Humanistic Psychotherapies
Humanistic Psychotherapies
Humanistic Psychotherapies
Psychotherapies
Handbook of Research and Practice
Second Edition
Edited by
David J. Cain
Kevin Keenan
Shawn Rubin
Published by To order
American Psychological Association APA Order Department
750 First Street, NE P.O. Box 92984
Washington, DC 20002 Washington, DC 20090-2984
www.apa.org Tel: (800) 374-2721; Direct: (202) 336-5510
Fax: (202) 336-5502; TDD/TTY: (202) 336-6123
Online: www.apa.org/books/
E-mail: [email protected]
In the U.K., Europe, Africa, and the Middle East, copies may be ordered from
American Psychological Association
3 Henrietta Street
Covent Garden, London
WC2E 8LU England
The opinions and statements published are the responsibility of the authors, and such
opinions and statements do not necessarily represent the policies of the American
Psychological Association.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-000
To Bobbi, my soulmate, love of my life, best friend,
and center of my universe.
—David J. Cain
To all those who have taught us about being human and humanistic:
our mentors, therapists, colleagues, clients, and students.
—Kevin Keenan
To my loving parents Cheryl and Fred and to my brothers
and their beloved wives and children: Harley, Shannon,
Alexandra, and Norah; and Jonathan, Stephanie,
Logan, and Grant.
—Shawn Rubin
CONTENTS
Contributors.................................................................................................. xi
Preface......................................................................................................... xiii
Acknowledgments..................................................................................... xvii
Introduction ................................................................................................. 3
David J. Cain, Kevin Keenan, and Shawn Rubin
vii
Chapter 3. Qualitative Research and Humanistic
Psychotherapy................................................................. 81
Heidi M. Levitt
Chapter 4. The Role of Empathy in Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, and Practice..................................... 115
Jeanne C. Watson
Chapter 5. Emotion in Psychotherapy:
An Experiential–Humanistic Perspective..................... 147
Antonio Pascual-Leone, Sandra Paivio,
and Shawn Harrington
contents ix
CONTRIBUTORS
Philip Brownell, MDiv, PsyD, Gestalt Center for Psychotherapy and Train-
ing, New York, NY
David J. Cain, PhD, ABPP, CGP, Fellow in Clinical Psychology of the
American Board of Professional Psychology, Carlsbad, CA
Mick Cooper, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Roehampton,
London, England
Edgar A. Correia, PgD, MA, Doctoral Candidate, Department of Counsel-
ling, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, Scotland
Meghan Craig, PgD, PsychD, CPsychol, Independent Practitioner and
Consultant, YSC Business Psychology, London, England
Louise G. Fidalgo, BS, Clinical Psychology Training Program, Brigham
Young University, Provo, UT
Rhonda N. Goldman, PhD, Illinois School of Professional Psychology at
Argosy University, Schaumburg
David M. Gonzalez, PhD, Counseling Psychology Program, University of
Northern Colorado, Greeley
Madeline R. Greaves, BS, Applied Psychology Program, Eastern Washing-
ton University, Cheney
xi
Shawn Harrington, MA, Clinical Psychology Training Program, University
of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada
Akira Ikemi, PhD, Graduate School of Professional Clinical Psychology,
Kansai University, Osaka, Japan
Kimberly M. Jayne, PhD, LMHC, NCC, RPT, Department of Counselor
Education, Portland State University, Portland, OR
Stephen Joseph, PhD, School of Education, University of Nottingham,
Jubilee Campus, Nottingham, England
Kevin Keenan, PhD, Michigan School of Professional Psychology, Farming-
ton Hills
Kevin C. Krycka, PsyD, Psychology Department, Seattle University,
Seattle, WA
Michael J. Lambert, PhD, Department of Psychology, Brigham Young
University, Provo, UT
Heidi M. Levitt, PhD, Clinical Psychology Program, Department of Psychol-
ogy, University of Massachusetts Boston
Catalina Woldarsky Meneses, PhD, Private Practice, Geneva, Switzerland
David Murphy, PhD, School of Education, University of Nottingham,
Jubilee Campus, Nottingham, England
Sandra Paivio, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Windsor,
Windsor, Ontario, Canada
Antonio Pascual-Leone, PhD, Psychological Services and Research Center,
Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario,
Canada
Dee C. Ray, PhD, LPC-S, NCC, RPT-S, Department of Counseling and
Higher Education, University of North Texas, Denton
Shawn Rubin, PsyD, Chair, School of Clinical Psychology, Saybrook
University, San Francisco, CA
Robert F. Scuka, PhD, MSW, National Institute of Relationship Enhance-
ment, Bethesda, MD
Joël Vos, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Roehampton,
London, England
Jeanne C. Watson, PhD, Department of Applied Psychology and Human
Development, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of
Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Every book has a story. This one’s started in 1997 when I began to pon-
der the possibility of assembling the research literature on the major variables
and theories of humanistic psychotherapies. At the time, humanistic psycho-
therapies were thought by many clinicians and academics to have little or no
empirical support for their effectiveness, except for research supporting client-
or person-centered therapy. This assumption was erroneous but common in
mainstream psychology and psychotherapy. By the early 1990s, humanistic
therapists and scientists had generated substantial research evidence support-
ing its effectiveness. However, much of this research remained unknown to
university faculty, clinical practitioners, or the general public. Even by the
late 1990s, although there were chapter reviews on research in humanistic
psychotherapies, there was no text available to provide comprehensive evi-
dence that humanistic psychotherapies were indeed effective for a wide range
of clients and problems. Moreover, the extant reviews of humanistic psycho-
therapies did not illustrate how research might be applied to practice, some-
thing that the first and second editions of Humanistic Psychotherapies provide.
To remedy this deficit, I invited Jules Seeman to coedit Humanistic
Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice. Jules Seeman was one of
our best and brightest and had for many decades served as a role model of an
xiii
exemplary person-centered–humanistic psychologist, teacher, researcher,
and therapist. In 2002, the first edition of Humanistic Psychotherapies
was published. Fortunately, Humanistic Psychotherapies received positive
reviews and was well received by practicing therapists, graduate students,
and psychotherapy researchers. Consequently, a second edition was pro-
posed to the American Psychological Association, and the final product
is in your hands.
Thirteen years have passed since the publication of the first edition
of Humanistic Psychotherapies, which remains the primary text in the field
devoted to demonstrating how research may be transformed into effective
practice. When the first edition was published, there was a need to demon-
strate that humanistic approaches were effective for a wide variety of client
populations. This was important, as mentioned previously, because main-
stream psychotherapy remained uninformed or skeptical that humanistic
psychotherapies were supported by sound research. This goal was achieved,
because the research evidence showed that all major humanistic therapies
were indeed effective. In fact, as the chapters in this volume attest, the
evidence now indicates that humanistic psychotherapies are equivalent or
superior to all major therapeutic systems, including cognitive–behavioral
therapy, which remains the most frequently practiced form of therapy in
the United States.
Despite the progress in conducting psychotherapy research, the
authors of Humanistic Psychotherapies strongly advocate that researcher–
practitioners carry out further studies. This is especially important because
students trained as researcher–practitioners are most likely to conduct
needed research that will enable us to continually improve and refine prac-
tice. The reality is that, despite the enormous impact of humanistic psycho-
therapy on the larger field, humanistic psychotherapists remain a relatively
small group, partly because students in graduate programs in clinical coun-
seling psychology and clinical social work are often not adequately exposed
to humanistic models. Clearly, more graduate faculty still need to join the
ranks of these programs to provide students with the opportunity to learn
humanistic approaches well enough to implement them with competence.
A parallel need is for students to have more humanistically oriented super-
visors available to them. Postgraduate training programs are also needed so
that professionals who want to practice humanistic psychotherapies have
resources for in-depth learning.
We are happy to report that the authors of this volume are, indeed,
the kind of researcher–practitioners needed to carry out the studies that will
help advance the theory and practice of humanistic psychotherapy. Although
many of them are long-time veterans in the field, a good portion of them are
what I refer to as the fourth generation—that is, those who are in their 30s
preface xv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
xvii
Art Bohart pointed to the central role of the client in making therapy
work effectively and helped us understand how important it is to encour-
age clients’ active participation in therapy. He also served in the role of
my “thinking partner” when I was pondering various therapeutic issues. Les
Greenberg and Robert Elliott and their associates have enabled me to value
the central role that emotion plays in the lives of our clients. Irv Yalom and
other existentialists raised my consciousness about the challenges of the exis-
tential givens of life with which we must all grapple.
In addition to those named above, enormous appreciation and rec-
ognition is due to the three generations of humanistic psychologists, psy-
chotherapists, and researchers who laid the ground on which contemporary
humanistic psychotherapies now stand.
My coeditors, Kevin Keenan and Shawn Rubin, made substantive and
distinctive contributions to the creation of this text.
I want to thank the American Psychological Association for publishing
a second edition of Humanistic Psychotherapies. Susan Reynolds was of great
assistance throughout. Her wise counsel proved to be valuable in dealing with
some of the inevitable challenges in such a project.
Special recognition is due to my parents, Esther and Russell Cain, for
teaching and modeling for me how to do a good job at whatever I undertook
in life.
There are a lot of good reasons to read this book. If you are a gradu-
ate student learning therapy, a professor teaching therapy, a psychotherapy
researcher, or a therapist who wants to incorporate humanistic concepts, rela-
tional emphases, and response styles into your current approach, this book has
much to offer. Your teachers, the authors of the chapters, are among the most
accomplished humanistic therapists and researchers in the field. The primary
goal of the book, to translate psychotherapy research into practice, is accom-
plished with ample therapist–client dialogue that demonstrates how the core
humanistic variables (e.g., empathy) and major schools (e.g., person-centered,
Gestalt) can be optimally implemented in a seamless manner. No matter
what your experience level as a therapist, this book has a range and depth
of learning that will enhance your skills and benefit your clients whether
Proper steps were taken in all case studies reported in the second edition of Humanistic Psychotherapies to
protect the confidentiality of clients.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-001
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
3
in individual, couples, child, or family therapy. Humanistic Psychotherapies:
Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition is unique in its review of the
humanistic psychotherapy research literature from its inception. It illustrates
clearly how to become a more evidence-based therapist while retaining one’s
core humanistic principles and values.
The reader may fairly ask, “What defines therapy as humanistic?” The
term humanistic, as used here, is understood to incorporate approaches vari-
ously defined as experiential, existential, relational, and phenomenological.
Therapies that come under the humanistic umbrella share a number of core
variables and emphases. They include
77 an optimistic view of clients as resourceful and naturally inclined
to grow and develop their potential.
77 a belief that the quality of the relationship between therapist
and client provides a safe sanctuary that supports the thera-
peutic process and that is growth inducing in and of itself. The
primary endeavor of humanistic therapists, therefore, is the cre-
ation of an optimal therapeutic relationship that is individual-
ized to adapt to clients’ needs as they evolve throughout the
course of therapy.
77 the core role of therapist empathy in grasping the personal mean-
ing of the client’s experience, facilitating self-exploration and
setting in motion a self-reflective process that promotes intraper-
sonal and interpersonal learning that leads to fresh perspectives
and more effective ways of living.
77 a phenomenological emphasis that focuses on clients’ subjective
world with the understanding that their current perception is
reality and can best be understood by putting aside hypotheses,
diagnoses, and preconceptions in an attempt to take in their
experience as freshly as possible.
77 a strong emphasis on the critical role emotion plays in both psy-
chopathology and psychological health with an understanding
of the adaptive role emotion plays in effective decision making,
emotion regulation, and effective functioning.
77 a focus on the self and self-concept of the client with the under-
standing that the way clients experience the self has a great
influence on their behavior. Enhancing self-exploration, self-
definition, reconstrual of the self, and the development of
introduction 5
in emotion schemes. Furthermore, we are now beginning to understand that
therapeutic learning is likely to be more integrated and lasting when it is
experienced emotionally.
Other areas in which humanistic psychotherapies have made an impact
include an emphasis on therapist presence, relational depth, a focus on the
self and self-concept of the client, and more reliance on the client’s resourceful-
ness for change. The positive psychology movement is clearly grounded in
humanistic premises and values. Almost every major school of psychotherapy
now understands and embraces the critical importance of Rogers’ core thera-
pist conditions of empathy, acceptance, and genuineness in facilitating client
growth. Emphases on phenomenology and working in the subjective world of
the client have also been embraced in mainstream psychotherapy.
Humanistic psychotherapists have also increasingly integrated concepts
and methods from many therapeutic approaches. While remaining true to
their core values and beliefs, humanistic therapies have incorporated aspects
of other therapies in a manner that retains the integrity of the model used.
This means that clients are viewed as capable partners with whom the thera-
pist collaborates to make therapeutic adjustments and to create an optimal
relationship and style of therapy that best fits their needs. This approach is
guided by research of the past few decades that has clearly shown that client
involvement is the best predictor of a good outcome.
Although it is likely that individual schools of psychotherapy will be
maintained and taught in their pure forms, it is clear that most therapists
will increasingly use concepts and methods from other sources to supple-
ment their work and to meet the needs of clients who are not responding
optimally to the core model. Emotion-focused therapy, while essentially
grounded in person-centered therapy, has integrated aspects of experien-
tial, Gestalt, and existential therapies. Therefore, it may be considered one
of the first humanistic–integrative therapies. Similarly, integrated models
of person-centered and existential–humanistic and Gestalt therapies have
become more prominent in the past decade.
Developments in the field of humanistic psychotherapy have demon-
strated that it has a renewed impact on the practice of psychotherapy that
is far reaching. Kirk Schneider and Alfried Längle (2012) edited a special
section of the journal Psychotherapy that made the case that
the renewal of humanism is a viable and growing phenomenon among
the leading areas of psychotherapy and that . . . humanism is (1) a foun-
dational element of therapeutic effectiveness; (2) a pivotal (and needed)
dimension of therapeutic training and (3) a critical contributor to social
well-being. (p. 427)
Invited authors of this special section of Psychotherapy indicated that (a) human-
istic psychotherapy has become widespread in Europe, (b) multiculturalism
introduction 7
Regrettably, some areas of humanistic therapies could not be included in
the second edition, primarily because of either space limitations or an inade
quate research base. These include culture and diversity, the self and self-
concept in therapy, group and transpersonal therapy, feminist therapies, and
therapies for extreme forms of psychopathology. I hope that these areas will be
given the coverage they deserve in future reviews of humanistic psychotherapy.
In a recent article (Angus, Watson, Elliott, Schneider, & Timulak, 2014)
reviewing the humanistic psychotherapy literature over the past 25 years
(1990–2015), the results are consistent with those reported in this book.
The article makes a strong case that “qualitative and quantitative research
findings, including meta-analyses, support the identification of [human-
istic psychotherapy] approaches as evidence-based treatments for a variety
of psychological conditions” (Angus et al., 2014, p. 1). They also indicate
that many humanistic therapies meet the standard for evidence-based treat-
ments for many problems. Therefore, the findings presented in Humanistic
Psychotherapies are increasingly supported by other current reviews of the
research literature. Fortunately, the rate of new studies is increasing and
continues to provide useful evidence that guides and refines practice. Our
future looks bright, and humanistic psychotherapists have good reason to feel
confident that they are providing their clients with a high level of care that
consistently leads to good outcomes.
REFERENCES
Angus, L., Watson, J. C., Elliott, R., Schneider, K., & Timulak, L. (2014). Human-
istic psychotherapy research 1990–2015: From methodological innovation to
evidence-supported treatment outcomes and beyond. Psychotherapy Research.
Advance online publication. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pubmed/25517088
Schneider, K. J., & Längle, A. (Eds.). (2012). Humanism in psychotherapy [special
section]. Psychotherapy, 49, 427–481.
