Components of Lang and Reading Instruction
Components of Lang and Reading Instruction
Components of Lang and Reading Instruction
Chapter 5
Components of Language
Reading would not exist without the human capacity for language.
Because the components of language and their associated terminology align
with our demarcations for many of the elements of reading, they are
described briefly in this section. Linguists have identified five basic
components (phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics)
found across languages.1 Language acquisition progresses across these
components with increasing quantity (e.g., sounds, words, and sentence
length) and gradual refinement, and understanding of the subtler and more
complex points of usage (e.g., using “taught” rather than “teached”). Readers
are encouraged to explore the literature in the field of language development
to better understand and appreciate the oral language skills students may
bring to the reading process. Speech and language pathologists are a great
resource for identifying resources in this area and assisting in determining
whether a child’s language skills are developing normally and providing
support when assessment and intervention may be required.
Phonology
Morphology
Moving to the next level of language, we find the study of the smallest
units of meaning, morphemes. Morphemes include base words, such as “hat,”
“dog,” or “love,” as well as affixes, such as “un-,” “re-,” the plural “s” or “es,”
and the past tense “ed.” Knowledge of the morphology of our language is
critical to vocabulary development and reflects the smallest building blocks
for comprehension.
41
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Syntax
The study of how individual words and their most basic meaningful
units are combined to create sentences is known as syntax. As words are
grouped together when we communicate, we must follow the rules of
grammar for our language, in other words, its syntax. It is the knowledge of
syntax that allows us to recognize that the following two sentences, while
containing different word order and levels of complexity, have the same
meaning.
Semantics
Not only does the grammatical structure of our language provide the
needed clues for understanding, we also have a wealth of figurative language
and rich description that adds color and nuance to our communication.
Semantics refers to the ways in which a language conveys meaning.3 It is our
understanding of semantics that allows us to recognize that someone who is
“green with envy” has not changed hue, or that “having cold feet” has less to
do with the appendage at the end of our legs and more to do with our anxiety
about a new experience. Because semantics moves beyond the literal meaning
of words and is culture-dependent, this is among the most difficult aspects of
language for individuals who are not native speakers and even those who
speak the same language but come from different cultures and convey
meaning using words in unique ways. Anyone who has attempted to converse
with a teenager in his own vernacular can appreciate the importance of
sharing a semantic base for communicating clearly.
Pragmatics
42
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
storybook to a toddler. Knowing the difference and when to use which style is
the essence of pragmatics.
43
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
44
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Phonemic Awareness
45
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Rhyme Being able to match the Hit, pit, sit, lit, mitt
ending sounds in words. (remember this is sounds,
not letters)
Sentence segmentation Being able to break spoken Tia hit the ball.
sentences into separate
1234
words.
46
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Elkonin sound boxes for phonemes: Move one smiley face counter into
each box for each sound in “take”; slide the counters together
and say the word “take.” (A “parking lot” format with cars or
trucks may be used, as well as many other motivators.)
take
47
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
48
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Phonics
49
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
is seen as a critical building block for these students.25 Thus, explicit phonics
instruction in the primary grades, as noted in previously cited studies, was
associated with more effective classrooms as defined by acquisition of reading
skills; however, an emphasis on phonics in later grades was less effective.
Table 5 outlines developmental steps children go through in developing word
recognition skills, which is the purpose of phonics instruction.26
50
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Phase Description
Despite the ability to directly teach all possible phonics rules and letter
combinations, this component of reading instruction plays an important role
in early reading development. Rather than ensuring students master all the
rules for decoding words, phonics provides children with an awareness of
word structure, and this awareness, in turn, allows them to generalize the
rules they have mastered to read new words. Practice in writing letters to
represent words, a common way to practice phonics skills, allows children to
recognize that their spoken words can be separated into smaller units of
sounds and a visual representation can be assigned. “Armed with this
awareness, a child can then go on to induce for himself the multitude of
spelling-sound correspondences that are actually required to read.”29
Students need to understand the goals and rationale for the
instruction they receive as it allows them to develop metacognitive control
over the word-learning process. For example, they can think about how they
are learning words, the relationship between their reading and classroom
instruction, and even how to adjust their approach to reading tasks when
they are not successful.
There are several approaches to teaching phonics. Synthetic phonics
emphasizes letter-by-letter phonological decoding to combine sounds into
whole words, whereas analytic phonics focuses on breaking words into their
component sounds. A third approach involves the use of analogies with onsets
and rimes taught through the use of keywords or other known words to
51
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
52
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
“If children successfully negotiate all the texts normally encountered by the
end of eighth grade, they will encounter over 80,000 words. In third grade
alone, they will encounter over 25,000 distinct words”33
Not all words can be deciphered by applying phonics rules; such words
are described as “sight words.” Students will need to learn additional
strategies to tackle the texts and storybooks they want to read. Juel and
Minden-Cupp34 explored primary-grade reading to determine which and how
many strategies for word recognition should be used with first graders. (It
should be noted that the classrooms involved in the study were stable.
