Oreg Et Al 2011

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 66

The Journal of Applied

Behavioral Science http://jab.sagepub.com/

Change Recipients' Reactions to Organizational Change : A 60-Year


Review of Quantitative Studies
Shaul Oreg, Maria Vakola and Achilles Armenakis
Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 2011 47: 461 originally published online 7
February 2011
DOI: 10.1177/0021886310396550

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jab.sagepub.com/content/47/4/461

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

NTL Institute

Additional services and information for The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jab.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jab.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jab.sagepub.com/content/47/4/461.refs.html

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


>> Version of Record - Nov 15, 2011

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Feb 7, 2011

What is This?

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science

Change Recipients’ 47(4) 461­–524


© 2011 NTL Institute
Reprints and permission: http://www.
Reactions to sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0021886310396550
Organizational Change:  http://jabs.sagepub.com

A 60-Year Review of
Quantitative Studies

Shaul Oreg1, Maria Vakola2, and Achilles Armenakis3

Abstract
This study reviews quantitative empirical studies of change recipients’ reactions
to organizational change. The authors reviewed studies published between 1948
and 2007, out of which 79 met the criteria of being quantitative studies of change
recipients’ reactions to an organizational change. Through an inductive review, the
authors unravel a model of (a) explicit reactions to change, in which these reactions
are conceptualized as tridimensional attitudes; (b) reaction antecedents that comprise
prechange antecedents (viz., change recipient characteristics and internal context) and
change antecedents (viz., change process, perceived benefit/harm, and change content);
and (c) change consequences, including work-related and personal consequences. On
the basis of their review the authors conclude by proposing directions for future
research and practical managerial implications.

Keywords
change research, change recipients, reactions to organizational change

Since 1974 (Friedlander & Brown, 1974), literature reviews on the topic of organizational
change and development have been published primarily in two journals (i.e., the
Annual Review of Psychology and the Journal of Management). Some of these

1
University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
2
Athens University of Economics and Business, Athens, Greece
3
Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA

Corresponding Author:
Shaul Oreg, University of Haifa, Mt. Carmel, Haifa 31905, Israel
Email: [email protected]

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


462 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

reviews were intended to define the emerging field of organizational development


(OD; see Alderfer, 1977; Faucheux, Amado, & Laurent, 1982; Friedlander & Brown,
1974). Others summarized the then-contemporary trends of change interventions and
the applications of these change interventions to unique organizational settings,
including international contexts (see Faucheux et al., 1982; Sashkin & Burke, 1987;
Woodman, 1989).
Most of the research covered in previous organizational change reviews (see
Alderfer, 1977; Armenakis & Bedeian, 1999; Faucheux et al., 1982; Friedlander &
Brown, 1974; Pasmore & Fagans, 1992; Porras & Silvers, 1991; Sashkin & Burke,
1987; Weick & Quinn, 1999; Woodman, 1989) focused on how organizations prepare
for, implement, and react to organizational change. At the heart of events, however,
and a main determinant of the extent to which any change can succeed, is how change
recipients react to organizational change. Although a consideration of change recipi-
ents’ reactions to change is embedded within many of the works on organizational
change, the focus in most has been at the organizational level. There exists, however,
a related, yet distinct, line of research in which the focus has been on reactions of the
individuals (i.e., change recipients) to organizational change (Judge, Thoresen, Pucik,
& Welbourne, 1999). This line of research is based on the growing consensus about
the key role that change recipients’ reactions to change have in determining the change’s
potential to succeed (e.g., Bartunek, Rousseau, Rudolph, & DePalma, 2006). Indeed,
a surge of recent studies of organizational change demonstrated the meaningfulness
of change recipients’ attitudes toward change for understanding the organizational
change process (e.g., Caldwell, Herold, & Fedor, 2004; Fugate, Kinicki, & Prussia,
2008; Oreg, 2006; Rafferty & Griffin, 2006).
As is often the case, however, different researchers have taken on different perspec-
tives, which in this case, has led to a disintegrated and convoluted picture of the field.
Much of the problem stems from the jingle–jangle fallacies (Block, 1995), by which
different constructs are given the same label by different researchers (i.e., the jingle
fallacy) and equivalent constructs are offered different labels (i.e., the jangle fallacy).
An integrative review of this literature would therefore be appropriate for offering a
clearer depiction of the field’s state at this time.
Accordingly, in this article, we summarize research on change recipients’ reactions
to organizational change. As we explain below, for our review to be manageable, we
restricted it to quantitative investigations of change recipients’ reactions to organiza-
tional change. Our aim was to provide an overarching view of change recipients’ reac-
tions, and to propose an organizing structure for the various study themes. Our review
complements previous reviews and is distinct in seven important ways. First and fore-
most, as noted above, our focus is on studies of change recipients rather than on the
more broadly defined category of organization change. Second, we extend the insights
of previous review authors by developing a coding scheme for integrating the find-
ings and classifying the key variables, and propose a model consisting of change
recipients’ explicit reactions to organizational change (i.e., cognitive, affective, and
behavioral), prechange antecedent categories (i.e., change recipient characteristics,

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 463

internal context), change antecedent categories (i.e., change process, perceived benefit/
harm, and change content), and change consequences (i.e., work-related and personal
outcomes). Third, each of the previous reviews covered a relatively restricted time
period. We, on the other hand, identified research studies published between 1948 and
2007. Fourth, for both practical reasons, given the large number of studies on reactions
to change, and to form a pool of studies that would more naturally lend themselves for
comparisons and classification, we included in our analysis only studies that used
quantitative research methodology. Fifth, our review can assist organizational change
researchers in the design of change investigations by identifying variables to select
in assessing organizational change. This could be beneficial to researchers in helping
them decide whether they want to replicate earlier findings or investigate new vari-
ables. Sixth, the tables provided in our review offer a compendium of the variables
used in examining reactions to change and research context descriptors. Seventh, we
provide a valuable analysis of the 79 articles pointing out useful information for groups
of articles as well as for specific articles we found to be unique.

Method
Selection of Studies

To identify studies for our review, we searched the literature using terminology typi-
cally associated with organizational change. Specifically, in the PsychInfo and
Proquest databases, we conducted an electronic search of the abstracts for the terms
reactions to change, resistance to change, openness to change, attitudes toward
change, willingness to change, readiness to change and receptivity to change. This
initial search yielded more than 600 articles published (a) as early as 1948 (Coch &
French, 1948) and (b) in many diverse journals, which complemented those that
typically publish organizational change research.
Furthermore, we manually searched 10 journals known to have published empirical
articles on organizational change, for the period 1980 through 2007, which resulted in
an additional 78 articles that were not identified in the electronic search. Our selection
of journals included the following: Academy of Management Journal, Human
Relations, The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, Journal of Applied Psychology,
Journal of Management, Journal of Management Studies, Journal of Organizational
Behavior, Journal of Organizational Change Management, Organization Science, and
Personnel Psychology. Thus, the total number of studies we considered for our analysis
approximated 700.
Many of the articles found were quickly discarded from our pool after a reading of
the abstract revealed that they were clearly not relevant for our review (e.g., articles on
pigeons’ resistance to change in Pavlovian learning tasks). Based on the abstract and
the method section of each of the remaining articles, we discarded works that (a) were
not in the context of organizational change, (b) addressed change only conceptually or
hypothetically and did not pertain to an actual organizational change, and (c) did not

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


464 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

assess any form of change recipient reaction to the change. Given the large amount of
articles that remained, we then decided to further restrict our review to include only
quantitative studies. The number of articles that met our criteria amounted to 79,
which spanned the period 1948 through 2007.

Coding Scheme
The preliminary scheme with which we began coding was based on previous catego-
rizations of reactions to change (e.g., Armenakis, Harris, & Mossholder, 1993; Oreg,
2006; Piderit, 2000; Vakola, Tsaousis, & Nikolaou, 2004). However, previous reviews
did not focus on change recipient reactions and typically incorporated only a small set
of variables. Thus, our review and coding process were primarily inductive in nature.
Rather than impose a preexisting scheme on our assessment of the articles, we modi-
fied the scheme as we gained information during the review. Thus, on a number of
occasions, category titles were refined and new categories were added when a suffi-
cient body of empirical work justified this. After making each of these modifications,
we recoded the articles to fit the updated coding scheme.1 An example of an article
coded using the final coding scheme is presented in Table 1. For each article, one of
the three authors of this article read and coded the article based on the coding scheme
that existed at the time, submitted the completed form to each of the other authors,
who after reading the article assessed the coding for each category. When appropri-
ate, the coding form was modified and the article and all previous articles were
recoded. In those cases where disagreements were identified, each disagreement was
discussed until an agreement was reached.

A Model of Change Recipient


Reactions to Organizational Change
Through our review of these 79 articles, we inductively developed the model of
change recipient reactions presented in Figure 1. We emphasize that Figure 1 was
developed by content analyzing the information provided in Tables 2 to 4. Within
each category of our model are examples of relevant variables. The antecedent catego-
ries consist of prechange antecedents (i.e., change recipient characteristics and inter-
nal context), and change antecedents (i.e., change process, perceived benefit/harm,
and change content). The variables comprising these antecedent categories have been
linked with individuals’ explicit reactions (namely, affective, cognitive, and behavioral
reaction components) to an organizational change, and/or in some cases with the
longer-term, indirect, impact of an organizational change, consisting of (a) work-
related, and (b) personal, consequences. Thus, the model is intended to depict the
relationships among antecedents, explicit reactions, and consequences of an organiza-
tional change. We found it to be an effective guide for organizing the variables in the
studies we reviewed. The complete set of variables in our analysis is available in
Tables 2 to 4.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 465

Table 1. Sample of Coded Article


Reference Cunningham, Woodward, Shannon, MacIntosh, Lendrum,
Rosenbloom & Brown (2002). Readiness for organizational
change: A longitudinal study of workplace, psychological
and behavioural correlates. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 75(4), 377-392.
Organizational context Large Canadian teaching hospital
Change content Yearlong reengineering program; work redesign
Research design (longitudinal, Longitudinal
cross sectional, etc.)
Type of data (self-report, Self-report
interview, archival, etc.)
Sample (managers/ 654 (Time 1)/528 (Time 2) hospital change recipients, from a
executives, operative large variety of occupations (e.g., nurses, physiotherapists)
change recipients, etc.)
Change recipient Job-change self-efficacy, active approach to problem solving,
characteristics depression, emotional exhaustion
Internal context (e.g., Job demands, decision latitude, social support, org. staff
organizational conditions) relations, service quality, attention to quality improvement,
staff competence (the latter four were conceptualized as
“potential for improvement” [potential benefits of change])
Change process (how the  
change is/was implemented)
Perceived benefits/harm Risks of change (job insecurity, job interference)
of change (e.g., personal
economic, social, political
impact on change recipient)
Change content (what was  
the change about)
Explicit reactions Affective  
  Cognitive Readiness for organizational change (Time 1)
  Behavioral Readiness for organizational change (Time 1), participation
in reengineering (Time 2)
Change consequences  
Findings Active job and active approach to job problem solving
were the best predictors of readiness. Change recipients
in active positions with more control over challenging
jobs reported a higher readiness for organizational
change scores and were more likely to participate in
organizational redesign
Notes:  

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


466 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Antecedents

Pre-Change Antecedents

Change Recipient Characteristics


Change Consequences
Traits; Coping styles; Needs;
Demographics
Work-Related
Internal Context Explicit Reactions
Consequences
Supportive environment and trust;
Affective reaction Job satisfaction
Commitment; Culture; Job
Negative, e.g., Stress Org. commitment
characteristics
Positive, e.g., Pleasantness Performance
Change Antecedents Cognitive reaction
Change evaluation
Change Process Change beliefs
Personal Consequences
Participation; Communication and info; Behavioral reaction
Well-being
Interactional and procedural justice; Change recipient
Health
Principal support; Management involvement
Withdrawal
competence Behavioral intentions
Coping behaviors
Perceived Benefit/Harm

Anticipated outcomes; Job


insecurity; Distributive justice

Change Content
Compensation; Job design;
Office layout; Shift schedule

Figure 1. Antecedents, explicit reactions, and change consequences of organizational change


Note. The variables in each box constitute only a sample of the relevant variables in each category.

The structure of our review follows our model depicted in Figure 1. First, we define
the term reactions to change. We should point out that throughout this review we use the
terms explicit reactions interchangeably with reactions. We distinguish between explicit
change recipient reactions to the change, which we label explicit reactions to organiza-
tional change and more indirect change recipient consequences, which we refer to as
change consequences. We emphasize that all the studies included in our analysis
described the participants as employees (except Lau & Woodman, 1995, which included
both employees and undergraduate students), which means they were on a payroll rather
than students participating in a simulation exercise. In the second part, we describe the
types of variables that have been considered as antecedents of the explicit reactions, and
in the third part we review findings on the change consequences. Finally, we discuss the
practical implications of our findings and offer recommendations for future research on
change recipients’ reactions to organizational change.

Explicit Reactions to Organizational Change2


One of the first problems we encountered as we reviewed the studies was that research-
ers have used a variety of ways for conceptualizing change recipients’ reactions to
organizational changes, with little consistency in the terms used or their definitions.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 467

Table 2. Classification of Explicit Reactions Using the Tripartite Conceptualization, on the


Basis of the Scales Used
Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items

Affective Amiot et al. Stressfulness of the “Concern over promotion


(2006) merger prospects,” “Concern
about having to learn new
procedures” (participants
rated how difficult they found
each concern to be)
  Armenakis et al. Normative Used the Herscovitch and
(2007) commitment to Meyer scale. Sample items:
changea “I would feel guilty about
opposing this change,” “I feel a
sense of duty to work toward
this change”
  Armstrong- Emotional reaction “Angry,” “worried,” “fearful”
Stassen (1998) to the change
  Ashford (1988) Emotional Emotional discharge: “how often
discharge, stress [you] shared worries and
concerns with others”; Stress:
“tired,” “depressed,” “restless”
  Bartunek et al. Pleasantness and Items from Whissell’s Dictionary
(2006) activation of Affect in Language. Sample
items were not provided.
  Begley and Stress “How stressful do the changes
Czajka (1993) make you feel?”
  Bordia et al. Change-related Respondents rated how
(2006) stress stressful the changes were
using four bipolar dimensions
such as “not at all stressful” to
“extremely stressful” and “not
at all upsetting” to “extremely
upsetting”
  Cartwright and Potential sources of “Role ambiguity,” “work
Cooper (1993) stress overload”
  G. B. Cunningham Normative Used the Herscovitch and
(2006) commitment to Meyer scale. Sample items:
changea “I would feel guilty about
opposing this change,” “I feel a
sense of duty to work toward
this change”
  Fugate et al. (2002) Negative emotions Negative emotions: “anger,”
“resentment”
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


468 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Herscovitch and Normative “I would feel guilty about
Meyer (2002) commitment opposing this change,” “I feel a
to changea sense of duty to work toward
this change”
  Kiefer (2005) Negative emotions “Anger,” “mistrust,” “frustration”
  Martin et al. (2005) Perceived Perceived change-related stress:
change- participants rated the degree
related stress, of “stress,” “disruption,”
psychological “difficulty,” and “extent of
well-being during upset” with respect to the
the change change process; psychological
well-being: “Felt constantly
under strain”
  K. I. Miller and Anxiety “Anxious,” “worry,” “concern”
Monge (1985)
  V. D. Miller et al. Anxiety “I feel anxious about the
(1994) implementation of work
teams,” “the thought of
working in the work teams
worries me”
  Mossholder et al. Affect (evaluation) Affect was assessed by using
(1995) toward the Whissell’s Dictionary of Affect in
changes Language to code open-ended
questions about their feelings
during the change. Examples of
words coded are “confusion,”
“problems,” and “calm”
  Mossholder et al. Affect Affect was assessed by using
(2000) (pleasantness Whissell’s Dictionary of Affect in
and arousal) Language to code open-ended
toward the questions about their feelings
changes during the change. No sample
items were provided
  Oreg (2003) Affective response “I’m worried about what things
to the change will be like after the [change],”
“I’m overwhelmed by all the
things that need to be done
because of the [change]”
  Oreg (2006) Affective reaction “I was afraid of the change,” “I
to the change had a bad feeling about the
change”
  Parsons et al. Equipment “All in all, I am pretty happy with
(1991) satisfaction (the the equipment”
change involved
the adoption of
new equipment)
continued
Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012
Oreg et al. 469