Thanks to Meghan Craig and Jo Pybis for comments on earlier versions of this chapter.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-002
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
11
psychology, in stark contrast to psychotherapeutic orientations such as cogni-
tive–behavioral therapy, in which training texts, competencies, and journal
articles all draw extensively from—and are closely allied with—current
developments in psychology.
An example of a contemporary field of research with direct applicability
to humanistic psychology is self-determination theory (SDT), proposed by
Ryan and Deci (2000, 2002). SDT posits that human beings have three basic
psychological needs that are “essential for facilitating optimal functioning
of the natural propensities for growth and integration” (Ryan & Deci, 2000,
p. 68). The first of these is a need for relatedness—defined as feeling con-
nected to others and caring for and being cared for by those others; it sits
alongside the second need, that for autonomy (having a sense of agency and
ownership of one’s behavior), and the third, the need for competence (a sense
of mastery and accomplishment). SDT is becoming increasingly influential
in the fields of motivation and emotion, personality and social psychology,
and positive psychology; its conceptualization of need satisfaction is essen-
tially a positively phrased way of expressing what person-centered therapists
strive to achieve through the negatively phrased concepts of nondirectivity
and unconditional positive regard (Rogers, 1957). SDT provides powerful
indirect support for traditional humanistic practice (see Patterson & Joseph,
2007, for a full discussion of the compatibility between SDT and the person-
centered approach), yet it remains relatively unknown and underused in the
field of humanistic psychotherapy and counseling.
To some extent, this distancing from psychology may have emerged
because of the values of humanistic psychotherapy (see Cain, 2002; Cooper,
2007), which question the natural scientific assumptions underlying much
of psychological theory and research. Whereas psychology, for instance, aims
to establish generalized laws of human behavior, humanistic psychotherapies
emphasize the uniqueness of each person’s lived experience. Similarly, whereas
psychology tends to construe human activity in deterministic terms, humanis-
tic psychotherapies emphasize the human capacity for freedom and choice. Yet
there is also much research from a nonpositivistic, human sciences standpoint
(e.g., Giorgi, 1985; Moustakas, 2001; Wilkins, 2010), which has the potential
to inform humanistic psychotherapeutic practices. Furthermore, as Cain (2002)
pointed out, a preference for a human sciences standpoint need not necessitate
a rejection of a natural sciences one because the humanistic psychotherapies
embrace “multiple ways of knowing” (p. 9).
More important, perhaps, the humanistic psychotherapy community
may face significant risks in becoming detached from contemporary psycho-
logical theory and research. Indeed, if a psychotherapeutic approach is likened
to the body of a tree, psychological theory and evidence can be likened to
its roots in two ways: First, psychological theory can provide nourishment
psychological foundations 13
emphasized the importance of a collaborative and nonjudgmental thera
peutic relationship (e.g., Beck, John, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). However, what
is unique among the humanistic therapies is the belief that a particular kind
of therapeutic relating has, in itself, the power to bring about positive thera-
peutic change (Rogers, 1957).
For many theorists and practitioners across the humanistic field, it is
the existence of a deep, authentic connection between therapist and client
that has the greatest potential for psychological benefit. Much of this is
derived from Buber’s (1958) concept of I–Thou and his work on dialogue
(Buber, 1947), which argued that human beings achieve their authentic
personhood only through genuine relationships with another. Rogers’s (1959,
1961) later work, with its emphasis on encounter and congruence, was partic-
ularly influenced by this argument (Anderson & Cissna, 1997); more recent
developments in the person-centered field have also been oriented around
the positive psychological potential of dialogue (Schmid, 2001a, 2001b)
or relational depth (Knox, Murphy, Wiggins, & Cooper, 2013; Mearns &
Cooper, 2005). Along similar lines, contemporary gestalt therapy has also
seen an emphasis on the deep soul nourishment that a genuine encounter
between therapist and client can provide (Hycner, 1991; Lee & Wheeler,
2013), and existential therapists such as Binswanger (1963), Laing (1965),
and Friedman (1985) have emphasized the healing power of being with
others and love.
Mearns and Cooper (2005) defined relational depth as a state in which
two people are copresent (Bugental, 1976)—that is, concomitantly open to
the impact of the other (receptivity) while also willing to share themselves in
an authentic and open way (expressivity; Cooper, 2005). Such a definition is
close to what social psychologists have defined as intimacy or intimate inter-
actions, consisting of self-revealing behaviors (cf. expressivity), a positive
regard for others, and a knowledge of their inner experiencing (cf. receptivity;
Prager & Roberts, 2004; Reis, 2001). As with conceptions of a deepened thera-
peutic encounter (Cooper, 2009), social psychologists have also argued that a
core component of intimacy is responsiveness—the extent to which partners
communicate understanding, validation, and caring in response to the other’s
self-disclosures (Laurenceau, Rivera, Schaffer, & Pietromonaco, 2004; Reis,
Clark, & Holmes, 2004).
psychological foundations 15
Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project, 2008). This public health
directive stated,
Connect . . . with the people around you. With family, friends, colleagues
and neighbours. At home, work, school or in your local community.
Think of these as the cornerstones of your life and invest time in devel-
oping them. Building these connections will support and enrich you every
day. (Aked et al., 2008, p. 5)
psychological foundations 17
At present, however, this evidence of the importance of interpersonal
relationships has provided only tentative support to humanistic relational
practices, for two key reasons. First, evidence for the relationship between
psychological well-being and close interpersonal connections is primar-
ily correlational, such that it is not certain that the experience of closeness
with others actually causes improvements in psychological well-being (Reis,
2001). It may be, for instance, that people who are happier tend to form closer
relationships or that a third variable—such as level of extraversion, optimism,
or secure attachment—causes people to experience both greater well-being
and closer relationships with others (Diener & Seligman, 2002; Reis, 2001;
Uchino, 2009). In fact, evidence is beginning to emerge that suggests a specific
causal relationship from interpersonal connection to psychological well-being
(Kaniasty & Norris, 2008; Powdthavee, 2008; Reis, 2001; Stadler, Snyder,
Horn, Shrout, & Bolger, 2012; Uchino, 2009; Whisman & Baucom, 2012)—
particularly the experience of feeling understood (Reis, 2001)—but it is still
in its early stages. Second, even if this causal relationship can be established,
one must still be cautious about extending this research to the client–therapist
encounter. It may be, for instance, that the amount of time that clients spend
with psychotherapists is too limited to be of positive benefit or that clients
would discount experiences of intimacy and connection with their therapist
on the grounds that therapists are just doing their job.
Nevertheless, given the strength of the evidence linking interpersonal
relating to well-being, the humanistic hypothesis regarding the healing power
of the therapeutic relationship would seem to be highly plausible. It is also con-
sistent with the large body of psychotherapy research that has demonstrated
the importance of the psychotherapeutic relationship, as well as direct clinical
feedback, for clinical outcomes (e.g., Norcross, 2011).
An example of the latter comes from Sabine, a bright and friendly young
Asian woman who self-referred to a university psychotherapy clinic to help
overcome feelings of low self-esteem and worry, to resolve difficulties with
her mother, and to find the courage to pursue what she wanted to do. At
assessment, Sabine scored in the severe ranges for both generalized anxiety
(Generalized Anxiety Disorder seven-item scale) and depression (the nine-
item Patient Health Questionnaire) and was offered 24 sessions of humanistic
therapy in a pluralistic framework (Cooper & McLeod, 2011) by Mick Cooper.
This pluralistic approach meant that, although Sabine was primarily offered
an empathic and affirming relationship through which to find ways of moving
forward in life, several other methods were also introduced—by collabora-
tive agreement—into the psychotherapeutic work. For instance, in Session 2,
Socratic dialogue (Beck et al., 1979) was used to test out Sabine’s assumptions
about her mother; in Session 5, Sabine agreed to give herself some “worry
time” to concentrate specifically on her anxieties; and, in Session 19, focusing
psychological foundations 19
is to really own your feelings and say something like, “Mum, when
you do that, it makes me feel really awkward and uncomfortable,”
and not go into whether it’s right or wrong. Not get into all
of that. Because, in a sense, she can’t argue with that. It is
your experience, and maybe you’ll get into less of an argument
about it and she might be less defensive and hear what it’s like
for you.
Sabine thought about this and decided that she would be better off directly
telling her mother how she felt.
This exchange with Sabine had more of a psychoeducational quality
than might be typical of some humanistic practices, but it was based on a
fundamentally humanistic understanding of what it means to communicate
in constructive ways: assertively, openly, and drawing from phenomenological
experiencing (Ornish, 1998). In this respect, although it is an example of
how the psychological evidence may stimulate humanistic psychotherapists to
consider different ways of working with clients, it is not about compromising
humanistic practices or values. Rather, what the psychological evidence may
do is to help humanistic psychologists identify and actualize different elements
of humanistic thinking, philosophy, or values that may, to date, have lain
dormant in the field.
Received Support
Visibility
Research has suggested that one of the principal reasons why this may be
the case is because the offer of social support has the potential to undermine
recipients’ feelings of self-worth, independence, and self-efficacy (Rafaeli &
Gleason, 2009; Shrout, Herman, & Bolger, 2006; Uchino, 2009). It may also
create a sense of indebtedness to the supporter, drawing the recipient’s atten-
tion to his or her problems (Rafaeli & Gleason, 2009).
In this respect, a series of studies have suggested that, although visible
support (i.e., support that the recipient is aware of receiving) is associated
with increases in psychological distress, invisible support (i.e., support that a
provider reports giving but that the recipient is not aware of receiving) is asso-
ciated with reductions in psychological distress (Bolger et al., 2000; Howland
& Simpson, 2010; Maisel & Gable, 2009; Shrout et al., 2006). Here, visible
support consists of overt, recognizably supportive transactions. By contrast,
invisible support is covert, equal, and conversation-like; it deemphasizes the
supporter and supportee roles. It also deflects away from the supportee’s prob-
lems to discuss difficulties in a third-person or self-focused context (Howland
& Simpson, 2010). So visible support might be when a friend says to you,
“Let’s talk about your problems,” whereas invisible support might be when
a friend casually chats to you about problem areas in your—and perhaps his
or her—life, without making it explicit that this is intended for your benefit.
What the research has suggested is that, in some respects, the latter may be
more helpful than the former because it is less likely to leave you feeling needy
or to dent your sense of self-worth.
Mutuality
Consistent with the research on invisible support and the need to main-
tain positive self-worth, Gleason, Iida, Shrout, and Bolger (2008) found that,
on average, the receipt of support does not have a negative effect if an indi-
vidual is providing support to the other at the same time—that is, if the provi-
sion of support is mutual rather than one way. More specifically, Gleason et al.
psychological foundations 21
found that individuals tend to feel most negative when they are receiving
support and not providing it and least negative when they are both receiv-
ing and providing support. This finding would seem to be consistent with
Baumeister and Leary (1995), who reported that “love is highly satisfying
and desirable only if it is mutual” (p. 514). That is, people who gave love
without receiving it, and received it without giving it, tended to describe the
experience as aversive.
Responsivity
Another reason why attempts to be supportive may be associated with
higher distress is because, in some instances, such attempts may be unrespon-
sive to the actual needs and wants of the supportee (Maisel & Gable, 2009;
Rafaeli & Gleason, 2009; Uchino, 2009). For instance, emotional support
seems to be more helpful when events are uncontrollable, whereas instru-
mental, practical support seems to be more helpful when circumstances can
be changed (Rafaeli & Gleason, 2009; Shrout et al., 2006; Uchino, 2009).
Hence, if a supporter is offering practical advice to a supportee when nothing
can be done—that is, support that is unresponsive to the situation—it may
increase such negative feelings as helplessness and not being understood.
Skillfulness
Finally, it may also be that the kinds of support that people are providing—
albeit well intentioned—are simply not very skillful (Bolger et al., 2000).
For instance, it seems important that support be given in a noncritical way
(Rafaeli & Gleason, 2009). Similarly, research has suggested that interper-
sonal support may be most helpful when the supportee feels understood, valued,
and cared for (Maisel & Gable, 2009).
psychological foundations 23
client is specifically identified as the one coming to the psychotherapist for
help, there is rarely mutuality of support, and the client’s difficulties are
typically given priority as the principal focus of the psychotherapeutic work.
Clearly, these aspects of the psychotherapeutic enterprise are there for good
reason. However, if the psychological evidence can be transposed to the
psychotherapeutic arena, it would suggest that some clients may experience
a drop in self-worth, independence, and self-efficacy just by virtue of being
psychotherapy clients.
Given that the humanistic psychotherapies are consistently associated
with client benefit (Elliott, Greenberg, Watson, Timulak, & Freire, 2013),
it seems likely that any detrimental effects would be offset by more positive
aspects of the humanistic psychotherapy process. However, this psychological
research does point to the potential advantages of humanistic psychotherapies
in which interpersonal support is provided in a more mutual, nonvisible
ways, for instance, group psychotherapy (e.g., Spiegel, Bloom, & Yalom,
1981), cocounseling (Kauffman & New, 2004), or peer support (Cowie,
2000). Even in more traditional psychotherapeutic formats, however, there
may still be ways in which humanistic psychotherapists can decrease the
visibility and one sidedness of the support process without compromising
the integrity or focus of the therapeutic work. For instance, in certain cir-
cumstances, it may be appropriate for therapists to share their vulnerabilities
(L. H. Farber, 2000; Spinelli, 1994), to disclose how they have experienced,
or do experience, some of the same psychological difficulties as their client
(Cooper, 2015), or to actively challenge the imbalance of status in the thera-
peutic relationship.
After completing his degree, for instance, Dane spent a number of
months becoming increasingly anxious about where he was going in his life.
With his psychotherapist, Mick, he explored his feelings about this as well
as the kinds of work that might give him a sense of meaning and purpose.
In a few sessions, however, Mick also disclosed how much he had struggled
at a similar point in his life, which was something that Dane described (on
postsession feedback forms) as useful, in that it helped depathologize his
feelings of anxiety. In many of the psychotherapeutic sessions, Mick and
Dane also explored the power dynamic between them and how Dane tended
to project authority onto Mick as well as others in his life. Consequently,
he would act deferentially, but then feel belittled and angry, and end up
acting in aggressive and critical ways. Here, again, Mick would sometimes
disclose his vulnerability to Dane—for instance, that when they first worked
together he had felt intimidated and anxious that he was not doing his job
well enough—and he also challenged Dane to take more authority in the
psychotherapy relationship: to acknowledge his strengths, abilities, and
capacities.
psychological foundations 25
Dane: Yeah.
Mick: Or, kind of, yeah . . . that it’s difficult for you to put that out
there. And I think that crosses lots of situations.
Dane: Yeah.
In this example, then, Dane was encouraged to be more than just a recip-
ient of psychotherapeutic support, but also an expert who could know more
about some things than his psychotherapist. The traditional psychotherapist–
client power relationship was temporarily subverted; from the psychological
evidence, it would seem that this might be an important process in supporting
a client’s sense of competence and self-worth. As can be seen in this example,
it also provided a valuable opportunity for the client to explore in more detail
the dynamics of his relationships in everyday life.
Much of the work pertaining to this question comes from research into
self-disclosure, which includes—but is not limited to—the disclosure of feel-
ings and affect (for research on disclosure in the psychotherapeutic relation-
ship, see B. A. Farber, 2006). Pennebaker’s (1997) program of research into
written emotional expression is of particular importance here. In his basic
research design, participants were randomly distributed to one of two writing
conditions: one in which they were asked to spend 15 to 30 minutes per day,
for 3 to 5 days, writing about their deepest thoughts and feelings and about an
extremely important emotional issue that affected them, and a control condi-
tion in which they were instructed to spend the same amount of time writing
about something more superficial (Pennebaker, 1997). Pennebaker’s studies
found that writing about emotional topics brought about significant reduc-
tions in psychological distress, as well as improvements in physiological func-
tioning, such as antibody levels against hepatitis B (Petrie, Booth, Pennebaker,
Davison, & Thomas, 1995), with a mean effect size equivalent to a Cohen’s d
of about 0.15 (Frattaroli, 2006). Although this effect size would be classed as
small, one should bear in mind that many of these interventions were very brief,
and studies with longer periods of disclosure, or more instances of disclosure,
have demonstrated significantly larger effect sizes. It is interesting, how-
ever, that studies that have compared writing versus talking to a therapist or
a tape recorder have found comparable biological, mood, or cognitive effects.
