Music Listening Habits and Psychological Well-Being in Young Adults
Music Listening Habits and Psychological Well-Being in Young Adults
Music Listening Habits and Psychological Well-Being in Young Adults
IN YOUNG ADULTS
A THESIS
In Partial Fulfillment
Committee Members:
College Designee:
May 2021
ABSTRACT
Music listening is an activity that nearly every young adult engages in. Research on the
psychological effects of music listening show varying results. This study’s purpose was to
explore possible relationships between young adults’ listening habits and psychological well-
being. A survey, completed by 192 young adults between the ages of 18 and 25, was used to
identify the participants’ music listening habits, level of psychological well-being, and
relationships between several music factors, demographic factors, and psychological well-being.
The results of these analyses show that there is a statistically significant positive relationship
between young adults’ music listening habits and their psychological well-being. More than any
other factor measured, how an individual engaged in music listening determined their level of
psychological well-being. In addition, the lyric content of music had a significant relationship to
higher or lower psychological well-being. These results suggest that music listening habits and
the lyric content of music may have an impact on young adults’ psychological well-being.
ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................... ii
1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................... 1
3. METHODOLOGY ............................................................................................................ 20
4. RESULTS .......................................................................................................................... 24
5. DISCUSSION.................................................................................................................... 31
APPENDICES ............................................................................................................................... 37
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................. 57
iii
LIST OF TABLES
2. Scales ................................................................................................................................. 25
iv
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Although music is widely used as a method to bring about positive change, there are
instances where music may be contraindicated (Silverman et al., 2020). Kennaway (2015)
suggests that harm can in fact be caused by music, and ultimately lead to physical and mental
illness. In contrast, proponents of music use cite research that indicates music as effective in
In particular, music has been suggested as a method for regulating emotions. Due to the
emotional nature of music, it is often used as an approach to express and contain negative
feelings (Groarke et al., 2020; Hereld, 2019). However, Carlson et al. (2015) found the effects of
music on emotional regulation to be inconclusive and vary widely between individuals. Some
individuals are able to utilize music in a healthy manner that allows them to cultivate hope,
regulate negative emotions, and reduce risky behavior (Hereld, 2019). Other individuals
demonstrate unhealthy uses of music, allowing the music to encourage rumination or distract
them from addressing problems (Carlson et al., 2015). Stewart et al. (2019) found that young
people intending to use music to regulate emotion were not effectively achieving the desired
outcome. Similarly, Garrido and Schubert (2015) found that individuals with a tendency for
depression were more likely to ruminate in music and reported increased sadness when engaging
in music listening.
could have an arduous impact on the mental health of individuals. Exploring how music listening
habits can have an effect on young adults’ psychological well-being can result in implications for
1
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship between young adults’ music
listening habits and their psychological well-being. The study examined whether or not music
factors and utilization style have an impact on young adults’ psychological well-being.
Research Questions
1. Is there a relationship between young adults’ music listening habits and their
psychological well-being?
2. Are there music factors (genre, volume, and lyric content) that promote or impede
psychological well-being?
Defined Terms
(Davies, 2012).
Musician: an individual that engages in the music making process for work, pleasure
Music listening habits: The nature of an individuals’ music listening activities, including
environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relationships with others, purpose in life, and
2
Relevance to Social Work
strategies with mental health professionals (Stewart et al., 2019). Because social workers are
often providing information on healthy coping mechanisms, it is important for there to be a more
well-being. In particular, young adults aged 18-25 are most at risk for mental illness among
adults in the United States (National Institute Mental Health [NIMH], 2017). Understanding how
music may impact the psychological well-being of young adults could lead to implications for
3
CHAPTER 2
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
For many decades, neurologists, psychologists, and musicologists alike have been
investigating the phenomenon of music and its effects on the human brain. Throughout the
late19th century, researchers and philosophers created theories and hypotheses on the brain’s
music’s influence on the human brain and is considered one of the first researchers to address
both neurology and psychology within his musicology theories. His work has led to the
formation of a new area of study: the psychology of music (Graziano & Johnson, 2015). Over the
decades, research on the psychology of music has suggested that music plays a unique role in
communicating and inducing emotions (Brattico & Pearce, 2013; Juslin & Västfjäll, 2008).
led to the formal use of music in therapeutic settings early in the 20th century (Vest, 2020).
Musicians, both amateur and professional, around the country began playing music for patients
in hospitals and psychiatric wards. Medical professionals validated the use of music as a benefit
to patients when it was observed that patients demonstrated positive emotional changes after
music listening (American Music Therapy Association, 2020). Since then, mental health
professionals have been recommending and utilizing music listening on the basis of its unique
effect on the brain (Vest, 2020). Evidence suggests that music is able to convey emotions to
listeners with ease and have an influence on the emotions the listener experiences (Eerola &
Increased interest in this topic of music and mood emerged in the 1980s and 1990s
(Graziano & Johnson, 2015). This may be due to the societal concern at the time that heavy
4
metal music encouraged deviance in young people, prompting researchers to determine the
validity of the concern (Sharman & Dingle, 2015). For example, Ballard and Coates (1995)
examined the impact of homicidal, suicidal, and nonviolent heavy metal music on college-aged
men. In this particular study, it was found that heavy metal music had no effect on the
participants’ suicidal ideation, anxiety, or self-esteem (Ballard & Coates, 1995). Although the
concern for potential deviancy prompted the research of heavy metal music’s impact on mood,
these studies were widely inconclusive and produced varied results (Sharman & Dingle, 2015;
Silverman et al., 2020). Overall, this increased interest in the area of music and mood in the late
20th century has since inspired many research studies that examine the relationship between
The literature surrounding this topic of music and mood presents contradicting evidence
for music’s influence on mood and well-being. Although music has been shown to have a
significant effect on mood, it is debatable whether that effect is primarily positive or negative
(McFerran, 2016). Today, music is often encouraged as a coping mechanism for individuals
suffering from a variety of mental health issues, and in some instances has been found to
improve mood (Groarke et al., 2020; Hereld, 2019). In contrast, evidence also suggests that
music can be harmful in regard to mental health. Unfortunately, possible disadvantages of using
music have not been studied as comprehensively as benefits. This leaves little room for making
definitive conclusions on the generalizations of music’s effects on mood (Garrido & Schubert,
5
Music as a Help Agent
Music can be a vehicle for adaptive emotion regulation (Boothby & Robbins, 2011; Cook
et al., 2017; Karreman et al., 2017). In 2015, young adults identified music as a method for
modifying emotions, specifically by distracting from undesirable thoughts and feelings and
altering arousal levels (Boothby & Robbins, 2011; Papinczak et al., 2015). This was verified in a
Depression Scale, the Big Five Questionnaire, the Hospital Anxiety Scale, music-related affect
regulation, and a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning session. The study
found that participants who reported increased psychological stress often sought relief through
music listening. By utilizing music as a diversion, participants were able to distract themselves
from psychological distress and improve their mood (Carlson et al., 2015). Cook et al. (2017)
saw similar results with college-aged students experiencing elevated mood when listening to
music. In another study, listening to music for the specific purpose of modifying emotions was
Karreman et al. (2017) also concluded that music is effective in increasing individuals’
mood. Participants in Karreman et al.’s (2017) study rated their emotions on a Likert scale before
and after listening to a selection of music. Those that engaged in the music listening activity
reported elevated mood at the conclusion of the song, compared to participants that did not
engage in a music listening activity. Interestingly, the piece of music that was utilized in the
study was “Stairway to Heaven” by Led Zeppelin (Karreman et al., 2017). The successful use of
a rock song for emotion regulation contradicts previous beliefs that rock music is detrimental to
6
Koelsch et al. (2016) found that music listening was associated with more positive moods
following a stressful event. Acute stress was induced in 143 participants, resulting in increased
levels of hormones such as NA, ACTH, and cortisol. Instrumental music of various genres was
introduced to an experimental group of participants, including classical, jazz, Irish folk, South
American, and reggae. Participants in the music group showed increased levels of cortisol,
resulting in elevated mood. This finding indicates that listening to music can help regulate
negative emotions and increase mood when experiencing acute stress (Koelsch et al., 2016).
