Contributions of Nordoff-Robbins

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 210

CONTRIBUTIONS OF NORDOFF-ROBBINS MUSIC THERAPY WITHIN A DEVELOPMENTAL, INDIVIDUAL-DIFFERENCES, RELATIONSHIP-BASED (DIR)/FLOORTIME FRAMEWORK TO THE TREATMENT OF CHILDREN WITH AUTISM:

FOUR CASE STUDIES

A Dissertation Submitted to The Temple University Graduate Board

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

by John A. Carpente May, 2009

ii

by John A. Carpente 2009 All Rights Reserved

The DVD of work with these children has not been included in the online version of the dissertation to protect the anonymity of the children.

iii

AKNOWLEDGEMENTS This dissertation holds far more than the culmination of years of study. These pages also reflect the relationships with many generous and inspiring people that I have had the pleasure and honor of meeting throughout my journey. First of all I would like to thank my dissertation committee: Drs. Kenneth Bruscia, Cheryl Dileo, and Cynthia Folio. I thank each of you for graciously agreeing to be on my committee and for offering your expertise, time and attention to detail during the busy semesters. To Dr. Kenneth Bruscia, my committee Chair, advisor, and gracious mentor, I thank you for always being available, supportive, encouraging, and always ready to guide. You have expanded my ability to think, write, and understand. Without your guidance and persistent help this dissertation would not have been possible. And to Dr. Cheryl Dileo, I thank you for your authentic encouragement, care, and belief in my abilities. You have always gone above and beyond the call of your teaching duties, lending support and inspiration whenever needed. I thank you for your dedication and commitment. To the students in the Music Therapy PhD program, I am grateful to you all for making my time at Temple so memorable. I would like to especially mention Nadine Cadesky, Lillian Eyre, Jim Hiller, Andi McGraw Hunt, Nancy Jackson, Roberta Kagin, Doug Keith, John Mahoney, Bryan Muller, Kathy Murphy, and Carol Shultis. I am honored and grateful to have worked closely with each of you. And to my incredibly talented and dedicated colleagues at The Rebecca Center for Music Therapy: Heejin Chung, Jill Lucente, Jenn Toti, and Reika

iv

Yoshihara, I thank you all for taking on many additional responsibilities during my academic pursuits. Without your tireless efforts, loyalty and commitment, The Rebecca Center would cease to exist. To my colleagues at Molloy College, Dan McGann, Evelyn Selesky, and Dr. Suzanne Sorel, I thank you for your support, friendship, and flexibility. And to Wenhui Chen and Norman Weil, library staff at Molloy College, I am grateful to you both for working diligently in helping me acquire the many books, and journal articles necessary for me to complete this dissertation. To Jean Ambrosio, my talented and intuitive music therapy intern who co-lead each session with me during this study, I thank you for your commitment and dedication to this dissertation. Dr. Gil Tippy, you have been a colleague, mentor, and friend throughout the process of this dissertation and beyond. I am indebted to you for your support, jokes, and for constantly stretching my knowledge of child development and the DIR Model. I would also like to give many thanks to my good friend and colleague, Alex Klein, for your insightful feedback, support, encouragement, and philosophical discussions during our many late nights in some of New Yorks finest brew-pubs. And to Tina McCourt, Director of Rebecca School, I thank you for enthusiastically allowing me to conduct this research study during my work days, and for consistently being supportive and trusting of my work. To Alison Berkley, Alla Sheynkin, and Rebecca Starr, classroom teachers, I thank you all for your assistance, time and efforts during the process of this study. And to the brilliant Dr. Stanley Greenspan, co-creator of the DIR/Floortime

Model, I thank you for your weekly afternoon clinical meetings at Rebecca School. It was a privilege and an honor to be a part of those weekly discussions, as they have insurmountably influenced my clinical work, thinking, and parenting skills. I extend my deepest gratitude to the children and parents who agreed to participate in this study. This study would not have been possible without you all, and I appreciate your contributions. Thank you for being my teachers. To my late mom, Regina Carpente, my biggest fan and hero, I am forever grateful for all that you have given me. If it were not for you, music, and all that comes with it, would not be in my life. I can not thank you enough for all that you have instilled in me; and for the love, encouragement and support that you provided throughout the years. I am blessed to have been touched by you. I love you ma. You are forever in my heart-When I sing, you sing. And to my dad, Santiago Carpente, the hardest working man I know, I thank you for your love and support throughout the years, and, for teaching me the definition of work ethic. To my uncle, Jack Nathan, whos been like a second father to me, I am grateful for your love, voice of reason, and for constantly busting my chops. And to my sister, Genevieve Carpente, I thank you for your love, and support through all of my music and music therapy endeavors throughout the years. To my wonderful in-laws, Willie and Maria Kern, I am indebted to you both for your constant support, love, and for always being there in time of need. To my beautiful daughter, Olivia Rebecca Carpente (my lil angel), I am eternally grateful for your love, wonder, curiosity, kisses, dances, songs, and patience with me during my academic pursuits. And to my new son, Jason John Carpente (sport), I thank

vi

you for your contagious smile, laughter and ability to put a grin on my face at anytime. My final and most heartfelt acknowledgment must go to my incredible wife, Susan Carpente who has patiently been with me throughout the highs, lows, and inbetweens of this journey. I thank you for your love, patience, reassurance, and for keeping it all together at the home-front. For all of that, and for being everything that I am not, you have my everlasting love.

vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ......................................................................................... iv LIST OF TABLES ...................................................................................................... xii LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................................... xiii CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................. 1 Definition of Autism ........................................................................................ 3 Differential Diagnosis ...................................................................................... 6 Incidence .......................................................................................................... 7 Music Therapy and Those with Autism ........................................................... 9 Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy (NRMT) ................................................... 10 Personal Experience with NRMT and Autism............................................... 14 Developmental, Individual-Difference, Relationship-based Model. ............. 16 A Comparison of NRMT and DIR ............................................................. 19 Purpose of Research ....................................................................................... 21 2. RELATED LITERATURE.................................................................................. 23 Musical Characteristics of Individuals with Autism ...................................... 23 Case Studies of Music Therapy and Autism .................................................. 27 Outcome Studies ............................................................................................ 37 Clinical Case Studies of DIR/Floortime and Autism .............................. 41 Effects of DIR/Floortime with Children with Autism ............................ 43 Implications for Present Study ....................................................................... 44

viii

3. METHOD ............................................................................................................ 47 Participants ..................................................................................................... 47 Design ............................................................................................................ 47 Measures ........................................................................................................ 48 Procedures ...................................................................................................... 51 Materials and Equipment ............................................................................... 54 Seven Areas of Musical Responsiveness ....................................................... 55 Data Analysis ................................................................................................. 56 4. RESULTS: MATTHEW...................................................................................... 58 History ........................................................................................................... 58 DIR Profile .................................................................................................. 59 Music Therapy Assessment ........................................................................... 60 Treatment Process .......................................................................................... 62 Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (Pre- and Post-Test) ...................... 70 Goal Attainment Scaling ................................................................................ 73 Summary ........................................................................................................ 76 5. RESULTS: JANET .............................................................................................. 80 History ........................................................................................................... 80 DIR Profile .................................................................................................. 80 Music Therapy Assessment ........................................................................... 81 Treatment Process .......................................................................................... 83 Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (Pre- and Post-Test) ...................... 93 Goal Attainment Scaling ................................................................................ 96

ix

Summary ........................................................................................................ 98 6. RESULTS: TIMMY .......................................................................................... 104 History ......................................................................................................... 104 DIR Profile ................................................................................................ 105 Music Therapy Assessment ......................................................................... 105 Treatment Process ........................................................................................ 107 Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (Pre- and Post-Test) .................... 118 Goal Attainment Scaling .............................................................................. 121 Summary ...................................................................................................... 124 7. RESULTS: MELINDA...................................................................................... 128 History ......................................................................................................... 128 DIR Profile ................................................................................................ 129 Music Therapy Assessment ......................................................................... 133 Treatment Process ........................................................................................ 130 Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (Pre- and Post-Test) .................... 144 Goal Attainment Scaling .............................................................................. 148 Summary ...................................................................................................... 151 8. DISCUSSIONS AND CONCLUSIONS ............................................................. 156 Comparing NRMT and the DIR/Floortime Models.............................. 156 Comparing Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches ................................. 158 Commonalities among the Four Children .................................................... 158 Contributions of NRMT to DIR/Floortime .......................................... 159 Common Areas of Improvement on FEAS .................................................. 159

Common Areas of Improvement on the GAS.............................................. 159 Limitations of the Study............................................................................... 160 Implications for NRMT Clinical Theory and Practice ................................ 161 Implications for General Clinical Theory and Practice ............................... 161 Implications for Research ............................................................................ 162 Implications for Education and Training ..................................................... 162 REFERENCES CITED ............................................................................................ 164 APPENDIXES A. IRB APPROVAL ............................................................................................... 174 B. PARTICIPANT CONSENT FORMS................................................................ 177 C. FUNCTIONAL EMOTIONAL ASSESSMENT SCALE (FEAS).................... 183 D. DIGITAL VERSATILE DISC TRACKS .......................................................... 194 E. GOAL ATTAINMENT SCALE: MATTHEW. ................................................ 196 F. GOAL ATTAINMENT SCALE: JANET ......................................................... 197 G. GOAL ATTAINMENT SCALE: TIMMY. ....................................................... 198 H. GOAL ATTAINMENT SCALE: MELINDA ................................................... 199

The DVD of work with these children has not been included in the online version of the dissertation to protect the anonymity of the children.

xi

LIST OF TABLES

Table

Page

1. Sequence of Measures and Treatment ................................................................. 53 2. Comparison of Matthews Pre- and Post FEAS Scores ....................................... 70 3. Comparison of Janets Pre- and Post FEAS Scores ............................................. 93 4. Comparison of Timmys Pre- and Post FEAS Scores ....................................... 119 5. Comparison of Melindas Pre- and Post FEAS Scores ...................................... 145

xii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figures

Page

1. Matthew: Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS) ......................................... 73 2. Matthew: Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS) ........................................ 75 3. Janet: Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS) ............................................... 96 4. Janet: Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS) .............................................. 98 5. Timmy: Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS) ......................................... 122 6. Timmy: Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS)......................................... 125 7. Melinda: Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS)........................................ 147 8. Melinda: Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS) ....................................... 150

xiii

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Overview This study was concerned with the effectiveness of Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy (NRMT) carried out within a Developmental, Individual-Difference, Relationship-based (DIR)/Floortime framework in addressing the individual needs of children with autism. In NRMT, the child is an active participant in the music making process, playing various instruments that require no formal training. The therapists task is to improvise music built around the childs musical responses, reactions, responses, and/or movements to engage him or her in a musical experience that will facilitate musical relatedness, communication, socialization, and awareness. The DIR model provides a comprehensive framework for assessing, understanding, and treating the child. It centers on helping the child master the building blocks of relating, communicating, and thinking through the formulation of relationships via interactive play, using Floortime (a systematic way of working with the children to help them reach their developmental potential) (Greenspan & Weider, 1998). This study sought to determine the effectiveness of NRMT in meeting musical goals specifically established for each individual child, and to conclude if progress in musical goals paralleled progress in non-musical (DIR) goals. In the introduction that follows, topics of discussion include a definition and the essential diagnostic features of autism, differential diagnosis, and incidences of autism. In addition, music

therapy, NRMT, and DIR/ Floortime are defined and discussed in regard to assessing, evaluating, and treating individuals with autism. Definition of Autism Autism is a brain development disorder that manifests itself before three years of age. It is marked by impaired social interaction, impaired communication, and restricted and repetitive behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Autism Society of America, 2007). The word, autism, derives from the Greek word autos, meaning self. The term was first coined in 1911 by Swiss psychiatrist Eugen Bleuer (1950) as he was defining symptoms of schizophrenia in adults; autism referred to the individuals withdrawal from the outside world. It was not until 1943, however, that its classification as a disorder originated, when Dr. Leo Kanner (1943) introduced the term early infantile autism to describe his work with children who showed no interest in connecting with other people. Although its causes remain unknown, autism is generally accepted as a neurological disorder present at birth (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Currently, however, there are no medical or genetic tests to identify or detect autism. The clinical tool most widely used to diagnose the disorder is the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.) Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Listed as one of the five Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD) including Asperger Syndrome, Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, Retts Syndrome, and PDD Not Otherwise Specified (NOS), autism is a puzzling and heterogeneous disorder that manifests in various ways depending on the developmental level and chronological age of the individual (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). The three

essential diagnostic features in detecting autism are impairments in social interaction, communication, and behavior (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Impairments in Social Interactions According to the DSM-IV-TR, social impairments are both significant and varied in autism. Individuals with autism do not have an awareness of others and often lack any concept of the needs of others. They are often unable to empathize or sympathize. They tend to seek isolation and to avoid the outside world and have no interest or exhibit difficulty in developing appropriate peer relationships. Instead, they prefer solitary activities and avoid participating in social games. Those who interact with others may do so using inappropriate behaviors (e.g., those lacking in social and emotional reciprocity). Individuals with autism may also have difficulties in the use and understanding of verbal and nonverbal cues, such as facial expressions, eye contact, and body gestures. In addition, they have difficulties with symbolic or dramatic play. Impairments in Communication Communication impairments in individuals with autism vary considerably and affect verbal and non-verbal skills, as well as expressive and receptive language (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Individuals may experience a delay in or a complete lack of language development. In addition, communication impairments also affect the individuals ability to create internal symbols, develop ideas, and engage in pretend play (Greenspan & Shanker, 2004; Greenspan & Weider, 2006a; 2006b; DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Individuals with autism who have not developed speech and the ability to understand conventional body language may be particularly challenged and frustrated in expressing needs

and in understanding simple questions and gestures. For those that have developed speech, their language often consists of the rote imitation of words or phrases (echolalia) or the use of words that are not relevant to the topic at hand (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Repetitive and Restricted Behaviors Individuals with autism exhibit restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behaviors in a variety of ways: engaging in the same act again and again, repeatedly uttering the same phrase, insisting on the same routine or ritual, or obsessing on a favorite topic (CarcaniRathwell, Rabe-Hasketh, & Santosh, 2006). In addition, repetitive motor-movements such as hand-flapping, toe walking, rocking, swaying, and pacing are common in people with autism, as are preoccupations with spinning and gazing (stimming) at linear and circular objects. Interrupting routines, rituals and/or self-stimulatory behaviors may result in the individual having a meltdown (e.g., tantrumming, hitting, biting, etc.) (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Sensory Integration Dysfunction Sensory integration dysfunction (a problem in how the child experiences sensations) is a common characteristic of autism. Generally, individuals with autism have not developed the neurological capacity to integrate and modulate information from the five senses (vision, auditory, touch, olfaction, and taste), the sense of movement (vestibular system), and/or the positional sense (proprioception) (Berger, 2002; Stock-Kranowitz, 1998;). The individual may crave jumping, being swung, hand waving, pacing, and deep pressure applied to his/her body. In addition to craving sensations, the individual may overreact to certain sensory sensations, such as high or low pitched sounds, bright lights, touch, and smell.

Furthermore, an additional area of functioning affected in people with autism is visualspatial processing and their ability to negotiate and map out the space in which they move (Furth & Wachs, 1974). Visual-spatial difficulties can manifest in how the individual moves in the environment (e.g., wandering aimlessly from one thing to another, walking into things, etc.). Motor-Planning Motor-planning is also a common area in which individuals with autism have difficulty (Greenspan & Weider, 1998, 2006a, 2006b). Motor-planning includes the individuals ability to sequence two or more movements, such as picking up a drum stick and hitting a drum, or picking up an object and putting it into something else. In addition, oral-motor difficulties (ability to move the tongue and muscles in the mouth) are also common, and impede the ability to develop speech (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001). Differential Diagnosis It is important that Autistic Disorder be differentiated from the other four Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Rett's Syndrome differs primarily from Autistic disorder in that it is only manifested in females. In addition, it is also associated with the loss of previously acquired hand skills and the display of poorly coordinated gait or trunk movements (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Motor skills are usually replaced with repetitive movements of the hands. Childhood Disintegrative Disorder differs from the Autistic Disorder in that the child shows significant losses (regression) in social behavior, language, play, and adaptive behavior after development was apparently normal for at least the first two years of birth (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Asperger's Syndrome differs from Autistic Disorder in that impairment is primarily within the domain of social interactions (Greenspan & Weider, 2006a, 2006b). Development of other adaptive behaviors, cognition, and language are not significantly delayed (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Repetitive patterns of behavior or rituals must also be present in individuals with Aspergers Syndrome. This disorder tends to be recognized and diagnosed later, usually after three years of age, rather than two years as in Autistic Disorder. The diagnosis of Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD NOS) is very similar to the Autistic Disorder. It is used when severe impairments in reciprocal social interaction and/or communication skills are evident, or when restricted activities and interests are displayed. It is a borderline category and is sometimes referred to as "atypical autism" (Greenspan & Weider, 1998; 2006a; 2006b; Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2003). Incidence Recently autism has been recognized as the fastest growing developmental disorder in the USA. It can affect any child regardless of race, ethnicity, economic status, and educational level. It is four times more prevalent in boys than in girls (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). According to the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (Center for Disease Control, 2007), there are more than 1.5 million cases of autism in the United States alone. One in 150 children is born with autism. The need for effective educational and therapeutic programs for people with autism continues to grow every year. The New York City Department of Education reports that there are 4,423 children with autism in New York City public schools (Robinson, 2006). Frequently, children with autism end up in special education classes alongside other children who are

emotionally disturbed and/or developmentally delayed. In the schools, there is usually only one teacher for every six to twelve students, and the average cost to the city is $34,816 per child, per year (Robinson, 2006). According to a recent report in New York Magazine (Katz, 2006), this year alone, the New York City public schools have budgeted nearly $824 million to pay private schools to educate children with autism. That amount has increased from $82 million a decade ago, in part because of the national surge in the incidence of autism. In 2006 President Bush signed The Combating Autism Act of 2006, and increased government funding to expand activities related to autism research, prevention, and treatment (The White House, 2006). The Combating Autism Act authorizes: Research under National Institutes of Health (NIH) to address the entire scope of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). (In addition, since 2002, NIH funding for autism-related research has increased by over 80 percent from $56 million in 2001 to an estimated $101 million in the 2007 budget) (The White House, 2006). Regional centers of excellence for ASD research and epidemiology (These Centers collect and analyze information on the number, incidence, correlates, and causes of ASD and other developmental disabilities) (The White House, 2006). Activities to increase public awareness of autism, improve the ability of health care providers to use evidence-based interventions, and increase early screening for autism (The White House, 2006). The increases in the incidence of autism, as well as the growing need for effective educational and therapeutic programs for individuals who have the disorder, point to the significance of the present study. The aim of the present study is to determine the efficacy of

music therapy in meeting the therapeutic needs of children with autism. Before presenting more details on the present study, it is necessary to describe music therapy and its applications with those who have autism. Music Therapy with Those Who Have Autism Music therapy is the use of music and all of its facets to achieve therapeutic goals in the physical, behavioral, mental, emotional, social, and spiritual domains (Bruscia, 1998). It is used with many client populations including developmentally delayed children, mentally ill adults, atrisk youth, people afflicted with HIV/AIDS, adults and children with chronic physical diseases, geriatric patients, and adults in psychotherapy. It is also a common treatment modality for children with autism (Bruscia, 1998). There are four main methods used in music therapy. They are: 1) receptive music experiences in which the clients listen to music; 2) composition experiences in which clients compose or contribute to the composition of a song or instrumental piece; 3) re-creative music experiences in which the clients play or sing music that has already been composed; and 4) improvisation experiences in which clients create music spontaneously using instruments or their voice (Bruscia, 1998). Each one of these types of musical experiences offers a variety of benefits based on the clients clinical needs. Music therapy has a long history of treating children with autism (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Nordoff & Robbins, 1964; 1977; Stevens & Clark, 1969), and the literature is replete with clinical writings and research studies on the topic. As will be shown in the next chapter, the clinical writings demonstrate that all four methods of music therapy have been used with individuals with autism in order to facilitate nonverbal expression and communication,

socialization, self-awareness, sensory integration, and musical and interpersonal relatedness (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Berger, 2002; Nordoff & Robbins, 1968). The research literature deals with two topics: the musical characteristics of individuals with autism, and the efficacy of music therapy in their treatment. Empirical studies on their musical characteristics have shown that individuals with autism may exhibit special musical abilities, particularly in pitch perception (Bonnel et al., 2003a; 2003b; Heaton, 2005), as well as an affinity for music (Blackstock, 1978; Thaut, 1987). Empirical efficacy studies have shown that music therapy is effective in increasing socialization and communication (Hollander & Juhrs, 1974; Edgerton, 1994), decreasing perseverative behaviors (Brownell, 2004; Hollander & Juhrs, 1974; Orr, Myles & Carlson, 1998); developing musical and non-musical communication (Edgerton, 1994), improving parentchild relationships (Allgood, 2005), and facilitating peer interactions (Kern, 2006). These empirical studies will be reviewed in greater detail in the next chapter. The present study was concerned with the clinical use of improvisation experiences in the treatment of autism. More specifically, the purpose of the study was to evaluate the effectiveness of Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy (NRMT) within a DIR/Floortime framework in meeting the therapeutic needs of individuals with autism. As outlined below, NRMT is one of the most established approaches to improvisational music therapy. In addition, the NRMT approach will be implemented employing DIR concepts (Greenspan & Weider, 2006a; 2006b) as a way of conceptualizing and assessing the childs needs and strengths, and evaluating the childs progress. These concepts and models will be described in greater detail below.

Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy (NRMT), originally coined Creative Music Therapy is an improvisational and compositional approach to music therapy developed by Paul Nordoff, an American composer/pianist, and Clive Robbins, a special educator from Great Britain (Bruscia, 1987; Kim, 2004). They worked closely together from 1959 to 1976 as therapist and co-therapist with children with special needs in various settings (Nordoff & Robbins, 1971). After the death of Nordoff in 1976, Robbins continued to develop the work worldwide. Today NRMT is practiced and studied throughout the globe. Traditionally, NRMT involves two therapists participating in sessions: the primary therapist is responsible for spontaneously creating music on a harmonic instrument, usually on guitar or piano; the co-therapist interacts directly with the child, facilitating musical participation and engagement. A central concept in NRMT is the music child, which is based on the belief that all children have an inborn musicality and the capacity, regardless of pathology, to respond to musical experiences (Nordoff & Robbins, 1977, p.1). However, in order for the music child to flourish the child must be open to experiencing himself, others, and the world around him; for it is through these experiences that receptive, cognitive, and expressive capabilities are developed (Robbins & Robbins, 1991, p. 57). What prevents the music child from functioning is what they called the condition child (Nordoff & Robbins, 1964; 1977). The condition child, encasing the music child, represents what the child has come to behis learned responses to the world and his personality development based on how he has internalized lifes experiences.

10

Nordoff and Robbins (1964; 1968; 1971; 1977) found that, through the use of music improvisation, the condition child can be bypassed, reaching the inborn musicality of the individuals music child. In nurturing, challenging, and supporting the music child through musical experiences, the child develops beyond the conditioned self and into new ways of experiencing the self and the world. A new self is born, and the condition child becomes the old self (Robbins & Robbins, 1991, p.59). The primary focus of interaction in NRMT, then, is the musical relationship between the child and therapists. Verbal interaction is not essential in NRMT, rather, it is the musical experience and level of musical relatedness of the child that is important. Because the primary focus is on music, the musical process is the clinical process (Aigen, 2005, p. 94), meaning that the therapists primary concern is to develop and incorporate musical interventions to deepen the childs musical engagement and interaction. In NRMT, musical goals are clinical goals (Aigen, 2005). What a child accomplishes musically is regarded as a clinical or therapeutic accomplishment. For example: NR therapists may work on a childs ability to increase the dynamic range (loud and soft) of his drum playing, or expand the range of tempi (fast and slow) while playing the xylophone. Although the focus is on musical goals and the widening of the childs musical experiences, it is clear that these goal areas can also address cognitive, expressive, sensory, and social deficits. In considering all that is involved to achieve the above musical goals: motor planning, auditory cuing, fine and gross motor skills, visual spatial processing, and sensory modulation, it becomes clear that developmental goals are realized through musical goals and experiences.

11

In NRMT, the child is an active creator of music, playing a vital role in the direction of the musical process. The child plays various instruments that require no formal training or experience, while the therapist improvises music built around the childs music-making, emotional state, and/or movements. The purpose is to musically engage, match, support, and enhance whatever the child is offering, musically or non-musically, therein promoting relatedness, communication, socialization, and awareness within the music itself. In working within the Nordoff-Robbins approach, the therapist directly responds or calls out to the child, creating musical questions and answers to elicit a back and forth musical dialogue. The therapist may create a musical scenario that seeks a musical response; create music that reflects a childs being or emotionality; or improvise music to address the childs sensory needs for musical engagement. Furthermore, the therapist may create music that seeks to provoke a child into a certain musical response, or improvise music to a childs idiosyncratic, perseverative behaviors to make the behaviors communicative and interactive. In all forms of NRMT, the therapist uses his or her musicianship, creativity, intuition, and clinical knowledge of the child to improvise music that will activate the childs will, motivation, and passions, thereby bringing the child into musical mutuality and intentionality. Through the experience of co-actively improvising music, children can experience themselves in a new way, beyond their pathology, and perhaps free from their habitual behaviors (Sorel, 2005). Over time, the therapists and child together develop various musical materials (e.g., themes, songs, and instrumental pieces), based on spontaneous events and experiences that arise in each session. Within these co-created musical materials, clinical goals and treatment plans are identified and pursued by the therapists. All sessions are videotaped and documented so that

12

significant responses may be noted, and musical ideas and new songs can be transcribed for future use. In reviewing each session, therapists may observe that a particular musical theme (e.g., an interval, motif, or rhythm pattern) or song evokes an instrumental or vocal response from the child; the therapist can then incorporate that music into the clinical repertoire to explore further in subsequent sessions. As will be discussed in the next chapters, NRMT has been used extensively in the treatment of children with autism (Nordoff & Robbins, 1964; 1968; 1971; 1977; 1983). The very essence of this approach lends itself to address the core deficits of individuals with autism. Because NRMT seeks to bring a child out of isolation and into a world of musical mutuality and relatedness (through instruments or voice), it can facilitate: nonverbal communication (Brown, 1994; Nordoff & Robbins, 1977; 1992); self-expression (Brown, 1994; Howat,1995); socialization (Turry & Marcus, 2003); relatedness (Brown, 1994; Nordoff & Robbins,1964; 1968); creativity (Howat, 1995); awareness, intentionality, and purposefulness (Brown, 1994; Turry & Marcus, 2003). Nordoff and Robbins (1977) specifically write about the value and importance of bringing a child with autism into musical interactiveness saying: Inherent musical responsiveness can impel an autistic child into tenuous instrumental activity. As you work to engage this, to extend it and make it purposeful, he is exposed to unaccustomed experiences of contact and interactivityfrom which he tends usually to withdraw. (Nordoff & Robbins, 1977, p. 120) In the next section, I will describe how my own experiences with NRMT and with individuals with autism have led to this study and have shaped its purpose and design. I will also

13

explain how my work in NRMT with individuals with autism has been influenced by Greenspans (2006a; 2006b) concepts of DIR. Personal Experience with NRMT and Autism In 2000, following the completion of graduate school and the Nordoff-Robbins certification program, I began to focus solely on working with children with autism. I began a private practice that later became The Rebecca Center for Music Therapy, located in Rockville Centre, N.Y. There I had the opportunity to work with many children with PDD. It was during this time that I truly began to find myself as a music therapist. My clinical improvisational skills began to develop, as I felt myself beginning to internalize and integrate the knowledge acquired during my NRMT training. As my client base grew, so did my interactions with parents. My observation skills began to expand beyond the music room and into the reception area, as I paid close attention to how parents interacted with me and their children. I became more cognizant that my interactions with parents were influencing my sessions with their children. In addition, parent interactions also influenced how the children responded to me and my music and so on. NRMT had inculcated me with the notion that not just any music will do, and that the application and manipulation of the musical elements (tempo, rhythm, meter, melody, harmony, idioms, and styles) are carefully crafted clinical interventions. This way of thinking has become increasingly prevalent in my work today, as I recognize the importance of meeting and/or following the child (musically and emotionally) in the musical moment. The idea of musical experiences shifting in the moment to bring the child with autism into musical relatedness and

14

relationship, which in turn facilitates developmental growth, has taken on greater meaning for me when considering the core deficits of autism. The idea that autism is not a memory disorder, but a disorder that affects relating and communicating (G. Tippy, personal communication, July 8, 2007), has led me to believe that improvisation in NRMT directly deals with autisms core deficits. In NRMT, the child is responding or reacting to the improvised music in the context of an immediate and interactive relationship. In more structured and activity-based approaches using familiar songs, the child may tend to sing along, and this may work against the idea of relating and communicating, because the child may be relying on his memory skills to avoid here-and-now interaction. To that end, I have come to the conclusion that the focal point of my work is grounded in the idea of learning and communicating through relationships. This concept has led me to the work of Drs. Stanley Greenspan and Serena Weider and the Developmental, IndividualDifferences, Relationship-based model (DIR). In 2005, I was invited to develop a music therapy program at The Rebecca School in New York City. Rebecca, a private school, was slated to become the largest school for children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD) in the country, with 50,000 square feet of space and rooms including: several sensory gyms, playrooms, art and pottery rooms, a roof-top playground, rock climbing, and a music room just to name a few. The Rebecca School would be the first DIR School in New York City, and would be guided by Dr. Stanley Greenspan. This would be the missing link to my clinical work.

15

Developmental, Individual-Differences, Relationship-based Model (DIR)/Floortime DIR is a developmentally-based approach that is used as the foundation for a comprehensive intervention approach (Greenspan & Weider, 1998; 2006b). The focus of the model is to help children with special needs build healthy foundations for social, emotional, and intellectual capacities, rather than simply focusing on isolated behaviors. The term, Developmental, includes six levels of development that help the child build these capacities. The six levels of development are described below as affected by ASD in children of any age (Greenspan & Weider, 1998; 2006b): Level one: Shared attention and regulation occurs from age 03 months. An infant at risk for ASD may show difficulty in sustaining his attention to sights or sounds, and may prefer to engage in self-stimulatory behaviors. Level two: Engagement and relating occurs from 25 months. An infant at risk for ASD may show difficulty sustaining engagement and will usually withdraw from interaction and become self-absorbed. Level three: Purposeful emotional interactions occur from 410 months. An infant at risk for ASD may display no interest in interacting, or engage in brief back-and-forthexchanges with very little initiative and may engage in random or impulsive behaviors. Level four: Chains of back-and-forth (joint attention) emotional signaling and shared problem-solving occur from 1018 months. A child at risk for ASD will show an inability to initiate and sustain several back-and-forth interactions of emotional signals (e.g., showing mom or dad a toy) and may engage in perseverative behavior patterns.

16

Level five: Creating ideas occurs from 1830 months. A child at risk for ASD will have difficulty using words or phrases meaningfully and engaging in pretend play; he/she will repeat words of what has been heard or seen (echolalia).

