Review: Stroke, Cognitive Deficits, and Rehabilitation: Still An Incomplete Picture
Review: Stroke, Cognitive Deficits, and Rehabilitation: Still An Incomplete Picture
Review: Stroke, Cognitive Deficits, and Rehabilitation: Still An Incomplete Picture
predictor of recovery only in visual memory (24). Other studies Though the damage is diffuse, it may be possible to identify
have shown that lesion volume, while correlated with initial some regional specificity in the relationship between white matter
aphasia severity, was not associated with recovery in language abnormalities and cognition. Slowed processing and attentional
(25,26). The impact of lesion laterality on cognitive recovery is and executive impairments have been associated with the severity
unclear, with examples of right hemispheric superiority (27), left of WMHs in the internal capsule, caudate, and thalamus of stroke
hemispheric superiority (28), and no difference (24). patients (40). Lesions in these areas disrupt fronto-striato-
thalamic circuits and thus impact upon dorsolateral prefrontal
Aphasia and neglect cortex and anterior cingulate, regions known to support attention
In clinical practice, two of the most prominent focal cognitive and executive function. Cognitive impairment has been corre-
deficits after stroke are aphasia and hemispatial neglect (29). The lated with WMHs in the frontal lobes and internal capsule,
relationship between type of aphasia or neglect and location of with hyperintense lesions in the basal ganglia and thalamus
infarction is well described. The limitation to spoken output that most closely associated with neuropsychological performance
is characteristic of Broca’s aphasia is associated with damage (6). Others have found that, while WMHs were associated with
in the left posterior, inferior frontal gyrus, in addition to other reduced mental speed, executive function, memory, and visuo-
regions supplied by the upper division of the left middle cerebral spatial ability, regional correlation was relatively weak (41). The
artery (30). Wernicke’s aphasia, on the other hand, is character- impact of small-vessel disease is not restricted to stroke: incidence
ized by fluent but relatively meaningless speech alongside poor of new lacunes in an elderly population has been linked to decline
language comprehension and is linked to damage in the left in executive function and processing speed (9).
posterior, superior temporal gyrus (30). In hemispatial neglect, Advanced imaging techniques, such as diffusion tensor imaging
the visuospatial component is linked to the right inferior parietal (DTI), can identify subtle abnormalities in axonal function that
lobule, the visuomotor component to the right dorsolateral may be a marker for generalized cognitive impairment. Calculat-
prefrontal cortex, and the object-centered component to the deep ing functional anisotropy from DTI scans provides a metric for
temporal lobe regions (31). Structure–function evidence of this the integrity of white matter structures, with lower anisotropy
kind is often derived from lesion mapping, where lesions from a reflecting greater diffusivity. Lower anisotropy has been identified
group of stroke patients with a particular cognitive deficit can be in those with vascular cognitive impairment, even in regions
superimposed to determine the area of greatest overlap. This without visible white matter abnormalities on conventional MRI
powerful technique employs group data while accounting for (42). In ischemic stroke patients, cognition appears more closely
individual variability in brain structure. The problem is that while associated with anisotropy in frontal and parietal regions than in
the mapping approach can work well for focal syndromes associ- occipital and temporal areas (43). We know that predicting cog-
ated with lesions in circumscribed areas, more complex cognitive nitive outcome using the features of a focal lesion is imperfect;
deficits such as executive dysfunction involve more widely distrib- DTI offers the promise of increased explanatory power by deter-
uted injury and more likely a broad network. Neuropsychological mining the effects of altered connectivity between brain regions.
(32) and functional imaging (33) evidence indicates that execu-
tive processes are not limited to the frontal lobes but are sub- Hypoperfusion
served by a distributed network of cortical, sub-cortical, and Compromised blood flow is relevant to both focal and diffuse
infratentorial areas. Even in the case of neglect, it has been argued deficits. In ischemic stroke, focal cognitive deficits arise not just
that dysfunction of distributed cortical networks for attention from the infarction itself but also from hypoperfusion in adjacent
provides a better account than structural damage to specific tissue. In the acute setting, aphasia and neglect are more closely
regions (34). associated with the volume of peri-infarct perfusion failure than
the infarct itself (44). White matter hyperintensities, thought to
Diffuse neuronal dysfunction represent incomplete infarction, have been attributed to transient
There is a broad distinction between focal damage, which can lead decreases in cerebral blood flow (45). At the whole-brain level, the
to selective cognitive impairments, and diffuse neuronal dysfunc- dynamic nature of information processing, attention, and working
tion, which produces a more uniform profile of mental slow- memory may be uniquely susceptible to dynamic changes in blood
ing, memory problems, and executive deficits (35). Clouding the flow across widely distributed brain regions (46).