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Paterson and Cary Change anxiety
Respondents rated “the extent
(2002) to which the changes made
participants feel in control
of their lives, insecure about
their jobs, confident about
their careers, and anxiety over
the future”
  Pierce and Dunham Stress and fatigue Stress and fatigue were assessed
(1992) with a physiological and
psychological symptoms of
a fatigue scale and a stress
scale. Sample items were not
provided
Cognitive Armenakis et al. Affective Affective commitment: Used
(2007) commitment,b the Herscovitch and Meyer
Organizational scale. Sample item: “This
Change change is a good strategy for
Recipients’ Beliefs this organization,” “I think
Scale (OCRBS; that management is making
discrepancy, a mistake by introducing this
appropriateness, change” (reverse coded);
efficacy, principal Appropriateness OCRBS
support, and dimension: “I believe the
valence) proposed organizational
change will have a favorable
effect on our operations,”
“The change that we are
implementing is correct for
our situation”
  Ashford (1988) Cognitive Cognitive redefinition: “I try to
redefinition, look at the restructuring as
cognitive an opportunity.”; Cognitive
avoidance avoidance: “I don’t even think
about the restructuring,”
“I focus on my current job
and try to think about the
restructuring as little as
possible”
  Axtell et al. (2002) Openness to Items included “the extent to
change which [employees] welcome
the introduction of new
technology,” “whether
[employees] would rather
such changes not take place”
(reverse coded)
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


470 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 2. (continued)
Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Bartunek et al. Rating of “In general, the NFDC has been
(1999) the change effective in the network,” “In
effectiveness general, the NFDC has had
positive effects on my school”
  Bernerth et al. Change “I believe in the value of this
(2007) commitment change,” “I think management
is making a mistake by
introducing this change”
  G. B. Cunningham Affective Used the Herscovitch and
(2006) commitment to Meyer scale. Sample item: “This
changeb change is a good strategy for
this organization,” “I think
that management is making
a mistake by introducing this
change” (reverse coded)
  Gaertner (1989) Support for the “I am encouraged by the
current business direction I see this company
strategy taking today,” “This company’s
future does not look bright”
  Herscovitch and Affective “I believe in the value of this
Meyer (2002) commitment to change,” “This change serves
changeb an important purpose”
  Holt et al. (2007) Readiness for “I think that the organization
change (includes will benefit from this change,”
four subscales, “It doesn’t make much sense
only one of for us to initiate this change”
which, the
appropriateness
dimension, taps
the attitude
toward the
change)
  Iverson (1996) Attitude toward The impact of budget cuts
the OER (items on tendering and closure of
appear to tap some services. Is the hospital
the cognitive a better place to work since
component) the OER?
  Lam and Behavioral beliefs “The [post-change] Fill-Gap
Schaubroeck (about the approach helps me serve my
(2000) change) customers better,” “The [post-
change] Fill-Gap approach is
something I like to do”
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 471

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Lok et al. (2005) Process “We have increased the number
improvement of employees involved in
effectiveness Continuous Improvement
(continuous programs in the last three
improvement, years,” “Our Continuous
reengineering, Improvement programs
benchmarking) contribute to bottom-line
improvement”
  Oreg (2003) Cognitive response “I don’t really think the [change]
to the change was necessary,” “The [change]
will do us all good”
  Oreg (2006) Cognitive reaction “I believed the change would
to change make my job harder” (reverse
coded), “I believed that the
change would benefit the
organization”
  Parsons et al. Decision “I feel the equipment purchase
(1991) satisfaction, decision process was done
training well by the task force”
dissatisfaction,
skill deficiency,
equipment
inconvenience;
work impact
  Walker et al. (2007) Affective responses “The change serves an
to changeb important purpose”
  Wanberg and Banas Openness toward “Overall, the proposed changes
(2000) change (positive are for the better,” “I think the
view of the changes will have a negative
changes) effect on the clients we serve”
Behavioral/ Ashford (1988) Information Two items were used to
intentional seeking, inquiry assess how characteristic
for feedback, or uncharacteristic it was
monitoring for for each individual to seek
feedback information about the
restructuring and its impact.
A monitoring scale and an
inquiry scale were used to
measure how frequently
respondents sought feedback
on performance and potential
for advancement. Sample items
were not provided
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


472 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Bartunek et al. Behavioral change Behavioral change was assessed
(1999) via two means. First, by
analyzing archival records
and looking for cases where
employees had written about
innovations they had carried
out (pp. 467-468). Second, by
asking about the degree to
which employees had been
taking part in the postchange
committees (“Have you served
on a committee in your own
home school that addresses
faculty/staff development?”)
  Bovey and Hede Intentions to resist A scale was developed for
(2001) the change measuring the “behavioral
intentions toward the
organizational change”
(p. 539). Sample items were
not provided
  Coyle-Shapiro Participation in the Employees were asked to
(1999) change (i.e., TQM) indicate the extent to which
they were “participating in the
activities of the intervention.”
This provided an index of
cooperation with the change
  C. E. Cunningham Participation in Employees were asked whether
et al. (2002) reengineering they participated in seven
possible reengineering (i.e.,
postchange) activities
  G. B. Cunningham Coping with change ‘‘I think I cope with change
(2006) better than most of those with
whom I work’’
  Daly and Geyer Intention to remain “I’ve become more interested in
(1994) because of the looking for another job since
change the relocation occurred”
  Fedor et al. (2006) Commitment to “I am doing whatever I can to
change (defined help this change be successful,”
as: “a behavioral “I am fully supportive of this
intention to work change”
toward success of
the change”)
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 473

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Herold et al. (2007) Change “I am doing whatever I can to
commitment help this change be successful,”
“I have tried (or intend to try)
to convince others to support
this change”
  Herscovitch and Behavioral support For measuring behavioral
Meyer (2002) for the change support for the change,
“a 101-point behavioral
continuum constructed to
reflect a range of resistance
and support behavior” (p. 478);
behavioral support was also
measured through measures
of compliance (e.g., “I comply
with my organization’s
directives regarding the
change”), cooperation (e.g., “I
try to keep myself informed
about the change”), and
championing (e.g., “I try
to overcome co-workers’
resistance toward the
change”)
  Hornung and Change “I am personally committed
Rousseau (2007) commitment to bringing issues to the
attention of the Councils,” “I
am personally committed to
speaking up at the Councils
when requested”
  Jones et al. (2005) System usage “In a typical week, how many
times do you utilize the
[postchange] system?”
  Judge et al. (1999) Coping with change “When dramatic changes
happen in this company, I feel
I handle them with ease,”
“When changes happen in this
company, I react by trying to
manage the change rather than
complaining about it”
  Lam and Compliance with “How often have you practiced
Schaubroeck the change this [post-change] quality
(2000) (both self-report guideline: give personal
and supervisor attention?”
ratings)
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


474 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  Madsen et al. Readiness for “My willingness or openness
(2005) change to work more because of the
change is (Very Unlikely to
Very Likely),” “My willingness
or openness to support
change is (Very Unlikely to
Very Likely)”
  V. D. Miller et al. Willingness to Right now, I am somewhat
(1994) support the resistant to the proposed
change changes in work teams
  Oreg (2003) Change of course The Enrollment Procedures
schedule, Questionnaire asked whether
adoption of the students had pre-enrolled
new software for courses, and if so, whether
system, behavior/ they had added or dropped
functioning at any courses from their
work following a schedule during the changing
change period; a number of questions
regarding the adoption and use
of a new system; postchange
behavior/functioning at work:
“When possible, I try to work
out of the office as much as I
can these days”
  Oreg (2006) Behavioral reaction “I looked for ways to prevent
to change the change from taking place,”
“I protested against the
change”
  Paterson and Cary Acceptance of Used scale from V. D. Miller et
(2002) change al. (1994)
  Peach et al. (2005) Intentions to Items involved questions
support change about the extent to which
employees intended to carry
out specific supportive change
behaviors
  Sagie and Change acceptance Sample items are not provided
Koslowsky (1994)
  Stanley et al. (2005) Intentions to Intentions to resist: ‘‘I will resist
resist the change, any efforts to impose this
resistance/ change”; resistance/support
support for for change was assessed using
change the behavioral continuum
developed by Herscovitch and
Meyer (2002, see above)
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 475

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
  van Dam (2005) Attitudes toward “I am not willing to change
job changes my job content,” “I object to
(changing job performing my job in one of
content, changing the other hospitals”
department,
relocation,
turnover)
  Wanberg and Banas Openness toward “I am somewhat resistant to
(2000) change (change the changes,” “I am quite
acceptance) reluctant to accommodate and
incorporate these changes into
my work”
Confounded Amiot et al. (2006) Coping strategies Scale includes both cognitive
reactions and behavioral items.
Cognitive: “Think about
challenges I can find in the
merger”; Behavioral: “Try to
work faster if I can”
  C. E. Cunningham Readiness for Scale included both cognitive
et al. (2002) organizational and behavioral items.
change Cognitive: “program does not
need changing”; Behavioral:
“We are trying to make
sure we keep changes/
improvements my program/
area has made”
  Eby et al. (2000) Readiness for “employees here are resistant
change to change,” “employees here
act as agents of change”
  Fugate et al. (2002) Negative appraisal Scale includes items that could
be considered both affective
and cognitive items (e.g., “the
change is threatening,” “the
change is harmful”)
  Giacquinta (1975) Innovation Scale items were adjective pairs,
receptivity some of which appear to be
affective (“tense” vs. “relaxed”),
and others cognitive (“good”
vs. “bad”)
  Jones et al. (2005) User satisfaction “Are you satisfied with the
with postchange accuracy of the system,”
system “Does the system provide
information that meets your
needs?”
continued

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


476 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 2. (continued)

Reaction
component Reference Variable Sample items
(continued)
  Lau and Woodman Specific attitude Scale includes items for all three
(1995) toward change attitude components: “I enjoy
changes like this,” “I think the
change in bonfire tradition is
excellent,” “If I can, I will do my
best to help this happen”
  V. D. Miller et al. Openness toward Scale includes both cognitive
(1994) change and behavioral items: “Right
now, I am somewhat resistant
to the proposed change
in work teams,” “From my
perspective, the proposed
changes in the work teams will
be for the better”
  Paterson and Cary Acceptance of V. D. Miller et al.’s (1994)
(2002) change Openness toward change
scale was used, which included
cognitive and behavioral items
  Shapiro and Resistance to Some of the sample items
Kirkman (1999) change provided were behavioral
(e.g., “resist,” “comply”), some
appeared to be more affective
in nature (e.g., “feel frustrated,”
or “feel eager”)
  Susskind et al. Openness to Some items were cognitive:
(1998) change “I think the implementation
of the recent downsizing
positively effects how I
accomplish my work,” others
were behavioral/intentional: “I
am quite reluctant to consider
changing the way I now do my
work”

Note. NFDC = Network Faculty Development Committee; OER = Operational Efficiency Review;
TQM = Total Quality Management.
a. Despite its label, the items comprising this normative commitment scale involve affective content
(what employees feel about the change), which is why we classified it here as an affective reaction.
b. Despite its label, the items comprising this affective commitment scale involve cognitive content (what
employees think about the change), which is why we classified it here as a cognitive reaction.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 477

Sometimes different terms were used for describing the same phenomenon (the
“jangle” fallacy, Block, 1995), and at other times the same term was given to constructs
with diverse definitions (the “jingle” fallacy, Block, 1995). As a means for organizing
the concepts used, we define explicit reactions to change by employing Piderit’s (2000)
tripartite definition of resistance to change, which includes affective, cognitive, and
behavioral components of the reactions to the change (see Figure 1). By considering
how recipients’ reactions to change were measured, we classified explicit reactions in
each of the studies reviewed into affective, cognitive, or behavioral reactions. As noted
above, regarding the jingle–jangle fallacies, the names of the variables used to capture
the change recipients’ reactions often suggest several possible classifications into
Piderit’s tripartite conceptualization. Thus, we turned to the actual scales used for tap-
ping these variables in determining how to classify variables (see Table 2). Our main
criterion for considering a variable to be an explicit reaction was that it pertains directly
to how change recipients feel (affect), what they think (cognition), or what they intend
to do (behavior) in response to the change.

Affective Reactions
A first set of studies we reviewed focused on change recipients’ affective reactions
to change (e.g., Armstrong-Stassen, 1998; Ashford, 1988; Martin, Jones, & Callan,
2006). A number of these studies focused on negative reactions, such as the stress
experienced by change recipients as a result of the change (Amiot, Terry, Jimmieson,
& Callan, 2006; Ashford, 1988; Begley & Czajka, 1993; Bordia, Jones, Gallois,
Callan, & Difonzo, 2006; Cartwright & Cooper, 1993; Martin, Jones, & Callan, 2005).
Other forms of psychological distress have also been considered, including anxiety
(e.g., K. I. Miller & Monge, 1985; V. D. Miller, Johnson, & Grau, 1994; Oreg, 2006;
Paterson & Cary, 2002), fatigue (Pierce & Dunham, 1992), and negative emotions
(Kiefer, 2005). Contrary to the negative frame of psychological distress, some studies
used a positive frame and measured factors such as pleasantness (Bartunek et al.,
2006; Mossholder, Settoon, Armenakis, & Harris, 2000; Mossholder, Settoon, Harris,
& Armenakis, 1995), change-related satisfaction (Jones, Jimmieson, & Griffiths,
2005; Parsons, Liden, O’Connor, & Nagao, 1991), and affective aspects of organiza-
tional change commitment (Walker, Armenakis, & Bernerth, 2007).

Cognitive Reactions
A second set of studies considered the cognitive aspects of change recipients’ explicit
reactions to change (see Figure 1). The scales used in these studies to tap recipients’
reactions to the change pertained to recipients’ assessment of the change’s value for
themselves, for the organization, or both. For example, in one study, one of the
aspects considered in change recipients’ reactions to the change was the degree to
which change recipients had a positive view of the change (“Overall, the proposed
changes are for the better,” Wanberg & Banas, 2000). Following a cognitive approach

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


478 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

to viewing organizational change, Bartunek et al. (2006) conceptualized change


recipients’ reactions to organizational change using terms such as sensemaking, that
is, what do the change recipients believe the change means, and effectiveness
(Bartunek, Greenberg, & Davidson, 1999; for a similar view of the effectiveness con-
cept, see also Lok, Hung, Walsh, Wang, & Crawford, 2005). Although the terms that
researchers have used may not explicitly indicate this, in our examination of the scales
used to measure recipient reactions, we found several additional terms that appear to
involve a cognitive conceptualization, such as decision satisfaction (Parsons et al.,
1991), change commitment (Bernerth, Armenakis, Feild, & Walker, 2007), support
for the business strategy (Gaertner, 1989), openness to the change (Axtell et al.,
2002), and perceived fairness (Daly & Geyer, 1994), as well as several others
(C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002; Iverson, 1996; Oreg, 2006).

Behavioral Reactions
A third set of studies focused on behavioral reactions (see Figure 1). In these studies,
behavioral reactions to change were conceptualized either as explicit behaviors in
response to the change or as reported intentions to behave. In a number of studies,
researchers measured the degree to which change recipients became actively involved
in activities that were encouraged as part of the change (Bartunek et al., 1999; Coyle-
Shapiro, 1999; C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002; Jones et al., 2005; Lam & Schaubroeck,
2000; Oreg, 2003). Contrary to acceptance and involvement behaviors, others focused
on withdrawal behaviors such as quitting intentions due to the change (Daly & Geyer,
1994; Martin et al., 2005).
Other studies explicitly analyzed change recipients’ behavioral intentions to resist or
support the change (Bovey & Hede, 2001; Madsen, Miller, & John, 2005; V. D. Miller
et al., 1994; Oreg, 2006; Paterson & Cary, 2002; Peach, Jimmieson, & White, 2005;
Sagie & Koslowsky, 1994; Stanley, Meyer, & Topolnytsky, 2005). For example, in one
study the “behavioral intention to resist” the change was measured with a scale includ-
ing 20 items, such as “undermine,” “oppose,” or “support,” in response to which change
recipients were asked to rate their intentions (Bovey & Hede, 2001). Contrary to the
studies cited with respect to the affective or cognitive component of change recipients’
explicit reactions to change, several of the studies on behavioral reactions used a change
commitment scale consisting of items such as “I am doing whatever I can to help this
change be successful” or “I have tried (or intend to try) to convince others to support
this change” (Herold, Fedor, & Caldwell, 2007, p. 946), all of which explicitly
pertain to individuals’ behaviors or intentions in response to the change (Fedor,
Caldwell, & Herold, 2006; Herold et al., 2007; Herscovitch & Meyer, 2002; Hornung
& Rousseau, 2007).
Finally, a number of studies looked at coping as a behavioral outcome representing
change recipients’ explicit reactions to change (Amiot et al., 2006; G. B. Cunningham,
2006; Judge et al., 1999). These studies either considered individuals’ stress-related

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 479

coping strategies or individuals’ reported functioning given the conditions of change


(G. B. Cunningham, 2006; Judge et al., 1999).

Multiple Reactions3
Although not with the expressed purpose of tapping separate reaction components,
several of the studies we reviewed assessed more than a single component. For
example, Bartunek et al. (1999) assessed both the cognitive evaluation of the change
(i.e., rating of change effectiveness) as well as the behavioral response to it (i.e., par-
ticipation in postchange activities). In another study, both affective (i.e., change-
related stress) and behavioral (i.e., absenteeism and intentions to quit in direct response
to the change) responses were considered (Martin et al., 2005). In a few cases, all
three explicit reaction components were included as distinct constructs (Ashford,
1988; Herscovitch & Meyer, 2002; Oreg, 2003, Study 7), and in only one case was
this done explicitly with the purpose of separately measuring each of the three change
reaction components (Oreg, 2006).

