .3) and
28 participants to detect a large effect size (r = .5). The smaller sample size does therefore not
4.3.1. Inclusion criteria. The minimum age of participants was set at 5 years. This
decision was informed by Reynolds and Hickman’s (2004) statement that a linear relationship
between age and raw DAP: IQ score is observed between the ages of 4 and 14, as well as
Davis’ (2006) finding that the published norms of the DAP: IQ are valid with local South
African children between the ages of five and seven years. The maximum age was set at the 7
year mark in order to reduce the possibility of a ceiling effect on the GMDS-ER. Such an
effect may occur when advanced children younger than 8 years can perform tasks exceeding
the 8 year equivalent ceiling of the GMDS-ER.The age range for inclusion in the sample for
the proposed study was therefore 5 years to7 years of age. This is also the age range at which
children will benefit most from developmental testing in preparation for entrance into the
formal schooling system. Completed GMDS-ER protocols in which the participant did not
reach item ‘DIII.9 Draws a person: Stage 1’ on the eye-hand co-ordination subscale were
excluded as human figure drawings could not be obtained from these protocols.
Griffiths developed the original Griffiths Scales of Mental Development in 1954. The
original measure’s upper age range has since been extended to allow the assessment of
children up to the age of eight years four months (Luiz et al., 2006a). The measure has also
61
been revised, but remains true to Griffith’s original aim insofar as it provides a tool for the
early diagnosis of mental conditions in children. The Griffiths Mental Development Scales –
Extended Revised (Luiz et al., 2004a, 2004b) draws on an extensive study of mental
development in order to assess the developmental level of children from birth to 8 years 4
months of age. Assessment of children up to 2 years of age utilises five subscales, namely
with a sixth subscale (Practical Reasoning) being added when evaluating children between 2
and 8 years 4 months of age (Luiz et al.,2004b). Material for the test items emerged from the
careful observation of children in their natural environments. This material was then
incorporated into test items that are placed in order of gradually increasing difficulty (Luiz et
al., 2004b). These diverse items tap the main aspects of a child’s development by having each
subscale measure “only one avenue of learning or process of development, but measuring this
one aspect as completely as possible” (Griffiths, 1970, p. 34). Results on the GMDS-ER are
subscale. Munro (1968) however noted that there is no empirical evidence to suggest that
each subscale measures an independent aspect of ability and suggested that the subscales may
tend to measure a general factor. This information informed the research design decision of
correlating HFD scores with GMDS-ER GQ scores and not to include the individual
subscales. This decision is in line with the aims of the study which are to investigate the
usefulness of human figure drawing tests in the screening for developmental delays in the
Griffiths’ (1954, 1970, 1984, 1986) approach to the measurement of the mental
cognisant of the complex interactions between various avenues of learning and employed a
holistic approach that favoured a broad view of mental development. This approach
62
foreshadowed later developments including Gardner’s (1993) multiple intelligences and the
emergence of ecological models (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994). Her appreciation of the
interaction between mental development on the one hand and social and emotional influences
on the other prefigured Goleman’s (1996) notion of emotional intelligence. This once novel
approach has since been broadly accepted and has become commonplace in the assessment of
which have shown the Griffiths Scales (i.e., both the Original Scales and the GMDS-ER) to
be of value in the evaluation and treatment of infants and young children from various
cultural backgrounds (Allan, 1988, 1992; Bhamjee, 1991; Brandt, 1983, 1984; Cobos,
Rodriques, & De Venegas, 1971; Collins, Jupp, Maberly, Morris, & Eastman, 1987;
Knoesen, 2003; Laroche, Gutz, & Desbiolles, 1974; Luiz et al., 2001a; Luiz et al., 2001b;
Mothuloe, 1990; Ramsay & Fitzharding, 1977; Sletten, 1970, 1977). The Griffiths Scales
have also been extensively researched in South Africa (Allan, 1988; 1992; Barnard, 2000;
Bhamjee, 1991; Jakins, 2009; Knoesen, 2003, 2005; Kotras, 1998; Luiz, 1988a, 1988b,
1988c, 1988d, 1994a, 1994b; Luiz, Folsher & Lombard, 1989; Luiz, Foxcroft & Povey,
2006b; Luiz, Foxcroft & Stewart, 2001a; Luiz, Foxcroft, Worsfold, Kotras & Kotras, 2001b;
Luiz & Heimes, 1994; Moosajee, 2007; Povey, 2008; Sweeney, 1994; Tukulu, 1996; Van
Rooyen, 2005; Ward, 1997; Wills, 2011). In addition to this strong research interest, the
Griffiths Mental Development Scales are also widely used by trained professionals in South
Africa (Povey, 2008). The Original Griffiths Scales are also available in Afrikaans (Allan,
The reliability of the general quotient is .96, with reliability coefficients for the
different subscales ranging from .90 to .97 (Luiz et al., 2004a). Content validity was
investigated by an extensive literature review, interviews with experts, and a facet analysis of
each subscale. This found each item to be representative of its content domain, as well as
63
having a satisfactory degree of relevance to the construct being measured. A factor analysis
provided further evidence of the construct validity of the Griffiths Scales (Luiz et al., 2004a).
Further research has confirmed the reliability and validity of the GMDS-ER (Beail, 1985;
Griffiths, 1984; Luiz, 1988c; Mothuloe, 1990; Stewart, 1997; Worsfold, 1993).
The human figure drawings utilized in the present study were obtained from the single
human figure drawing of each protocol used to score items “Draws a person: Stage 1” (Luiz
et al., 2006a, p.53), “Draws a person: Stage 2” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 56), and “Draws a
person: Stage 3” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 59) on the GMDS-ER. These items contribute to the
child’s subscale score on the eye and hand co-ordination subscale of the GMDS-ER. The
average of the six subscales, including the eye and hand co-ordination subscale, is used to
for Children, Adolescents, and Adults (DAP: IQ). The human figure drawing (HFD)
scores used in the current study were obtained by scoring HFDs from the collected GMDS-
ER protocols according to the scoring criteria of the Draw-a-Person Intellectual Ability Test
for Children, Adolescents, and Adults (DAP: IQ). This section first provides information on
the DAP: IQ. This is followed by a discussion of differences between the administration of
the HFDs used in the current study and standardized administration guidelines of the DAP:
IQ.
obtaining and scoring human figure drawings. The test is normed for the ages 4 years 0
months 0 days through 89 years 11 months 30 days. The only material required for the
administration and scoring of the DAP: IQ is the examiner’s manual, a scoring form, a
drawing form or blank A4 pages, and 2 sharpened pencils. Scores on the 23 scoring elements
64
are summed to obtain a raw score which is then converted to standard scores with a mean of
100 and a standard deviation of 15 using manual tables. The manual also provides percentile
The DAP: IQ has been found to demonstrate “very good reliability, especially
considering the brevity of the task and its simple, rapid scoring system” (Reynolds &
Hickman, 2004, p. 20). Research revealed a high internal consistency, with a coefficient
coefficients were found to range from .91 to .95 (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004).