Level six: Building bridges between ideas: Logical thinking occurs at 3042 months (e.g., I want to eat because Im hungry). A child at risk for ASD will display either no words, use memorized scripts with random ideas or use words and ideas illogically. Individual-Differences refer to how the child processes: information and language;

underlying motor and sensory capacities, such as touch, sound, and other sensations; auditory processing; visual-spatial processing; and motor-planning and sequencing abilities. For each of the six stages described above, the therapist needs to look at these particular individualdifferences of the child and determine how they interfere with the childs moving up the developmental ladder. This gives the clinician an integrated picture of the childs development. The term Relationships in the DIR model refers to how the child interacts with others (e.g., family members, teachers, therapists, and caregivers) and what patterns of interaction should be included in the therapeutic program to support enhanced development of the child. As can be seen, each component of the DIR model complements the other. First, it is important to understand at what level the child is functioning developmentally. Secondly, one must ascertain what stands in the way of a childs development and how the child is processing the world around himself or herself. To understand this, one must know how the child processes information and navigates in the world. Lastly, it is critical to know how the child relates to others in the world. Once there is a developmental picture and a sensory profile of the child, the

17

therapist can guide him/her into ways of interacting and relating that will provide the proper sensory input necessary to move up the developmental ladder. One of the main components of the DIR Model, which parallels NRMT, is Floortime (Greenspan & Weider, 2006a). Floortime is a systematic way of working with the child to help move him/her up the developmental ladder. Similarities between Floortime and NRMT are that they both involve improvisation, creativity, spontaneity, emotionality, and a playful spirit. The main difference is in the medium. NR therapy primarily involves the use of music, and Floortime primarily involves the use of objects, toys or sensory items. Described below are the five primary steps involved in Floortime (Greenspan & Weider, 1998; 2006b): 1) Observation: Both listening to and watching a child is essential (facial expressions, tone of voice, gestures, body posture, and use of or lack of words). 2) ApproachOpen circles of communication: Once assessing the childs mode of interacting or responding, the therapist can approach the child with the appropriate words, gestures, and affect. He or she can open the circle of communication with a child by acknowledging the child's emotionality, then elaborating and building on whatever interests the child at the moment. 3) Follow the child's lead: After the initial approach, following a child's lead simply means being a supportive play partner who is an "assistant" to the child and who allows the child to set the stage by directing the action, and creating dramas. 4) Extend and expand play: As one follows the child's lead, extending and expanding play themes involves making supportive comments about the child's play without being

18

intrusive, helping him/her to express ideas and move to ideas into different directions. (Asking questions to stimulate creative thinking may keep playful exchanges moving along, while also helping the child understand emotional themes involved). 5) Child closes the circle of communication: As the therapist opens the circle of communication when the child is approached, the child closes the circle when he/she builds on the therapists comments with comments of his own. One circle flows into another, and many circles may be opened and closed in quick succession as one interacts with the child. By building on each other's ideas and gestures, the child begins to appreciate and understand the value of two way communication. A Comparison of NRMT and DIR/ Floortime The moment that I began developing and implementing a music therapy program at Rebecca School, I immediately recognized the similarities between DIR and NRMT. The main difference in approaches involved the medium being used to engage and interact with the children. In traditional Floortime, the practitioner observes and follows the childs lead, using a variety of symbolic and sensory-based toys and objects (e.g., dolls, puppets, toys, swings, balls, etc.) to regulate and engage the child into a joint interactive experience. The goal is to help the child climb the developmental ladder. In NRMT, the primary therapist (music-maker), observes the child and follows his/her lead using music as the medium and object. The music being improvised attempts to create affect and emotionally-charged experiences intended to help the child regulate, musically engage, and interact in a joint musical relationship to facilitate development. Both are action-based approaches in which the child is an active and leading participant in the therapeutic process. Both approaches view relationships as a core component of

19

child development. Both focus on the creative process between the child and therapist. In addition, both require therapists to rely on their creativity, spontaneity, and playful spirit to help regulate and engage the child. Finally, both models respect the individual differences of each child and view whatever the child is doing as important (e.g., respecting idiosyncratic and selfstimulatory behaviors without attempting to extinguish them, but to embrace them and help make them interactive and communicative). In the present study, NRMT and DIR were used in tandem. NRMT was used as the primary treatment approach, and focused primarily on musical goals and the establishment of musical relationships between therapists and child. DIR was used as the primary means of conceptualizing and assessing the childs strengths and needs in nonmusical modes of interaction and relationship, and evaluating the childs progress in these areas. A combination of quantitative and qualitative data was collected, analyzed, and compared. The outcomes of therapy with each child were measured and evaluated quantitatively; the process of achieving these will be described and analyzed qualitatively. (See Method section.) Purpose of the Research The purpose of this study was to examine the process and outcome of NRMT within a DIR framework in meeting individualized goals of children with autism (DIR was the method of evaluating the child, and NRMT was the intervention used throughout the study). Four research questions were posed. 1) Will each child make significant progress in achieving the individualized musical goals formulated for the child in the NRMT sessions? Progress toward meeting NRMT-

20

specific goals will be measured using Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) (Kiresuk & Lund, 1974; 1978), an interdisciplinary approach to formulating individualized treatment goals and evaluating their accomplishment. (See Method Section.) 2) Will each child make significant progress in achieving the individualized nonmusical goals formulated for the child in the DIR model? Progress in meeting these goals will be measured using the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2003). (See Method section.) 3) What is the process by which goals for each child will be addressed by the therapists and accomplished by the child? 4) For each child, will progress in musical goals (NRMT) parallel progress in nonmusical (DIR) goals? As designed, this was the first quantitative study of individualized outcomes in NRMT with children with autism. In addition, it was the first study in the utilization of NRMT within a DIR/Floortime framework. The insights gained from this study may create interest in additional research, program development, funding opportunities and training programs using this collaborative approach.

21

CHAPTER 2 RELATED LITERATURE This review deals with five main topical areas found in the literature on individuals with autism. The main areas are: 1) research studies on the musical characteristics of individuals with autism; 2) clinical case studies on how music therapy is used with these individuals; 3) research studies on the efficacy of music therapy in treating individuals with autism; 4) clinical case studies on how DIR/ Floortime is used with children with autism; and 5) a study on the effectiveness of DIR/ Floortime in treating children with autism. The Musical Characteristics of Individuals with Autism A review of the literature on musical characteristics of individuals with autism reveals several empirical studies that have focused on tonal abilities and musical affinities. The research suggests that individuals with autism display strong abilities in pitch perception (Bonnel et al., 2003; Heaton, 2005), melody discrimination (Applebaum, Egel, Koegel, & Imhoff, 1979; Mottron, Peretz, & Menard, 2000), pitch recall (Heaton, Hermlin & Pring 1998), disembedding notes within chords (Heaton, 2003), and in improvising melodies (Thaut, 1988). Studies have also indicated that individuals with autism have a strong affinity for musical stimuli when compared to other auditory and visual stimuli (Blackstock, 1978; Thaut, 1987), and that they understand affective connotations in music (Heaton, Hermlin, & Pring 1999). These studies will be reviewed below in further detail. Tonal Abilities Bonnel et al. (2003) compared twelve high-functioning individuals with autism with twelve age-matched normal developing individuals on their ability to discriminate same-

22

different in the frequency of two pitches presented in pairs. The autistic group scored significantly higher in pitch frequency discrimination scores than the control group. In the same study, Bonnel et al. (2003) also compared twelve high-functioning individuals with autism with twelve age-matched typically developing individuals on their ability to judge whether a tone was higher or lower than the reference tone (1000-Hz tone) presented immediately beforehand. The results showed that the autistic group scored significantly higher in determining the direction of pitch than the age-matched control group. Heaton (2005) compared thirteen children with autism and two with Aspergers Syndrome with two age-matched typically developing groups on their ability to judge the pitch direction of different intervals. Fortyeight intervals ranging from a minor second (one semitone) through an octave (twelve semitones) were used in the experiment. The two notes of each interval were played successively, and participants were asked to quickly determine whether the second note of each interval was higher or lower then its starting pitch. The children with autism scored significantly higher than the typical children in correctly identifying the pitch direction of small intervals (14 semitones). There was no significant difference between groups in identifying pitch direction of medium (58 semitones) and large (912 semitones) intervals. Mottron, Peretz, and Menard (2000) compared thirteen individuals with autism to thirteen typically developing age-matched individuals on their ability to judge same-different in pairs of diatonic melodies. Both groups performed similarly in identifying melody pairs that were the same; however, the individuals with autism performed better than their counterparts in identifying melody pairs that were different.

23

Applebaum et al. (1979) compared the pitch abilities of normal developing children who had prior musical training, with children with autism who had no prior musical training. The children were asked to sing back tones and musical phrases ranging from one to four tones atonally configured. Tones were presented by voice, piano, and synthesizer. The study revealed that children with autism were able to sing back correct pitches as accurately or better then the age-matched typical children. Heaton et al. (1998) compared children with autism with typically developing children on their ability to recall pitches and speech sounds that were both paired with pictures of animals. Each participant was presented with four pitches, each paired with a picture, and on a separate occasion, four speech sounds, each paired with a picture. After a seven day period, the participants were presented with the previously presented pitches and asked to identify the matching picture. The same procedure was used to identify and recall speech sounds to their matching pictures. Both groups displayed similar performance levels when pairing speech sounds with pictures, whereas performance levels of pairing pitches with pictures were significantly higher in the group with autism. Heaton (2003) compared children with autism with typically developing children on their ability to identify missing tones within a chord structure. Each tone in a four-note chord was paired with a picture. The experimenter played three notes together, excluding one of the four tones forming a chord, and then asked each participant to point to the picture paired with the tone that was missing. The children with autism performed at a significantly higher level of accuracy than the typically developing children. Thaut (1988) compared children with autism with typically developing children

24

and adults with mental retardation on their ability to improvise a pentatonic melody on the xylophone. The improvisations were rated according to the following criteria: Rhythm: The extent to which the improvisation contained different rhythmical elements and the presentation of rhythmic sequences; Restriction: The extent to which available intervals and tones were used; Complexity: The number of tones that were included in a recurring melodic motive; Rule adherence: The total number of single tones in the entire tone sequences which belonged to a motive; and Originality: The presentation of melodic patterns that occur only once but fullfill certain criteria for complex melodic and rhythmic shape. The results revealed that the autistic childrens improvisations scored significantly higher than adults with mental retardation and approximated the scores of the typically developing children. The highest individual score was achieved by a child with autism. Music Preferences Thaut (1987) compared five children with autism and ten typically developing children on their perceptual preferences between visual stimuli (fifty colored slides of animals) and musical stimuli (10 minutes of instrumental music). Preference was measured by the length of time spent with each stimulus with the children pressing a button to access either the musical or visual stimuli. The study revealed that children with autism spent significantly more time with the musical stimuli than the typical children. Blackstock (1978) compared children with autism and typically developing children on their listening preferences for spoken and sung versions of songs. Participants were tested

25

individually and were given a choice between music consisting of excerpts of ten songs, and spoken words comprised of the same lyrics as the musical excerpts. The results showed that the children with autism preferred sung versions of songs, while the typically developing children showed no preference when given a choice between spoken and sung versions. Affect in Music Heaton et al. (1999) compared twelve children with autism and two with Aspergers Syndrome with two control groups of age-matched typically developing children on their ability to identify the affective connotations (sad or happy) of melodies in major and minor modes. Each participant was shown pictures of two faces (happy and sad) as they listened to twenty-four recorded melodies, four bars in length. Participants were then asked to indicate, either by pointing to one of the two pictures or by saying whether the music was happy (major mode) or sad (minor mode). The children with autism were not significantly different from typically developing counterparts in their ability to assign the musical examples to the two affective categories. Case Studies on Music Therapy and Autism A review of the case study literature on music therapy and autism reveals that therapists have used diverse methodological approaches. When working within a behavioral framework (Mahlberg, 1973; Starr & Zenker, 1998), therapists use music as a behavioral reinforcement to increase attention span and relatedness, develop communication and speech, and interrupt autistic behaviors. In the structured-activity approaches (Griggs-Drane & Wheeler, 1997; Wager, 2000), therapists utilize music instruction, pre-composed songs, and re-creative musical activities

26

to facilitate self-expression, fine and gross motor functions and listening skills, and to decrease aggressive behaviors. In the psychodynamic approaches (Lecourt, 1991; Levinge, 1990; Warwick, 1995; Woodward, 2004), therapists use music to facilitate musical and interpersonal relationships (including transference) to help clients develop a sense of self, enhance their creativity and selfexpression, and form an identity. In the improvisational approaches (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Berger 2003; Bryan, 1989; Saperston, 1973; Wimpory, Chadwick, & Nash, 1995), therapists employ a non-directive approach, using clinical improvisation on a variety of instruments, and movement with music to address issues involving: self-expression, body awareness, anxiety and aggression, sensory integration, socialization, communication, and the mother-child relationship. In the Nordoff-Robbins approach to improvisational therapy, therapists implement improvised music based on the clients musical and non-musical responses, reactions, and emotionality to develop a musical relationship. Typically, the goals are to enhance spontaneous communication, musical flexibility, musical relatedness, musical and interpersonal relationships, socialization skills, language development, and self-confidence. Behavioral Approaches Mahlbergs (1973) case study examined the use of music therapy with a six-year-old boy with autism who was prone to tantrums, did not pay attention to noises, had speech and language impairments, and displayed aggressive behaviors. Working within a behavioral orientation, Mahlberg had the child tap the rhythm of words on the tambourine to improve speech, clap to pre-composed songs to increase attention, and engaged the child in movement activities with

27

music to decrease maladaptive behaviors. Mahlberg found that music therapy was effective in accomplishing all targeted goals. Starr and Zenker (1998) presented case studies of three boys, aged four, five, and six. The therapists worked within a behavioral orientation using pre-composed music. The first case, Jeff, was four years old. The primary goal was to increase the use of first person pronouns. The therapist created a musical game involving the pre-composed song, Beat that Drum, which posed various questions for Jeff to answer. When answered correctly, Jeff was reinforced positively by giving him an opportunity to play the drum. The second case involved a five year old boy, Tom. The primary goal of music therapy was to increase his socialization skills. The therapist played some of Toms favorite childrens songs (pre-composed) on the piano, using hand-over-hand technique with Tom to increase his ability to share. The final case described was Bob, a six year old boy. The main goal was to decrease his level of anxiety over transitions. The therapist used a composed song, Line-Up, to increase Bobs understanding of waiting his turn before leaving the classroom. Starr and Zenker found that music therapy was effective in achieving all of their therapeutic goals. Structured Music Activity Approach Griggs-Drane and Wheeler (1997) reported on a case study with a blind female adolescent with autism. The therapist worked within a structured format using pre-composed and composed songs (live and recorded music) of various musical styles based on a functional assessment. A functional assessment is a method used to identify variables that influence challenging behaviors (e.g., over-stimulating environment, lighting, and sounds, as well as the function of these behaviors in children with autism). As such, it is concerned with: when the

28

behavior occurs, whether the behavior is linked to a specific activity, what triggers the behaviors, whether the behaviors stem from medical or physical issues, whether there are activities in which the behavior does not occur and whether the child has a daily routine. The music therapist uses the functional assessment to document the occurrence of challenging behaviors during musical selections. Musical selections, based on a functional assessment, were paired with prompts (e.g., objects, pictures, written words, verbal prompts) to enhance the clients ability to accomplish her goals. For example: the therapist paired verbal prompts, such as hands down, with selected music to address the clients aggressive behaviors. The writers found that functional assessment procedures combined with music therapy treatment were effective in decreasing aggressive behaviors toward self and others during the sessions. Wagers (2000) case study examined how an adult male with autism and mental retardation responded to individual music therapy during a four year period. Wager worked within a structured activity-based approach, focusing on musical instruction to achieve clinical goals. Musical activities included teaching the client: how to play drums to pre-recorded music, how to play familiar melodies on the piano, and musical terminology. She found that music therapy was effective in: providing the client with a joyful leisure activity (staff commented that the client consistently wanted to attend sessions, and that he focused his attention in music longer then in any other activity); developing the ability to listen; responding to and recalling music while playing drums or piano; facilitating fine and gross motor skills; helping to facilitate selfexpression; and providing the client with a means for affective expression.

29

Psychodynamic Approaches Levinges (1990) clinical case study examined how music therapy was used with a twoand-a-half-year-old girl during eighteen months of treatment. The study focused on the clients process in developing a personal identity through the musical relationship between client and therapist. Working within a psychodynamic model using improvisation, Levinge concluded that music therapy may be effective in guiding clients to the experience of a separate self wherein they can begin to define themselves and create an identity. Lecourts (1991) case study examined the use of psychoanalytically-based music therapy to treat a four-year old, nonverbal boy with autism. The author worked with him in individual music therapy one time per week for two years and three months. Initially, the focus of the work was to explore the clients relationships with his caregivers. Lecourt speculated that there was a connection between the childs autistic behaviors and difficulties in these relationships. She used improvised music, musical composition, pre-recorded sounds from previous sessions, and sound games. Emphasis was given to the organization of the clients experience of sounds and relationships within the dynamic of a transference relationship, defined as the processes of unconscious desires being realized in material or human objects within a specific relationship (Lecourt, 1991, p. 97). She found that psychoanalytical music therapy was effective in facilitating self-expression, creativity, relationship, and a transference relationship within the music. Warwicks (1995) case study describes the process of music therapy education and intervention in home-based, family treatment work with a six-year-old boy with autism and his mother. Warwick worked within a psychodynamic model and used clinical improvisation to

30

facilitate the musical and interpersonal relationship between child and mother. Emphasis was given to modes of relating, sharing and communicating. Warwick discovered that music therapy helped develop a musical relationship between mother and child, one in which the mother was able to take over the role of therapist and to continue treatment without the support of the therapist. Woodward (2004) conducted three clinical studies to examine how music therapy may be used to treat children with autism and their parents. The three studies included: a group of seven children (and a few parents), a four-year-old girl and her mother, and two brothers and their mother. Woodward worked within a psychodynamic developmental model using structured songs (e.g., familiar greeting and goodbye songs, various pre-composed songs chosen by the children and therapist) and musical games/activities (conducting game, movement songs chosen by the children, and start and stop musical games). Emphasis was given to the development of roles and relationships. Woodward found that music therapy was effective in: enhancing parentchild relationships, altering parents perceptions of their child, and providing parents with emotional support. Improvisational Approaches Alvin (1978) reported several case studies of children with autism in individual therapy. The goals were to develop self-expression, establish relationship, increase tolerance to sounds, develop communication, enhance body awareness, and decrease aggressive urges and anxiety. The studies included: a six-year-old girl, an eight-year-old boy; and an eleven-year-old-boy. Alvins model employed free improvisation in which the therapist does not impose any structure on the childs playing, but rather provides him/her with the opportunity and support to explore an

31

instrument on his/her own terms. The therapist plays a supporting role using receptive and active music therapy techniques adapted to the childs reactions to the music and his behaviors. In the case of Oliver, Alvin used receptive techniques initially, playing pre-composed and improvised music for him on the cello to help increase his tolerance and relationship to sound, while gradually increasing the repertoire and the length of each piece. Case Pamela illustrated the use of free improvisation on various instruments in which the child began to establish a relationship to sounds outside of herself (e.g., imitating, matching, and harmonizing sounds within the musical relationship). The third case presented, Kevin, utilized free improvisation, musical games and movement to music to establish a relationship, develop a musical identity, and decrease his aggressive urges. Case Martin, demonstrated how the cello (played by the child while being supported by the therapist) and movement activities were used to liberate Martin from his anxiety. Alvin found that free improvisational music therapy was effective in facilitating self-expression, a sense of self, relationship, tolerance to sounds, communication, body awareness, and a decrease in aggressive urges. Berger (2003) reported several brief case vignettes of children with autism to support her theoretical framework in which music therapy interventions are used to address sensory integration difficulties in children with autism. Goals are centered around the childs sensory processing disorders (e.g., vestibular, propreoceptive, tactile, visual and auditory systems). Berger worked primarily using improvisation, but also utilized pre-composed music, musical games, and movement to music activities. She utilized a range of instruments with various timbres including un-pitched and pitched percussion, string and wind instruments. Case Jason illustrates how the therapist structured sessions around Jasons auditory processing/tracking

32

difficulties to gain his attention. Although Jason was unable to tolerate loud sounds, he was fascinated with the sound of a gong after striking it with a mallet. The gong appeared to regulate Jason by balancing his auditory needs. The therapist carefully created music around the gong sounds, playing simple melodies on the recorder to draw Jasons attention. Ethan, a ten-year-old boy with perfect pitch and limited expressive language, screamed when a song was played in a key other then when it was first presented, or when words were spoken with various timbres. Music therapy interventions involved repeating sound sequences and responding to musical cues, presented instrumentally and vocally. The goal was to expand his tolerance for change in regard to musical elements within familiar songs. Bryans (1989) group case study involved six children with autism (1214 years old) who met weekly for a five month period. Bryan worked within a non-directive improvisational approach using a wide variety of pitched and unpitched percussive instruments including a piano. Bryan found that music therapy was effective in developing self-awareness and, in turn, in enabling group members to participate collectively, relate to each other, and be a part of something larger then themselves. Saperston (1973) reported on his work with an eight year old child with autism. The child participated in thirty-minute individual music therapy sessions three times per week. The therapist worked within an improvisational framework, creating music on the piano to match the childs responses and individual movements. As the client became increasingly aware of his ability to influence the music played, he began to smile, establish eye contact with the therapist, and vocalize more frequently. In addition, the client developed the ability to interact with people

33

outside of therapy sessions. Saperston found that improvisational music therapy can be an effective treatment intervention in developing communication skills in children with autism. Wimpory, Chadwick, and Nash (1995) conducted a study to examine the effects of music therapy on a three-year old child with autism and her mother. Music therapy consisted of twenty minute sessions two times per week for seven months. The therapist worked within an improvisational framework, using Musical Interaction Therapythe synchronization of live music to adult-child interactions, aimed at enabling the child to anticipate her parents actions on the basis of music synchronized to those actions. The childs development of social skills outside of music therapy was monitored by videotaping semi-structured settings in the home. The authors found that music therapy was effective in facilitating changes in social interactions, including a decrease in the amount of time it took the child to acknowledge her mother, an increase in eye contact, and an increase in initiating involvement with her mother. Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy Nordoff and Robbins (1964) presented three case studies involving the use of their own model (Nordoff-Robbins Music Therapy or NRMT). The three studies included: a six-year-old girl, a six-year-old boy, and a three-and-a-half-year old boy. In their model, one therapist improvises music on the piano based on what the child is doing or playing (using various percussive instruments), while the other encourages and supports the child in music making. Emphasis is given to the musical relationship between the client and therapist. Nordoff and Robbins found that improvisational music therapy was effective in establishing communication, developing relationship, speech development, and decreasing pathological behavior patterns.

34

Nordoff and Robbins (1977) provided an in-depth musical analysis, through text and audio session excerpts, to describe the progress of a five-and-a-half-year-old boy with autism, Edward. The therapists provided a musical emotional environment for Edward, matching and meeting his screaming and crying through musical improvisation. The music, being improvised by the therapist on the piano, began to bring Edwards emotional tantrum into two-way communication, enhancing the childs sense of self and others, which led to singing-crying responses that were musically related (e.g., in pitch, in rhythm and in melody patterns). The therapists found that clinical improvisation was effective in creating an appropriate emotional environment for the child in order to increase awareness, socialization, and communication. Brown (1994) reported five brief case studies of children with autism in individual NRMT. The goals were to enhance cognition, socialization, and emotionality. The five studies included: a twelve-year-old girl, an eight-year-old boy, a ten-year-old boy, and 2 thirteen-yearold boys. She found that NRMT was effective in facilitating: spontaneous expression (verbal, musical, and vocal), musical flexibility, musical relatedness, and, musical and interpersonal relationships. Howats (1995) case study involved a ten-year-old girl with autism who participated in individual NRMT for thirty minutes, one time per week. Emphasis was given to the use of improvised songs to facilitate purposeful singing. Howat found that NRMT was effective in enhancing social development, tolerance for change, self-expression, and the ability to sing freely and purposefully. Turry and Marcus (2003) examined the use of Nordoff Robbins Music Therapy (NRMT) with a group of four adults diagnosed with autism, aged twenty to twenty-three. Each session

35

was co-led by two therapists. One therapist led the group musically by improvising on piano. The other therapist, sitting within the group itself, was responsible for facilitating musical interaction with the therapist (music-maker) and among the group members. Following treatment, parents were interviewed for the purpose of determining any significant gains as a result of music therapy. They reported music therapy to be effective in motivating their child to attend sessions, in increasing self confidence in and outside of music therapy, and in increasing their ability to function in social situations. Outcome Studies In addition to case studies, several research studies deal with the effectiveness of music therapy in treating individuals with autism and two meta-analyses have been performed on the quantitative literature. These are reviewed below. Hollander and Juhrs (1974) examined the effectiveness of the Orff-Schulwerk method to increase socialization and communication and decrease perseverative behavior of thirty children with autism (ages four to sixteen). The Orff-Schulwerk method involves the use of music set to the tempo of body movements and speech to facilitate client responses. Various specialized percussive instruments, tuned to pentatonic scales, were available to the children for musical expression. Hollander and Juhrs taught the children rhythmic motions to replace perseverative movements, encouraged children to play with instruments to improve fine-motor coordination, and had children interact and imitate one another in groups of four to six to enhance social skills. They found that Orff-Schulwerk is an effective method in enhancing socialization and communication, while also providing children with autism a meaningful group experience.

36

Edgerton (1994) examined the effects of improvisational music therapy on musical and non-musical communication in children with autism. Eleven children with autism (69 years old) participated in individual music therapy for ten weeks. Edgerton devised the Checklist of Communicative Responses/Acts Score Sheet (CRASS) to evaluate communication responses of children with autism. The CRASS, composed of items from several rating scales and assessments dealing with communication and musical communicativeness, was used to evaluate communicative responses (verbal, vocal, gestural, and or musical events) and acts performed by the children. Edgerton worked within a Nordoff-Robbins model using improvisation. During sessions, the therapist improvised music on the piano while the child played snare drum, cymbal and other percussive instruments. The structure of the study consisted of the presentation of the music intervention followed by one session of withdrawal and then another music intervention. During the intervention, the therapist would improvise music with the child. During the withdrawal sessions, the therapist played precomposed music for the child. The CRASS scores of all eleven children consistently increased during intervention, decreased during withdrawal, and increased again during intervention. All individual scores on the CRASS were higher after the last session of treatment than after the first session. Orr, Myles, and Carlson (1998) studied the use of rhythmic entrainment to decrease headjerking and screaming in an eleven-year-old girl with autism. Entrainment was defined as the use of music to facilitate relaxation by externally produced rhythms that are designed to bring the body to its natural rhythmic patterns. Music at 5060 beats per minute was used to increase the alpha waves responsible for relaxation. Thirty sessions of data were collected utilizing an ABAB design, with alternating baseline and musical intervention periods. Researchers analyzed the

37

childs head-jerks and screaming during the first twenty minutes of the session during both conditions. Results showed that the average number of screams and head-jerks on days with music was fewer than that of baseline days. Brownell (2004) examined the effect of musical presentations of social stories on target behaviors of four children with autism between the ages of six and nine. Target behaviors for each of the four cases included: Case 1: delayed echolalia of movie and television media (scripting); Case 2: difficulty following directions; Case 3: the use of the clients loud voice, with the goal of reducing the frequency with which he used it; and Case 4: the clients use of his loud voice at inappropriate times. Brownell utilized pre-composed songs within a behavior modification approach. He created individualized social stories and composed music for each story, using the text as lyrics based on each target behavior. Reading and singing versions of the stories were alternately presented to each participant (Treatment conditions: baseline, reading social stories, and singing the social stories). The singing condition was significantly more effective in reducing target behaviors than the reading condition, suggesting that musically adapted social stories can be an effective intervention for modifying behaviors of children with autism. Allgood (2005) examined parents perceptions of a seven week family-based group music therapy intervention with children with autism (46 years old) and their parents. Parents participated in the pre-session interview, seven weekly sessions, and the post-session focus group following treatment. Allgood worked primarily within a structured-activity based approach using

38

pre-composed song as well as improvisation to promote joint attention, interaction, selfexpression, and collaboration. Data were collected through interview sessions with parents to assess their experience of the intervention. Parents reported positive responses to the music therapy treatment, such as identifying group cohesion and co-interaction among the group members, appreciating the bonding with other families, and developing a realization about their childs abilities and limitations. Kaplan and Steele (2005) analyzed data related to goals and outcomes over two years for forty clients, nine through forty-nine-years old, diagnosed with an autistic spectrum disorder. Primary goal areas were language/communication (41%), behavioral/psychosocial (39%), cognitive (8%), musical (7%), and perceptual motor (5%). One-hundred percent of the participants reached their initial objectives in these goal areas within one year or less. In addition, one-hundred percent of parents and caregivers surveyed indicated that participants generalized skills acquired in music therapy. Kern (2006) examined the effectiveness of a collaborative approach (with classroom teachers) using a musically-adapted playground and individualized composed songs to improve peer interactions in four boys with autism (35 years old). Teachers were each trained in music therapy principles and were asked to learn originally-composed songs written by a music therapist to include the specific goals for each of the four participants (e.g., improve turn-taking, increase choice making, etc.). Following the training, each child participated in four conditions: 1) baseline; 2) teacher walking the child through the musically adapted playground giving him drum sticks and asking him to play the instruments; 3) teacher entering the musical playground with the targeted child and a chosen peer and initiating interaction by singing the composed song

39

and playing the instruments; 4) teacher entering the playground with the target child and peer, without initiating interaction. Kern found that condition C (3) (teacher-mediated intervention) significantly increased positive peer interactions. Whipple (2004) conducted a meta-analysis of the use of music as an intervention for children and adolescents with autism. The review included ten experimental studies with individuals with autism ranging in ages two-and-a-half to twenty-one-years-old. The analysis examined the effects of music, including music therapy interventions and music versus no music on social interaction and behavior patterns. Results showed a significant effect size of d =77 and a mean correlation of r = .36 (p = .00). The significant effect size suggests that music interventions can be an effective treatment modality for children and adolescents with autism. Gold and Wigram (2005) also conducted a meta-analysis of the effects of music therapy for individuals with autism spectrum disorders. The review included three studies that examined the short-term effect of brief music therapy treatment interventions for children with autism. Results showed that music therapy intervention was superior to placebo therapy in regards to verbal and communicative skills (verbal: 2 Randomized control trials (RCT), n = 20, Standard mean deviation (SMD) .36 Confidence interval (CI) .15 to .57; gestural: 2 RCTs, n = 20, SMD 0.50 CI .22 to 0.79). Clinical Case Studies on DIR/Floortime and Autism The literature on DIR/Floortime is sparse. An extensive literature review reveals that there are no published outcome studies on the effectiveness of the model. There are, however, anecdotal reports from case studies that reveal that the DIR /Floortime model may be an effective intervention for individuals with autism.