Confounded Reactions
In a number of cases, it was not possible to classify the explicit reaction to change
variable to either of the reaction components. This is because in many of the studies
the reactions to change were not assessed with a tridimensional definition of reactions in
mind. Therefore, measures of reactions to change in these studies combined items that
tap different components (C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002; Eby, Adams, Russell, &
Gaby, 2000; Fugate, Kinicki, & Scheck, 2002; Giacquinta, 1975; Lau & Woodman,
1995; Shapiro & Kirkman, 1999; Susskind, Miller, & Johnson, 1998). In other stud-
ies, questions about the reaction to change were very general (e.g., “employees here
are resistant to change,” Eby et al., 2000) and thus do not tap any particular compo-
nent (affect, cognition, behavior) of the reaction toward change. The relationships
sought in these studies were therefore between the hypothesized antecedents and
change recipients’ overall orientation toward the specific change.

Antecedents of Change
Recipient Reactions to Change4
It is important to note that the above summary of findings deals with change recipients’
explicit reactions to change. The antecedents to explicit reactions are appropriately
conceptualized as the reasons for the reactions rather than the reaction itself (see Figure 1).
These involve variables that predict either change recipients’ explicit reactions (as
reviewed above), or the indirect, and often longer-term change consequences (these
will be reviewed below). Depicted in Figure 1 are the five primary antecedent catego-
ries we identified in our review: (a) change recipient characteristics, (b) internal con-
text, (c) change process, (d) perceived benefit/harm, and (e) change content (see Table 3).

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


480 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 3. Antecedents Variables Considered in Articles Reviewed

Antecedent
category Variable References
Recipient Personality traits  
characteristics  (1) Locus of control; (1) Fried et al. (1996); Holt et al. (2007);
    (2) personal control Judge et al. (1999); Lau and Woodman
(1995); Naswall et al. (2005) (2) Ashford
(1988); Fugate et al. (2002); Martin et al.
(2005); Paulsen et al. (2005); Wanberg
and Banas (2000)
   Self-efficacy Amiot et al. (2006); Armenakis
et al. (2007); Ashford (1998);
C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002); Eby et al.
(2000); Herold et al. (2007); Holt et al.
(2007); Hornung and Rousseau (2007);
Judge et al. (1999); Logan and Ganster
(2007); Martin et al. (2005); Peach et al.
(2005); Wanberg and Banas (2000)
   Positive and negative Begley and Czajka (1993); Fugate et al.
 affectivity (2002); Holt et al. (2007); Iverson (1996);
Judge et al. (1999); Naswall et al. (2005)
    Tolerance for ambiguity Ashford (1988); Judge et al. (1999);
Walker et al. (2007)
   Dispositional resistance to Oreg (2003); Oreg (2006)
 change
   Self-esteem Ashford (1988); Giacquinta (1975); Judge
et al. (1999); Wanberg and Banas (2000)
    Attitude toward change Holt et al. (2007); Lau and Woodman
(1995)
    Openness to experience Jones et al. (2005); Judge et al. (1999)
   Other predispositions: (1) Stanley et al. (2005); (2) Wanberg and
  (1) cynicism; (2) optimism; Banas (2000); (3) Rafferty and Griffin
  (3) neuroticism; (2006); (4) Rafferty and Griffin (2006);
  (4) conscientiousness; (5) Lau and Woodman (1995);
  (5) dogmatism; (6) Bordia et al. (2004);
  (6) uncertainty; (7) Fried et al. (1996); (8) Holt et al.
  (7) helplessness; (2007); (9) Hornung and Rousseau
  (8) rebelliousness; (2007); (10) Judge et al. (1999);
  (9) initiative; (10) risk (11) C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002);
  aversion; (11) depression; (12) Ashford (1988); (13) Gopinath and
  (12) freedom from Becker (2000); (14) Giacquinta (1975);
 self-denigration; (15) Giacquinta (1975); (16) Caldwell
  (13) dispositional et al. (2004); (17) Coyle-Shapiro (1999);
  impression management; (18) Eby et al. (2000)
  (14) orientation toward
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 481

Table 3. (continued)

Antecedent
category Variable References
  teacher–pupil relationship;
  (15) attitude toward
  education; (16) mastery
  of motivational orientation;
  (17) ability to contribute;
  (18) preference for working
  in teams
  Coping styles Bovey and Hede (2001); Cartwright and
Cooper (1993); Fugate et al. (2002)
  Needs  
    Higher order needs Bhagat and Chassie (1980); Coyle-Shapiro
and Morrow (2003);V. D. Miller et al.
(1994)
    Need for feedback Johnson et al. (1996)
    Need for privacy K. I. Miller and Monge (1985)
    Need for interdependence K. I. Miller and Monge (1985)
    Need for affiliation V. D. Miller et al. (1994)
  Demographics  
   Age Begley and Czajka (1993); Bordia et al.
(2004); Caldwell et al. (2004); Coyle-
Shapiro (1999); Coyle-Shapiro (2002);
Giacquinta (1975); Hornung and
Rousseau (2007); Iverson (1996); Jones
et al. (2005); Kiefer (2005); Madsen et al.
(2005); Martin et al. (2005); Parsons
et al. (1991); Peach et al. (2005); Rafferty
and Griffin (2006); Spreitzer and Mishra
(2002); Weber and Weber (2001)
   Gender Armstrong-Stassen (1998); Begley and
Czajka (1993); Bordia et al. (2004);
Coyle-Shapiro (1999); Giacquinta
(1975); Hornung and Rousseau (2007);
Iverson (1996); Jones et al. (2005);
Kiefer (2005); Madsen et al. (2005);
Martin et al. (2005); Morgeson et al.
(2006); Parsons et al. (1991); Peach et al.
(2005); Spreitzer and Mishra (2002)
   Tenure Begley and Czajka (1993); Coyle-Shapiro
(1999); Coyle-Shapiro (2002); Hornung
and Rousseau (2007); Iverson (1996);
Kiefer (2005); Madsen et al. (2005);
Martin et al. (2005); Morgeson et al.
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


482 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 3. (continued)

Antecedent
category Variable References
(2006); Peach et al. (2005); Rafferty and
Griffin (2006); Spreitzer and Mishra
(2002); Weber and Weber (2001)
    Organizational status Armstrong-Stassen (1998); Giacquinta
(1975); Iverson (1996); Martin et al.
(2005); Parsons et al. (1991); Zalesny
and Farace (1987)
   Other demographics: (1) Begley and Czajka (1993); Hornung
  (1) education; (2) marital and Rousseau (2007); Madsen et al.
  status; (3) no. of children; (2005); Parsons et al. (1991); Spreitzer
  (4) employment status; and Mishra (2002); Weber and Weber
  (5) computer experience; (2001); (2) Begley and Czajka (1993);
  (6) survivor/victim; C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002); Madsen
  (7) income (8) elderly et al. (2005); (3) C. E. Cunningham et al.
  dependents; (9) job type; (2002); Giacquinta (1975); Madsen et al.
  (10) care of family members; (2005); Spreitzer and Mishra (2002);
  (11) job centrality; (4) Martin et al. (2005); (5) Parsons et al.
  (12) career opportunity; (1991); (6) Fried et al. (1996); Paulsen
  (13) personal conditions; et al. (2005); (7) C. E. Cunningham et al.
  (14) empowerment; (2002); Spreitzer and Mishra (2002);
  (15) religion; (16) exposure (8) Spreitzer and Mishra (2002);
  to technology; (9) Coyle-Shapiro (1999);
  (17) emotional exhaustion (10) C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002);
Iverson (1996); (11) Gaertner (1989);
(12) Gaertner (1989); (13) Kiefer
(2005); (14) Spreitzer and Mishra (2002);
(15) Giacquinta (1975); (16) Axtell
et al. (2002); (17) C. E. Cunningham
et al. (2002)
Internal context Supportive environment/trust  
   (1) Management support (1) Coyle-Shapiro and Morrow (2003);
  (2) social support Iverson (1996); Martin et al. (2005);
Peach et al. (2005); Rafferty and Griffin
(2006); (2) C. E. Cunningham et al.
(2002); Fugate et al. (2002); Madsen et al.
(2005); Wanberg and Banas (2000)
    Trust in management Eby et al. (2000); Oreg (2006); Spreitzer
and Mishra (2002); Stanley et al. (2005)
    Trust in colleagues Coyle-Shapiro and Morrow (2003); Eby
et al. (2000)
  Organizational commitment Begley and Czajka (1993); Covin et al.
(1996); Coyle-Shapiro and Morrow
(2003); Coyle-Shapiro (1999); Coyle-
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 483

Table 3. (continued)

Antecedent
category Variable References
Shapiro (2002); Herscovitch and Meyer
(2002); Lau and Woodman (1995);
Madsen et al. (2005); Lee and Peccei
(2007); van Dam (2005)
  Organizational culture and Cartwright and Cooper (1993);
climate C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002); Iverson
(1996); Jones et al. (2005); Martin et al.
(2005)
  Job characteristics Bhagat and Chassie (1980);
C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002); Eby
et al. (2000); Hornung and Rousseau
(2007); Iverson (1996); Weber and
Weber (2001)
  Miscellaneous factors  
   (1) Job control/power; (1) Logan and Ganster (2007); Peach et al.
  (2) job value, alternatives, (2005); (2) van Dam (2005); (3) Covin
  investment; (3) job et al. (1996); Iverson (1996); Lam and
  satisfaction; (4) uncertainty; Schaubroeck (2000); van Dam (2005);
  (5) role ambiguity, conflict, (4) Ashford (1988); Iverson (1996); Lee
  overload; (6) turnover and Peccei (2007); Shapiro and Kirkman
  intentions; (7) management/ (1999); (5) Iverson (1996); Kiefer (2005);
  staff competence; V. D. Miller et al. (1994); (6) Covin et al.
  (8) change turbulence, (1996); Lam and Schaubroeck (2000);
 frequency; (7) Gaertner (1989); Holt et al. (2007);
  (9) communication; C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002); Jones
  (10) product, service quality; et al. (2005); Stanley et al. (2005);
  (11) customer satisfaction; (8) Fedor et al. (2006); Rafferty and
  (12) perceived organizational Griffin (2006); (9) Covin et al. (1996);
  support; (13) teamwork; Holt et al. (2007); (10) C. E. Cunningham
  (14) merger and acquisition et al. (2002); Lam and Schaubroeck
  (i.e., acquirer vs. acquired (2000); (11) Lam and Schaubroeck
  firm); (15) organizational (2000); Martin et al. (2005); (12) Lee
  type; (16) organizational and Peccei (2007); (13) Covin et al.
  structure/strategy; (17) (1996); (14) Covin et al. (1996);
  organizational cynicism; (15) Cartwright and Cooper (1993);
  (18) organizational justice; (16) Lok et al. (2005); (17) Bernerth
  (19) organizational systems; et al. (2007); Gaertner (1989); Walker
  (20) organization based self- et al. (2007); Stanley et al. (2005); (18)
  esteem; (21) organizational Coyle-Shapiro (1999); Iverson (1996);
 identification; Kiefer (2005); (19) Eby et al. (2000); Lok
  (22) organizational et al. (2005); Morgeson et al. (2006);
 information; (20) Lee and Peccei (2007); (21) V. D. Miller
  (23) discrepancy et al. (1994); (22) V. D. Miller et al.
(1994); (23) Armenakis et al. (2007)
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


484 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 3. (continued)

Antecedent
category Variable References
Change process Participation Amiot et al. (2006); Axtell et al. (2002);
Bartunek et al. (1999); Bartunek et al.
(2006); Coch and French (1948); Coyle-
Shapiro (2002); Daly and Geyer (1994);
Eby et al. (2000); Hatcher and Ross
(1991); Holt et al. (2007); Korsgaard
et al. (2002); Lau and Woodman (1995);
Lok et al. (2005); Parsons et al. (1991);
Paterson and Cary (2002); Sagie and
Koslowsky (1994); Steel and Lloyd
(1988); Wanberg and Banas (2000)
  Communication Amiot et al. (2006); Axtell et al. (2002);
Bordia et al. (2004); Gaertner (1989);
Gopinath and Becker (2000); Johnson
et al. (1996); Lau and Woodman
(1995); K. I. Miller and Monge (1985);
V. D. Miller et al. (1994); Oreg (2006);
Paterson and Cary (2002); Peach et al.
(2005); Schweiger and DeNisi (1991);
Wanberg and Banas (2000)
  Interactional and procedural Armenakis et al. (2007); Armstrong-
justice Stassen (1998); Bernerth et al. (2007);
Caldwell et al. (2004); Coyle-Shapiro
(2002); Daly and Geyer (1994); Daly
(1995); Fedor et al. (2006); Gopinath
and Becker (2000); Herold et al. (2007);
Korsgaard et al. (2002); Paterson and
Cary (2002); Shapiro and Kirkman
(1999); Spreitzer and Mishra (2002)
  Principal support Armenakis et al. (2007); Caldwell et al.
(2004); Coyle-Shapiro (2002); Eby
et al. (2000); Gaertner (1989); Lam and
Schaubroeck (2000); Logan and Ganster
(2007); Lok et al. (2005); Paterson
and Cary (2002); Peach et al. (2005);
Wanberg and Banas (2000)
  Management change Amiot et al. (2006); Rafferty and Griffin
competence (2006)
  Other change process  
   (1) Attention to change (1) Gaertner (1989); (2) Armenakis et al.
 recipients; (2007); Gaertner (1989); (3) Stanley
  (2) appropriateness of et al. (2005); (4) Ashford (1988);
  change; (3) change specific (5) Bordia et al. (2006)
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 485

Table 3. (continued)

Antecedent
category Variable References
  cynicism; (4) uncertainty
  about change; (5) change-
  related rumors
Perceived Anticipation of negative or Armenakis et al. (2007); Ashford (1988);
benefit/harm positive outcomes Coyle-Shapiro (2002); Gaertner
(1989); Holt et al. (2007); Hornung and
Rousseau (2007); K. I. Miller and Monge
(1985); Peach et al. (2005); Wanberg and
Banas (2000)
  Job insecurity about change Armstrong-Stassen (1998); Naswall
et al. (2005); Oreg (2006); Paulsen et al.
(2005)
  Distributive justice Armenakis et al. (2007); Bernerth et al.
(2007); Fried et al. (1996); Paterson
and Cary (2002); Shapiro and Kirkman
(1999); Spreitzer and Mishra (2002)
  Other perceived benefit/harm  
   (1) risk; (2) job factors; (1) C. E. Cunningham et al. (2002);
  (3) organizational impact; Giacquinta (1975); (2) Axtell et al.
  (4) career impact; (2002); Bartunek et al. (2006); Caldwell
  (5) financial rewards; et al. (2004); Fedor et al. (2006); Fried
  (6) transformational change et al. (1996); Hall et al. (1978); Herold et al.
(2007); Morse and Reimer (1956); Oreg
(2006); Susskind et al. (1998); van Dam
(2005); (3) Daly (1995); Bartunek et al.
(2006); Coyle-Shapiro (2002); Coyle-
Shapiro (1999); Gaertner (1989); Herold
et al. (2007); Lam and Schaubroeck
(2000); Susskind et al. (1998);
(4) Bartunek et al. (2006); Fried et al.
(1996); Johnson et al. (1996); Paterson
and Cary (2002); (5) Johnson et al.
(1996); (6) Rafferty and Griffin (2006)
Change content Compensation system Hatcher and Ross (1991)
  Downsizing Johnson et al. (1996)
  Office design Zalesny and Farace (1987)
  Work schedule Pierce and Dunham (1992)
  Job redesign Morgeson et al. (2006)
Organizational practices Latona and LaVan (1993)

Merger Kiefer (2005)

Extent of change Caldwell et al. (2004); Herscovitch and

Meyer (2002); Lau and Woodman (1995)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


486 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

We distinguish here between prechange antecedents, which constitute conditions that


are independent of the organizational change and which existed prior to the introduc-
tion of the change (i.e., recipient characteristics and internal context), and change
antecedents, which involve aspects of the change itself that influence change recipients’
explicit reactions (i.e., change process, perceived benefit/harm, and change content).