Receiving feedback from others does not seem to enhance the effectiveness of
the procedure (Pennebaker, 1997).
Self-Concealment
psychological foundations 27
likely to experience cancer and infectious diseases than those who conceal
their sexuality (Cole, Kemeny, Taylor, & Visscher, 1996).
As with the research on self-disclosure, such evidence could be read as
supporting humanistic psychotherapeutic practice and its emphasis on facili-
tating emotional expression. However, the correlational nature of these find-
ings means that causation cannot be established. In particular, rather than
self-concealment causing psychological distress, it may be that people with
a generally open, nonconcealing personality type also tend to experience
greater well-being. Here, Kelly and Yip (2006) distinguished between being
a secretive person and keeping a secret, providing evidence that, although
the predisposition to keep secrets is associated with higher symptom
atology, the actual act of keeping a secret may be associated with a lowering
of distress.
In fact, more recent research has suggested that self-concealment, both
as a personality trait and as a daily activity, is associated with reduced well-
being (Uysal et al., 2010). However, Kelly and Yip’s (2006) challenge to the
assumption that self-concealment is inherently harmful—alongside other
evidence that emotional expression is not always beneficial (e.g., Stroebe,
Stroebe, Schut, Zech, & van den Bout, 2002)—indicates that the disclosure
of emotional experiences is not a ubiquitously positive activity. In attempting
to understand, therefore, when and where it might be beneficial, it is impor-
tant to examine the specific mechanisms by which emotional expression
might bring about positive change.
Processing Emotion
Expression of emotions may also be important, because, through articu-
lating and talking about their deeper feelings, people may come to learn more
about themselves—for instance, the way in which they perceive the world,
their organismic wants, and the situations that trigger distressing emotions.
Through this experience, they may then find ways of being in the world that
are more satisfying and fulfilling. This view is consistent with the psychological
research that has shown that emotional catharsis, in itself, is inadequate for
producing positive change—it needs to be combined with some cognitive
processing of the emotion (Bohart, 1980; Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999).
For example, venting anger is most likely to be helpful when it “results in
changes in the perception of the expresser or the behavior of the target”
(Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999, p. 41)—that is, when it helps the person
to resolve the source of the anger. Indeed, when asked why writing about a
trauma was beneficial, 75% referred to insight, with just 10% focusing on the
purging of emotions (Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999).
As well as helping people transform their way of being in the world,
emotional processing may also be beneficial because it can reduce people’s
confusion about unclear emotions and their source, hence reducing levels of
distress. Kennedy-Moore and Watson (1999) wrote, “Poor understanding of
one’s feelings increases the chance of being caught in an aversive ruminative
process, trying to figure out one’s internal state” (p. 78).
psychological foundations 29
the trauma. Similarly, “reduced self-disclosure is thought to be particularly
detrimental to [obsessive–compulsive disorder] as it prevents individuals
from gathering information to challenge their catastrophic interpretations”
(Alden & Regambal, 2010, p. 457).
Second, disclosure of emotions may help individuals challenge their
perception that there is something bad, shameful, or wrong in the emotion
that is being concealed (Uysal et al., 2010), particularly if they are talking
it through with positively regarding others. Third, if individuals conceal
an emotion because they are afraid of being overwhelmed by its expres-
sion, disclosing it in a safe and containing environment may help them
to recognize that its expression is tolerable (Kennedy-Moore & Watson,
1999), which may then reduce feelings of fear and apprehension toward
the emotion.
BEING AUTHENTIC
psychological foundations 31
Despite this consensus, the construct of authenticity has received little
empirical research attention until recently. A little more than a decade ago,
Harter (2002) commented that “there is no single, coherent body of litera-
ture on authentic self-behavior, no bedrock of knowledge” (p. 382). Since
then, the past decade has seen authenticity become the focus of much new
research by personality, developmental, and positive psychologists (Harter,
2012). Several psychometric tests developed in this period have led to
research interest. We describe two such measures that have attracted inter-
est next.
Social Functioning
A recent topic for research was the association between authenticity and
social functioning. Pinto, Maltby, Wood, and Day (2012) asked participants
psychological foundations 33
to engage in a computer task in the laboratory that involved participants
pressing a button in relation to a message that appeared on the screen. If
they pressed a certain button in the time allocated, the participants earned
points that they could exchange for money. Participants were told that they
were playing against another person in an adjoining laboratory who could
steal points from them. The task was designed to mirror real-life situations in
which people may sometimes take credit for others’ hard work.
However, the experiment was rigged, in that participants were not actu-
ally playing against another person. It was designed so that participants would
think that someone else was stealing points from them. The idea was that
participants would feel cheated and provoked to play the game aggressively.
To test whether they would behave aggressively, participants were told that
they could steal points from their opponent next door if they wanted. Pinto
et al. (2012) found that players high on the Authenticity Scale were actually
less likely to respond aggressively—they continued to do their best to earn
points for themselves rather than turning their attention to getting their
own back. Those high on the Authenticity Scale were, in short, less punitive
toward others.
It has also been hypothesized that authentic people’s relationships are
more intimate. Swann, De la Ronde, and Hixon (1994) surveyed 176 mar-
ried and dating couples. Partners were seated at the opposite ends of a long
table, so that they could not see each other’s answers, and asked to complete
a questionnaire describing first themselves and then their partner. Whereas
dating people were most intimate with partners who evaluated them favor-
ably, married people were most intimate with spouses whose evaluations
mirrored their own self-ratings. This was true regardless of whether spouses
rated themselves negatively or positively. This finding is consistent with the
hypothesis that, as relationships deepen, people want to be known for who
they are.
Neff and Suizzo (2006) asked people to rate the relationship between
them and their partner in terms of who has the most say. Participants were
asked to rate the level of equality in their relationship on a 5-point visual
scale ranging from dominant through equal to submissive. Those who per-
ceived themselves to be in a subordinate relationship were more likely to
admit that they acted phony with their partner and felt less able to be
themselves.
Lopez and Rice (2006) identified two key components of authentic rela-
tionships. The first was unacceptability of deception (e.g., “I would rather be
the person my partner wants me to be than who I really am,” a negative item)
and the second was intimate risk taking (e.g., “I share my deepest thoughts
with my partner even if there’s a chance he/she won’t understand them”).
psychological foundations 35
“I am so tired,” she said. “Whenever someone wants me to be this per-
son, then someone else wants me to be that person, and another one
wants me to be another person, and it just goes on and on until there is
nothing left of me except the mirrors and the glass and they are so very
heavy to bear.”
And the young woman sat down and wept. And as she wept her tears
ran down the mirrors and the glass.
And the years passed, and the young woman grew older, and still she
wept. And she had three beautiful children, and still she wept. And those
children had children, and still she wept and still the tears ran down
the mirrors and the glass.
But this was no ordinary woman, as we said before, and she knew
that each of her tears held a tiny drop of magical transformation. So her
weeping slowly worked away at the mirrors and the glass. And the more
she wept, the greater the transformation.
Then very extraordinary people came who looked right through the
glass and the mirrors, and a most curious thing came to pass—the mirrors
and the glass started to dissolve. They became thinner and thinner and
weighed less and less.
And then another most curious thing happened—the thinner the
mirrors became, the taller the woman grew; and the lighter the glass
became, the stronger the woman grew, until the mirrors and the glass
were like gossamer, and the woman was tall and strong.
Then the woman stood up, and dried her tears, and laughed a great
shout of laughter. And the last of the mirrors and the last of the glass
shattered and were blown into nothingness.
And the woman stretched out her hands to the world, stepped out
onto the path, and walked away laughing.
Fiona’s story not only illustrates this movement toward authenticity and
its psychological value, but also the way in which emotional expression (the
young woman’s tears) and a deep therapeutic connection (someone who looked
right through the glass) were key to facilitating this process.
However, the psychological evidence regarding the salutogenic poten-
tial of authenticity can also encourage humanistic psychotherapists to con-
sider other ways in which they might support clients in this direction: For
instance, might there be psychoeducational, perhaps web-based, methods
that could support individuals in developing their authenticity? In addition,
the development of valid and reliable measures of authenticity indicates that
humanistic psychotherapists can gain the confidence to use such measures in
their own practice, research, and outcome evaluations. In terms of practice,
individual practitioners may find the use of measures of authenticity consis-
tent with their own needs for outcome measurement and more suitable than
traditional measures of psychopathology.
psychological foundations 37
REFERENCES
Aked, J., Marks, N., Cordon, C., & Thompson, S. (2008). Five ways to wellbeing: The
evidence. London, England: nef.
Alden, L., & Regambal, M. J. (2010). Interpersonal processes in the anxiety dis
orders. In L. M. Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology:
Theory, research, assessment and therapeutic interventions (pp. 449–469). New York,
NY: Wiley.
Alexopoulos, G. S., Raue, P., & Areán, P. (2003). Problem-solving therapy versus
supportive therapy in geriatric major depression with executive dysfunction.
American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 46–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/
appi.ajgp.11.1.46
Anderson, R., & Cissna, K. N. (1997). The Martin Buber–Carl Rogers dialogue: A new
transcript with commentary. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Aron, A. P., Mashek, D. J., & Aron, E. N. (2004). Closeness as including other in
the self. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy
(pp. 27–42). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
497–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Beck, A. T., John, R. A., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depres-
sion. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Binswanger, L. (1963). Being-in-the-world: Selected papers of Ludwig Binswanger
(J. Needleman, Trans.). London, England: Condor Books.
Birtchnell, J. (1999). Relating in psychotherapy: The application of a new theory. Hove,
England: Brunner-Routledge.
Bohart, A. C. (1980). Toward a cognitive theory of catharsis. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research, & Practice, 17, 192–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085911
Bohart, A. C., & Tallman, K. (1999). How clients make therapy work: The process of
active self-healing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/10323-000
Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and adjustment
to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 953–961. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.953
Bowens, M., & Cooper, M. (2012). Development of a client feedback tool: A qualita-
tive study of therapists’ experiences of using the Therapy Personalisation Forms.
European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 14, 47–62. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/13642537.2012.652392
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London, England:
Routledge.
psychological foundations 39
Dalgleish, T., Joseph, S., Thrasher, S., Tranah, T., & Yule, W. (1996). Crisis support
following the Herald of Free-Enterprise disaster: A longitudinal perspective.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 833–845. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490090411
Das-Munshi, J., Goldberg, D., Bebbington, P. E., Bhugra, D. K., Brugha, T. S., Dewey,
M. E., . . . Prince, M. (2008). Public health significance of mixed anxiety and
depression: Beyond current classification. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 171–
177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.036707
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13,
81–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00415
Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us
happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with sub-
jective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 94–122. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.001
Duncan, B. L. (2010). On becoming a better therapist. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12080-000
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection
hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1126/science.1089134
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J. C., Timulak, L., & Freire, E. (2013). Research
on humanistic-experiential psychotherapies. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and
Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp. 495–538).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Emmelkemp, P. M. G. (2004). Behavior therapy with adults. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.),
Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed.,
pp. 393–446). Chicago, IL: Wiley.
Farber, B. A. (2006). Self-disclosure in psychotherapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Farber, L. H. (2000). The ways of the will: Selected essays (expanded ed.). New York,
NY: Basic Books.
Fehr, B. (2004). A prototype model of intimacy interactions in same-sex friend-
ships. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy
(pp. 9–26). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. (2008). Final project report. London,
England: Government Office for Science.
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 823–865. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.
132.6.823
Friedman, M. (1985). The healing dialogue in psychotherapy. New York, NY: Jason
Aronson.
Gendlin, E. T. (1996). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy: A manual of the experiential
method. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Giorgi, A. (Ed.). (1985). Phenomenological and psychological research. Pittsburgh, PA:
Duquesne University Press.
psychological foundations 41
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London, England: Penguin.
Kaniasty, K., & Norris, F. H. (2008). Longitudinal linkages between perceived social
support and posttraumatic stress symptoms: Sequential roles of social causa-
tion and social selection. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 274–281. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/jts.20334
Kauffman, K., & New, C. (2004). The theory and practice of re-evaluation co-counselling.
Hove, England: Brunner-Routledge.
Kelly, A. E., & Yip, J. J. (2006). Is keeping a secret or being a secretive person linked
to psychological symptoms? Journal of Personality, 74, 1349–1370. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00413.x
Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing emotion: Myths, realities, and
therapeutic strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicomponent conceptualization of
authenticity: Theory and research. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimen-
tal social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 283–357). San Diego, CA: Elsevier/Academic
Press.
Kifer, Y., Heller, D., Perunovic, W. Q. E., & Galinsky, A. D. (2013). The good life
of the powerful: The experience of power and authenticity enhances subjective
well-being. Psychological Science, 24, 280–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0956797612450891
Knox, R., Murphy, D., Wiggins, S., & Cooper, M. (Eds.). (2013). Relational depth:
New perspectives and developments. Basingstoke, England: Palgrave.
Laing, R. D. (1965). The divided self: An existential study in sanity and madness. Harmonds
worth, England: Penguin.
Lakey, C. E., Kernis, M. H., Heppner, W. L., & Lance, C. E. (2008). Individual
differences in authenticity and mindfulness as predictors of verbal defensive-
ness. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 230–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.jrp.2007.05.002
Larson, D. G., & Chastain, R. L. (1990). Self-concealment: Conceptualization, mea-
surement, and health implications. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9,
439–455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1990.9.4.439
Laurenceau, J.-P., Rivera, L. M., Schaffer, A. R., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (2004).
Intimacy as interpersonal process: Current status and future directions. In D. J.
Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 61–78).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lee, R. G., & Wheeler, G. (Eds.). (2013). The voice of shame: Silence and connection
in psychotherapy. New York, NY: Gestalt Press.
Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (Eds.). (2004). Positive psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470939338
Lopez, F. G., & Rice, K. G. (2006). Preliminary development and validation of a
measure of relationship authenticity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 362–
371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.362
psychological foundations 43
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process.
Psychological Science, 8, 162–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.
tb00403.x
Perls, F., Hefferline, R. F., & Goodman, P. (1951). Gestalt therapy: Excitement and
growth in the human personality. New York, NY: Julian Press.
Petrie, K. J., Booth, R. J., Pennebaker, J. W., Davison, K. P., & Thomas, M. G. (1995).
Disclosure of trauma and immune response to a hepatitis B vaccination program.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 787–792. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0022-006X.63.5.787
Pinto, D. G., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., & Day, L. (2012). A behavioral test of
Horney’s linkage between authenticity and aggression: People living authenti-
cally are less-likely to respond aggressively in unfair situations. Personality and
Individual Differences, 52, 41–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.023
Powdthavee, N. (2008). Putting a price tag on friends, relatives, and neighbours:
Using surveys of life satisfaction to value social relationships. Journal of Socio-
Economics, 37, 1459–1480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2007.04.004
Prager, K. J., & Roberts, L. J. (2004). Deep intimate connection: Self and intimacy in
couple relationships. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness
and intimacy (pp. 43–60). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rafaeli, E., & Gleason, M. E. (2009). Skilled support within intimate relationships.
Journal of Family Theory & Review, 1, 20–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-
2589.2009.00003.x
Reinecke, M. A., & Freeman, A. (2003). Cognitive therapy. In A. S. Gurman & S. B.