In a study exploring the use of music as a calming agent, 100 participants were observed
during a 12-min driving simulation. All participants were assigned to 1 of 5 groups with a
different selection of music provided to each, with the exception of the control group. During the
driving simulation participants encountered traffic jams, which has been known to cause anger,
frustration, and stress. Negative emotions were equated to cardiovascular reactivity in this study,
which was measured using blood pressure, heart rate, and cardiovascular impedance. The study
found that participants who listened to low activation music during the driving simulation
demonstrated significantly less negative emotions. In contrast, drivers that were not listening to
any music demonstrated higher blood pressure and a higher heart rate (Fairclough et al., 2014).
Music was also shown to act as a mood enhancement in Boothby and Robbins’ (2011)
study. Individuals that participated in the study were assigned to various groups and instructed to
engage in an art activity for 10 min. Participants that were assigned to music listening groups
demonstrated significantly increased mood compared to control groups (Boothby & Robbins,
2011). This finding is consistent with many research studies that show the efficacy of music as
an effective mood regulator (Cook et al., 2017; Karreman et al., 2017; Koelsch et al., 2016;
7
Dingle and Fay (2016) designed a curriculum based on this idea that music had the
capability of helping individuals manage their mood. Their program, Tuned In, was piloted with
51 young adults aged 18-25. The Tuned In curriculum consists of four group sessions that teach
emotion regulation skills aided by music listening using self-selected music. To verify the
efficacy of this program a wait-list control group was utilized during the pilot process. Overall,
participants in the program increased emotional awareness and emotion regulation skills at
significantly higher rates than non-program participants. This result suggests that music listening
activities are beneficial in regulating emotions (Dingle & Fay, 2016). Garrido et al. (2017) also
found that there were positive effects from listening to self-selected music when sad, including
increased mood.
growing body of research that indicates music listening has negative effects on mood. In some
cases, young adults have been found to listen to music that intensifies their emotions, as opposed
to modifying them (Garrido & Schubert, 2011; Papinczak et al., 2015). In fact, Garrido and
Schubert (2011) found that 50% of their study’s participants agreed or strongly agreed that they
concern were found to be more likely to enjoy listening to music that makes them feel sadness or
grief (Garrido et al., 2017). Research suggests that sad-evoking music includes songs in minor-
keys, which induce unpleasant emotions such as “gloomy” and “miserable” (Kawakami et al.,
2013).
When interviewing seven young people aged 19-28 with a tendency for depression,
Stewart et al. (2019) discovered that many participants intended to use music listening as a
8
method of self-care. There were two primary strategies that were utilized: some individuals
selected music that reflected their negative mood, and other individuals selected music that was
the opposite of their negative mood. Neither strategy was more or less effective than the other,
and the intended purpose of music listening did not always produce the desired result (Stewart et
al., 2019).
Similarly, another study suggests that listening to sad music was a maladaptive mood
regulation strategy utilized primarily by ruminators (Garrido & Schubert, 2015). A sample size
of 335 participants rated their mood before and after listening to self-selected sad music. It was
found that participants had significant decreases in mood after listening to sad music but reported
perceived positive effects (Garrido & Schubert, 2015). Garrido et al. (2017) studied the
phenomenon of ruminating in music and found that participants with a tendency for depression
were more likely to listen to music unaccompanied. Individuals that scored high on psychometric
scales for rumination did not expect elevated mood when listening to sad music, but rather
engaged in music listening to not feel alone. This indicates that a perceivable benefit of music
listening is a feeling of social belonging, but does not indicate a benefit to mood (Garrido &
Schubert, 2015).
Research indicates that music’s potential for harm may vary depending on the content of
the music and the characteristics of the listener (Choppin et al., 2015; Garrido et al., 2017;
Moore, 2013; Stewart et al., 2019). Moore (2013) systematically reviewed the neural effects of
music on emotion regulation, and found that some music characteristics were strongly correlated
with emotion dysregulation. In particular, dissonant, complex, and unexpected music excerpts
resulted in undesired activation patterns in the brain (Moore, 2013). In 2015, Choppin et al.
examined the effects of music listening on patients diagnosed with bipolar disorder. While in an
9
euthymic state, participants were exposed to 12 excerpts of instrumental music. Participants with
bipolar disorder were more likely than participants without a diagnosis to experience negative
effects from the music excerpts. Music excerpts that had been classified as inducing joy and
wonder were more likely to induce tension and sadness amongst participants with bipolar
disorder. This indicates that an individual’s mental health may have an effect on their emotional
McFerran (2016) examined 23 research studies. The study’s goal was to draw conclusions on the
relationship between music listening, emotions and well-being. McFerran (2016) found that
across these studies music’s influence on an individual’s mental health varied widely. Therefore,
it was suggested that contextualizing the utilization of music would be more useful than making
Stewart et al. (2019) found that participants who demonstrated more self-awareness in
regard to their music listening habits were more likely to have positive outcomes. Although
intention for music listening was not a mediating factor, awareness and insight when selecting
songs was found to be a mediating factor (Rickard, 2012; Stewart et al., 2019). Similarly, another
study concluded that competency in emotion regulation skills was a mediating factor for music
listening and emotional, psychological, social, and subjective well-being. Participants in the
study that scored high on the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire were more likely to have
increased well-being regardless of their music engagement strategies (Chin & Rickard, 2014).
Evidence also suggests that musicianship may play a role in mediating the impacts of
music listening. Studies show that musicians are more likely than non-musicians to be happier
10
and have increased psychological well-being (Kokotsaki & Hallam, 2007; Ros-Morente et al.,
2019). Madsen and Geringer (1990) found that musicians and non-musicians process music
differently. Musicians tend to focus first on melody, then rhythm. In contrast, non-musicians
tend to focus first on volume, then melody. How an individual attends to music listening may be
an indicator for their music use in general (Madsen & Geringer, 1990). For example, a study
indicates that musicians, compared to non-musicians, were less affected by sad music
(Kawakami et al., 2013). Ros-Morente et al. (2019) suggests that musicians are more likely than
non-musicians to utilize music in a way that helps them cope with their emotions.
