Motivation, Pay Satisfaction
Motivation, Pay Satisfaction
Motivation, Pay Satisfaction
www.emeraldinsight.com/1176-6093.htm
Motivation
Motivation, pay satisfaction, of front-line
and job satisfaction of front-line employees
employees
161
Carolyn Stringer
University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Jeni Didham
Westpac, Sydney, Australia, and
Paul Theivananthampillai
University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Abstract
Purpose – This paper aims to explore the complex relationships between intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation, pay satisfaction and job satisfaction at the retailer that uses a pay-for-performance plan for
front-line employees.
Design/methodology/approach – This paper draws on a single organization case study across
seven stores, and uses a survey, archival documents, open-ended questions and researcher interaction
with employees and managers.
Findings – The results provide some support for the complementary nature of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation. Intrinsic motivation was positively associated with pay and job satisfactions, whereas
extrinsic motivation was negatively associated with job satisfaction, and not associated with pay
satisfaction. The qualitative insights indicate that pay fairness is important, and those who perceived
pay was not fair generally made comparisons with others or felt that pay did not reflect their effort.
It is also found that the majority of employees perceived that goals were clear.
Research limitations/implications – The dominance of extrinsic motivation without including
behavioural, social, and psychological factors in agency theory research is questioned. The research
finds no support for “crowding out”, but rather finds some evidence of “crowding in” where intrinsic
motivation is enhanced, to the detriment of extrinsic motivation.
Practical implications – The findings highlight that managers should enhance both intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation, and pay employees well to increase job satisfaction.
Originality/value – Few studies examine incentives for front-line employees, and there is evidence
that minimum wage employees can have high intrinsic motivation. Perceptions of pay fairness can
vary across motivation levels, age, and gender.
Keywords Incentives (psychology), Motivation (psychology), Intrinsic motivation, Pay satisfaction,
Job satisfaction, Employees
Paper type Case study
The authors would like to thank Professor Ralph Adler, Dr Rosalind Whiting, Associate
Professor David Lont, Professor Roger Willett, Dr Michael Falta, Sriyalatha Kumarasinghe, and Qualitative Research in Accounting &
Management
seminar participants in the Department of Accountancy and Business Law at the University of Vol. 8 No. 2, 2011
Otago for their insightful comments. The authors are also grateful for the comments provided by pp. 161-179
q Emerald Group Publishing Limited
Associate Professor Keith T. Jones and the participants of the 2008 American Accounting 1176-6093
Association Conference in Los Angeles. DOI 10.1108/11766091111137564
QRAM 1. Introduction
8,2 No other incentive or motivational technique comes close to money[1].
Such a quotation reflects the dominant view in the management accounting literature that
pay-for-performance incentive systems have a motivational effect. In fact, some authors
assert that the primary aim of incentives is to enhance extrinsic motivation by satisfying
162 an individual employee’s needs indirectly through means of pay and bonuses (Anthony
and Govindarajan, 2007; Kunz and Pfaff, 2002). The link between pay-for-performance
and extrinsic motivation is explicit in the agency theory literature. Extrinsic motivation is
the importance placed on external rewards, such as bonuses and promotion (Van Herpen
et al., 2005), or the threat of wage cuts or dismissal (Frey, 1997). In an agency theory
context, incentive contracts designed to encourage extrinsic motivation are held to be
indispensable (Osterloh et al., 2002). Similarly, expectancy theory argues that linking
incentives to performance motivates employees to increase their effort and performance
(Jenkins et al., 1998; Lawler, 1973; Vroom, 1964).
The literature has shown that remuneration issues play a critical role in organizations;
however, there has been little advice that companies could use in terms of when certain
incentive system designs have been more (or less) appropriate (Heneman et al., 2002a;
Merchant, 1989; Merchant et al., 2003). The need for more theoretical development is
important because the use of pay-for-performance plans is growing (Broadbent and
Cullen, 2005; Lawler, 2000), and there is a trend to increasing variable pay as a percentage
of total remuneration (Heneman et al., 2002b). Another gap in the literature has been that
most of the economics-based incentives research has been at the chief executive officer
level, and few studies have examined incentives at the business unit or for front-line
employees (Kominis and Emmanuel, 2005; Merchant et al., 2003; Prendergast, 1999).