40

A review of the case study literature of DIR/Floortime reveals how the model is used with children with autism. Greenspan and Weider (2006b) presented three case studies of children with autism: two boys aged two-and-a-half, three, and three-and-a-half; and a threeyear-old girl. The first case, David, a two-and-a-half-years-old, presented as self-absorbed, perseverative, and given to self-stimulation. The goal for David was to develop the ability to engage in two-way communication by strengthening the childs auditory visual processing capacities. Floortime, an intervention that involves spontaneous play of following the childs lead using affectively toned interactions through gestures and words, was used. The floortimer transformed the childs perseverative interest in cars into elaborate symbolic dramas in which the child became the expert on brands and models. Floortime was used to spontaneously explore and practice new emotional themes and abstract thinking. The second case involved a three-year-old boy, Joey. He was extremely avoidant, pulling away from relationships, and displaying perseverative and self-stimulatory behaviors. Joey relied on visual stimuli to process the environment and had an incredible memory for places and things. The goals for Joey were to expand pleasurable interactions and establish a continuous flow of interactions through affect cuing and problem-solving. The floortimer utilized characters from Joeys favorite shows and books. Initially scripting, Joey began to slowly engage in spontaneous back-and-forth continuous flow of interaction through re-creating the stories into real life experiences. The third case described is Sarah, a three-and-a-half-year-old, whose pattern of behavior included flight and avoidance after getting what she wanted, followed by not knowing what to do next. The primary goal of Floortime was to increase Sarahs capacity for continuous flow of

41

affective interactions. Although Sarah had weak receptive processing, she demonstrated the ability to understand through visual and affect cues, which transferred into the use of puppet play. Puppets opened the door to role-playing, in which Sarah took the part of a cook or doctor, increasing a continuous flow of interaction between her and the floortimer. Weider and Greenspan (2003) examined the use of DIR/ Floortime with a two-and-ahalf-year-old boy, Joey, who was withdrawn and self-absorbed. Joey did not respond to his name or appear to understand words when spoken to him. The goals were to: deepen Joeys relationship with his dad (Joey enjoyed rough-housing with dad; dad became his toy), and to help him develop the ability to mutually attend and engage. Joeys intervention plan included: six daily Floortime sessions at home, four semi-structured and sensory-motor activities, intensive speech and occupational therapies, three to five weekly play dates, inclusion in a preschool, and various music, gym and sports activities. Through rough housing, dad (as the floortimer) helped Joey develop functional developmental capacities. For example, mutual attention and engagement were facilitated through affect cuing, getting Joey to initiate and communicate what he wanted dad to do. By waiting and being playfully obstructive, dad was getting Joey to elaborate on getting more out of the rough-house play in addition to wooing him into more complex gestures (e.g., Joey would initiate and grab dads hands, look at him, pull and figure what was going to happen next). Weider and Greenspan found that the DIR intervention helped develop a comprehensive treatment plan that enhanced the relationship between Joey and his dad, and developed his capacities for mutual attention and engagement.

42

Effects of DIR/Floortime with Children with Autism Greenspan and Weider (1997) reviewed the charts of two-hundred children with autism, ranging in ages from twenty-two months to four years, to examine the effectiveness of the DIR/floortime intervention. Each child participated in DIR/ Floortime for two to eight years. The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale was used to assess functional developmental levels of emotional, social, and cognitive at the initial evaluation and at each follow-up visit. The initial assessment indicated the following: 5% of the children showed no affective engagement, 31% displayed intermittent engagement, 40% demonstrated intermittent engagement and some reciprocity, 24% showed intermittent engagement and reciprocity and islands of symbolic capacity, and all of the children displayed an inability to maintain extended chains of reciprocal interaction. In addition, all of the children demonstrated severe impairments in auditory processing and motor-planning. With regard to reactivity to sensation, 39% of the children showed to be underreactive, 19% displayed hyperactivity, and 36% demonstrated mixed reactions. Floortime, a systematic relationship-based approach tailored to the childs individual differences and developmental level, was used. Outcomes were put into three categories: 1) Good to outstanding (evidenced by joyful relating, preverbal gestures, ability to problem-solve and engage in purposeful engagement, and engage in pretend play); 2) Medium (evidenced by the ability to relate, communicate with gestures, engage in reciprocal affective cueing and interactions). Children in this category displayed significant challenges in developing symbolic capacities); and 3) Continuing significant difficulties (evidenced by impairments in their ability to attend, enter into simple gesturing, displaying self-absorption, self-stimulation, and perseveration). The chart review revealed the effectiveness of DIR/ Floortime

43

intervention as follows: 58% of the children were in the good to outstanding group; 25% were in the medium group; and 17% were in the continuing significant difficulties group. Implications for the Present Study The literature on the musical characteristics of individuals with autism demonstrates how responsive they are to music and especially to its tonal components. This, in turn, suggests that music therapy can be a particularly appropriate method of therapy. The clinical case study literature demonstrates that music therapy has, in fact, been used effectively in accomplishing myriad goals relevant to the developmental growth of individuals with autism. While various approaches to music therapy have been used in these clinical studies, the majority of cases involved improvisation, and NRMT in particular. The clinical case study literature and chart review demonstrates that the DIR/Floortime has been used effectively in achieving developmental goals based on the FEAS of individuals with autism. The present study sought to determine the effectiveness of NRMT in meeting musical goals specifically established for each individual child, and to conclude if progress in musical goals paralleled progress in non-musical (DIR) goals. Thus, the present study was similar to the case study literature in its focus on meeting the unique therapeutic needs of each client. In comparing the quantitative outcome studies in the literature to the present study, only the Edgerton study used improvisation in the NRMT tradition; no other outcome studies have tested the effects of individual NRMT. In addition, none of the outcome studies individualized clinical goals for each participant, as was done in the present study; instead all of the studies employed the same measure to evaluate outcomes across all participants. The present study, then,

44

was the first to quantitatively assess the efficacy of NRMT in meeting individualized goals, as measured by Goal Attainment Scaling. No other study has utilized NRMT within a DIR framework, and no other study has compared musical and nonmusical outcomes. The present study was also unique in combining quantitative and qualitative methods of data collection and analysis. It evaluated outcomes quantitatively, and analyzed the process in reaching these outcomes qualitatively.

45

CHAPTER 3 METHOD Participants Four participants enrolled in the Rebecca School in New York City were selected by the school psychologist for participation in the study, based on the following criteria: 1) a diagnosis of an Autistic Spectrum Disorder (ASD), as specified in the Diagnostic Statistical Manual IV (American Psychiatric Association [DSM-IV-TR] 2000); 2) newly enrolled at the therapeutic day-school (Rebecca School), where the researcher is employed as a music therapist; and 3) no prior experience in music therapy. All were expected to have a wide range of neurodevelopmental disorders in relating and communicating. A parent or guardian of each participant was asked to sign a consent form for their childs participation in the study. The researcher would have informed the parents or guardians if their child demonstrated an intolerance for participation in the study. The parent or guardian would have been free to stop the childs participation in the study at any time with no consequences for the childs program. All personal information and data were kept strictly confidential; pseudonyms were used to identify the data, and real names would not be used in any publications that might result from the study. The study was reviewed by the Institutional Review Boards of the Rebecca School (place of study) and Temple Universitys Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. Design A single-case design was used to examine the processes and outcomes of four individual case-studies. The single-case design is an individually based method and treatment design that is used to study isolated cases. According to Hilliard (1993), single case research can be viewed as

46

a subclass of intrasubject research in which aggregation across cases is avoided, and the generality of ones findings is addressed through replication on a case-by-case basis (pp. 373 374). In short, each participant acts as his/her own control, whereby the researcher is focusing on the unique process of therapeutic change and the unique outcomes for each individual participant. In studying the process of change, the researcher takes into account beginning and ending points in therapy, as well as the form of the function between these points (Greenberg, 1986a, p.4). Thus, a separate case study is presented on the researchers music therapy work with each of the four participants. Each case study consists of a qualitative description of the music therapy process (what the therapist did, how the participant responded, how therapy unfolded, how therapist and child approached goals), and a quantitative evaluation of its therapeutic outcomes. The process is described through narrative and DVD. The quantitative measures of outcomes include: the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS), and Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). Measures The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) was used as a pre- and post-test measure of each childs progress in achieving goals pertinent to DIR. The FEAS is a standardized, age-normed assessment tool developed by Greenspan and DeGangi (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001) that can be applied to videotaped interactions between children with autism and their caregivers (Solomon, Necheles, Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007). The FEAS provides a framework for observing and assessing a childs emotional-social functioning in the context of the relationship with his/her caregiver, assessing the child in six areas of social-emotional

47

development: (1) regulation and interest in the world, (2) forming relationships (attachment), (3) intentional two-way communications, (4) development of a complex sense of self, (5) representational capacity and elaboration of symbolic thinking, and (6) emotional thinking or development and expression of thematic play(Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001). (See Appendix A.) The FEAS has been validated on four samples of infants and children ranging in ages from seven to forty-eight months: (1) 197 typical developing children; (2) 190 children with a regulatory disorder; (3) 41 children with pervasive developmental disorders; and (4) 40 children from multi-problem families and with drug exposure in utero (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). Construct validity was shown by comparing scores obtained by normative and clinical samples of young children using discrimination index, t-tests, and analysis of variance (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). In addition, an interobserver reliability study was conducted on the FEAS on forty-six children among five observers. The alpha coefficients between pairs of observers viewing between 1546 videotaped caregiver-child interactions ranged from .8961 to .9196 for the caregiver scale and .9119 to .9786 for the child scale. The alphas between a pair of observers viewing 15 interactions, one coding the interactions live and the other a videotape of the interactions, were .83 for the caregiver scale and .89 for the child scale, and .88 for the total child scores(Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). In the present study, the caregiver was the researcher who acted as the therapist in working with each child. The researcher has had extensive training in the DIR model, and is an experienced and certified NR therapist. The rater was the school psychologist at the Rebecca School, who is experienced in scoring the FEAS.

48

The FEAS is divided into two sections (caregiver and child) with six subtests in each section corresponding to six levels of social and emotional development: (1) regulation and interest in the world, (2) forming relationships, (3) intentional two-way communications, (4) development of sense of self, (5) capacity of symbolic thinking, and (6) emotional development and expression of thematic play (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). According to Greenspan and DeGangi (2007), typically developing children achieve early two-way communication by seven to nine months, correlating to Functional Developmental Levels (FDL) one to two. FDL three is achieved between ten and twelve months, FDL four between thirteen to eighteen months, FDL five from twenty-five to thirty-five months and FDL six between three to four years of life (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001). Scoring is based on a 2-point scale for most items, except where indicated. The scorer identifies three possible outcome levels (0, 1, and 2) for all areas within each of the six levels of core capacities (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001). The FEAS scored the child on their mastery of the skill as follows: 0 = behavior is not seen or is briefly observed (skill not mastered). 1 = behavior is present some of the time or observed several times (skill partially mastered). 2 = behavior is consistently observed or observed many times (skill mastered). The ratings can be summed to obtain subtest scores for the caregiver and for the child, as well as total test scores (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). In order to determine if a child or a caregiver is at risk (below cut-off scores), the total scores are compared to cutoff scores of a given category (cutoff score categories include: normal, at-risk, and deficient). In the present

49

study, total scores of the child on the pre-and post-tests of the FEAS, administered at session 1 and 26, were compared to assess the amount of the childs progress in each capacity. Musical outcomes were evaluated through Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994). The GAS consists of a set of musical goals established for each child by the researcher and an independent observer (e.g., an experienced, board-certified, NordoffRobbins therapist), following the music therapy assessment, along with five possible outcome levels for each goal. The outcome levels consisted of a 5-point continuum, ranging from the most unfavorable possible outcome to the most favorable possible outcome. Thus, each child had a set of individualized musical goals, which had been operationally defined in terms of five levels of outcomes, and had been weighted for relative significance for the childs therapeutic program. At the end of session 25, an independent observer (an experienced, board-certified, Nordoff-Robbins therapist) evaluated the childs progress by selecting the outcome level on each childs GAS that best described his or her musical responses in the session, as compared to the original evaluation. When the child made significant progress during treatment, goals and outcome scales were adjusted accordingly for the next period. See Data Analysis for statistical procedures used to determine the significance of clinical progress. Procedures Each child received a total of 26 sessions. Two sessions involved pre- and post-testing on the FEAS, and the other 24 sessions consisted of NRMT. Each NRMT session lasted 5-30 minutes and was dependent upon on the childs tolerance. The sessions were given twice per week, on Mondays and Wednesdays.

50

In the first session, each child participated in a pre-test FEAS, facilitated by the researcher, who has had extensive training in DIR/Floortime Model. The researcher used the following five steps of Floortime: 1) observation, 2) opening circles of communication, 3) following the child's lead, 4) extending and expanding play, and 5) facilitating the child in closing circles of communication. The pre-test FEAS was videotaped and then scored by a rater (school psychologist experienced in rating FEAS). Following the FEAS scoring, the researcher reviewed records at the school to gather information about each participant pertinent to formulating initial goals and treatment plans for music therapy treatment. In addition, the researcher interviewed each participants parents (mother, father, or legal guardian), teachers, and therapists. Based on this information, the researcher established preliminary therapeutic goals pertinent to FEAS and NRMT. In sessions 2 and 3, each child received NRMT sessions conducted by the researcher and co-therapist. The co-therapist was a music therapy intern who had been working at the school under the supervision of the researcher, and was therefore knowledgeable of both NRMT and DIR. Following the first two treatment sessions, 46 individualized goals were created for each child based on his/her performance on the FEAS and clinical observations. An independent observer (an experienced, board-certified, Nordoff-Robbins music therapist) assisted the researcher in creating 46 goals for each child. The researcher then incorporated these goals into the format of Goal Attainment Scaling by specifying levels of outcomes and weighting their therapeutic significance. Therapy continued bi-weekly for sessions 4 through 13, according to the established goals. At the end of the 25th session, the childs progress in meeting NRMT goals (GAS) was

51

evaluated by the independent observer. At the 26th session the FEAS was administered again by the researcher, as a post-test measure of child progress. Again, the rater of the post-test was the school psychologist. Table 1 shows the sequence of pre- and post-test measures and their timing in relation to treatment. Sessions were scheduled twice a week on Mondays and Wednesdays. Table 1 Sequence of Measures and Treatment Session/s 1 Treatment Researcher administered the FEAS (pre-test) to the child while implementing Floortime techniques. School psychologist (rater) viewed video-recording of the FEAS and scores the results. Music therapy treatment began with the primary therapist (researcher), cotherapist and child (clinical assessment and formulation of goals) After two music therapy treatment sessions, and before the third treatment session, 46 goals were formulated for each child by the researcher and independent observer (e.g., experienced board-certified NR therapist). Each childs progress was evaluated following music therapy treatment session twenty-five by the independent observer (an experienced, boardcertified, NR music therapist). . Researcher administered the FEAS (post-test) to the child while implementing Floortime techniques. School psychologist (rater) viewed video-recording of the FEAS and scored the results.

2-3 4

25

26

All sessions were video-recorded. The researcher viewed the video for each session given prior to the participants subsequent session. This was done so that significant responses of the child could be noted and musical ideas that elicited responses could be transcribed for use in subsequent sessions. Session videos were transferred to the researchers portable hard drive and stored on the researchers personal home computer. All data stored on the computer were protected and secured by a password that was known only to the researcher. The researcher also kept a clinical journal on each participant. The clinical journal included brief descriptions of each 52

session, including: what the therapists did, how the child responded, and what happened from a therapeutic standpoint. The computer files for these journals were stored in the researchers personal home computer; printed copies were stored in a locked file cabinet at the researchers home. Only the researcher had access to the cabinet. All written materials on each subject generated through the FEAS and GAS were stored in a locked file cabinet at the researchers home. Only the researcher had access to this cabinet. All electronic data generated for each subject were stored in the researchers home computer, which was password protected to provide access only to the researcher. The researcher conducted one-hour, weekly supervision for the co-therapist specifically to support the clinical work with each child, such as to review treatment planning, and therapeutic process. Relationships Throughout this study the researcher played multiple roles, such as researcher, clinician, and clinical supervisor. In order to adhere to the highest standards of intellectual honesty and integrity within this research study while also ensuring the integrity of the clinical work with each child, the researcher participated in peer supervision with the independent observer (e.g., an experienced, board-certified, Nordoff-Robbins music therapist) during weeks three, six, nine, and twelve). Materials and Equipment The outcomes particular to NRMT (e.g., musical outcomes) were formulated through the actual NRMT sessions, following the music therapy assessment (first 2 sessions). The music

53

therapy assessment, guided by Greenspan and Weiders Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL) (2006a), focused on seven areas of musical responsiveness: 1. Musical Awareness: the child responds or reacts in a reflexive or intentional manner related to any of the musical elements being offered. 2. Musical Relatedness: the child exhibits the ability to engage musically in an intentionally and related manner to the therapists music. 3. Relationship within Musical Play: the child exhibits an emotional interest in connecting with the music and therapist, based on the childs own initiative. 4. Music Interresponsiveness: the child exhibits the ability to imitate or copy a musical idea, and then incorporate it into the musical play with therapist 5. Musical Communicativeness: the child exhibits the ability to open (initiate a musical idea) and close circles of musical communication (end or complete a musical phrase) during musical play; including the ability to engage in call-and-response inter-play. 6. Musical Interrelatedness: the child exhibits the ability to connect his/her musical idea with the therapists idea, and then elaborate on it during musical play. 7. Musical Expressiveness: the child exhibits the ability to play using a range of musical expressiveness (e.g. dynamics, tempo, etc.) during musical play. All of the sessions were conducted in the schools music therapy treatment room, approximately 500-square feet in size. The room was equipped with a video network system which was used to record all sessions. The musical instruments used were from the current music therapy instrument inventory at the school; they included various pitched and unpitched percussion instruments (e.g., drums,

54

tambourines, cymbals, maracas, xylophones, marimbas, and a variety of sizes of drum sticks and mallets), an acoustic piano, an electric keyboard, and an acoustic and electric guitar. Data Analysis The FEAS pre-test ratings for each child were added together for subtest scores, as well as a total test score. Pre- and post subtest and total scores were compared for each child to determine if progress has been made in each subgroup as well as in the total test score (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). The GAS system includes a statistical procedure for analyzing the extent to which the participant is meeting all of the individually weighted goals. GAS emphasizes both the idiographic approach to measuring the uniqueness of the individual, while also arriving at nomothetic conclusions about these individuals (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillio, 1994). Although GAS was originally developed to evaluate mental health treatment outcomes, it has been used in many disciplines including education, rehabilitation, medicine, corrections, nursing, chaplain training, social-work, and chemical dependency (1994). At the end of the study, the researcher and independent observer used the raw score conversion key for GAS, developed by Cardillo (1994), to convert the outcome scores that best described each childs progress on his or her goals. Raw scores were converted to T-scores with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (Kiresuk & Lund, 1976; 1978). A standardized score of 50 represented an acceptable outcome; a score of 60 and above indicated that the childs progress has exceeded expectations, while a score of 40 or below indicated that the child had not achieved an acceptable outcome.

55

Qualitative data on process and outcomes were used to confirm, illuminate and expand upon pre- and post-data comparisons from the FEAS and GAS. Two questions were posed in analyzing the qualitative data and its relationship to the quantitative data: 1) are the qualitative data and quantitative data consistent with one another? 2) to what extent do the qualitative data illuminate or explain the quantitative data? The researcher then used his findings to write a case study on each participant. The researcher also created a video documenting each case study.

56

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS: MATTHEW History Reportedly, Matthew was the product of an uncomplicated, full-term pregnancy and Csection delivery. He weighed 6 lbs., 8 oz. at birth and no complications were indicated. Developmental milestones were met within normal limits by age two; however speech was delayed. Matthew was three years-old when he was diagnosed with Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) and speech apraxia. In addition, testing revealed a Mental Development Index of <50 on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, suggesting a rating of Significantly Delayed Performance. On the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Matthew obtained an Adaptive Composite score of 57, at the Low Level. All other domains were also in the low range except motor skills, which was within the Moderately Low range. Currently, Matthew resides with his mother, his maternal grandparents, aunt, and cousin. He sees his father occasionally. Prior to entering Rebecca School, Matthew received occupational and speech therapies three times per week, and Applied Behavioral Analysis (ABA) 20 hours per week at home and at school. Matthew has been attending the Rebecca School since September 2007. He has been a participant in individual music therapy since December 2007, two times per week for 30 minutes. During this time Matthew had also been receiving occupational, speech, and physical therapies 5 times per week, individually for 30 minutes.

57

DIR Profile The following DIR profile is based on 1) pre-test scores of the FEAS (See table 1); 2) interviews of staff; 3) and observations of Matthew in various settings throughout the school day, including the classroom, occupational therapy, and speech therapy: Matthew presents as an active, sensory seeking child. He has difficulty maintaining selfregulation (ability to be calm and available to interact) and shared attention with the caregiver (level I), and displays intermittent capacities of engagement and relating (level II) for brief moments. He seeks movement, and is in constant motion, which appears to be a strategy to selfregulate when exposed to an over stimulating environment, demonstrating his need for vestibular input. Matthew displays with islands of capacities in his ability to engage in two-way purposeful communication (level III), however, he has difficulty sustaining this due to an inability to maintain self-regulation and engagement for extended periods. His intent to communicate is often demonstrated through nonverbal gestures as well as attempts at word approximations. In addition, Matthew appears to have difficulty processing and filtering auditory stimulation, and will melt down in an aggressive manner (e.g., biting, pinching, hitting, etc.) when the environment is over-stimulating. Matthew seeks out tactile sensory input, such as play-doh and will often bring it to his mouth, demonstrating his need for oral stimulation. Matthew enjoys heavy massage on his hands and feet and deep pressure through his knees and feet. These techniques have been used in order to regulate him during high states of arousal. When over-aroused, Matthew will exhibit

58

aggressive behaviors such as pinching and head butting staff and therapists. These behaviors are also exhibited when Matthew becomes frustrated in his inability to communicate. Music Therapy Assessment The music therapy assessment, guided by Greenspan and Weiders Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL) (2006a), focused on seven areas of musical responsiveness (see Materials in Method Chapter). The music therapy room was set up with a variety of percussive and melodic instruments, an assortment of different size mallets with various handle textures, pitched horns, and a variety of sensory based items (e.g., scarves, play-doh, mini-trampoline, etc.). The assessment focused on Matthews ability to interact during musical play with the therapist. Matthews assessment took place during the course of two 30-minute sessions. Clinical improvisation was used during the assessment in that the therapist improvised music built around Matthews emotionality and his behavioral responses and reactions to the music. During the assessment, Matthews primary mode of interacting was through vocalizations. Due to his tactile sensitivities, he displayed difficulty holding mallets for extended periods, but occasionally played the tambourine and other hand-played drums when prompted for brief periods (1 to 2 measures). When Matthew was unable to be emotionally calm and available for engagement (dysregulated), he exhibited a low level of musical awareness. He occasionally presented reflexive tonal responses during bouts of intense crying and screaming, while covering his ears with his hands (this may have been a sign of auditory over-load to his sensory system).

59

His inability to maintain self-regulation inhibited Matthew from engaging in a musically related or communicative manner for extended periods. In addition, he exhibited no desire to initiate musical or interpersonal contact with the therapist, and would attempt to climb on window ledges, the piano and large drums, before attempting to leave the room in an aggressive and agitated state. When Matthew was able to self-regulate, his level of musical awareness and relatedness increased. During these moments, he demonstrated his ability to imitate and fill-in the ends of musical phrases (closing circles of communication) through both nonverbal and verbal (word approximations) singing, prompted by musical cues (displaying receptive language skills, and sequencing ability). In addition, during moments of musical relatedness, through vocalizing, Matthew displayed islands of capacities in the areas of musical interrelatedness and communicativeness; however he showed a limited ability to expand on the therapists musical ideas, or to initiate musical ideas (open circles of musical communication). The assessment indicated that Matthews main areas of difficulty were in his inability to maintain self-regulation (ability to be calm and available for engagement) and musical engagement due to: his difficulties with language processing (specifically expressive); a mixed reactive sensory system (hyper- and hypo- sensitive to sensory stimuli); and auditory processing difficulties. To that end, Matthews sensory integration and language processing difficulties appeared to limit his ability to display musical awareness and the ability to use music in a related and/or inter-responsive manner. Although he displayed islands of capacities in his ability to

60

engage and relate in musical play, his self-regulatory challenges kept him from experiencing a continuous flow of musical communication for extended periods. Following the assessment sessions, the researcher and independent observer formulated a Goal Attainment Scale (GAS) for Matthew (see Appendix A). Treatment Matthews treatment consisted of twenty-five sessions over a five month period. During this time, Matthew passed through four separate stages. In the first stage, he exhibited selfregulatory challenges which impeded his ability to: enter and remain in the music room, engage and relate during musical play. In the second stage, in providing Matthew with sensory input to facilitate self-regulation, he began to increase his ability to engage and relate during musical play. During stage three, the relationship (interpersonal and musical) began to develop as Matthew began to open (initiate) and close circles of musical communication during musical play. Finally, in stage four, Matthews ability to engage in intentional and reciprocal musical interactions, while displaying causal thinking during musical play, began to develop. Stage One: Whats Getting in the Way? Developing the Playing Field While Considering Individual-Differences Stage one comprised of sessions one through six. This stage was considered a period of getting to know each other. Generally, sessions were very similar to the assessment sessions. He entered each session distressed and dysregulated and usually either leaned on the wall (near the door), attempted to climb on something, or tried to hide behind something. His crying and screaming were extremely intense. Each session ended with Matthew desperately wanting to leave the room, which he eventually did in a dysregulated manner.

61

Musically, I initially attempted to improvise music on the piano, around his meltdowns, trying to meet the intensity of the crying in order to facilitate musical contact and relatedness. There were moments when Matthews crying and screaming became increasingly related to my improvised music, tonally and sequentially. Although he was clearly displaying musical awareness, his responses were reflexive and did not demonstrate any intentionality in regards to relating to my music. Although the musical relationship, thus far, had been based on Matthew being resistive (Nordoff & Robbins, 2007) to the music-making process, our musics (sounds) were related. Looking at the interaction through the lens of the DIR Model, Matthew had difficulty maintaining self-regulation, which prevented him from engagement. Although musically we were tonally connected, the music was fueling his melt-downs by overloading his auditory system. In addition, by solely focusing on music, I may have lost sight of his sensory needs. The plan for subsequent sessions was to begin developing clinical parameters and boundaries in regards to time (length of session), pacing (e.g., when and how to transition into other music; the use of silence, and gauging the rhythm of the interaction), and sensory input from the environment. In addition, it would be important for Matthew to leave each session in a regulated manner and avoid a power struggle over when to leave music. To that end, sessions were shortened, (5-10 minutes). Pacing, in terms of when to start and end improvisations was controlled by Matthews affect, emotionality, and the quality and rhythm of the musical interaction. Finally, the sensory environment was altered to cater to Matthews individual differences, including his auditory processing, tactile sensitivity and vestibular system (sensory system located in the inner ear that allows us to maintain balance and process movements).

62

Stage Two: Integrating Matthews Sensory & Musical Profiles Stage Two, sessions seven through twelve, consisted of balancing Matthews sensory profile with added musical stimuli to facilitate self-regulation and engagement. To do that, I incorporated a rolling chair, which spins around, to provide him with vestibular input. In addition, I shut off the rear-room lights to cater to his hypersensitivity to light, and brought playdoh into the room, in case he required tactile input. Musically, due to his auditory processing difficulties, I paid close attention to the register, dynamics, chord voicings, and touch while I played the guitar and/or piano and voice. DVD Cut 1: Pirouetting into Musical Awareness & Relatedness Cut 1 is taken from Matthews seventh session. It illustrates the beginning of our musical relationship as Matthew enters the music room on his own initiative for the first time, displaying musical awareness, relatedness and engagement through movement. Before the session begins, Matthew is being escorted to the music room by two teachers assistants (TAs). He is very resistant to entering the music room as he pinches and attempts to bite one of the TAs while sitting on a rolling chair outside of the room. After 15 minutes of trying to coax Matthew into the room, Jean (intern) begins to slowly spin him in the chair. As Matthew is slowly being spun on the chair, outside of the music room, I begin to accompany the spinning chair on the piano. The music attempts to create a circular motion through sound as I harmonize a repeated melodic sequence of three notes (triplets). As the music continues to play, Matthew stands from the chair and begins to pirouette into the room. This is the first time that he enters the room on his own initiative, exhibiting self-regulation, musical awareness, and engagement.

63

Stage Three: Developing the Musical Relationship: Initiating Circles of Musical Communication Stage Three consisted of sessions thirteen through sixteen, and continued to follow the strategy of providing Matthew with the necessary sensory input to maintain self-regulation and engagement in intentional communication within the context of a musical relationship. During this stage, Matthew continued to develop his ability to be musically responsive (vocally) in a related and communicative fashion. The duration of each musical interaction also began to expand, and our relationship, both musically and interpersonally was developing. In addition, sessions began to increase in length (between 20-30 minutes). The focal point within his ability to be musically communicative was that he was exhibiting his ability to initiate circles of musical communication (initiating and completing musical ideas in tandem with the therapist). In addition, intentional and reciprocal musical interactions and the ability to understand cause-and-effect relationships within musical play, began to emerge at this stage. DVD Cut 2: Rockin for Musical Communicativeness Cut 2 from session fifteen illustrates how the music transforms Matthewss isolated activity of rocking into a joint interactive musical experience facilitating musical engagement and two-way purposeful communication. Sessions fifteen and sixteen take place in Matthews classroom while the rest of the class had gone to gym. Prior to each session, his teacher tells Jean and me that he is having a difficult day and that it will be hard to get him out of the classroom. In addition, she asks if she could observe the session and provide support if needed; I reply positively.

64

The excerpt begins with Matthew being seated on a padded chair that he rocks forward and backward. He begins to rock, providing himself with sensory input, while at the same time engaging in an isolating activity. I begin to improvise on the guitar, using the tempo of Matthews rocking motion and develop a non-verbal melody. Occasionally he acknowledges the connection between my music and his rocking through eye-contact. While I play I raise my right hand in front of him for him to hit, transforming it into a melodic/percussive instrument by accenting specific melodic notes for each time that he hit my hand. After several minutes into this musical interaction he appears to be getting distressed and say, no, no, no. I proceed to play the guitar with the same harmonic structure, basing the rhythm and tempo on his rocking, while gradually moving away from him. During this interaction, the rocking chair provides Matthew with a form of proprioceptive and vestibular input, while the hand slapping provids tactile support. This moving in tandem with musical stimuli facilitates an increase in his level of musical interaction, specifically in the area of musical communicativeness. In addition, Matthew is also experiencing a wide range of affects within the back-and-forth of two-way musical communication, engaging and imitating during the musical interaction. Furthermore, he is integrating and synchronizing multiple sensory stimuli (vestibular, tactile, proprioceptive, visual and auditory), while utilizing his expressive and receptive language within the context of musical play.

65

DVD Cut 3: Blowin to the Blues with Intentionality: Musical Relatedness & Communicativeness Cut 3, excerpted from session sixteen, illustrates Matthew playing a pitched reed horn in a communicative manner with the therapists music, relying solely on his auditory processing skills while being out of visual range of the therapist. Session sixteen is also held in Matthews classroom. The lights are turned off prior to the session to help him self-regulate and calm down, as he is coming off of a serious melt-down before I enter the room. While he is lying down, on a padded chair, I offer him a pitched horn. He takes it from my hand and begins exploring the texture and look of the instrument. He starts to blow into the horn as I improvise, on the guitar, in a blues style. Initially, his blowing appears to be disconnected from my music, and the horn seems to be an object to fulfill his need for oral stimulation; however, Matthews music begins to find its way into my musical form, as I intentionally incorporate pauses and variations in the rhythm and tempo to connect our musics. His horn playing begins to take on a rhythmic form that matches mine, followed by a selfinitiated vocalization. Stage Four: Moving up the Developmental Ladder: Assimilation and Integration Sessions seventeen through twenty-five focused on strengthening Matthews capacities in levels I, II, III, (self-regulation, engagement, and intentional communication) to help move him into higher levels of thinking (level IV) and music making (musical interresponsiveness).