It is also interesting to note that listening to sad music with peers is more likely to
provide social benefits and lessens depressive tendencies (Garrido et al., 2017). Young people
often use music as a tool for maintaining social relationships with friends, peers, and family
members. The connection fostered within music experiences allows young adults to
communicate their feelings and emotions (Papinczak et al., 2015). In 2017, Garrido et al.
examined social interactions between peers when engaging in music listening. The study aimed
individuals with depression. Participants, which numbered 697, completed an online survey that
explored their consumption of music and its effects. Results from the survey showed that
ruminating with sad music in a social setting was predictive of positive mood effects. This
indicates that using music listening as a social activity can mediate the effects of ruminating in
11
Young Adults and Music
personality. Due to life transitions and withdrawal from family of origin, individuals are
compelled to gain their own views, values, and beliefs (Murtonen, 2018). Research has indicated
that music culture is particularly influential in the lives of adolescents and young adults
(Kokotsaki & Hallam, 2007; Saarikallio et al., 2015). Young adults are more likely than other
age groups to consume music to a high degree and report music to be an important part of their
The heightened involvement in music activities during young adulthood can include the
participation in music festivals and concerts (Kokotsaki & Hallam, 2007; Papinczak et al., 2015).
However, young adults aged 18-25 were reported to perceive both negative and positive effects
of “loud music” listening. This remained true across several countries, including India, Iran,
Portugal, the United Kingdom and the United States. The most common perceived positive effect
of listening to loud music was positive emotions or actions. Perceived negative effects included
physical ailment, party and alcohol, and hearing problems (Manchaiah et al., 2017). Papinczak et
al. (2015) found that young adults enjoyed attending live music festivals in order to connect with
others and create shared experiences. However, another study concluded that using music for
social connection was negatively correlated with other aspects of well-being, including
psychological, subjective and eudaimonic (Chin & Rickard, 2014). Although the merits of music
festivals are debatable, hundreds of thousands of young adults attend live music events across the
Young adulthood is also a critical time when individuals form their sense of spirituality.
Growing research has suggested that religious institutions may not be influential in young adult’s
12
spiritual development (Murtonen, 2018). Instead, spiritual development is influenced by an
individual’s connection to others, nature, values, morals and sense of purpose (King et al., 2014).
Murtonen (2018) found that music, in particular, helped young adults to enhance their spiritual
connection, nurture their spiritual life, and construct a world view. Music may also assist young
adults in their development of emotional expressivity and communication skills (Kokotsaki &
In the United States, young adults aged 18-25 are more at risk for mental illness than any
other age group (NIMH, 2017). The development of psychopathology in young adulthood
suggests that this stage in life can be particularly stressful, uncertain and discouraging. Young
adults may struggle with mental health problems for years before seeking help, due to
misunderstanding the severity of their symptoms (Bluhm et al., 2014). The vulnerability of this
age group is worth noting when discussing music listening, as they also tend to engage in a large
amount of music activities (Saarikallio & Erkkilä, 2016). Ros-Morente et al. (2019) argue that
participation in music allows young adults to acquire emotional skills. They found that young
adults that engaged in active music-making were more likely to be emotionally aware,
demonstrate emotion regulation skills, and exhibit autonomy and social competence (Ros-
McFerran (2016) suggested that music listening as a coping mechanism was neither
inherently good or bad for an individual’s psychological well-being. Because music use varies
from one person to the next, it is more imperative to investigate which music listening habits
have a connection to psychological well-being. Music listening habits include how an individual
engages in music listening and also the type of music that is being consumed (McFerran, 2016).
13
Music Engagement
The way in which individuals engage in music listening has an impact on music’s effect
on their mood and psychological well-being. Various research has been conducted to gain an
understanding on what behaviors constitute unhealthy music listening habits and healthy music
listening habits. This literature provided the foundation for the development of the Healthy-
Unhealthy Music Scale (HUMS) and has helped clinicians to recognize instances where music
may cause harm to their clients (Saarikallio et al., 2015; Silverman et al., 2020).
It has been found that unhealthy music behaviors include ruminating in music that
conveys negative emotions and listening to music to distract from confronting issues (Garrido &
Schubert, 2015; Hense et al., 2018; Saarikallio et al., 2015). Rumination is defined as the
Lavrijsen, 2014, p. 120). Overall, individuals engaging in music listening tend to select music
that reflects their current mood (Thoma et al., 2011). Ruminating habits such as listening to
music that elicits “bad memories” and listening to songs “over and over again” even though it
decreases mood have been positively correlated with depressive symptoms in youth (Saarikallio
et al., 2015). Garrido and Schubert (2015) concluded that individuals with tendencies towards
ruminating behaviors in general were more likely to utilize music for rumination purposes,
resulting in decreased mood. Music-induced harm can occur when the content and duration of
the music and the intention and self-awareness of the listener lend themselves towards
Saarikallio et al. (2015) found that participants who utilized music to distract from their
issues were also more likely to experience depressive symptoms. Participants that had been
diagnosed with major depressive disorder identified that they “hid” in their music to “block
14
people out,” used music to “escape the real world,” and listened to music to distract from “hard
feelings” (Saarikallio et al., 2015, p. 214). In contrast, participants that did not use music as a
al., 2015).
Positive music habits include listening to music for relaxation, connection and energy
(Saarikallio et al., 2015). A large body of research exists demonstrating the validity of music in
aiding relaxation (de Niet et al., 2009). Music is widely used as a sleep aid, but particularly
amongst young people. Specifically, music induces a relaxed state that is conducive to sleep and
can also distract from other stimuli that may disrupt sleep (Trahan et al., 2018). It was found that
youth who utilized music for relaxation purposes were less likely to exhibit depressive symptoms
Research suggests that youth are able to find emotional connectedness with others
through music engagement (McFerran et al., 2010). Participants in McFerran et al.’s (2010)
study reported feeling more accepted when engaging in music activities. Loneliness was
identified as an emotion that decreased when listening to music (McFerran et al., 2010). Music as
a means of connecting with others and building a sense of belonging has been associated with
increased mood and heightened psychological well-being (Garrido et al., 2017). Saarikallio et al.
(2015) found that listening to music to connect with others was negatively correlated with
depressive symptoms.
In addition, music is commonly utilized as a tool for increasing energy, motivation, and
stamina. This has been shown to be effective amongst individuals of all ages (Chin & Rickard,
2014; Karageorghis & Priest, 2012). In particular, listening to music for the purpose of
increasing energy was associated with lower levels of depression in youth (Saarikallio et al.,
15
2015). Chin and Rickard (2014) found that utilizing music for physical exercise was positively
correlated with psychological, social and eudaimonic well-being. Music has been found to
enhance affect, reduce perceived exertion, improve energy efficiency, and increased work output
when utilized during high endurance activities (Karageorghis & Priest, 2012).