A challenge to the view that pay increases motivation comes from cognitive evaluation
theory, which argues that incentive pay has a negative effect on intrinsic motivation – a
“crowding-out” effect (Gagne and Deci, 2005). However, research findings into the
crowding-out effect have been mixed (Kunz and Pfaff, 2002). To explain the mixed results
from the crowding-out studies, self-determination theory argues that it depends on the
extent to which individuals feel autonomy (i.e. their actions are self-determined) or that
they are being controlled, such as where pay is linked to achieving specific outcomes (Deci
and Ryan, 2008). There are also implications for pay-for-performance plans where pay is
perceived as inequitable (Adams, 1965), as individuals are continually making
comparisons between themselves and others (Festinger, 1954).
Understanding the design implications of incentive systems is important to our
understanding of employee motivation and whether these incentive systems are related to
satisfaction. The motivation for this paper comes from calls for future management
accounting research to address three issues. First, we are challenged to examine the design
implications of pay-for-performance plans at lower levels in organizations (Kominis and
Emmanuel, 2005, 2007; Merchant et al., 2003). Second, we seek to understand more about
the relationship between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation (Kominis and Emmanuel, 2005,
2007; Kunz and Pfaff, 2002). Third, we draw on the organizational psychology and human
resource literatures to develop the existing management accounting literature further
(Kominis and Emmanuel, 2005).
The purpose of this paper is to examine employee motivation, pay satisfaction and job
satisfaction for a retailer that uses a pay-for-performance plan for front-line employees.
The survey data have been used to examine four research propositions. We find no Motivation
significant association between extrinsic motivation and pay satisfaction, which of front-line
questions the rudiments of the agency theory. Intrinsic motivation is positively
associated with pay satisfaction. This suggests the “crowding-out” effect may be employees
overstated (Kunz and Pfaff, 2002). We find that extrinsic motivation and job satisfaction
are negatively associated. Finally, intrinsic motivation is positively associated with job
satisfaction. 163
We make four contributions to the literature. First, we find some evidence to suggest
that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation may be complementary. Almost half the sample
of employees highly rate elements of both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. This
combination of high intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, coupled with the negative
association between pay and extrinsic motivation, challenges the agency theory
argument that extrinsic rewards are necessary to ensure employees do not minimize
their effort (i.e. shirk) (Bonner and Sprinkle, 2002). The potential for intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation to be complementary has been discussed in the management
accounting literature (Dermer, 1975; Kominis and Emmanuel, 2005, 2007; Kunz and
Pfaff, 2002; Ronen and Livingstone, 1975). The second contribution is the positive
association between pay and intrinsic motivation, which suggests that the
“crowding-out” literature may have overstated the case that extrinsic rewards have a
negative impact on intrinsic motivation (Kunz and Pfaff, 2002). Our third contribution is
finding that intrinsic motivation increases job satisfaction, whereas extrinsic motivation
decreases job satisfaction. This is consistent with the “crowding-in” argument
(Frey, 1997). The fourth contribution is that perceptions of pay unfairness cut across
levels of motivation, age, and gender and are generally based on comparisons with other
employees or feelings that pay did not reflect an individual’s effort.
The literature section is followed by a description of the case study and survey, then
the discussion and analysis of the quantitative and qualitative results, and finally the
conclusions.
2. Literature
2.1 Motivation and pay satisfaction
The philosophy behind pay-for-performance plans draws upon reinforcement theory
that suggests that pay be linked to performance by setting specific targets and then
rewarding individuals for achieving these targets (Heneman, 1992; Skinner, 1953).
Agency theory is based on compensation contracts designed to balance trade-offs
between risk and incentives for individuals and groups in order to align the interests of
managers and shareholders (Eisenhardt, 1989; Jensen and Meckling, 1976). Agency
theory focuses on extrinsic rewards that are tangible and quantifiable (Merchant et al.,
2003). Actions are rewarded and punished, and so this determines employees’ actions
and effort and performance (Jensen and Murphy, 2004; Kreps, 1997). Therefore, pay is a
powerful lever to influence extrinsic motivation (Locke et al., 1980).
This gives rise to the following research proposition:
P1. The association between pay satisfaction and extrinsic motivation is positive.
The other form of motivation, intrinsic motivation, captures the aspects of doing work
for its own sake (Osterloh and Frey, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2000b), provides psychological
benefits of well-being (Ryan and Deci, 2000b), accomplishment (Dermer, 1975),
QRAM increasing responsibility (Herzberg, 2003), self-actualisation (Kunz and Pfaff, 2002), and
8,2 is self-sustaining (Osterloh and Frey, 2002). A number of authors argue that money is a
poor motivator and can actually impede intrinsic motivation, such as reducing creativity
and innovation (Amabile, 1998; Frey and Osterloh, 2002; Herzberg, 2003; Kerr, 1975;
Kohn, 1993). In fact, the specific focus on extrinsic motivation may distract attention
from the task; this has been termed the “hidden cost of rewards” (Lepper and Greene,
164 1978). This view has been incorporated in the “crowding theory” (Frey and Jegen, 2001).