Change Recipient Characteristics


A large portion of studies on change recipient explicit reactions considered character-
istics of the recipient that predict and help explain their reactions. These studies high-
lighted the fact that individuals are predisposed to respond in certain ways when
encountering change, across different change situations. As depicted in Figure 1, the
change recipient characteristics include differences in individuals’ personality traits,
coping styles, motivational needs, and demographics (e.g., Ashford, 1988; C. E.
Cunningham et al., 2002; Judge et al., 1999).
Personality traits. One trait that has been linked with reactions to change is locus of
control (Rotter, 1966). In a number of studies, an internal locus of control—reflecting
individuals’ beliefs that they are responsible for their own fate—was positively cor-
related with positive reactions to organizational change (e.g., Fried, Tiegs, Naughton,
& Ashforth, 1996; Holt, Armenakis, Feild, & Harris, 2007; Lau & Woodman, 1995;
Naswall, Sverke, & Hellgren, 2005). For example, managers with an internal locus of
control were less likely to report experiences of losing control over their jobs during
an organizational acquisition (Fried et al., 1996). Similarly, the tendency to make
internal attributions was negatively related to levels of mental health complaints, job
dissatisfaction, and job-induced tension, and positively related to emotional adjust-
ment under conditions of job insecurity (Naswall et al., 2005).
Several researchers tested the effects of change recipients’ change-related sense of
control on their reactions to organizational change (e.g., Martin et al., 2005; Wanberg
& Banas, 2000). Results confirmed that an increased sense of control over the change
yields improved reactions to the change, including greater acceptance of change
(Wanberg & Banas, 2000), higher psychological well-being and job satisfaction
(Martin et al., 2005), and lower psychological strain (Ashford, 1988; Bordia, Hunt,
Paulsen, Tourish, & DiFonzo, 2004; Paulsen et al., 2005).
In other works, researchers argued that change recipients’ self-efficacy is related
to their reactions to organizational changes. Whereas some of these researchers con-
sidered a generalized self-efficacy concept (Judge et al., 1999), which is a stable
aspect of one’s personality, others focused on a more specific and malleable self-
efficacy that is particularly change related (e.g., change-related self-efficacy, role-
breadth self-efficacy; Ashford, 1988; Herold et al., 2007; Hornung & Rousseau,
2007; Wanberg & Banas, 2000). Overall, higher levels of self-efficacy were associ-
ated with increased change acceptance (Wanberg & Banas, 2000), higher levels of
readiness to change, increased engagement in the change (C. E. Cunningham et al.,
2002), increased commitment to the change (Herold et al., 2007), and a greater

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 487

likelihood of using problem-focused coping strategies, with improved coping and


adjustment to the change (Amiot et al., 2006; Ashford, 1988; Judge et al., 1999;
Martin et al., 2005).
Related to both self-efficacy and perceptions of control, another set of change
recipient characteristics involved individuals’ dispositional affective states. Namely,
positive and negative affectivity were linked with change recipients’ reactions to
change. Positive affectivity was related to coping with change (Judge et al., 1999),
acceptance of organizational change (Iverson, 1996), and readiness for organizational
change (Holt et al., 2007). In fact, in one study positive affectivity was found to be one
of the strongest and most consistent dispositional variables related to coping with
change (Judge et al., 1999). Correspondingly, change recipients prone to negative or
pessimistic thinking were more likely to experience negative outcomes in the context
of organizational change. Specifically, they were more likely to suffer from job-
induced tension, mental health–related symptoms, and job dissatisfaction (Begley &
Czajka, 1993; Naswall et al., 2005). More directly related to change recipients’ change
reactions, negative emotions were strongly associated with negative appraisals of a
merger (Fugate et al., 2002). In one study, however, depression and emotional exhaus-
tion were unexpectedly linked with higher readiness and willingness to participate in
an organizational reengineering program (C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002).
Other traits linked with change recipient reactions to change include tolerance
for ambiguity (Ashford, 1988; Walker et al., 2007), dispositional resistance to change
(Oreg, 2003, 2006), dispositional cynicism (Stanley et al., 2005), openness to experi-
ence (Judge et al., 1999), and neuroticism and conscientiousness (Rafferty & Griffin,
2006), all of which have been shown to correlate with change recipients’ explicit reac-
tions to, or consequences of, organizational change.
Coping styles. A number of studies examined individuals’ coping styles in the
context of organizational change (e.g., Cartwright & Cooper, 1993; Fugate et al.,
2002). In one study, change recipients who adopted a problem-focused coping style
reported greater readiness for the organizational change, increased participation in
the change process, and an overall greater contribution to it (C. E. Cunningham et
al., 2002). In another study, use of maladaptive defense mechanisms, such as denial,
dissociation, and isolation yielded greater behavioral resistance to an organizational
change in comparison with the use of adaptive mechanisms, such as humor and
anticipation (Bovey & Hede, 2001). In yet another study, change recipients in orga-
nizations undergoing a merger tended to be more engaged in problem solving rather
than emotion-focused coping throughout various stages of the merger (Amiot et al.,
2006).
Needs. Another group of studies focused on individuals’ motivational needs as
antecedents of their reactions to change. Individuals driven by higher order needs,
such as achievement and growth, were more willing to engage in continuous organiza-
tional improvement in the context of implementing a total quality management pro-
gram (Coyle-Shapiro & Morrow, 2003), to participate in organizational restructuring
(V. D. Miller et al., 1994) and to experience positive affective reactions to their job

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


488 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

during changes to their work schedules (Bhagat & Chassie, 1980). Similarly, change
recipients high in personal initiative—a disposition consisting of an active and autono-
mous orientation—tended to evaluate the outcomes of an organizational change more
positively (Hornung & Rousseau, 2007). In another study, a mastery motivational
trait, reflecting a learning orientation, moderated the relationship between change pro-
cess and the degree to which individuals’ person–job fit was perceived as being altered
in the context of a variety of organizational changes (Caldwell et al., 2004).
Demographic variables. Beyond differences in individuals’ personal dispositions,
several demographic variables were also linked with change recipients’ reactions to
change. Specifically, tenure, level of education, and union membership were linked
with acceptance of organizational change (Iverson, 1996). Whether or not one is a
manager has been shown to influence perceptions of the change process, with manag-
ers perceiving the process to be fairer; however, managerial status was not related to
the ultimate reaction to the change (Armstrong-Stassen, 1998). Similarly, one’s posi-
tion in the organization (conceptualized as status) was associated with receptivity to
innovation (Giacquinta, 1975). In another study, change recipients’ ages moderated
the relationship between perceived change fairness and perceived person–organiza-
tion fit, with older change recipients exhibiting a weaker relationship between the two
(Caldwell et al., 2004). Other demographic variables were considered as potential
antecedents of change reactions, yet did not yield significant findings. These included
gender (Armstrong-Stassen, 1998), domestic demands (C. E. Cunningham et al.,
2002), and job level (Parsons et al., 1991). Although not establishing hypotheses for
such variables, researchers in several other studies controlled for demographic vari-
ables in their analyses (e.g., Begley & Czajka, 1993; Bordia et al., 2004; Coyle-
Shaipro, 1999, 2002; Kiefer, 2005; Madsen et al., 2005; Martin et al., 2005; Morgeson,
Johnson, Campion, Medsker, & Mumford, 2006; Naswall et al., 2005; Rafferty &
Griffin, 2006; Spreitzer & Mishra, 2002; Zalesny & Farace, 1987).
Overall, studies that considered change recipient characteristics as antecedents of
reactions to organizational change outnumbered the other four antecedent categories.
Apparently, researchers have been most interested in exploring dispositional sources
of change recipients’ reactions to change. A particular focus has been given to person-
ality characteristics such as self-efficacy and locus of control. There have also been a
number of studies that examined the role of neuroticism. Interestingly, these three fac-
tors (self-efficacy, locus of control, and neuroticism) are three of the four dispositions
that comprise the construct of core-self evaluations, which pertains to individuals’
deeply rooted beliefs about the self. From the research we reviewed, it appears that
these core beliefs have an important role in shaping change recipients’ reactions to
organizational changes. Far less attention has been given to change recipients’ coping
styles and motives, which address the questions of how change recipients deal with
change, and why they deal with it as they do.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 489

Internal Context
Beyond individuals’ personal preexisting attributes, many of the studies we reviewed
included variables that involve aspects of the prechange organizational environment
(viz., internal context, see Figure 1).
Supportive environment and trustworthy management. Change recipients who reported
holding high levels of trust in management, who perceive management as supportive,
and who feel respected, were more receptive to suggested changes and reported a
greater willingness to cooperate with the change (Coyle-Shapiro & Morrow, 2003;
C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002; Eby et al., 2000; Kiefer, 2005; Wanberg & Banas,
2000). Contrarily, organizational members who perceived their work environment as
generally unsupportive were more likely to possess cynical reactions, suffer from
negative emotions, and ultimately reject the change (Kiefer, 2005; Martin et al., 2005;
Stanley et al., 2005).
A number of works addressed the importance of a trusting relationship not only
between management and change recipients but also among colleagues (e.g., work
team members and opinion leaders (Coyle-Shapiro & Morrow, 2003; Eby et al., 2000;
Iverson, 1996). Some studies showed that social support in general (and not specifi-
cally during the change process) and the extent to which significant others (e.g., opin-
ion leaders) have been supportive, increased the level of comfort that change recipients
experienced with respect to the change and their intentions to support it, and decreased
emotional exhaustion due to the change (C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002; Eby et al.,
2000; Fugate et al., 2002; Madsen et al., 2005; Peach et al., 2005).
Organizational commitment. Change recipients who are committed to their organiza-
tion, accept its values, are willing to exert effort on its behalf, and wish to remain in it
(Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979). Several studies showed that committed change
recipients tended to report higher levels of readiness to change and change acceptance
(Iverson, 1996; Madsen et al., 2005; Shapiro & Kirkman, 1999). Furthermore, orga-
nizational commitment served as a buffer, dampening the detrimental influence of
change-related stress on change recipients’ job satisfaction, intentions to remain in the
organization, and work-related irritation (Begley & Czajka, 1993).
However, in a study on job changes, higher commitment (as well as higher job
satisfaction) prior to the change actually yielded reactions that were less positive
toward the change compared with the reactions of those who were less committed to
the organization (van Dam, 2005). The rationale provided for this finding was that those
who were committed to, and satisfied with, the old way of doing things would be less
willing to change things in comparison with those who disapproved of the current
mode. This suggests an important distinction between commitment to the job and the
organization’s current mode of operation and commitment to those who initiate and
apply the change.
Organizational culture and climate. Another factor that was found relevant for change
recipients’ reactions to change was the general atmosphere in which change was
applied. Perceiving the working environment in positive terms was found to predict

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


490 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

change recipients’ readiness for change, openness to change, and adjustment to it


(V. D. Miller et al., 1994). Similarly, a positive communication climate, or the exis-
tence of an “information environment,” also predicted change recipients’ readiness to
change (Armenakis, Bernerth, Pitts, & Walker, 2007; Holt et al., 2007). In other stud-
ies, cultural fit predicted reactions to the change. Specifically, the degree to which the
organization’s existing cultural values were aligned with the change vision and objectives
predicted change recipients’ readiness to change (Jones et al., 2005). In another study,
the degree of perceived fit between the cultures of two merging organizations was
negatively correlated with change-related stress (Cartwright & Cooper, 1993). Simi-
larly, the degree of alignment between the organization’s structure, strategy, and tech-
nology was linked with the effectiveness of the change implementation and organizational
performance (Lok et al., 2005).
Job characteristics. The degree to which one’s job allowed for the use of a variety of
skills was also related to favorable perceptions of the change recipients’ readiness for
change (Eby et al., 2000). Similarly, those change recipients who were involved in
psychologically demanding jobs that allowed high decision latitude, reported higher
readiness for change, participated more in change, and felt they made a greater contri-
bution to the change (C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002). Increases in skill variety, task
identity, task significance, autonomy, and feedback of one’s job were linked with
higher general satisfaction, growth satisfaction, internal work motivation, increased
meaningfulness and responsibility as a result of the change (Bhagat & Chassie, 1980),
and increased readiness for change (Weber & Weber, 2001). Similarly, autonomy was
linked to proactivity (Hornung & Rousseau, 2007) and organizational commitment
(Iverson, 1996), both of which were related to acceptance of organizational change.
Miscellaneous factors. In addition to the factors above, a variety of other organiza-
tional characteristics were linked with positive reactions to the change. Such factors
include the degree of perceived participation at work and the existence of flexible poli-
cies (Eby et al., 2000); perceived organizational integrity (Bernerth et al., 2007;
Walker et al., 2007); perceived organizational constraints, such as an unmanageable
workload and lacking necessary information (Kiefer, 2005); perceived organizational
capabilities and competencies (Gaertner, 1989; Holt et al., 2007; Stanley et al., 2005);
and in the context of mergers, being a member of the acquiring, versus acquired, orga-
nization (Covin, Sightler, Kolenko, & Tudor, 1996).
Overall, the factor that yielded perhaps the most consistent and strongest relation-
ship (i.e., strongest effect size) with change reactions is the extent to which change
recipients trust management (e.g., Eby et al., 2000; Oreg, 2006; Stanley et al., 2005).
Another interesting finding from our analysis has to do with the relationship between
commitment and reactions to change. Interestingly, it is not clear what one should
predict when considering the relationship between the two. This is because commit-
ment can predispose individuals to both support change initiatives and resist them. On
one hand, a highly committed employee may want to preserve things as they are and
will therefore resist a change to the organization. On the other hand, a highly commit-
ted employee may support change because he or she will feel commitment toward the

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 491

change agent (e.g., management). In all but one of the studies in our review, this latter
conceptualization was presumed and supported. Nevertheless, van Dam’s (2005)
rationale and findings of a negative relationship between commitment and support for
change suggests that under certain conditions the former dynamic may also be relevant.
These conditions may depend on the particular organizational aspects employees are
committed to as well as on the change practices used in designing and implementing
the change. Future studies could directly explore the variables that moderate the rela-
tionship between commitment and reactions to change.
Six of the studies reviewed in this section addressed factors related to organiza-
tional culture and climate, such as the fit between existing and change values or a posi-
tive communication climate, both of which were found to be associated with positive
reactions to change (e.g., Cartwright & Cooper, 1993; Jones et al., 2005; V. D. Miller
et al., 1994). However, despite the centrality and prevalence of research on organiza-
tional culture and climate overall, evidence that links culture and climate with recipients’
reactions to organizational change remains limited.

Change Process
Perhaps the most frequently studied category of antecedents to reactions to change
involved the manner in which change was implemented (see Figure 1). Forty-two of
the studies in our review included variables that pertained to the process through which
change was managed and sought to use these variables for explaining change recipi-
ents’ reactions to the change. We classified these variables into five process categories:
participation, communication and information, interactional and procedural justice,
principal support during the change, and management change competence.
Participation. Among the most prevalent variables considered in this category, with
14 studies in our review to have assessed it, was participation, starting with Coch and
French’s (1948) classic study at the Harwood Manufacturing Corporation. Studies on
participation focused on the effect of the degree to which change recipients were
involved in planning and implementing the change. Such participation creates a sense
of agency, contribution, and control over the change (Armenakis & Bedeian, 1999).
As a rule, change recipients who experienced high levels of participation tended to
report higher readiness and acceptance of change, appraised change as less stressful
and exhibited overall support for the change (Amiot et al., 2006; Coch & French,
1948; Coyle-Shaipro, 2002; Holt et al., 2007; Sagie & Koslowsky, 1994; Steel &
Lloyd, 1988). Participation during the change process was also linked with the experi-
ence of positive emotions, a greater understanding of the meaning of change, realizing
possible gains associated with the change and greater involvement in implementing
behavioral changes (Bartunek et al., 1999; Bartunek et al., 2006). In addition, partici-
pation contributed to change recipients’ sense of competence, improved interpersonal
trust, and increased attachment to the organization (Steel & Lloyd, 1988). Similarly,
involvement in the early stages of the change decreased change recipients’ change-
related stress and withdrawal behaviors (Parsons et al., 1991).