Music Factors
An individual’s music listening habits can also vary with the type of music that is being
selected. Stewart et al. (2019) found that music factors were more likely to contribute to the
effects of music listening than intention. This includes messages conveyed by lyrics and the
frequency and duration of music listening. It has been suggested that if the lyric content of the
music being consumed is largely positive, music listening will produce positive benefits (Stewart
et al., 2019). Greitemeyer (2009) suggested that individuals that are exposed to lyrics that
promote social activity are more likely to engage in social activities and demonstrate stronger
social skills. This was verified when 38 college students’ behavior was analyzed after listening to
various music selections. Students that listened to prosocial songs demonstrated more empathy
towards others, had more prosocial thoughts, and were more likely to donate money than
The frequency and duration of music listening episodes has also been shown to have an
effect on mood (Stewart et al., 2019). In a study conducted by Papinczak et al. (2015),
participants’ frequency of music listening was positively correlated with relationship building,
modifying cognitions, modifying emotions, and immersing in emotions. This indicates that
listening to music more often may increase its capacity to have an influence on well-being
16
Music genre has long been a music factor that has been suggested as an indicator for
emotional well-being. Particular music genres are associated with specific emotions (Cook et al.,
2017; Papinczak et al., 2015; Sharman & Dingle, 2015). For example, heavy metal music is
commonly associated with the emotion “anger” and Western Classical music is commonly
associated with “relaxation” (Susino & Schubert, 2019). In contrast, a popular rock song
“Stairway to Heaven'' by Led Zeppelin, was shown to increase mood amongst study participants
(Karramen et al., 2017). Cook et al. (2017) specifically studied the differences in music listening
outcomes based on genre of music. The study enlisted 794 university students who reported their
preferences for music to be pop, rap, hip hop, soul/funk, and electronic/dance music. In
particular, soul/funk music was shown to be the most effective in assisting participants in
It has also been suggested that elements of music, such as volume, tempo,
instrumentation, and rhythmic complexity can change music’s effectiveness in regulating mood
(McFerran, 2016). Cook et al. (2017) concluded that energetic and rhythmic music was
positively associated with positive emotions. In contrast, songs that were designated as reflective
or complex were associated with both positive and negative mood management. Kawakami et al.
(2013) found that in Western culture, music selections in minor-keys are associated with sad
emotions. In addition, participants that used music for negative mood regulation were more
likely to listen to intense, rebellious, reflective, and complex music (Cook et al., 2017).
Music listening habits vary widely across individuals. Many factors can be examined to
analyze music listening habits, including music engagement style, lyric content, and musical
17
implementation, and outcomes, may be an indicator for psychological well-being (Hense et al.,
Evidence suggests that music listening may have an impact on young adults’
psychological well-being (Carlson et al., 2015; Chin & Rickard, 2014; Eerola & Vuoskoski,
individual’s ability to display autonomy, master their environment, grow personally, establish
positive relationships with others, feel a sense of purpose in life and practice self-acceptance
(Ryff, 1989). The role that music can play in an individual’s psychological well-being has been
studied extensively, but with varying results (McFerran, 2016). It has been suggested that music
has the opportunity to be both beneficial in this regard, but also harmful (Silverman et al., 2020).
Although music has been shown to influence psychological well-being, it is also likely
that an individual’s well-being can influence their engagement in music (Garrido & Schubert,
2011; Stewart et al., 2019). As suggested in Garrido and Schubert’s (2015) study, an individual’s
mood may be an indicator for their style of music use. Ruminators and young people with
tendencies for depression, in particular, have been found to utilize music in unhealthy ways
(Garrido et al., 2017; Stewart et al., 2019). In contrast, music listeners that are more reflective
have a tendency to use music in healthy ways. This indicates that personality and mood may
influence how an individual utilizes music, which will reinforce moods they already experience
(Garrido & Schubert, 2015). Therefore, the relationship between music and an individual’s
influence and reinforce each other (Garrido & Schubert, 2015; Karreman et al., 2017; McFerran,
18
It is hypothesized that there is a direct positive correlation between a young adult’s
psychological well-being and their music listening habits. It is anticipated that young adults that
utilize healthy music listening habits are more likely to have increased psychological well-being,
and young adults that utilize unhealthy music listening habits will have decreased psychological
well-being. In addition, this study hypothesizes that there may be demographic factors that
Summary
Music listening as a coping skill has been recommended by mental health professionals
on the basis of its unique effect on the brain, particularly with young adults (Groarke et al., 2020;
Hereld, 2019; Saarikallio & Erkkilä, 2016; Vest, 2020). However, McFerran (2016) suggested
psychological well-being. In fact, there is evidence to support that there are both positive and
negative effects of music listening for young adults struggling with mental health issues (Eerola
& Vuoskoski, 2013; McFerran, 2016; Silverman et al., 2020; Thoma et al., 2011). Because music
use varies from one person to the next, it is simplistic to suggest that all music is inherently good
or bad. Instead, music behaviors are more indicative of the effects music may have on
psychological well-being (McFerran, 2016; Saarikallio et al., 2015). Behaviors that are
associated with negative effects include ruminating in music that conveys negative emotions and
listening to music to distract from confronting issues (Garrido & Schubert, 2015; Hense et al.,
2018; Saarikallio et al., 2015). In contrast, behaviors that are associated with positive effects
include listening to music for relaxation, connection and energy (Saarikallio et al., 2015). It is
suggested that young adults that engage in healthy music listening habits will also experience
19
CHAPTER 3
METHODOLOGY
Design
listening habits and psychological well-being in young adults. A statistical analysis of the
gathered data was used to determine the presence of a relationship. However, this correlation
does not indicate that music is the cause of higher or lower psychological well-being.
Sample
Participants included young adults between the ages of 18 and 25. A snowball sampling
method was utilized to obtain a sample of the general population. Participants were notified of
the study by email, direct message and social media posts on Facebook and Instagram. A total of
213 responses to the survey were recorded. However, seven responses were removed due to
participants not meeting the age requirement, and 14 responses were removed for not completing
an adequate portion of the survey. The resulting sample consisted of 192 participants between
Instrument
This study used an online survey to collect data on the participants’ demographic
information, music preferences, music listening habits and psychological well-being. Two
validated scales were utilized to measure the participants’ psychological well-being and music
listening habits.
First, Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scale (PWBS), which was developed in 1989,
was utilized to measure participants’ level of psychological well-being in the following areas:
autonomy, positive relationships with others, environmental mastery, purpose in life, personal
20
growth, and self-acceptance (Ryff, 1989). For the purposes of this study, the shortened 18-item
scale was used, which uses a seven-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly
disagree” (Abbot et al., 2010; Ryff, 1989). A Cronbach’s alpha reliability test was used to verify
Second, the HUMS, developed in 2015 by Saarikallio, Gold, and McFerran, was used to
measure participants’ uses of music. The survey consists of 13 questions, 5 healthy items and 8
unhealthy items, that address the following: mood when listening to music, intentions for music
listening, and outcomes of music listening (Saarikallio et al., 2015). The HUMS utilizes a 5-point
Likert scale, ranging from “never” to “always,” and has an acceptable internal consistency
Data Collection
This study recruited participants using snowball sampling. The primary investigator
encouraged individuals in her personal network to participate in the study, and asked that they
also invite other individuals to participate. A recruitment message was posted on the primary
investigator's personal facebook page with a link to the survey and directions for participation.
The primary investigator also contacted friends, colleagues, acquaintances, and family members
using email and direct messaging (text message and through messenger app). These messages
included a brief explanation of the study, an invitation to participate and recruit others, and a link
to the survey. Clicking on the survey link directed participants to the Informed Consent page.
After reading the informed consent document, participants were asked if they agree to participate
in the study. By selecting the response "I have read the above information and agree to
participate in this study," participants were directed to the online survey hosted via Qualtrics.