When an activity is intrinsically appealing (e.g. challenging), the positive effects can be
undermined if extrinsic rewards are also linked to the activity (Frey and Osterloh, 2002)
and crowd out intrinsic motivation (Lee and Whitford, 2007). This crowding out has
been further elaborated under the cognitive evaluation theory, which proposes that pay
(extrinsic motivation) can erode intrinsic motivation, and this in turn reduces
performance (Gagne and Deci, 2005; Kunz and Pfaff, 2002).
This leads to the research proposition:
P2. The association between pay satisfaction and intrinsic motivation is negative.
3.2 Sample
The survey data were collected using a sample of the retailer employees in seven stores
in two regions. Participants were chosen based on accessibility and availability
(Champion, 2002). The main advantage of this convenient sample was to achieve a
larger response rate, so that each store was visited by one of the researchers, rather
than posting the questionnaire. One area manager acted as a liaison for the researchers
and enabled access to five stores in his region.
3.4 Instruments
We used three established instruments. The short form Minnesota Satisfaction
Questionnaire was used to measure job satisfaction (Weiss et al., 1967), Vroom’s (1964)
model by Nadler and Lawler (1977) and the Pay Satisfaction Questionnaire (PSQ)
developed by Heneman and Schwab (1985) were used to measure pay satisfaction. The
descriptive statistics for the variables are shown in Table II.
The correlation matrix in Table III identifies the Cronbach’ alpha as the leading
diagonal, and the correlation between constructs lie below that diagonal[4]. As the
matrix highlights, the Cronbach’ alphas are all above 0.8, which Nunnally (1978) views
as excellent. Furthermore, the Cronbach’ alphas are higher than the correlation
between constructs, which supports that each of the constructs are reliable and have
high discriminant validity. Table III shows that each of the variables are reliable and
also that they are distinct from each other.
Gender
Female 60 (66)
Male 29 (32)
No details 2 (2)
Age
15-24 40 (44)
25-34 18 (20)
35-44 16 (18)
Over 45 14 (15)
No details 3 (3)
Type
Full time 60 (66)
Part time 26 (29)
No details 5 (5)
Table I.
Note: Percent values are represented in parenthesis Demographic details
QRAM 3.5 Analysis of constructs
8,2 The three constructs: pay satisfaction, motivation (intrinsic and extrinsic), and job
satisfaction were initially examined as distinct from one another, as they were all
measured through separate instruments (Igalens and Roussel, 1999).
Pay satisfaction. The PSQ was adapted from Heneman and Schwab (1985) to include
four questions on bonuses. The mean score for all items was calculated for each
168 respondent within the sample, and these were then combined to gain an overall mean
of 3.1 for the total sample. Analysing the individual scores shows that the majority
(87 percent) of employees were neither “very satisfied” nor “very dissatisfied” with
their compensation package (ratings between 2 and 4). While this is not an ideal
situation, one store manager noted that:
[. . .] it is very difficult to satisfy individuals with regard to pay, to hear that so many of the
employees are not dissatisfied, but rather neutral, is not a disappointing result.
Work motivation – intrinsic and extrinsic. Analysis of the work motivation construct
was calculated using the questions of ratings of importance of intrinsic and extrinsic
rewards adapted from Nadler and Lawler (1977). Factor analysis shows that ratings of
importance for pay, bonuses and benefits loaded onto one factor categorised as extrinsic
motivation[5]. The importance of feeling good, gaining new skills and abilities,
job security, chances to learn new things, promotion, accomplishment, freedom, praise
from supervisor and friendship loaded onto a factor termed intrinsic motivation. Praise
from supervisors loaded on both factors and was added to the intrinsic factor.
The purpose of this analysis was not only to examine the research propositions but also
to enable an understanding of the qualitative comments by classifying the intrinsic and
extrinsic motivations of employees.
Job satisfaction. A mean job satisfaction score was calculated for each individual, and
using this analysis, an overall job satisfaction score of 3.5 was then available for the
whole sample. This is an outcome which illustrates that, in general, employees are
moderately satisfied with their job. Those mean scores equal to or over 4.0 were deemed
“very satisfied” (22 percent), those less than or equal to 2.0 were “very dissatisfied”
Out of 91 employees, 77 provided some qualitative comments for at least one of these
questions.