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


492 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Communication and information. Closely related to participation, another variable


studied had to do with the amount and quality of change information with which
change recipients were provided. Additional information and realistic, supportive and
effective communication during change, was associated with several positive reactions,
such as greater change acceptance and support for the change (Axtell et al., 2002;
Gaertner, 1989; Wanberg & Banas, 2000). In addition, communication about the
change was linked with a number of other responses (such as lower levels of anxiety
and uncertainty, increased trust in management) and consequences (such as decreased
turnover intentions; Ashford, 1988; Bordia et al., 2004; Gopinath & Becker, 2000;
Johnson, Bernhagen, Miller, & Allen, 1996; K. I. Miller & Monge, 1985; V. D.
Miller et al., 1994; Paterson & Cary, 2002). Correspondingly, lack of communication
during the change can lead to uncertainty, which may be a key source of change recipi-
ents’ difficulties during change implementation (Schweiger & DeNisi, 1991).
In one study, however, contrary to what was hypothesized, additional information
about the change corresponded with negative evaluations of the change (Oreg, 2006).
The rationale provided for this finding was that it is not merely the amount of informa-
tion that determines reactions to change but also the content of this information.
Alongside the value for change recipients in receiving additional information, some-
times learning more about the change can give change recipients all the more reason
to resist it. Thus, the overall picture concerning the role of information may be more
complex than has been initially proposed.
Interactional and procedural justice. Beyond the substance and details about the change
that are conveyed through change communications, information and participation alle-
viate resistance to change through their impact on change recipients’ perceptions
of justice (Oreg, 2006; see also discussion of this issue in Oreg & van Dam, 2009). In
particular, several studies linked interactional and procedural justice with reactions to
organizational change (Armenakis et al., 2007; Bernerth et al., 2007; Paterson & Cary,
2002). In addition to interactional justice, procedural justice was associated with
higher acceptance, readiness, and commitment to organizational change (Korsgaard,
Sapienza, & Schweiger, 2002).
Principal support during change. The principals who affect an organizational change
are change agents and opinion leaders. Some works highlighted the effect of principal
support during the change on change recipients’ reactions to change (Amiot et al.,
2006; Daly & Geyer, 1994; Eby et al., 2000). Such support is distinct from a general
supportive atmosphere, as discussed in the previous section, and refers to specific sup-
port that is provided as part of the change implementation. In one study, principal
support during change was associated with higher readiness to change and lower per-
ceived negative effects of the change (Logan & Ganster, 2007). In addition, principal
support was shown to influence affective and behavioral resistance to an organiza-
tional restructuring (Oreg, 2006). Contrary to other studies on support, in one study, of
multiple organizations, management support was assessed by aggregating change
recipients’ support ratings to the organizational level. This aggregate assessment of

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 493

management support was found to be critical in influencing change recipients’ adapta-


tion in changing role demands (Caldwell et al., 2004).
Management change competence. Several studies addressed the degree to which
management was perceived as competent and effective in managing the change. Two
studies found perceived management commitment to the change and its perceived
effectiveness in managing it to yield positive outcomes, such as better implementation
of the change (Lok et al., 2005) and lower levels of change recipient stress (Amiot
et al., 2006). In another study, change recipients who perceived that the change had
been implemented after deliberation and planning exhibited less psychological uncer-
tainty and more favorable reactions toward the organization (Rafferty & Griffin,
2006). Finally, the degree to which management was perceived as “change compe-
tent” was negatively associated with change recipients’ skepticism toward the change
(Stanley et al., 2005).
Overall, the studies linking change process to change reactions are consistent in
demonstrating that a participative and supportive process, with open lines of commu-
nication, and management that is perceived as competent and fair in its implementa-
tion of the change, is effective in producing positive reactions toward the change.
Most of the studies in this category focused on the variables of participation and infor-
mation, whereas a small number of studies explored the role of management’s compe-
tence in implementing the change.

Perceived Benefit/Harm From the Change


A key determinant of whether change recipients will accept or resist change is the
extent to which the change is perceived as personally beneficial or harmful (see Figure 1).
Anticipated benefit and harm constitute straightforward and sensible reasons change
recipients may have for supporting or resisting a particular change (Dent & Goldberg,
1999; Nord & Jermier, 1994). Indeed, in 34 of the studies we reviewed, at least one
variable pertained to the personal impact change recipients perceived the change to
have. As would be expected, these studies demonstrate that when change is perceived
as personally beneficial, change recipients exhibited a more positive reaction to it. We
elaborate below on the various types of variables that have been considered within
this category.
Anticipation of negative or positive outcomes. On several occasions, researchers con-
sidered change recipients’ reactions to changes that entail negative outcomes, such as
downsizing, a greater workload, increased job complexity, or loss of job control. In
these cases, change recipients tended to experience greater stress and psychological
withdrawal (Ashford, 1988; Axtell et al., 2002; Fried et al., 1996), were less open to
accept changes (C. E. Cunningham et al., 2002), and exhibited lower levels of job
satisfaction and involvement (Hall, Goodale, Rabinowitz, & Morgan, 1978) and lower
levels of perceived person–job fit (Caldwell et al., 2004; Susskind et al., 1998), fol-
lowing the change.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


494 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

In contrast, other studies explored the effects of anticipated positive outcomes,


including more interesting and challenging work, increased personal development or
improved employability, and increased pay (Bartunek et al., 2006). A number of works
showed that anticipation of a positive outcome following the change was associated
with greater readiness and acceptance of the change and higher commitment and
willingness to participate in it (van Dam, 2005). In other works, perceived benefits
from change were related to change recipients’ postchange job attitudes, such that
there was a positive relationship with organizational commitment and job satisfaction
and a negative one with turnover intentions (Fedor et al., 2006; Herold et al., 2007;
Rafferty & Griffin, 2006; Wanberg & Banas, 2000).
Job insecurity. In a number of studies, researchers were interested in the impact of
specific change-related outcomes. In five of the studies we reviewed, particular atten-
tion was paid to perceived threats to job security. Overall, perceived job insecurity was
associated with greater job dissatisfaction, mental health complaints, job-induced ten-
sion, and emotional exhaustion (Naswall et al., 2005; Paulsen et al., 2005). Job inse-
curity was also associated with greater affective resistance to the change (Oreg, 2006)
and less support for it (Gaertner, 1989). Greater uncertainty with one’s job future and
potential for career development following a change were also positively correlated
with change recipients’ turnover intentions (Fried et al., 1996; Johnson et al., 1996).
Distributive justice. Distributive justice, reflecting the perceived fairness of the out-
comes resulting from the change, has also been shown to influence change recipients’
reactions to change (Armenakis et al., 2007; Bernerth et al., 2007; Paterson & Cary,
2002). Specifically, anticipation of distributive injustice during change was signifi-
cantly correlated with change recipients’ cynicism, anxiety, and resistance to change,
as well as with organizational commitment, commitment to change, and turnover inten-
tions (Armenakis et al., 2007; Bernerth et al., 2007; Shapiro & Kirkman, 1999;
Spreitzer & Mishra, 2002).
In all these studies, as expected, variables reflecting anticipated positive outcomes
were associated with positive (or less negative) reactions to the change and those
reflecting anticipated negative outcomes were associated with negative (or less posi-
tive) reactions. Beyond reiterating the relevance of the personal impact of the change,
several of the studies in this category contribute to our understanding of reactions to
change by exploring a variety of paths through which perceived benefit and harm
ultimately influence change recipients’ reactions. These include both examinations of
mediated paths and moderated relationships. Specifically, in five of the studies, the
perceived benefit/harm mediated relationships between other antecedents and the ulti-
mate reaction to change (Bartunek et al., 2006; Fried et al., 1996; Giacquinta, 1975;
Hornung & Rousseau, 2007; Johnson et al., 1996). In other words, perceived benefits/
harm were found to be a more proximal determinant of change recipients’ reactions
than other antecedents, such as recipient characteristics (e.g., Hornung & Rousseau,
2007) or the change process (e.g., Johnson et al., 1996). In yet other studies, perceived
benefit/harm (e.g., change favorability) moderated, rather than mediated, the relation-
ships between other antecedents (e.g., change process) and the reaction to change

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 495

(Daly, 1995; Fedor et al., 2006; Gaertner, 1989), such that when change was favor-
able, weaker relationships tended to emerge between the other antecedents and the
reaction to change. In other words, when change is perceived as beneficial, reactions
to it tend to become favorable regardless of the other change antecedents. We draw
this conclusion tentatively, however, considering that only few studies explored this
possibility.

Change Content
A small number of studies considered the mere nature or type of change as a possible
determinant of change recipient reactions to it. These studies explored the possibility
that beyond the manner in which change was managed or the implications that change
was expected to have, the content of the change may also affect change recipients’
reactions. To examine the impact of change content, studies compared change recipi-
ent reactions with different pre- and postchange situations, or with different types
of organizational changes. Some of the changes, such as a shift from piece-rate com-
pensation to gain-sharing bonuses (Hatcher & Ross, 1991), or the implementation of
a change recipient involvement program (Latona & La Van, 1993), yielded positive
change consequences, such as more favorable job attitudes and improved perfor-
mance. Others assessed changes that yielded negative outcomes. In one study, after
shifting from the use of traditional work groups to semiautonomous teams, change
recipients reported positive change consequences, such as exerting greater effort at work,
making better use of their skills, and more effectively solving problems (Morgeson
et al., 2006).
Such comparisons of pre- and postchange reactions were also conducted with
respect to changes in the objective working environment and conditions. In a govern-
ment agency, changing from traditional offices to an open-plan office design yielded
negative responses, such as decreased trust in management and job satisfaction, in
particular among clerical and managerial change recipients (Zalesny & Farace, 1987).
A change from a rotating 8-hour shift schedule to a 12-hour compressed shift schedule
yielded positive job attitudes, decreased stress, and improved performance, among
police personnel (Pierce & Dunham, 1992).
Some studies operationalized content as the degree or perceived meaningfulness of
change. For example, perceptions of change as ongoing were associated with negative
emotions, such as anger, mistrust, and frustration (Kiefer, 2005). Furthermore, in sev-
eral organizations undergoing a variety of different types of change, the perceived
extent of change moderated the relationship between perceived change process and
reactions to change, such that high extent of change yielded weaker relationships
between change process and reactions to change (Caldwell et al., 2004). Moreover,
change schema, defined and measured, among other aspects, by the degree to which
the change is perceived as meaningful, impactful, and salient, mediated the relation-
ship between change recipients’ personal orientations and their reactions toward the
organizational change (Lau & Woodman, 1995).

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


496 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Contrary to each of our other antecedent categories, only very few (nine) of the
studies in our review explored the role of change content. This is likely influenced by
the greater logistic difficulties that studying the impact of content entail. To study
content, at least two organizational changes need to be compared. This typically
requires access to more than a single organization, which is often difficult to secure.
Not only that, but for a study of change content to yield valid findings, the researcher
must be attentive to the sources of internal invalidity (see Cook, Campbell, &
Peracchio, 1990), otherwise any difference found could be attributed to extraneous
factors. Unfortunately, these obstacles are likely to continue to be a challenge in study-
ing the impact of change content on reactions to change.

Change Consequences5
Whereas most of the studies in our review focused on the explicit reactions to the
change, and although several considered both the explicit and immediate reactions to
the change and the postchange attitudes toward the organization (e.g., Armstrong-
Stassen, 1998; Paterson & Cary, 2002; Wanberg & Banas, 2000), some studies
focused only on the postchange attitudes toward the organization as outcomes (see
Figure 1). In these studies, the various antecedents (e.g., change process, internal
context) were directly linked to change recipient orientation toward the organization
following the change.

Work-Related Consequences
Numerous studies we reviewed investigated change recipient orientations toward the
job or the organization as the change outcome. Researchers in these studies were
interested in how the change situation influenced change recipients’ subsequent atti-
tudes or behaviors toward the organization. Studies on change consequences were
identified with each of the antecedent categories presented above, yet most considered
the change process (e.g., Armenakis et al., 2007; Holt et al., 2007; Oreg, 2006;
Paterson & Cary, 2002) and recipient characteristics (e.g., Fried et al., 1996; Judge
et al., 1999; Logan & Ganster, 2007).
The consequence most frequently considered was organizational commitment (e.g.,
Cartwright & Cooper, 1993; Fedor et al., 2006; Gopinath & Becker, 2000; Oreg, 2006;
Shapiro & Kirkman, 1999; see Table 4 for the complete list, including related concepts,
such as attachment to the organization, Spreitzer & Mishra, 2002, and organizational
identification, Johnson et al., 1996). At a close second came studies on job satisfaction
(e.g., Amiot et al., 2006; Axtell et al., 2002; Gardner, Dunham, Cummings, & Pierce,
1987; Judge et al., 1999), followed by studies on turnover or intentions to leave the
organization (Coch & French, 1948; Fried et al., 1996; Gardner et al., 1987; Schweiger
& Denisi, 1991). Other related constructs were investigated, such as motivation (e.g.,
Pierce & Dunham, 1992), organizational citizenship behavior (Shapiro & Kirkman,
1999), and morale (Paterson & Cary, 2002). In several studies, more than one of these

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 497

Table 4. Change Consequences in Articles Reviewed

Outcome
category Variables Articles
Work-related Satisfaction
consequences   Job satisfaction Amiot et al. (2006); Axtell et al.
  (2002); Begley and Czajka (1993);
Bordia et al. (2004); Cartwright
and Cooper (1993); Gardner et al.
(1987); Holt et al. (2007); Judge
et al. (1999); Lam and
Schaubroeck (2000); Logan and
Ganster (2007); Martin et al.
(2005); Morse and Reimer (1956);
Mossholder et al. (2000); Naswall
et al. (2005); Oreg (2006); Paulsen
et al. (2005); Pierce and Dunham
(1992); Rafferty and Griffin
(2006); Schweiger and DeNisi
(1991); Steel and Lloyd (1988);
Wanberg and Banas (2000);
Zalesny and Farace (1987)
    Work satisfaction Bhagat and Chassie (1980); Johnson
et al. (1996); Hall et al. (1978);
Sagie and Koslowsky (1994)
   Satisfaction with Covin et al. (1996); Lam and
 change Schaubroeck (2000)
  Commitment/Identification  
   Organizational Cartwright and Cooper (1993);
 commitment Coyle-Shapiro (1999); Fedor
et al. (2006); Gopinath and
Becker (2000); Holt et al.
(2007); Judge et al. (1999); Lee
and Peccei (2007); Logan and
Ganster (2007); Martin et al.
(2005); Mossholder et al. (1995);
Oreg (2006); Pierce and Dunham
(1992); Schweiger and DeNisi
(1991); Shapiro and Kirkman
(1999); Spreitzer and Mishra
(2002); Steel and Lloyd (1988)
   Identification Amiot et al. (2006)
  Intentions to quit Begley and Czajka (1993); Bordia
et al. (2004); Coch and French
(1948); G. B. Cunningham (2006);
Fried et al. (1996); Gardner et al.
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


498 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 4. (continued)

Outcome
category Variables Articles
(1987); Holt et al. (2007); Johnson
et al. (1996); Korsgaard et al.
(2002); Lam and Schaubroeck
(2000); Martin et al. (2005);
Mossholder et al. (2000); Oreg
(2006); Rafferty and Griffin
(2006); Schweiger and DeNisi
(1991); Shapiro and Kirkman
(1999); Spreitzer and Mishra
(2002); Steel and Lloyd (1988);
Wanberg and Banas (2000)
  Job involvement Hall et al. (1978); Mossholder
et al. (2000); Pierce and Dunham
(1992)
  Work outcomes  
    Job performance Gardner et al. (1987); Hall et al.
(1978); Hatcher and Ross (1991);
Schweiger and DeNisi (1991);
Steel and Lloyd (1988); Morse and
Reimer (1956)
   Effectiveness Logan and Ganster (2007); Pierce
and Dunham (1992); Sagie and
Koslowsky (1994)
   Effort Morgeson et al. (2006)
  Trust  
   In the Schweiger and DeNisi (1991); Steel
 organization and Lloyd (1988)
    In management Gopinath and Becker (2000);
Kiefer (2005); Paterson and Cary
(2002); Weber and Weber (2001);
Zalesny and Farace (1987)
    In supervisor Korsgaard et al. (2002)
  Motivation  
   Extrinsic Judge et al. (1999)
   Intrinsic Armenakis et al. (2007); Bhagat and
Chassie (1980); Gardner et al.
(1987)
  Productive/counterproductive work  
behavior
   OCB Shapiro and Kirkman (1999)
   Withdrawal Kiefer (2005)
(continued)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 499

Table 4. (continued)

Outcome
category Variables Articles
   Absenteeism Martin et al. (2005); Schweiger and
DeNisi (1991)
  Climate  
   Teamwork Coyle-Shapiro and Morrow (2003);
Hatcher and Ross (1991)
   Continuous Coyle-Shapiro (2002)
 improvement
 orientation
   Leadership Latona and LaVan (1993)
   Performance Lok et al. (2005)
   Morale Paterson and Cary (2002)
   Support and Weber and Weber (2001)
  readiness to
 change
   Communication Zalesny and Farace (1987)
  Other work-related consequences  
   (1) Style of coping with the merger (1)Armstrong-Stassen (1998);
  (2) P-J fit, P-O fit perceived fit, (2) Caldwell et al. (2004);
  after the change, between person (3) Korsgaard et al. (2002);
  and job and between person (4) Mossholder et al. (1995);
  and organization (3) Perceived (5) Pierce and Dunham (1992);
  organizational obligations, change (6) Parsons et al. (1991); (7) Steel
  recipient obligations (4) Perceived and Lloyd (1988); (8) Daly (1995)
  role-ambiguity, perceived role-
  conflict (5) Work-schedule-related
  interference with personal
  activities, work-schedule attitudes
  (6) Equipment usage amount
  (hours per week, usage breadth,
  equipment inconvenience
  (7) Personal competence
  (8) Procedural fairness
Personal Psychological health  
consequences   Anxiety and stress Axtell et al. (2002); Begley and
  Czajka (1993); Parsons et al.
(1991); Schweiger and DeNisi
(1991)
    Irritation and tension Begley and Czajka (1993); Naswall
et al. (2005); Wanberg and Banas
(2000)
   Depression Axtell et al. (2002); Begley and
Czajka (1993)

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


500 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Table 4. (continued)
Outcome
category Variables Articles
    Psychological withdrawal Fried et al. (1996); Parsons et al.
(1991)
    Psychological uncertainty Rafferty and Griffin (2006)
  Physiological health  
    Health complaints Begley and Czajka (1993);
Cartwright and Cooper (1993);
Naswall et al. (2005)
    Exhaustion and strain Bordia et al. (2004); Paulsen et al.
(2005)
  Personal growth  
    Psychological well-being Martin et al. (2005)
    Psychological success Hall et al. (1978)
    Self-actualization and growth Morse and Reimer (1956)
    Leisure satisfaction Pierce and Dunham (1992)

outcomes was considered. Beyond postchange attitudes toward the organization, sev-
eral studies focused on more behaviorally oriented job-related outcomes, including
job performance and indices of adjustment to the new job situation (e.g., Hall et al.,
1978; Judge et al., 1999; Lok et al., 2005; Sagie & Koslowsky, 1994).
Other work-related consequences studied pertain to the internal context following
the change. Variables considered here as outcomes were often the same as those
assessed as antecedents in other studies. For example, in several studies trust in man-
agement was viewed as an indirect consequence of the change (e.g., Kiefer, 2005;
Paterson & Cary, 2002; Zalesny & Farace, 1987), rather than an antecedent of the
reactions to change. Other studies analyzed the perceived job characteristics, such as
perceived job control, meaningfulness of work, perceived career outcomes, or the
degree of fit between the person and the job, as an indirect consequence of the change
(e.g., Bhagat & Chassie, 1980; Bordia et al., 2004; Caldwell et al., 2004; Judge et al.,
1999).