Whether these results would apply to classrooms with high mobility is
unknown.) The researchers observed students and teachers in four first-grade
classrooms that used different reading approaches (e.g., structured phonics or
trade book emphasis) and tracked when and how students were encouraged
to:
53
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
How to balance the needs of highly mobile students who may be older
but lack mastery of phonetic relationships has not been addressed in the
literature to date and is an area for further research.
While meaning is the ultimate goal of reading, it is believed that
decoding must come first. A good reader uses meaning to determine if
decoding was done properly, but readers should not start by looking at
picture clues or context. They must attend to letters first. For skilled readers,
this occurs at such a rapid rate that it is almost automatic and they often are
unaware that the decoding process is occurring.38
54
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Vocabulary
The knowledge that students have for many words is far more
complex than could be attained through instruction that relies
primarily on definitions. Not only are there too many words to
teach them all to students one by one; there is too much to learn
about each word to be covered by anything but exceptionally rich
and multifaceted instruction.40
55
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
The fact that exposure to rich written and oral language is so critical
for this component of reading makes it a likely area for further research for
children who are highly mobile as a result of poverty or other family
stressors. Such families are less likely to have the mental energy to engage in
rich dialogues with their children (or such interactions may not be part of
their cultural experience).45 In addition, families living in poverty and moving
frequently are not likely to have expansive libraries in their homes, nor may
they find it easy to access books through the public library. Checking out
books is often tied to residency—something families on the move may have
difficulty substantiating.46 Similarly, students with limited English
proficiency may have little access to print, especially in the family’s native
language, compared low income and middle income schools and
neighborhoods. There tend to be significantly fewer written sources in
preschools, libraries, and neighborhoods in high-poverty communities.47
Spanish is a common language found in U.S. schools today, especially
among one subpopulation of highly mobile students—those of migrant
families. Certain characteristics of Spanish may assist these students in
acquiring English vocabulary. For example, researchers have noted that both
languages share many cognates with similar spelling, pronunciation, and
meaning. The large number of English words with Latin roots reinforces this
claim. Thus, researchers found that Spanish-English bilingual students’
ability to recognize morphological relationships increased dramatically
between 4th and 8th grade. Whether this was due to increased ability or
greater sensitivity at this age was unclear.48 Looking for such commonalities
and sharing the similarities with all students in the class may provide
students who are learning English with an opportunity to be the “expert” and
instruct their classmates. Such acknowledgment of the special skills these
students have can enhance their self-esteem, build greater understanding of
similarities rather than differences, and strengthen community in the
classroom.
Effective vocabulary instruction must provide students with multiple
and varied encounters with words.49 Table 6 summarizes key elements that
are part of the development of vocabulary skills.
56
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
57
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Students need at least some information about the nature of words if they
are to take an active role in word learning and assume increasing
responsibility for their own vocabulary growth.56
o Talk about words—where they come from, how they are used.
o Read aloud from high-quality children’s literature that uses rich,
descriptive language and discuss the author’s choice of words
and why they make the story more exciting and engaging.
o Provide students with opportunities to copy an author’s style in
their own writing or have them suggest alternative words to
make a dull passage more lively.
Context training can increase students’ ability to learn words.57
o Since meaning is not clear when words are in isolation, play
word games in which the same word has different meanings
depending upon the rest of the sentence or passage. Help
students identify cues surrounding the word that assist in
understanding its meaning.
o Use cloze passages (passages in which words are omitted) and
have students practice identifying possible ways to fill in the
blank. Discuss how those different options can change the
meaning of the passage.
Metacognition (thinking about thinking), as used in strategy instruction,
can provide a structure for thinking about the meanings of words.
o When reading, model the thought process you use when
approaching an unknown word.
o Have students share their approaches to figure out words that
are unfamiliar.
58
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
Fluency
59
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
doubled that rate. For oral reading, the target rates listed in Table 7 are
suggested.64
Second 85 wpm
(50-80 wpm range at beginning of year)
60
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
reading level. Repeated readings of familiar texts is one way to help students
increase their rate of speed while reading and become more expressive while
reading. How do we get students to reread materials they have already read?