The research protocols used in the data analysis involved several matrices to display
the data, as they are useful to highlight consistency and differences across employees
(Miles and Huberman, 1994). The matrices have been developed so that contrasting
patterns, if any, could be examined (Corbin and Strauss, 1990; Eisenhardt and Graebner,
2007). First, the qualitative comments for each of the questions were typed into a matrix.
Second, the questions relating to pay fairness, goal clarity, and motivation were Motivation
analysed. This involved coding the themes from the qualitative comments as follows: of front-line
.
goal clarity as clear goals/unclear goals; employees
.
pay fairness was coded as fair/unfair; and
.
motivation was coded as motivation has increased/decreased/remained
unchanged. 171
To increase construct validity, two researchers independently coded the qualitative
comments, and coding checks showed that the two raters agreed in over 98 percent of
cases (this is over the 90 percent suggested by Miles and Huberman (1994)). A further
reliability check was undertaken, whereby a random selection of qualitative comments
was re-coded to ensure consistency between raters and over time.
We find that 82 percent of employees who commented perceived that store and
department goals were clear. This may be a result of organizational structures and
processes, such as a visual display of goals in the staff room. As most employees
perceived that their goals were clear, we have focused on employee perceptions of pay
fairness to categorise the groups.
For the high extrinsic/high intrinsic employees who have provided comments, we
find 20 have commented that pay was fair (group 1), while 14 perceived pay was unfair
(group 2). For comparison, we look at the other employees and we find that
17 employees felt that pay was unfair (group 3), whereas 19 employees perceived pay
was fair (group 4) (Table V). We also found no differences between these four groups in
relation to gender, age, store, and full- or part-time employment.
Insights into perceptions of pay fairness in the high intrinsic/high extrinsic motivation
group. Of the 42 employees who have high intrinsic and high extrinsic motivation, we
find that there are two groups within this cluster: those who feel pay was fair (group 1)
and those who feel pay was unfair (group 2). Group 1 includes 20 employees who have
perceived that pay was fair. The insights from employees who perceive that pay was
fair showed that their comments on employee motivation has increased or was
unchanged. These quotes reflect their views and typically they discussed how they
were motivated intrinsically (e.g. challenge, work):
Motivated by the challenges each day brings. Reasonably happy with pay. [Goals] very clear
(Respondent no. 19)[10].
Regardless of what I receive my motivation is always high, i.e. I would love my job on $9 or
$20 per hour. Pay is reasonably fair, although in retrospect feel I do more than the pay scale
I am in allows. I actually feel the goals to some degree are redundant in respect of my dept.
My goals are always to be the top whatever I do. I take pride in my job and performance but
feel some of the goals are very pedantic again that is because the [retailer] is evolving and
branching into different areas, i.e. sales and expertise in specialist areas (No. 55).
Comments
Pay fair Pay unfair received(n)
5. Conclusions
This study examines the implications of a pay-for-performance system on intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation and job satisfaction of front-line employees at a retailer. We find
positive associations between pay and intrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation
and job satisfaction, while there was a negative association between extrinsic
motivation and job satisfaction. Pay satisfaction had the strongest association with job
satisfaction. The practical implications of this for managers are to pay their front-line
employees well and job satisfaction will be high. While focusing on extrinsic rewards
has merit, future agency theory research needs to draw on behavioural models from
psychology and sociology to recognise the potential of intrinsic, as well as extrinsic
rewards to motivate employees (Frey, 1997; Kreps, 1997; Merchant et al., 2003). We find
pay satisfaction and intrinsic motivation are positively correlated and some evidence
that “crowding in” does occur with intrinsic motivation increasing and extrinsic
motivation decreasing. These findings are in contrast to the focus in the literature on
“crowding out” (Frey, 1997). The relationship between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation
needs to be the focus of future studies.
One of the most interesting findings is that around half of these front-line employees
rate extrinsic and intrinsic motivation as of high importance. In a retail outlet where the
minimum wage is the norm, to find employees that are high on both intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation is a pleasant surprise. This evidence provides support for the
argument in the management accounting literature that intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation could be complementary in organizational settings (Dermer, 1975; Kominis
and Emmanuel, 2005, 2007; Kunz and Pfaff, 2002; Ronen and Livingstone, 1975).
Our findings also are in contrast to the self-determination theory literature that argues
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are on a continuum from autonomous to controlled
motivation, which necessarily means that employees could not be high on both intrinsic
QRAM and extrinsic motivation (Deci and Ryan, 2008). Future research needs to focus on how
8,2 intrinsic and extrinsic motivation can enhance employee performance at different levels
in organizations (Bonner and Sprinkle, 2002; Merchant et al., 2003).
We find that 82 percent of the employees who provided qualitative comments
considered store and departmental goals were clear, which is pleasing for the retailer.