picture is the observation that certain strategic infarcts (e.g., genu Perhaps the clearest evidence for a cerebral hemodynamic
of the internal capsule) can produce what clinically appears to be effect is in large-vessel disease, where loss of perfusion pressure
a diffuse cognitive syndrome (36). Diffuse dysfunction typically to a cerebral hemisphere supplied by a blocked carotid artery
results from underlying sub-clinical cerebrovascular disease, such induces a series of hemodynamic responses, including dilation of
as white matter disease, or an accumulation of small infarcts as in cerebral arterioles and increase of oxygen extraction fraction.
small-vessel disease (37). Over the four years following stroke, TIA patients with recently symptomatic carotid artery occlusion
higher load of white matter hyperintensities (WMHs) is strongly who had single-hemisphere cerebral hypoperfusion had greater
associated with dementia and cognitive decline (38). Stroke cognitive impairment than those without this hypoperfusion
patients with white matter lesions and silent infarcts were worse (47). Hypoperfusion may also impact cognition through reduc-
on cognitive tasks at baseline and two-year follow-up than those tions in brain volume. Gray matter volume reductions have been
without this damage (39). identified in cognitively impaired ischemic stroke patients,
Domain-specific interventions decline (risk ratio = 19%), most likely by preventing the advance
Originally used in the context of upper extremity and motor of additional lacunar infarcts (85). This positive result, however,
retraining after stroke, the principles of constraint-induced was not reproduced in the PRoFESS trial, where treatment with
therapy have been applied to aphasia. Constraint-induced treat- antiplatelet and antihypertensive therapy made no difference to
ment protocols can improve functional communication in cognitive outcomes (86). Blood pressure control may also have a
chronic aphasia after stroke (73). There is also evidence that low- downside, given the association between hypoperfusion and cog-
frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) nitive performance. A quarter of a century ago, Meyer et al. (87)
can improve language abilities in patients with chronic nonfluent followed 52 multi-infarct dementia patients over two years.
aphasia. The theory is that the stimulation modulates and inhibits Among hypertensive patients, improved cognition was correlated
overactivity in the right hemisphere homologous language sites, with systolic blood pressure control within upper normal limits
such as the inferior frontal gyrus. Significant improvements (135–150 mmHg) and cognition worsened if blood pressure was
following rTMS have been reported in the picture naming reduced below this level. As more direct evidence of this link, a
(74), expressive language and auditory comprehension (75), and pilot study showed that cortical reperfusion with temporary
semantic fluency (76) of stroke patients with chronic aphasia. blood pressure increase for patients with large-vessel stenosis in
In stroke patients with hemispatial neglect, using prism glasses the acute stage resulted in improved cognition, both in the short
to create an optical shift of the visual field to the right has been and longer terms (88).
successful. The original 1998 study included patients in the first Pharmacological agents can influence cognition. Normally
year after stroke and revealed improvements in sensorimotor used to treat depression, escitalopram has been found to benefit
and spatial function after a single prism adaptation (77). More cognitive function in stroke patients (89). Compared with
recent work has shown that prism adaptation also works in patients who received placebo or problem solving therapy, those
chronic stroke. In patients who were one to seven years post- receiving escitalopram had improved global cognition on the
stroke and had persistent neglect, an eight-week intervention RBANS (Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsycho-
with prism glasses produced improved eye movements on the logical Status), particularly in memory. Rivastigmine, typically
neglected side and improved standing center of gravity (78). used in Alzheimer’s disease, was tested in a randomized con-
Prism adaptation may actually be best suited to the sub-acute trolled trial among stroke patients with cognitive impairment but
and chronic stages of stroke when the nature of the neglect not dementia (90). The treatment group exhibited significantly
deficit has been established. Prism adaptation in acute stroke improved performance on verbal fluency for animals relative to
patients yielded some benefits on spatial tasks (line bisection and controls. The results of these two studies and the previously cited
cancellation), but these benefits were not maintained at one study on attention process training (84), however, need to be
month posttreatment (79). It is likely that much of the improve- interpreted with caution until successfully replicated. All three
ment in both treatment and placebo control groups was due to studies featured improvement in one or two cognitive measures in
spontaneous recovery. the context of multiple other cognitive outcomes that were not
Cochrane reviews have revealed the paucity of studies in post- affected, and in each case there was an imbalance in baseline
stroke cognitive rehabilitation for both memory deficits (80) and performance between the groups.
attention deficits (81). Unlike the example of constraint-based Increasing physical activity is another potential treatment; it
therapy, the principles of task-specific training do not seem to has been shown to improve cognitive performance in the cogni-
translate from motor rehabilitation to cognitive rehabilitation. tively impaired (91), those at risk of Alzheimer’s disease (92), and
Repetition and rehearsal of the targeted cognitive task is insuffi- patients with multiple sclerosis (93). Benefits seem to be greatest
cient to produce meaningful improvement. Results from one in cognitive speed and attention (94) and executive control (95).