Validity analysis focussed on correlations between the DAP: IQ and other human
figure drawing tests of conceptual maturity. A correlation between .85 and .86 was obtained
between the DAP: IQ and the Koppitz (1986) system and a correlation coefficient of .86 was
obtained between the DAP: IQ and the Goodenough-Harris (1963) system. Correlations
between the DAP: IQ and the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children Third Edition were
modest, with a coefficient of .49 with Performance IQ and .33 with Verbal IQ. Although
modest, the DAP: IQ correlated at a statistically significant level with all three WISC-III IQs
The published DAP: IQ norms are appropriate for use with children in the local South
African context (Davis, 2006). In light of the issues surrounding appropriate test use in South
Africa as discussed in the introductory chapter, this valuable information further informed the
decision to use the DAP: IQ scoring criteria and norms in the current study.
The study utilised secondary data, namely archival data collected at a University
Psychology Clinic in the Nelson Mandela Metropole. Each data set consisted of a completed
65
consent form providing permission for the data to be used for research purposes and a
completed GMDS-ER protocol. “Draws a person: Stage 1” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 53) on the
GMDS-ER requires the child to draw a human figure. This HFD is then scored on the number
of features present. A circle for a head and two other features is sufficient to ensure a pass at
stage 1, while six additional features allow a pass of item “Draws a person: Stage 2” (Luiz et
al., 2006a, p. 56). Evidence of creativity is used as criteria for a pass on the item “Draws a
person: Stage 3” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 59). This simple scoring procedure contributes to the
child’s score on the GMDS-ER Eye and Hand Co-ordination subscale. This procedure was
not used for the purposes of the current study. Instead, human figure drawings obtained from
the GMDS-ER protocols were scored according to the 23 scoring elements provided in the
examiner’s manual of the DAP: IQ (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). This more refined scoring
system better reflected the current use of human figure drawing tests and as such provided a
more accurate indication of the value of human figure drawing tests in screening for
developmental delays.
protocols. The human figure drawings in the GMDS-ER protocols were then scored
Intellectual Ability Test for Children, Adolescents, and Adults (DAP: IQ) (Reynolds &
Hickman, 2004). These raw scores were subsequently converted to standard scores using the
appropriate tables in the DAP: IQ examiner’s manual (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). DAP: IQ
standard scores and general quotients were chosen as the subjects of analysis because it
reflects the figures on which decisions are based when the DAP: IQ and GMDS-ER are used
in clinical settings. The use of these scores was therefore more appropriate toward meeting
the aims of the current study than the use of raw scores would be. Computerised data analysis
66
was then conducted utilizing SPSS for Windows (Version 19), which is a popular statistical
software package.
statistics were used to summarize relevant information regarding sample characteristics and
participants’ performance on the two research measures. Descriptive statistics are “methods
used to summarize, organize, and describe observations (Sadock & Sadock, 2007, p. 175) by
statements regarding the central tendency and variability in the data. The arithmetic mean,
defined as “the sum of a set of numbers divided by how many numbers are in the set”
(Matsumoto, 2009, p. 300) was used as a measure of central tendency. Variability in the data
was described using the standard deviation (SD), a statistic calculated by obtaining “the
square root of the average of the squared differences from the mean of a set of numbers”
(Matsumoto, 2009, p. 516). The standard deviation indicates the degree to which scores differ
variables and usually a linear index scaled so that 0 indicates no relationship, +1 indicates a
2009, p. 135). Towards achieving the aims of the study, the appropriate correlation
coefficient was calculated in order to describe the direction and size of the relationship
between participants’ DAP: IQ scores and their GMDS-ER general quotients. The specific
statistic chosen to obtain the correlation coefficient was based on the data’s adherence to the
assumptions of parametric tests. The procedures followed to test these assumptions are now
discussed.
67
whether the data conformed to the assumptions of parametric tests. These assumptions must
be met in order to use parametric tests based on the normal distribution (Field, 2005). It is
essential to ensure that data conforms to these assumptions before choosing a test because the
use of a parametric test on nonparametric data will likely yield an inaccurate result (Field,
2005). The assumptions of parametric tests, as discussed in Field (2005) are as follows:
1. Data must be normally distributed. The data must come from one or more normally
distributed populations. This assumption may be tested by calculating whether the sampling
distribution significantly deviates from the skewness and kurtosis of the normal distribution
said to be skewed when the most frequent scores cluster at one end of the scale with the
frequency of scores gradually declining towards the other end of the scale (Field, 2005).
with many scores in the tails while a leptokurtic distribution has fewer scores in the tails and
appears pointy when viewed as a histogram (Field, 2005). This was calculated by obtaining z-
scores for skewness and kurtosis using the skewness and kurtosis values and their respective
standard errors as produced by SPSS. This process is expressed in the following equations:
To test whether the data was normally distributed, these values were then compared to
the absolute value of 1.96 that indicates a significant deviation from the normal distribution at
68
variable should remain stable at all levels of the other variable (Field, 2005). Levene’s test
was used to “tests the hypothesis that the variances in different groups are equal” (Field,
2005, p. 736) and whether this assumption was met. Levene’s test was computed for two
pairs of variables, namely: (a) Chronological age and human figure drawing raw score; and
(b) Standardized human figure drawing score and GMDS-ER general quotient.
3. Interval Data. For this assumption to be met the distance between points on the
scale must be equal throughout the scale (Field, 2005). The standardized HFD scores and
GMDS-ER GQ scores adhere to this expectation and this assumption was therefore met.
4. Independence. This assumption requires that the behaviour from one participant
does not influence the behaviour of others (Field, 2005). The current sample consisted of data
from individually administered tests and there was no reason to assume that one participant’s
As discussed above, it was known that the data conformed to the assumptions of
interval data and independence before data analysis was conducted. Subsequent data analysis
utilizing the statistical package SPSS was conducted to test whether the assumptions of
normally distributed data and homogeneity of variance was met. The results of these analyses
moment correlation coefficient is a parametric test that requires the assumptions of such tests
to be met (Field, 2005). It was concluded that the data met the assumptions of parametric
tests. The Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coefficient was therefore the statistic used
for all correlations. Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient is the most commonly used correlation
coefficient (Matsumoto, 2009). It measures the degree of linear relationship between two
69
participants’ chronological age and HFD raw scores. The result was then compared to
literature discussed earlier that suggests a relationship between age and the complexity of
human figure drawings. A second Pearson Correlation Coefficient was then computed to
investigate the relationship between standardized HFD scores and GMDS-ER general
quotients.
The significance of the obtained correlation was tested to investigate whether the
results were due to chance (Matsumoto, 2009). A statistically significant result, that is a result
that is not due to chance, does not mean that the result is practically meaningful. The effect
size was therefore calculated to investigate the size of the relationship between standardized
human figure drawing scores and general quotients. An effect size is “an objective and
standardized measure of the magnitude of the observed effect” (Field, 2005, p. 32). Pearson’s
Correlation Coefficient can also be used as a standardized measure of the effect size (Field,
2005). The use of such a standardized measure allows for the comparison of effect sizes
across different studies that have measured different variables (Field, 2005). With Pearson’s
effect (Field, 2005). Cohen (1988, 1992) made the following widely accepted suggestions
regarding effect size, which were used to interpret the resulting statistics in the present study:
r = .30 (medium effect): an effect of this magnitude accounts for 9% of the variance
r = .50 (large effect): the effect accounts for 25% of the variance
70
A factor analytic study by Luiz and colleagues (2001a) found that the subscales of the
GMDS-ER tap one underlying dimension. A subsequent article identified this dimension as
“general intelligence” (Luiz et al., 2006b, p.194). The decision to investigate the relationship
between HFD scores and GMDS-ER general quotients was based on these findings, as well
as Munro’s (1968) finding that the Griffiths Scales tend to measure a general underlying
factor and not distinct abilities as referred to in the section on measures used. The relationship
between HFD scores and specific GMDS-ER subscales was therefore not investigated.