Personal Consequences
A smaller set of studies considered the personal consequences that change had for
change recipients. Specifically, several of the studies we reviewed included variables
that pertained, in one way or another, to change recipients’ psychological well-being.
In addition to explicit assessments of mental health and somatic health complaints
(Begley & Czajka, 1993; Cartwright & Cooper, 1993; Naswall et al., 2005), these
studies included assessments of depression (Axtell et al., 2002; Begley & Czajka,
1993), anxiety (Axtell et al., 2002), stress or strain (Bordia et al., 2004; Parsons et al.,

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 501

1991; Schweiger & Denisi, 1991), psychological withdrawal (Fried et al., 1996;
Parsons et al., 1991), work-related irritation (Begley & Czajka, 1993; Wanberg &
Banas, 2000), perceived psychological success or personal growth (Hall et al., 1978;
Morse & Reimer, 1956), leisure satisfaction (Pierce & Dunham, 1992), emotional
exhaustion (Paulsen et al., 2005), and perceived control and uncertainty (Bordia et al.,
2004; Rafferty & Griffin, 2006).
As a rule, consistent with findings examining antecedents of the reactions to
change, these studies found that as the conditions within which the change was applied
were more favorable (e.g., supportive atmosphere, trustworthy management), as the
change process was more inclusive (e.g., high participation), and as change recipients’
personalities were more resilient and change oriented, change recipients’ attitudes and
behaviors toward the organization and toward their jobs, as well as their psychological
well-being following the change, had improved. Thus, as would be expected, the
impact the various antecedents had on the explicit reactions to change were compara-
ble with their impact on the change consequences.
As noted above, several of the studies in our review considered both the explicit
reaction to change and the change consequences. However, most of them did not dis-
tinguish between the two, and considered all the outcome variables as forms of the
reaction to change. Nonetheless, a number of studies did explicitly distinguish between
the two and suggested and demonstrated that the explicit reactions to change mediated
the relationships between the antecedents and the change consequences (Amiot et al.,
2006; G. B. Cunningham, 2006; Judge et al., 1999; Kiefer, 2005; Lok et al., 2005;
Oreg, 2006; Paterson & Cary, 2002; Wanberg & Banas, 2000). Thus, in one study, for
example, managers’ effectiveness in coping with an organizational change (i.e., a
behavioral reaction) mediated the relationships between their personality (e.g., risk
aversion) and work-related attitudes, such as job satisfaction and organizational com-
mitment (Judge et al., 1999).
Change recipients’ attitudes toward an organizational change mediated the relation-
ships of both personality and context variables with job satisfaction, continuance com-
mitment, and turnover intentions (Oreg, 2003; Wanberg & Banas, 2000). Furthermore,
change recipients’ emotions with respect to the change mediated the relationships
between the working conditions, the supportiveness of the organization (i.e., organiza-
tional treatment), and change recipients’ personal status in the organization on one
hand and trust in the organization and organizational withdrawal on the other.
We found some studies in which the antecedent considered was the explicit reac-
tion to change and the outcome was the change consequences. For example, Mossholder
et al. (1995, 2000) examined the relationship between affect toward the change,
assessed with open-ended questions (coded using the Dictionary of Affect in Language,
Whissell & Dewson, 1986), and work-related outcomes (e.g., optimism about the
organization, the perceived autonomy in the organization, job satisfaction, organiza-
tional commitment, and turnover intentions). In another study, G. B. Cunningham
(2006) considered coping with change (i.e., a behavioral reaction) as a mediator
between change commitment and turnover intentions.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


502 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Research Context Descriptors


and Studies’ Methodologies
Table 5 provides the research context descriptors of the 79 studies. These include type
of organization, type of change implemented, research design, sample composition,
and sample size. The type of organization within which organizational change was
investigated varied considerably. In some instances, researchers disguised the organi-
zations by using general descriptors such as private sector or public sector organizations.
Others were slightly more descriptive (e.g., airlines, telecommunications, hospitals,
automobile parts manufacturer, and military organization). As as can be seen, many
different types of organizations have been studied. The types of change described in
the studies were also quite varied. Many were in organizations engaged in a merger
or divestiture, downsizing, technological change, and work redesign. It should be
emphasized, as discussed above, that of the 79 studies in our review, only 9 investi-
gated change content as an explicit reaction antecedent. The other 70 studies simply
described the organizational change being implemented, but did not measure the
change attributes. The job classifications of the research samples were quite varied
and included operative-level workers, managers, pilots, nurses, police, military per-
sonnel, and school teachers. Sample sizes ranged from a low of 22 to 2,845. Mean
sample size was approximately 254.
Nearly 50% of the research designs in the 79 studies were longitudinal, 3 employed
comparison/controlled groups, and one of these used a randomized experimental/
control group design. The vast majority of studies relied solely on self-reports, for all
variables. We identified only seven studies that supplemented self-reports with archi-
val data, such as absenteeism or turnover (Bartunek et al., 1999; Gardner et al., 1987;
Hatcher & Ross, 1991; Morse & Reimer, 1956; Schweiger & DeNisi, 1991; Spreitzer
& Mishra, 2002; Wanberg & Banas, 2000). Three additional studies reported indepen-
dent ratings, namely, customer satisfaction and supervisor rating of subordinates
(Gardner et al., 1987; Judge et al., 1999; Lam & Schaubroeck, 2000). We discuss the
implications of these methodological approaches below.

Discussion
Unlike previous reviews of organizational change research, the last of which was
published in 1999, our focus in the present review was exclusively on change recipi-
ent reactions to organizational change. We covered a 60-year period of quantitative
research on the topic and considered approximately 700 published articles on organi-
zational change. We summarized and coded literally hundreds of variables included
in the 79 articles we reviewed, all of which are presented in Tables 2 to 5. We used
these variables to inductively construct our model (see Figure 1) consisting of
prechange antecedents (i.e., change recipient characteristics and internal context),
change antecedents (i.e., change process, perceived benefit/harm and change content),

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Table 5. Research Context Descriptors

503
Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Amiot et al. (2006) Airlines Merger Longitudinal Pilots and Flight T-1 = 662;
Engineers T-2 = 465
Armenakis et al. Study 2 (S2) = Medical Division; S2 = team-based culture; Cross-sectional S2 = admin., S2 = 117;
(2007) Study 3 (S3) = Durable Goods S3 = company spinoff; technical, S3 = 117;
Manufacturer; Study 4 (S4) = S4 = merger and scientific S4 = 247
Public Service Organization employees
Armstrong-Stassen Fortune 100 company Downsizing Cross-sectional Clerical, N = 236
(1998) technicians,
supervisors
Ashford (1988) Telecommunications company Company divestiture Cross-sectional Employees T-1 = 180;
and longitudinal T-2 = 83
Axtell et al. (2002) U.K.-based distribution company Technological change Longitudinal Managers, T-1 = 325;
engineers, and T-2 = 227
operators
Bartunek et al. Independent schools Empowerment Longitudinal Participants, N = 315
(1999) nonparticipants
and change
agents
Bartunek et al. Hospital Shared governance- Cross-sectional Nurses N = 501
(2006) decentralization

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Begley and Czajka Hospital Divisional consolidation Longitudinal N = 82
(1993)
Bernerth et al. Automobile parts manufacturer Spin off from parent Cross-sectional Operative N = 117
(2007) employees
Bhagat and Aircraft manufacturing company Transition to 4-day work Longitudinal Managers and N = 65
Chassie (1980) week nonmanagers
continued

Table 5. (continued)

Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Bordia et al. (2006) Hospital Restructuring, Cross-sectional Nurses, medical N = 1,610
privatization, relocation, staff
technological change
Bordia et al. (2004) Hospital Restructuring and Cross-sectional Staff members N = 222
relocation
Bovey and Hede 9 different public/private sector Restructuring; Cross-sectional Employees N = 615
(2001) organizations reorganization of
systems; technological
change
Caldwell et al. 34 work units in Extent of change, Cross-sectional Employees N = 282-299
(2004) 21 organizations, e.g., consequences of change,
transportation, technology, individual job impact
consumer products,
government
Cartwright and U.K. Building Societies Merger Cross-sectional Middle managers N = 157
Cooper (1993)
Coch and French Harwood Manufacturing Changes in work methods Field experiment Plant employees Not indicated
(1948) Corporation and jobs
Covin et al. (1996) Fortune 500 company Merger Cross-sectional Employees N = 2845
Coyle-Shapiro and U.K. supplier of electrical TQM program Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 186;

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Morrow (2003) components T-2 = 166;
T-3 = 118
Coyle-Shapiro U.K. supplier of electrical TQM program Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 186;
(1999) components T-2 = 166;
T-3 = 118
continued

504
Table 5. (continued)
Sample

505
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Coyle-Shapiro U.K. supplier of electrical T-1 = TQM; T-2 = profit Longitudinal Study 1: Operators, Study 1:
(2002) components sharing craftsmen, T-1 = 166;
engineers, T-2 = 118.
purchase Study 2:
controllers; T-1 = 186;
administrators T-2 = 141
C. E. Cunningham Canadian hospital Reengineering, work Longitudinal Hospital N = 654
et al. (2002) redesign employees
G. B. Cunningham Athletic departments Significant organizational Cross-sectional Employees N = 299
(2006) changes
Daly (1995) 7 private sector orgs Relocation Cross-sectional Employees N = 183
Daly and Geyer 7 private sector orgs Relocation; change in Cross-sectional Employees N = 171
(1994) strategy and structure
Eby et al. (2000) National sales organization Organizational change; Cross-sectional Employees and N = 117
segmented sales teams managers
Fedor et al. (2006) 34 organizations Significant work unit Cross-sectional; Employees N = 806
changes longitudinal
Fried et al. (1996) Fortune 500 service company Merger, including Longitudinal Middle managers N = 91
downsizing, relocation
Fugate et al. (2002) Aerospace company Merger—downsizing, Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 216;

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


restructuring, etc. T-2 = 138;
T-3 = 119;
T-4 = 81
Gaertner (1989) Information services Change in business Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 147;
strategy—reorganization T-2 = 789
with downsizing,
technological change,
change in product line
continued

Table 5. (continued)

Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Gardner et al. Insurance company 23 job improvement Randomized exp/ Clerical personnel Exp group =
(1987) teams used to identify control group 300; control
technostructural job design group = 160
changes
Giacquinta (1975) Elementary schools Sex education curriculum Cross sectional Educators— N = 66
board member,
administrators,
teachers
Gopinath and Chemical company Sale of division, Longitudinal Survivors T-1 = 314;
Becker (2000) downsizing, layoffs T-2 = 318;
matched
pairs = 144
Hall et al. (1978) Canadian transportation Reorganization, new Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 233;
ministry administrators, T-2 = 233;
relocations, job redesign, T-3 = 162.
4-day work week T-1 and T-2
and T-3 = 153
Hatcher and Ross Automotive supplier Gain sharing Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 147;
(1991) T-2 = 147;
questionnaires
were not

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


matched
Herold et al. 25 organizations in finance, Work unit changes— Cross-sectional Employees Personal
(2007) manufacturing, education, technology, change
consumer products, and reorganization, strategy, surveys = 287;
technology relocation, outsourcing, organizational
leadership, and change

506
downsizing surveys = 266
continued
Table 5. (continued)

Sample

507
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Herscovitch and Hospitals Mergers of departments, Cross-sectional Nurses Study 2 = 157;
Meyer (2002) technological change, Study 3 = 108
modifications to shift
work, and hiring health
care aids
Holt et al. (2007) Government organization and Change in organizational Cross-sectional Employees Study 1 = 264;
private sector organization structure Study 2 = 228
Hornung and Hospital Shared leadership and Longitudinal Health care T-1 = 166;
Rousseau (2007) decentralization of personnel, T-2 = 207
decision making technical
personnel and
administrators
Iverson (1996) Australian hospital Restructuring jobs, Cross-sectional Hospital N = 761
technological change, employees
increasing efficiency
Johnson et al. Insurance company Downsizing Modified time Employees T-1 = 64;
(1996) series design T-2 = 44;
T-3 = 37
Jones et al. (2005) Australian state government Technological change Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 156;
department T-2 = 98;
T-1 = T-2 = 67

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Judge et al. (1999) Shipping company, banks, Reorganization, Cross-sectional Middle- and N = 514
university, manufacturing downsizing, mergers and upper-level
company acquisitions, business management
divestments, changes in
top management
Kiefer (2005) Company offering HR online Merger Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 155;
(Germany, Switzerland, and T-2 = 76
Austria)
continued

Table 5. (continued)

Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Korsgaard et al. Electric generation plants Reengineering Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 144;
(2002) T-2 = 141;
T-1 + T-2 =
104
Lam and Hong Kong bank Service quality initiative Longitudinal Bank tellers T-1 = 159;
Schaubroeck T-2 = 159;
(2000) T-3 = 159;
N = 159
Latona and La Van Electronic instrumentation Employee involvement Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 22;
(1993) manufacturer program T-2 = 22
Lau and Woodman Public university Change in university Cross-sectional Undergraduate N = 346;
(1995) tradition students; undergrads =
university staff 331; staff = 15
Lee and Peccei Korean bank Restructuring: Downsizing, Cross-sectional Employees N = 910
(2007) salary reductions, salary
and promotion systems
Logan and Ganster Trucking company Empowerment Longitudinal Project managers Experimental
(2007) intervention group = 38;
comparison
group = 30

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Lok et al. (2005) Australian private sector Reengineering, Cross-sectional Employees N = 260
organizations benchmarking,
and continuous
improvement
Madsen et al. 3 private sector and 1 nonprofit Continuous change Cross-sectional Employees and N = 454
(2005) organizations managers

508
continued
Table 5. (continued)

509
Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Martin et al. (2005) Public sector organization (PSO) Relocation, job Cross-sectional Employees; PSO = 779;
and public sector hospital (H) changes, downsizing, hospital staff H = 877
multidisciplinary teams
K. I. Miller and State department of education Open architecture Cross-sectional Employees N = 146
Monge (1985)
V. D. Miller et al Insurance company Restructuring including Cross-sectional Managers, analysts, N = 168
(1994) interdependent teams, tele counselors
involving new roles and
status levels
Morgeson et al. Printing company Semi-autonomous teams Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 914;
(2006) T-2 = 1,030
Morse and Reimer A department of a large Changes in rank-and-file Field experiment Employees Approximately
(1956) nonunionized industrial and upper management 200 (an exact
organization decision-making figure was not
discretion provided)
Mossholder et al. Fortune 100 company Restructuring and Cross-sectional Managers N = 173
(1995) downsizing
Mossholder et al. Fortune 100 company Restructuring and Cross-sectional Managers N = 173
(2000) downsizing

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Naswall et al. Hospital Cost reduction and Cross-sectional Nurses N = 512
(2005) productivity increase
Oreg (2003) University Student schedule change, Cross-sectional Undergrad/grad 3 samples:
technological change, students, faculty, N1 = 44,
relocation and staff N2 = 47,
N3 = 48
and 43
(T1 and T2)
continued

Table 5. (continued)

Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Oreg (2006) Defense organization Merger and restructuring Cross-sectional Managers and N = 177
nonmanagers
Parsons et al. Insurance trade association Technological change Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 105;
(1991) T-2 = 82;
T-3 = 62
Paterson and Cary Australian public sector Restructuring to Cross-sectional Employees N = 71
(2002) organization semiautonomous work
teams and downsizing
Paulsen et al. Australian hospital Relocation, restructuring Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 222;
(2005) to teams, and downsizing T-2 = 189;
T-3 = 117
Peach et al. (2005) Australian governmental Relocation Cross-sectional Managers and N = 149
organizations nonmanagers
Pierce and Police department Revised work shift and Longitudinal Uniformed police T-1 = 74;
Dunham (1992) compressed work week personnel T-2 = 67;
schedule T-1 + T-2 = 50
Rafferty and Griffin Australian public sector Change in top leader; Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 207;
(2006) organizations restructuring; T-2 = 168
downsizing, changes in
the HR function