Messer (Eds.), Essential psychotherapies (pp. 224–271). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Reis, H. T. (2001). Relationship experiences and emotional well-being. In C. D.
Ryff & B. H. Singer (Eds.), Emotion, social relationships, and health (pp. 57–85).
Oxford, England: Oxford University. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/
9780195145410.003.0003
Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as
an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J. Mashek
& A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston, MA: Houghton & Mifflin.
Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic per-
sonality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95–103. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/h0045357
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships as
developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study
of science (Vol. 3, pp. 184–256). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of therapy. London,
England: Constable.
psychological foundations 45
Stroebe, M., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., Zech, E., & van den Bout, J. (2002). Does dis-
closure of emotions facilitate recovery from bereavement? Evidence from two
prospective studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 169–178.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.169
Swann, W. B., Jr., De la Ronde, C., & Hixon, J. G. (1994). Authenticity and positiv-
ity strivings in marriage and courtship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 66, 857–869. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.857
Uchino, B. N. (2009). Understanding the links between social support and physical
health: A life-span perspective with emphasis on the separability of perceived
and received support. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 236–255. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x
Uysal, A., Lin, H. L., & Knee, C. R. (2010). The role of need satisfaction in self-
concealment and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 187–
199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167209354518
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A.,
& Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological
Review, 117, 575–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018697
Watson, J. C. (2011). The process of growth and transformation: Extending the pro-
cess model. Person-Centered & Experiential Psychotherapies, 10, 11–27. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/14779757.2011.564760
Whisman, M. A., & Baucom, D. H. (2012). Intimate relationships and psycho
pathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15, 4–13. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1007/s10567-011-0107-2
Wildes, J. E., Simons, A. D., & Harkness, K. L. (2002). Life events, number of social
relationships, and twelve-month naturalistic course of major depression in a com-
munity sample of women. Depression and Anxiety, 16, 104–113. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/da.10048
Wilkins, P. (2010). Researching in a person-centered way. In M. Cooper, J. C. Watson,
& D. Hölldampf (Eds.), Person-centered and experiential therapies work: A review
of the research on counseling, psychotherapy and related practices (pp. 215–239).
Ross-on-Wye, England: PCCS Books.
Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The
authentic personality: A theoretical and empirical conceptualization and the
development of the authenticity scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55,
385–399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.385
You, S., Van Orden, K. A., & Conner, K. R. (2011). Social connections and suicidal
thoughts and behavior. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 180–184. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020936
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-003
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
49
includes psychotherapies known as client- or person centered, emotion focused
(also known as process–experiential), Gestalt, existential, focusing oriented, rela-
tional, experiential, and narrative, to name the most prominent.
In this chapter, we provide a very brief summary of research conducted
up to 2000 (covered in the first edition of this book). Research since then is
reviewed in more detail, particularly meta-analytic reviews of outcome studies
that describe the impact of treatment (a) over the course of therapy, (b) com-
pared with no treatment, (c) compared with placebo and related controls, and
(d) contrasted with therapies derived from other theoretical systems. In addi-
tion, when research from other orientations bears directly on the outcomes
and processes of humanistic psychotherapy, we note it as well. This review is
followed by an examination of studies (process research) that have explored
in-session attitudes and behaviors (moment-to-moment therapist activities)
and either their consequences for moment-to-moment client responses or the
summed effects of such activities on client well-being. The goal is to summa-
rize findings and translate them into practice and training. At the end of the
chapter, we place special emphasis on innovations for increasing the effective-
ness of humanistic psychotherapy.
Elliott et al. (2013) were able to locate 135 treatment comparisons based
on studies examining humanistic–experiential therapies compared with a vari-
ety of other treatments, usually in the context of helping clients manifesting
a primary diagnostic class or disorder. This analysis indicated mixed effects
in comparative treatment studies, with considerable variability. When consid-
ered as a whole, they found no overall difference between treatments in the
135 studies (d = -0.02). In this analysis, 60% of the studies showed that the
treatments (humanistic vs. nonhumanistic) were equivalent, and 21% of
the studies favored the nonhumanistic therapy and 19% favored the human-
istic treatment over the nonhumanistic one. Elliott et al. suggested that such
widely inconsistent findings can be explained, to a large degree, by researcher
allegiance effects, a construct operationalized by Luborsky et al. (1999), who
found that many studies comparing treatments were not conducted by research-
ers who equally included representatives of the two competing therapies—that
is, researchers were not equally invested in both treatments. This has been a
problem especially in comparison studies that use a treatment-as-usual con-
trol group (Wampold et al., 2010) and has led to the suggestion that such
groups, rather than being called treatment as usual, should be labeled intention
to fail controls because the groups are often offering very poor treatment rather
than a reasonable alternative therapy. When Elliott et al. controlled for the
effects of researchers’ allegiance and bias on effect size, the results demonstrated
that humanistic therapies are clinically and statistically equivalent to other
treatments.
Given the widespread belief, especially within academic departments
in the United States, that CBT is a uniquely effective treatment, compar-
ing treatment outcomes between CBT and humanistic treatments seems
especially interesting to humanistic theorists and practitioners. Elliott et al.
(2013) found a surprising large (n = 76) number of studies that compared
humanistic and CBT treatments (allowing for the fact that many types of
both CBT and humanistic therapy exist). These included studies of differ-
ent types of CBT and experiential therapy and many different client popu-
lations. Overall, the weighted effect size was d = -0.13, which was small
and favored CBT. When only the subset of studies that reported randomized
controlled trials (the most rigorous designs) was considered, the number of
studies shrank to 65 and produced an effect size of -0.14, with both results
favoring CBT.
At face value, effect sizes of this kind suggest that the relative success
rate of CBT-treated clients would be about 54%, and the rate for humanisti-
cally treated clients would be 46%. Over long periods of time, if large client
SUMMARY
Much of the research that has attempted to estimate the effects of psycho
therapy on clients is relevant to humanistic treatments, even though it was
not designed to test their effects as such. Efforts to promulgate evidence-based
psychotherapies have been noble in intent and are praiseworthy efforts to
distill scientific research into clinical applications and to guide practice and
training. Research results have demonstrated that, in a climate of account-
ability, psychotherapy stands up to empirical scrutiny with the best of health
care interventions. At the same time, many practitioners and researchers
have found efforts to codify evidence-based treatments seriously incomplete.
Although scientifically laudable in their intent, these efforts have largely
ignored the therapy relationship, the person of the therapist, and the con-
tribution of clients (outside of diagnosis). If one were to read previous efforts
literally, they suggest that disembodied therapists apply manualized interven-
tions to discrete Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders disorders.
Not only is that language offensive to some practitioners on clinical grounds,
but the research evidence is weak for validating treatment methods in isola-
tion from the therapy relationship and the individual client.
Given the evidence for the value of progress feedback and use of CST
feedback, in this section we first highlight a client whose therapist added
these feedback methods to humanistic practice methods. In the second sce-
nario, we illustrate the issue of improving outcomes in clients with anxiety.
In the first case the client’s progress has been monitored from the begin-
ning of psychotherapy through the use of the Outcome Questionnaire—
45 (Lambert et al., 2013). The client, Mary, entered treatment with a score
of 86, which is typical of outpatients and is at the 97th percentile of the
normative population (higher scores indicate more psychological pain). The
REFERENCES
This chapter is dedicated to the memory of David L. Rennie, who left a legacy of scholarly contributions
in both humanistic and qualitative psychology. I thank Ethan Lu and Fredrick Wertz for their comments
on points of this chapter.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-004
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
81
2015; Rennie, 2012; Stiles, 1993). The close ties between the humanistic
worldview and qualitative ontology and epistemology have encouraged the
development of these approaches.
The strong connection between qualitative approaches and humanistic
psychotherapies and psychotherapists can be traced to their foundational
values. Qualitative research, sometimes termed human science, refers to scien-
tific approaches that have been developed to deal with the distinct challenges
of studying the subjective aspects of people that distinguish them from material
objects. Rennie (1995) observed that all psychological research is embedded
within rhetoric—that is, the art of persuading readers to share a particular point
of view. Along with others (e.g., Bazerman, 1987), he described the traditional,
quantitative, scientific praxis as based on disciplinary standards requiring work
to be presented in objectivist formats if it is to be recognized as credible—
thereby avoiding discussion of researchers’ perspectives or the mutual influ-
ence of participants and researchers. Human science rhetoric is different. Its
subjectivist framework assumes that researchers have expectations and hopes
about their work and that culture and language can sensitize investigators to
certain concepts and associations at the expense of others—that is, it assumes
that researcher–participant influence is unavoidable. As a result, engaging in
explicit consideration of researchers’ reflexive processes increases the rigor
within subjectivist rhetoric. Given these different approaches, a discussion
of ontological and epistemological foundations is in order.
Postpositivist Approach
Constructivist–Interpretive Approaches
Critical–Ideological Approaches
Therapist: It can be hard to figure out what you are feeling. [Pause]
There is something hard about that.
Therapist: It feels like you have to figure out what I want. It’s hard to
just stay focused on what you are feeling and notice what
feelings are inside you. Let’s see what happens if we just stay
focused for a little bit on what feelings you have. [Pause]
Client: I feel like pushing away. I feel scared. It’s hard to let myself
notice my feelings.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-005
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
115
as the ability to see the world through others’ eyes so as to sense their
hurt and pain and to perceive the source of their feelings in the same way
as they do. In this regard, he was careful to distinguish identification from
empathy, seeing the former as indicating a loss of boundaries otherwise
conceptualized as emotional contagion. Rogers (1959, 1975) and Kohut
(1971, 1977) used the term to describe a way of being with others to pro-
mote healing in psychotherapy. This usage drew on the meaning of the word
that emphasized the capacity to understand the experiences, thoughts, and
feelings of another.
Numerous theorists have seen empathy as a basic relationship skill that
is required to understand others at even the most basic level (Bohart &
Greenberg, 1997; Feshbach, 1997; Hoffman, 1982; Jordan, 1997; Linehan,
1997; Trop & Stolorow, 1997; Watson, Goldman, & Vanaerschot, 1998).
There are different levels of understanding and different types of empathic
process. We can understand others by knowing what they mean intellec-
tually, by comprehending their values, worldviews, goals, and objectives.
However, for a fuller understanding we need to know how people are affected
emotionally, because this reveals the significance or meaning of events
for them (Greenberg, Rice, & Elliott, 1993; Gross, 2007; Rogers, 1951/1965;
Taylor, 1985).
Barrett-Lennard (1993) posited a model of empathy as an active, cycli-
cal process characterized by three phases: empathic resonance, empathic
communication, and perceived or received empathy. In the first phase, thera-
pists resonate to their clients’ experiences using information from their own
bodily reactions and inner experience to understand how their clients are
feeling moment to moment and to understand the significance and mean-
ing of events for them. In the second phase, therapists communicate their
understanding to their clients, and in the third phase, clients apprehend and
receive their therapists’ empathy to feel understood (Watson, 2007).
Bohart and Greenberg (1997), in their review of empathy, differenti-
ated among three types of empathy: person, affective, and cognitive. Person
empathy is an understanding of the whole person in situ. It requires an under-
standing of what clients have experienced, including their histories and life
stories, to acquire a holistic understanding of who they are and how they
developed. Affective empathy refers to being attuned to the affective experi-
ence of others as revealed by their body language and narratives to under-
stand clearly the impact and significance of events for them. Cognitive empathy
is the capacity to understand and make sense of clients’ narratives. According
to neuroscientists, empathy is a “complex form of psychological inference that
enables us to understand the personal experiences of another person through
cognitive, evaluative and affective processes” (Danziger, Prkachin, & Willer,
2006, p. 2494).
The evidence for the positive impact of therapist empathy is among the
highest and strongest of all factors in the psychotherapy literature, and it con-
tinues to accumulate (Elliott, Watson, Bohart, & Greenberg, 2012; Lambert &
Barley, 2001; Norcross & Lambert, 2011a, 2011b; Watson & Watson, 2010).
Although an earlier meta-analytic study suggested that empathy might be more
important in cognitive–behavioral therapy than in humanistic psychotherapy
(Bohart, Elliott, Greenberg, & Watson, 2002), a more recent update has shown
that empathy is predictive of outcome across different psychotherapies (Elliott
et al., 2011). Empathy was found to have a medium or moderate effect size,
accounting for approximately 10% of the variance in outcome. This is more
than that attributed to the impact of therapeutic interventions (Elliott et al.,
2011; Wampold, 2001).
Spurred by Norcross’s (2001) observation that insufficient attention has
been paid to understanding what changes in psychotherapy as a function of the
A CASE STUDY
Alan was a man in his 60s who had a serious heart condition as well as
diabetes and high blood pressure. When he came to therapy, he was seeking
help with his depression. His doctor wanted to prescribe antidepressants, but
they caused an adverse reaction with the other medications that he was taking.
He wanted to try to find an alternative way to deal with his depression. In the
first few sessions, he disclosed that he felt burdened and experienced consider-
able strain in his marriage. His wife had been diagnosed with a mental disorder
20 years previously, and Alan had cared for her and their children without the
support of other family members, who lived far away. The couple had emigrated
from South America 26 years earlier and had raised three children, two daugh-
ters and a son. By the time Alan came to therapy, the children were grown up;
one was single and pursuing a career as a doctor, and the other two were stay-
at-home mothers with children.
Alan’s primary objectives since his wife became ill had been to ensure
their children’s well-being and protect them from their mother’s condition. He
wanted to make certain that they were not burdened by their mother’s illness.
He felt ashamed and was concerned that if his daughters’ husbands were
fully aware of their mother’s condition, it would compromise their marriages.
His wife suffered from fits of rage as well as depression and delusions. Alan
had resisted institutionalizing her, against the recommendations of doctors,
because he wanted to protect his family from the shame. However, his doc-
tor had recently expressed concern, telling him that if his blood pressure did
not decrease, he would need to do something drastic to protect his physical
well-being. Alan hoped that psychotherapy might help to alleviate some of
the strain that he was feeling and have a positive impact on his blood pressure
and heart condition.
Clients feel safe when they are listened to empathically. They feel heard,
understood, and supported. Safety in the relationship enables clients to focus
on their concerns within the therapeutic hour. It promotes exploration and
enables clients to examine and look at aspects of their experience that might
have been denied or relegated outside awareness. In addition to creating a
safe place, therapists’ empathy is important in forming and maintaining the
therapeutic alliance and in negotiating agreement on the tasks and goals of
therapy. Empathic therapists are able to monitor their interactions with their
Empathy helps clients to regulate their affect and learn to soothe them-
selves. Affect regulation is an important topic of investigation for social,
developmental, and neuropsychologists (DeSteno, Gross, & Kubzansky, 2013;
Feshbach, 1997; van der Kolk, 1994, 1996; van der Kolk, Roth, Pelcovitz,
Sunday, & Spinazzola, 2005). These researchers recognize the important role
that early attachment experiences play in people’s abilities to regulate their
emotions and their neurophysiological functioning. Clients who come to
therapy are often experiencing acute and chronic conditions related to dys-
regulation in their affective systems. Empathic responding begins to help
clients regulate their emotions, both within the session and in the long term.
Human beings experience a sense of relief and comfort when they feel
understood by another, especially when they are experiencing intense and
painful emotions. The expression and symbolization of emotions act to contain
and moderate them in the same way that rating them on a scale of 1 to 10 does.
Naming or labeling feelings begins the process of affect regulation. Once emo-
tions have been labeled, clients are able to reflect on them to understand the
impact of events and identify what they need to protect or nourish themselves.