The present study utilised archival data collected at a University Psychology Clinic in
the Nelson Mandela metropol in the Eastern Cape of South Africa. As such, written informed
consent allowing for the collected data to be used in research had already been obtained when
the measures were originally administered. The Faculty of Health Sciences Research
Technology and Innovations Committee gave ethics approval and granted permission for the
study to be conducted. To ensure confidentiality only the researcher had access to the data
and participants’ identification details were not included in the research report. The inclusion
and exclusion of participants was based on the criteria set out in this document. No person
4.8. Conclusion
This chapter discussed the research methodology of the present study. The study
Considerations that informed the choice of this research design were discussed. The study
may be described as exploratory in terms of the literature review and the research aim to
determine whether a linear relationship exists between children’s human figure drawing
scores and general development. It is also descriptive insofar as the research aimed to
71
describe characteristics of the sample and to investigate the strength and directionality of the
relationship between human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients. The
rationale for choosing to employ non-probability convenience sampling and the implications
of this decision were discussed. The chapter also presented information on the population,
sampling frame, and sample size and provided a discussion of the implication of these sample
characteristics on the results of the study. Information was provided on the two research
measures used in the current study, as well as the research procedure. The chapter presented
the reader with an explanation of the data analysis techniques used to describe the data and
draw conclusions towards achieving the objectives of the study. In conclusion, ethical
considerations pertaining to the present study was discussed. The results of the research are
72
Chapter 5
5.1. Introduction
This chapter presents the results of the present research study. These results are
discussed in the context of the literature reviewed in earlier chapters and related to the
research objectives. Before the results of the data analysis described in the previous chapter
are provided it is however important to revisit the objectives of the study. The current study
aimed to investigate the usefulness of human figure drawings for the measurement of mental
development. This primary research objective led to four specific objectives, namely: (a) To
explore and describe the mental development scores of the sample; (b) To explore and
describe the human figure drawing scores of the sample; (c) To investigate whether there is a
relationship between the standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general
quotients of the sample; and (d) To investigate the directionality and strength of the
relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general
quotients of the sample. The remainder of this chapter presents empirical findings relevant to
these objectives and provides a discussion of these findings in relation to literature reviewed
earlier.
5.2. Participants
As noted earlier, the current study utilized archival data collected from a University
Psychology Clinic in the Nelson Mandela metropol in the Eastern Cape of South Africa. All
GMDS-ER protocols that were completed by participants that met the study’s inclusion
criteria were collected during the first half of December 2010. The following results are
5.3.1. Age distribution of the participants. The age of participants in the sample (n
=30) ranged from 5 years 0 months to 7 years 0 months with a mean age of 6 years 0 months
and a standard deviation of 6 months and 22 days. These results are presented in Table 1
Table 1
Std.
N Minimum Maximum Mean
Deviation
Chronological
30 60.60 84.00 72.0967 6.74139
age in months
The age distribution of the participants conforms to the inclusion criteria as outlined
in the methodology section. Based on their ages, participants in the present study were in
Piaget’s second stage, which spans the ages of two to seven years and is known as the stage
of preoperational thought (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000). Their mental
development at this stage is marked by an increase in symbolic thought, that is, thought that
relies on the use of language and other symbols (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Zgourides, 2000).
Children in the preoperational stage still rely on primitive concepts, but their increased
mastery of language allows for improved reasoning ability that is also manifested in their
drawing productions (Sadock & Sadock, 2007; Toomela, 2002, Zgourides, 2000).
In Bruner’s (1966) theory, the participants would have relied mostly on iconic and
symbolic modes of representation. With normal development, the younger participants would
have relied mostly on iconic representation, which involves the ability to represent reality in
74
terms of images or spatial schemas (Bruner, 1966). The older participants are expected to
have relied increasingly on symbolic representation, in which they are able to represent their
experience by means of true symbols such as words that do not have to resemble the referent
Finally, the participants would have been in the second stage of Luquet’s (1927)
theory of drawing development, namely the pre-schematic stage that includes the ages
between 4 and 7 years. Based on their age, children at this stage would already have noticed
that their drawings can represent aspects of the world – a realization known as fortuitous
realism (Luquet, 1927). These children normally try to make representational drawings
instead of the random scribbling associated with earlier stages (Luquet, 1927).
The age range of participants in the sample falls within the range of 4 years and 14
years in which Reynolds and Hickman (2004) reported a linear relationship to exist between
chronological age and raw DAP: IQ score. It was investigated whether this relationship was
also present in the current sample. The results of this analysis are presented shortly.
5.3.2. Standardized human figure drawing scores. The results presented here
provide a conclusion to the first research objective of the present study, namely ‘To explore
and describe the standardized human figure drawing scores of the sample’. The standardized
human figure drawing scores ranged from 62 to 129 (n=30) with a mean of 95.3 and standard
deviation of 20.74. Skewness was calculated as -.336 with standard error of skewness being
.427. Kurtosis was -1.027 with standard error of kurtosis being .833. The results are presented
in Table 2.
75
Table 2
Skewness Kurtosis
Std.
N Minimum Maximum Mean Statistic Std. Statistic Std.
Deviation
Error Error
Std.
Score
5.3.3. GMDS-ER general quotients. The following results meet the second research
objective of the study which was ‘To explore and describe the GMDS-ER general quotients
of the sample’. The GMDS-ER general quotient scores ranged from 31.50 to 120.30 (n=30)
with a mean of 88.635 and standard deviation of 19.73297. This variable had a skewness of -
.665 with standard error of skewness being .427 and a kurtosis of .995 with standard error of
Table 3
Skewness Kurtosis
Std.
N Minimum Maximum Mean Statistic Std. Statistic Std.
Deviation
Error Error
Prior to data analysis, it was already known that the variables ‘standardized human
figure drawing scores’ and GMDS-ER general quotients’ met the assumptions of interval data
76
methodology section, were used to test whether these assumptions were met. The results of
the analysis of the normality of the data distribution is first discussed, which is followed by
the analysis of homogeneity of variance. The standardized human figure drawing score
value by the standard error of skewness) of -0.787 and a standardized kurtosis coefficient
(calculated by dividing the kurtosis value by the standard error of kurtosis) of -1.233.
Following the same procedure, the GMDS-ER general quotient score variable produced a
None of the obtained values had an absolute value greater than 1.96, therefore it was
concluded that the variables came from normally distributed populations (as suggested by
Field, 2005). The assumption of normally distributed data was therefore met.