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Sagie and Israeli public sector A—MBO; B— Cross-sectional Employees from all N = 249
Koslowsky organizations—national tax restructuring, organizations
(1994) authority (A), educational downsizing, job redesign;
institution (B), and 3 C-E—job redesign,
elementary schools (C-E) participation of teachers,
parents and community

510
continued
Table 5. (continued)

511
Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
(continued)
Schweiger and Two manufacturing plants Merger Longitudinal- Employees T-1; T-2; T-3; T-4.
DeNisi (1991) experimental Experimental
plant and plant = 82;
comparison comparison
plant plant = 86
Shapiro and Two Fortune 500 companies Self-managed work teams Cross-sectional Operative N = 492
Kirkman (1999) employees
Spreitzer and Aerospace company Downsizing Cross-sectional Engineers, sales, N = 350
Mishra (2002) admin
Stanley et al. S-1 = Various organizations; Organizational changes, S-1 = cross- Employees S-1 = 65;
(2005) S-2 = Energy company including restructuring sectional; S-2 = S-2, T-1 = 712;
and culture change longitudinal S-2, T-2 = 637
Steel and Lloyd U.S. Air Force Quality circles (QCs) Longitudinal; Military and civilian N = 213; QC
(1987) experimental employees group = 25;
and comparison comparison
groups group = 188
Susskind et al. Hospital company Downsizing Longitudinal Employees from T-1 = 97; T-2 =
(1998) accounting, 97; T-3 = 97
finance,

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


marketing,
engineering, etc.
van Dam (2005) Dutch hospitals Merger, relocation of Cross-sectional Nurses, medical N = 953
employees, job redesign assistants,
administrative
assistants
continued

Table 5. (continued)
Sample
Article Type of organization Type of change Research design composition Sample size
Walker et al. Auto parts manufacturer Spinoff Cross-sectional Operative N = 117
(2007) employees
Wanberg and U.S. government agency Radical reorganization Longitudinal and Employees T-1 = 173;
Banas (2000) cross-sectional T-2 = 133
Weber and Weber Fire department Organizational change Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 86;
(2001) under a new chief T-2 = 78;
T-1 + T-2 = 56
Zalesny and Farace Governmental agency Open architecture office Longitudinal Employees T-1 = 426;
(1987) T-2 = 372;
T-1+ T-2 = 247
Note. TQM = Total Quality Management.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


512
Oreg et al. 513

explicit reactions (i.e., affective, cognitive and behavioral attitude components), and
change consequences (i.e., work-related and personal consequences).
We believe our review, the tables we provide, and the resulting model offer at least
four benefits to researchers in the field. First, Tables 2 to 4 provide a compendium of
variables (including sample items from the scales used in each of the studies) that
have been used to quantitatively assess organizational change. Researchers can scruti-
nize these tables for potential scales that can be used in assessing organizational change.
Second, our model and these tables (a) describe our classification scheme of pre-
change antecedents, change antecedents, explicit reactions, and change consequences;
(b) facilitate the comparison of findings; and (c) organize and integrate an otherwise
disjointed body of literature. Thus, researchers can use these tables to review studies
and capitalize on others’ experiences in assessing change when designing their own
research. Third, these tables and our review provide the necessary information for a
change researcher to design a comprehensive assessment of a change effort.
Finally, we believe our tables and review highlight important construct distinctions
that have been previously overlooked and emphasize the importance of ensuring good
fit between the nominal and operational definitions used in studies of reactions to
change. Related to this point is that our model and review will sensitize researchers to
the importance in being specific about their intent in assessing variables. For example,
is the objective of a study to assess organizational commitment as internal context or
as change consequence? This question deserves conscious deliberation.

Theoretical Implications and Directions for Future Research


Despite the variety of factors that have already been considered in extant research on
change reactions, our review revealed several gaps that remain to be filled and a num-
ber of problems that make it difficult to compare and integrate findings from different
studies. First, given the inconsistency in how terms have been used in the research on
reactions to change, researchers should be clear about the distinction between pre-
change antecedents, change antecedents, explicit reactions, and change conse-
quences, and reflect these distinctions in the terminology used. For example, variables
such as organizational commitment and job satisfaction can be prechange antecedents
as well as change consequences. In a related vein, researchers should acknowledge the
distinction between the three reaction components and be explicit about the particular
component(s) they aim to investigate. They may find the model we present in Figure
1 useful in organizing and classifying the variables on which they choose to focus.
A related point involves the particular distinction between explicit reactions and
change consequences. We found in our review several instances where researchers
did not make this distinction and considered both to be comparable outcomes of an
organizational change. As we accumulate more research findings on change recipient
reactions, we need to be more specific in designing research to investigate whether
explicit reactions are different from change consequences. And, in what instances
might they differ?

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


514 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Third, greater attention needs to be given to the match between the nominal defi-
nitions provided and the scales used to tap variables. For example, if the researcher
intends to assess an explicit reaction to an organizational change, such as, organiza-
tional change commitment (Herscovitch & Meyer, 2002), then the scale items
should be specific about assessing commitment toward the change. On the other
hand, if organizational commitment (Meyer & Allen, 1991) is intended to be an
indirect consequence resulting from the change, it should be construed and mea-
sured accordingly.
Fourth, as indicated above, a majority of the studies in our review were based on
cross-sectional data, and almost all of them were based on self-report data from a
single source, thus subjecting findings to the potential of mono-method bias (Podsakoff,
MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003; see exceptions in Caldwell et al., 2004 and
Judge et al., 1999). Furthermore, because of survey anonymity, many of the longitudi-
nal studies considered in our review could not tie change recipients’ responses in Time
1 to their responses in Time 2, which restricted longitudinal findings to overall trends
and prohibited the analysis of trends at the individual level. Although researchers can
still use statistical procedures (Green & Feild, 1976) for assessing group change under
conditions of anonymity (see Armenakis & Zmud, 1979, for an application of this
procedure in a field setting), future work should use longitudinal designs that allow for
tracing changes at the individual level, and should aim to collect multisource data,
including data that are based on objective indicators, to supplement self-report infor-
mation. This will allow for a clearer and perhaps more accurate picture of the change
process. At the least, when data from different sources cannot be obtained, researchers
should apply procedures that can assess the degree of mono-method bias in one’s data
(e.g., Lindell & Whitney, 2001). In those instances, where a researcher employs a
longitudinal design, at a minimum the sources of internal invalidity (see Cook et al.,
1990; Stone-Romero, 2010) should be discussed. The importance of those sources that
cannot be discounted should be acknowledged and limitations in drawing cause and
effect conclusions should be explained.
Fifth, the vast majority of studies explored change processes that occurred in a
single organization (or department). This prevents the consideration of variables (e.g.,
antecedents) at the organizational level, such as change content. A main reason for the
limited amount of studies with organization-level variables is that such studies require
data from multiple organizations, which are logistically difficult to obtain. Nevertheless,
a small number of the studies we reviewed were based on data from several organiza-
tions, each undergoing a different type of change (Caldwell et al., 2004; Fedor et al.,
2006; Herold et al., 2007). Although these studies indeed allow for the examination of
organization-level antecedents and provide findings that can be generalized across
types of change content, data in future studies, from comparable organizations,
simultaneously undergoing the same type of change, could nicely complement previ-
ous work by allowing for a cleaner assessment of antecedents, without confounding
antecedents with type of organization and type of change. Such data can be sought
from companies with multiple branches, undergoing a company-wide change, or in the

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 515

public sector, in which a government-initiated change simultaneously influences


numerous organizations.
Sixth, although change recipient reactions have been extensively addressed, studies
have generally ignored the role of the change agents’ responses to these reactions.
How managers and change agents respond to change recipients’ reactions is likely to
have a direct influence on the change progress and on the ultimate success of the
change initiative. Furthermore, in their current focus on change recipients’ reactions,
many researchers seem to imply some fault on the recipients’ part, whereby they serve
as an obstacle in change agents’ path toward benefiting the organization. A similar
point was raised by Dent and Goldberg (1999) in their critique of researchers’ use of
the term resistance to change, which overlooks the possibility that, at least some of the
time, the emergence of resistance reflects fault with the change agent, or the change
itself, rather than the employee. Accordingly, transferring at least part of the research
focus to the change agents’ actions and reactions could help introduce into the litera-
ture a different perspective on change recipients’ role vis-à-vis that of the change agent.
Seventh, although change researchers studied a plethora of antecedent variables,
the analysis of their interrelationships, including mediation and moderation effects,
requires much greater attention. Although some conceptual work has been devoted to
proposing the variables that might moderate the impact of organizations on individuals’
responses to change, little empirical work has been conducted to test such propositions.
One promising point of departure could be to test Woodman and Dewett’s (2004)
propositions concerning the moderating role of changeability (the degree to which or
the ease by which some individual characteristics might be changed), depth (the mag-
nitude of change), and time (different changes take differing amounts of time).
More broadly, although we introduce in our model one general set of relationships
among antecedent categories, reactions, and consequences, it is likely that additional,
more complex, causal paths may exist among these categories. For example, some
antecedents may serve as mediators between other antecedents and recipients’ reac-
tions. One such mediated relationship may consist of perceived benefit/harm mediat-
ing the relationship between change process and recipients’ reactions. In other words,
the reasons for which the change process may ultimately influence recipients’ reac-
tions is because of the influence that process has on recipients’ perceptions of the
benefit/harm from the change. Similarly, some antecedents may serve as moderators
of the relationship between other antecedents and recipients’ reactions. Recipient
characteristics, for example, may moderate the relationship between the remaining
four antecedent categories and recipient reactions, through their impact on recipients’
perceptions. Yet another possible elaboration of our proposed path model may include
reversed paths of influence, such that recipients’ reactions influence some of the ante-
cedent categories. For example, following the influence of the change process on
employees’ reactions, these reactions may in turn influence how change agents man-
age the process at the later stages of the change. Given that change is dynamic and often
continuous, such reciprocal paths of influence seem very likely. Thus, alongside the

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


516 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

research that the model we present in Figure 1 may elicit, such additional sets of rela-
tionships should also be considered.
Finally, two interesting complexities that were revealed in our review are worthy of
further investigation. One concerns the type of organizational commitment studied
and its role in influencing reactions to change. Whereas some studies found a positive
relationship between commitment and change reactions (e.g., Iverson, 1996), van
Dam (2005) hypothesized and found a negative relationship. This suggests that greater
attention should be devoted to asking about the target of change recipients’ commit-
ment. Different effects are expected when organizational commitment is conceptual-
ized as referring to managers versus when it is conceptualized as referring to
organizational routines, norms, and values. In the former, it would indeed be expected
that commitment will be positively associated with a favorable approach toward a
management-initiated change, whereas in the latter situation change is likely to be
perceived as a threat to those committed to the “old ways of doing things,” thereby
yielding a negative relationship between commitment and support for change.
A second complexity we identified has to do with the role of information about the
change in explaining the reaction to change. Whereas most studies found that informa-
tion alleviates resistance (e.g., V. D. Miller et al., 1994), in one study (Oreg, 2006),
information was positively related to resistance. The explanation provided to this latter
finding was that the impact of information is likely to depend on the content of this
information. Furthermore, the degree to which individuals perceive themselves as hav-
ing control over the outcome is also likely to moderate the effect of information on the
reaction to change. Additional information without the ability to change anything may
lead to increased frustration, and thus resistance, rather than support. Therefore, future
studies on information and reactions to change should take into consideration possible
moderators that will reveal a more complex picture than has been considered to date.

Practical Implications
Taken together, results from the studies in our review suggest a number of directions
for organizations to follow when aiming to increase support for proposed organiza-
tional changes. First and foremost, the internal context and the change process anteced-
ent categories offer the most straightforward prescriptions for change management.
Each factor within these antecedent categories prescribes a practical direction for orga-
nizations to adopt in improving change recipients’ responses to change. For example,
the consistent finding concerning the link between organizational trust and support for
change highlights the special significance of trust in times of change. Furthermore,
increasing change recipient involvement in the change and setting change recipients at
greater ease, by allowing participation and ensuring a just process, have been shown to
go a long way in alleviating resistance. Therefore, beyond the overall importance of
trust and commitment, managers should invest special attention in creating a support-
ive and trusting organizational culture if they expect change recipients’ support and
cooperation in times of change. Given that creating such an atmosphere requires an
ongoing process that typically takes a long time, an important first step will be the
adoption of a supportive and participatory change process.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 517

Second, findings on the dispositional characteristics associated with positive reactions


to change present the possibility for organizations to select change recipients on the basis
of these dispositions for positions or assignments in which successfully dealing with
change is key. In addition, change agents and HR specialists can provide special training
and support to those individuals who have a harder time coping with change. Furthermore,
the focus on change recipient characteristics has also highlighted the importance of
opinion leaders in successfully implementing change (Lam & Schaubroeck, 2000).
Finally, change recipients are naturally concerned with the personal impact that the
change will have on them. If perceived risks/costs outweigh benefits, change recipi-
ents will understandably tend to resist change. This may seem obvious, but findings
demonstrate that managers are often oblivious to how change recipients will respond
to the change and do not give enough thought to change recipients’ perspectives. As a
start, global and local change agents need to be clear, early on, about the precise rami-
fications the change program will have for change recipients. More importantly, how-
ever, change agents must give special consideration to these ramifications and aim to
understand and incorporate change recipients’ perspectives in the design of the change.
Practically, they should carefully plan the change effort and make every effort to
explain how any threat can be dealt with, and at the same time introduce and highlight
the personal benefits change could have for employees, beyond its importance for the
organization.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests


The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or
publication of this article.

Funding
The authors received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.

Notes
1. Our inductive approach prohibited us from computing an intercoder agreement index.
Furthermore, a very broad variety of variables have been considered in the different studies,
with little overlap in variables across studies. This prevented us from conducting a more
quantitatively based meta-analysis.
2. For each theme we discuss in the narrative for Explicit Reactions to Organizational Change,
we provide citations of specific studies as examples. The complete list of relevant studies,
however, is included in Table 2.
3. The studies in this section appear in more than one location in Table 2. Each study will
appear once for every reaction component (i.e., affective, cognitive, and behavioral/intentional)
that was examined in it.
4. For each theme we discuss in the narrative for Antecedents of Change Recipients’ Reactions
to Change, we provide citations of specific studies as examples. The complete list of relevant
studies, however, is included in Table 3.
5. For each theme we discuss in the narrative for Change Consequences, we provide citations
of specific studies as examples. The complete list of relevant studies, however, is included
in Table 4.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


518 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

References

The quantitative studies included in this review are marked with an asterisk.
Alderfer, C. P. (1977). Organization development. Annual Review of Psychology, 28,
197-223.
*Amiot, C., Terry, D., Jimmieson, N., & Callan, V. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of
coping processes during a merger: Implications for job satisfaction and organizational iden-
tification. Journal of Management, 32, 552-574.
Armenakis, A. A., & Bedeian, A. G. (1999). Organizational change: A review of theory and
research in the 1990s. Journal of Management, 25, 293-315.
*Armenakis, A. A., Bernerth, J. B., Pitts, J. P., & Walker, H. J. (2007). Organizational change
recipients’ beliefs scale: Development of an assessment instrument. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 43, 495-505.
Armenakis, A. A., Harris, S. G., & Mossholder, K. W. (1993). Creating readiness for organiza-
tional change. Human Relations, 46, 681-703.
Armenakis, A. A., & Zmud, R. W. (1979). Interpreting the measurement of change in organiza-
tional research. Personnel Psychology, 32, 709-723.
*Armstrong-Stassen, M. (1998). The effect of gender and organizational level on how survivors
appraise and cope with organizational downsizing. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,
34, 125-142.
*Ashford, S. J. (1988). Individual strategies for coping with stress during organizational transi-
tions. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 24, 19-36.
*Axtell, C., Wall, T., Stride, C., Pepper, K., Clegg, C., Gardner, P., & Bolden, R. (2002).
Familiarity breeds content: The impact of exposure to change on employee openness and
well-being. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 75, 217-231.
*Bartunek, J. M., Greenberg, D. N., & Davidson, B. (1999). Consistent and inconsistent impacts
of a teacher-led empowerment initiative in a federation of schools. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 35, 457-478.
*Bartunek, J. M., Rousseau, D. M., Rudolph, J. W., & DePalma, J. A. (2006). On the receiving
end: Sensemaking, emotion, and assessments of an organizational change initiated by others.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 42, 182-206.
*Begley, T. M., & Czajka, J. M. (1993). Panel analysis of the moderating effects of commitment
on job satisfaction, intent to quit, and health following organizational change. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 78, 552-556.
*Bernerth, J. B., Armenakis, A. A., Feild, H. S., & Walker, H. J. (2007). Justice, cynicism,
and commitment: A study of important organizational change variables. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 43, 303-326.
*Bhagat, R. S., & Chassie, M. B. (1980). Effects of changes in job characteristics on some
theory-specific attitudinal outcomes: Results from a naturally occurring quasi-experiment.
Human Relations, 33, 297-313.
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187-187.
*Bordia, P., Hunt, E., Paulsen, N., Tourish, D., & DiFonzo, N. (2004). Uncertainty during orga-
nizational change: Is it all about control? European Journal of Work and Organizational
Psychology, 13, 345-365.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 519