This is consistent with the literature that clear goals are important to ensure that
174 employees understand what is expected from them; this enables them to focus their effort
and leads to higher levels of motivation (Emmanuel et al., 2008; Latham and Baldes, 1975;
Locke and Latham, 1990). We also find that a substantial number of employees who are
high on both extrinsic and intrinsic motivation, who according to self-determination
theory, exhibit high levels of autonomy, also perceive their pay is unfair. This indicates
that pay fairness and equity are not determined by one’s motivation levels or even goal
clarity. However, the 40 percent of employees who perceived that pay was unfair
generally held views that this was driven by comparisons to others or that pay was not
reflective of their effort. Our findings support social comparison and equity theory
that pay inequities can have important outcomes (Adams, 1965; Festinger, 1954;
Greenberg et al., 2007). Future research is needed to examine perceptions of fairness and
the implications for employee motivation, as few studies have examined these issues in
any depth (Van Herpen et al., 2005; Zapata-Phelan et al., 2009).
There are a number of limitations with this research. This study examines a
pay-for-performance scheme in one organization for front-line employees, and care needs to
be taken when generalizing to other contexts (e.g. managers). However, a single case site is
appropriate when the unit of analysis is at the employee level (Anderson and Widener,
2007). The single site has the advantage of controlling for external factors and enables us to
focus on differences at the employee level (Burney et al., 2009). While single case studies are
useful and rich research sites, the challenge for future research will be to develop multiple
case studies to provide a stronger base for theory building and testing (Yin, 1994; Eisenhardt
and Graebner, 2007). Another limitation is that we were unable to test non-response bias, as
employees were assured anonymity. As a surrogate, we have tested the responses between
the stores where the researcher stayed for the whole time and the stores where
questionnaires had to be left for staff to complete over several days. We find no significant
differences with regard to all the key variables tested in the research propositions.
Despite these limitations, the paper extends our understanding of employee
motivation with regard to the complementary nature of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation. Future papers are needed that extend this analysis, so organizations can
design incentive systems to leverage the potential of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation
and to take steps to improve employee perceptions of pay fairness.
Notes
1. This quote is from Locke et al. (1980) cited in Currall et al. (2005, p. 620).
2. Further information cannot be disclosed for confidentiality reasons.
3. If a team member leaves before the end of the bonus period, a payment will not be made;
however, if the team member completes the bonus period but leaves before the date of the
payment, the team member will still be eligible to receive the bonus.
4. We have used Spearman’s rho, as the distribution for extrinsic motivation was not normal
and the sample size was larger than 20.
5. Principal components analysis with Promax rotation was used. Motivation
6. We have used correlation analysis for P1 and P2, as we are not arguing for a causal or of front-line
directional relationship. Pay satisfaction may influence extrinsic or intrinsic motivation and
vice versa (Gardner et al., 2004; Gerhart and Milkovich, 1992). The use of correlation analysis for employees
hypothesis testing has held to be sufficient in prior papers (Merchant, 1990; Giraud et al., 2008).
7. The key assumptions of multiple regression were tested, and there was no evidence of
multi-colinearity and heteroscedasticity and the residual plots were normal. 175
8. There were no employees who could be held as having low intrinsic motivation, that is,
a score of below 2 on the scale.
9. We use the Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon W and find no significant difference between the
42 high extrinsic/high intrinsic and the 49 other employees.
10. The number given is attributed to an employee.
References
Adams, J.S. (1965), “Inequity in social exchange”, in Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 175-93.
Amabile, T.M. (1998), “How to kill creativity”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 76 No. 5, pp. 77-87.
Anderson, S.W. and Widener, S.K. (2007), “Doing quantitative field research in management
accounting”, in Chapman, C.S., Hopwood, A.G. and Shields, M.D. (Eds), Handbook of
Management Accounting Research, Vol. 1, Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 319-42.
Anthony, R. and Govindarajan, V. (2007), Management Control Systems, 12th ed., Irwin,
Singapore.
Austin, W., McGinn, N.C. and Susmilch, C. (1980), “Internal standards revisited: effects of social
comparisons and expectancies on judgments of fairness and satisfaction”, Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 16 No. 5, pp. 426-41.
Bodur, S. (2002), “Job satisfaction of health care staff employed at health centres in Turkey”,
Occupational Medicine, Vol. 52 No. 6, pp. 353-5.
Bonner, S.E. and Sprinkle, G.B. (2002), “The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task
performance: theories, evidence, and a framework for research”, Accounting,
Organizations and Society, Vol. 27 Nos 4/5, pp. 303-45.