Participants answered questions as prompted until the survey was completed. At the conclusion
21
of the survey resources were provided to the participant for risk management. The survey was
distributed to participants beginning on November 25, 2020 and data collection ended on
Data Analysis
In accordance with the research questions, gathered data was analyzed to explore possible
relationships between music use and psychological well-being. First, descriptive statistics were
correlation test determined if there was a statistically significant relationship between the
participants’ level of healthy music use (HUMS) and psychological well-being (PWBS). Third,
well-being based on genres of music that are consumed. Fourth, ANOVA tests were used to
measure possible relationships between music factors (volume and lyric content) and
psychological well-being. Fifth, independent t-tests and ANOVA tests were utilized to measure
differences between demographic groups (gender, race/ethnicity, musician status, and level of
Ethical Issues
Potential risk of harm as a result of this study was expected to be minimal. However, it is
possible that participants felt uncomfortable when responding to survey questions that evaluate
psychological well-being and music listening habits. To combat these ethical issues, mental
health resources and information on music and mood were provided to participants at the
conclusion of the survey. Mental health resources given included the National Alliance on
Mental Illness (NAMI) website, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration
website, and Mental Health First Aid resources page. Links to informational websites about
22
music and psychology, as well as the American Music Therapy Association (2020) website were
also provided.
was requested. Preserving confidentiality also encouraged honesty when engaging in the survey
and was likely to result in more accurate data. However, self-reported information is not free
23
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
Sample
The majority of participants in the study were female (78.6%) and had received a
(60.4%), but also included Latinx/Hispanic individuals (25%), Asian/Pacific Islander individuals
(8.9%) and others that include African American and biracial individuals (5.7%). Most
Frequency Percentage
24
Scales
The shortened 18-item PWBS was utilized to measure the participants’ current level of
psychological well-being. The PWBS scores were calculated by adding together individual item
scores, following the necessary reverse scoring. The minimum score possible for the PWBS is
18, and the maximum score possible is 126, with a higher PWBS score indicating increased
psychological well-being. A Cronbach’s alpha reliability test was used to verify the reliability of
the 18-item scale (ɑ = .80). As can be seen in Table 2, the sample had an average PWBS score of
96.75, which indicates that the participants in this study were overall experiencing relatively high
psychological well-being.
The 13-item HUMS scale was also utilized to measure the participants’ music listening
habits. The HUMS scale has an acceptable internal consistency reliability (ɑ = .75), which was
verified using a Cronbach’s alpha reliability test. In the HUMS scale, a minimum score of 13 can
be achieved, and a maximum score of 65 can be achieved. After the appropriate reverse scoring,
individual item scores are added together to determine the overall HUMS score. A higher HUMS
TABLE 2. Scales
Item M SD N
psychological well-being and music listening habits. As can be seen in Table 3, there was a
significant correlation between young adults’ music listening habits and psychological well-
25
being. Healthy music listening habits were significantly, positively related to increased
psychological well-being between participants that listen to various genres. As shown in Table 4,
there were some differences approaching significance amongst participants that listen to
pop/electronic/dance music were somewhat more likely to have decreased psychological well-
being than those who do not t(190) = -1.77, p < .10, and participants that listen to rap/hip
hop/R&B music were somewhat more likely to have decreased psychological well-being than
those who do not t(190) = -1.78, p < .10. Participants that listen to rock/metal/alternative music
were also somewhat more likely to report decreased levels of psychological well-being compared
to those who do not t(190) = -1.71, p < .10. Most significantly, participants that listen to Latin
music were more likely to have decreased psychological well-being than those who do not t(190)
26
TABLE 4. Music Genre and Psychological Well-Being
Psychological Well-Being
M SD N t
An ANOVA test was performed in order to determine if volume of music was associated
with the listener’s psychological well-being. As shown in Table 5, there were no significant
differences between groups that listen to various volumes of music F(2, 188) = .52, p > .05.
27
TABLE 5. Music Volume and Psychological Well-Being
Psychological Well-Being
M SD N
well-being between participants based on the lyric content of the music they most often listened
to. Significant differences were found between groups in regard to the lyric content that was
most frequently listened to F(5, 186) = 4.375, p < .01. Participants that selected
“Sad/Depressing” also scored significantly lower on the PWBS when compared with the
Psychological Well-Being
M SD N
based on gender, and an ANOVA test was performed to determine any differences based on
race/ethnicity. As can be seen in Table 7, there were no significant differences between males
and females, in regard to music listening habits t(186) = 1.30, p > .05. In addition, there were no
significant differences between Ethnic groups, as shown in Table eight F(3, 188) = 1.15, p > .05.
Male Female
M SD N M SD N
M SD N
An independent t-test was used to assess any differences in music listening habits
between individuals of different musician status. It was found that there were no significant
differences between musicians and non-musicians, which can be seen in Table 9 t(190) = 1.01, p
> .05.
29
TABLE 9. Musician Status Differences and Music Listening Habits
Musicians Non-Musicians
M SD N M SD N
participants’ level of education in regard to music listening habits. As can be seen in Table 10,
there were not any significant differences between groups based on level of education F(3, 188)
= 2.13, p > .05, however, between group differences are approaching significance with a p-value
less than .10. In particular, the difference between the “High School/GED” group and
M SD N
30
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
This study found that there is a positively correlated relationship between music listening
habits and young adults’ level of psychological well-being. As predicted, young adults who
engaged in positive music listening behaviors reported higher levels of psychological well-being,
and individuals who engaged in negative music listening behaviors reported lower levels of
psychological well-being. According to the HUMS scale, healthy music listening habits include
utilizing music for relaxation, social connection, and energy or motivation. Unhealthy listening
habits include ruminating in music that conveys negative emotions and listening to music as a
In accordance with previous literature, the results of this study indicate that utilizing
music for rumination is correlated with lower psychological well-being (Garrido & Schubert,
2015; Saarikallio et al., 2015; Silverman, 2020). Behaviors such as listening to music that leads
to getting stuck in “bad memories,” music listening that decreases self-esteem, or listening to
songs repeatedly that make the listener “feel worse” were correlated with decreased levels of
Another unhealthy music listening habit measured in this study was the tendency to
utilize music as a method for withdrawal, isolation, and “escape.” Previously, Saarikallio et al.
(2015) found that adolescents withdrawing in their music demonstrated increased levels of
depression. Similarly, this study found that young adults that reported listening to music in order
to “escape” their issues were more likely to demonstrate lower psychological well-being.
31
Healthy Music Listening Habits
In contrast, this study also demonstrates that music use can also be correlated with
increased psychological well-being. Healthy music habits, such as listening for relaxation, have
been shown to elevate mood and increase psychological well-being (de Niet et al., 2009; Trahan
et al., 2018; Saarikallio et al., 2015). Participants in this study who reported that music “helps me
to relax” were more likely to report higher psychological well-being. As has been reported in the
literature, this study found that young adults who utilize music in order to foster social
connection were more likely to demonstrate increased psychological well-being (Garrido et al.,
Saarikallio et al. (2015) also determined that music listening for the purpose of gaining
motivation, inspiration, and energy was a healthy music listening habit. In this study, young
adults who specified that music “gives me the energy to get going” demonstrated increased
psychological well-being. These results are comparable to previous studies that have concluded
that music can be a valuable tool in increasing energy and motivation (Chin & Rickard, 2014;
Music Factors
In addition to the methodology to which music is being utilized, the type of music that is
used was found to have an impact on young adults’ psychological well-being. Music genre, for
example, was shown to be a factor approaching significance. Similar to the results of Susino and
psychological well-being in young adults, and rock/metal/alternative music was associated with
decreased psychological well-being in young adults. Pop and rap/hip hop were also found to be
correlated with decreased psychological well-being, as has been reported by Cook et al. (2017).
32
This study also found that the genre most significantly correlated with decreased psychological
well-being was latin music, which has not been reported in previous research.