*Bordia, P., Jones, E., Gallois, C., Callan, V. J., & Difonzo, N. (2006). Management are aliens!
Rumors and stress during organizational change. Group and Organization Management,
31, 601-621.
*Bovey, W. H., & Hede, A. (2001). Resistance to organisational change: The role of defence
mechanisms. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 16, 534-548.
*Caldwell, S. D., Herold, D. M., & Fedor, D. B. (2004). Toward an understanding of the relation-
ships among organizational change, individual differences, and changes in person-environment
fit: A cross-level study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 868-882.
*Cartwright, S., & Cooper, C. L. (1993). The psychological impact of merger and acquisition
on the individual: A study of building society managers. Human Relations, 46(3), 327-347.
*Coch, L., & French, J. R. P., Jr. (1948). Overcoming resistance to change. Human Relations,
1, 512-532.
Cook, T., Campbell, D., & Peracchio, L. (1990). Quasi-experimentation. In M. Dunnette &
L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1,
pp. 491-576). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
*Covin, T. J., Sightler, K. W., Kolenko, T. A., & Tudor, K. R. (1996). An investigation of
post-acquisition satisfaction with the merger. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 32,
125-142.
*Coyle-Shapiro, J. A. M. (1999). Employee participation and assessment of an organizational
change intervention: A three-wave study of total quality management. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 35, 439-456.
*Coyle-Shaipro, J. A. M. (2002). Changing employee attitudes: The independent effects of
TQM and profit sharing on continuous improvement orientation. Journal of Applied Behav-
ioral Science, 38, 57-77.
*Coyle-Shapiro, J. A. M., & Morrow, P. C. (2003). The role of individual differences in
employee adoption of TQM orientation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 320-340.
*Cunningham, C. E., Woodward, C. A., Shannon, H. S., MacIntosh, J., Lendrum, B.,
Rosenbloom, D., & Brown, J. (2002). Readiness for organizational change: A longitudi-
nal study of workplace, psychological and behavioural correlates. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 75, 377-392.
*Cunningham, G. B. (2006). The relationships among commitment to change, coping with
change, and turnover intentions. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology,
15, 29-45.
*Daly, J. P. (1995). Explaining changes to employees: The influence of justification and change
outcomes on employees’ fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 31,
415-428.
*Daly, J. P., & Geyer, P. D. (1994). The role of fairness in implementing large-scale change:
Employee evaluations of process and outcome in seven facility relocations. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 15, 623-638.
Dent, E. B., & Goldberg, S. G. (1999). Challenging “resistance to change.” Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 35, 25-41.
*Eby, L. T., Adams, D. M., Russell, J. E. A., & Gaby, S. H. (2000). Perceptions of organiza-
tional readiness for change: Factors related to employees’ reactions to the implementation
of team-based selling. Human Relations, 53, 419-442.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


520 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

Faucheux, C., Amado, G., & Laurent, A. (1982). Organizational development and change.
Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 343-370.
*Fedor, D. B., Caldwell, S., & Herold, D. M. (2006). The effects of organizational changes on
employee commitment: A multilevel investigation. Personnel Psychology, 59, 1-29.
*Fried, Y., Tiegs, R. B., Naughton, T. J., & Ashforth, B. E. (1996). Managers’ reactions to a
corporate acquisition: A test of an integrative model. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
17, 401-427.
Friedlander, F., & Brown, L. D. (1974). Organization development. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 25, 313-341.
Fugate, M., Kinicki, A., & Prussia, G. (2008). Employee coping with organizational change:
An examination of alternative theoretical perspectives and models. Personnel Psychology,
61, 1-36.
*Fugate, M., Kinicki, A. J., & Scheck, C. L. (2002). Coping with an organizational merger over
four stages. Personnel Psychology, 55, 905-928.
*Gaertner, K. N. (1989). Winning and losing: Understanding managers’ reactions to strategic
change. Human Relations, 42, 527-546.
*Gardner, D. G., Dunham, R. B., Cummings, L. L., & Pierce, J. L. (1987). Employee focus of
attention and reactions to organizational change. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,
23, 351-370.
*Giacquinta, J. B. (1975). Status, risk, and receptivity to innovations in complex organizations:
A study of the responses of four groups of educators to the proposed introduction of sex
education in elementary school. Sociology of Education, 48, 38-58.
*Gopinath, C., & Becker, T. E. (2000). Communication, procedural justice and employee
attitudes: Relationships under conditions of divestiture. Journal of Management, 26(1),
63-83.
Green, S., & Feild, H. (1976). Assessing group change under conditions of anonymity: A problem
in personnel research. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 49, 155-159.
*Hall, D. T., Goodale, J. G., Rabinowitz, S., & Morgan, M. A. (1978). Effects of top-down
departmental and job change upon perceived employee behavior and attitudes: A natural
field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 62-72.
*Hatcher, L., & Ross, T. L. (1991). From individual incentives to an organization-wide gain-
sharing plan: Effects on teamwork and product quality. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
12, 169-183.
*Herold, D. M., Fedor, D. B., & Caldwell, S. D. (2007). Beyond change management: A mul-
tilevel investigation of contextual and personal influences on employees’ commitment to
change. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 942-951.
*Herscovitch, L., & Meyer, J. P. (2002). Commitment to organizational change: Extension of a
three-component model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 474-487.
*Holt, D. T., Armenakis, A. A., Feild, H. S., & Harris, S. G. (2007). Readiness for organiza-
tional change: The systematic development of a scale. Journal of Applied Behavioral Sci-
ence, 43, 232-255.
*Hornung, S., & Rousseau, D. M. (2007). Active on the job-proactive in change: How auton-
omy at work contributes to employee support for organizational change. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 43, 401-426.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 521

*Iverson, R. (1996). Employee acceptance of organizational change: The role of organizational


commitment. International Journal of Human Resource Management, 7, 122-149.
*Johnson, J. R., Bernhagen, M. J., Miller, V., & Allen, M. (1996). The role of communication
in managing reductions in work force. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 24,
139-164.
*Jones, R. A., Jimmieson, N. L., & Griffiths, A. (2005). The impact of organizational culture
and reshaping capabilities on change implementation success: The mediating role of readi-
ness for change. Journal of Management Studies, 42, 361-386.
*Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Pucik, V., & Welbourne, T. M. (1999). Managerial coping with
organizational change: A dispositional perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,
107-122.
*Kiefer, T. (2005). Feeling bad: Antecedents and consequences of negative emotions in ongoing
change. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 875-897.
*Korsgaard, M. A., Sapienza, H. J., & Schweiger, D. M. (2002). Beaten before begun: The role
of procedural justice in planning change. Journal of Management, 28, 497-516.
*Lam, S. S. K., & Schaubroeck, J. (2000). A field experiment testing frontline opinion leaders
as change agents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 987-995.
*Latona, J. C., & La Van, H. (1993). Implementation of an employee involvement programme
in a small, emerging high-technology firm. Journal of Organizational Change Manage-
ment, 6(4), 17-29.
*Lau, C. M., & Woodman, R. W. (1995). Understanding organizational change: A schematic
perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 537-554.
*Lee, J., & Peccei, R. (2007). Perceived organizational support and affective commitment: The
mediating role of organization-based self-esteem in the context of job insecurity. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 28, 661-685.
Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common method variance in cross-
sectional research designs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 114-121.
*Logan, M. S., & Ganster, D. C. (2007). The effects of empowerment on attitudes and perfor-
mance: The role of social support and empowerment beliefs. Journal of Management Studies,
44, 1523-1550.
*Lok, P., Hung, R. Y., Walsh, P., Wang, P., & Crawford, J. (2005). An integrative framework
for measuring the extent to which organizational variables influence the success of process
improvement programmes. Journal of Management Studies, 42, 1357-1381.
*Madsen, S. R., Miller, D., & John, C. R. (2005). Readiness for organizational change: Do
organizational commitment and social relationships in the workplace make a difference?
Human Resource Development Quarterly, 16, 213-233.
*Martin, A. J., Jones, E. S., & Callan, V. J. (2005). The role of psychological climate in facili-
tating employee adjustment during organizational change. European Journal of Work and
Organizational Psychology, 14, 263-283.
Martin, A. J., Jones, E. S., & Callan, V. J. (2006). Status differences in employee adjustment
during organizational change. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 21, 145-162.
Meyer, J., & Allen, N. (1991). A three-component conceptualization of organizational commit-
ment. Human Resource Management Review, 1, 61-89.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


522 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

*Miller, K. I., & Monge, P. R. (1985). Social information and employee anxiety about organi-
zational change. Human Communication Research, 11, 365-386.
*Miller, V. D., Johnson, J. R., & Grau, J. (1994). Antecedents to willingness to participate in
a planned organizational change. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 22, 59-80.
*Morgeson, F. P., Johnson, M. D., Campion, M. A., Medsker, G. J., & Mumford, T. V. (2006).
Understanding reactions to job redesign: A quasi-experimental investigation of the moder-
ating effects of organizational context on perceptions of performance behavior. Personnel
Psychology, 59, 333-363.
*Morse, N. C., & Reimer, E. (1956). The experimental change of a major organizational variable.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 52, 120-129.
*Mossholder, K. W., Settoon, R. P., Armenakis, A. A., & Harris, S. G. (2000). Emotion during
organizational transformations: An interactive model of survivor reactions. Group & Orga-
nization Management, 25, 220-243.
*Mossholder, K. W., Settoon, R. P., Harris, S. G., & Armenakis, A. A. (1995). Measuring
emotion in open-ended survey responses: An application of textual data analysis. Journal
of Management, 21, 335-355.
Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. (1979). The measure of organizational commit-
ment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14, 224-247.
*Naswall, K., Sverke, M., & Hellgren, J. (2005). The moderating role of personality characteris-
tics on the relationship between job insecurity and strain. Work & Stress, 19, 37-49.
Nord, W. R., & Jermier, J. M. (1994). Overcoming resistance to resistance: Insights from a
study of the shadows. Public Administration Quarterly, 17, 396-409.
*Oreg, S. (2003). Resistance to change: Developing an individual differences measure. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 88, 680-693.
*Oreg, S. (2006). Personality, context, and resistance to organizational change. European Journal
of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15, 73-101.
Oreg, S. & van Dam, K. (2009). Organisational justice in the context of organisational change.
Netherlands Journal of Psychology, 65, 127-135.
*Parsons, C. K., Liden, R. C., O’Connor, E. J., & Nagao, D. H. (1991). Employee responses to
technologically-driven change: The implementation of office automation in a service orga-
nization. Human Relations, 44, 1331-1356.
Pasmore, W., & Fagans, M. (1992). Participation, individual development, and organizational
change. Journal of Management, 18, 375-397.
*Paterson, J. M., & Cary, J. (2002). Organizational justice, change anxiety, and acceptance of
downsizing: Preliminary tests of an AET-based model. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 83-103.
*Paulsen, N., Callan, V. J., Grice, T. A., Rooney, D., Gallois, C., Jones, E., . . . Bordia, P.
(2005). Job uncertainty and personal control during downsizing: A comparison of survivors
and victims. Human Relations, 58, 463-496.
*Peach, M., Jimmieson, N. L., & White, K. M. (2005). Beliefs underlying employee readiness
to support a building relocation: A theory of planned behavior perspective. Organization
Development Journal, 23(3), 9-22.
Piderit, S. K. (2000). Rethinking resistance and recognizing ambivalence: A multidimensional view
of attitudes toward an organizational change. Academy of Management Review, 25, 783-794.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


Oreg et al. 523

*Pierce, J. L., & Dunham, R. B. (1992). The 12-hour work day: A 48 hour, eight-day week.
Academy of Management Journal, 35, 1086-1098.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method
biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879-903.
Porras, J. I., & Silvers, R. C. (1991). Organization development and transformation. Annual
Review of Psychology, 42, 51-78.
*Rafferty, A. E., & Griffin, M. A. (2006). Perceptions of organizational change: A Stress and
coping perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1154-1162.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforce-
ment. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80, 1-28.
*Sagie, A., & Koslowsky, M. (1994). Organizational attitudes and behaviors as a function of
participation in strategic and tactical change decisions: An application of path-goal theory.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 37-47.
Sashkin, M., & Burke, W. W. (1987). Organization development in the 1980’s. Journal of
Management, 13, 393-417.
*Schweiger, D. M., & Denisi, A. S. (1991). Communication with employees following a
merger: A longitudinal field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 34, 110-135.
*Shapiro, D. L., & Kirkman, B. L. (1999). Employees’ reaction to the change to work teams:
The influence of “anticipatory” injustice. Journal of Organizational Change Management,
12, 51-67.
*Spreitzer, G. M., & Mishra, A. K. (2002). To stay or to go: Voluntary survivor turnover
following an organizational downsizing. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 707-729.
*Stanley, D. J., Meyer, J. P., & Topolnytsky, L. (2005). Employee cynicism and resistance to
organizational change. Journal of Business and Psychology, 19, 429-459.
*Steel, R. P., & Lloyd, R. F. (1988). Cognitive, affective, and behavioral outcomes of participa-
tion in quality circles: Conceptual and empirical findings. Journal of Applied Behavioral
Science, 24, 1-17.
Stone-Romero, E. (2010). Research strategies in industrial and organizational psychology: Non-
experimental, quasi-experimental, and randomized experimental research in special pur-
pose and non-special purpose settings. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and
organizational psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 37-72). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
*Susskind, A. M., Miller, V. D., & Johnson, J. D. (1998). Downsizing and structural holes:
Their impact on layoff survivors’ perceptions of organizational chaos and openness to
change. Communication Research, 25, 30-65.
Vakola, M., Tsaousis, I., & Nikolaou, I. (2004). The role of emotional intelligence and personal-
ity variables on attitudes toward organisational change. Journal of Managerial Psychology,
19, 88-110.
*van Dam, K. (2005). Employee attitudes toward job changes: An application and extension
of Rusbult and Farrell’s investment model. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 78, 253-272.

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012


524 The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 47(4)

*Walker, H. J., Armenakis, A. A., & Bernerth, J. B. (2007). Factors influencing organizational
change efforts: An integrative investigation of change content, context, process and indi-
vidual differences. Journal of Organizational Change Management, 20, 761-773.
*Wanberg, C. R., & Banas, J. T. (2000). Predictors and outcomes of openness to changes in a
reorganizing workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 132-142.
*Weber, P. S., & Weber, J. E. (2001). Changes in employee perceptions during organizational
change. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 22, 291-300.
Weick, K. E., & Quinn, R. E. (1999). Organizational change and development. Annual Review
of Psychology, 50, 361-386.
Whissell, C. M., & Dewson, M. R. J. (1986). A dictionary of affect in language. III: Analysis of
two biblical and two secular passages. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 127-132.
Woodman, R. W. (1989). Organizational change and development: New arenas for inquiry and
action. Journal of Management, 15, 205-228.
Woodman, R., & Dewett, T. (2004). Organizationally relevant journeys in individual change. In
M. S. Poole & A. H. Van de Ven (Eds.), Handbook of organizational change and innovation
(pp. 32-49). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
*Zalesny, M. D., & Farace, R. V. (1987). Traditional versus open offices: A comparison of
sociotechnical, social relations, and symbolic meaning perspectives. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 30, 240-259.

Bios
Shaul Oreg is a senior lecturer at the Department of Sociology and Anthropology at the
University of Haifa in Israel, and is currently the chair of the department’s Organizational
Division. In his research, he focuses on individual-differences in social and organizational
contexts, with a particular focus on employees’ reactions to organizational change. Further
information about his research can be obtained at: http://soc.haifa.ac.il/~oreg/

Maria Vakola, PhD, is an organizational psychologist working as an assistant professor at the


Athens University of Economics and Business, Greece. Her research interests focus on the
psychology of change, such as the role of individual differences in reactions to organizational
change and the relationship between behavior change and learning transfer.

Achilles Armenakis is the James T. Pursell, Sr., eminent scholar in management ethics and
director of the Auburn University Center for Ethical Organizational Cultures at Auburn
University. His research efforts have concentrated on organizational change and manage-
ment ethics. Detailed biographical information can be obtained at http://business.auburn
.edu/nondegreeprograms/ethics/

Downloaded from jab.sagepub.com at UNIV KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA on March 9, 2012

You might also like