small stroke study (n = 12) indicated that teaching mnemonic This is important as these central cognitive processes provide the
strategies resulted in better memory performance, but improve- foundation for many other aspects of cognition. The relationship
ments did not generalize beyond the trained memory tasks (82). between physical and mental activity may also apply to stroke
In a small trial (n = 27), a computerized training program had patients, but there is scant empirical evidence to date. In a recent
significant effects on alertness and sustained attention, but the review, only 12 studies were identified that employed physical
benefits did not generalize to other attentional and cognitive activity interventions in stroke and had cognitive outcomes (96).
functions (83). In the years since the Cochrane reviews, one of the One of the few that had cognition as a primary focus found stroke
most promising studies was a randomized controlled trial of patients exposed to an eight-week exercise program had improved
‘attention process training’ in 78 stroke patients with attention information processing speed on a serial reaction time task
deficits (84). Postintervention change scores on the Integrated relative to control patients (97). Combining increased physical
Visual Auditory Continuous Performance Test were superior activity with mental challenges, sensory stimulation, and social
in the treatment group, and this superiority was maintained at interaction in an ‘enriched environment’ may contribute to
six-month follow-up. improvements in cognition after stroke. Most of the current evi-
dence for this comes from animals (98), but there are data from
Interventions for generalized cognitive impairment humans showing that listening to music early after stroke can
Hypertension is an obvious treatment target. In the PROGRESS enhance cognitive recovery (99). Epidemiological studies have
trial, active blood pressure lowering reduced the risk of cognitive demonstrated the cognitive value of interactions with other
31 Verdon V, Schwartz S, Lovblad KO, Hauert CA, Vuilleumier P. 52 Frank B, Maschke M, Groetschel H et al. Aphasia and neglect are
Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional compo- uncommon in cerebellar disease: negative findings in a prospective
nents: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Brain study in acute cerebellar stroke. Cerebellum 2010; 9:556–66.
2010; 133(Pt 3):880–94. 53 O’Halloran CJ, Kinsella GJ, Storey E. The cerebellum and neuro-
32 Vataja R, Pohjasvaara T, Mantyla R et al. MRI correlates of executive psychological functioning: a critical review. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
dysfunction in patients with ischaemic stroke. Eur J Neurol 2003; 2012; 34:35–56.
10:625–31. 54 Molinari M, Petrosini L, Misciagna S, Leggio MG. Visuospatial abili-
33 Fassbender C, Murphy K, Foxe JJ et al. A topography of executive ties in cerebellar disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;
functions and their interactions revealed by functional magnetic 75:235–40.
resonance imaging. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004; 20:132–43. 55 Ravizza SM, McCormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, Fiez JA.
34 Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working
Annu Rev Neurosci 2011; 34:569–99. memory. Brain 2006; 129(Pt 2):306–20.
35 de Haan EH, Nys GM, Van Zandvoort MJ. Cognitive function fol- 56 Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syn-
lowing stroke and vascular cognitive impairment. Curr Opin Neurol drome. Brain 1998; 121(Pt 4):561–79.
2006; 19:559–64. 57 Lagarde J, Hantkie O, Hajjioui A, Yelnik A. Neuropsychological dis-
36 Tatemichi TK, Desmond DW, Prohovnik I. Strategic infarcts in vas- orders induced by cerebellar damage. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2009;
cular dementia. A clinical and brain imaging experience. Arzneimit- 52:360–70.
telforschung 1995; 45:371–85. 58 Hochstenbach J, van Spaendonck KP, Cools AR, Horstink MW,
37 Pantoni L. Cerebral small vessel disease: from pathogenesis and Mulder T. Cognitive deficits following stroke in the basal ganglia.
clinical characteristics to therapeutic challenges. Lancet Neurol 2010; Clin Rehabil 1998; 12:514–20.
9:689–701. 59 Van der Werf YD, Scheltens P, Lindeboom J, Witter MP, Uylings
38 Dufouil C, Godin O, Chalmers J et al. Severe cerebral white matter HBM, Jolles J. Deficits of memory, executive functioning and atten-
hyperintensities predict severe cognitive decline in patients with tion following infarction in the thalamus: a study of 22 cases with
cerebrovascular disease history. Stroke 2009; 40:2219–21. localised lesions. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:1330–44.