66

DVD Cut 4:Play-dohto facilitate Musical Interresponsiveness Cut 4, taken from session twenty-five, illustrates Matthews ability to engage in musical play while copying and initiating musical ideas in a related and communicative fashion with the music and Jean (intern). Ten minutes into the 25th session, with play-doh in hand, Matthews attention and focus are completely on the play-doh, and he displays no interest in interacting. Jean begins to sing (improvise) a non-verbal melody in a gentle and legato manner as she proceeds to apply light pressure to his legs by squeezing. I begin to play the piano, accompanying both her melody and squeezing patterns. After several measures, Matthew begins vocalizing on his own initiative in a related and communicative manner in relationship to the music being offered. The interaction moves into a call-and-response between Jean and him. The interaction patterns move beyond call-and-response interactions. In addition, Matthew explores his voice, utilizing new sounds and manipulating musical elements that enhance his musical expressiveness. In short, it appears that Matthews ability to initiate is leading to musical independence. His independence, which in the past had been self-directed and unrelated, is now being viewed as being related and communicative. The above interaction, again, begins by providing Matthew with specific sensory input followed by musical accompaniment. The music is used as a way of taking an isolated behavior, such as squeezing play-doh and making into a joint interaction. This makes it possible for Matthew to engage in musical communication and interresponsiveness.

67

While Matthew continued to receive music therapy, the 25th session was the final clinical session of the present research protocol. The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale post-test was administered and Goal Attainment Scoring followed Matthews final session. Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) In addition to the qualitative case study analysis of Matthews treatment process, two quantitative measures were used to evaluate outcomes of music therapy: the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) and Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). The FEAS (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001) was used as a pre- and post-test measure for Mathews progress in achieving social-emotional goals. The GAS (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994) was used to evaluate each childs musical goals. Table 2 gives a comparison of Matthews pre- and post-test scores on the FEAS. The table shows raw scores for each developmental area, sub-scores, level of functioning, percentages, and the number of functioning levels changed out of three possible levels (e.g., deficient, at risk, and normal). Scoring is based on a 2-point scale: 0 = behavior is not seen or is briefly observed, 1 = behavior is present some of the time or observed several times, and 2 = behavior is consistently observed or observed many times. The ratings were summed to obtain subtest scores for each area, as well as total test scores (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). Total scores are compared to cutoff scores (see Appendix A) to determine if the child is: normal, at-risk, and deficient.

68

Table 2 Comparison of Matthews Pre- and Post FEAS Scores Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 9 (Deficient) 57.41% Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 8 (Deficient) 50%
2 2 0 2 1 1 0

Areas I. Self-regulation

Change in Functioning Level 0

Attentive to play with toys 2 Explores objects/toys freely 2 Remains calm during play 2 Touching textured toys & caregiver 2 Shows content affect 1 Focused without distraction 0 Appears over aroused 0

II. Forming relationships and engagement


Emotional interest in caregiver Relaxed when near caregiver Anticipates with curiosity Uncomfortable with caregiver Initiates closeness to caregiver Avoids care-giver Socially references caregiver Communicates from across space

6 (Deficient) 37.5%
2 0 0 2 0 2 0 0

7 (Deficient) 43.75%
2 1 1 2 0 0 1 0

III. Two-way purposeful communication


Opens circles of communication Initiates intentional actions Closes circles of communication Uses words, or gestures, or sounds

5 (Deficient) 50%
1 1 1 2

6 (Deficient) 60%
1 1 2 2

IV. Behavioral organization 0 (Deficient) and problem solving 0%


Communicates in several modes Copies caregiver & incorporates 0 0

2 (Normal) 100%
0 2

+2

V. Representational capacity
Engages in symbolic play

1 (Deficient) 7.41%
0

0 (Deficient) 0%
0

69

Table 2. (Continued) _____________________________________________________________________________________________

Areas

Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores

Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores

Change in Functioning Level

Engages in pretend play Communicates intentions Expresses dependency Expresses pleasure/excitement Expresses assertiveness Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas

0 0 1 0 0 0

0 0 0 0 0 0

VI. Representational differentiation

0 (Deficient) 0%

0 (Deficient) 0%
0 0 0 0 0

Bridges 2 unrealistic idea 0 Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 Use pretend to express dependency 0 Use pretend to express pleasure 0 Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0

Based on Table 2, Matthews ability to self-regulate and show interest in the world (area I) decreased by one point bringing his total score from 8 to 7. His ability to be engaged (area II) increased by one point bringing his total score from 6 to 7. In addition, Matthew displayed improvement in his ability to communicate purposefully (area III), increasing his score by one point bringing his total score from 5 to 6. Furthermore, Matthews ability to problem solve (area IV) increased by one point bringing his total score from 0 to 2, while his ability to think symbolically (area V) decreased by one point bringing his total score in from 1 to 0. Finally, Matthews ability to build bridges between ideas (area VI) showed no change between pre- and post-tests, leaving his score at 0.

70

In clinical terms, during the pretest Matthew was interested and attentive to the play toys, exploring them freely and remaining calm during play period; however, he showed no interest in playing or interacting with the therapist. He displayed content but neutral affect, and became frequently distracted, unable to focus on play for more then a few seconds at time. In addition, at times he was very active moving quickly from one toy to another and frequently wandering away from therapist. He exhibited limited ability to use communication purposefully, and although he initiated play, would engage in stereotypic actions of lining up and mouthing toys. In addition, he displayed difficulty in the ability to problem-solve, think symbolically and bridging ideas. During the post test Matthew continued to demonstrate the ability to be interested in and attentive to play toys, however, he displayed difficulty in remaining calm and regulated during play. He demonstrated the ability to focus and socially reference the therapist while engaged in play for brief moments, but continued to show difficulty initiating closeness. He continued to exhibit some ability to open circles of communication and initiate intentional actions, though at times continued to engage in stereotypical behaviors. He displayed ability to problem-solve, by imitating and copying something that the therapist had introduced, and then incorporated it into his own play; however, he continued to exhibit limited ability to think symbolically and bridge ideas. Figure 1 shows the amount of change in functioning levels made by Matthew in each developmental area: 1 represents deficient, 2 at risk, and 3 normal. As shown, Matthew improved by two functioning levels in one area, and stayed the same in five areas.

71

Figure 1 Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS)


Changes in Developmental Levels
Pre 2 Post

L e v e l s

Area I

Area II

Area III

Area IV

Area V

Area VI

Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) The quantitative data analyzed below used GAS to evaluate progress made toward Matthews musical goals. Musical goals for Matthew were created following session two, by the independent observer, and then were evaluated after session 25. Five music therapy (MT) goals were formulated for Mathew; all were weighted equally. The first goal for Matthew was musical awareness, that is, for him to be able to react or respond to, display any kind of awareness of, or show interest in the therapists improvised music; either vocally, instrumentally and/or through movement. Upon beginning music therapy, Matthew exhibited less than expected outcome in musical awareness (-1); he rarely displayed awareness of the therapists music. Upon completion of therapy, he displayed the expected level outcome (0); he sometimes exhibited musical awareness during the session. The second MT goal was musical relatedness, that is, for him to play in a related manner to the music that the therapist improvised. Upon beginning music therapy, Matthew

72

exhibited the most unfavorable outcome in relating his music to the therapists music (-2); he never played in a related way. However, at the conclusion of music therapy, he demonstrated the expected outcome level (0); his playing related to the therapists music sometimes throughout the session. The third MT goal was relationship within musical play, that involved his initiating a connection with the music and/or therapist, vocally, instrumentally and/or through movement/gesture. Upon beginning music therapy, Matthew displayed the most unfavorable outcome (-2) in initiating relationship within musical play; he never initiated a relationship within musical play. Upon completion of therapy, he displayed the expected outcome level (0); he initiated relationship within musical play sometimes throughout the session. Matthews fourth MT goal was musical interresponsiveness, that is for him to be able imitate or copy the therapists musical idea, and then incorporate it into musical play. At the start of music therapy, Matthew displayed the most unfavorable outcome (-2) in playing interresponsively to the therapists music; he never played in an interresponsive manner. Upon the completion of music therapy, Matthew displayed the expected outcome level (0); his playing was interresponsive to the therapists music sometimes throughout the session. The fifth MT goal for Matthew was musical communicativeness, that is, for him to be able to be spontaneously communicative in relation to the therapists music, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play (e.g., the ability to engage in call-and-response inter-play; and/or punctuate the end of a phrase; and/or initiate the opening of a phrase, and/or notice therapists music and respond contingently by elaborating on it). Upon beginning music therapy, Matthew exhibited the most unfavorable outcome (-2) in playing communicatively with

73

the therapists music; he never played in a communicative way; however, at the conclusion of therapy he exhibited the expected outcome level (0); he played communicatively sometimes throughout the session. Figure 1 shows all five goal areas and the amount of change Matthew demonstrated as a result of music therapy. The table shows raw scores for each goal area (-2 represents most unfavorable outcome, -1 less than expected outcome, 0 expected level, +1 more than expected success, and +2 most favorable outcome). Taking all of these goals collectively, Matthew advanced 1 level in one goal area, and 2 levels in the remaining four goal areas. These changed scores on all goal areas were converted to a T- score, according to the formula of Kiresuk and Sherman (1968). Matthews T-score was 50, based on the assumption that together these scales have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (see Kiresuk & Sherman). Figure 2 Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS) Changes in Goal Attainment Levels
Pre G A S L e v e l s Post

2 1 0 -1 -2
Musical Awareness Musical Relatedness Relationship in Musical Play Musical Communicativeness Musical Interrelatedness

Musical Goal Areas

74

Summary Client Process Stage one, Whats Getting in the Way?: Developing the Playing Field While Considering Individual-Differences, consisted of the first six sessions, where Matthew exhibited difficulty adapting to the new environment and medium music due to his complex sensory system. To that end, it was difficult to engage him, musically or interpersonally, as he consistently melted-down before, during and at the end of music sessions. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies, implementing clinical parameters that included shortening sessions, being sensitive to when and how to transition into other music, gauging the rhythm of the interaction, and altering the sensory environment to cater to Matthews individualdifferences. Stage two, Integrating Matthews Sensory & Musical Profiles, consisted of sessions seven through twelve. During this time, in providing Matthew with the proper sensory input, his ability to maintain self-regulation for extended periods increased, which led to an increase in his ability to engage in musical play in a related manner for brief moments. In addition, during this stage, the development of the relationship (interpersonal and musical) was developing. This stage ended when the therapist began to increase the length of sessions, based on the emerging development of the relationship and Matthews increased ability to engage in musical play in a related fashion. Stage three, Developing the Musical Relationship: Initiating Circles of Musical Communication, featured sessions thirteen through sixteen. During this stage, Matthew continued to increase his ability to engage in music in a related manner for longer periods;

75

however; the key element during this phase was that he began to initiate ideas, opening and closing circles of musical communication. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies in providing more opportunities for Matthew to lead and guide the musical improvisations; trusting the process between the music and the players (child, intern, and therapist) to facilitate independence, and higher levels of thinking (e.g., initiating, reciprocating, and intentional and purposeful communication). Finally, stage four, which consisted of sessions seventeen through twenty-five, Moving up the Developmental Ladder: Assimilation and Integration, featured Matthew integrating and assimilating his abilities to musically engage, relate and communicate. To that end, he began to develop an increased sense of self, exhibiting independence during musical play in a related and communicative manner. In addition, during this stage, he displayed his desire to be in a relationship for the sake of relating to another person, as opposed to relating for the purpose of fulfilling a sensory need. This new awareness also brought to light, for Matthew, that people are not objects to solely fulfill wants and needs (as seen by typical developing infants), but rather, as social beings that experience a relationship. Therapists Method Focusing on what gets in the way (individual-differences) of a childs social-emotional development is at the crux of this treatment plan. For Matthew, creating the proper sensory environment helped to facilitate his ability to self-regulate and to be available for interactive musical experiences that provided an avenue for relationship, communication, and the exchange of ideas.

76

The non-verbal medium of music provided a non-threatening field of play for Matthew. Because of his difficulty with expressive language, in terms of using words, music provided him with an outlet in which he could communicate and experience relatedness non-verbally. Finally, during Matthews treatment process, music not only became the vehicle for communication, but also the medium that converted his isolated sensory-motor play into joint musical interactions. The music seemed to help Matthew make meaning out of something that had no meaning, in regards to relating to others, thus promoting continuous flows of musical interaction that built a musical relationship and the desire to be in a shared world. Relationship between FEAS and GAS Results of Matthews FEAS pre-test and GAS indicated slight differences in several categories. Although in both settings (FEAS and GAS) he displayed difficulty self-regulating, during GAS, he engaged in melt-downs in response to the environment and musical stimuli, whereas during the FEAS he remained calm while exploring action figurines (although he rejected any attempt the therapist made for joint interaction). During both procedures he demonstrated an over-reactive sensory system, engaged in constant movement and attempted to climb on things including the therapist and intern. During both procedures he displayed difficulty in his ability to engage and relate to the therapist; however, during GAS he demonstrated some musical awareness and ability to relate vocally during musical play. During the FEAS he did not acknowledge the presence of the therapist, other then to climb on him. Although Matthew displayed difficulties in his ability to engage in two-way purposeful communication (level III) during the FEAS, he displayed some ability to close circles of musical

77

communication (ending musical phrases). During both procedures he displayed difficulty initiating ideas/circles of communication. Finally, during both procedures Matthew exhibited difficulty in problem-solving, creating and bridging ideas. The progress that Matthew displayed in the FEAS post-test and GAS also slightly differed from the Goal Attainment Scoring. The FEAS results indicated a decline in Matthews ability to self-regulate, however, Goal Attainment Scoring indicated an increase in his ability to self-regulate. In addition, although the FEAS illustrated slight (insignificant; maintaining deficiency in this area) increases in Matthews ability to relate, engage, and get into two-way purposeful communication (areas I, II, III), Goal Attainment Scoring reflected greater increases in areas I, II, and III, specifically in the area of initiating ideas. Furthermore, Goal Attainment Scoring indicated an increase in Matthews ability to create and bridge musical ideas, however, in the FEAS, he showed no progress in creating and bridging ideas. Relationship Between Qualitative and Quantitative Data Matthews areas of strengths and weaknesses were inconsistent between qualitative and quantitative data. The FEAS indicated decreased scores in Matthews ability to self-regulate (area I), and create ideas (area V) by 1 point. In the areas of engagement, two-way purposeful communication and bridging ideas his level of functioning remained the same according to FEAS. Clinically, however, Matthew displayed significant change in the areas of self-regulation, engagement, two-way purposeful communication, and in his ability to bridge ideas in the context of musical play.

78

CHAPTER 5 RESULTS CASE: JANET History Janet, age 8 years and 8 months at the time of this study, was the product of an uncomplicated, full-term pregnancy and C-section delivery. She weighed 7 lbs., 13 oz. at birth, and no complications were indicated. Her medical history revealed mild ear infections at age 2. She reached developmental milestones for sitting and crawling within normal limits, with independent walking appearing at 11 months. Janet was reported to have babbled during her first 6 months, was responsive to speech and was not an unusually quiet baby. She developed several words such as peek-a-boo and David (her brothers name); however at 18 months of age, she stopped using words all together. She was diagnosed with autism at 22 months. Janet lives at home with her mother, father, and her 11 year-old brother. Prior to entering the Rebecca School Janet attended another school where she received occupational and speech therapies five times per week, and Applied Behavioral Analysis 20 hours per week. Janet has been attending the Rebecca School since September 2007, where she is receiving occupational and speech therapies three times per week and music therapy two times per week for 30 minutes. DIR Profile The following DIR profile is based on: 1) pre-test scores of the FEAS (See table 1); 2) interviews of staff; 3) and observations of Janet in various settings throughout the school day, including the classroom, occupational therapy, and speech therapy.

79

Janet is an energetic child who displays difficulty maintaining engagement (level II) and 2-way purposeful communication (level III) with caregivers for extended periods. She constantly engages in perseverative body movements, displays difficulty with simple motor planning tasks, and presents with low muscle tone and poor balance. She likes vestibular input (swings, hammock, etc.), but seems to have difficulty sensing her center of gravity. In addition, she exhibits visual spatial processing difficulties, wandering aimlessly, moving from one thing to the next, and bumping into things frequently because of difficulty in scanning a room systematically. Janet displays the ability to problem-solve (level IV) and get her needs met. She has a plethora of memorized phrases, scripts, and words that she often repeats, but is unable to build ideas onto these scripts. In short, she does not appear to have an internal flow or cohesion of ideas. Rather, she appears to be observing or hearing a stream of random visual or audio snippets, like a ticker tape of ideas or words, which she verbalizes as they come to her. Music Therapy Assessment The music therapy assessment, guided by Greenspan and Weiders Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL) (2006a), focused on seven areas of musical responsiveness (see Materials in Method Chapter). The music therapy room was set up with a variety of percussive and melodic instruments, an assortment of different size mallets with various handle textures, pitched horns, and a variety of sensory based items (e.g., scarves, play-doh, mini-trampoline, etc.). The assessment focused on Janets ability to interact during musical play with the therapist. Janets assessment took place during the course of two 30 minute sessions. During the assessment, Janets primary modes of interacting were through movement,

80

instrumental play (primarily the drum and cymbal), gestures, and memorized scripts. She exhibited a moderate level of musical awareness. Her vocal responses, although tonal, appeared to be reflexive rather than intentional. Words that she sang/spoke were often repeated scripts that she tended to revert to during moments of stress or excitement. Janet displayed discomfort and anxiety to the new environment, as she engaged in constant motion, moving rapidly from playing one instrument to the next. Finally, when she would stop moving, she usually sat on the window ledge, near the heating pipe, and would attempt to hide behind the pipe. It appeared that she thought that Jean and I were unable to see her (the pipe is only two inches wide). Janet required visual prompting and direction in order to engage in musical play. During these moments, Jean held out a buffalo drum in one hand and a cymbal in the other directly in front of Janet. Janet usually responded by playing on the beat for one to four beats, and then either walked away, or began beating in an excited and unrelated manner, in which she became over-aroused and over-stimulated. Janet demonstrated some rhythmic perception through brief and fleeting moments of beating the drum and cymbal. She displayed an ability to join with the tempo of my music, however, she never initiated musical play and rarely referenced Jean or me at a distance. Physically, she played the drum using only her arms and upper body, as she swayed her body with arms outstretched. Her musical responses, either through vocalizations or instrumental play, displayed a limited expressive range, playing only very loudly on the drum and cymbal, and, when

81

vocalizing, exhibiting no range of affect. In addition, her beating was inconsistent, sporadic, and inflexible in relation to my music. Janet demonstrated difficulty in initiating a relationship within musical play, requiring prompts and hand-over-hand support. In addition, she kept herself at a distance from Jean and me and constantly withdrew from musical and physical contact. When Janet entered into a musical interaction, she displayed the ability to close circles of musical communication (completing or filling in the endings of musical phrases) when prompted, however, she showed difficulty initiating circles of musical communication. In summary, Janets inability to maintain musical engagement and relatedness for sustained periods prevented her from experiencing a continuous flow of musical interaction. In addition, contributing to her difficulty in relating for extended periods, were issues dealing with visual spatial processing and motor planning combined with her poor body awareness. Finally, because of her intense ABA program, prior to entering Rebecca School, she learned to engage only through prompt and repetition. She was accustomed to a memory-based approach to learning, whereas the DIR model is a thinking-based approach. Thus, her previous learning through prompts and external rewards made it difficult for Janet to think spontaneously and to initiate ideas. Treatment Janets treatment consisted of twenty-five sessions over a five month period. During this time, Janet passed through five distinct stages. In the first stage, she displayed difficulty engaging in musical play due to her being in constant motion; this resulted in scattered, perseverative and inflexibly loud playing. During the second stage, Janet became increasingly

82

engaged and related as her perseverative movements became transformed into interactive dancing. During Stage three, Janets musical participation moved from movement to instrumental play. Stage four consisted of Janet increasingly becoming communicative and related in and out of musical play. Finally, during stage five, Janet began to display the ability to connect her ideas in the context of musical play. Stage One: Everywhere But Nowhere: Wheres Janet? Janet entered each of our first five sessions in an ambivalent and cautious manner. Generally, after entering the room she spent a lot of time moving aimlessly, playing each instrument in her sight while in constant motion. Her playing was scattered, perseverative, and inflexibly loud. In addition, although she exhibited some sense of musical awareness and moments of relatedness, it seemed as though Janets playing was more about her meeting some kind of sensory need, than it was about connecting to the music being played. Her need for motion made it difficult for her to get into a continuous flow of musical contact, thereby impeding the development of any kind of musical relationship. At times Jean (intern) followed Janet, as she moved continuously around the room, and held a drum out in an attempt to capture Janets attention and bring her into musical play. The plan for subsequent sessions was to help Janet develop greater body awareness while slowing her body down, thereby facilitating her self-regulation (ability to be calm and available for interaction), musical engagement, and musical relationship. To that end, more emphasis was placed on embracing Janets perseverative movements through music and dance; less emphasis was put on Janets ability to make music. The idea was to use music and Jean

83

(intern) as a way to create form, both physically and musically, which would help organize Janets movements into a meaningful interactive experience. Stage Two: Transforming Perseverative Movements into Joint Interaction Sessions six through ten focused on transforming Janets perseverative movements into an interactive dance with Jean, thereby promoting engagement, relatedness, and relationship. To that end, I asked Jean to try and be more hands-on with Janet and engage her in a joint movement experience. In addition, I asked Jean not to chase or follow Janet around the room, but to always try to be in front of her (within her visual range). I also asked her to experiment with getting in Janets way, playfully obstructing her from wherever she was headed. This, in a sense, would force Janet to interact with Jean. Musically I experimented with several time signatures and song forms as I attempted to meet Janets movements in music. In addition, I incorporated legato type phrases, both vocally and in my piano playing, to create the experience of connection and flow. DVD Cut 5: From Scripted Words to Relatedness and Initiating Relationship Cut 5 is taken from Janets tenth session. It illustrates Janet and Jean dancing to an improvised waltz. The music incorporates random words (scripted) that Janet initiates: Puffpastries for ta, ta, ta (These words would become important reoccurring themes in subsequent sessions). In addition, the excerpt displays Janets ability to initiate an interaction with Jean. At fifteen minutes and thirty seconds into the tenth session, Janet and Jean engage in an interactive dance. The improvised music captures a waltz feel, based on Janets movement, and the words that she initiates during the interaction. Jean incorporates Janets words into the

84

song form while dancing. Janet recognizes the words while dancing with Jean and begins smiling. Janet accepts Jean holding her hands and appears to be excited during their dance, as they both are in synchrony with the music, swaying in time. On separate occasions, when Jean and I sing Ta and puff pastries, Janet becomes overly-excited and each time breaks away from Jean, skipping with excitement to the other end of the room. After several measures, Janet returns to Jean on her own initiative, each time holding her hands out to re-engage in dance with Jean. During the interaction, it appears that whenever Janet experiences sustained moments of engagement and relatedness with Jean and/or in the music, she becomes over-aroused and then engages in perseverative movements and loses the interaction. Stage Three: Moving into Instrumental Play Sessions eleven through fifteen concentrated on facilitating Janets ability to engage in instrumental play in a related manner. During this time, she exhibited difficulty with playing instruments due to her poor body awareness and difficulty with motor planning. To that end, I asked Jean to use a lot of prompts, such as holding the instruments in front of Janet (within visual range) in order to indicate when to play. In addition to visual prompts, I asked Jean to use a lot of affect (e.g., over exaggerated facial expressions and gestures) to help maintain Janets ability to be engaged and interested in playing. During this time, Janet began to display islands of capacities in the areas of two-way purposeful communication (musical communicativeness), connecting her musical ideas with Jeans, and elaborating on musical ideas during musical play (musical interrelatedness).

85

DVD Cut 6: Cymbal Play to Increase Relatedness and Relationship Cut 6 is taken from Janets fifteenth session. It illustrates Janet sitting in a chair for an extended period of time while musically interacting with Jean on the cymbal. During this excerpt, Janet and Jean get into a long series of back-and-forth musical exchanges, taking turns on the cymbal. During this interaction Janet is calm and engaged in musical play while socially referencing both Jean and me with eye-contact and smiles. At twelve minutes and twenty-six seconds into session fifteen, Janet is sitting on a rolling chair and is swaying back and forth. Jean holds the cymbal in front of her prompting Janet to play at specific moments within the music. Shortly after the interaction begins, Janet becomes increasingly interested and engages in the musical play, as they both take turns playing on the cymbal in a related manner to the improvised music. During the interaction, Jean utilizes affect and facial gestures to help guide and prompt Janet into interactive play on the cymbal. The quality in which Janet plays varies in touch and in dynamics, based on the music. In addition, she demonstrates moments of musical flexibility and a desire to be connected to Jeans playing. She also exhibits an emotional connection to Jean, smiling, laughing, and intently watching for an extended period while playing. In addition, at times Janet imitates the way Jean plays the cymbal (touch and motion), while incorporating her own ideas into the music play. Stage Four: Relating and Communicating In and In-between Musical Play Sessions sixteen through nineteen continued to focus on Janets ability to engage in instrumental play in a related and communicative manner to the music. The plan was to provide

86

Janet with less prompt and more opportunities to initiate (open circles of communication) and connect musical ideas with Jeans and/or mine (musical interrelatedness). Furthermore, at this point, I began to look and listen beyond Janets music-making responses in order to accurately evaluate her ability to relate and communicate. Because of Janets individual-differences (craving movement, getting over-aroused when over-stimulated, motor-planning issues, etc.), it was difficult for her to engage in deep, extended musical experiences. A lot of Janets relatedness and communication, musically and interpersonally, were evident when she was not making music (during moments of perseverate movements). Yet, she continued to display her emotional connection to Jean and me through: joyful facial expressions, eagerness to come to music; getting excited while engaged in music-making; and the ability and desire to work through sensory issues to engage in related music-making experiences. To that end, when Janets music making was interrupted by her perseverative movements, she continued to exhibit relatedness and communication to the music (and Jean and me) through movements, gestures, and affect. DVD Cut 7: Working through Perseverative Movements for Musical Communicativeness Cut 7 is taken from session 16. It illustrates Janet having difficulty maintaining a basic beat due to her perseverative body movements. She intermittently plays the drum, while constantly swaying back-and-forth in her chair. She engages in a related manner to the music, either though movements or through her playing. Finally, during the moments in which she interrupts the music with her perseverative movements, Janet appears to be trying hard to work through these movements so that she can beat in tempo with the music.

87

DVD Cut 8: Working through Perseverative Movements for Musical Communicativeness (Part II) Cut 8 is taken from Janets nineteenth session. It illustrates Jean prompting Janet to beat the drum, using gestures combined with high-affect singing. Janets playing is connected and related to the music, as she watches Jean and is listening to the music. At the start of session nineteen, Jean kneels down in front of Janet, and mirrors Janets swaying while she sings a melody on la. Janet, for the first time asks Jean to sing by simply saying: Sing. Jean begins singing 5ths, in an operatic style combined with over-exaggerated arm movements, prompting Janet to play. Janet plays the drum interchangeably with swaying back and forth, but all the while is watching Jean intently. Janet then initiates a dynamic change, playing more softly while playing a basic beat for three measures. Stage Five: Connecting Ideas While Maintaining Engagement and Relatedness In the past stages, Janet withdrew from an interaction when Jean or I attempted to connect an idea to hers. In addition, she generally fixated on being self-directed in expressing her unorganized and random ideas; any attempt to build on her ideas to facilitate spontaneous thinking would result in her withdrawing from the interaction. To that end, sessions twenty through twenty-two focused on helping Janet to connect her ideas (musical play, words, gestures, etc.), either to mine or to her own, to facilitate internal organization. My intervention at times included imposing my ideas (music, words, etc.) onto Janets before she moved on to an unrelated topic, while still helping her to maintain engagement and relatedness. DVD Cut 9: Using the Spanish Idiom to Help Organize and Facilitate Initiation

88

Cut 9 is taken from session twenty-two. It illustrates Janet engaging in drum and cymbal play within a Spanish style improvisation (without the use of prompts from Jean). The music appears to help her organize her beating as she plays in a related fashion. Her beating is interrupted by her desire for movement, however, her movement is related to the tempo of the music. Eight minutes into session twenty-five, Janet is seated in front of the drum and cymbal and is preoccupied with the mallets. She looks down and withdraws, as I sing, Were waiting for Janet, while I wait for her to initiate play. She looks in my direction and sings, ta, ta, ta. I begin to improvise in a Spanish style and she begins to play on the drum in an unrelated manner, stops, and becomes withdrawn. This is followed by her becoming musically engaged, and related, beating in tempo to the music. She stops playing, as I sing again, were waiting, as Janet comes back into beating at times using big arm movements and alternating hands while beating. The musical interaction is soon followed by Janet withdrawing from the interaction. I begin singing, while playing the piano, were waiting for Janet . Janet begins playing the drum and seems to realize that the music was waiting for her to initiate play. I incorporate the familiar melody and sing Ta. Janet recognizes this musical pattern from previous sessions and begins playing in a consistent and related manner on the drum. She plays the drum with big arm movements, and initiates changes in tempo and dynamics while referencing me though eyecontact and brief smiles. DVD Cut 10: Take a Walk to the Moon: Joining and Staying in the Interaction Cut 10 is taken from Janets twenty-fifth session. It illustrates Janet playing the piano with me for the first time. She initiates a random lyrical idea: Take a walk to the moon. My

89

music and singing become more expressive than usual, as I sang back only three words of her phrase: Take a walk, and then I change the phrase to, take a walk to the wild side in music (connecting an idea of mine to Janets). Janet attempts to correct what Im singing, so she could hear Take a walk to the moon. I quickly organize my music in a pop-rock form using expressive singing in order to maintain her focus to the task at hand. She regains focus and begins playing the piano in a rhythmically related manner to my music. The momentum of the music builds, and Jean (intern) begins to dance and sing non-verbally, however, the moment Jean begins to sing with words, Janet becomes distracted and withdraws from the interaction. I again begin singing. Janet once again comes over to correct me, as she says, Take a walk to the moon. While she says this, she pretends to draw a moon in the air (demonstrating symbolic thinking), desperately trying to figure out a way (problem-solving) to get me to sing Take a walk to the moon. DVD Cut 11: Take a Walk to the Moon: Joining and Staying in the Interaction Part II During cut 11 from session twenty-five, Janets piano playing appears to show some flexibility, both rhythmically and dynamically in relation to my music. I begin singing the familiar theme. This time I sing back Janets words, however, when I get to the word moon I sing it expressively with a melodic leap to generate additional affect, while attempting to prompt Janet to sing with expressiveness and inflection. Janet responds positively during the last phrase, by singing the word moon with inflection and a melodic leap. While Janet continued to receive music therapy, the 25th session was the final clinical session of the present research protocol. The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale post-test was administered and Goal Attainment Scoring followed Janets final session.