As therapists listen to their clients, and attend to and accept their expe-
riences, they model positive ways of being with the self. Barrett-Lennard
REFERENCES
Angus, L. E., & Greenberg, L. S. (2011). Working with narrative in emotion-focused
therapy: Changing stories, healing lives. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12325-000
Angus, L. E., & Kagan, F. (2009). Therapist empathy and client anxiety reduction
in motivational interviewing: “She carries with me, the experience.” Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 65, 1156–1167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20635
Bachelor, A. (1988). How clients perceive therapist empathy: A content analysis
of “received” empathy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 25,
227–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085337
Barkham, M., & Shapiro, D. A. (1986). Counselor verbal response modes and expe-
rienced empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 3–10. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0022-0167.33.1.3
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1962). Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in
therapeutic change. Psychological Monographs, 76, 1–36.
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1993). The phases and focus of empathy. British Journal
of Medical Psychology, 66, 3–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1993.
tb01722.x
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1997). The recovery of empathy towards self and others. In
A. C. Bohart & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in
psychotherapy (pp. 103–121). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso-
ciation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10226-004
The idea that accessing and exploring painful emotions and bad feelings
in a therapeutic relationship may result in one feeling better in the long
term is now a widely held belief among several schools of psychotherapy,
but it has always been a central position among humanistic and experiential
therapists beginning with Rogers (1951) and Perls (1969). In this chapter,
we review process and outcome research on emotion in psychotherapy, with a
special focus on the theoretical framework and interventions of experiential
treatments.
Humanistic and experiential therapies have led the way in developing
interventions that address emotion directly in a manner that is sensitive to
personal development and idiosyncratic meaning. The recent emergence of
affective neuroscience has further stimulated interest in the role of emotion
in psychotherapy by providing a means to observe and measure affect in vivo.
Early work examining emotional experience included Rogers’s (1951) focus
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-006
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
147
on the importance of attending to and prizing a client’s internal frames of
reference, Gendlin’s (1964) articulation of how productive experience unfolds,
and Gendlin’s (1996) emphasis on affective expression, enactments, and evoc-
ative encounters.
Drawing on this foundation, Greenberg and others (Greenberg, 2002;
Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, 2006; Greenberg & Watson, 2005) have identi-
fied five distinct types of emotion processes that are useful in therapy, depend-
ing on a client’s presenting concern. They are (a) emotional awareness and
engagement; (b) arousal and enactment; (c) emotional regulation and self-
soothing; (d) reflection on emotion and meaning-making, which involves
enduring cognitive change; and (e) emotional transformation, or changing
emotion with emotion. Research findings are summarized below and illustrated
with brief examples of client process. We go on to highlight some of the practi-
cal implications of working with emotion from a humanistic and experiential
approach.
After relationship conditions have been established, and after the initial
contact in emotional awareness, deepening of experience, and clear expression,
experiential therapies move toward transforming emotion. Such approaches
use a process-guiding style to create change by evoking affect to promote emo-
tional processing and access to additional material (Greenberg, 2002). Rice
(1974), a pivotal influence on Greenberg and the development of emotion-
focused therapy, was one of the first to underscore the evocative function of
a client-centered therapist. Thus, after previously unacknowledged experi-
ence has been accessed, the focus shifts to transforming certain emotional
experiences by using emergent and alternative emotions to expand a person’s
repertoire.
The term emotion scheme is often used to capture the dynamic nature
of emotion as a multimodal network of feelings and meanings (i.e., a self-
organization) that could exist in one’s repertoire at various levels of activa-
tion (Greenberg et al., 1993). Attending to a current (maladaptive) emotion
scheme that is in need of transformation, such as feeling worthless, makes it
accessible to new inputs that might change it. Identification of and attention
to unfulfilled needs embedded in a maladaptive state stimulate alternative
self-organizations, which are tacit, emotionally based schemes; they begin to
organize the individual toward meeting an identified need. It is the synthesis of
this new possibility with the old ones that leads to lasting change (Greenberg,
2002; Greenberg & Watson, 2005; A. Pascual-Leone & Greenberg, 2007).
Thus, experiential approaches of this kind make use of the power of affect to
catalyze change, producing a restructuring of core emotion-based schemes.
Angus, L. E., & Greenberg, L. S. (2011). Working with narrative in emotion-focused
therapy: Changing stories, healing lives. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12325-000
Angus, L. E., & McLeod, J. (2004). The handbook of narrative and psychotherapy: Prac-
tice, theory, and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Davis, D. D. (2004). Cognitive therapy of personality
disorders (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and consequences of feelings. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606106
Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., Mohr, D., Daldrup, R. J., Bergan, J., Meredith, K., &
Merry, W. (1991). Predictors of differential response to cognitive, experiential,
and self-directed psychotherapeutic procedures. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
cal Psychology, 59, 333–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.333
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin
(Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 77–108).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis,
rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 724–731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167202289002
Carryer, J. R., & Greenberg, L. S. (2010). Optimal levels of emotional arousal in
experiential therapy of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
78, 190–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018401
Chard, K. M. (2005). An evaluation of cognitive processing therapy for the treat-
ment of posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood sexual abuse. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 965–971. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
006X.73.5.965
Cloitre, M., Koenen, K., Dohen, L., & Han, H. (2002). Skills training in affect
and interpersonal regulation followed by exposure: A phase-based treatment for
PTSD related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
70, 1067–1074. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.5.1067
Coombs, M. M., Coleman, D., & Jones, E. E. (2002). Working with feelings: The
importance of emotion in both cognitive–behavioral and interpersonal therapy in
the NIMH treatment of depression collaborative research program. Psycho-
therapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39, 233–244.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of
consciousness. New York, NY: Harcourt.
Davidson, R. (2000). Affective style, mood, and anxiety disorders: An affective neuro-
science approach. In R. Davidson (Ed.), Anxiety, depression, and emotion
(pp. 88–108). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195133585.003.0005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-007
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
185
OVERVIEW OF THE THEORY
A New Psychotherapy
Contemporary Developments
Integrative Approaches
Process-Guiding Approaches
Existential Approach
There has long been a debate over the differences between person-
centered and experiential therapies. However, a particular tribe that is emerg-
ing in both the United Kingdom and Europe favors a synergistic apprecia-
tion of these two core dimensions, person centeredness and experience. Lietaer
(2002), an early proponent, suggested that these two terms define P-CT. Schmid
(2003) countered, arguing that the difference between person-centered and
experiential therapy exists at the level of the image of a person that each
person holds. Our view is that, rather than see the person-centered and the
experiential therapies as totally separate, person-centered and experiential
dimensions can be considered as two axes on which all tribes could be plot-
ted. A third axis might also possibly be added to represent the level and
style of intervention used by a therapist (Warner, 2000). As we have shown
above, a therapist can be high in terms of intervening while maintaining a
nondirective attitude, although this would mark a clear divergence from the
classical school, wherein nondirectivity refers to both behavior and attitude.
This being said, a person-centered experiential approach accommodates the
potential for integration at the technical (pragmatic) level and acknowl-
edges the experiential dimension to making meaning, but retains the image
of the person proposed in the dialogic–relational school. We believe that this
approach offers one of the most promising potential directions of movement
for future developments in P-CT.
Outcome Research
Nondirectivity
The therapist then helps the client through the relaxation exercise,
which lasts for about 15 minutes. As the client feels more relaxed and at ease,
the dialogue continues:
Client: Well, I certainly feel calmer now, much more relaxed than
at the start of the session today. Right now I’m wondering
about sharing something that came to me when I was just
sitting back there concentrating on my breathing.
Therapist: Right, so, as you were relaxing, something came up for you,
and now you’re wondering whether or not to share that,
maybe wondering if there’s enough time today and perhaps
whether it’s something you want to get into.
Client: Yeah, well, let me give it a go and see what happens.
Therapist: Okay, give it a go.
Client: Well, as I was just there, relaxed and all that, I started think-
ing about some of those thoughts. But it was kinda differ-
ent. They were coming much slower, and I was able to notice
more things. Fewer thoughts were coming, but I got more
detail to the ones that did come. I was remembering some
stuff from the schools.
Therapist: I see, you want to go on more . . . about the thoughts that
were coming?
Client: I remembered there was someone there for me. It was that
every year or two my dad came to visit me. He couldn’t be
there all the time, I know that. But it made me think that
he hadn’t forgotten us. And till now I’d never really realized
that. I thought we were just dumped in the school and for-
gotten about. But we weren’t.
Therapist: So this came to you right now, for the first time; you can
recall that you weren’t forgotten about, not just dumped and
forgotten. Someone remembered you, and you can remem-
ber being remembered—you were thought about.
In the summary and conclusion for the first edition of this volume,
Bozarth et al. (2002) titled their chapter “Client-Centered Therapy: Evolution
of a Revolution.” On the basis of the evidence we have reviewed, there is
no doubt of the contribution that this revolutionary approach has made to
contemporary psychotherapy. P-CT continues to offer a therapeutic approach
premised on a revolutionary and radical ontology. Our review of the literature
and extrapolation of research into practice shows that modern understanding
of the nondirective attitude can be helpful to clients even when maintained
by minimally trained therapists such as those involved in NDSTs. Moreover,
when adhered to by highly trained therapists, person-centered therapies can
facilitate the development of a growth-supporting therapeutic environment
with some of the most distressed clients.
The P-CT model now has support not only from the wider field of human-
istic psychology, but also from the growing field of psychological inquiry
evidence from self-determination theory and posttraumatic growth theory,
which lend support for the growth paradigm (Joseph & Linley, 2006a; Ryan &
Deci, 2000). One of the main theories of posttraumatic growth now attract-
ing research and clinical attention is an explicit integration of the person-
centered approach with trauma theories (Joseph & Linley, 2005). There is a
need for practitioners and scholars in the field of P-CT to engage more fully
with the mainstream field of psychology and the new developments that build
on and are consistent with the person-centered approach (Joseph & Murphy,
2013a, 2013b).
Although the person-centered approach has seen a reduction in avail-
ability in the United States and to some extent in part of northern Europe,
in the United Kingdom the approach remains strong. In fact, P-CT is one
of the most widely available approaches found in training courses. The
National Health Service and the National Institute for Clinical Excellence
have recently supported the development of an integrated version of P-CT
for depression. Called counseling for depression (Sanders & Hill, 2014), the
approach is now supported as an evidence-based therapy and is offered as a
high-intensity therapy and credible alternative to CBT. The increase in out-
come research studies reported in the meta-analyses reviewed in this chapter
suggests that research on the approach is in a state of revival. This is in large
part attributable to the evolution and integration of classical and experien-
tial theories and practices. In our own training program at the University of
Nottingham, we have taken the step to name the course Person-Centered
Experiential Counseling and Psychotherapy, highlighting the fusion of these
two core dimensions to the approach. We see this direction of movement
in the field as the logical position for P-CT to take. Although our staff have
REFERENCES
Gordon Wheeler (Lobb & Wheeler, 2013) described the focus of Gestalt
therapy as understanding the processes and structures by which human beings
organize and interpret their perceived worlds, that is, a process of discovery.
It is a hermeneutic and phenomenological perspective achieved in the course
of Gestalt therapy through the relationship between therapist and client in
the midst of a complex situation.
In this chapter, I provide a theoretical overview of the core theory of con-
temporary Gestalt therapy. It has come a long way since the days of Frederick
and Laura Perls, who focused on the revision of psychoanalysis (Perls, 1947/1969)
in an early theoretical integration of existential, phenomenological, and organ-
ismic theories (Brownell, 2010; Perls, Hefferline, & Goodman, 1951/1972).
Since then, classical Gestalt therapy’s pragmatic roots have developed into a
grounded faith in process. Its phenomenological roots evolved from aware-
ness of current experience to appreciation for embodied cognition (Frank &
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-008
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
219
La Barre, 2011; M. Johnson, 2007) and the interpretation of experience
(Gallagher & Zahavi, 2008). Its emphasis on contacting, the meeting of the
organism at the boundary with others in the environmental field, became both
an enriched understanding of relationship and a more complex understand-
ing of causation in the organism–environment field itself. The field theory of
Kurt Lewin and others became refined in the understanding of intersubjective
processes occurring in groups, societies, and cultures. The experiential aspects
of Gestalt therapy matured into an understanding of kinesthetic processes at
the base of primordial experience (Frank, 2001).
THEORETICAL OVERVIEW
The options available to a Gestalt therapist are derived from the four
main tenets of its theory, unified in a process of contacting in the therapist–
client field. That process illuminates the patterns and sequences people use to
make meaning. The therapist can (a) follow the emerging experience of the
client through a modified phenomenological method, (b) engage the client
through dialogue, (c) strategically address aspects of the field, and (d) nego-
tiate an experiment—a move to enactment in the service of awareness and
learning (Brownell, 2008; Mackewn, 1997).
Dialogical Relationship
1
This same focus has often been attributed to the influence of Taoism and Buddhism in the formation
of Gestalt therapy, with their respective interests in awareness in the current moment and the way in
which any given thing is taking place.
2
An icon points to something more significant beyond itself and is in contrast to an idol, which points
to itself as that which is of most importance.
Existential Experimentalism
The meeting between therapist and client takes place in a context that is
at once physical, material, phenomenal, and relational. Both client and thera-
pist bring something to their meeting from outside the context of therapy.
Both of them assimilate from this meeting what they can and reject what they
cannot; it is the creative adjustment they make in the process of their meeting
(Bandín, 2012).
Contacting is the best term for this meeting, because it is a process and
not a static event. Contacting takes place between persons, but it also takes
place between any given person and the environment. This contacting,
including the sensory quality or nature of it—what some in Gestalt therapy
call an aesthetic criteria5 (Bloom, 2003, 2011)—is a center of gravity that pulls
together the core of Gestalt therapeutic practice into one, unified, approach.
In Gestalt therapy, all the various core tenets are active simultaneously.
It is not simply multimodal. The phenomenological method of tracking the
emerging experience of the client, the dialogical relationship in which each
grows in experiential knowledge of one another, the field-theoretic strate-
gies in which causative influences are both understood and initiated, and the
experimentalism in which both client and therapist move to enactment are
all in play at the same time, during the very first meeting between therapist
and client.
When Gestalt therapy formed in the middle of the last century, its
founders were not interested in research. In spite of this, over time scattered
research was conducted in a few places, and studies of one kind or another
were reported in the first edition of this book. Strümpfel and Goldman (2002)
5
The word aesthetic refers to the senses, so in this context it is about what can be known through the
senses, through meeting others and the environment.
Gestalt-Specific Research
Gestalt-Consilient Research
6
Specific identifying information on this client has been changed to protect her identity.
Process Comments
Butollo et al. (2014) and Man Leung et al. (2013) referred to the con-
struct of contact (and the process of contacting) as being important in their
research. Man Leung et al. attributed the contacting through dialogue, experi-
ment, and the aware presence of those concerned to the increase in agency and
hope in their study subjects. In turn, they related those increases to Gestalt’s
construct of self-regulation. Butollo et al. claimed that the research conducted
that showed Gestalt therapy was effective in the treatment of PTSD implied
the relevance of contact and contacting as skills that facilitate the organi-
zation of experience. Thus, contacting is a skill that Gestalt therapists can
manifest through their work in following the subjective experience of their
clients, meeting their clients through presence in the dialogical relationship,
and supporting clients for behavioral experimentation.
The research on ACT supports the Gestalt therapy construct of the par-
adoxical theory of change—acceptance of what is and the actualization of
the person in the current moment. This construct allows the Gestalt thera-
pist to work in a descriptive manner, using the phenomenological method
without having to make sense of the client’s presentation; the therapist
simply accepts the process as it unfolds, trusting that, paradoxically, the
increased awareness of what is in the current moment will develop into
something more. It is an existential and pragmatic trust that the field will,
indeed, supply what is needed.
The CORE study conducted in England (Stevens et al., 2011) provided
evidence that, when Gestalt therapists work naturally in accord with their
training, they can be every bit as effective as therapists from other clinical
perspectives, including CBT, for clients with anxiety, depression, and rela-
tionship and self-esteem issues, among other disorders. This is encouraging.