Boswell, W.R., Bingham, J.B. and Colvin, A.J.S. (2006), “Aligning employees through ‘line of
sight’”, Business Horizon, Vol. 49 No. 6, pp. 499-509.
Broadbent, M. and Cullen, J. (2005), “Divisional control and performance”, in Berry, A.J.,
Broadbent, J. and Otley, D. (Eds), Management Control: Theories, Issues and Performance,
Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke, pp. 137-66.
Burney, L.L., Henley, C.A. and Widener, S.K. (2009), “A path model examining the relations
among strategic performance measurement system characteristics, organizational justice,
and extra- and in-role performance”, Accounting, Organizations and Society, Vol. 34
Nos 3/4, pp. 305-21.
Carr, S.C., McLoughlin, D., Hodgson, M. and MacLachlan, M. (1996), “Effects of unreasonable pay
discrepancies for under- and over-payment on double demotivation”, Genetic, Social, and
General Psychology Monographs, Vol. 122, pp. 475-94.
Carré, F. and Tilly, C. (2008), America’s Biggest Low-wage Industry: Continuity and Change in
Retail Jobs, The Institute for Research on Labor and Employment, University of California
Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, available at: www.escholarship.org/uc/item/6k91m7mk
(accessed 10 June 2010).
QRAM Champion, R. (2002), “Sampling can produce solid results”, Journal of Staff Development, Vol. 23
No. 1, pp. 1-3.
8,2
Cimete, G., Gencalp, N.S. and Keskin, G. (2003), “Quality and life and job satisfaction of nurses”,
Journal of Nursing Care Quality, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 151-9.
Corbin, J. and Strauss, A. (1990), “Grounded theory research: procedures, canons and evaluative
criteria”, Qualitative Sociology, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 3-21.
176 Cowherd, D.M. and Levine, D.I. (1992), “Product quality and pay equity between lower-level
employees and top management: an investigation of distributive justice theory”,
Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 37 No. 3, pp. 302-20.
Currall, S.C., Towler, A.J., Judge, T.A. and Kohn, L. (2005), “Pay satisfaction and organizational
outcomes”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 58 No. 3, pp. 613-40.
Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2008), “Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being
across life’s domains”, Canadian Psychology, Vol. 49 No. 1, pp. 14-23.
Dermer, J.D. (1975), “The interrelationship of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation”, Academy of
Management Journal, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 125-9.
Eisenhardt, K.M. (1989), “Agency theory: an assessment and review”, The Academy of
Management Review, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 57-74.
Eisenhardt, K.M. and Graebner, M.E. (2007), “Theory building from cases: opportunities and
challenges”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 50 No. 1, pp. 25-32.
Emmanuel, C.R., Kominis, G. and Slapincar, S. (2008), “The impact of target setting on
managerial motivation and performance”, paper presented at the American Accounting
Association Annual Meeting, Anaheim, CA.
Ferreira, L.D. and Merchant, K.A. (1992), “Field research in management accounting and control:
a review and evaluation”, Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal, Vol. 5 No. 4,
pp. 3-34.
Ferris, K.R. (1977), “A test of expectancy theory of motivation in an accounting environment”,
The Accounting Review, Vol. 52 No. 3, pp. 605-15.
Festinger, L.A. (1954), “A theory of social comparison processes”, Human Relations, Vol. 7 No. 2,
pp. 117-40.
Frey, B.S. (1997), “On the relationship between intrinsic and extrinsic work motivation”,
International Journal of Industrial Organization, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 427-39.
Frey, B.S. and Jegen, R. (2001), “Motivation crowding theory”, Journal of Economic Surveys,
Vol. 15 No. 5, pp. 589-611.
Frey, B.S. and Osterloh, M. (2002), Successful Management by Motivation, Balancing Intrinsic and
Extrinsic Incentives, Springer, Berlin.
Gagne, M. and Deci, E.L. (2005), “Self-determination theory and work motivation”, Journal of
Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 331-62.
Gardner, D.G., Van Dyne, L. and Pierce, J.L. (2004), “The effects of pay level on
organization-based self-esteem and performance: a field study”, Journal of Occupational
& Organizational Psychology, Vol. 77 No. 3, pp. 307-22.
Gerhart, B. and Milkovich, G.T. (1992), “Employee compensation: research and practice”,
in Dunnette, M.D. and Hough, L.M. (Eds), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational
Psychology, 2nd ed., Vol. 3, Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA, pp. 481-570.
Giraud, F., Langevin, P. and Mendoza, C. (2008), “Justice as a rationale for the controllability
principle: a study of managers’ opinions”, Management Accounting Research, Vol. 19 No. 1,
pp. 32-44.