Levene’s test was used to test whether the variances of the variables ‘standardized
human figure drawing score’ and ‘GMDS-ER general quotient’ were equal. The test was
conducted using SPSS for Windows (Version 19). Levene’s test produced a statistic of F =
.318, which was not significant at the standard α-level of .05 (F = .318, p > .05). A non-
significant result on Levene’s test indicates that the variances are not significantly different
(Field, 2005). The assumption of homogeneity of variance was therefore not violated, and it
was concluded that the variables ‘standardized human figure drawing score’ and ‘GMDS-ER
general quotient’ conformed to the assumptions of parametric tests. Based on these results,
coefficient to investigate the relationship between human figure drawing scores and GMDS-
77
5.4. The Relationship between Standardized Human Figure Drawing Scores and
The third research objective of the study was to establish whether a linear relationship
exists between human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients. A scatterplot
Figure 2, where the curve estimation command in SPSS was used to insert a diagonal line
representing the best fitting linear model. The SPSS output for the regression analysis used to
produce this figure is presented in Table 4. Exploration of the data indicated that the variables
met the assumptions of normality and the Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coefficient
was therefore used to measure the correlation between standardized HFD scores and GMDS-
obtained between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general
Figure 1
78
Figure 2
Scatterplot representing relationship between standardized HFD scores and GQ with line of
best fit
Table 4
SPSS Output for Curve Estimation between Standardized HFD Scores and GQ
Parammeter
Equation
Model Summary Estimates
79
Table 5
Correlation between Standardized HFD Scores and GMDS-ER General Quotients (GQ)
scores and GMDS-ER general quotients produced a correlation of r = .758, and this
correlation was significant at α = .01 (r = .758, p< .01). This result provided an answer to the
standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients of the sample.
This finding indicated that a positive linear relationship existed between standardized human
figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients in the current sample.
According to this finding it is highly unlikely that the correlation was the result of
chance, but it did not however indicate that this amounted to a meaningful or important effect
(Field, 2005). The effect size was therefore calculated to determine the strength of the
section, the finding was indicative of a correlation with a large effect size (r > .50) between
standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients. Other
coefficient provides a measure of the amount of variability in one variable that is explained
by the other (Field, 2005). The amount of variability in GMDS-ER general quotients
80
explained by standardized human figure drawing scores was therefore obtained by squaring r
and multiplying by 100 to express this value as a percentage. This revealed that 57.45% of
the variability in GMDS-ER general quotients was explained by standardized human figure
figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients provided an answer to the third
objective of the study, namely: ‘To investigate whether there is a relationship between the
standardized human figure drawing scores and general mental development quotients of the
sample’. It was concluded that such a relationship did exist in the present sample.
The fourth objective of the study was ‘To investigate the directionality and strength of
the relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general
quotients of the sample.’ This objective was reached by two findings. Firstly: a positive linear
relationship was found to exist between standardized human figure drawing scores and
human figure drawing score and GMDS-ER general quotient had a large effect size and was a
The literature review chapter of the present study discussed the developmental
measures used in the study, namely the GMDS-ER and an adapted administration of the
DAP: IQ. Theoretical contributions from the broader field of developmental psychology was
also applied to human figure drawings in particular and related to Luquet’s theory of drawing
development. As discussed in the literature review, several indications suggest that the
proficiency with which children produce drawings of the human figure is related to mental
81
development in general. Statistical and a priori evidence of a relationship between the key
constructs of the study were also discussed. The observed relationship between standardized
human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients may be understood in the
context of the literature reviewed earlier. It is hypothesized that various shared factors
underlie mental development in general and account for the developmental progression seen
in human figure drawings. The theoretical contributions reviewed earlier are now discussed
in relation to the observed relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores
conceptual maturity. It has been discussed that conceptual maturity is closely associated with
based on the increased ability to use concepts and symbolic ways of representing experience.
It is therefore hypothesised that human figure drawings may measure the mental development
of children by measuring their ability to use representational thought that underlies mental
development. This possible explanation for the observed relationship between standardized
human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients will now be discussed in more
detail.
Piaget proposed that the semiotic function develops during the preoperational period
(Sadcock & Sadock, 2007). The emergence of this function allows children to represent
things, including objects, events, or conceptual schemes by means of a signifier (Sadock &
terms of concepts. The degree to which the child has mastered the semiotic function will
82
influence his performance on the GMDS-ER because many of the measure’s items, for
example the naming of objects and the ability to count, rely on the ability to use concepts and
think in symbolic terms. Drawing also relies on the ability to use symbols (Sadock & Sadock,
2007). Cherney and colleagues (2006, p. 127) noted that “children’s drawings are thought to
the child’s drawing of an object is an index of his concept of that object, and his
The observed relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and
GMDS-ER general quotients can therefore be explained, at least in part, by the hypothesis
that both measures tap the conceptual maturity that underlies mental development.
With respect to conceptual maturity, Reynolds and Hickman (2004, p. 2) noted that
“as children mature cognitively and their thought processes become more complex, their
drawings of a person become more detailed, complete, and complex”. Harris (as cited in Cox,
1993) believed that a human figure drawing provides an index of a child’s level of conceptual
disequilibrium to account for cognitive development (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). Individuals
strive to attain a state of balance between their cognitive structures and the environment – a
state that Piaget referred to equilibrium (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). This state is maintained by
incorporated into existing cognitive schemes, but a state of disequilibrium arises when there
is a contradiction between experience and existing thought processes (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010).
83
In such cases the existing cognitive schemes are adapted to incorporate the new experiences –
a process known as accommodation (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). The example of a child’s
drawing of a dog mentioned earlier may serve to illustrate this principle. Such a child will
accommodate his existing cognitive structures by expanding them to include concepts such as
‘collie’ and ‘poodle’ in addition to the more general concept of ‘dog’. This would allow him
to assimilate differences between collies and poodles into his thought processes and account
for cognitive development. Of course the processes discussed here are not only relevant to an
understanding of dogs, but help the child to adapt to all aspects of his environment. The
higher levels of conceptual maturity and increased ability to use representational and abstract
thought will allow him to perform better on human figure drawing tests and developmental
Piaget noted that the emergence of the semiotic function during the preoperational
stage allows for rapid progress to be made in the use of language (Sadock & Sadock, 2007;
Zgourides, 2000). Vygotsky’s (1978) sociocultural theory proposes that culture transforms
elementary mental functions such as attention, sensation, perception, and memory into higher
mental functions, such as thought, linguistic ability, mathematical ability, and problem-
solving. He believed that these higher mental functions first emerge externally in social
interaction with other individuals and that they are only later internalized by means of
language (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). In Vygotsky’s (1978) theory, the ability to use symbols such
drawing as graphic speech and believed that it is based on verbal speech and precedes written
language. In Vygostky’s (1978) theory, language is the most important cultural tool and
allows the development of thought. Bruner (1966) believed that cognitive development is
drawings, and to use words as symbols to refer to something else both depend on the semiotic
84
function that Piaget described. Since Vygostky regarded drawing to be a form of language,
the observed relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER
general quotients may be due to human figure drawings measuring a child’s proficiency in
using the symbolic thought that underlies language, with language in turn driving mental
development.
and symbolic modes of thought may further explain the relationship between standardized
human figure drawing scores and general mental development quotients. In the enactive stage
children’s drawings are non-representational and they would therefore perform poorly on
human figure drawing tests. This corresponds to Reynolds and Hickman’s (2004) statement
that the linear relationship between age and human figure drawing score only begins at age 4.