It should settle the issue and free up Gestalt therapists simply to work in
accordance with what they have learned.
REFERENCES
We thank Toshihiro Kawasaki for his efforts in identifying and organizing studies conducted in Japan.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-009
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
251
Mathieu-Coughlan, & Kiesler, 1986). This summary positively correlates expe-
riencing level and participant-reported positive therapy outcome. We present a
discussion of the philosophical foundation for FOT. We also include transcripts
from client sessions at key points to help illuminate the principles discussed. We
conclude with suggestions on how process-oriented research modalities might
shape the trajectory of future research on FOT.
Experiencing Scale
Clearing a Space
EXP
Studies using the EXP have continued from 2000 to the present, with
researchers investigating such areas as the manner of experiencing in senile
dementia (Ichioka, 2000) and using changes in EXP levels before and after
the pause (i.e., moments of silence in focusing and therapy) to explore what
happens within that pause (Uchida, 2002). A Five-Stage EXP (Miyake,
Ikemi, & Tamura, 2008) has been developed that simplified the original rat-
ing criteria to eventually develop a paper-and-pencil therapist evaluation
form of the EXP. In addition, this scale has been used by other FOT research-
ers in similar therapeutic approaches to help validate their own theories of
CAS Protocol
A number of recent studies, including several from Japan, have exam-
ined the usefulness of the CAS protocol as a valuable addition to FOT
research. CAS studies have been applied to a wide range of subject popula-
tions, including college students, patients with cancer, people with chronic
Children’s Focusing
The application of focusing to children has continued to grow since
Martha Stapert integrated focusing into individual child psychotherapy in
1985 in the Netherlands. Since 1998, the International Children’s Focusing
Conference has been held every other year. National organizations for chil-
dren’s focusing have formed in the Netherlands, Japan, and Romania, testi-
fying to the significance of this development. Literature regarding children’s
focusing is found on The Focusing Institute website (http://www.focusing.
org), which currently carries 78 articles on the subject. These articles embrace
such applications as child psychotherapy, methods of teaching focusing to
children, the significance of focusing for school teachers, and the use of
focusing-oriented teaching methods in the classroom.
Therapist Focusing
A substantial body of studies is developing, particularly in Japan, on
the use of focusing by therapists. This application includes using focusing
REFERENCES
Questions about the nature of human existence have puzzled the human
mind for millennia, for instance, “What is the meaning of my life?” “How
do I cope with my mortality?” (e.g., Greenberg, Koole, & Pyszczynski, 2004;
Tillich, 1952). For some people, these concerns may evoke such anxiety,
uncertainty, and crisis that they may experience severe psychological distress
(Yalom, 1980). People may be especially vulnerable to experiencing such dis-
tress when they are in boundary situations (Jaspers, 1925)—that is, when they
are confronted with issues about their very existence, such as the diagnosis of
terminal illness. Many forms of psychotherapy and counseling implicitly help
clients to address such existential concerns. However, one group of therapies
that explicitly claims to do this—and to help clients develop a deeper under-
standing of their lived existence within a close relational encounter—consists
of the existential approaches to psychotherapy.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-010
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
283
Until recently, research on the outcomes and processes of these exis-
tential therapies has been relatively scarce. In other psychotherapeutic tradi-
tions, most notably cognitive–behavioral therapy, a wide range of studies on
the effectiveness and mechanisms of psychotherapeutic practice have been
carried out (see Lambert, 2013). But the positivist underpinnings of such
research mean that it has largely been rejected in the existential psycho-
therapy field, in which human experience is viewed as unique and irreducible
and therefore outside the realm of natural science inquiry (Cooper, 2003;
Spinelli, 2005). Hence, when research is undertaken, it tends to be based
on a human science paradigm, which prioritizes the subjective experiencing
of the individual (e.g., Giorgi, 1985). Extensive case studies have also been
written (see DuPlock, 1997; Yalom, 1989) that can give powerful insights
into clients’ experiences of existential issues and the existential therapeutic
journey (most commonly from the psychotherapist’s perspective), but they
have provided little by way of a systematic, critical evaluation of whether,
and how, existential psychotherapies may bring about positive change.
A previous review of the research in relation to existential psychotherapies
has provided the most comprehensive evaluation to date (Walsh & McElwain,
2002). In it, Walsh and McElwain (2002) reviewed the evidence in support of
key existential assumptions (freedom, intersubjectivity, temporality, and becom-
ing) and concepts (existential anxiety, guilt, and authenticity), providing a vivid
description of existential theory and practice. However, Walsh and McElwain
did not directly review the available evidence on the effectiveness of existential
therapies or the wider psychotherapy research findings bearing on the potential
effectiveness of this approach.
The aim of this chapter, therefore, is to provide the first comprehensive
review of the evidence for the effectiveness of existential therapies. We begin
with a definition of the existential approaches to psychotherapy before going
on to consider the main elements and forms of existential psychotherapeutic
practice. We then review the process-outcome research bearing on the effec-
tiveness of existential therapies before looking more specifically at evidence
from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) and other systematic methods that
directly evaluate their outcomes. In the final part of the chapter, we discuss,
and illustrate, the implications this evidence has for practice.
General Practices
Phenomenological Practices
Correia et al.’s (2015) survey suggested that the methods most character-
istic of existential psychotherapy fit within the domain of phenomenological
Relational Practices
Data from Sousa and Alegria (2015), Correia et al. (2015), and
Norcross (1987) have suggested that a second common set of existential
psychotherapeutic practices constellates around the establishment of an
Meaning-Oriented Therapies
These forms of existential therapy use meaning as their central organiz-
ing construct, drawing from empirical research on the psychology of meaning
(Wong, 1998). Their origins can be traced back to Viktor Frankl’s (1986)
logotherapy, which aims to help clients discover purpose in their lives and
overcome feelings of meaninglessness and despair. Logotherapy is grounded
in the assumption that “he who has a why to live can bear with almost
any how” (Frankl, 1984, p. 97). That is, individuals who are experiencing
psychological distress can still do things that feel meaningful and important;
moreover, encountering situations in this way may give people the strength
and resilience to cope with life’s hardships.
To help clients reflect on their meanings in life, logotherapy uses rel-
atively directive techniques (such as Socratic questioning and paradoxical
injunctions; Frankl, 1984, 1986). Although logotherapists do not tell their
clients what should be meaningful in their lives, they will actively stimulate
an exploration of past, present, and future meanings to help clients identify,
connect, or reconnect with potential sources of meaning in their lives.
Today, meaning-oriented therapies are among the most widely practiced
forms of existential psychotherapy (Correia, Cooper, & Berdondini, 2014),
with a number of recent developments emerging from Frankl’s (1984) original
work. Most prevalent is Längle’s et al.’s (2005) existential–analytic approach,
which broadens logotherapeutic principles to consider a wider range of fun-
damental conditions for a fulfilled existence: an acceptance of one’s being,
feeling that one’s life is good, and sensing that one has a right to be oneself. This
approach has also proposed a number of new therapeutic techniques, although it
tends to be less directive than Frankl’s original stance. Wong’s (1998) meaning-
centered counseling is another reformulation of logotherapeutic principles
that integrates a focus on meaning with evidence and principles from positive
psychology and cognitive–behavioral practice. A third new form of individual
meaning-oriented practice is the meaning-making intervention (e.g., Henry
et al., 2010), based on Folkman’s theory of meaning-making as a coping strat-
egy (Park & Folkman, 1997). This therapy aims to help clients who have been
diagnosed with cancer reconcile shattered assumptions about self-worth as
well as the controllability and fairness of life events.
Existential–Humanistic Therapies
In the United States, an existential–humanistic approach to therapy
emerged under the leadership of Rollo May. In 1958, May coedited Existence:
A New Dimension in Psychiatry and Psychology (May, Angel, & Ellenberger,
1958), which brought the writings and practices of European existential and
phenomenological psychiatrists to the United States for the first time. Three
of Rollo May’s mentees have become prolific writers about the existential
approach in the United States: James Bugental (1965, 1978), Irvin Yalom
(1980, 1989), and Kirk Schneider (2008).
From an existential–humanistic perspective, people are understood to
experience psychological difficulties as a consequence of trying to defend
against deep-seated anxieties. In this respect, it can be considered similar
to a classical psychoanalytical perspective (e.g., Wolitzky, 2003). However,
from this existential–humanistic standpoint, what creates people’s most basic
fears is their knowledge of the unavoidable givens of life—in particular, their
mortality, freedom, aloneness, and meaninglessness (Yalom, 1980). Hence,
existential–humanistic therapy tries to help clients overcome problematic
defenses and meet the anxieties of existence with an attitude of decisive-
ness and resolve (Cooper, 2012). Therapeutic strategies are primarily expe-
riential, ranging from the gently exploratory to the highly confrontational,
often oriented around an exploration of the dynamics of the therapeutic
relationship.
In recent decades, Yalom’s (1970) work on group psychotherapy, com-
bined with his articulation of an existential–humanistic standpoint (Yalom,
1980), has led to the development of a range of group-based psychotherapies
for people experiencing life-limiting illnesses. Supportive–expressive therapy
was designed by Spiegel and Yalom (1978) as a relatively unstructured, sup-
portive therapy for the treatment of cancer patients. It aims to foster sup-
port among members, facilitating expression of emotion about cancer and its
effects on their lives (Bordeleau et al., 2003). Discussions are oriented around
themes such as fears of dying and death, an examination of life goals and life
priorities, and the integration of a changed self and body image (Classen
et al., 2001). The impact of terminal illness on personal relationships is also
As was the preceding section, this section of the chapter is divided into
two parts. In the first part, we look at the evidence from the field of psycho-
therapy research and psychology that bears on the general practices of exis-
tential psychotherapists. In the second part, we then look more specifically at
the outcomes of particular forms of existential psychotherapy, reporting on the
findings of a new meta-analysis of the effectiveness of existential approaches.
General Practices
Relational Practices
The emphasis within the existential therapies, particularly the British and
existential–humanistic schools, on the quality of the therapeutic relationship
is strongly supported by empirical research. As the American Psychological
Association Task Force concluded, based on the most comprehensive analysis
of the data to date, “the therapy relationship makes substantial and consistent
contributions to psychotherapy outcomes independent of the specific type of
treatment” (Norcross & Wampold, 2011, p. 423).
The aspects of the therapeutic relationship that have been found to
relate to positive outcomes are also closely associated with those practices
that are characteristic of existential psychotherapeutic work. Alongside empa-
thy, the therapeutic alliance—the degree of collaboration and bond between
client and therapist (Bordin, 1979)—has been identified as a demonstrably
effective element of the therapeutic relationship (Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger,
& Symonds, 2011). Levels of positive regard have been identified as a prob-
ably effective element of the therapeutic relationship (Farber & Doolin,
2011), and psychotherapists’ congruence has been named a promising ele-
ment of the therapeutic relationship, albeit with insufficient research to judge
(Kolden, Klein, Wang, & Austin, 2011; Safran, Muran, & Eubanks-Carter,
2011). Similarly, therapists’ capacity to repair alliance ruptures is also seen
as promising in this regard. A previous meta-analysis (Hill & Knox, 2002)
found that therapist self-disclosures were a promising and probably effective
element of the therapeutic relationship, particularly when they were positive
self-involving statements, such as, “I get a real sense of excitement when you
talk about your photography.” However, reviews of the evidence have yet to
Meaning-Oriented Therapies
Research on the outcomes of meaning-oriented therapies has become
widespread in recent years (Vos et al., 2014), particularly in the care of patients
with chronic physical diseases. Our systematic review identified 25 studies
in which such an evaluation had been carried out. All studies examined changes
in meaning and purpose directly through such measures as the Purpose-in-Life
test (Crumbaugh & Maholick, 1964) and the Seeking of Noetic Goals test
(Crumbaugh, 1977).
Six RCTs investigated the effects of meaning-oriented therapies, four of
which were group interventions for cancer patients. These interventions sig-
nificantly increased positive meaning in life compared with either treatment
as usual (e.g., routine medical care) or other therapeutic interventions (e.g.,
counseling), with a moderate to large average effect size (d = 0.64, p < .01,
n = 5 studies). The level of psychopathology was also significantly reduced to
a moderate extent (d = 0.47, p < .05, n = 2), self-efficacy increased moderately
(d = 0.48, p < .05, n = 2). However, one study examining physical well-being
did not find significant effects (p > .05, n = 1). In the 19 non-RCT studies
that assessed the effects of meaning-oriented psychotherapy, similar effect sizes
were found: moderate to large positive effects on meaning in life (d = 0.65,
p < .01, n = 10), reduction of psychopathology (d = 0.40, p < .05, n = 2), and
self-efficacy (d = 0.71, p < .01, n = 2). Similar findings were seen in two sys-
tematic qualitative case studies (Rogina & Quilitch, 2006, 2010) and in three
studies with a qualitative component (Lantz, 1996; Lantz & Gregoire, 2000;
Lee, Cohen, Edgar, Laizner, & Gagnon, 2006).
Thus, the analysis of RCTs, non-RCTs, and qualitative studies seems
to suggest that meaning therapies can help clients to a moderate to large
extent to experience life as meaningful, to increase their self-efficacy, and to
reduce their symptoms of psychopathology. However, more research is needed
because all effects were measured only immediately after the intervention (just
one study showed moderately large effects on long-term improvement related
to meaning in life; d = 0.57, p < .01), and the samples and control groups were
limited (small samples, with mainly group therapies for cancer patients, and
mainly care as usual as control).
Experiential–Existential Therapy
One RCT (Barren, 2005) and one pre–post single-group-design study
(De Vries et al., 1997) investigated the psychological outcomes of experiential–
existential therapy for those diagnosed with a malignant tumor. Participant
numbers in the studies were low, and changes in depression as an outcome
On the basis of Buber’s (1958) concept of the I–Thou stance and a range
of existential writings (e.g., Yalom, 2001), Cooper (2015) suggested eight prac-
tical strategies psychotherapists can use to enhance the possibility of a deep-
ened therapeutic encounter. The first is to try to stand alongside clients as a
“fellow traveler” (Yalom, 2001) rather than surveying, studying, or analyzing
them. Second is to relate to clients as subjects rather than as objects, to engage
with them as a source of agency and experiencing. Third is to acknowledge
clients’ capacities for freedom and choice—to engage with them as someone
who is responding to their world, rather than determined by it. Fourth is to lis-
ten to clients holistically and in an embodied way, breathing in and responding
to the totality of their being. Fifth is to be open to the otherness of clients—to
put one’s assumptions about who clients are to one side and meet them from a
place of indefiniteness and unknowing. Sixth is to affirm clients, but to do so
from a place where the psychotherapist holds onto his or her own difference,
rather than attempting to merge with them. Seventh is relating to clients as
wholenesss, bringing one’s own totality into the encounter. This means relating
in both affective and cognitive ways, and being willing to bring such aspects of
oneself as one’s vulnerabilities into the therapeutic meeting. The final strategy
is a willingness to take risks, to allow oneself to be changed in the meeting with
the other and let go of one’s certainty of and security in what one know.
An example of such practice comes from the work of the fourth author,
Edgar Correia, with a psychiatric outpatient who had spent 3 days at home,
isolated, before the session. The young man, in his early 30s, arrived with a
desperate and anxious look. For 15 minutes, the client, Viktor, told his thera-
pist how sad, desperate, and lonely he was feeling.
Viktor: I can’t see any solution! I’ve reached my limit! I don’t think
I can take this much longer. . . . I’m so completely scared.
[The therapist nods, feeling and receiving Viktor’s pain.] I think
again and again about going home . . . with no one waiting
for me, no one there for me. I’m so afraid of not being able to
Meaning-Oriented Practice
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-011
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
319
those mapping the relationship between emotional change and outcome.