Greenberg, J., Ashton-James, C.E. and Ashkanasy, N.M. (2007), “Social comparison processes in Motivation
organisations”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 102 No. 1,
pp. 22-41. of front-line
Hackman, R.J. and Oldman, G.R. (1976), “Motivation through the design of work: test of a employees
theory”, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 250-79.
Heneman, H.G. and Schwab, D.P. (1985), “Pay satisfaction: its multidimensional nature and
measurement”, International Journal of Psychology, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 129-41. 177
Heneman, R.L. (1992), Merit Pay: Linking Pay Increases to Performance Ratings,
Addison-Wesley-Longman, Reading, MA.
Heneman, R.L., Fay, C.H. and Wang, Z. (2002a), “Compensation systems in the global context”,
in Heneman, R.L. (Ed.), Strategic Reward Management: Design, Implementation, and
Evaluation, Information Age Publishing, Greenwich, CT, pp. 5-34.
Heneman, R.L., Greenberger, D.B. and Strasser, S. (1988), “The relationship between
pay-for-performance perceptions and pay satisfaction”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 41
No. 4, pp. 745-59.
Heneman, R.L., Ledford, G.E. Jr and Gresham, M.T. (2002b), “The changing nature of work and
its effects on compensation design and delivery”, in Heneman, R.L. (Ed.), Strategic Reward
Management: Design, Implementation, and Evaluation, Information Age Publishing,
Greenwich, CT, pp. 35-73.
Herzberg, F. (2003), “One more time: how do you motivate employees?”, Harvard Business
Review, Vol. 81 No. 1, pp. 53-62 (reprinted from 1968).
Igalens, J. and Roussel, P. (1999), “A study of the relationship between compensation package,
work motivation and job satisfaction”, Journal of Organisational Behaviour, Vol. 20 No. 7,
pp. 1003-25.
Jenkins, G.D., Mitra, A., Gupta, N. and Shaw, J.D. (1998), “Are financial incentives related to
performance? A meta-analytic review of empirical research”, Journal of Applied
Psychology, Vol. 83 No. 5, pp. 777-87.
Jensen, M.C. and Meckling, W.H. (1976), “Theory of the firm: managerial behavior, agency costs
and ownership structure”, Journal of Financial Economics, Vol. 3 No. 4, pp. 305-60.
Jensen, M.C. and Murphy, K.J. (2004), “Remuneration: where we’ve been, how we got here, what are the
problems, and how to fix them”, Working Paper No. 044/2004, European Corporate Governance
Institute, available at: www.ecgi.org/wp/wp_id.php?id¼65 (accessed 10 June 2010).
Kerr, S. (1975), “On the folly of rewarding A, while hoping for B”, The Academy of Management
Journal, Vol. 18 No. 4, pp. 769-83.
Kohn, A. (1993), “Why incentive plans cannot work”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 71 No. 5,
pp. 54-63.
Kominis, G. and Emmanuel, C.R. (2005), “Exploring the reward preferences of middle level
managers”, Qualitative Research in Accounting & Management, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 54-76.
Kominis, G. and Emmanuel, C.R. (2007), “The expectancy-valence theory revisited: developing an
extended model of managerial motivation”, Management Accounting Research, Vol. 18
No. 1, pp. 49-75.
Kreps, D.M. (1997), “Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives”, The American Economic
Review, Vol. 87 No. 2, pp. 359-64.
Kunz, A. and Pfaff, D. (2002), “Agency theory, performance evaluation and the hypothetical
construct of intrinsic motivation”, Accounting, Organizations and Society, Vol. 27 No. 3,
pp. 275-95.
QRAM Latham, G.P. and Baldes, J.L. (1975), “The practical significance of Locke’s theory of goal
setting”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 60 No. 1, pp. 122-4.
8,2
Lawler, E.E. III (1973), Motivation in the Workplace, Brooks/Cole Publishing, Monterey, CA.
Lawler, E.E. III (2000), Rewarding Excellence: Pay Strategies for the New Economy, Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco, CA.
Lee, S.-Y. and Whitford, A.B. (2007), “Exit, voice, loyalty, and pay: evidence from the public
178 workforce”, Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, Vol. 18 No. 4,
pp. 647-71.
Lepper, M.R. and Greene, D. (1978), “Overjustification research and beyond: towards a means-ends
analysis of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation”, in Lepper, M.R. and Greene, D. (Eds),
The Hidden Costs of Reward: New Perspectives on the Psychology of Human Motivation,
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 109-48.