90

Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) In addition to the qualitative case study analysis of Janets treatment process, two quantitative measures were used to evaluate outcomes of music therapy: the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) and Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). The FEAS (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001) was used as a pre- and post-test measure for Janets progress in achieving social-emotional goals. The GAS (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994) was used to evaluate Janets progress toward musical goals. Table 3 gives a comparison of Janets pre- and post-test scores on the FEAS. The table shows raw scores for each developmental area, level of functioning, percentages, and the number of functioning levels changed out of three possible levels (e.g., deficient, at risk, and normal). Scoring is based on a 2-point scale: 0 = behavior is not seen or is briefly observed, 1 = behavior is present some of the time or observed several times, and 2 = behavior is consistently observed or observed many times. The ratings were summed to obtain subtest scores for each area, as well as total test scores (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). Total scores are compared to cutoff scores (See Appendix A) to determine if the child is: normal, at-risk, and deficient.

91

Table 3 Comparison of Julias Pre- and Post FEAS Scores Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 11 (At risk) 78.5% Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 12 (Normal) 85.7%
2 2 2 2 1 2 1

Areas I. Self-regulation

Change in Functioning Level +1

Attentive to play with toys 2 Explores objects/toys freely 2 Remains calm during play 2 Touching textured toys & caregiver 2 Shows content affect 1 Focused without distraction 1 Appears over aroused 1

II. Forming relationships and engagement


Emotional interest in care-giver Relaxed when near care-giver Anticipates with curiosity Uncomfortable with care-giver Initiates closeness to care-giver Avoids care-giver Socially references care-giver Communicates from across space

11 (Deficient) 68.75%
2 1 1 2 2 1 2 1

16 (Normal) 100%
2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2

+2

III. Two-way purposeful communication


Opens circles of communication Initiates intentional actions Closes circles of communication Uses words, or sounds, or gestures

5 (Deficient) 50%
0 1 2 2

7 (At risk) 70%


2 1 2 2

+1

IV. Behavioral organization 3 (Normal) and problem solving 75%


Communicates in several modes Copies caregiver & incorporates 1 2

4 (Normal) 100%
2 2

V. Representational capacity

3 (Deficient) 21.5%

7 (At risk) 50%

+1

92

Table 3. (Continued) Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores


0 2 0 0 0 0 1

Areas
Engages in symbolic play Engages in pretend play Communicates intentions Expresses dependency Expresses pleasure/excitement Expresses assertiveness Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas

Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores


1 2 2 1 1 0 0

Change in Functioning Level

VI. Representational differentiation

1 (Deficient) 10%

4 (Normal) 40%
2 2 0 0 0

Bridges 2 unrealistic idea 1 Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 Use pretend to express dependency 0 Use pretend to express pleasure 0 Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0

Janets ability to self-regulate and show interest in the world (area I) increased by one point bringing her total score from 11 to 12. Her ability to be engaged (area II) increased by five points bringing her total score from 11 to 16, and she also showed improvement in her ability to communicate purposefully (area III), increasing her score by two points bringing her total score from 5 to 7. In addition, Janets ability to problem solve (area IV) increased by one point bringing her total score from 3 to 4, and her ability to think symbolically (area V) increased by four points bringing her total score in from 3 to 7. Finally, Janets ability to build bridges between ideas (area VI) also increased by three points bringing her total score from 1 to 4. To summarize, during the pre-test Janet was regulated, but was not particularly interested in the outside world. She showed difficulty in her ability to attach, and generally

93

reverted to isolated behaviors and scripts, fragmenting and/or discontinuing the flow of interaction. In addition, she showed limited ability to use communication purposefully (in context to a situation), reverting to scripts and echolalia. She displayed the ability to problemsolve and get her needs met during the pre-test; however, she displayed difficulty in her ability to think symbolically and connect her ideas with the therapists ideas. During the post-test she continued to demonstrate the ability to be regulated, but was interested in interacting with the therapist, and displayed the ability to engage in a related manner. She also exhibited the ability to be more purposeful while communicating, using fewer scripts, and generating language appropriate to the situation at hand. In addition, she displayed the capacity to create and build bridges between ideas, engaged in symbolic play, and showed islands in the capacity to think emotionally. Figure 3 shows the amount of change in functioning levels made by Janet in each developmental area: 1 represents deficient, 2 at risk, and 3 normal. As shown, Janet improved by one functioning level in three areas, two levels in two areas, and stayed the same in one area.

94

Figure 3 Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS)


Changes in Developmental Levels

Pre Post
L e v e l s
2

Area I

Area II

Area III

Area IV

Area V

Area VI

Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) The quantitative data analyzed below used GAS to evaluate progress made toward Janets musical goals. Musical goals for Janet were created following session two, by the independent observer, and then were evaluated after session 25. Four music therapy (MT) goals were formulated for Janet, all were weighted equally. The first goal was musical relatedness, that is, for Janet to play in a related way to the music that the therapist improvised. Upon beginning music therapy, Janets music related to the therapists improvised music less than expected (-1); she rarely played in a related manner. Upon completion of therapy, she exhibited more than expected success (+1); her playing often related to the therapists music. The second MT goal was relationship within musical play, which involves Janet being able to initiate some kind of relationship with music and the therapist, vocally and/or

95

instrumentally and/or through movement/gesture. Upon beginning music therapy, Janet exhibited a less then expected outcome (-1); she rarely initiated relationship within music. However, upon completion of therapy she displayed a more than expected outcome (+1), often initiating relationship within musical play throughout the session. The third MT goal for Janet was musical communicativeness, that is, for her to be able to be spontaneously communicative in relation to the therapists music, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play (e.g., the ability to engage in call-and-response interplay, punctuate the end of a phrase, initiate the opening of a phrase, and/or notice therapists music and respond contingently by elaborating on it). Upon beginning music therapy, Janets musical communicativeness was a less than expected outcome (-1); rarely using music communicatively. At the conclusion of therapy she exhibited a more than expected outcome (+1), often using music communicatively throughout the session. Finally, Janets fourth MT goal was musical interrelatedness, that is for her to be able to connect a musical idea of her own to the therapists idea, and elaborate on the idea during musical play. At the start of music therapy Janets music was interrelated to the therapists music less than expected (-1); she rarely played in an interrelated manner. However, upon completion of therapy, she demonstrated more than expected success outcome (+1), often using music in an interrelated fashion throughout the session. Figure 4 shows all four goal areas and the amount of change Janet displayed as a result of music therapy. The figure shows raw scores for each goal area (-2 represents most unfavorable outcome; -1 less than expected outcome; 0 expected level; +1 more than expected success; and +2 most favorable outcome).

96

Taking all of these goals collectively, Janet improved by two levels in each of her four goal areas. These changed scores on all goal areas were converted to a T- score, according to the formula of Kiresuk and Sherman (1968). Janets T-score was 64.51, based on the assumption that together these scales have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (see Kiresuk & Sherman). Figure 4 Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS)
Changes in Goal Attainment Levels Pre G A S L e v e l s Post

2 1 0 -1 -2 Musical Relatedness Relationship in Musical Play Musical Communicativeness Musical Interrelatedness

Musical Goal Areas

Summary Client Process Stage one, Everywhere But Nowhere: Wheres Janet?, consisted of the first five sessions in which Janet engaged in moving aimlessly around the music room, playing each instrument while being in constant motion, impeding her ability to self-regulate and engage. Although she displayed some sense of musical awareness and relatedness, it appeared as though Janets playing was related more to her meeting some kind of sensory need, than it was about 97

connecting to the music being played. Any attempt to engage her in a continuous flow of musical play for an extended period, was interrupted by her need for constant motion. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies, which included embracing Janets perseverative movements both physically and musically and transforming them into a meaningful interactive experience. Stage two, Transforming Perseverative Movements into Joint Interaction, featured sessions six through ten. It consisted of guiding Janets perseverative movements into interactive dance with Jean (intern), facilitating self-regulation, engagement, and relatedness, musically, physically and interpersonally. This stage ended when the therapist changed clinical strategies, based on Janets ability to self-regulate for extended periods, to engage her in musical play that involved music making. Stage three, Moving into Instrumental Play, consisted of sessions eleven through fifteen. During this time, Janet demonstrated a greater ability to self-regulate and to be seated in a chair (based on her own initiative) for extended periods, making her available for musical interaction. Although she exhibited difficulty in playing instruments during musical play due to poor body awareness and motor planning difficulties, through prompting, she displayed an ability to engage in musical relatedness and communicativeness; however, the quality of the musical interactions was fragmented and intermittent. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies by reducing prompting, and taking greater notice to Janets non-musical behaviors. Relating and Communicating In and In-between Musical Play, Stage four, consisted of sessions sixteen through nineteen. During this stage it became more evident that, biologically,

98

it was difficult for Janet to engage in a continuous flow of musical interactions. Although she displayed this difficulty; Janet exhibited relatedness, communication and emotional connection to Jean and me through facial expressions (smiles and laughter), and social referencing (gestures and eye-contact) from across the room. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies, and focused on connecting (bridging her ideas to mine) Janets ideas and/or music to help her develop internal organization. Finally, Stage five, Connecting Ideas While Maintaining Engagement and Relatedness, consisted of sessions twenty through twenty-five. During this stage, Janets usual behavior of fixating on expressing random and unrelated ideas (including musical play, words, gestures, etc.), while rejecting any attempt by the therapist to connect her ideas (musical play, words, gestures, etc.) began to diminish. She began to display some capacity in her ability to connect her ideas (musical play, words, gestures, etc.) with the therapists while maintaining engagement and relatedness. Therapist Method Embracing and respecting a childs self-stimulatory or perseverative behaviors can be a gateway into interaction. These behaviors can be looked at and responded to in a creative manner to facilitate engagement and relatedness. For Janet, her perseverative movements turned into dancing, which led the way into developing relationship, communication, and socialization. Dr. Stanley Greenspan (2007) writes: Whenever there is a momentary pause in the interaction, see what the child is doing next. Remember, shes always doing something, even if its wandering

99

around the room. Wandering may be the doorway to a new dance step, for example, or a new musical symphony where the music changes with each step (the caregiver makes the music coming from their wonderful vocal cords contingent on how the child moves). (Greenspan, 2007, pp. 23) Rather then force-feeding Janet with ideas, and trying to lead her into our world (get her to do what we thought was the right thing), based on our own needs and expectations; we took her lead and allowed ourselves to join into her world. We took her direction, followed her lead, and let her guide our intervention. In following Janets lead to help her guide our intervention, she taught us to truly listen and watch for things beyond musical responses; to see whats really happening, observably, as opposed to what we assumed was happening based on our own prejudices and orientation. Because of Janets individual-differences on a biological level, there were certain things musically that she was unable to experience. The outsider, may interpret this as her not wanting to engage in musical play, or a cognitive deficit of her not understanding the concept of joint musical play, or her just not liking music. It became imperative then, to not only listen to her responses during her musical play, but also to watch and listen to her responses when she was not engaged in music making. Although at times Janet withdrew from music making, especially during the in-between times of musical engagement and withdrawing from musical play, she was emotionally and relationally connected to the music, the therapist and intern. Relationship between FEAS and GAS Results of Janets FEAS pre-test and GAS indicated differences in several categories.

100

During the FEAS she showed an ability to be calm, attentive to play toys, and emotionally interested in the therapist. During GAS, however, she displayed difficulty remaining calm and attentive to instruments; displaying anxiety while constantly being in motion. While she displayed contrasting characteristics in her ability to self-regulate and be attentive to play toys/instruments during both procedures (FEAS and GAS), she showed similar difficulties in her ability to focus and use the toys/instruments in an interactive manner. Similarities in both settings (FEAS pre-test and GAS) were reflected in the area of twoway purposeful communication, specifically in Janets difficulties initiating ideas and communication, musically and interpersonally. Similarities were also evident in both settings in Janets being uncomfortable and avoidant of the therapist. Furthermore, similarities during the FEAS and GAS also surfaced in Janets ability to problem-solve and understand the concept of cause-effect, musically and interpersonally. Finally, in both settings, she displayed difficulty in creating ideas, as she constantly reverted to memorized scripts and words and lacked the capacity to bridge ideas with the therapists. Results of Janets FEAS post-test and Goal Attainment Scale indicated several similarities. During both procedures she demonstrated her ability to self-regulate and engage with the therapist, being relaxed and emotionally interested in the therapist. In addition, during both, FEAS (post-test) and Goal Attainment Scale, she exhibited her ability to: open (initiate) and circles of communication (musically and interpersonally), engage in symbolism, communicate her intentions during play, and bridge ideas with the therapists, musically and interpersonally.

101

Relationship between Qualitative and Quantitative Data Janets areas of strengths and weaknesses were consistent in most areas between qualitative and quantitative data. Inconsistencies appeared in area I (self-regulation), specifically in remaining clam during play, in which the FEAS scored Janet as constantly being calm, however, clinically, she appeared to require support in order to maintain calm throughout each session. To that end, in general, the quantitative data had difficulty revealing the quality of certain outcomes relating to Janets abilities, with regard to how much prompt and support was or was not needed in order for her to reach a particular developmental milestone. A key similarity, in both quantitative and qualitative data, was in Janets difficulty in the area of two-way purposeful communication. Although she demonstrated an ability to engage in two-way purposeful interactions, her inability to maintain a constant flow of back-and-forth dialogue for extended periods was reflected in both qualitative and quantitative results.

102

CHAPTER 6 RESULTS: TIMMY History Timmy was born on September 14, 1999. His mother had an uncomplicated, full-term pregnancy. Pre- and post-natal histories were unremarkable; and no medical complications were noted. Timmys mother reported that he did not respond to his name at approximately eight months of age; and at two years and ten months, he was diagnosed with Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD). Timmy is an only child living with both of his parents. According to parent reports, Timmy accomplished early milestones (e.g., breastfed well, gained weight on time, and had good head control); however, when he approached the toddler years, milestones were demonstrated later then expected (e.g., sat up without support at18 months, began walking without holding onto something at 21 months, and was toilet trained at 48 months). Prior to entering the Rebecca School, Timmy had been in an Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) classroom. He received speech therapy five times per week for sixty minute sessions, occupational therapy fives times per week for forty minute sessions, and ten hours per week of ABA (home visits). Timmy has been attending the Rebecca School since September 2007. He has been a participant in individual music therapy since December 2007, two times per week for 30 minutes. During this time Timmy was also receiving occupational, and speech therapies 5 times per week, individually, for 30 minutes.

103

DIR Profile The following DIR profile is based on: 1) pre-test scores of the FEAS (see table 1); 2) interviews of staff; 3) and observations of Timmy in various settings throughout the school day, including the classroom, occupational therapy, and speech therapy. Timmy is a pleasant boy who consistently displays curiosity with the mechanical elements of his environment, such as the duct work in the sensory gyms, various steam pipes, and elevators. In addition, he is fascinated by trains of all sorts, including subways, air trains, etc. Although Timmy has a large vocabulary of words, he has difficulty conversing in 2-way purposeful communication with caregivers, and tends to be echolalic, frequently repeating what someone says to him. Timmy presents with postural control challenges; his trunk rotation is restricted when moving through the environment, and he displays difficulty isolating head and eye movements (looking with his entire head instead of simply shifting his eyes). In addition, Timmy displays visual-motor challenges, as demonstrated through his difficulty in finding large pieces of equipment in the gym. Furthermore, Timmy displays dyspraxia in the quality of his movements and play strategies. (The brain is unable to plan muscle movements and carry them out, such as drawing, writing, buttoning, and other tasks requiring fine motor skill, or in sequencing the necessary movements.) Music Therapy Assessment The music therapy assessment, guided by Greenspan and Weiders Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL) (2006a), focused on seven areas of musical responsiveness (see Materials in Method Chapter).

104

Timmys assessment took place during the course of two 30 minute sessions. During the assessment, Timmys primary modes of interacting were through movement, instrumental play (primarily playing various drums and the cymbal), and gestures. Although Timmy has use of words, he rarely spoke during the sessions. Timmy can self-regulate and can be engaged during musical play, however, he displayed frequent moments of losing himself, and showed difficulty in filtering out auditory stimuli in the environment. In addition, Timmy appeared to have difficulty with grading his force of movement and depth-perception; he constantly played the drum and cymbal very loudly. There were moments when Timmy displayed a keen sense of musical awareness, specifically in the area of rhythm (e.g., joining into the rhythms played by the therapist); however, for the most, his playing during these moments appeared to be reflexive, showing no intent to play in a related and connected manner with the therapist. In addition, he tended to play very loudly and required a lot of visual and verbal prompts in order to play more softly (he plays the drum with so much force, that he has broken several drum sticks and mallets). In addition to playing loudly, Timmy interrupted musical play by stopping in the middle of a musical interaction to explore the mechanics of the snare drum and/or piano. In addition, Timmy, at time broke-off musical contact to continuously change the mallets while playing (he would collect all of the drum sticks and mallets in the room, and kept them by his side when playing). Timmy displayed the ability to initiate musical play; however, it was unclear if this was based on wanting to engage in a joint musical interaction, or, if it was a random act without any intent to be related or communicative with the therapist, intern, or music.

105

In summary, Timmys inability to maintain musical relatedness for sustained periods prevented him from experiencing a continuous flow of musical interaction. In addition, contributing to his difficulty in relating for extended periods were issues dealing with visualmotor processing, focus and attention, an obsession with the mechanics of instruments, and a fixed manner of playing very loudly. Finally, because of his previous ABA school program, prior to entering Rebecca School, Timmy has learned to engage through prompt and repetition in order to shape his behaviors. During his past learning experiences (ABA), emphasis was placed solely on changing behaviors through external reinforcers, with little or no attention given to socialemotional development, such as creative and big-picture thinking (e.g., seeing the forest, not the trees) (G.Tippy, Personal Communication, January, 12, 2008), spontaneous communication, and the concept of learning through relationships. Following the assessment sessions, the researcher and independent observer formulated a GAS for Timmy (See Appendix F). Treatment Timmys treatment consisted of twenty-five sessions over a five month period. During this time, Timmy passed through three distinct stages. In the first stage, he interrupted the flow of all activities, thereby making it difficult to develop a musical and interpersonal relationship. In the second stage, Timmy began to engage and relate through familiar musical structures and activities. Finally, in the third stage Timmy became increasingly spontaneous, communicative and expressive during musical play.

106

Stage One: Interrupting Musical Flow: Who is this Boy? Stage One consisted of sessions one through seven, all of which appeared to be very much the same. As Timmy entered each session, it was unclear whether he wanted to be in music or not. When observing him walk into the room, it appeared that he wanted to be there, as evidenced by how quickly he walked from the hallway and would sit in front of the drum and cymbal and begin to play without hesitation. At the same time, he displayed little or no affect in his playing, appearing a bit lost, and completely unaware that Jean and I were in the room. Timmy consistently played instruments very loudly; Jean responded by using verbal and gestural prompts to play more softly and to make him more aware of our music and presence. Although he was eventually able to follow directions through repeated prompts, he was unable to initiate dynamic changes or to rely on musical cues alone to play more softly and in a more related way. The plan was to continue to use prompts to provide him with the experience of playing more softly, while bringing a greater awareness of my musical presence. In addition to prompts, I began to introduce him to different types of drum sticks (brushes and bundle sticks) that could facilitate softer sounds when beating the drum. Because most of our interactions during musical play where based on directing and prompting Timmy, the sessions felt artificial and rarely allowed for any spontaneity to evolve. To that end, although we were engaged in extemporaneous music making, I did not feel connected to Timmy, musically or interpersonally. The sessions lacked any emotional qualities and spontaneity, thereby indicating a need to shift strategies.

107

Nevertheless, during this stage, Timmy responded to prompts in a positive manner, playing in a softer dynamic and in a related manner. During this time we began to try and gradually phase out verbal prompts, however, this proved to be difficult, as Timmy constantly interrupted musical experiences by 1) playing loudly and erratically on the drum and cymbal, 2) stopping musical play every few measures to change drum sticks and/or mallets, or, 3) stopping his playing and moving to play another instrument. These fragmented musical experiences kept us at distance and stalled the development of our relationship. Stage Two: The Building Blocks for the Relationship: Balancing Between Predictability and Unpredictability Sessions eleven through seventeen focused on the development of a musical and interpersonal relationship with Timmy. To that end, Jean and I worked on facilitating more spontaneity during musical play and limiting our use of prompts. The plan was to implement the same musical structures and activities into subsequent sessions as a way to create familiarity and predictability. Although structure would be an important tool in Timmys therapy process, it was equally as important for me to be flexible and spontaneous within structure (e.g., to improvise music and adapt new ideas within a familiar song and activity) to gradually provide opportunities for spontaneous interacting, such as creating ideas, engaging in spontaneous communication, and experiencing a wide range of affect. The balance between being predictable and unpredictable during musical play was an important element to the intervention of using familiar music. Because of Timmys learning style, based in ABA, predictability would be, in a sense, meeting Timmy where he was. The

108

expectation of a particular outcome during a musical interaction empowered him as he knew what was happening next. In addition, predictability during musical play engaged him, provided comfort and security, and became a way to maintained relatedness during a musical interaction. On the other hand, using unpredictability within the structured musical play would provide Timmy with opportunities to remain alert, think spontaneously, and problem-solve. DVD Cut 12: The First Encounter through the Basic Beat: The Opening of Relationship & Musical Interresponsiveness Cut 12 is taken from Timmys eleventh session. It illustrates our first real encounter in music with Timmy, in that his music finds its way into mine, displaying moments of musical mutuality (Nordoff & Robbins, 2007), and an emotional connection to the experience. During the interaction, I create a musical form by repeatedly playing a progression within a jazz style. Jean plays the cymbal at specific moments in order to enhance and define the musical form. This appears to help Timmy organize his beating and attention, as he plays the basic beat for the first time. The excerpt begins with my attempt to improvise music around Timmys playing on the floor-tom. I attempt to bring up the intensity and affect of the music by vamping on a dominant seventh chord, while trying to make a musical connection with Timmy. He does not appear to be aware of my efforts to connect. He then stops playing to change drum sticks. During his pause, I continue to play and cue Jean to punctuate on the cymbal, creating affect and adding to the intensity of the music, while attempting to lead Timmy into joint musical play. This appears to get Timmys attention momentarily. Jean plays the cymbal on each first beat of every measure, drawing Timmys attention, as he looks at me and Jean, and

109

then plays the cymbal on his own initiative. Jean continues to play the cymbal, helping to form the music while Timmys beating begins moving into the basic beat of my music. I begin to sing non-verbally in a staccato manner, emphasizing the downbeats to maintain Timmys basic beating. After several measures, I feel that Timmy is slowly moving out of the interaction, as I cue Jean again to play the cymbal to bring Timmy back into basic beating. Timmy then copies Jeans cymbal play and incorporates it into his playing for one measure. I begin increasing the dynamics of my singing to gain Timmys attention and cue him to play softly. He responds positively, playing the temple blocks in a softer and related manner (demonstrating some range of expressiveness regarding dynamics). During this interaction, it appears that the combination of musical form and change of affect lead Timmy into: 1) an increase in musical and interpersonal awareness; 2) an increase in his level of relatedness, in that his playing became more organized and connected; 3) a synchronization and integration of multisensory experiences within a musical context as he watched and listened while playing; 4) a display of some capacity to problem-solve throughout the interaction within a musical context (e.g., adjusting his playing to match the music in regards to maintaining tempo and dynamic level; copying Jeans cymbal play and later incorporating it into musical play on his own initiative); and 5) the ability to find and play the basic beat. Although several musical changes occurr during this interaction, the establishment of the basic beat and the experience of musical mutuality for the first time would be important in laying the groundwork for developing a musical relationship. According to Nordoff and Robbins (2007):

110

The basic beat is the pulse that carries the flow of music in time, the underlying time base of coherent musical activity and experience, and the foundation of rhythmic order. Two individuals responding to the pulse together are experiencing the most universally natural way of finding mutuality in being physically active to music (p. 298). DVD Cut 13: Good Morning to Percy: Initiating and Expressing an Idea Cut 13 is taken from session thirteen. It illustrates the continuous development of our relationship and shows signs of Timmys ability to spontaneously create an idea and express it in the context of what is happening in the moment. In addition, he demonstrates longer periods of eye-contact and engagement with me, and for the first time, he is smiling during our interactions. Session thirteen begins with Timmy entering the music room with a doll, which he has named Percy. This is the first time that he brings Percy into music. After he tells Jean and me the dolls name after my asking, I begin to play our familiar greeting song, as of session eleven, of: Were back in the music. As I play the song, I try to evoke some kind of vocal response from him, specifically by filling in names of who is in the music room. Timmy does not respond vocally, however, he maintains eye-contact with me and gives me a little smile. As I continue to sing hello, including Percys name, Timmy begins to sing over me, initiating an entirely new idea within the context of hello by singing: Good morning to Percy its nice to see you. He is vocalizing in a mono-tone voice with limited affect. I pick up on his words and quickly change the music to accompany his singing/talking.

111

DVD Cut 14: Good Morning to Percy Part II Cut 14 from the same session (cut 14) illustrates an increase in Timmys ability to engage and relate during our greeting song, as I extend the song to include everyones name, as Timmy was fixed on only saying/singing good morning to Percy. The video clip begins with Jean holding the doll (Percy), pretending to have it play the tambourine. After Timmy sings the phrase once, I asked him to keep going, in which he continues to sing good morning to Percy. I begin improvising a B section while focusing Timmy back into singing. DVD Cut 15: Fun for Four Drums: Engagement, Flexibility, Problem-solving, and a Continuous Flow of Communication Cut 15 is taken from session seventeen. It illustrates Timmys ability to focus, engage, and relate within a pre-composed song: Fun for Four Drums by Nordoff and Robbins (1968). The premise of this activity is that each of the four drums is cued to a distinct piano accompaniment. In order for the child to play the correct drum and rhythm, he must be able to discriminate each of the four piano accompaniments. Although we have used this song with Timmy for three consecutive sessions prior, each time we present it, we alter and vary parts in order to keep him alert, anticipating and constantly thinking about which drum and rhythm to play. During this interaction Jean is playing the piano, as I felt that I needed to lead the activity from the floor, to change the musical ideas around based on Timmys responses. In addition, the schools Floortime coach is observing the session.

112

The fourth excerpt begins with me kneeling on the floor and making sure Timmy and I are on the same eye level before beginning the activity. As the song moves into drumming (one by one, by one, by one), I change the order of the drums. Throughout the excerpt I try to be unpredictable in both the singing and drum playing sections; Timmy responds positively. The accompaniment for drum one occurs, I look at drum one, and Timmy plays the correct beat (watching me attentively). Drum one is complete as we go back to the chorus, as I use my body for cueing and jumping to the drum. DVD Cut 16: Fun for Four Drums Part II Cut 16, from the same session and activity, illustrates Timmy on drum two, as I am directing Jean on how loudly and quickly to play. Timmy displays his ability to alter the tempo and dynamics based on Jeans playing. We begin to combine drums one and two, and Timmy is able to distinguish the differences in the piano accompaniment as he plays in the correct manner. DVD Cut 17: Fun for Four Drums Part III Cut 17, taken from the same session and activity, shows Timmy determining drum patterns for the three different drums based on the piano accompaniment. During the interaction, he is concentrating and listening, correcting himself to play the appropriate drum. Stage Three: Spontaneous Music Making for Higher Levels of Interaction During sessions eighteen to twenty-five, the focus is primarily on spontaneous music making, in which the aim is to have Timmy engage in give-and-take musical experiences, relying on an enhanced musical sensitivity to play in a related and communicative manner. This would provide Timmy with opportunities to continue to develop and strengthen his ability to partake in higher levels of musical interaction, such as: initiating and closing circles of musical

113

dialogue, connecting his musical ideas with my ideas, copying a musical idea and then incorporating it into the musical play, and displaying flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo. DVD Cut 18: Exploring and Initiating Musical Changes on the Piano: Musical Communicativeness and Interrelatedness Cut 18 of Timmy is taken from the twenty-fourth session. It illustrates Timmy exploring his touch and the dynamic range on the piano in a communicative, interrelated and interresposnsive manner. In addition, he displays his spontaneity, musical sensibility, and his ability to initiate a musical idea and connect it to mine. The excerpt begins with Timmy initiating gentle clusters in a legato fashion within a nonfixed tempo on the piano. I accompany his playing by moving between pentatonic and diatonic harmony. Timmy then initiates an accelerando followed by a decrescendo. He then begins to play the black keys in a staccato and detached manner. This is followed by Timmy playing clusters in an increasing tempo and dynamic range. I accompany his playing, adding syncopation, as Timmy looks to me with a big smile. While accompanying, I incorporate silences for Timmy to play through. He immediately realizes that his music is filling in mine, as he momentarily stops before each pause before filling in. DVD Cut 19: Thats Beautiful Music: Exploring the Elements Cut 19 of Timmy is also taken from session twenty-four. It illustrates Timmy continuously exploring musical elements while playing the piano in a related and interactive manner. It also shows Timmy using musical interresponsively, taking an idea of mine and bridging it with an idea of his own.