39 Rasquin SM, Verhey FR, Lousberg R, Lodder J. Cognitive perfor- 60 Szirmai I, Vastagh I, Szombathelyi E, Kamondi A. Strategic infarcts
mance after first ever stroke related to progression of vascular brain of the thalamus in vascular dementia. J Neurol Sci 2002; 204:91–7.
damage: a 2 year follow up CT scan study. J Neurol Neurosurg 61 Williamson JD, Miller ME, Bryan RN et al. The Action to Control
Psychiatry 2005; 76:1075–9. Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes Study
40 Burton E, Ballard C, Stephens S et al. Hyperintensities and (ACCORD-MIND): rationale, design, and methods. Am J Cardiol
fronto-subcortical atrophy on MRI are substrates of mild cogni- 2007; 99:112i–22i.
tive deficits after stroke. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003; 16: 62 Hillis AE, Barker PB, Beauchamp NJ, Gordon B, Wityk RJ. MR per-
113–8. fusion imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with
41 Jokinen H, Kalska H, Mantyla R et al. White matter hyperintensities aphasia and neglect. Neurology 2000; 55:782–8.
as a predictor of neuropsychological deficits post-stroke. J Neurol 63 Tatemichi TK, Paik M, Bagiella E, Desmond DW, Pirro M, Hanzawa
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76:1229–33. LK. Dementia after stroke is a predictor of long-term survival. Stroke
42 Medina D, DeToledo-Morrell L, Urresta F et al. White matter changes 1994; 25:1915–9.
in mild cognitive impairment and AD: a diffusion tensor imaging 64 Pasquini M, Leys D, Rousseaux M, Pasquier F, Henon H. Influence of
study. Neurobiol Aging 2006; 27:663–72. cognitive impairment on the institutionalisation rate 3 years after a
43 Williamson J, Nyenhuis D, Stebbins GT et al. Regional differences in stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007; 78:56–9.
relationships between apparent white matter integrity, cognition and 65 Claesson L, Linden T, Skoog I, Blomstrand C. Cognitive impairment
mood in patients with ischemic stroke. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010; after stroke – impact on activities of daily living and costs of care for
32:673–81. elderly people. The Goteborg 70+ Stroke Study. Cerebrovasc Dis 2005;
44 Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Barker PB et al. Subcortical aphasia and neglect 19:102–9.
in acute stroke: the role of cortical hypoperfusion. Brain 2002; 125 66 Heruti RJ, Lusky A, Dankner R et al. Rehabilitation outcome of
(Pt 5):1094–104. elderly patients after a first stroke: effect of cognitive status at admis-
45 Pantoni L, Garcia JH. Pathogenesis of leukoaraiosis: a review. Stroke sion on the functional outcome. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2002; 83:
1997; 28:652–9. 742–9.
46 Marshall RS, Lazar RM. Pumps, aqueducts, and drought manage- 67 Skidmore ER, Whyte EM, Holm MB et al. Cognitive and affective
ment: vascular physiology in vascular cognitive impairment. Stroke predictors of rehabilitation participation after stroke. Arch Phys Med
2011; 42:221–6. Rehabil 2010; 91:203–7.
47 Marshall RS, Festa JR, Cheung YK et al. Cerebral hemodynamics and 68 Ryan JD, Polatajko HJ, McEwen S et al. Analysis of Cognitive
cognitive impairment: baseline data from the RECON trial. Neurol- Environmental Support (ACES): preliminary testing. Neuropsychol
ogy 2012; 78:250–5. Rehabil 2011; 21:401–27.
48 Stebbins GT, Nyenhuis DL, Wang C et al. Gray matter atrophy in 69 Wilson BA, Emslie HC, Quirk K, Evans JJ. Reducing everyday
patients with ischemic stroke with cognitive impairment. Stroke memory and planning problems by means of a paging system: a
2008; 39:785–93. randomised control crossover study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
49 Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, Laman DM et al. Transcranial Doppler 2001; 70:477–82.
blood flow assessment in patients with mild heart failure: correlates 70 Fish J, Manly T, Emslie H, Evans JJ, Wilson BA. Compensatory strat-
with neuroimaging and cognitive performance. Congest Heart Fail egies for acquired disorders of memory and planning: differential
2008; 14:61–5. effects of a paging system for patients with brain injury of traumatic
50 Festa JR, Jia X, Cheung K et al. Association of low ejection fraction versus cerebrovascular aetiology. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;
with impaired verbal memory in older patients with heart failure. 79:930–5.
Arch Neurol 2011; 68:1021–6. 71 Winkens I, Van Heugten CM, Wade DT, Habets EJ, Fasotti L. Efficacy
51 Almeida OP, Garrido GJ, Beer C, Lautenschlager NT, Arnolda L, of time pressure management in stroke patients with slowed infor-
Flicker L. Cognitive and brain changes associated with ischaemic mation processing: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med
heart disease and heart failure. Eur Heart J 2012; 33:1769–76. Rehabil 2009; 90:1672–9.