Here are some practices teachers frequently employ that can be used in
tutoring programs as well as classrooms:
Comprehension
61
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
A 1998 study found a mean of 16% for the ratio of expository texts to
total text types in classrooms compared with 38% on standardized
tests;
A 2000 study found 14% of materials primary teachers read to their
classes was informational; another study identified only 6% of all
material read (read aloud and by students) was expository;
There was a discrepancy in percentage of informational texts between
high and low SES districts with the gap more than doubling at middle-
high school levels. Higher poverty classrooms tended to have fewer
informational resources for students to read.
62
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
or describing the animals and their habitats that were seen on a visit to the
zoo, young students can benefit from such exposure.
Duke72 offered the following arguments in favor of informational texts:
63
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
The five components identified in the National Reading Panel Report and
incorporated in the Reading First Act were selected based on the presence of
research to support their importance; however, additional elements play a role in
successful programs, even if they are less objective and more difficult to measure.
One of these critical factors has been included in justifications for instructional
practices already listed. That is, a student must be excited and interested to remain
engaged in reading tasks. In other words, educators should consider motivation
when selecting instructional practices and materials. Allowing students to choose
topics of interest, collaborate with one another, and work with materials with which
they can experience success increases their motivation and interest in reading.75
In addition, relationships are a powerful force. Building rapport with
students and being able to enjoy one another’s company even when tackling
challenging skills is important with all students. For students experiencing
mobility, the opportunity to feel connected to an adult, whether a teacher, tutor, or
mentor, can provide a needed anchor. For older students who have experienced
much moving, building rapport may require extra effort, as these students may be
cautious about establishing a relationship that will soon end. Patience and
consistent efforts to learn about the student while respecting personal boundaries
as trust is established may help the student feel more comfortable. Sometimes
asking another staff person or peer to take the role of mentor works well. Different
students may be more comfortable with different partners. While true for many
students, but especially for students experiencing mobility, feeling welcome, safe,
and valued is the foundation that must be established for learning to occur.
Summary
What is the ultimate goal for adult proficiency in reading? The answer to this
question will shape how teachers craft benchmarks and goals for interim levels
throughout students’ educational careers. To be considered literate in today’s highly
technological society requires a variety of skills, including the ability to read,
comprehend, critically analyze, and apply information from a vast array of sources.
Reading for pleasure and having a working knowledge of traditional and new
“classics” may impact one’s impression of being culturally literate; however, the
ability to read technical manuals in the course of carrying out one’s job or installing
a new home appliance, to analyze stock performances when deciding upon a
retirement plan, or to sift through the massive amounts of media information to
decide which candidates to support in an election are among the day-to-day reading
skills required to be a competent adult. Given the increasing demands of a literate
society for economic survival, there is an increasing expectation for our schools to
ensure 100% of the population is literate, a significant shift from the days of the
industrial revolution or the expectations of many other societies outside the United
States.76
64
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
It is this expectation for a fully literate society that has led to the increasing
attention to early literacy experiences that are seen as critical to preventing reading
failure and may be the key to achieving high levels of adult literacy. As Snow and
her colleagues described, early reading difficulties are highly indicative of future
reading success or failure. Research is emerging to suggest that if we can intervene
to change those early difficulties, we can prevent young readers from experiencing
later reading failure. One of the initial steps to providing appropriate instruction
and intervention (when needed) is to identify developmentally appropriate reading
skills that children should acquire at different ages and grades. A summary table
that identifies critical skills expected from preschool through the elementary grades
can be found in the Tools section. The guidelines for these benchmarks are based on
the work of the National Research Panel. It is important to note that these are
benchmarks, not hard and fast rules for each child. In fact, the NRC commented in
an addition to the preface of the third printing of their report concern regarding
over-interpretation of the recommendations for grade levels. Use these as general
guidelines, remembering that individual students have unique needs and may be a
different level of development than their peers or even vary in their mastery of
different components of reading. Identifying students’ unique needs requires the
ability to assess students in ways that inform effective instruction.
65
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
20
Taylor, B. M., & Pearson, P. D. (2002). The CIERA school change project: Supporting schools as they implement
home-grown reading reform. Retrieved July 29, 2003from Ovid, (ED468690). CIERA.
21
Fromkin & Rodman. (1974). pp. 297-298.
22
Depending on the linguist, the estimate of distinct sounds (phonemes) in English ranges from 34 to 52. Websites
of interest include: http://www.antimoon.com/forum/posts/4025.htm and
http://www.putlearningfirst.com/language.08sounds/08sounds.html
23
Juel, C. (1994). Learning to read and write in one elementary school. New York: Springer-Verlag.
24
Fromkin & Rodman. (1974). p. 33.
25
Anderson. (2003). p. 20.