Another music factor that has been found to impact the psychological well-being of the
listener is the music’s volume (McFerran, 2016). However, the results of this study do not show
any significant differences in psychological well-being between young adults that primarily
Possibly the least studied music factor, but arguably the most relevant, is the lyric content
found within music selections. Similar to conclusions made by Greitemeyer (2009) and Stewart
et al. (2019), this study’s results indicate that lyric content had a statistically significant impact
on the listener’s psychological well-being. In particular, young adults that primarily listen to
calming/comforting music.
This study found that there were no significant differences between gender groups and
ethnic groups. The data suggests that young adults do not engage in music listening behaviors
differently on the basis of gender and race/ethnicity. In addition, this study also found that there
and non-musicians. This result is in contradiction to Ros-Morente et al.'s (2019) study that
concluded that musicians are more likely than non-musicians to utilize music listening in a
healthy way.
Another demographic factor that was examined in this study was the participant’s level of
education. Young adults with a high school/GED level education were more likely than young
33
adults with a bachelor’s level education to utilize music in an unhealthy manner. Although this
Limitations
Due to the sampling method utilized in this study, the data gathered may not be
generalizable to the population of the United States. First, the sample size may be considered too
small to make definitive conclusions. Also, the study’s sample demographics do not reflect the
general population of the United States, particularly in regard to gender. According to the U.S.
Census Bureau (2019), only 50% of the U.S. population is female. The majority of participants
in this study (78.6%), however, identified themselves as female. Therefore, this study is more
It could also be argued that this study lacks the ethnic/racial diversity to be applied across
all racial and ethnic groups. Although the participants in this study were overwhelmingly
white/caucasian, this is reflective of the United States population. In July 2019, the U.S. Census
Bureau estimated that 76.3% of the population was white, and this study’s sample was 60.4%
white. The U.S. Census Bureau (2019) also estimates that 18.5% of the population is
Hispanic/Latinx, and this study’s sample was 25% Hispanic/Latinx. This study was unable to
obtain data from an important demographic group, Black/African Americans, who make up
13.4% of the U.S. population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2019). This makes the data gathered
Another limitation of this study is the time of year that data gathering occurred. Surveys
were distributed beginning at the end of November 2020 and data collection concluded in early
December 2020. Although this time of year is stereotypically stressful for all U.S. residents, it is
34
particularly stressful for college students. This study’s sample of young adults were likely to be
busy and overwhelmed, which may have had an effect on the data gathered. In addition, this
study was conducted during a global pandemic and divisive political climate, which may have
Implications
The results from this study suggest that more nuanced discussions about music listening
as a coping skill are needed within the mental health profession. How a young adult utilizes
music listening is an important factor when considering music as either a beneficial coping skill
or agent of harm. Mental health professionals should take music listening behaviors into account
when addressing its use as a coping skill. For example, is the young adult using music to escape
from their issues and withdraw from their social supports? Is the young adult using music to
validate feelings and connect with others who might feel similarly? Is the lyric content of the
music typically depressing and discouraging? Or is the lyric content typically calming and
positive? The intent behind music listening, the habits in which clients engage in, and the type of
music that is being utilized should all be taken into consideration when recommending music as
a coping skill.
Interestingly, music listening habits did not significantly vary between different
demographic groups. Musicians were just as likely as non-musicians to utilize healthy music
listening habits. Men were just as likely to utilize unhealthy music listening habits as women.
This implies that assumptions about a young adult’s music listening habits cannot be made based
Although a significant positive relationship was found between music listening habits and
psychological well-being, this does not imply that music is the cause of an individual’s level of
35
psychological well-being. Although it has been suggested that music listening influences a young
adult’s psychological well-being, it is also probable that a young adult’s level of psychological
well-being leads them to use music in unhealthy ways. It is more likely that the two factors
reinforce each other, as opposed to one causing the other. In alignment with many other studies,
this study’s findings show that music listening can be associated with both lower and higher
psychological well-being. Therefore, listening to music is not inherently good or bad, but
This study presents interesting findings that can be a topic for further research. For
example, more information can be gathered regarding Latin music and its apparent relationship
with decreased psychological well-being. In addition, this study suggests that lyric content is a
stronger indicator for music’s effect on psychological well-being compared to genre and volume.
36
APPENDICES
37
APPENDIX A
SURVEY INSTRUMENT
38
Music Habits and Psychological Well-Being
o Yes
o No
Skip To: End of Survey If Is your age 18-25? = No
End of Block: Qualifier
▢ Other
39
Which of the following best describes the music you MOST OFTEN listen to?
o Quiet
o Medium Volume
o Loud
Which of the following best describes the lyric content of the music you MOST OFTEN listen
to?
Do you consider yourself a musician? In this case, "musician" refers to an individual that
engages in the music making process for work, pleasure and/or socialization.
o Yes
o No
End of Block: Music Background
40
Neither
Strongly Somewha agree Somewha Strongly
Agree Disagree
Agree t agree nor t disagree disagree
disagree
I like most
parts of my
personality
o o o o o o o
When I look
at the story of
my life, I am
pleased with
how things
o o o o o o o
have turned
out so far
Some people
wander
aimlessly
through life, o o o o o o o
but I am not
one of them
The demands
of everyday
life often get o o o o o o o
me down
In many
ways I feel
disappointed
about my o o o o o o o
achievements
in life
Maintaining
close
relationships
has been
difficult and
o o o o o o o
frustrating
for me
I live life one
day at a time
and don't
really think o o o o o o o
about the
future
41
In general, I
feel I am in
charge of the
situation in
o o o o o o o
which I live
I am good at
managing the
responsibiliti
es of daily
o o o o o o o
life
I sometimes
feel as if I've
done all there
is to do in
o o o o o o o
life
For me, life
has been a
continuous
process of
learning,
o o o o o o o
changing,
and growth
I think it is
important to
have new
experiences
that
challenge
o o o o o o o
how I think
about myself
and the world
People would
describe me
as a giving
person,
willing to o o o o o o o
share my
time with
others
I gave up
trying to
make big
improvement
s or changes
o o o o o o o
in my life a
long time ago
42
I tend to be
influenced by
people with
strong
o o o o o o o
opinions
I have not
experienced
many warm
and trusting o o o o o o o
relationships
with others
I have
confidence in
my own
opinions,
even if they
are different
o o o o o o o
from the way
most other
people think
I judge
myself by
what I think
is important,
not by the
values of
o o o o o o o
what others
think is
important
43
"When I listen to music I get stuck in bad memories"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
"I hide in my music because no one understands me and it blocks people out"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
44
"When I try to use music to feel better I actually end up feeling worse"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
45
"I like to listen to songs over and over even though it makes me feel worse"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
"Music helps me to connect with other people who are like me"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
46
"Music gives me an excuse not to face up to the real world"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
"It can be hard to stop listening to music that connects me to bad memories"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
47
"When I'm feeling tired or tense in my body music helps me to relax"
o Never
o Rarely
o Sometimes
o Often
o Always
End of Block: Music Listening Habits
o Latinx/Hispanic
o African American/Black
o White/Caucasian
o Asian Pacific Islander
o Middle Eastern
o Native American
o Biracial
o Other
48
What is your gender?
o Male
o Female
o Transgender Male
o Transgender Female
o Nonbinary
o Other
49
APPENDIX B
50
Approved November 23, 2020 by the CSULB IRB.