114

The excerpt begins with Timmy playing chord clusters in a very gentle and purposeful manner, as he brings his whole body into the movement while playing. At times he looks over to me and alters the dynamic level of his playing as I accompany and sing, Thats beautiful music. This is followed by Timmy playing the basic beat, and then initiating a ritardando at the end of the phrase before exploring single tones while watching my hands play the piano. DVD Cut 20: Consolidating: Piecing it all Together through Spontaneous Music Making Cut 20 of Timmy is taken from session twenty-five. It illustrates Timmy applying his musical and communicative skills in a spontaneous, related, and expressive manner in his playing (displaying freedom, musical sensibility, and mutuality). In addition, he appears to be more emotionally connected while playing, as he smiles, smirks, and displays a range of expressiveness (tempo and dynamics shifts). In addition, the excerpts display moments of musical give-and-take between Timmy and me, as he initiates as well as follows musical changes. Cut 20 begins with Timmy playing the drum and cymbal, playing the basic beat on the drum and cymbal, as Jean begins singing. He momentarily gets distracted, and begins playing again, and joins right into the basic beat, while turning his head in my direction. I begin to change the accents while incorporating syncopation in my playing. Timmy notices this. He initiates a slower tempo, and then appears to change the rhythm of his playing based on my playing. He then initiates a tempo change; the music follows his change. This is followed by Timmy initiating a ritardando; the music now becomes grander and more dramatic, and then slows down gradually until Timmy stops playing. The music continues, and I begin to play a trill, waiting for him to begin playing again. I play a loud bass note, in a dramatic fashion;

115

Timmy follows this and beats on the drum and cymbal as the interaction continues. At the end of the clip Timmy and I engage in rhythm imitation. DVD Cut 21: Consolidating Part II Cut 21, also from session twenty-five, begins with me improvising music that contains a lot of energy and affect, expressing urgency in order to engage Timmy in musical play. He appears to be having a difficult time gaining focus, however, as the music builds and then begins to slow down, he smiles and joins into playing the basic beat. This is followed by a pause, as I wait for him to initiate a vocalization. During the pause, there is a nice moment of Timmy and me staring and smiling at each other, before engaging in the music. While Timmy continued to receive music therapy, the 25th session was the final clinical session of the present research protocol. The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale post-test was administered, and Goal Attainment Scoring followed Timmys final session. Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) In addition to the qualitative case study analysis of Timmys treatment process, two quantitative measures were used to evaluate outcomes of music therapy: the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) and Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). The FEAS (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001) was used as a pre- and post-test measure for Timmys progress in achieving social-emotional goals. The GAS (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994) was used to evaluate Timmys progress toward musical goals. Table 4 gives a comparison of Timmys pre- and post-test scores on the FEAS. The table shows raw scores for each developmental area, sub-scores, level of functioning, percentages, and

116

the number of functioning levels changed out of three possible levels (e.g., deficient, at risk, and normal). Scoring is based on a 2-point scale: 0 = behavior is not seen or is briefly observed, 1 = behavior is present some of the time or observed several times, and 2 = behavior is consistently observed or observed many times. The ratings were summed to obtain subtest scores for the child, as well as total test scores (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). Total scores are compared to cutoff scores (see Appendix A) to determine if the child is: normal, at-risk or deficient. Table 4 Comparison of Timmys Pre- and Post FEAS Scores Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 10 (Deficient) (71.4%) Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 13 (Normal) (92.8%)
2 2 2 2 1 2 2

Areas I. Self-regulation

Change in Functioning Level +2

Attentive to play with toys 2 Explores objects/toys freely 2 Remains calm during play 2 Touching textured toys & caregiver 0 Shows content affect 1 Focused without distraction 2 Appears withdrawn/sluggish 1

II. Forming relationships and engagement


Emotional interest in care-giver Relaxed when near care-giver Anticipates with curiosity Uncomfortable with care-giver Initiates closeness to care-giver Avoids care-giver Socially references care-giver Communicates from across space

12 (Deficient) (75%)
2 2 0 2 2 1 1 2

16 (Normal) (100%)
2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2

+2

117

Table 4. (Continued) ___________________________________________________________________________ Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 5 (Deficient) (50%)


1 1 1 2

Areas III. Two-way purposeful Communication


Opens circles of communication Initiates intentional actions Closes circles of communication Uses sounds/words/gestures

Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 8 (Normal) (80%)


2 2 2 2

Change in Functioning Level +2

IV. Behavioral organization 3 (Normal) and problem-solving (75%)


Communicates in several modes Copies caregiver & incorporates 1 1

4 (Normal) (100%)
1 1

V. Representational capacity
Engages in symbolic play Engages in pretend play Communicates intentions Expresses dependency Expresses pleasure/excitement Expresses assertiveness Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas

5 (Deficient) 35.71%
1 2 0 0 0 2 0

14 (Normal) 100%
2 2 2 2 2 2 2

+2

VI. Representational differentiation

0 (Deficient) 0%

7 (Normal) 70%
2 2 2 0 1

+2

Bridges 2 unrealistic ideas 0 Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 Use pretend to express dependency 0 Use pretend to express pleasure 0 Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0

Based on Table 4, Timmys ability to self-regulate and show interest in the world (area I) increased by three points bringing his total score from 10 to 13. His ability to be engaged (area II) also increased by four points bringing his total score from 12 to 16. In addition, Timmy 118

showed improvement in his ability to communicate purposefully (area III), increasing his score by three points bringing his total score from 5 to 8. He demonstrated improvement in his ability to problem solve (area IV), increasing his score by one point, bringing his total score from 3 to 4. Furthermore, Timmys ability to think symbolically (area V) increased by nine points bringing his total score from 5 to 14, and finally, Timmy exhibited improvement in his ability to build bridges between ideas (area VI) increasing his score by seven points bringing his total score from 0 to 7. In clinical terms, during the pretest, Timmy was regulated, but appeared to be withdrawn, ambivalent, and not focused on any particular thing. He showed difficulty in his ability to engage and relate to the therapist, and generally reverted to scripts, fragmenting the flow of interaction. In addition, he showed limited ability to use communication intentionally, and showed difficulty initiating ideas and circles of communication. He displayed the ability to problem-solve and organize behaviorally. However, he demonstrated difficulty in his ability to think symbolically and connect his ideas to the therapists ideas. During the post test, he continued to demonstrate the ability to be regulated, however, he was now more focused and attentive to the therapist, and displayed the ability to engage in a related manner. He also exhibited the ability to be more intentional and purposeful while communicating, using fewer scripts, and exhibiting verbal and gestural language appropriate to the situation at hand. In addition, he displayed the capacity to create and build bridges between ideas, engaged in symbolic play, and showed islands in the capacity to think abstractly, as evidenced by answering why and when questions, and getting more in-depth during play.

119

Figure 5 shows the amount of change in functioning levels made by Timmy in each developmental area: 1 represents deficient, 2 at risk, and 3 normal. As shown, Timmy improved by two functioning levels in five areas, and stayed the same in one area. Figure 5 Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS)
Changes in Developmental Levels
Pre 2 Post

L e v e l s

Area I

Area II

Area III

Area IV

Area V

Area VI

Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) The quantitative data analyzed below used GAS to evaluate progress made toward Timmys musical goals. Musical goals for Timmy were created following session two, by the independent observer, and then were evaluated after session 25. Five music therapy (MT) goals were formulated for Timmy, all were weighted equally. The first MT goal for Timmy was musical interrelatedness, that is for him to be able to connect a musical idea, of his own, to the therapists idea, and elaborate on the idea during musical play. At the start of music therapy, Timmys music was interrelated to the therapists music less than expected (-1); he rarely played in an interrelated manner. However, upon completion of therapy,

120

he exhibited the most favorable outcome (+2); he consistently played in an interrelated way throughout the session. The second MT goal was musical interresponsiveness, that is for him to be able imitate or copy the therapists musical idea, and then incorporate it into musical play. At the start of music therapy, Timmy exhibited the most unfavorable outcome (-2); he never played in an interresponsive manner. Upon the completion of music therapy, Timmy displayed a more than expected outcome level (+1); his playing was interresponsive to the therapists music often throughout the session. Timmys third goal was relationship within musical play, which involves him being able to initiate some kind of relationship with music and the therapist, vocally and/or instrumentally and/or through movement/gesture. Upon beginning music therapy, Timmy exhibited a less then expected outcome (-1); he rarely initiated relationship within music. However, upon completion of therapy he displayed the most favorable outcome (+2); he initiated relationship within musical play consistently throughout the session. The fourth MT goal for Timmy was musical communicativeness, that is, for for him to be able to be spontaneously communicative in relation to the therapists music, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play (e.g., call-and-response interplay, punctuate the end of a phrase, initiate the opening of a phrase; and/or notice the therapists music and respond contingently by elaborating on it). Upon beginning music therapy, Timmys musical communicativeness showed a less than expected outcome (-1); rarely using music communicatively. At the conclusion of therapy he exhibited a more than expected outcome (+1), often using music communicatively throughout the session.

121

Finally, Timmys fifth MT goal was range of expressiveness, that is for him to be able to play using some kind of range in tempo and/or dynamics during musical play. Upon beginning music therapy, he exhibited the most unfavorable outcome level (-2) in being able to play with a range of expressiveness, never playing using a range of expressiveness. After completing therapy, however, he displayed the expected outcome level (0), playing expressively sometimes throughout the session. Figure 6 shows all four goal areas and the amount of change Timmy displayed as a result of music therapy. The figure shows raw scores for each goal area (-2 represents most unfavorable outcome; -1 less than expected outcome; 0 expected level; +1 more than expected success; and +2 most favorable outcome). Taking all of these goals collectively, Timmy advanced 4 levels in one goal area, 3 levels in two and 2 levels in two. These changed scores on all goal areas were converted to a Tscore, according to the formula of Kiresuk and Sherman (1968). Timmys T-score was 68.09, based on the assumption that together these scales have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (see Kiresuk & Sherman).

122

Figure 6 Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS)


Changes in Goal Attainment Levels
Pre G A S L e v e L s Post

2 1 0 -1 -2
Musical Interrelatedness Relationship in Musical Play Musical Communicativeness Musical Interresponsivess Range of Expressiveness

Musical Goal Areas

Summary Client Process Stage one, Interrupting Musical Flow: Who is this Boy? consisted of the first 10 sessions, where Timmy displayed difficulty engaging and relating to the therapists music for extended periods during musical play. During this stage, it became difficult to develop a musical or interpersonal relationship with Timmy, as he continuously interrupted musical experiences, impeding his ability to engage in a continuous flow of musical dialogue with the therapist and intern. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies and implemented improvisation within predictable musical structures and activities in order increase relatedness during musical play. Stage two, Building Blocks for the Relationship: Balancing between Predictability and Unpredictability, consisted of seven sessions. During this time, through familiar musical

123

structures and activities, Timmy became increasingly engaged, related and communicative with the therapist and intern during musical play. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies, eliminating activity-based-structures in order to offer improvisational musical experiences that would facilitate musical spontaneity and higher levels of musical interaction. Finally, the last eight sessions, stage three, Spontaneous Music Making for Higher Levels of Interaction, consisted of Timmy engaging in more spontaneous musical interactions with the therapist and intern. To that end, he engaged in higher levels of musical interaction that included: spontaneity, flexibility, freedom, and displayed a wider range of affect and expressiveness during musical play with the therapist and intern. Therapist Method Because of Timmys learning style based on ABA, familiar songs and musical structures were used, specifically, Fun for Four Drums (Nordoff & Robbins, 1968) to create familiarity and predictability. This educational approach that required learning musical parts, helped Timmy develop: the ability to listen and respond to musical cues, auditory discrimination, and the ability to maintain engagement during musical play. It also created the foundation of our musical and interpersonal relationship. The skill set that Timmy acquired from this educational approach provided him with the tools needed to engage in more spontaneous music making in a related and expressive manner, that is a therapeutic approach. In short, at the onset, Timmy had a difficult time engaging in musical play though improvisation because it appeared that he did not have the social-emotional skills needed to engage in improvisational music making (without the use of prompts). In our bridging education with therapy, Timmy became more spontaneous,

124

expressive, communicative and creative during musical play, in that he learned and assimilated the skills necessary to increase his ability to relate and communicate. Relationship Between FEAS and GAS Results of Timmys FEAS pre-test and GAS indicated similarities across domains. During both procedures (FEAS and GAS), he displayed the ability to be self-regulated (calm) while playing and exploring both toys and instruments; however, he exhibited difficulty in the ability to use them (toys and instruments) in a related and interactive manner. He presented with a neutral affect, a limited range of emotion and lacked spontaneity during play (musical and interpersonal). In both settings, he avoided the therapist, isolating himself during play. In addition, Timmy demonstrated difficulty in the ability to open (initiating) and close circles of communication both musically and interpersonally, and consistently interrupted musical and interpersonal interactions by engaging in stereotypic behavior patterns (e.g., playing loudly, collecting all of the mallets, lining up toys, and moving aimlessly around the room from toy to toy and instrument to instrument). Finally, during both procedures, Timmy exhibited difficulty in creating and bridging ideas with the therapists on musical and interpersonal levels. Inconsistencies if the FEAS and GAS existed in the area of problem-solving (level IV). FEAS scored Timmy in the normal range on level IV, problem-solving; however, during GAS, he displayed difficultly in his ability to problem-solve during music making (e.g., he didnt appear to grasp the concepts of cause and effect play, call-and-response dialogue, and ways to alter the elements in order to meet the music of the therapist).

125

The progress that Timmy displayed in the FEAS post-test was also reflected in the Goal Attainment Scoring. Results indicated progress in Timmys ability to engage and relate musically and interpersonally. He also exhibited progress in his ability to open (initiate) and close circles communication and engage in a continuous flow of interaction in a spontaneous and flexible manner, musically and interpersonally. In addition, Timmy showed improvements in his ability to: initiate and connect his ideas with the therapists, both musically and interpersonally, be symbolic, and engage in pretend play. Relationship between Qualitative and Quantitative Data Timmys areas of strengths and weakness were consistent in both the qualitative and quantitative data of the pre-test in each of the six levels of development, with exception to area IV (problem-solving). During the FEAS pre-test, Tommy got better scores because he was able to behaviorally organize and understand how to get his needs met; however, it was not a social or relational means of problem-solving, but rather a memorized sequence of figuring things out. In short, the quantitative test score showed that Timmy appeared to have a repertoire of memorized scenarios in which he understands the effect based on a certain cause, however, qualitatively, he has not generalized cause and effect thinking into other situations (this is characteristic of ABA learning). To that end, it appeared that the quantitative component may not be reflective of the quality (memorized or spontaneous interactions in the moment). Although the quantitative data indicated that he was responding appropriately (in context), according to what the therapist is asking, the data do not seem to be reflective of the in-the-moment interaction.

126

CHAPTER 7 RESULTS: MELINDA Melinda, five years of age at the time of this study, was the product of an induced labor and Cesarean section delivery; she weighed 9 lbs., 8 oz. at birth. The prenatal maternal history was significant for gestational diabetes and antiviral and antidepressant medication. In addition, Melindas medical history was also significant for frequent colds and coughs; early ear infections (three to four by age 2); scarlet fever at age two, with fever above 104 degrees; and a head injury, also at age two, when she fell out of bed. Early development history revealed significant delays in her physical, communication and language development. At age three, Melinda was diagnosed with Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD). As a result she received Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) as well as occupational and speech therapies through Early Intervention. Results from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-III) and Standford-Binet-IV, revealed that Melinda had significant difficulties in her receptive and expressive language skills as well as her ability to attend to and process information. In addition, the Vineland Behavior Scales-II (VABS-II), completed by Melindas mother, revealed low to moderately low global adaptive functioning, specifically in the areas of: expressive (ageequivalent six months) and receptive language (age-equivalent ten months), self-care skills (ageequivalent twenty months), daily living skills (age-equivalent twelve months), interpersonal relationships (age-equivalent three months), and gross (age-equivalent thirty months) and fine motor skills (age-equivalent twenty-five months). Melinda is an only child, currently living with her mother in New York City. Her father

127

resides in Nevada. Prior to entering Rebecca School, Melinda was enrolled at another school where she received occupational and physical therapies fours times per week, speech therapy seven times per week, and Applied Behavioral Analysis 10 hours per week. Melinda has been attending the Rebecca School since September 2007, where she is receiving occupational therapy four times per week, physical therapy three times per week, speech therapy three times per week, and music therapy two times per week all for 30 minutes. DIR Profile The following DIR profile is based on: 1) pre-test scores of the FEAS (see table 1); 2) interviews of staff; 3) and observations of Melinda in various settings throughout the school day, including the classroom, occupational therapy, and speech therapy: Melinda presents with both an under-reactive and over-reactive sensory system (mixedreactivity). To that end, she may withdraw from interactions by becoming self-absorbed (underreactive) or by being in constant motion, seeking sensory stimulation, and screaming (overreactive). (Both under-reactivity and over-reactivity are forms of dysregulation). Melinda displays difficulty in her ability to process sensory stimuli. She can become easily distracted by external stimuli, both visual and auditory, and becomes over stimulated in noisy environments. She exhibits difficulty maintaining self-regulation (area I) and engagement during peer interactions within the classroom, constantly seeking vestibular, tactile and proprioceptive stimulation, or by becoming self-absorbed. When she becomes dysregulated, in an under-reactive manner, she requires high-affect sensory motor-play in order to become regulated and available for interaction. High-affect and sensory-motor play appears to up-regulate Melindas sensory system, making her available for

128

engagement; however, at times, because of her difficulty with sensory and emotional modulation, this strategy can also dysregulate her in an over-reactive manner. At times when Melinda is provided with tactile and vestibular sensory stimulation, she may display the ability to engage in two-way purposeful communication for very brief moments (two to three circles of communication). In addition, because of her regulatory difficulties, her ability to maintain engagement (areas II); and two-way purposeful communication (area III) is limited, and impedes her ability to engage in higher levels of thinking. In addition, at times Melinda requires a great deal of proprioceptive sensory stimulation throughout the day such as: deep body pressure, vibration on her arms and legs, and joint compression to facilitate self-organization and self-regulation. She also enjoys and will seek out activities that provide vestibular sensory stimulation, such as riding on various swings, and swinging in various body socks. Furthermore, Melinda exhibits difficulty in processing auditory stimuli (expressive and receptive language), and as a result will either get agitated and melt-down, or, become self absorbed (under-reactive) during activities. During play she is self-directed; however, she exhibits difficulty interacting with her peers and will get frustrated and withdraw when others attempt to play with her. Finally, Melinda exhibits difficulty with motor-planning skills and requires verbal and visual prompt in order to carry out a two-step activity. In addition, she demonstrates limited flexibility in her play repertoire and will often become frustrated when she is unable to execute her desired motor plan. She displays low muscle tone and poor body awareness, and as a result has difficulty completing physical and gross motor activities.

129

Music Therapy Assessment The music therapy assessment, guided by Greenspan and Weiders Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL) (2006a), focused on seven areas of musical responsiveness (see Materials in Method Chapter). The music therapy room was set up with a variety of percussive and melodic instruments, an assortment of different size mallets with various handle textures, pitched horns, and a variety of sensory based items. The assessment focused on Melindas ability to interact during musical play with the therapist. Melindas assessment took place during the course of two 30 minute sessions. During the assessment, Melindas primary modes of interacting were through reflexive vocalizations and brief, fleeting (24 beats at a time) moments of playing the snare drum and cymbal during musical play. Initially, Melinda displayed difficulty leaving her classroom to come to music, as her teacher escorted her to the music room. Typically, she entered the room with ambivalence, and showed fleeting moments of interest in the instruments, specifically the drum and cymbal (with visual and hand-over-hand support). Although Melinda displayed some sense of musical awarenessvocalizing tonally, playing in tempo on the drum and cymbal, and on occasion changing her facial expressions when the music became dramatic and emotionally chargedher responses (musical and nonmusical) were generally reflexive and lacked intentionality. In addition, Melinda avoided any interpersonal contact with Jean (intern) and me. She rarely referenced or engaged us in eye-contact, and never responded in any manner to greetings or good-bye (musically and interpersonally).

130

With a lot of support, through hand-over prompting and the use of high musical and interpersonal affect (using a wide range of dynamics, dissonance, unfixed tempos), Melinda displayed the ability to engage in shared attention (area I) and musical engagement and relatedness (area II) on the drum and cymbal for brief moments consisting of 24 beats. Following these fleeting moments of musical contact, Melinda withdrew from engagement by either physically leaving the instrument, resisting hand-over-hand support, becoming selfabsorbed or by melting down. During the second assessment session, Melinda spent a great deal of time attempting to hoard and collect the instruments and mallets. When Jean (intern) tried to take something away, or place instruments out of her reach, Melinda would get frustrated, begin screaming, and drop to the floor while kicking the instrument cabinet. After she attempted to collect all of the instruments in sight, she surrounded herself with them or placed them in her mouth. She rarely attempted to use the instruments in a functional, musical manner. It appeared that Melinda did not understand the purpose of the instruments. The concept that these objects (instruments) made sounds, appeared to be too abstract for her. To that end, it appeared that her difficulty with cause and effect thinking (inability to understand that her actions have an impact on the world), and poor self-awareness (emotionally and physically) impeded her ability to grasp the concept of musical play between herself and someone else. In summary, Melindas difficulties in her ability to self-regulate, process sensory information and motor planning impeded her capacity to engage in musical play in a related and communicative manner. In addition, these biological factors also obstructed her ability to grasp the concept of back-and-forth dialogue (musically or interpersonally). To that end, it appeared

131

that Melinda was experiencing a neurological disconnect between sensation (sensory), affect, and motor movement. Greenspan and Shanker (2004) write: When biological factors interfere with the formation of the critical sensory-affect motor connection, the infant and child has difficulty successfully negotiating the developmental pathways leading to emotional intellectual capacities. Our observations suggest that this is what happens in many children with autistic spectrum disorders, there may not be a total lack of connection between the sensory-affect and motor system but rather a relative one. This relative lack of sensory-affect-motor connections make it difficult for affect to guide motor actions and, therefore, we observe a compromised ability for complex, purposeful, meaningful behavior (p. 304). In addition, Melindas difficulty in her sensory-affect motor connection impinged on her ability to understand cause and effect relationships, which in turn affected her capacity in developing a sense of self and problem-solving skills. Following the assessment sessions, the researcher and independent observer formulated a GAS for Melinda (see Appendix G). Treatment Melindas treatment consisted of twenty-five sessions over a five month period. During this time, Melinda passed through three distinct stages. In the first stage, her difficulties in her ability to maintain self-regulation and to understand the functional use of the musical instruments impeded her ability to engage in musical play. In the second stage, Melinda became increasingly calm, musically engaged and related, and self-aware in regards to her role in the musical

132

relationship. Finally, in the third stage Melinda became increasingly aware of her surroundings and her impact on the world, which led to her being more spontaneous, playful, communicative and expressive during musical play. Stage One: Self-Regulation and Interest in the World: Adjusting to Individual-Differences Stage one consisted of sessions one through nine. This stage involved Jean and I making adjustments in our patterns of interacting with Melinda to facilitate her ability to self-regulate and engage, interpersonally and musically. Because of Melindas complex sensory system, it was difficult to engage her in musical play. At times she became over-reactive to the sensory stimuli in the environment by dysregulating and running around room; at times she became underreactive to the situation by dysregulating and becoming self-absorbed. Generally, Melinda required a teacher escort to accompany her to music. Usually, once the teacher left the room, Melinda had difficulty remaining calm and available (self-regulated) for engagement. To that end, she would either meltdown by throwing herself to the ground and jerking her head in a rapid and back-and-forth manner, or, become self-absorbed, by isolating herself from interacting, musically or interpersonally. In addition, she rarely socially referenced Jean or me, appearing as though she was completely unaware of our presence. Musically, she displayed difficulty engaging with the instruments in a functional manner and showed little awareness of the music being played. At times I played very dynamic and loud music on a variety of instruments (piano, drums or cymbals) and tried to utilize the element of surprise both musically and interpersonally to engage her; however, she appeared as though she was not processing the sound (sitting in her chair self-

133

absorbed and staring into space). In short, while she was apparently hearing the sounds, she was unable: to connect what she was hearing to an appropriate sensorimotor response, to understand that the sounds were inviting a response, and/or to make a distinction between the musical sounds and outside auditory stimuli. At times when Melinda engaged in musical play with support (high-affect, hand-overhand techniques, and prompting though gestures), she displayed some sense of musical awareness, rhythmically and tonally; however, her responses appeared to be reflexive and lacked the intent to be related or communicative. The plan was to utilize highly supported (visual and gestural prompt, hand-over-hand prompt and high-affect) musical interactions to help Melinda work through motor-planning and processing difficulties to facilitate greater musical awareness, engagement, and relatedness. At the same time, I began to limit the number of instruments in the room. I also reminded Jean (intern) not to allow Melinda to hoard the instruments, and if need be, to take them away from her in a playful and interactive manner. Furthermore, another strategy employed to facilitate engagement and affective interactions was to have Jean use the instruments to tease Melinda in a playful manner. Although that strategy resulted in several meltdowns, it also provided for spontaneous engagement and a back-and-forth dialogue between Jean and Melinda. DVD Cut 22: Engagement: Working in and through a Meltdown Cut 22 is taken from session 5. It demonstrates Jean musically teasing Melinda with the snare drum, creating an interactive game of chase as Melinda becomes upset and agitated. The cut begins with Melinda attempting to get the snare drum from Jean. Jean holds onto the drum in a playful manner and begins running around the room as Melinda chases Jean,

134

becoming increasingly upset, screaming and finally falling to the floor. Jean brings the drum in front of Melinda, lowering it in time with the dissonant sounding music, in order to facilitate some sort of musical interaction. Jean follows the music using her singing voice to create highaffect in order to engage Melinda. While Melinda is on the floor she appears to be self-regulated momentarily and attuned to what Jean is doing, as she watches the drum being lowered. In addition, at times it seems as though Melinda is not lunging at the drum to take it, but rather to poke it as Jean lows it. Finally, Melinda lifts herself up from the floor and once again becomes agitated and begins to chase Jean. During this interaction, although Melinda is upset and at times dysregulated, she is also engaged in brief moments of two-way communication, musically and interpersonally, as Jean has something that she wants. DVD Cut 23: Self-absorption into Interaction: An Opening to Musical Awareness and Relationship Cut 23, from session eight illustrates Melinda moving from being dysregulated in an under-reactive manner (self-absorbed and withdrawn) to becoming regulated (available for interaction), engaged and related (interpersonally and musically). In addition, for the first time, Melinda displays an emotional connection with me, as she smiles and intently watches me throughout the interaction. The cut begins with me moving from the piano to the drum, positioning myself directly in front of her, as I sing and play the drum and cymbal, attempting to evoke any kind of emotional, social, or musical response. Initially, Melinda is self-absorbed, and appears to be staring into space. The moment

135

I begin to use my high pitched voice playfully and slowly lift the cymbal, she begins to smile at me. I place a mallet in her hand and provide hand-over-hand support. She plays the cymbal while smiling. She then plays the cymbal with her own initiative, following a short musical phrase that I sing. All along, Melinda is watching me and smiling. When I move back to the piano, she continues to watch and smile in my direction, and begins to play the cymbal on her own initiative while Jean holds it. DVD Cut 24: Connecting Verbal Jargon: Snap-shots of Engagement leading to Musical Awareness Cut 24, from session nine, illustrates Melinda engaging in moments of musical relatedness after being engaged in several isolated behaviors, such as pacing, running, twirling and making random sounds (jargon). The cut begins with Jean and I attempting to transform Melindas verbal jargon into an intentional and related musical interaction. At times Melinda becomes connected musically and interpersonally (walking toward Jean to explore the cymbal momentarily), as she repeats some sounds that Jean is making. The interactions are intermittent, and Melinda displays difficulty maintaining focus, drifting between being regulated and dysregulated. At the end of this cut, Melinda stops moving, turns to look at me, and at the very moment that I pause the music, she comes over to me and expressively vocalizes in an intentional and communicative manner. Stage Two: Cause and Effect Thinking, and the Emergence of the Self: Making Meaning of Related Music Making Sessions ten through eighteen, Stage two, displayed Melindas social-emotional development in several areas, including, developing an understanding of: 1) the purpose and

136

functional use of the musical instruments; 2) the social dynamics within musical play, such as musical communicativeness (e.g., in that two or more people are interacting together, and that each individual has a place in the musical whole): 3) cause and effect thinking; and 4) the impact that she has on something outside of herself within the context of musical play (sense of self). To that end, other islands of social-emotional capacities were beginning to manifest during musical play, such as her ability to: engage musically in a related manner while initiating interactions within the musical relationship. In addition, because the instruments now had newly found meaning for Melinda, she stopped attempting to collect and hoard them, using them functionally and socially within the context of musical play. Furthermore, although we continued to use various forms of prompting (visual, gestural and musical), Jean and I held back from using hand-over-hand prompt as a way to facilitate an increase in Melindas ability to initiate play based on her own ideas and motor movements (motor planning). DVD Cut 25: Connecting Sensory (music), Affect (emotion), and Motor though Cymbal Play: Musical Relatedness and Two-way Purposeful Interaction Cut 25 is taken from Melindas tenth session. It illustrates Melindas ability to engage and make meaning of the musical play as she begins to make the connection, neurologically, between music, emotion (experiencing joy in the interaction), and motor planning (playing the cymbal with emotional connection to the interaction with Jean, me and the music). In addition, she displays some ability to anticipate and motor plan within the tempo of the interaction, and at times exhibited the ability to understand that she was controlling the music.

137

The cut begins with Jean singing in an ascending manner and accompanying that movement with the raising of the cymbal, creating affect and anticipation so as to prepare Melinda to play. Melinda, smiling throughout the interaction, is enjoying the manner in which Jean is presenting the cymbal, as the music is used to organize and support the interaction. Musically, I am playing the piano to accent Melindas playing, enhancing her produced sounds and reinforcing what she plays, while developing the musical phrase for the next musical question. Melinda responds by punctuating on the cymbal, completing each phrase, as Jean brings the cymbal to her and then gradually moves it away while singing. Melinda enjoys this and is smiling while anticipating the cymbal coming back. Finally, at the end of the cut, Melinda, gets up off of her chair to initiate play on the cymbal, hitting it once before returning to her chair and repeating the interaction. DVD Cut 26: Exploring the Drum and Cymbal: Shared Attention and Musical Relatedness Session thirteen, cut 26, displays Melinda exploring the drum through various types of beating (e.g., using two arms together and alternating between right and left arms) during musical play. In addition, she demonstrates the ability to organize her beating within the musical structure and incorporates the cymbal and floor-tom based on her own initiative. She appears to be more aware of her environment, and although at times she is split off from the interaction, she regains focus and re-enters into the musical relationship on her own initiative. The cut begins at the beginning of the session, as Melinda is carried in by her teacher, Rebecca. The moment Rebecca places Melinda down; she immediately comes to the snare drum while smiling and begins to play. I was completely surprised by Melindas self-initiated drum

138

play, as I begin to fumble musically, attempting to meet her in the music. I shift my playing from legato to a more detached musical form in order to meet her drumming. Melinda and I finally meet in the basic beat. I follow this by incorporating stops into the music; Melinda plays through the stops, beating with both arms simultaneously. She then changes her beating and begins to beat by alternating her arms in sequential manner, maintaining the basic beat. She briefly stops playing as the music continues. She sees the cymbal, plays it, and then incorporates it within the snare drum play, exercising her ability to plan motorically. Melinda then stops playing again, as the music continues. She then begins to beat in an unorganized manner, visually spotting the floor tom from a distance, running over to it, playing it quickly, and then returning to the snare drum (creating an idea and carrying it out within the musical interaction). This is followed by her playing the cymbal, while socially referencing both Jean and me, before coming back to the snare drum, and organizing her beating as the music repeats the original theme. She then stops playing, runs over to Jean, and then on her own initiative, comes back to engage in drum and cymbal play. DVD Cut 27: Cause and Effect Relating though Horn Blowing Cut 27 is from session eighteen; it illustrates Melinda exploring the horn for the first time. In past sessions, she used the horn as an object to self-stimulate, focusing on its shiny and reflective qualities. During this interaction, she figures out two ways of making sounds with the horn. One way requires her to blow inward, the other outward. In addition, while engaged in musical play on the horn, she constantly references Jean, watching her throughout the interaction.