At this age normally developing children begin to rely more heavily on the symbolic mode of
representation than on the iconic mode of representation and begin to develop the semiotic
function described by Piaget (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969). As discussed earlier, proficiency in
drawing a human figure is not only dependent on the iconic mode of representation
(representing the world through the use of a mental image) but also on the symbolic mode of
representation that allows a child to select which aspects to include in his drawing and to
organize his efforts by means of self-directed speech. The same principle applies to tasks on
the GMDS-ER that require the child to produce graphic representations, such as the item
“Draws a house: stage 1” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 54), and other items such as “Builds ‘gate’ to
model using 3 boxes and lids” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 72). As children develop, the enactive
mode of representation is first supplemented by the iconic mode and later by the inclusion of
the symbolic mode of representation (Bruner, 1966; 2006a). This progression may account
for the increase in their human figure drawing scores and their ability to pass more advanced
85
items on the GMDS-ER, and as such it helps to explain the relationship between standardized
individual’s cultural and historical context. According to Bruner and Haste (1987), social
interaction allows a child to develop a framework that helps him to make sense of his
this way, “culture gives shape to our thoughts” (Bruner, 2006b, p. 4). Vygotsky believed that
is to be learned and how it is to be learned (Lutz & Huitt, 2004). It is therefore clear that
Bruner and Vygotsky agreed on the important role of culture and social interaction in
cognitive development. Vygotsky (1978) proposed that cultural tools such as language and
mathematics are passed on from one generation to the next through social interaction. It may
be argued that the ability to create a representational drawing is one such cultural tool.
Parents supply children with the materials needed to practice drawing, such as crayons and
paper and may encourage them to make increasingly realistic representations. Likewise,
many items on the GMDS-ER measure a child’s ability to perform tasks that depend on the
individual’s culture and would have been acquired through social interaction. The item
“Names 12 objects in a box” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 43) on the language subscale is an
example of such an item that requires a child to name such objects like a knife and a fork. To
pass this item the child would have had to learn the names of these items that occur in his
society by means of language (that relies on the semiotic function) and social interaction.
Another example is the item “Copies 6+ letters” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 55) on the Eye and
Hand Co-ordination subscale. It may be reasoned that the ability to make such graphic
representations is a cultural tool that is passed on socially in cultures that place importance on
literacy. The child’s culture and the success with which he has internalized the cultural tools
86
of his society therefore influence his performance on human figure drawing tests and the
GMDS-ER.
nervous system. This association between cognitive development and biological maturation is
supported by Zgourides’ (2000) observation that concrete operations (Piaget’s third stage) is
dependent on the maturation of the brain and nervous system. He also suggests that the
increase in neural connections that occurs with maturation allow for more advanced ways of
thinking (Zgourides, 2000). Most of the neuronal axons in the brain are insulated by cells
called myelin that allow information to be transferred more quickly and efficiently between
neurons (Webb et al., 2001). Different regions in the brain mature at different times.
Myelinisation occurs earlier in areas associated with vision and motor skills than in areas
responsible for higher cognitive functions (Gibson, 1991). The timing of myelination in the
prefrontal cortex, for example, explains the emergence of higher cognitive skills such as
those referred to in Piaget’s formal operations stage (Couperus & Nelson, 2005; Fuster,
2002). This physiological process of brain maturation may therefore explain developmental
Piaget (as cited in Shaffer & Kipp, 2010, p. 55) held that biological maturation allows
for the development of “increasingly complex cognitive schemes that help [children] to
speed (Kail & Salthouse, 1994) and working memory (Chi, 1977) occur with development.
The increased complexity of children’s drawings that occurs with advancing age is associated
complexity is in turn related to increases in working memory (Bensur, Eliot, & Hedge, 1997;
complexity and can therefore account for developmental stages (Pascual-Leone, 2000).
87
Developmental increases in working memory might therefore account for the observed
relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores GMDS-ER general quotients,
Poor motor control and poor attention are other factors that might influence children’s
Luquet’s (1927) theory, children in the current study are in the pre-schematic stage. He
suggested that poor motor control accounts for the incorrect use of line and that poor
attention underlies the omission of details frequently seen at this stage of drawing
development (Luquet, 1927). Bensur and Eliot (1993) likewise suggest that improved fine
motor skills that occur with development influence children’s drawing ability. Poor motor
control will also be detrimental to a child’s performance on the GMDS-ER, as such a child
may have difficulty with tasks on the GMDS-ER such as the item “Train under bridge
successfully” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p. 71) that requires the child to use fine motor skills to push
a train of blocks under a bridge without knocking the bridge down. It is also clear that poor
attention will impede performance on the GMDS-ER, as such a child will have difficulty
The obtained correlation between standardized human figure drawing scores and
GMDS-ER general quotients in the current sample (r = .758) was substantially larger than the
correlations between DAP: IQ scores and WISC-III Performance IQ (r = .40) and between
DAP: IQ scores and WISC-III Verbal IQ (r = .33) reported by Reynolds and Hickman
(2004). This finding suggests that although the test developers purport the DAP: IQ to
measure intellectual ability, this test might provide a better indication of mental development
than of intellectual ability. The hypothesis generated by the present study may be further
88
5.5. The Relationship between Chronological Age and Human Figure Drawing Raw
Scores
The literature suggests that a linear relationship exists between age and raw DAP: IQ
score for participants between the ages of 4 and 14 (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004).
Exploratory data analysis was conducted to determine whether the variables of human figure
drawing raw score and chronological age in months conform to the requirements of
parametric tests.
Chronological age in months scores ranged from 60.60 months to 84 months with a
mean of 72.1 months and a standard deviation of 6.74 months (n = 30). This variable had a
skewness of -.03 with standard error of skewness being .427 and a kurtosis of -.882 with
standard error of kurtosis being .833. Human figure drawing raw scores ranged from 0 to 24
with a mean of 10.47 and a standard deviation of 7.413 (n = 30). This variable had a
skewness of -.004 with a standard error of skewness of .427. Kurtosis was -1.091 with
standard error of kurtosis being .833. The standardized skewness coefficient for the
chronological age in months variable was calculated by dividing the skewness value of the
variable by the standard error of skewness of the variable. The obtained standardized
skewness coefficient was -0.07. Dividing the variable’s kurtosis value by the standard error
of kurtosis produced a standardized kurtosis coefficient of -1.06 for the chronological age in
months variable. With respect to the human figure drawing raw score variable, the
standardized skewness coefficient obtained by following the same procedure while using this
variable’s skewness value and standard error of skewness was -0.01. The standardized
kurtosis for the human figure drawing raw score calculated by the means described above
was -1.31. It was concluded that the skewness and kurtosis of the variables human figure
drawing raw score and chronological age in months did not differ significantly from that of
89
the normal distribution, since the absolute values of none of the obtained statistics exceeded
Levene’s test produced a statistic of F = .788, which was not significant at the
standard α-level of .05 (F = .788, p > .05). The assumption of homogeneity of variance was
therefore not violated. The variables were both measured on interval level and because the
tests were administered individually there was no reason to believe that the performance of
one child influenced that of another. The assumptions of interval data and independence were
therefore met.