A case example is used to demonstrate theory and illustrate the implica-
tions of research findings.
Growth
Emotion
VIEW OF DYSFUNCTION
EFT has thus shifted away from its traditional client-centered roots that
conceived of congruence between self-concept and experiencing as the main
mechanism of dysfunction. It has adopted a principle of coherence to replace
it. In this view, problems are seen as arising from the way experience is con-
structed rather than solely from the nonacceptance of experience. They are
also seen as arising from the dysregulation of emotions (being overwhelmed
by them), as well as from maladaptive emotional responding based on pain-
ful feelings (such as fear and shame) derived from past experience. Adaptive
functioning involves both discovery of experience and coordination of differ-
ent aspects of experience. Synthesizing these different aspects of experience
generates a coherent whole that makes conscious sense and forms part of
one’s self-identity. People thus are seen not as possessing a self-concept but
rather as constructing a coherent view of themselves. Through the change
process, in a given moment, people can reorganize themselves from being
afraid and timid to becoming strong and enabled. The manner in which one
constructs oneself in a particular therapy session can then be carried over
outside the session.
EFT was getting its start in the later part of the last century, and it began
to grow more significantly after 2000 as research began to proliferate. This
included quantitative, qualitative, and mixed-methods research and an investi-
gation of both the effect of treatment on various disorders, as well as careful
study of the change process.
On the basis of studies conducted in the past decade, support for the
effectiveness of EFT has been solidified. It is now recognized by the American
Psychological Association Division 12 Task Force as an empirically supported
treatment for depression.
In the past decade, a manualized form of EFT for depression (Goldman,
Greenberg, & Angus, 2006; Greenberg & Watson, 2005; Watson, Gordon,
Stermac, Kalogerakos, & Steckley, 2003) was found to be equally or more
effective than a client-centered empathic treatment or a cognitive–behavioral
treatment (CBT). In a study comparing EFT with CBT (Watson et al.,
2003), both treatments were found to be effective, although EFT was more
effective in reducing interpersonal problems. Small to large pre–post effect
sizes (0.30–1.69) were obtained, considering the sample size in each group
(n = 33) across all measures. Effect sizes for differences between groups at
posttreatment on the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Steer, & Garbin,
1988) and the global symptom index of SCL-90-R (Derogatis, 1983) were
very small (0.14 and 0.05, respectively), and differences between effect
sizes on the Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and the Dysfunctional
Attitudes Scale (Weissman & Beck, 1978) were somewhat larger (0.30 and
0.34, respectively), suggesting that investigation with larger sample sizes
may be worthwhile.
In the York II depression study, Goldman et al. (2006) replicated the
York I study that was reviewed in Elliott (2002a). The replication study com-
pared the effects of client-centered therapy and EFT on 38 clients with major
depressive disorder. The client-centered treatment emphasized the establish-
ment and maintenance of the client-centered relationship conditions and
empathic responding that are viewed as a central component of EFT. The
EFT treatment added to the client-centered treatment the use of specific
tasks, in particular, systematic evocative unfolding, focusing, two-chair, and
empty-chair dialogue. The York II study obtained a comparative effect size of
0.71 in favor of EFT therapy. The York I and II samples were then combined
In EFT, both the therapeutic relationship (Rogers, 1957) and the work-
ing alliance (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989) are strongly emphasized, although
they are considered conceptually distinct. Beyond theory, EFT researchers
have also gone to some trouble to empirically distinguish the three compo-
nents of tasks, bonds, and goals. Studies have shown impressive links among
outcome, therapist empathy, and the working alliance (Elliott, Greenberg,
Watson, Timulak, & Freire, 2013; Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, & Symonds,
2011; Lambert, 2005). In an attempt to distinguish the two constructs,
Watson and Geller (2005) examined relationships among clients’ ratings on
the Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory (Barrett-Lennard, 1962), a mea-
sure of perceived empathy, psychotherapy outcome, and the working alliance
in CBT and EFT. Overall, client reports of therapist positive regard, uncon-
ditionality, empathy, and congruence on the Barrett-Lennard Relationship
Inventory correlated .72 with clients’ self-reports of the working alliance,
pointing to the possibility of conceptual overlap. Nevertheless, client ratings
of the four relationship conditions were predictive of treatment outcome on
a wide range of outcome measures. The impact of the relationship condi-
tions on outcome was mediated by therapeutic alliance for three of four out-
come measures. Researchers thus concluded that the relationship conditions
appeared to be instrumental in facilitating the formation of the therapeutic
The case of Brad explicates how research has been applied to practice
and further illustrates how EFT is conducted. Brad is a semiretired 64-year-old
professor, married for a second time, with five children. One of the children
is from his current marriage, two are stepchildren in his current marriage,
and two are children from a previous marriage. He wants to address his worry
about his health and his future, along with his depression over his perceived
lack of accomplishment. He also feels unloved in his marriage. He was not
able to make his first marriage work and sees his current marriage crumbling.
He is unhappy and lonely and considers leaving on a regular basis. He and his
wife have tried couples therapy and found it somewhat helpful, but she lost
interest in the process. He feels he has failed in matters of love.
CONCLUSION
EFT has grown primarily out of two major humanistic therapy tradi-
tions: client-centered and Gestalt therapies. Evolving from its theoretical
roots, integrating emotion and constructivist theories, EFT has emerged as
a therapy in its own right in the past 30 years. Research has greatly aided
the development and specification of EFT. Outcome research has helped to
establish EFT in the world of evidence-based treatments for specific disorders.
Recent studies have clearly established support for EFT in the treatment of
REFERENCES
Adams, K. E., & Greenberg, L. S. (1996, June). Therapists’ influence on depressed clients’
therapeutic experiencing and outcome. Paper presented at the 43rd annual conven-
tion for the Society for Psychotherapy Research, St. Amelia Island, FL.
Angus, L., & Greenberg, L. (2011). Working with narrative in emotion-focused therapy:
Changing stories, healing lives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
tion. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12325-000
Auszra, L., & Greenberg, L. (2008). Client emotional productivity. European Psycho-
therapy, 7, 139–152.
Barrett-Lennard, G. (1962). Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in
therapeutic change. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(43),
1–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093918
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the
Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology
Review, 8, 77–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5
Boritz, T. Z., Angus, L., Monette, G., Hollis-Walker, L., & Warwar, S. (2011). Nar-
rative and emotion integration in psychotherapy: Investigating the relationship
between autobiographical memory specificity and expressed emotional arousal in
brief emotion-focused and client-centred treatments of depression. Psychotherapy
Research, 21, 16–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2010.504240
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-012
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
353
(FFT), emotion-focused family therapy (EFFT), and dyadic developmental
psychotherapy (DDP).
Research on EFT for couples has proliferated over the past 15 years.
A multitude of studies focusing on the efficacy of using this approach with
diverse populations has been conducted by Johnson and her colleagues. Case
studies have also been published demonstrating the application of the model
for specific needs (e.g., sexuality; Johnson & Zuccarini, 2010).
Dalton, Greenman, Classen, and Johnson (2013) conducted the first
controlled trial for couples in which the female partner had experienced child-
hood abuse. Couples were randomly assigned to 22 sessions of EFT (n = 12)
or to a wait-list (n = 10). Couples in the treatment group experienced a sig-
nificant reduction in relationship distress, whereas couples on the wait-list
did not. However, no significant reductions in trauma symptoms were found
for either group.
Couples experiencing ongoing stress related to having a child with a
chronic illness have also benefitted from EFT, as evidenced by Cloutier et al.’s
(2002) findings. These authors examined changes in marital satisfaction for
13 couples who had received 10 sessions of EFT, finding statistically signifi-
cant improvements on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976) scores
between pre- and posttherapy. An examination of clinical change from pre-
treatment to 2 years following the end of therapy revealed that five couples
moved from the “distressed” to the “nondistressed” range on the Dyadic
Adjustment Scale, three couples maintained their gains, four couples showed
no change, and one couple deteriorated.
EFT for couples is also considered an appropriate treatment interven-
tion for depressed women and their partners. An early pilot study compared
the outcome of 12 couples randomly assigned to 16 sessions of EFT or to anti
depressants (Dessaulles, Johnson, & Denton, 2003). The female partners with
depression in both groups demonstrated significant reductions in depressive
symptoms over the course of treatment; however, only the women who received
EFT continued to improve 6 months following the end of treatment. Denton,
Wittenborn, and Golden (2012) compared EFT in combination with anti-
depressants to medication use only. Both groups showed an improvement in
depressive symptoms; however, only the women receiving EFT reported signif-
icant improvement in relationship quality. These findings were echoed in the
analysis by Denton and colleagues (2012), in which 24 couples were randomly
assigned to either 15 sessions of EFT, in combination with antidepressants, or
6 months of antidepressants alone. Significant changes were observed under
both conditions; however, significant improvements in relationship sat-
isfaction were reported only by couples who received both EFT-C and
antidepressants.
Forgiveness
Resolving EIs—understood as betrayals related to issues of attachment
(e.g., infidelity, abandonment during a time of need) and identity (e.g., per-
ceived humiliation)—has been the focus of extensive clinical research since
2000. For example, Greenberg, Warwar, and Malcolm (2010) developed an
effective EFT intervention for resolving EIs. Twenty couples acting as their
own wait-list controls in a 10- to 12-session treatment fared significantly better
compared with the wait-list period on measures of relational satisfaction, trust,
and forgiveness, as well as on global symptoms and target complaints. These
changes were maintained for up to 3 months following therapy, with the excep-
tion of trust, which declined over time. At the end of treatment, 11 couples
were identified as having completely forgiven their partners, and six couples
had made progress toward forgiveness. No one on the wait-list reported hav-
ing completely forgiven, and only three injured partners indicated partial
forgiveness.
Although still in its infancy, existing research in EFFT suggests that this
model is appropriate for working with families experiencing severe clinical
issues. For example, Efron (2004) applied the EFFT model to working with
1
From Relationship Enhancement® Family Therapy: Experiential Format (P-Family) [DVD], by B. G. Guerney, Jr.
(therapist), 1991, Silver Spring, MD: IDEALS, Inc. Copyright 1991 by IDEALS, Inc. Adapted with
permission.
Johnson (1996, 2004) organized the 1988 EFT model into three stages:
(a) cycle deescalation, (b) restructuring of interactions, and (c) integra-
tion and consolidation. As noted previously, Johnson focused on working
with attachment (i.e., closeness) in her approach to EFT. In contrast,
Greenberg and Goldman (2008) expanded on the original EFT framework
by proposing a five-stage treatment model that addresses the dimensions of
attachment and identity (validation; self-worth). In brief, Greenberg and
Goldman’s approach to EFT includes the following stages: (a) validating each
partner’s current position and forming an alliance, before working on (b) neg-
ative cycle deescalation. This is followed by (c) accessing underlying vulner-
able feelings, and (d) restructuring the negative interaction and the self, where
the emphasis is on acceptance of expressed vulnerability and exploration of
CONCLUSION
Accordino, M. P., & Guerney, B. G., Jr. (2002). The empirical validation of relation-
ship enhancement couple and family therapies. In D. J. Cain & J. Seeman (Eds.),
Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice (pp. 403–442).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10439-013
Axline, V. M. (1947). Play therapy. Cambridge, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Becker-Weidman, A. (2006). Dyadic developmental psychotherapy: A multi-
year follow-up. In S. M. Sturt (Ed.), New developments in child abuse research
(pp. 43–60). New York, NY: Nova Science.
Becker-Weidman, A., & Hughes, D. (2008). Dyadic developmental psychotherapy:
An evidence-based treatment for children with complex trauma and disorders of
attachment. Child & Family Social Work, 13, 329–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
j.1365-2206.2008.00557.x
Benjamin, L. S. (1974). Structural analysis of social behavior. Psychological Review,
81, 392–425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0037024
Bradley, B., & Furrow, J. L. (2004). Toward a mini-theory of the blamer softening
event: Tracking the moment-by-moment process. Journal of Marital and Family
Therapy, 30, 233–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2004.tb01236.x
Bratton, S. C., Ray, D., Rhine, T., & Jones, L. (2005). The efficacy of play therapy
with children: A meta-analytic review of treatment outcomes. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 36, 376–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-
7028.36.4.376
Brock, G. W., & Joanning, H. (1983). A comparison of the Relationship Enhancement
Program and the Minnesota Couple Communication Program. Journal of Mari-
tal and Family Therapy, 9, 413–421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.1983.
tb01530.x
Buehlman, K. T., Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1992). How a couple views their past
predicts their future: Predicting divorce from an oral history interview. Journal of
Family Psychology, 5, 295–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.5.3-4.295
Cloutier, P. F., Manion, I. G., Walker, J. G., & Johnson, S. M. (2002). Emotion-
ally focused interventions for couples with chronically ill children: A 2-year
follow-up. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 28, 391–398. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2002.tb00364.x
Dalton, J., Greenman, P., Classen, C., & Johnson, S. M. (2013). Nurturing connec-
tions in the aftermath of childhood trauma: A randomized controlled trial of
emotionally focused couple therapy for female survivors of childhood abuse.
Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice, 2, 209–221. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/a0032772
Denton, W. H., Wittenborn, A. K., & Golden, R. N. (2012). Augmenting antide-
pressant medication treatment of depressed women with emotionally focused
therapy for couples: A randomized pilot study. Journal of Marital and Family Ther-
apy, 38(Suppl. 1), 23–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2012.00291.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-013
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
387
relationship, and they defined the therapist’s experience of relational depth
as follows:
A feeling of profound contact and engagement with a client, in which
one simultaneously experiences high and consistent levels of empathy
and acceptance toward that Other, and relates to them in a highly trans-
parent way. In this relationship, the client is experienced as acknowl-
edging one’s empathy, acceptance and congruence—either implicitly or
explicitly—and is experienced as fully congruent in that moment. (p. 36)
Mearns and Cooper (2012) further proposed that this relational depth
is constructive to all forms of therapy and is related to more meaningful and
successful outcomes. In child psychotherapy, the need for relational depth
as well as the demonstration of high levels of empathic understanding and
unconditional positive regard through genuine presentation of self are often
dismissed (Ray, 2011) or viewed as unessential when working with children
(Wilson & Ryan, 2005). Although obvious, many therapists may need to
be reminded that children are people who respond to relational variables as
people of various ages do. Just as adults benefit from deep levels of relational
contact to work through their most troubling issues, children require the same
quality of contact.
The most observable difference between humanistic work with children
and work with adults involves the nature, intensity, and quantity of verbal
contact. Children use play as their language (Landreth, 2012), drawing on
toys, artwork, and scenes as a way to express their internal and external expe-
riences. The therapist’s ability to provide an environment facilitating full and
free expression of those experiences and the child’s ability to accept that envi-
ronment is the core of the therapeutic relationship. The self of the therapist,
which is central to the therapeutic environment, requires provision of open-
ness to relational depth with the child. Because children are individuals, some
may respond to a therapist’s provision of empathic understanding, uncondi-
tional positive regard, and genuineness in a verbal way, but the majority of
children respond nonverbally through play, touch, physical proximity, and
uninhibited expression of internal experiences through play materials. Hence,
the humanistic child therapist offers self, as well as an environment, in a way
that provides multiple materials for expression, space to move, and freedom
to express at a self-directed pace.