139

The cut begins with Melinda attempting to take Jeans horn away. When she is unable to take Jeans horn, she walks away, and then returns to her seat on her own initiative, while offering Jean a mischievous smile as she avoids interacting with her. Once seated, however, Melinda and Jean begin interacting with their horns within the music. Melinda displays excitement about her accomplishment of blowing the horn by making a celebratory vocalization. This is followed by Melinda withdrawing from the interaction and then returning back on her own initiative while vocalizing a word approximating horn, after I sing you can blow Stage Three: Exploring her Impact on the World: Musical Interresponsiveness and Communicativeness Stage three was comprised of sessions nineteen through twenty-five. During this stage, although Melinda continued to display difficulty in maintaining long continuous flows of musical dialogue, the quality of her relatedness improved into higher levels of musical interactions through a range of affects. To that end, Melinda displayed the ability to imitate or copy an idea and incorporate it into the context of music play (musical interresponsiveness). In addition, she displayed the ability to initiate ideas and open and close circles of communication (musical communicativeness) while expanding her emotional range in the context of musical play. (It is important to point out, that although Melinda displayed the ability to engage on a higher level of thinking, both socially and emotionally, she continued to exhibit difficulty in consistently engaging in this manner) DVD Cut 28: Initiating Dynamic and Tempo Changes Cut 28, taken from session nineteen, illustrates Melinda copying Jean and joining my music on a drum within the established tempo and dynamic, followed by her initiating a change

140

in tempo and dynamics during musical play (interresponsiveness). In addition, this interaction is the first time in which she appeared to bring humor into the musical play. The cut begins with Melinda joining into the music while playing the floor-tom, as Jean prompts her by modeling on the drum. Melinda engages in the drum playing with big arm movements, matching the affect of the music. Her playing is a bit sporadic; however, she appears to be engaged (musically and interpersonally) before stopping the playing. The music continues to play, attempting to guide her back into playing. Finally, Melinda joins back into playing, initiating a drastic tempo and dynamic change, playing very controlled and softly while smirking at Jean and me. I begin to play music based on Melindas dynamic change while singing in a playful high pitched voice, as she plays while smiling at me. She stops playing momentarily, looks and smiles at me, as I begin to play, and she follows my lead by re-joining into the musical interaction. This is followed by Melinda again playing very softly and slowly while being focused and controlled in terms of her beating. This is followed by her playing an intentional loud beat, which I reflect on the piano, as she looks to Jean and smirks. DVD Cut 29: Connected by the Gleam in her Eye Cut 29 is taken from session twenty-five. It displays Melinda again, exploring a range of dynamics of the drum while engaged musically and interpersonally with Jean and me. In addition, she displays her ability to create an idea and bridge it to the musical interaction at hand, as she implements dancing into the musical play. The cut begins with Melinda teasingly playing the drum softly, appearing as though she was creating anticipation intentionally for Jean and me before hitting the drum loudly. She then hands the mallets to Jean, moves away from the drum, and to our surprise, she begins to dance in

141

a march-like manner while Jean mirrors her march while playing the drum. This is followed by Melinda rejoining the musical play, coming back to the drum, accepting the mallets from Jean, and beginning to play in a musically related manner. While Melinda continued to receive music therapy, the 25th session was the final clinical session of the present research protocol. The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale post-test was administered and Goal Attainment Scoring followed Melindas final session. Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) In addition to the qualitative case study analysis of Melindas treatment process, two quantitative measures were used to evaluate outcomes of music therapy: the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) and Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). The FEAS (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Weider, 2001) was used as a pre- and post-test measure for Melindas progress in achieving social-emotional goals. The GAS (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994) was used to evaluate each childs musical goals. Table 5 gives a comparison of Melindas pre- and post-test scores on the FEAS. The table shows raw scores for each developmental area, sub-scores, level of functioning, percentages, and the number of functioning levels changed out of three possible levels (e.g., deficient, at risk, and normal). Scoring is based on a 2-point scale: 0 = behavior is not seen or is briefly observed, 1 = behavior is present some of the time or observed several times, and 2 = behavior is consistently observed or observed many times. The ratings were summed to obtain subtest scores for the child, as well as total test scores (Greenspan, DeGangi & Weider, 2001). Total scores were

142

compared to cut-off scores (see Appendix A) to determine if the child is: normal, at-risk, or deficient. Table 5 Comparison of Melindas Pre- and Post FEAS Scores Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 11 (At risk) 78.5% Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores 13 (Normal) 92.8%
2 2 2 2 2 2 1

Areas I. Self-regulation

Change in Functioning Level +1

Attentive to play with toys 2 Explores objects/toys freely 2 Remains calm during play 2 Touching textured toys & caregiver 2 Shows content affect 1 Focused without distraction 1 Appears over aroused 1

II. Forming relationships and engagement Table 5. (Continued)

6 (Deficient) 37.5%

16 (Normal) 100%

+2

Areas
Emotional interest in care-giver Relaxed when near care-giver Anticipates with curiosity Uncomfortable with care-giver Initiates closeness to care-giver Avoids care-giver Socially references care-giver Communicates from across space

Pre-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores


0 2 0 2 2 0 0 0

Post-Test Raw Scores Sub-scores


2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2

Change in Functioning Level

III. 2-way-purposeful communication


Opens circles of communication Initiates intentional actions Closes circles of communication

4 (Deficient) 40%
0 1 1

7 (At risk) 70%


2 1 2

+1

143

Uses sounds/words/gestures

IV. Behavioral organization 0 (Normal) and problem solving 0%


Communicates in several modes Copies caregiver & incorporates 0 0

4 (Normal) 100%
2 2

+2

V. Representational capacity
Engages in symbolic play Engages in pretend play Communicates intentions Expresses dependency Expresses pleasure/excitement Expresses assertiveness Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas

0 (Deficient) 0%
0 0 0 0 0 0 0

2 (Deficient) 14.28%
0 2 0 0 0 0 0

VI. Representational differentiation

0 (Deficient) 0%

0 (Deficient) 0%
0 0 0 0 0

Bridges 2 unrealistic idea 0 Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 Use pretend to express dependency 0 Use pretend to express pleasure 0 Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0

Melindas ability to self-regulate and show interest in the world (area I) increased by two points, bringing her total score from 11 to 13. Her ability to be engaged (area II) increased by 10 points bringing her total score from 6 to 16. In addition, Melinda demonstrated improvement in her ability to communicate purposefully (area III), increasing her score by three points and bringing her total score from 4 to 7. She also showed improvement in her ability to problem solve (area IV), increasing her score by four points and bringing her total score from 0 to 4. Furthermore, Melindas ability to think symbolically (area V) increased by two points bringing her total score from 0 to 2, while showing no change in her ability to build bridges between ideas (area VI).

144

To summarize, during the pretest Melinda was regulated, but not particularly interested in engaging with therapist. She showed difficulty in her ability to relate, and generally reverted to lining up and hoarding toys. She displayed limited ability in using communication purposefully, other than an occasional outburst when therapist would attempt to enter into her world; this limited her ability to engage in long and flowing circles of communication. She exhibited limited ability to problem-solve, engage in symbolic play, and build bridges between ideas. During her posttest she continued to demonstrate the ability to be regulated, but was affectively connected to the therapist, displaying more engagement and relatedness. She also demonstrated the ability to be more purposeful while communicating, vocalizing and paying attention to therapist, while incorporating the therapists ideas into her play. In addition, she exhibited the ability to initiate and open circles of communication. However, she showed no change in her ability in thinking symbolically and bridging ideas. Figure 7 shows the amount of change in functioning levels made by Melinda in each developmental area: 1 represents deficient, 2 at risk, and 3 normal. As shown, Melinda improved by one functioning level in two areas, two levels in two areas, and stayed the same in two areas.

145

Figure 7 Changes in Developmental Levels (FEAS)


Changes in Developmental Levels Pre 2 L e v e l s Post

Area I

Area II

Area III

Area IV

Area V

Area VI

Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) Five music therapy (MT) goals were formulated for Melinda; all were weighted equally. The first goal for Melinda was musical awareness, that is, for her to be able to react or respond to or show interest in the therapists improvised music, either vocally, instrumentally and/or though movement. Upon beginning music therapy, Melinda exhibited a less than expected outcome in musical awareness (-1); she rarely displayed awareness of the therapists music. Upon completion of therapy, she displayed the expected level outcome (0); she sometimes exhibited musical awareness during the session. The second MT goal was musical relatedness, that is, for her to play in a related manner to the music that the therapist improvised. Upon beginning music therapy, Melinda displayed the most unfavorable outcome in relating his music to the therapists music (-2); she never played in a related way. However, at the conclusion of music therapy, she demonstrated

146

the expected outcome level (0), e.g., her playing related to the therapists music sometimes throughout the session. The third MT goal was relationship within musical play, that involves Melinda initiating a connection with the music and/or therapist, vocally, instrumentally and/or through movement/gesture. Upon beginning music therapy, Melinda displayed the most unfavorable outcome (-2) in initiating relationship within musical play; she never initiated a relationship within musical play. Upon completion of therapy, she demonstrated the expected outcome level (0); she initiated relationship within musical play sometimes throughout the session. Melindas fourth MT goal was musical interresponsiveness, that is for her to be able imitate or copy a musical idea and then incorporate it into musical play with the therapist. At the start of music therapy, Melinda displayed the most unfavorable outcome (-2) in playing interresponsively to the therapists music; she never played in an interresponsive manner. Upon the completion of music therapy, Melinda displayed the less than expected outcome level (0); her playing was interresponsive to the therapists music rarely throughout the session. Finally, Melindas fifth MT goal was musical communicativeness, that is, for her to be able to be spontaneously communicative in relation to the therapists music, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play (e.g., the ability to engage in call-and-response inter-play, punctuate the end of a phrase, initiate the opening of a phrase, and/or notice therapists music and respond contingently by elaborating on it). Upon beginning music therapy, Melindas musical communicativeness was a less than expected outcome (-1); rarely using music communicatively. At the conclusion of therapy she exhibited the expected outcome (0), sometimes using music communicatively throughout the session.

147

Figure 8 shows all five goal areas and the amount of change Melinda displayed as a result of music therapy. The figure shows raw scores for each goal area (-2 represents most unfavorable outcome; -1 less than expected outcome; 0 expected level; +1 more than expected success; and +2 most favorable outcome). Taking all of these goals collectively, Melinda advanced 1 level in two goal areas, and 2 levels in the remaining three goal areas. These changed scores on all goal areas were converted to a T- score, according to the formula of Kiresuk and Sherman (1968). Melindas T-score was 46.98, based on the assumption that together these scales have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (see Kiresuk & Sherman). Figure 8 Changes in Goal Attainment Levels (GAS)
Changes in Goal Attainment Levels
Pre G A S L e v e L s Post

2 1 0 -1 -2
Musical Awareness Musical Relatedness Relationship in Musical Play Musical Interresponsivess Musical Communicativeness

Musical Goal Areas

148

Summary Client Process Stage one, Self-Regulation and Interest in the World: Adjusting to IndividualDifferences, consisted of sessions one through nine in which Melinda exhibited difficulty engaging in musical play as a result of her difficulty in her ability to: maintain self-regulation, motor-plan, and process sensory information. In addition, during this time she appeared completely unaware of her surroundings. Also, during this stage, it appeared as though she did not understand the functional purpose of the musical instruments, while lacking an understanding of cause and effect relationships. This affected her ability to comprehend the basic social dynamics of musical play. This stage ended when the therapist limited the amount of instruments in the room, while implementing visual, gestural, and hand-over-hand prompting during musical play. Stage two, Cause and Effect Thinking, and the Emergence of the Self: Making Meaning of Related Music Making, consisted of sessions ten through eighteen. During this stage, Melindas social-emotional development began to emerge in sessions, as she began using the instruments in a musically related manner, while developing the ability to understand cause and effect and relationships. In addition, a greater sense of self began to emerge in sessions. This stage ended when the therapist changed strategies, limiting the use of hand-over-hand prompt in order to facilitate her ability to initiate ideas and circles of communication in the context of musical play. Exploring her Impact on the World: Musical Interresponsiveness and Communicativeness, Stage three, consisted of sessions nineteen through twenty-five.

149

During this stage, Melinda continued to display difficulty in maintaining musical dialogue for extended periods; however, the quality of her relatedness improved into higher levels of musical interactions through a range of affects. She displayed the ability to imitate an idea and incorporate it into the musical interaction (musical interresponsiveness), and initiate ideas and communication (musical communicativeness) while expanding her emotional range in the context of musical play. To that end, she became more engaged and aware of the environment, with her newly found communication, mobility and intentions during musical play. Therapists Method Although it was important to view Melindas areas of difficulties in a developmental manner, they also needed to be looked at collectively. To that end, it was important that the therapist and intern did not isolate developmental areas in therapy, but to work with them as a whole as well as simultaneously. For example, in the first excerpt when Melinda was upset and began screaming, the intern did not stop and try to calm (regulate) Melinda. Instead, she worked on engaging her into some kind of purposeful interaction. By doing this, Melinda acquired an experience of relating and interacting with someone outside of herself. To that end, the process worked simultaneously on self-regulation (area I), engagement (area II), and two-way purposeful communication (area II). These three levels were embodied within the relationship of the interaction. In addition, the therapist and intern implemented the use of several types of prompting, interchangeably or at the same time, in order to facilitate areas I, II, and III. These included:

150

1) gestural; 2) high-affect, musically and interpersonally; 3) hand-over-hand; and 4) visual. Each prompt worked collectively and focused on increasing Melindas ability to regulate, engage and communicate in the context of musical play. In addition, it was imperative that the interventions were used to develop fundamental abilities necessary for Melinda to engage in musical play, such as understanding the functional use of instruments, motor-planning, and a sense of self. In short, in order for Melinda to engage in intentional and purposeful musical play, biological issues (individual-differences) needed to be addressed within the context of musical play. As the treatment process moved forward into stage two, less prompting was utilized, and hand-over-hand guiding was no longer implemented. The focus was to facilitate Melindas ability to initiate ideas, while connecting her ideas to motor movements (motor planning) in a musically related fashion. This intervention would also help facilitate her ability to understand cause and effect relationships and begin to develop a greater sense of self. To that end, the quality of her musical engagement and relatedness improved as she became increasingly aware of her environment and her impact within it, musically and interpersonally. During the final stage, prompting was limited to just musical cues (e.g., dynamics, tempo, and affect) and modeling play on the drum at times, to foster extended periods of sustained musical play. For the most part, the focus during stage three was on facilitating longer, continuously flowing dialogues of musical interactions, which at this point continued to be fragmented.

151

Relationship Between FEAS and GAS Results of Melindas FEAS pre-test and GAS indicted some differences. During the FEAS, in the area of self-regulation, she showed an ability to be calm (self-regulated) for the play period and showed no signs of distress. In addition, she displayed the ability to be interested and attentive to toys, while exploring objects freely. During the GAS, however, she exhibited difficulty maintaining calmness (self-regulated), and although she showed interest in the instruments, it was solely based on wanting to hoard them as opposed to exploring them in a functional (musical) manner. Furthermore, during the FEAS, Melinda displayed the ability to initiate physical closeness, while maintaining a visual connection with the therapist, however, during GAS, she appeared unaware of the therapist and intern musically and interpersonally. Similarities between the FEAS (pre-test) and GAS were observed in the areas of engagement and two-way purposeful communication. In both settings Melinda was avoidant and withdrew from back-and-forth play with the therapist. In addition, during both procedures (FEAS and GAS), she displayed difficulty in her ability to exhibit an emotional connection with the therapist. Also, during both procedures Melinda exhibited difficulties in her abilities to: 1) open (initiate) and close circles of communication, 2) initiate intentional actions with toys or instruments while engaging with the therapist, 3) engage in problem-solving, 4) and create and bridge ideas with the therapist. Results of Melindas FEAS post-test and Goal Attainment Scaling indicated several similarities. During both procedures she demonstrated her abilities to self-regulate and engage with the therapist, being relaxed and emotionally interested in the therapist musically and interpersonally. In addition, during both, FEAS (post-test) and Goal Attainment Scaling, she

152

exhibited her ability to open (initiate) and close circles of communication (musically and interpersonally), and communicate her intentions during play. Differences between the procedures (in FEAS, post-test and Goal Attainment Scaling) occurred in Melindas abilities to create and bridge ideas. She showed some ability in these developmental capacities during musical play, but displayed difficulty in these areas in FEAS. Relationship Quantitative and Qualitative data Melindas areas of strengths and weakness were consistent across the qualitative and quantitative data. Although progress was consistently noted in both sets of data, it appears that the quality of improvement in specific areas, in terms of self-regulation (area I), engagement (area II), two-way purposeful communication (area III), and problem-solving (area IV), were inconsistent. Quantitative data indicated that Melinda is functioning in the normal range in areas one through four. However, qualitatively, although she displayed capacities in each of the four developmental areas, consistency continued to be lacking in each area.

153

CHAPTER 8 DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS In the previous four chapters, the quantitative and qualitative findings for each case were presented separately. The purpose of the present chapter is to compare the findings across the four cases and identify any trends that might exist. Comparing NRMT and the DIR/Floortime Models Similarities Between NRMT and the DIR/Floortime Models NRMT and DIR/Floortime Model together make an extremely useful package in working with children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD). The similarities between the models with regard to clinical practice and the humanistic concerns for the child are quite evident. Both models focus on the importance of relationships, intrinsic learning based on relational experiences, and the acceptance of each child, while embracing his or her idiosyncratic behaviors. In addition, both models, in practice, involve the therapist meeting the child where he/she is at emotionally, and following the childs lead in order to enter into his/her world. Both models are action basedthe child becomes an active participant in guiding the therapy process. Furthermore, both models require the therapist to be creative, spontaneous, flexible, playful, and self-aware. Differences Between NRMT and the DIR/Floortime Models Although there are several similarities in some of the core components of the two models, there are also several differences, specifically in theory and application. During the FEAS (DIR assessment), the therapist uses his/her physical-self along with symbolic and sensory-based toys as the primary medium to engage the child. In NRMT, the primary means of connecting with the

154

child is through the music. In addition, where NRMT views the musical relationship as the primary agent of change DIR views the interpersonal relationship between therapist and child as the basis for learning. To that end, during assessment (FEAS), the therapist is seeking to understand how the child relates to others, interpersonally and through play. In addition, through play, the clinician begins to gain a profile of the child that includes 1) social-emotional developmental levels, 2) biological differences that interfere with the childs development, and 3) the manner in which the child relates to people. NRMT, however, is primarily looking at musical responses of the child and how they relate to the musical relationship between the child and therapist. Another component that separates the DIR Model from NRMT is that DIR is an interdisciplinary approach, wherein assessment and treatment occur across several disciplines. NRMT assesses the child based solely on his/her ability to engage in musical interactions. Furthermore, the models also differ regarding goal planning: in DIR, goals are based on socialemotional development, but in NRMT, goals are primarily musical, and developmental goals are viewed as secondary (Aigen, 2005). Further differences between the models are in the manner in which the child is conceptualized. The DIR model considers the childs biology in its attempt to understand how the child processes information and navigates through the world. NRMT is primarily concerned with the childs musical responses as a means to understanding and conceptualizing his or her musical world. To that end, if a child is unable to engage in musical play, NRMT may view this child as being resistive to the musical and clinical processes; however, the DIR clinician would seek to understand what biological differences are impeding the childs ability to engage in an

155

interaction. Where NRMT is viewing musical behaviors of the child as primary, the DIR clinician looks at the whole child. Comparing Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches In comparing both quantitative and qualitative approaches, FEAS pre- and post-test scores reflected what was clinically observed in NRMT. Therefore, the two approaches were very similar in what they each indicated. Generally, differences occurred between approaches (quantitative and qualitative) during the pre-test, as the children generally scored lower in NRMT in areas I, II, and III. Progress, however, for children with expressive language difficulty, such as Matthew was better during NRMT areas I, II, and III. Commonalities Among the Four Children Although each child in this study exhibited distinct characteristics, there were also similarities noted. The children with intense Applied Behavioral Analysis backgrounds (Timmy and Janet), were similar in that they knew how to problem-solve and get their needs met; however, both had difficulty in initiating ideas or a conversation musically and interpersonally. In addition, they both presented with a vast vocabulary; however, they were unable to organize their thoughts and engage in long and flowing circles of communication, musically and interpersonally. In addition, there were also similarities between both children, who were nonverbal, in that they both became expressive in music. They both utilized non-verbal singing and instrumental play to communicate musically, while also increasing their capacities in areas I, II, III, and IV.

156

Contributions of NRMT to DIR: Common Areas of Improvement Common Areas of Improvement: FEAS Contributions of NRMT to DIR were reflected in the common areas of improvement on the FEAS in each of the four children. Post-test FEAS scores indicated that three (Janet, Timmy, and Melinda) of the four children scored in the normal range in areas I (self-regulation) and II (engagement). Two children (Timmy and Janet) both scored in the normal range in area VI (representational differentiation), and improved at least 1 level in area V (representational capacity). In addition, two children (Melinda and Mathew) scored in the normal range in area IV (problem-solving), whereas three children (Janet, Melinda, and Timmy) improved at least 1 level in area III (two-way purposeful communication). Furthermore, two children (Matthew and Melinda) showed no improvement in areas V and VI. In conclusion, it appeared that NRMT contributed to Functional Emotional Developmental Levels (FEDL), and the areas in which it appeared to be most beneficial were areas I, II, III. Specifically, the flexibility employed in NRMT to meet and connect with the unique musicality of each child, combined with the non-verbal, social and emotional components of the music itself, appeared to facilitate self-regulation (area I), engagement (area II), and twoway purposeful communication (area III). Although other social-emotional areas improved among the children, as indicated by the FEAS; it appeared that NRMT showed consistent and clinically significant improvements in areas I, II, and III. Common Areas of Improvement: GAS In looking at common areas of improvement on the GAS across the four children, each child showed significant progress in two goal areas: 1) initiating interaction and forming

157

relationship within musical play, and 2) two-way purposeful communication through music. In addition, each child (Melinda, Timmy, and Melinda) whose GAS included the goals of problemsolving through musical interresponsiveness showed improvement. Those children (Melinda, Matthew, and Janet) who had the goal of engagement and shared attention through musical relatedness also improved in this area. Furthermore, improvements were evident in two children (Melinda and Matthew) who were given the goal of self-regulation and interest in the world through musical awareness, and two children (Timmy and Janet) showed improvement in the goal of bridging musical ideas and musical interrelatedness. In short, each of the four children in the study showed improvements in every individualized GAS goal area. Limitations of the Study The main limitations of this study were the small number of subjects studied, due to the time needed to provide sufficient treatment using the NRMT model. The small number of subjects in turn precluded statistical analysis, and therefore generalizations of the findings to other children with autism cannot be made. Nevertheless, the present findings provide confirmatory clinical evidence of two basic premises: 1) that NRMT can help autistic children gain more awareness of self and other, while also increasing their musical and interpersonal relatedness; and 2) that NRMT can enhance effective treatment within the DIR model. It is also important to note that the present study did not produce any findings that disconfirmed these premises. Further studies with a larger sample size are still needed to demonstrate statistically the effects of this treatment protocol on developmental growth in autistic children.

158

Implications of the Study Implications for NRMT Clinical Theory and Practice Implications of this study for Nordoff-Robbins theory are closely related to practical considerations. Although it is imperative to understand a childs musical behaviors and create musical goals based on his/her musical behaviors, it is equally important to understand biological factors that may impinge upon a childs ability to engage in musical play. To that end as indicated in this study, a child who has difficulty engaging in musical interactions may be experiencing factors completely unrelated to music (e.g., difficulties with motor-planning, auditory and/or visual processing, and/or self-regulation). In addition, because treatment focuses primarily on facilitating relatedness and communication in children with autism, it is important to look for these qualities both within and outside of music, such as in a smile, a gaze, or in a hug. Of course these responses can be accepted and processed within the context of musical play, or, may be a result of a musical interaction. However, in all of their forms these responses should be regarded as communicative and related responses. Implications for General Clinical Theory and Practice in General The primary implications for general theory and practice of this study pertain to how the clinician views and conceptualizes the child with regard to the core deficits of autism. This view of the child requires the music therapist to look outside of his/her discipline and begin collaborating with other disciplines to get a more comprehensive understanding of how the child processes the world around him. This lens can inform the music therapy treatment plan as it pertains to each child individually.

159

To that end, it is imperative that music therapy clinicians devise assessment and treatment protocols that are population-based and that address the core deficits of autism: relating and communicating. In doing so, as indicated by this study, autism is not a disorder of memory. Although memory-based interventions by music therapists are popular, the only purpose they serve is to isolate a child from spontaneous thinking and communicating, while reinforcing scripted ritualistic thinking. As suggested by this study, NRMT speaks directly to the core deficits of autism, facilitating musical engagement, relatedness, and spontaneous communication. NRMT has the potential to facilitate a childs ability to engage in music on a much higher level, cognitively; emotionally; and socially then memory-based music therapy interventions. Finally, results of this study also suggest that GAS is a practical, clinical measuring tool that can be used either qualitatively alone or both qualitatively and quantitatively. It is practical in that it does not require a lot of time to implement, and it is easy to use and develop. Implications for Research The results of this study support the use of quantitative data in measuring the effectiveness of improvisational protocols when working in individual music therapy sessions. In addition, this study also supports a method of understanding the connection between musical and non-musical goals (social-emotional) in its use of the FEAS and GAS with children on the autistic spectrum. Implications for Education and Training Several professional skill sets were essential in the implementation of the treatment protocol in this study. These include: clinical musicianship, a deeper understanding of Pervasive

160

Developmental Disorders (PDD) and child development, a knowledge base of other disciplines (speech therapy, occupational therapy, etc.) and how they relate to PDDs. It is considered important that information and training regarding these topics be incorporated into educational programs for music therapists. It is further recommended that specialized advanced training programs be created to equip music therapy professionals with the knowledge and skills required to work effectively with children with ASD.

161

REFERENCES Aigen, K. (2005). Music-centered music therapy. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers. Allgood, N. (2005). Parents perception of family-based group music therapy for children with autistic spectrum disorders. Music Therapy Perspectives, 23 (2), 9299. Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M. & Williams, T. I. (2005). Pitch perception in children with autistic spectrum disorders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 543558. Alvin, J. & Warwick, A. (1978). Music therapy for the autistic child (2nd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press. American Psychiatry Association (APA) (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.), Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author. American Music Therapy Association (2005) In A. Elkins (ed), AMTA Member Sourcebook, 2005. Silver Springs, MD: American Music Therapy Association. Applebaum, E., Egel, A. L., Koegel, R. L. & Imhoff, B. (1979). Measuring musical abilities of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9 (3), 279285. Autism Society of America (ASA) (2003). Autism information. Retrieved June 23, 2007 from http://www.autism-society.org. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The theory of mind hypothesis of autism: A reply to Boucher. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 199200. Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Berger, D. S. (2002). Music Therapy, sensory integration and the autistic child. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

162

Blackstock, E. G. (1978). Cerebral asymmetry and the development of early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 339353. Bleuler, E. (1950). Dementia praecox; or, The group of schizophrenias. Monograph series on schizophrenia, no. 1. New York: International Universities Press. Bonnel, A., Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Trudel, M., Gallun, E. & Bonnel, A. M. (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis. Journal of Cognition Neuroscience, 15, (2), 226235. Brown, W., A., Cammuso, K., Sachs, H., Winklosky, B., Mullane, J., Bernier, R., Svenson, S., Arin, D., Sheidley, B. R., & Folstein, S. E. (2003). Autism-related language, personality, and cognition in people with absolute pitch: Results of a preliminary study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33 (2), 163167. Brown, S. M. K. (1994). Autism and music therapyis change possible, and why music? British Journal of Music Therapy, 8 (1), 1525. Brownell, M. D. (2002). Musically adapted social stories to modify behaviors in students with autism: Four case studies. Journal of Music Therapy, 39 (2), 117144. Bruscia, K. E. (1987). Improvisational models of music therapy. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Bruscia, K. (Ed.) (1991). Case studies in music therapy. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers. Bruscia, K.E. (1998). Defining music therapy (2nd edition). Gilsum, New Hampshire: Barcelona Publishers. Bryan, A. (1989). Autistic group case study. British Journal of Music Therapy, 3 (1), 1621.

163

Carcani-Rathwell, I., Rabe-Hasketh, S., & Santosh, P.J. (2006). Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 573581. Cardillo, J. E. (1994). Summary Score Conversion Key. In Kiresuk, T.J., Smith, A. & Cardillo, J. E. (Eds.), Goal Attainment Scaling: Applications, Theory and Measurement (pp. 273 281). Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2007, February). Media relations. Retrieved June 25, 2007 from http://www.cdc.gov/od/oc/media/pressrel/2007/r070208.htm. Edgerton, C., L. (1994). The effect of improvisational music therapy on the communicative behaviors of autistic children. Journal of music therapy, 31 (1), 3162. Furth, H. & Wachs, H. (1974). Thinking goes to school: Piagets theory in practice. Oxford, England: Oxford U. Press. Gold, C., Wigram, T. & Elefant, C. (2006). Music Therapy for autistic spectrum disorder (Cochrane review). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 2. Greenberg, L. S. (1986a). Change process research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 49. Greenberg, L. S. (1986b). Research Strategies. In L. Greenberg & W.M Pinsof (Eds.), The Psychotherapeutic Process. A Research Handbook (pp. 707734). New York: Guilford Press. Greenspan, S.I., & Weider, S. (1997). Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and children with disorders in relating and communication: A chart review of 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. The Journal of Developmental and Learning

164

Disorders, 1 (1), 87141. Greenspan, S.I., & Weider, S. (1998). The child with special needs: Encouraging intellectual and emotional growth. Da Capo Lifelong Books. Greenspan, S.I., DeGangi, G., & Weider, S., (2001). The functional emotional assessment scale (FEAS) for infancy and early childhood: Clinical and research applications. Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental and Learning Disorders. Greenspan, S.I., & Shanker, S. G. (2004). The first idea: How symbols, language, and intelligence evolved from our primate ancestors to modern humans. Da Capo Lifelong Books. Greenspan, S.I., & Weider, S. (2006a). Engaging autism: Using the floortime approach to help children relate, communicate, and think. Da Capo Lifelong Books. Greenspan, S.I., Weider, S. (2006b). Infant and early childhood mental health: A comprehensive developmental approach to assessment and intervention. American Psychiatric Association. Greenspan, S.I. (2007). Initiative: A floortime essential and a must for childrens emotional and intellectual growth. The Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental and Learning Disorders , released June 22 on line at http://www.icdl.com/dirFloortime/Clinical%20pearls/ documents/Initiative-FloortimeEssential-6-22-07_2_.pdf Griggs-Drane, E.R., & Wheeler, J.J. (1997). The use of functional assessment procedures and individualized schedules in the treatment of autism: Recommendations for music therapists. Music Therapy Perspectives, 15 (2), 8793.

165

Heaton, P., Hermelin, B. & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability. Music Perception, 15 (3), 291305. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1999). Can children with autistic spectrum disorders perceive affect in music? An experimental investigation. Psychological Medicine, 29 (6), 14051410. Heaton, P., Pring, L., & Hermelin, B. (2001). Musical processing in high functioning children with autism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 443444. Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labeling and disembedding in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44 (4), 543551. Heaton, P. (2004). Interval and contour processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35 (6), 787793. Hilliard, R., B. (1993) Single-case Design methodology in psychotherapy process and outcome research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 373380. Holck, U. (2004). Interaction themes in music therapy: Definition and delimitation. Nordic Journal of Music therapy, 13 (1), 319. Hollander, F. M., & Juhrs, P. D. (1974). Orff-Shulwerk, an effective treatment tool with autistic children. Journal of Music Therapy, 11, 112. Howat, R. (1995). Elizabeth: A Case Study of an Autistic Child in Individual Music Therapy. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston & R. West (Eds.), The art and science of music therapy: A handbook (pp. 238257). Langhorne, PA, England: Hardwood Academic Publishers/Gordon. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, pp. 217250.

166

Kaplan, R., S., & Steele, A. L. (2005). An analysis of music therapy program goals and outcomes for clients with diagnoses on the autistic spectrum. The Journal of Music Therapy, 42 (1) 219. Katz, Alyssa (2006, October 30). The autism clause. New York Magazine, 38, 5061. Kern, P. (2006). Using embedded music therapy interventions to support outdoor play of young children with autism in an inclusive community-based child care program. Journal of Music Therapy, 43 (4), 270294. Kim, Y. (2004). The early beginnings of Nordoff-Robbins music therapy. Journal of Music Therapy, 41 (4), 321339. Kiresuk, T. J., & Lund, S. (1978). Goal Attainment Scaling. In C. Attkisson, W. Hargreaves, M. Horowitz, & J. Sorensen (Eds.), Evaluation of human service programs. New York: Academic Press. Kiresuk, T. J., & Lund, S. (1976). Process and measurement using goal attainment scaling, In Glass, G. V. (Ed.), Educations Studies review manual, (1), Sage, Beverly Hills, CA. Kiresuk, T. J., Smith, A., & Cardillo, J. (1994). Goal Attainment Scaling: Applications, theory and measurement, Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lecourt, E. (1991). Off-beat Music Therapy: A Psychoanalytic Approach to Autism. In K. Bruscia (Ed.), Case studies in music therapy (pp. 7398). Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers. Levinge, A. (1990). The use of I and me! Music therapy with an autistic child. British Journal of Music Therapy, 4 (2), 215.