Based on these results, it was concluded that the variables chronological age in
months and human figure drawing raw score conformed to the assumptions of parametric
tests. Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient was therefore the most appropriate
human figure drawing raw score produced a result of r = .365, which was significant at α =
.05 (r = .365, p< .05). This result indicated that the observed relationship was most likely not
due to chance. According to Cohen, (1988; 1992) a correlation of this magnitude is indicative
This finding was consistent with the existing literature. It supports Reynolds and
Hickman’s (2004) assertion that a linear relationship exists between age and raw DAP: IQ
score. The scoring criteria of the DAP: IQ (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004) consider proportions
and placement of body parts, for example, that the head must be smaller in height than the
trunk and that the ears are correctly placed and relative to other features. As such, the
90
obtained correlation between chronological age and HFD raw scores was consistent with
Schuyten’s (1904) observation that more realistic proportions of body parts are correlated
with a child’s age, as well as with Cherney and her colleagues’ (2006) assertion that the
amount of distinctive features included in children’s drawings increases with age. The finding
also supports the general notion that children’s drawings of the human figure follow a
The correlation between participants’ chronological age and their HFD raw scores
was smaller than the correlation between their standardized HFD scores and GMDS-ER
general quotients. A possible explanation for this may be found in the fact that the sample
was drawn from a pool of children who had undergone developmental assessment at a
University Psychology Clinic. It may be reasoned that the psychologists who administered
the GMDS-ER did so because they suspected the presence of a developmental problem. As
such, the participants in the present study might have tended to perform more poorly on the
GMDS-ER than would be expected of the general population. This hypothesis is supported
by participants in the current sample’s mean GMDS-ER general quotient of 88.64, which is
below the general mean of 100, but still within the average range for this measure.The
possibility therefore exists that the presence of developmental delays in some participants led
to a reduced correlation between chronological age and raw HFD score. This might further
explain the finding that standardized HFD score and GMDS-ER general quotient was more
highly correlated than chronological age and HFD score. The results of the study are not
conclusive in this regard and further research will be necessary to come to a more definitive
conclusion. The finding that there is a stronger relationship between standardized human
figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients than there is between chronological
age and human figure drawing raw scores lends support to the hypothesis that human figure
91
fortunate that the sample included children with developmental delays, since this is the
population in which human figure drawing tests may be used to identify developmental
delays.
5.6. Conclusion
This chapter presented the results of the present study, integrated these results with
the literature reviewed earlier, and provided conclusions to the research objectives.
Descriptive statistics on participants’ age range, standardized human figure drawings scores
and GMDS-ER general quotients were presented. The relationship between participants’
standardized human figure drawing scores and their GMDS-ER general quotients was
line fitted to illustrate the relationship between the general linear model and the actual data
points. This was followed by the results of the Pearson Product-Moment Correlation
Coefficient calculated between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER
general quotients. The obtained correlation coefficient was significant at the α = .01 level,
which indicated that the result was most likely not due to chance. It was shown that the
correlation represents a large effect size and that this high correlation is indicative of a
marked relationship between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER
general quotients in the sample. The above results were sufficient to meet the objectives of
the current study, but additional information was provided in the interest of future
researchers. To this end, descriptive statistics were provided for the variables of
chronological age in months and human figure drawing raw scores. The Pearson Product-
Moment Correlation Coefficient calculated to investigate the relationship between these two
variables indicated a positive relationship between the variables. The correlation was
significant at the α-level of .05 and therefore most likely not due to chance. This low
92
correlation with a small but definite relationship is consistent with existing literature on the
relationship between chronological age and human figure drawing scores. Calculations used
to test whether the assumptions of parametric tests were met have been provided where
applicable, since these were essential in ensuring the selection of appropriate statistical
techniques. The final chapter of the present study in which the conclusions of the study as
93
Chapter 6
6.1. Introduction
This chapter presents the conclusions of the present study, reviews limitations, and
provides suggestions for further research. The present study utilized a quantitative
drawing tests and general mental development in the sample. The present study provided
valuable contributions to this area of research. Key constructs of the study were explicated
and integrated in the extensive literature review. In particular, it was argued that conceptual
development, intelligence, and general mental development are interrelated constructs. The
limitations and recommendations that arose through the research process may be useful to
inform the priorities of further research in this area, and may provide guidance on methods to
be employed in future studies. In general, the present study yielded useful and promising
results that support the feasibility of conducting more extensive studies in the future.
6.2. Conclusions
The purpose of the study was to investigate the relationship between human figure
drawing tests and general mental development in order to guide clinical practice. More
specifically, the study aimed to explore whether there is value in the practice of using human
children. The first research objective: ‘To explore and describe the standardized human figure
drawing scores of the sample’ was met by descriptive statistics on the standardized human
figure drawing scores of the sample, as provided in the results chapter. The second research
objective: ‘To explore and describe the general mental development quotients of the sample’
was also met by descriptive statistics regarding the general mental development quotients of
94
the sample, as provided in the same chapter. With regards to the third research objective: ‘To
investigate whether there is a relationship between the standardized human figure drawing
scores and GMDS-ER general quotients of the sample’ it was found that such a relationship
did exist in the present sample. Specifically, the observed correlation between standardized
human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients was found to exist and this
result was most likely not due to chance. With regards to the fourth research objective: ‘To
investigate the directionality and strength of the relationship between standardized human
figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients of the sample’ it was found that the
drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients in the sample. The correlation had a large
effect size that indicated a high correlation with a marked relationship. It may therefore be
concluded that a strong relationship existed between human figure drawing scores and
GMDS-ER general quotients in the present sample. The finding that the correlation between
an adapted administration of the DAP: IQ and the GMDS-ER in the present study was larger
than the correlations between the DAP: IQ and WISC-III IQ scores reported by Reynolds and
Hickman (2004) generated the hypothesis that the DAP: IQ measures mental development to
Additionally, it was found that a low positive correlation indicative of a small but
definite relationship existed between chronological age and human figure drawing raw scores
in the current sample. This finding was most likely not due to chance and supports the notion
researchers.
In general, the findings of the present study support the use of non-projective human
figure drawing tests for the purpose of developmental testing. These tests may be particularly
useful in the local South African context where resources available for mental healthcare are
95
often scarce. This is because they are quick to administer, score, and interpret; may be
administered in group format, and their use is not limited to registered psychologists thus
allowing more people to assist in the identification of developmental delays. Furthermore, the
norms for children aged between 5 to 7 years of the DAP: IQ have been found to be
appropriate for use in the local context which places this test at an advantage over many tests
that are not appropriately standardized for use in South Africa. Given the multicultural and
multi-lingual nature of South African society, a culturally reduced test such as the DAP: IQ
that minimizes the effect of language differences between test-taker and test-administrator
has strong merits. Finally, the fact that human figure drawing tests may be used with non-
reading and non-English speaking children further adds to the suitability of such tests in
South Africa. Additional research is needed to ensure confidence in these conclusions, but the
findings of the present research suggest that non-projective human figure drawing tests may
developmental delays. This is a very positive finding as it can help to ensure that these
children are provided with appropriate interventions early enough to change the course of
6.3. Limitations
This section outlines the limitations of the present study. These limitations and the
and may be valuable to future researchers. The limitations of the present study are as follow:
1. The use of non-probability sampling limited the degree to which findings may be
generalised.