The purpose of this study is to explore the relationship between young adults’ music listening habits and their
psychological well-being. Participants in the study will include any individuals between the ages of 18 and 25. If
you decide to participate you will be asked to answer questions about your music listening habits, about your
demographic information, and about your general psychological well-being. No personal identifying information
will be collected. The total time of your participation is expected to last a maximum of 10 minutes and you have the
right to withdraw from the study at any time.
The risks possible to participate in this study include discomfort, loss of confidentiality, and coercion. The
investigator will make every attempt to reduce the risk of discomfort by allowing questions in the survey to be
skipped. In addition, the survey will be as brief as possible and resources will be provided at the conclusion of the
survey, including website links as well as the national suicide hotline number. To alleviate the risk of loss of
confidentiality, data collected from the online survey will be stored in an SPSS database located on the primary
investigator's personal computer. The SPSS files are encrypted with passwords. The primary investigator's personal
computer is also password protected. The investigator will also attempt to reduce the risk of coercion by only
including voluntary participation. Choosing not to participate in the study will not affect your relationship with the
researcher.
You may not directly benefit from participating in this study. However, the results of this study may benefit mental
health professionals, by gaining more understanding on the role music plays in young adults’ psychological well-
being.
Any information collected from you in this study will be stored in a secure location and will not be shared with
anyone who does not have appropriate provisions to access the information.
You may contact the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs at [email protected], or by calling
(562) 985-8147, if you have questions about your rights as a research participant. For questions about this particular
study, you may contact Brian Lam at [email protected].
By agreeing to participate in this survey you verify that you have read the above information regarding the study and
you voluntarily agree to participate.
51
APPENDIX C
52
CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, LONG
BEACH OFFICE OF RESEARCH & SPONSORED PROGRAMS
PROJECT TITLE: [1656323-2] Music Listening Habits and Psychological Well-Being in Young Adults
REFERENCE #: 21-085
SUBMISSION TYPE: New Project
REVIEW TYPE: Exempt Review
ACTION: APPROVED
APPROVAL DATE: November 23, 2020
This is to advise you that the Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects (IRB) of
California State University, Long Beach, has reviewed your protocol application.
Your application is approved by Exempt Review according to the U.S. Department of Health & Human
Services regulation at 45 CFR 46. 104(d)(2).
Based on CSULB IRB Memo in Response to COVID-19, Phase II, in-person interactions with human
subjects should be halted until further notice; otherwise, submit an appeal to the CSULB IRB via IRBNet
with a strong justification and detailed descriptions on how to mitigate the potential risk of COVID-19
infection.
Approval is effective beginning November 23, 2020 and conditional upon your willingness to carry out your
continuing responsibilities under University policy:
1. You must clearly indicate in the header or footer of each page of your approved Informed Consent Form and
recruitment material as follows: "Approved November 23, 2020 by the CSULB IRB."
2. If you need to make changes/revisions to this approved project, you must submit a Request for Amendment to
an Approved Protocol form in addition to any documents affected by the requested change. Submit these
documents as a subsequent package to your approved project in IRBNet. You are not allowed to implement
any changes to your research activities prior to obtaining final approval of your Amendment from the CSULB
IRB.
3. You are required to inform the Director of Research Integrity and Compliance, Office of Research &
Sponsored Programs, via email at ORSPCompliance within twenty-four hours of any adverse event in the
conduct of research involving human subjects. The report shall include the nature of the adverse event, the
names of the persons affected, the extent of the injury or breach of confidentiality or data security, if any, and
any other information material to the situation.
53
- 1 - Generated on IRBNet
4. Maintain your research records as detailed in the protocol.
Should you have any questions about the conduct of your research under this protocol, particularly about providing
informed consent and unexpected contingencies, please do not hesitate to call the Office of Research & Sponsored
Programs at (562) 985-8147. We wish you the best of success in your research.
This letter has been electronically signed in accordance with all applicable regulations, and a copy is retained within California State University,
Long Beach Institutional Review Board's records.
54
APPENDIX D
RECRUITMENT MESSAGES
55
FACEBOOK/INSTAGRAM:
I am conducting a study for my master’s thesis and would appreciate your help! I am looking for
any young adults between the ages of 18-25 to complete this survey about their music listening
habits and psychological well-being. If you could take the survey and pass it along to others in
your social circle, I would really appreciate it!
Approved November 23, 2020 by the CSULB IRB.
https://csulb.qualtrics.com/jfe/form/SV_aVFjhN2kUBCIQfz
EMAIL:
I am conducting a study for my master’s thesis and would appreciate your assistance. I am
looking for any young adults between the ages of 18-25 to answer some questions about their
music listening habits and psychological well-being. If you could take the survey and pass it
along to others in your social circle, I would really appreciate it.
Approved November 23, 2020 by the CSULB IRB.
https://csulb.qualtrics.com/jfe/form/SV_aVFjhN2kUBCIQfz
Thank you,
-Laura Torgeson
DIRECT MESSAGE:
I am conducting a study for my master’s thesis and would appreciate your participation! If you
yourself are between the ages 18-25 please take this survey, and if you know anyone else who
is a young adult send the link along to them!
Approved November 23, 2020 by the CSULB IRB.
https://csulb.qualtrics.com/jfe/form/SV_aVFjhN2kUBCIQfz
56
REFERENCES
57
REFERENCES
Abbot, R. A., Ploubidis, G. B., Huppert, F. A., Kuh, D., & Croudace, T. J. (2010). An evaluation
of the precision of measurement of Ryff’s psychological well-being scales in a population
sample. Social Indicators Research, 97, 357-373. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-
9506-x
American Music Therapy Association. (2020, October 8). Frequently asked questions. https://
musictherapy.org/faq/.
Ballard, M. E., & Coates, S. (1995). The immediate effects of homicidal, suicidal, and
nonviolent heavy metal and rap songs on the moods of college students. Youth and
Society, 27(2), 148-168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X95027002003
Bluhm, R. L., Covin, R., Chow, M., Wrath, A., & Osuch, E. A. (2014). “I just have to stick
with it and it’ll work”: Experiences of adolescents and young adults with mental health
concerns. Community Mental Health Journal, 50(7), 778–786. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10597-014-9695-x
Boothby, D. M., & Robbins, S. J. (2011). The effects of music listening and art production on
negative mood: A randomized, controlled trial. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38(3), 204-
208.
Brattico, E., & Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 48–61. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031624
Carlson, E., Saarikallio, S., Toiviainen, P., Bogert, B., Kliuchko, M., & Brattico, E. (2015).
Maladaptive and adaptive emotion regulation through music: A behavioral and
neuroimaging study of males and females. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 466.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00466
Chin, T., & Rickard, N. S. (2014). Emotion regulation strategy mediates both positive and
negative relationships between music uses and well-being. Psychology of Music, 42(5),
692-713. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613489916
Choppin, S., Trost, W., Dondaine, T., Millet, B., Drapier, D., Vérin, M., Robert, G., &
Grandjean, D. (2015). Alteration of complex negative emotions induced by music in
euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 15–23.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.10.063
Cook, T., Roy, A. K., & Welker, K. M. (2017). Music as an emotion regulation strategy: An
examination of genres of music and their roles in emotion regulation. Psychology of
Music, 47(1), 144–154. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735617734627
58
de Niet, G., Tiemens, B., Lendemeijer, B., & Hutschemaekers, G. (2009). Music-assisted
relaxation to improve sleep quality: Meta-analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 65(7),
1356–1364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2009.04982.x
Dingle, G. A., & Fay, C. (2016). Tuned in: The effectiveness for young adults of a group
emotion regulation program using music listening. Psychology of Music, 45(4), 513-529.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735616668586
Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2013). A review of music and emotion studies: Approaches,
emotion models, and stimuli. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(3), 307-
340. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.30.3.307
Fairclough, S. H., van der Zwaag, M., Spiridon, E., & Westerink, J. (2014). Effects of mood
induction via music on cardiovascular measures of negative emotion during simulated
driving. Physiology & Behavior, 129, 173–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.