26
Gaskins, I. W., Ehri, L. C., Cress, C., O’Hara, C., & Donnelly, K. (1997). Procedures for word learning: Making
discoveries about words. The Reading Teacher, 50(4), 312-327.
27
Lerner, J. (2000). Presentation at the International Association for Research in Learning Disabilities (IARLD),
Williamsburg, VA.
28
Gaskins et al. (1997). p. 316
29
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). p. 461.
30
Gaskins. (1998). p. 539.
31
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). p. 465.
32
Beck, I. L., & Juel, C. (1995). The role of decoding in learning to read. American Educator, 19(2), 8, 21-25, 39-
42.
33
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). Citing Adams, 1990, and Carroll, Davies, & Richman, 1971. p. 461.
34
Ibid.
35
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). p 459.
36
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). p. 481.
37
Juel & Minden-Cupp. (2000). pp. 487-488.
38
See, for example, the work of Michael Pressley and Joe Torgesen for further discussion of this topic.
39
Gaskins. (1998). p. 317.
40
Nagy & Scott (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds),
Handbook of reading research, Volume III. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 269-284. p. 273
41
Roberts, E. (1992). The evolution of the young child’s concept of word in text and written language. Reading
Research Quarterly, 30, 158-218.
42
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 279.
43
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 275.
44
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 280.
45
Hunter, P. (2003). Keynote address for the National Association for the Education of Homeless Children and
Youth Conference, Arlington, VA.
46
Allington. (2000).
47
Neuman, S., & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: An ecological
study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 8-26; and Pucci, S. L. (1994). Supporting Spanish
language literacy: Latino children and free reading resources in schools. Bilingual Research Journal, 18(1-2), 67-
82.
48
Hancin-Bhatt, B., & Nagy, W. (1994). Lexical transfer and second language morphological development. Applied
psycholinguistics, 15, 289-310.
49
Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of
Educational Research, 56, 72-110.
50
Paribakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. (1997). Vocabulary enhancement activities and reading for meaning in second
language vocabulary acquisition. In J. Coady & T. Hucking (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp.
174-200). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
51
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 271
52
Ibid.
53
Ibid., pp. 271-272.
54
Ibid., p. 272.
55
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 270.
56
Ibid.
57
Kuhn, M., & Stahl, S. (1998). Teaching children to learn word meanings from context: A synthesis and some
questions. Journal of Literacy Research, 30, 119-138.
66
Chapter 5: Components of Language & Reading
58
Nagy & Scott. (2000). Citing Nagy, Diakidoy, & Anderson, 1993, p. 274.
59
Carlisle, J. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological
reality (pp. 804-849). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
60
See for example Gwynne, F. (1988). A chocolate moose for dinner. A little girl pictures all the things her parents
talk about, such as chocolate moose, a gorilla war and shoe trees. ISBN: 0671667416 and Gwynne, F. (1988). The
king who rained. Confused by the different meanings of words that sound alike, a little girl imagines such unusual
sights as a “king who rained” and the “foot prince in the snow.” ISBN: 0671667440.
61
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 276.
62
Ibid.
63
Nagy & Scott. (2000). p. 281.
64
University of Texas Center for Reading and Language Arts (UTCRLA). (2001). Essential reading strategies for
the struggling reader: Activities for an accelerated reading program. Austin, TX: Author. Retrieved May 21,
2004 from http://www.texasreading.org. p. 9.
65
UTCLRA. (2002). Supplemental instruction for struggling readings, grades 3-5: A guide for tutors. Austin, TX:
Author. Retrieved May 21, 2004 from http://www.texasreading.org. p. 4.
66
Wheelock, A. (2000). The Junior Great Books Program: Reading for understanding in high-poverty urban
elementary schools. ERIC Document: ED441927 (EDRS) p. 6.
67
Taylor & Pearson. (2002). p. 18
68
Muniz-Swicegood, M. (1994). The effects of metacognitive reading strategy training on the reading performance
and fluent reading analysis strategies of third grade bilingual students. Bilingual Research Journal, 18, 83-97.
69
Duke, N., Bennet-Armistead, S., & Roberts, E. (2002.) Incorporating informational text in the primary grades. In
C. Roller (Ed.) Comprehensive reading instruction across the grade levels (pp. 40-54). Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
70
Taylor & Pearson. (2002). p. 26.
71
Duke, Bennet-Armistead, & Roberts. (2002).
72
Ibid.
73
Wheelock. (2000). p. 7.
74
Taylor & Pearson. (2002). p. 26.
75
Gaskins. (1998). Citing Deci (1995). p. 543.
76
Snow et al. (2001). pp. 19-20.
67