167

Mahlberg, M. (1973). Music therapy in the treatment of an autistic child. Journal of Music Therapy, 10, 189193. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Belleville, S. & Rouleau, N. (1999). Absolute pitch in autism: A case study. Neuropsychology of Developmental Disorders, 5 (6), 485501. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music in highfunctioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44 (4), 10571065. Muller, P. & Warwick, A., (1993). Autistic Children and Music Therapy: The Effects of Maternal Involvement in Therapy. In M. H. Heal & T. Wigram (Eds.) Music therapy in health and education (pp. 214234). Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1964). Music therapy and personality change in autistic children. Journal of the American Institute of Homeopathy, 57, 305310. Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1968). Fun for four drums: A rhythmic game for children with four drums, piano and a song. Theodore Presser Company Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1968). Clinical experiences with autistic children. In E.T. Gaston (Ed.), Music in therapy (pp.191193). NY, NY: The McMillian Company. Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (1971). Therapy in music for handicapped children. London: Victor Gollanez, Ltd. Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (1977). Creative music therapy. New York: Harper and Row Publishers. Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (1983). Music therapy in special education (revised). St. Louis: MMB

168

Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (1992). Therapy in music for handicapped children. London: Victor Gollanez Ltd. Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (2007). Creative music therapy: A guide to fostering clinical musicianship. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers. Orr, T., J., Myles, B.S., & Carlson, J.K. (1998). The impact of rhythmic entrainment on a person with autism. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 13 (3), 163166. Robbins, C. & Robbins, C. (1991a). Self-Communication in Creative Music Therapy. In K. Bruscia (Ed.), Case studies in music therapy (pp. 5572). Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers. Saperston, B. (1973). The use of music establishing communication with an autistic mentally retarded child. Journal of Music Therapy, 10, 184188. Solomon, R., Necheles, J., Ferch, C., & Bruckman, D. (2007). Pilot study of a parent training program for young children with autism: The play project home consultation program. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 11 (3), 205-224. Sorel, S. N. (2005). Presenting Carly and Elliot: Exploring roles and relationships in a motherson dyad in Nordoff-Robbins music therapy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University, NY. Starr, E., & Zenker, K. (1998). Understanding autism in the context of music therapy: bridging theory and practice. Canadian Journal of Music Therapy, 6 (1), 119. Stevens, E., & Clark, F. (1969). Music therapy in the treatment of autistic children. Journal of Music Therapy, 6, 93104.

169

Stock-Kranowitz, C. (1998). The out-of-sync child: Recognizing and coping with sensory integration dysfunction. Perigee Trade. Thaut, M. (1984). A music therapy treatment model for autistic children. Music Therapy Perspectives, 1 (4), 713. Thaut, M. (1987). Visual versus auditory (musical) stimulus preferences in autistic children: a pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 17, 425432. Thaut, M. (1988). Measuring musical responsiveness in autistic children: A comparative analysis of improvised musical tone sequences of autistic, normal, and mentally retarded individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18 (4), 561571. Thaut, M. (1992). Music therapy with autistic children. In W. Davis, K. Gfeller, and M. Thaut (Eds.) An Introduction to Music Therapy: Theory and Practice (pp.180196). Dubuque, Indiana: William C. Brown Publishers. The White House (2006, December). Fact sheet. Retreived June 25, 2007 from http://www.whitehouse.gov/news/releases/2006/12/20061219-3.html. Turry, A., & Marcus, D. (2003). Using the Nordoff-Robbins Approach to Music Therapy with Adults Diagnosed with Autism. In D. Weiner, and L.K. Oxford (Eds.), Action therapy with families and groups: Using creative arts improvisation in clinical practice, (pp. 197228). Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association. Turry, A., & Marcus, D. (2005). Teamwork: Therapist and co-therapist in the Nordoff-Robbins approach to music therapy. Music Therapy Perspectives, 23 (1), 5369. Wager, K., M. (2000). The effects of music therapy upon an adult male with autism and metal retardation: A four-year case study. Music Therapy Perspectives, 18 (2), 131140.

170

Warwick, A. (1995). Music Therapy in Education: Research with Autistic Children and their Mothers. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston & R. West (Eds.) The art and science of music therapy: A handbook (pp. 209225). Langhorne, PA, England: Hardwood Academic Publishers/Gordon. Weider, S., & Greenspan, S.I. (2003). Climbing the symbolic ladder in the DIR model through floortime/interactive play. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 7 (4), 425435. Whipple, J. (2004). Music in intervention for children and adolescents with autism: A metaanalysis. Journal of Music Therapy, 41 (2), 9106. Wimpory, D., C., Chadwick, P., & Nash, S. (1995). Brief report: Musical interaction therapytherapeutic play for children with autism: An evaluative case study with two-year followup. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25 (5), 541552. Wimpory, D., C. & Nash, S. (1999). Musical interaction therapy-therapeutic play for children with autism. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 1, (1), 1728. Woodward, A. (2004). Music therapy for autistic children and their families: a creative spectrum. British Journal of Music Therapy, 18 (1), 814.

171

APPENDIX A INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD APPROVAL

172

173

174

APPENDIX B CONSENT FORMS

175

176

177

178

179

180

Appendix C The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale Administration and Scoring Form Behaviors: Name of Child: Age of Child: Person Playing With Child: General Scoring Scoring is on a two-point scale for most items, except where indicated, and is: 0 = not at all or very brief 1 = present some of time, observed several times 2 = consistently present, observed many times Indicate N/O for behaviors that are not observed Where indicated to convert a score, transform the scoring as follows: 0 becomes a 2 1= 1 2 becomes a 0 Scores for symbolic play should be entered in the SYM column and scores for sensory play entered in the SENS column. When the examiner facilitates play with the child, enter scores in the EXAM column. The last column may be used for entering scores for additional caregivers (e.g., mother, father, foster parent, babysitter) observed playing with the child. Scores are interpreted for the primary caregiver playing with the child for the symbolic and sensory play situations. If scores do not differ for symbolic and sensory play, then only one score is interpreted. However, if behaviors differ for the different play situations, then two scores are calculated, one for symbolic play, one for sensory play. These are interpreted using the cutoff scores presented in the profile form. Mother: Caregiver: Father: Examiner: Caregiver Date of Testing:

Key: SYM = Symbolic; SENS = Sensory; EXAM = Examiner Observations 181

SYM Self-regulation and interest in the world 1. Shows interest in toys through facial or verbal expressions of interest or by handling and touching toys, but not so absorbed by toys that the caregiver plays with toys alone, ignoring the child. 2. Shows sustained interest in child, focuses on child's signals (gestures, vocalizations), keeping child involved in play. 3. Interacts calmly with child, able to wait for child's responses. 4. Shows pleasant or animated, happy affect throughout play Scoring: 0 = flat, somber, or depressed affect 1 = content, but neutral 2 = happy and animated with warm and engaging smiles 5. Is sensitive and responsive to child's need for touch by stroking or touching baby in pleasurable ways and/or encourages child to explore textured toys. 6. Provides pleasurable movement experiences to the child or encourages movement exploration Total for self-regulation and interest in the world FORMING RELATIONSHIPS, ATTACHMENT, AND ENGAGEMENT 7. Is relaxed during interchange with child, not overly attentive to child's every action. 8. Looks at child with affection, showing a warm connection. 9. Enjoys being with and playing with the child through smiles or a joyful look and emits a sense of warmth by providing inviting gestures. Keep in mind cultural differences in how this may be expressed. 10. Is overly anxious in attachment to child, overwhelming child with affectionate touching OR is not comfortable showing feelings and relating warmly and intimately with child, appearing overly vigilant towards child. (Circle which one.) Scoring: 0 = many times 1 = sometimes 2 = briefly or not at all 11. Maintains a verbal or visual connection with child, showing clear availability and interest in the child. Child may move away from caregiver to explore room, yet the caregiver maintains connected to the child across space through gestures, vocalizations, and facial expressions. Total for Forming Relationships, Attachment, and Engagement TWO-WAY, PURPOSEFUL COMMUNICATION 12. Opening circle of communication: Initiates interactions with child through vocalizations or gestures, creating interactive opportunities with child 182

SENS

EXAM

SYM 13. Responds to child's wishes, intentions, and actions in a contingent way, building on how the child wishes to play. For example, child may hand toy to parent, and parent responds by taking it and saying something about the toy, then gives the child an opportunity to respond to what they just did. Scoring: 0 = consistently does opposite to what baby seeks, misreads child's cues, changing activity from what child wants to do. 1 = misreads child's signals about 25 to 50% of time changing activity or toy while at other times reads child's signals accurately. 2 = responds to child's signals in appropriate way most of time (up to 75% time responsive to child), staying on the activity that the child has chosen. 14. Predominately handles toys, engaging in parallel play and removing attention from playing with child. Converted Score* Score of 0 converts to 2 15. Plays with child at developmentally appropriate level. Caregiver may play slightly above child's level of skill, modeling new ways to do things or labeling what child does or describing the functions of objects. 16. Stimulates child at pace that allows child to respond, waiting for child's responses. Avoids overstimulating child with language or actions. 17. Allows child to decide on the play topic, to initiate play and explore toys in ways that the child seeks or needs. Total for Two-Way, Purposeful Communication STOP HERE FOR PARENTS OF 7 TO 9 MONTH OLDS BEHAVIORAL ORGANIZATION, PROBLEM-SOLVING, AND INTERNALIZATION (A Complex Sense of Self) 18. Responds and initiates reciprocal back and forth chains of interactions with child, stringing together connected circles of communication or units of interaction. For example, caregiver introduces baby doll, baby touches doll's face, mother touches doll's hair, baby pats the doll, mother says "baby", and baby glances between mother and doll). The caregiver may imitate child (e.g., pushing car alongside child), then interject her turn by an action or verbalization related to the child's actions (e.g., "Oh, a bump!", then bumps her car into child's car). Scoring: 0 = 0 to 2 circles 1 = 3 to 5 circles 2 = 6 or more circles 19. Uses gestures and facial expressions as a modality to promote circles of communication. 183

SENS

EXAM

SYM 20. Uses touch or rough house play as a modality to promote circles of communication. 21. Shows pleasure and excitement in playing with child in whatever way the child wishes to play. Scoring: 0 = little pleasure and excitement shown by caregiver. 1 = pleasure and excitement sustained by parent over the course of several (3 to 5) circles of communication. 2 = pleasure and excitement sustained for many (6 or more) circles of communication. Note here if child is unable to sustain circles of communication if it affects caregiver's score: Child can sustain circles: _____ Child cannot: _____ 22. Expresses appropriate limits on baby. The caregiver may redirect child not to leave room, not to hit her, or not throw toy. If no need for limits arises during play, mark N/O and give 2 points. 23. Elaborates on and builds complexity into the child's play behaviors while engaged in interactive sequences between parent and child. The parent expands on what the child does while remaining on the child's play topic (e.g., the parent does not introduce a completely new play idea). The parent provides a small challenge or interesting twist to the play that requires the child to respond slightly differently than before, thus creating a problem solving opportunity for the child. For example, the parent and child are pushing a car back and forth towards each other. The parent expands on this by creating a wall with her leg to prevent the car from rolling, then waits to see how the child will solve this situation. 24. Allows child to assert self in play, exploring with confidence in what that he or she wishes (e.g., child expresses strong wish to play in a certain way such as banging toys, being silly, holding a doll, or running around room.) Parent supports the child's needs for dependency and closeness, assertiveness and curiosity, aggression, autonomy, or pleasure and excitement by admiring, showing interest, and/or by joining in to the child's play in whatever way the child seeks. Problems that may interfere with caregiver's capacity to support this area might be intrusiveness, withdrawal, overprotectiveness, or playing at level far above child's level of competence. Total for Behavioral Organization, Problem-Solving, and Internalization STOP HERE FOR PARENTS OF 10 TO 24 MONTH OLDS REPRESENTATIONAL CAPACITY (Elaboration) 25. Encourages child to engage in symbolic play by modeling or combining materials in ways that encourage representational actions (e.g., mother holds spoon near baby doll's mouth and says, "Feed 184

SENS

EXAM

SYM baby?"). Parent appears comfortable in playing make believe. 26. Elaborates on child's pretend play idea by building on child's ideas and adding some complexity to them. (e.g., Child puts doll in car and pushes it and caregiver says "Oh, is Daddy going to the store?). 27. Allows child to express pretend play themes involving closeness or dependency (e.g., nurturing doll) without competing for child's attention to be the one nurtured. 28. Sustains pretend play, showing interest, pleasure, and excitement about the child's pretend play idea by asking questions, laughing or smiling, and joining into the child's play with enthusiasm (e.g., Caregiver says "Oh, that's a good idea. What happens now? That's so funny!). 29. Allows child to express themes of assertiveness in pretend play (e.g., child pretends he's a policeman and puts caregiver in jail; child pretends to go to work and tells caregiver to stay home). Total for Representational Capacity (Elaboration) STOP HERE FOR PARENTS OF 25 TO 35 MONTH OLDS REPRESENTATIONAL DIFFERENTIATION (Building Bridges Between Ideas and Emotional Thinking) 30. Elaborates on child's pretend play, creating opportunities to logically connect ideas in play. The caregiver accomplishes this by asking questions to give depth to the drama such as "how", "why", or "when". If the child strays off the topic, the caregiver asks questions to bridge the circle of communication back to the pretend play theme (e.g., "But what happened to the crocodile? He was ready to go for a swim and now you're playing with the truck.) 31. Incorporates causality into pretend play by helping child to logically connect three or more ideas into a reality-based story sequence. For example, if the child is playing out how two animals fight, the caregiver might ask "How come they're fighting?", "Do they know each other?". 32. Helps child to elaborate on a wide range of emotional themes, whatever they might beassertiveness, pleasure and excitement, fearfulness, anger, or separation and loss. The caregiver is accepting of the child's expressions of different feelings and themes through play and shows no discomfort at the expression of different ideas from the child. Total for Representational Differentiation (Emotional Thinking) TOTAL CAREGIVER SCORE FOR SCALE

SENS

EXAM

185

The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale Administration and Scoring Form

Behaviors: Name of Child: Age of Child: Person Playing With Child:

Child Date of Testing:

_____________________ Mother: Father: Caregiver: Examiner:

General Scoring Scoring is on a two-point scale for most items, except where indicated, and is: 0 = not at all or very brief 1 = present some of time, observed several times 2 = consistently present, observed many times Indicate N/O for behaviors that are not observed Where indicated to convert a score, transform the scoring as follows: 0 becomes a 2 1= 1 2 becomes a 0 Scores for symbolic play should be entered in the SYM column and scores for sensory play entered in the SENS column. When the examiner facilitates play with the child, enter scores in the EXAM column. The last column may be used for entering scores for additional caregivers (e.g., mother, father, foster parent, babysitter) observed playing with the child. Scores are interpreted for the primary caregiver playing with the child for the symbolic and sensory play situations. If scores do not differ for symbolic and sensory play, then only one score is interpreted. However, if behaviors differ for the different play situations, then two scores are calculated, one for symbolic play, one for sensory play. These are interpreted using the cutoff scores presented in the profile form. Key: SYM = Symbolic; SENS = Sensory; EXAM = Examiner Observations 186

SELF-REGULATION AND INTEREST IN THE WORLD 1. Is interested and attentive to play with toys. 2. Explores objects freely without caution. 3. Remains calm for play period with no signs of distress (crying or whining), showing appropriate frustration. 4. Is comfortable touching textured toys and in being touched by caregiver. Shows happy, content affect. Scoring: 0 = flat, 1 = content but neutral, 2 = happy and content, robust, smiles, warm and engaging affect. 6. Remains focused on objects or caregiver without being distracted by sights or sounds. Scoring: 0 = distracted frequently; no focused play for more then a few seconds at a time, 1 = distracted some of the time with brief periods of focused play, 2 = remains focused in play most of the with only brief distractability. NOTE: SCORE ONLY ITEM 8 OR 9, WHICHEVER APPLIES. 7. Underreactivity: Appears sluggish or withdrawn Scoring: 0 = withdrawn, difficult to engage; 1 = sluggish or slow-paced in actions but can eventually be aroused or engaged; 2 = shows a bright, alert state with focused play throughout 8. Overreactivity: Appears overaroused by toys and environment. Scoring: 0 = Very active, moves quickly from one toy to the next or wanders away from caregiver and toys constantly; 1= Moderately active, occasional bursts of changing activity quickly or wandering away, then settles into play with one toy for short period; 2 = Well-modulated in pace and activity level, focusing on a toy or caregiver for long periods before changing activity. Total For Self-Regulation and Interest in the World FORMING RLEATIONSHIPS, ATTACHMENT, AND ENGAGEMENT 9. Shows emotional interest and connection with caregiver by vocalizing and smiling at her. 10. Evidences a relaxed sense of security and/or comfort when near caregiver. If child is active and moves away from caregiver, he references her from across space and shows relaxed security in distill space. 11. Anticipates with curiosity or excitement when caregiver presents an 187

interesting object or game. 12. Displays signs of discomfort, displeasure, or sadness during interactive play if caregiver should become unresponsive or engage in anticontingent behaviors. (If caregiver is responsive or contingent, note that this was not observed with "N/O", then assign 2 points.) 13. Initiates physical closeness to caregiver but is not clingy; If child is active and moves away from caregiver, child maintains a visual or verbal connection with caregiver. 14. Turns head away, averts gaze, moves away, or sits facing away from caregiver without social referencing caregiver. Appears indifferent, aloof, withdrawn, or avoidant of caregiver. Converted Score* Score of 0 converts to 2 15. Social references caregiver while playing with toys. 16. After moving away, communicates to caregiver from across space by looking, gestures, or vocalizations. Total for Forming Relationships, Attachment, and Engagement TWO-WAY, PURPOSEFUL COMMUNICATION 17. Opens circles of communication: Initiates intentional actions with objects while also engaged in interactions with caregiver (e.g., manipulates object then looks at mother and smiles or vocalizes). 18. Gives signals: Initiates purposeful and intentional actions in play with objects. Scoring: 0 = Needs considerable help to get started in play or to engage in purposeful actions; no clear gestures or organized intent 1 = Initiates play but engages in stereotypic actions; e.g., lining toys up, mouthing toys for long periods of time, banging toys without engaging in any other actions with the same toy OR initiates play but actions appear aimless or disorganized. 2 = Play shows intentionality and variety, engaging in two or more different behaviors with a given toy or activity. Gestures are specific and activity is functionally tied to objects. 19. Closes circles: Responds to caregiver's cues in contingent manner (e.g.,
mother offers toy, baby takes it and puts it in a container). Scoring: 0 = Does not notice caregiver's response; 1 = Notices caregiver's response and looks, but does not respond contingently through actions; instead does something that has nothing to do with what caregiver did (e.g., mother holds toy out for child; child looks at mother and toy, then returns to what he was doing before); 2 = Notices caregiver's response, then responds contingently by elaborating on what caregiver did, by taking toy held by caregiver and examining it, by imitating her, or some other response that is clearly linked to what caregiver did.

20. Uses language (e.g., sounds, words, and/or gestures) during 188

interactions. Circle which ones were used. Total for Two-Way, Purposeful Communication BEHAVIORAL ORGANIZATION, PROBLEM-SOLVING, AND INTERNALIZATION (A Complex Sense of Self) 21. Engages in complex patterns of communication stringing together several circles of communication with caregiver (initiated and elaborated on by child) using gestures, vocalizations, and/or words. Scoring: 0 = 0 to 2 circles 1 = 3 to 5 circles 2 = 6 or more circles 22. Imitates or copies something new that the caregiver introduces, then incorporates idea into play (e.g., caregiver feeds doll; child copies this). Total for Behavioral Organization, Problem-Solving, and Internalization REPRESENTATIONAL CAPACITY (Elaboration) 23. Engages in symbolic play with the various toys or equipment (e.g., plays out cars racing), going beyond simple concrete actions (e.g., feeding self with cup). 24. Engages in pretend play patterns of at least one idea in collaboration with caregiver (e.g., one part of a script or scenario played out). 25. Uses language or pretend play (e.g., playing out with doll figures) to communicate needs, wishes, intentions, or feelings. 26. Uses pretend play to express themes around closeness or dependency (e.g., putting dolls to sleep next to one another; feeding caregiver and dolls). 27. Uses pretend play to express themes around pleasure and excitement around humorous theme (e.g., imitating humorous behaviors). 28. Uses pretend play to express themes around assertiveness (e.g., cars racing). 29. Creates pretend drama with two or more ideas that are not related or logically connected. Total for Representational Capacity (Elaboration) REPRESENTATIONAL DIFFERENTIATION (Building Bridges Between Ideas and Emotional Thinking) 30. Pretend play, however unrealistic, involves 2 or more ideas, which are logically tied to one another. Child may build on adult's pretend play idea. 31. Elaborates on pretend play sequence of two or more ideas, which are logically connected and grounded in reality. There is a planned quality and child can elaborate to "how", "why", or "when" questions, giving depth to drama. 32. Uses pretend play or language to communicate themes containing 2 189

or more ideas dealing with closeness or dependency (e.g., doll gets hurt, then gets kiss from daddy, then plays ball together). 33. Uses pretend play or language to communicate themes containing 2 or more ideas dealing with pleasure and excitement in humorous game (e.g., imitates funny word heard, watches how caregiver reacts, then laughs). 34. Uses pretend play or language to communicate themes containing 2 or more ideas dealing with assertiveness (e.g., soldiers search for missing person, find her, then battle to save her again) Total for Representational Differentiation (Emotional Thinking) TOTAL CHILD SCORE FOR SCALE

190

Childs name:______________________ Functional Emotional Assessment Scale Profile Form (Cut-off Scores) Score

Subtest Caregiver
Self-regulation & interest in the world Forming relationships and engagement

SYM

SENS

EXAM

***

***

***

Normal ********** 4-6 7-8 9-10 12-14 6-10 2-6 42-54 12-14 14-16 8-10 2-4 8-14 2-10 48-96 93-120

At Risk ********** 6 8 11 5

Deficient ********** 0-3 0-5 0-7 0-10 0-4 0-1 0-39 0-10 0-12 0-6 0-1

2-way purposeful communication Behavioral organization and problem solving Representational capacity Representational differentiation Total Caregiver Score Child
Self-regulation & interest in the world Forming relationships and engagement

40-41 11 13 7

2-way purposeful communication Behavioral organization and problem solving Representational capacity Representational differentiation Total Score
Total FEAS Score

8-14

7 0-1 0-45 0-85

46-47 86-92

191

Appendix D Digital Versatile Disc Tracks DVD Cut 1: Matthew: Pirouetting into Musical Awareness & Relatedness DVD Cut 2: Matthew: Rockin for Musical Communicativeness DVD Cut 3: Matthew: Blowin to the Blues for with Intentionality: Musical Relatedness & Communicativeness DVD Cut 4: Matthew: Play-dohto facilitate Musical Interresponsivess DVD Cut 5: Janet: From Scripted Words to Relatedness and Initiating Relationship DVD Cut 6: Janet: Cymbal Play to Increase Relatedness and Relationship DVD Cut 7: Janet: Working through Perseverative Movements for Musical Communicativeness DVD Cut 8: Janet: Working through Perseverative Movements for Musical Communicativeness (Part II) DVD Cut 9: Janet: Using the Spanish Idiom to Help Organize and Facilitate Initiation DVD Cut 10: Janet: Take a Walk to the Moon: Joining and Staying in the Interaction DVD Cut 11: Janet: Take a Walk to the Moon: Joining and Staying in the Interaction Part II DVD Cut 12: Timmy: The First Encounter through the Basic Beat: The Opening of Relationship & Musical Interresponsiveness DVD Cut 13: Timmy: Good Morning to Percy: Initiating and Expressing an Idea DVD Cut 14: Timmy: Good Morning to Percy Part II DVD Cut 15: Timmy: Fun for Four Drums: Engagement, Flexibility, Problem-solving, and a Continuous Flow of Communication DVD Cut 16: Timmy: Fun for Four Drums Part II

192

DVD Cut 17: Timmy: Fun for Four Drums Part III DVD Cut 18: Timmy: Exploring and Initiating Musical Changes on the Piano: Musical Communicativeness and Interrelatedness DVD Cut 19: Timmy: Thats Beautiful Music: Exploring the Elements DVD Cut 20: Timmy: Consolidating: Piecing it all Together through Spontaneous Music Making DVD Cut 21: Timmy: Consolidating Part II DVD Cut 22: Melinda: Engagement: Working in and through a Meltdown DVD Cut 23: Melinda: Self-absorption into Interaction: An Opening to Musical Awareness and Relationship DVD Cut 24: Melinda: Connecting Verbal Jargon: Snap-shots of Engagement leading to Musical Awareness DVD Cut 25: Melinda: Connecting Sensory (music), Affect (emotion), and Motor though Cymbal Play: Musical Relatedness and Two-way Purposeful Interaction DVD Cut 26: Melinda: Exploring the Drum and Cymbal: Shared Attention and Musical Relatedness DVD Cut 27: Melinda: Cause and Effect Relating though Horn Blowing DVD Cut 28: Melinda: Initiating Dynamic and Tempo Changes DVD Cut 29: Melinda: Connected by the Gleam in her Eye

193

Appendix E
Goal Attainment Scale for Matthew Pre-test Date: 12/5/2007 *Pre-test Level of Functioning
Rating Levels Goal 1: Self-regulation and interest in the world: Musical awareness Child never demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

Post-Test Date: 4/14/2008 ***Post-Test Level of Functioning


Goal 2: Engagement & shared attention: Musical relatedness Goal 3: Initiates interaction: Relationship within musical play *Child never enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative Goal 4: Problem-solving: Musical interresponsiveness *Never uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Rarely uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play ***Sometimes uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Often uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Consistently uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Goal 5: 2-way purposeful communication: musical communicativeness *Never able to use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Rarely uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play ***Sometimes uses music communicatively opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Often uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Will consistently use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play

(-2) Most unfavorable outcome Never % of time (-1) Less then expected success Rarely 1%-25% of time

*When involved in musical play child never plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and or movement). When involved in musical play child rarely plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and/or movement). ***When involved in musical play child sometimes plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). When involved in musical play child often plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). When involved in musical play child consistently plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement).

*Child rarely demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

Child rarely enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

(0) Expected level Sometimes 26%-50% of time

***Child sometimes demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

***Child sometimes enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

(+1) More then expected success Often 51%-75% of time

Child often demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

Child often enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

(+2) Most favorable outcome Consistently 76%-100% of time

Child consistently demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

Child consistently enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

194

Appendix F
Goal Attainment Scale for Janet Pre-test Date: 11/28/07 *Pre-test Level of Functioning Post-Test Date: 4/14/2008 ***Post-Test Level of Functioning

Rating Levels

Goal 1: Engagement & shared attention: Musical relatedness When involved in musical play child never plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and or movement). *When involved in musical play child rarely plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and/or movement). When involved in musical play child sometimes plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). ***When involved in musical play child often plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). When involved in musical play child consistently plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement).

(-2) Most unfavorable outcome Never % of time (-1) Less then expected success Rarely 1%-25% of time (0) Expected level Sometimes 26%-50% of time (+1) More then expected success Often 51%-75% of time (+2) Most favorable outcome Consistently 76%-100% of time

Goal 2: Initiates interaction: Relationship within musical play Never enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative.

Goal 3: 2-way purposeful communication: musical communicativeness Never able to use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication, while engaged in musical play

Goal 4: Bridging musical ideas: Musical interrelatedness Never uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play *Rarely uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play Sometimes uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play ***Often uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play Consistently uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play

*Rarely enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative.

*Rarely uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication, while engaged in musical play Sometimes uses music communicatively opening and closing circles of communication, while engaged in musical play ***Often uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication, while engaged in musical play Will consistently use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication, while engaged in musical play

Enters into relationship, within musical play on own initiative.

***Often enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative.

Consistently enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative.

195

Appendix G
Goal Attainment Scale for Timmy Pre-test Date: 11/28/2007 *Pre-test Level of Functioning Post-Test Date: 4/14/2008 ***Post-Test Level of Functioning

Rating Levels

(-2) Most unfavorable outcome Never % of time (-1) Less then expected success Rarely 1%-25% of time (0) Expected level Sometimes 26%-50% of time (+1) More then expected success Often 51%-75% of time (+2) Most favorable outcome Consistently 76%-100% of time

Goal 1: Bridging musical ideas: Musical interrelatedness Never uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play *Rarely uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play Sometimes uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play Often uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play ***Consistently uses music interrelatedly, connecting musical ideas with therapists and elaborates during musical play

Goal 2: Problem-solving: Musical interresponsiveness *Never uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play

Goal 3: Initiates interaction: Relationship within musical activity Child never enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

Goal 4: 2-way purposeful communication: musical communicativeness Never able to use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play *Rarely uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Sometimes uses music communicatively opening and closing circles of communication during musical play ***Often uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Will consistently use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play

Goal 5: Emotional Range: Range of Expressiveness *Never displays flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo during musical play

Rarely uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Sometimes uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play ***Often uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Consistently uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play

*Child rarely enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

Rarely displays flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo during musical play ***Sometimes displays flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo during musical play Often displays flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo during musical play Consistently displays flexibility in dynamics and/or tempo during musical play

Child sometimes enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative Child often enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

***Child consistently enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

196

Appendix H
Goal Attainment Scale for Melinda Pre-test Date: 11/28/2007 *Pre-test Level of Functioning Post-Test Date: 4/14/2008 ***Post-Test Level of Functioning

Rating Levels

(-2) Most unfavorable outcome Never % of time (-1) Less then expected success Rarely 1%-25% of time (0) Expected level Sometimes 26%-50% of time (+1) More then expected success Often 51%-75% of time (+2) Most favorable outcome Consistently 76%-100% of time

Goal 1: Self-regulation and interest in the world: Musical awareness Child never demonstrates musical awareness during musical play *Child rarely demonstrates musical awareness during musical play ***Child sometimes demonstrates musical awareness during musical play Child often demonstrates musical awareness during musical play Child consistently demonstrates musical awareness during musical play

Goal 2: Engagement & shared attention: Musical relatedness

*When involved in musical play child never plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and or movement). When involved in musical play child rarely plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally, and/or movement). ***When involved in musical play child sometimes plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). When involved in musical play child often plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement). When involved in musical play child consistently plays in a related manner to improvised music (vocally and/or instrumentally and/or movement).

Goal 3: Initiates interaction: Relationship within musical play *Child never enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative Child rarely enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative ***Child sometimes enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative Child often enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative Child consistently enters into relationship within musical play on own initiative

Goal 4: Problem-solving: Musical interresponsiveness

Goal 5: 2-way purposeful communication: musical communicativeness *Never able to use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Rarely uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play ***Sometimes uses music communicatively opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Often uses music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play Will consistently use music communicatively, opening and closing circles of communication during musical play

*Never uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Rarely uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play ***Sometimes uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Often uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play Consistently uses music interresponsively by imitating or copying a musical idea then incorporate it into musical play

197

198

You might also like