Locke, E.A. and Latham, G.P. (1990), A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance,
Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Locke, E.A., Feren, D.B., McCaleb, V.M., Shaw, K.N. and Denny, A.T. (1980), “The relative
effectiveness of four methods of motivating employee performance”, in Duncan, K.D.,
Gruneberg, M.M. and Wallis, D. (Eds), Changes in Working Life, Wiley, New York, NY,
pp. 363-88.
Marginson, D. and Ogden, S. (2005), “Coping with ambiguity through the budget: the positive
effects of budgetary targets on managers’ budgeting behaviours”, Accounting,
Organizations and Society, Vol. 30 No. 5, pp. 435-56.
Maslow, A.H. (1954), Motivation and Personality, Harper & Row, New York, NY.
Merchant, K.A. (1989), Rewarding Results: Motivating Profit Centre Managers, Harvard Business
School Press, Boston, MA.
Merchant, K.A. (1990), “The effects of financial controls on data manipulation and management
myopia”, Accounting, Organizations and Society, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 297-313.
Merchant, K.A., Van der Stede, W. and Zheng, L. (2003), “Disciplinary constraints on the
advancement of knowledge: the case of organizational incentive systems”, Accounting,
Organizations and Society, Vol. 28 Nos 2/3, pp. 251-86.
Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994), An Expanded Sourcebook: Qualitative Data Analysis,
2nd ed., Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Moynihan, D.P. and Pandey, S.K. (2007), “Finding workable levers over work motivation,
comparing job satisfaction, job involvement, and organizational commitment”,
Administration & Society, Vol. 39 No. 7, pp. 803-32.
Nadler, D.A. and Lawler, E.E. III (1977), Motivation: A Diagnostic Approach, McGraw-Hill,
New York, NY.
Nunnally, J.C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, 2nd ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
Osterloh, M. and Frey, B.S. (2002), “Does pay for performance really motivate employees?”,
in Neely, A. (Ed.), Business Performance Measurement: Theory and Practice, Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, pp. 357-63.
Osterloh, M., Frost, J. and Frey, B.S. (2002), “The dynamics of motivation in new organizational
forms”, International Journal of the Economics of Business, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 61-77.
Pacheco, G. (2007), “A portrait of minimum wage workers in New Zealand: 1997-2004”,
New Zealand Journal of Employment Relations, Vol. 32 No. 3, pp. 2-17.
Pool, S.W. (1997), “The relationship of job satisfaction with substitutes of leadership, leadership
behavior, and work motivation”, The Journal of Psychology, Vol. 131 No. 3, pp. 271-83.
Porter, L.W. and Lawler, E.E. III (1968), Managerial Attitudes and Performance, R.D. Irwin, Motivation
Homewood, IL.
Prendergast, C. (1999), “The provision of incentives in firms”, Journal of Economic Literature,
of front-line
Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 7-63. employees
Ronen, J. and Livingstone, J.T. (1975), “An expectancy theory approach to the motivational
impacts of budgets”, The Accounting Review, Vol. 50 No. 4, pp. 671-85.
Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2000a), “Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and 179
new directions”, Contemporary Education Psychology, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 54-67.
Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2000b), “When rewards compete with nature: the undermining of
intrinsic motivation and self-regulation”, in Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J.M. (Eds),
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance,
Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 13-54.
Skinner, B.F. (1953), Science and Human Behavior, Macmillan, New York, NY.
Tietjen, M.A. and Myers, R.M. (1998), “Motivation and job satisfaction”, Management Decision,
Vol. 36 No. 4, pp. 226-31.
Van Herpen, M., Van Praag, M. and Cools, K. (2005), “The effects of performance measurement
and compensation on motivation: an empirical study”, De Economist, Vol. 153 No. 3,
pp. 303-29.
Vroom, V.H. (1964), Work and Motivation, Wiley, New York, NY.
Weiss, D.J., Dawis, R.V., England, G.W. and Lofquist, L.H. (1967), Manual for the Minnesota
Satisfaction Questionnaire, Industrial Relations Center, University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis, MN.
Wright, B.E. and Kim, S. (2004), “Participation’s influence on job satisfaction: the importance of
job characteristics”, Review of Public Personnel Administration, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 18-40.
Yin, R.K. (1994), Case Study Research: Design and Methods, Revised Edition, Sage,
Newbury Park, CA.
Zapata-Phelan, C.P., Colquitt, J.A., Scott, B.A. and Livingston, B. (2009), “Procedural justice,
international justice and task performance: the mediating role of intrinsic motivation”,
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, Vol. 108 No. 1, pp. 93-105.
Corresponding author
Carolyn Stringer can be contacted at: [email protected]