02.049
Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2011). Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative
emotion in music: A literature review and experiment. Music Perception: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 28(3), 279–296. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.279
Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2015). Moody melodies: Do they cheer us up? A study of the
effect of sad music on mood. Psychology of Music, 43, 244–261. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0305735613501938
Garrido, S., Tuomas, E., & McFerran, L. (2017). Group rumination: Social interactions around
music in people with depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.
3389/fpsyg.2017.00490
Graziano, A., & Johnson, J. (2015). Music, neurology, and psychology in the nineteenth
century. Progress in Brain Research, 216, 33-42.
Greitemeyer, T. (2009). Effects of songs with prosocial lyrics on prosocial thoughts, affect, and
behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 186– 190.
Groarke, J. M., Groarke, A., Hogan, M. J., Costello, L., & Lynch, D. (2020). Does
listening to music regulate negative affect in a stressful situation? Examining the effects
of self-selected and researcher-selected music using both silent and active controls.
Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 12(2), 288-311. https://doi.org/10.1111
/aphwb.12185
Hense, C., Silverman, M. J., & McFerran, K. S. (2018). Using the healthy-unhealthy uses
of music scale as a single-session music therapy intervention on an acute youth mental
health inpatient unit. Music Therapy Perspectives, 36(2), 46-52. https://doi.org/10.1093/
mtp/miy013
59
Hereld, D. C. (2019). Music as a regulator of emotion: Three case studies. Music and
Medicine: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 183-194.
Juslin, P. N., & Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider
underlying mechanisms. Behavioral Brain Science, 31, 559–575. https://doi.org/10.1017/
s0140525x08005293
Karageorghis, C. I., & Priest, D. L. (2012). Music in the exercise domain: A review and
synthesis (Part II). International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5(1), 67–84.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984x.2011.631027
Kawakami, A., Furukawa, K., Katahira, K., & Okanoya, K. (2013). Sad music induces
pleasant emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 311-328.
King, P. E., Clardy, C. E., & Ramos, J. S. (2014). Adolescent spiritual exemplars. Journal of
Adolescent Research, 29(2), 186–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558413502534
Koelsch, S., Boehlig, A., Hohenadel, M., Nitsche, I., Bauer, K., & Sack, U. (2016). The impact
of acute stress on hormones and cytokines and how their recovery is affected by music-
evoked positive mood. Scientific Reports, 6(1), Article 23008. https://doi.org/10.1038/
srep23008
Kokotsaki, D., & Hallam, S. (2007). Higher education music students' perceptions of the
benefits of participative music making. Music Education Research, 9(1), 93–109.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14613800601127577
Madsen, C. K., & Geringer, J. M. (1990). Differential patterns of music listening: Focus of
attention of musicians versus non-musicians. Bulletin of the Council for Research in
Music Education, 105, 45–57.
Manchaiah, V., Zhao, F., Widen, S., Auzenne, J., Beukes, E. W., Ahmadi, T., Tomé, D.,
Mahadeva, D., Krishna, R., & Germundsson, P. (2017). Social representation of “loud
music” in young adults: A cross-cultural study. Journal of the American Academy of
Audiology, 28(6), 522–533. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.16046
McFerran, K. (2016). Contextualizing the relationship between music, emotions, and the well-
being of young people: A critical interpretive synthesis. Musicae Scientiae, 20(1), 103-
121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864915626968
60
McFerran, K., Roberts, M., & O'Grady, L. (2010). Music therapy with bereaved teenagers: A
mixed methods perspective. Death Studies, 34(6), 541–565. https://doi.org/10.1080/
07481181003765428
Moore, K. S. (2013). A systematic review on the neural effects of music on emotion regulation:
Implications for music therapy practice. Journal of Music Therapy, 50(3), 198–242.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/50.3.198
Murtonen, S. (2018). The role of music in young adults’ spiritual development. International
Journal of Children's Spirituality, 23(2), 209-223. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364436X.
2018.1449737
Papinczak, Z. E., Dingle, G. A., Stoyanov, S. R., Hides, L., & Zelenko, O. (2015). Young
people's uses of music for well-being. Journal of Youth Studies, 18(9), 1119-1134.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13676261.2015.1020935
Ros-Morente, A., Oriola-Requena, S., Gustems-Carnicer, J., & Filella Guiu, G. (2019).
Beyond music: Emotional skills and its development in young adults in choirs and
bands. International Journal of Music Education, 37(4), 536–546. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0255761419853634
Saarikallio, S., Gold, C., & McFerran, K. (2015). Development and validation of the
healthy-unhealthy music scale. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 20(4), 210-217.
https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12109
Saarikallio, S., & Erkkilä, J. (2016). The role of music in adolescents' mood regulation.
Psychology of Music, 35(1), 88–109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607068889
Sharman, L., & Dingle, G. A. (2015). Extreme metal music and anger processing. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 9, 272-273. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00272
Silverman, M. J., Gooding, L. F., & Yinger, O. (2020). It’s… complicated: A theoretical model
of music-induced harm. Journal of Music Therapy, 57(3), 251-281.
61
Stewart, J., Garrido, S., Hense, C., & McFerran, K. (2019). Music use for mood regulation:
Self-awareness and conscious listening choices in young people with tendencies to
depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 199. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
2019.01199
Susino, M., & Schubert, E. (2019). Cultural stereotyping of emotional responses to music
genre. Psychology of Music, 47(3), 342-357. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735618755886
Thoma, M. V., Ryf, S., Mohiyeddini, C., Ehlert, U., & Nater, U. M. (2011). Emotion regulation
through listening to music in everyday situations. Cognition and Emotion, 26(3), 550-
560. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.595390
Trahan, T., Durrant, S. J., Müllensiefen, D., & Williamson, V. J. (2018). The music that helps
people sleep and the reasons they believe it works: A mixed methods analysis of online
survey reports. PloS One, 13(11), Article 0206531. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0206531
U.S. Census Bureau. (2019). Population estimates, July 1st, 2019. https://www.census.gov/
quickfacts/fact/table/US/PST045219
Verduyn, P., & Lavrijsen, S. (2014). Which emotions last longest and why: The role of event
importance and rumination. Motivation and Emotion, 39(1), 119–127. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s11031-014-9445-y
Vest, J. M. (2020). Prescribing sound: Willem Van de Wall and the carceral origins of
American music therapy. Modern American History, 3, 1-24.
Zhang, J. D., Susino, M., McPherson, G. E., & Schubert, E. (2018). The definition of a
musician in music psychology: A literature review and the six-year rule. Psychology of
Music, 48(3), 389-409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735618804038
62
ProQuest Number: 28320408
This work may be used in accordance with the terms of the Creative Commons license
or other rights statement, as indicated in the copyright statement or in the metadata
associated with this work. Unless otherwise specified in the copyright statement
or the metadata, all rights are reserved by the copyright holder.
ProQuest LLC
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 USA