2. The GMDS-ER instructions for obtaining a HFD are less formalised than that of
the DAP: IQ. As explained in chapter 4 the obtained scores are believed to be comparable to
96
scores that would have been obtained had the standardized administration instruction of the
DAP: IQ been followed. Nonetheless, due to this limitation the resultant human figure
3. The study did not include a screening procedure to exclude individuals with severe
motor problems that could have rendered candidates unsuitable for human figure drawing
tests. The extent to which this affected the findings of the present study is unknown.
Although the DAP: IQ scoring criteria were used, the HFDs used in the current study
were not obtained through the standardized administration instructions as outlined in the
DAP: IQ manual (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). For this reason the resulting scores are
therefore referred to as human figure drawing scores instead of DAP: IQ scores. Future
researchers who wish to make inferences regarding a specific human figure drawing test
should exercise more control in obtaining the HFDs. The DAP: IQ is not indicated for
individuals with severe motor problems (Reynolds & Hickman, 2004). Future studies will
benefit from employing a screening procedure to exclude participants with severe motor
problems. Subsequent studies may benefit from the use of probability sampling, as this will
allow the results to be generalized to broader populations. Future research may benefit from
drawing separate samples or splitting larger samples to allow the investigation of the
relationship between human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER general quotients to be
carried out on subgroups. Such studies will be able to build on the findings of the present
study by investigating the relationship between HFD scores and GQ in individuals with
identified developmental delays as opposed to those with normal development. The sample
included children with and without developmental delays and no categorical distinction was
made between these groups in the present study. Finally, future studies may contribute to the
97
research base by investigating the role of culture and ethnicity in the use of human figure
drawing tests.
6.5. Conclusion
The present study adds to the knowledge base on human figure drawing (HFD) tests
being conducted. The findings indicate that HFD tests may be useful to obtain estimates of
the mental developmental levels of South African children. These tests are attractive as
screening measures in the local South African context due to the existence of appropriate
norms for 5 to 7 year old children, the culturally reduced nature of these tests, the ability to
use the tests with non-reading or non-English speaking children, and the fact that they may be
administered quickly and easily. The study demonstrated the feasibility of further research to
investigate the usefulness of HFD tests in the measurement of the mental development of
South African children. In general, the findings of the present study suggest that HFDs may
developmental delays who could not have been tested with other measures are identified
using HFD tests these children can benefit from appropriate interventions. The finding that a
higher correlation existed between standardized human figure drawing scores and GMDS-ER
general quotients in the sample than the correlation between DAP: IQ scores and WISC-III
further research is needed, the findings of the present study indicate that HFD tests may
studies to investigate this finding further can provide valuable information that will help to
98
List of References
Abell, S. C., Von Briesen, P. D., & Watz, L. S. (1996). Intellectual evaluations of children
Abell, S. C., Wood, W. & Liebman, S. J. (2001). Children’s Human Figure Drawings as
Ahn, H. (2007). Visual pedagogy for student's learning of culture in world language classes:
Allan, M. M. (1992). The performance of South African normal preschool children on the
99
Babbie, E., & Mouton, J. (2006).The practice of social research. Cape Town: Oxford
University Press.
Bekhit, N. S., Thomas, G. V., & Jolley, R. P. ( 2005). The use of drawing for psychological
Bensur, B., & Eliot, J. (1993). Case’s developmental model and children’s drawings.
Bensur, B. J., Eliot, J., & Hedge, L. (1997). Cognitive correlates of complexity of children’s
Berk, L. E. (1997). Child Development (4th ed.). Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.
100
11. Deutscher Kongres für Perinatale Medizin. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag.
Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
101
Bruner, J. S. & Haste, H. (1987). Making sense. The child's construction of the world.
London: Methuen.
Bruner, J. S., Oliver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. M. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New
Burt, C. (1921). Mental and scholastic tests. London: P. S. King and Son.
Press.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
Cherney, I. D., Seiwert, C. S., Dickey, T. M., & Flichtbeil, J. D. (2006). Children’s drawings:
102
Cobos, F., Rodrigues, C., & De Venegas, A. V. (1971). Practical use of the Griffiths Scales in
older children. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 5 (3-4), 163-166.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). New
York: Academic Press.
Collins, J. K., Jupp, J. J., Maberly, G. F., Morris, J. G., & Eastman, C. J. (1987). An
exploratory study of the intellectual functioning of neurological and myxoedematous
cretins in China. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Development Disabilities, 19,
13-20.
Cormack, D. F., (1991). The research process in nursing. Victoria: Blackwell Scientific
Publications.
Cox, M. V. (1993). Children’s drawings of the human figure. East Sussex: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associated Ltd.
Cozby, P. C. (2004). Methods in behavioural research (8th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cozby, P. C. (2007). Methods in behavioural research (9th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychometric testing (5th ed.). New York: Harper
Collins.
103
Davis, D. L. (2006). South African normative data on human figure drawings of 5-7 year
old’s: A comparative study. Unpublished master’s thesis, Nelson Mandela
Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa.
Di Leo, J. H. (1970). Young children and their drawings. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Driscoll, M. (2000). Psychology of learning for instruction (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS (2nd ed.). London: Sage Publications Ltd.
Foxcroft, C., Paterson, H., Le Roux, N., & Herbst, D. (2004). Psychological assessment in
South Africa: A needs analysis. The test use patterns and needs of psychological
Foxcroft, C., Roodt, G., & Abrahams, F. (2001). Psychological assessment: A brief
psychological assessment in the South African context (pp. 11-33). Cape Town:
Frankenburg, W. K., Dodds, J., Archer, P., Bresnick, B., Maschka, P., Edelman, N., &
Shapiro, H. (1990). Denver II. Denver: Denver Developmental Materials.
(3–5), 373–385.
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.
104
Gelcer, E., & Schwartzbein, D. (1989). A Piagetian view of family therapy: Selvini-Palazzoli
Goleman, D. (1996). Emotional Intelligence: Why It Can Matter More than IQ. London;
Bloomsbury Publishing.
Griffiths, R. (1954). The abilities of babies. London: Child Development Research Centre.
Griffiths, R. (1970). The abilities of young children. London: Child Development Research
Centre.
Intelligence, 8, 179-203.
105
and extension of the Goodenough draw-a-man test. New York: Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich.
Horn, J. L. & Noll, J. (1994). A system for understanding cognitive capabilities: A theory and
Human Intelligence (Vol. 4): Theories of Intelligence (pp. 151-204). New Jersey:
Ablex.
Hunt, E. (2005). Information processing and intelligence: Where we are and where we are
going. In R. Sternberg and J. Pretz (Eds.), Cognition and Intelligence (pp. 1-25).
Institute for African Development. (2000). Profile – South Africa. Retrieved on 16 May 2011
from http://www.inadev.org/profile_-_south_africa.htm
boys and girls utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales – Extended Revised.
106
Jansen, J. M. (1991). Stress and coping in families with physically handicapped children.
Africa.
Jansen, P. & Greenop, K. (2008). Factor analysis of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for
Jolley, R. P. (2010). Children and pictures: Drawing and understanding. Oxford: Wiley-
Blackwell.
Kail, R. V. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. Developmental Psychology,
28, 899–904.
Kail, R. & Hall, L.K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental
Kail, R. & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity: Life span changes
Klepsch, M. & Logie, L. (1982). Children draw and tell: an introduction to projective uses of
Knoesen, N. (2003). An exploration of the relationship between the Revised Griffiths Scales
and grade one scholastic development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of
Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa.
Knoesen, N. (2005). Exploring the reliability and construct-related validity of the Locomotor
107
Kotras, N. (1998). A revision of a section of the Hearing and Speech scales of the Griffiths
Laroche, J. L., Gutz, H., & Desbiolles, M. (1974). Differenciation entre mere etranger chez
Appliquee. Montreal.
Light, P. H., & Barnes, P. (1995). Development in drawing. In V. Lee & P. Das Gupta (Eds.),
University Press.
J. Kalesnik (Eds.), Assessing and Screening Preschoolers (pp. 9-25). Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research papers No. C25)
Luiz, D. M. (1988b). A child with a hearing loss: A longitudinal study. In D. M. Luiz (Ed.),
Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research papers No.
108
South African Studies (Research Paper No. C25) (pp.16-20). Port Elizabeth:
Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research papers No. C25)
Luiz, D. M. (1994b). Multicultural Factors in the use of the Griffiths Scales: A comparative
Luiz, D. M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., & Kotras, N. (2004a). Griffiths Mental Development
Luiz, D. M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., Kotras, N., McAlinden, P., & O’ Connell, R. (2004b).
Luiz, D., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., Kotras, N., Horrocks, S., McAlinden, P., Challis, D., &
Revised: Two to eight years: Administration Manual. Oxford: Hogrefe – The Test
Agency Ltd.
Luiz, D. M., Folsher, A. C., & Lombard, M. (1989). The SETT and the Griffiths Scales of
conference.
109
Luiz, D. M., Foxcroft, C. D., & Stewart, R. (2001a). The construct validity of the Griffiths
Scales of Mental Development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 73-83.
Luiz, D. M., Foxcroft, C. D., Worsfold, L. B.., Kotras, N., & Kotras, H. (2001b). The
Luiz, D. M., Foxcroft, C. D., & Povey, J. L. (2006b). The construct validity of the Griffiths
Luiz, D. M. & Heimes, L. (1994). The Junior South African Intelligence Scales and the
Luiz, D. M. & Jansen, J. (2001). Assessment of young children, physically disabled and
Lutz, S., & Huitt, W. (2004). Connecting cognitive development and constructivism:
Mash, E. J. & Wolfe, D. A. (2005). Abnormal Child Psychology (3rd ed.). London: Thomson
Learning.
110
Mothuloe, V. B. (1990). The Aptitude Test for Beginners and the Griffiths Scales of Mental
Munro, J. A. (1968). Early abilities and their later development. Advancement of Science, 24,
464-468.
Murphy, R. (2002). A review of South African research in the field of dynamic assessment.
111
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D.
F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence:
Newborg, J., Stock, J. R., Wneck, L., Guidabaldi, J., & Svinicki, J. (1984). Battelle
Developmental Inventory Screening Test. Chicago: Riverside.
Newcombe, N. (1996). Child development: Change over time (8th ed.). New York: Harper
Nystrand, M. (1977). Language as a way of knowing. Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies
in Education Press.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge &
Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.
Povey, J. L. (2008). Exploring the construct-related validity of the eye and hand coordination
Ramsay, M., & Fitzharding, P. M. (1977). A comparative study of two developmental scales:
Reed, T. E., & Jensen, A. R. (1992). Conduction velocity in a brain nerve pathway of normal
Renner, J. Stafford, D., Lawson, A. McKinnon, J., Friot, E., Kellogg, D. (1976). Research,
teaching, and learning with the Piaget model. Norman: University of Oklahoma
Press.
Reynolds, C. R., & Hickman, J. A. (2004). Draw-a-Person Intellectual Ability Test for
Publisher.
Roscoe, J. T. (1969). Fundamental research statistics for the behavioural sciences. New
113
Rubin, A., & Babbie, E. (2001). Research methods for social work (4th ed.). Belmont:
Wadsworth.
Rydz, D., Shevell, M., Majnemer, A., & Oskoui, M. (2005). Developmental Screening.
Journal of Child Neurology, 20 (1), 4-21.
Sack, V. (2009). SP504 Assessment of childhood and adolescence: Human figure drawings
Sadock, B. J., & Sadock, V. A. (2007). Kaplan & Sadock’s Synopsis of Psychiatry:
Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd
Selltiz, C., Jahoda, M., Deutsch, M., & Cook, S. W. (1965). Research methods in social
Shaffer, D. R., & Kipp, K. (2010). Developmental Psychology: Childhood and Adolescence.
Canada: Wadsworth.
Slee, P. T., & Shute, R. H. (2003). Child Development: Thinking about Theories. London:
Arnold.
114
90, 1794-1797.
Sletten, T. (1977). Intensive treatment of babies and infants based on the Griffiths Mental
Spata, A. V. (2003). Research methods: Science and diversity. New York: Wiley.
Sperber, D. & Hirschfeld, L. R. (1999). Culture, cognition, and evolution. In R. Wilson & F.
Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. cxi-cxxxii).
Statistics South Africa. (2003). Census 2001: Census in brief. Pretoria: Statistics South
Africa.
Stewart, R. (1997). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A South African pilot
Stewart, R. (2005). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: Proposed new items for the
Sweeney, K. (1994). Cluster analysis of the Griffiths profiles of a white South African
115
guide for the use of the Stanford revision and extension of the Binet-Simon
Thomson, G. H. (1939). The factorial analysis of human ability. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal
Tukulu, A. N. (1996). The Denver II Scale and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A
Van Rooyen, K. (2005). The performance of South African and British children on the
Ward, N. (1997). The use of the Denver II Scales and the Griffiths Scales of Mental
116
Webb, S. J., Monk, C. S., & Nelson, C.A. (2001). Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). New
York: The Psychological Corporation.
Widerstrom, A. H., Mowder, B. A., & Sandall, S. R. (1997). Infant development and risk: An
Wills, N. (2011). The general and emotional development of a sample of South African
Appendix
Consent Form
SOUTH CAMPUS
client, whose full names, Identity No. and residential address are
Name………………………………………………………………………...
Residential Address…………………………………………………………
3. I agree that:
3.2. recordings may be made of the consultations and that such information may be
used for professional and training purposes, for the facilitation of professional
intervention or enquiry, and for the compliance with law;
3.3 UCLIN may contact General Practitioners, other professional persons and/or
Clinics to obtain or supply information which UCLIN may regard as being in
the interest of the client.
4. I confirm that:
4.2 UCLIN will be entitled to charge me for any consultations which are cancelled
less than 24 hours before the scheduled time thereof;
4.3 if legal action is taken by UCLIN for the recovery of any monies owing by
me, I shall be responsible for all costs which are thereby incurred, on the
attorney and client scale, and to include tracing costs;
4.4 I know and understand the contents of this Agreement, which has been
explained to me and is in a language that I understand. I have signed this
Agreement on a voluntary basis and I have been given the opportunity to ask
questions regarding the contents and the implications hereof.
Address .......................................................................................................................................
DATE