To provide an environment that is conducive to relational depth with
a child, humanistic therapists are apt to incorporate expressive materials into
their interventions. The most common approach to humanistic interven-
tion with children is play therapy. Anna Freud (1946) first acknowledged
the importance of the therapist–child relationship within play therapy by cit-
ing the need for the child to establish an attachment to the therapist before
analysis of nondirective play could be successful. Virginia Axline (1947), a
Externalizing/Disruptive Behaviors
Internalizing
Academic/Language
Relationships
Self-Concept/Sense of Competency
CASE EXAMPLE
The case of Rachel, who was 6 years old when she was referred for play
therapy, is provided to demonstrate how principles are concretely enacted
in CCPT. Rachel’s mother had been absent from her life since Rachel was
2 years of age because of alcohol addiction and domestic violence. Before
being separated from her mother, Rachel witnessed her mother being physi-
cally aggressive toward her father and being arrested and escorted to jail by
police officers. At the time Rachel began play therapy, she lived with her
father and paternal grandparents. Her father worked and attended school full-
time. Rachel frequently wet the bed at night and had intense nightmares sev-
eral times a week. She feared physical separation from her father, continuously
asked about her mother’s absence, and was difficult to soothe. Her father and
teachers reported that Rachel was very obedient and helpful at home and at
school, but was overly concerned about making mistakes and being punished.
Sessions were held in a playroom in a clinic setting. The playroom,
which included hundreds of toys on shelves, was organized according to
nurturing, aggressive, real-life, and expressive categories. The first session,
Rachel was nervous about coming to the playroom with the therapist and
concerned about separating from her father. She clung tightly to him.
Therapist: Rachel, you feel scared to come to the playroom. You’re
nervous about leaving your dad.
Rachel: [clinging tighter to dad and facing away from therapist]
Therapist: [bending down to be at the same height as the child] You’re not
too sure about all this. You would rather stay with your dad.
[Rachel’s father reassured her that she would be okay and that he
would be waiting for her after the play session.]
Rachel: [looking back and forth between her dad and the playroom]
Therapist: You’re scared, but you think maybe it would be okay since
your dad said he’ll be waiting for you.
[Rachel let go of her dad’s hand and walked into the playroom.]
The play therapist reflected Rachel’s feelings, acknowledging her desire
to stay with her father, to communicate understanding and acceptance of
Rachel’s experience. The play therapist aimed to support sensitively Rachel’s
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-014
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
421
therapists actually contribute to client improvement. Then, the particular
attributes of therapists that account for their helpfulness are examined. Note
that in this chapter, we deal with the effects of the personal and professional
qualities of therapists rather than with therapists’ techniques. Although
special attention is given to the therapist’s contribution to outcome in the
humanistic psychotherapies, in this review we examine the currents that run
throughout the entire research literature that have bearing on the person of
the therapist in therapy.
Throughout the chapter, the focus is on major reviews and meta-analyses
whenever possible, as the sheer number of studies bearing on therapist factors
precludes giving attention to individual studies unless they are of landmark
status. In sum, an effort is made to provide the reader with the best available
research evidence pertaining to therapist factors that contribute to effective
psychotherapy.
Therapists are generally effective, and some are more effective than others.
However, what about them is effective in helping clients make treatment gains?
Rogers’s (1957) articulation of the therapist’s facilitative conditions—empathy,
genuineness, and unconditional positive regard—have become the bedrock of
the humanistic psychotherapies and have become so widely accepted that they
have been assimilated into the culture of psychotherapy in general.
In their review of therapist relational variables in the first edition of this
volume, Asay and Lambert (2002) concluded that by the year 2000, “it would
probably be safe to say that there is general agreement between research-
ers and clinicians alike that therapist facilitative skills are essential to the
formation of positive therapeutic relationships and contribute significantly
to therapeutic outcome” (p. 537). Among the key findings of their review
is the conclusion that client ratings of therapist variables are more predic-
tive of outcome than clinician and objective raters. Asay and Lambert noted
that there are still unresolved issues regarding how the facilitative conditions
are defined, measured, and distinguished from one another because empathy,
positive regard, and congruence are highly correlated. Therefore, in their
review, they did not report the findings for each of the facilitative conditions
separately.
Other reviewers have maintained the distinction between the facil
itative conditions. For instance, a series of reviews commissioned by the
American Psychological Association Division of Psychotherapy Task Force
on Empirically Supported Therapy Relationships (Norcross, 2002) examined
the literature before 2000 on a number of relational variables. Those reviews
pertaining to each of the facilitative conditions are briefly described later. The
review of therapist empathy is done in more detail to illustrate the methods
used in meta-analyses that have become a standard for major reviews in recent
years. Prior to the advent of meta-analyses, reviewers used their judgment to
combine the results across studies. Different reviewers often arrived at dif-
ferent conclusions for the same group of studies (e.g., Bergin, 1971; Parloff,
Waskow, & Wolfe, 1978; Truax & Mitchell, 1971). To remedy this situation,
some reviewers presented tables indicating the number of studies that sup-
ported the hypothesis being reviewed. This method was used in the reviews
regarding congruence and positive regard that follow. These sorts of reviews
Empathy
Bohart, Elliott, Greenberg, and Watson (2002) conducted a meta-analysis
of 47 studies involving 3,026 clients published between 1971 and 2000 relat-
ing empathy with therapy outcome. For each study, the correlation between
empathy ratings and outcome measures was computed. These coefficients
were combined into an average weighted correlation to take into account
each sample’s size. The overall results across all measures in all studies showed
a medium effect size (r = .32, equivalent to 9% of the outcome variance).
This effect size indicates that empathy has a moderately strong influence
on therapeutic outcome across a variety of measures, therapies, populations
and settings. Bohart et al. found some surprising results from their secondary
analyses. Contrary to expectation, theoretical orientation had no influence
on the importance of empathy for therapeutic outcome. Therapist empa-
thy is important for both humanistic and nonhumanistic therapies (e.g.,
cognitive behavior therapy [CBT]). Additionally, it was found that experi-
ence level moderated the empathy–outcome relationship, with empathy
being more important to outcome for less experienced therapists relative
to more experienced therapists. Bohart et al. surmised that more experi-
enced therapists might have a wider variety of resources available to help
clients, and hence empathy alone would not be of such central importance
for them.
Positive Regard
There have been two large systematic reviews of the relationship between
positive regard and client outcome in the literature prior to 2000. Orlinsky,
Grawe, and Parks (1994) reviewed 25 studies published between 1979 and
1991, and Farber and Lane (2002) reviewed eight studies published between
1992 and 2000 that were not included in Orlinsky et al.’s review. Both
studies reported the results in terms of box score. Overall, 55% of the results
were positive, 42% were nonsignificant, and 2% were opposite to prediction.
Orlinsky et al. suggested that the variability in the relationship between posi-
tive regard and outcome is due to variability in research conditions such as
the different measurers of positive regard. Farber and Lane noted that signifi-
cant results were much more likely when findings were based on the evalua-
tions of clients as opposed to objective raters.
Empathy
In the most recent meta-analysis of empathy, Elliott, Bohart, Watson, and
Greenberg (2011) examined 59 studies with 224 separate effects (studies with
more than one measure of empathy have more than one effect). The average
correlation between therapist’s empathy and client improvement was .30, a
moderate effect size. Elliott et al. noted that there was significant variability
among studies indicating that other variables were moderating the empathy–
outcome relationship. Although therapist’s empathy plays an important role in
general, it has a significantly stronger role with clients who have more severe
problems and a weaker role for therapists with more experience.
Recent research has gone beyond demonstrating that empathy is a crit
ical predictor of client outcome and has examined how empathy influences
client change. The experience of an empathic relational bond influences
client expectancies for change and leads to enhanced motivation for engage-
ment (Westra, 2004). Therapists’ affective attunement helps clients develop
a better relationship with their own feelings (Elliott, Watson, Goldman, &
Greenberg, 2004), and that, in turn, reduces depression, improves self-esteem,
and improves coping ability (Watson, McMullen, Prosser, & Bedard, 2011).
Furthermore, therapist empathic validation of client narratives helps clients
make meaning out of their experiences (Hardtke & Angus, 2004; Kagan,
2007); can help to facilitate insight (Angus & Hardtke, 2007; Castonguay &
Hill, 2007); and helps clients achieve newer, more positive views of them-
selves (Goldfried, 2003; Kagan, 2007). Additionally, therapists’ empathic
understanding enhances the working alliance, and their active empathic
attunement to fluctuations in the therapeutic bond functions as an early
warning system for the detection and repair of strains in the alliance (Safran
& Muran, 2000).
Kolden, Klein, Wang, and Austin (2011) found a moderate effect between
therapist genuineness and client improvement in their meta-analysis of 16 stud-
ies. Most of the studies were conducted prior to 2000. The average effect size
in the three post-2000 studies was large (r = .37). Secondary analyses revealed
that congruence is more strongly associated with outcome for more experienced
therapists, for therapists who practice relational therapy, and for clients who
are less educated. Minority status and gender of the therapist did not moderate
the congruence–outcome relationship. However Kolden et al. surmised that a
congruent therapist is probably best suited for clients with a Western cultural
background, though they have no data in this regard. Certainly this is one
of many areas pertaining to therapist characteristics in which multicultural
research is needed.
In a qualitative study relating genuineness to perceived effectiveness of
therapy, Curtis, Field, Knaan-Kostman, and Mannix (2004) asked 75 psycho
analysts to reflect on their own experience as analysands and to describe
the most helpful part of their own analysis. Genuineness was described
Therapist Presence
The therapist’s behavior in the preceding vignette also illustrates thera-
pist presence. Perhaps no concept seems to express the essence of the human-
istic therapists’ contribution to therapy more than the concept of therapeutic
presence (Geller & Greenberg, 2012). Therapeutic presence is more than
facilitative conditions. According to Geller and Greenberg (2012), it involves
contact with self (i.e., checking in inwardly with what is being experi-
enced), deep contact with the client (i.e., felt experience of being with
and for the other, attuned deeply to the other’s present moment experi-
ence), and contact with a deeper sense of intuition, spirituality, or a
transcendental force. (p. 140)
Research on presence suggests it can be reliably measured and is mean-
ingful related to therapeutic process and outcome. Guided by a qualitative
study based on interviews of seven master therapists who have articulated
a conceptualization of presence, Geller, Greenberg, and Watson (2010)
used scales to rate therapist presence (a client and therapist version). These
scales were used as part of two randomized controlled trials that compared
process experiential person-centered and cognitive behavioral therapies.
Client improvement was associated with client rating of therapist presence.
Furthermore, clients rated the therapeutic alliance as stronger in sessions in
which they experienced the therapist as more present. Thus, presence seems
to be an important predictor of therapeutic alliance. However, therapist per-
ceptions of their own presence were not related to clients’ ratings of outcome
or alliance. This is a finding that is consistent with other findings that client
Managing Countertransference
In early work with addressing therapists’ management of their CT, Van
Wagoner, Gelso, Hayes, and Diemer (1991) developed the Countertransference
Factors Inventory (CFI). The CFI consisted of five factors: self-awareness, anxi-
ety management, empathy, self-integration, and conceptual skills.
In a survey asking 122 therapists to nominate colleagues whom they
considered to be excellent therapists, those who were deemed as excel-
lent were rated higher on all five of the CFI scales relative to the ratings of
therapists in general. With the exception of CT conceptual skills in which
humanistic therapists were rated relatively low compared to psychodynamic
clinicians, none of the other CT management characteristics differed as a
function of theoretical orientation (i.e., humanistic, behavioral, or psycho-
dynamic). Humanistic therapists’ low level of conceptual skills with manag-
ing CT is not surprising in light of humanistic psychology’s failure to develop
a theoretical framework regarding CT until recently.
Hayes et al.’s (2011) meta-analysis found that CT, as most often mea-
sured by ratings of therapist avoidance of client issues, had a significant but
small negative effect (r = -.16) on outcome in 10 studies. However, CT
management, most often measured by the CFI, had a significant, large, and
positive effect (r = .56) on outcome in seven studies. Thus, this literature
indicates that CT issues and their management have a significant effect on
client outcome. Of note, therapists’ self-ratings of their CT management had
a much smaller relationship to outcome (r = .18) than therapist CT ratings
by others (e.g., supervisors; r = .62). This echoes findings regarding therapists’
CONCLUDING REMARKS
REFERENCES
Ain, S. (2008). Chipping away at the blank screen: Therapist self-disclosure and the real
relationship (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Maryland, College Park.
Angus, L., & Hardtke, K. (2007). Margaret’s story: An intensive case analysis of insight
and narrative process change in client-centered therapy. In L. G. Castonguay &
C. Hill (Eds.), Insight in psychotherapy (pp. 187–205). http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
11532-009
Asay, T. P., & Lambert, M. J. (2002). Therapist relational variables. In D. J. Cain &
J. Seeman (Eds.), Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice
(pp. 531–557). http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10439-017
Baldwin, S. A., & Imel, Z. E. (2013). Therapist effects: Findings and methods. In
M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (pp. 258–297). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Baldwin, S. A., Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2007). Untangling the alliance–
outcome correlation: Exploring the relative importance of therapist and patient
variability in the alliance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 842–852.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.842
Bergin, A. E. (1971). The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.),
Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 217–270).
New York, NY: Wiley.
Beutler, L. E., Harwood, T. M., Kimpara, S., Verdirame, D., & Blau, K. (2011). Cop-
ing style. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-015
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan, and
S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
455
determinant of outcome” (p. 361). In their review of research related to cli-
ent variables, Bohart and Greaves Wade (2013) described the client “as an
active learner and problem solver who contributes to the therapy process and
outcome” (p. 219). However, researchers have come to appreciate the fact that
studying discrete client variables is important and yet a challenge. There is an
inherent difficulty in isolating a single client variable from a relational pro-
cess in which multiple variables contribute to the therapy outcome. Clients
do not live in a vacuum. Anyone can stand alone in the middle of a field and
declare oneself to be a great person. The statement only has meaning when
all that composes an individual interacts with the environment, the com-
plexities of people, and the various situations one encounters. Despite these
and other limitations involved in conducting research on client variables,
there are a number of informative studies that can assist us in understanding
and appreciating client variables related to psychotherapy outcome.
In searching for information about client variables and psychotherapy
outcome, some of the more recent studies have extended and confirmed prior
research. Furthermore, some research has introduced a modification in think-
ing about how best to study the effectiveness of psychotherapy. Some recent
studies have attempted to ascertain more specific information from the cli-
ents themselves, not only about what they perceived as helpful but also how
the change came about. What follows is a review of the major studies on
client variables and outcome prior to the year 2000, followed by a look at the
research done in this area since the year 2000.
CLIENT PARTICIPATION/ENGAGEMENT/INVOLVEMENT
Therapeutic Alliance
In a 1982 study, Moras and Strupp concluded that the level of inter-
personal relations prior to beginning therapy predicted clients’ level of col-
laborative, positively toned participation in a therapeutic relationship. Filak,
Abeles, and Norquist (1986) investigated whether clients’ interpersonal atti-
tudes prior to beginning therapy related to an affiliation–hostility dimension
would have a significant impact on therapy outcome. Of those with an affilia-
tive stance, 72% had a highly successful outcome, whereas only 38% of those
with a pretherapy hostile interpersonal stance had a successful outcome.
Orlinsky et al. (1994) summarized 55 findings related to the client’s contri-
bution to the therapeutic alliance and found significant relationship in 67%
of the cases. The client’s positive contribution to the therapeutic alliance
was associated with good outcome for therapy lasting 20 to 40 sessions but not
for the short term (i.e., fewer than 20 sessions). They also reported a positive
association between outcome and the client’s total affective response (both
negative and positive) in 50% of 10 findings (not differentiating between
positive and negative affects). When just positive affective responses were
considered, all nine findings in three relevant studies showed significant
associations with favorable outcomes. In other words, when clients respond
with positive feelings during sessions, it is likely an indication that therapy
is proceeding well.
Orlinsky et al. (1994) summarized 59 studies and found that client affir-
mation of the therapist had a more consistent association with outcome than
did therapist affirmation of the client (69% vs. 56%). They noted, though,
that client affirmation may be a result of therapeutic progress rather than a
precipitant. Logically, a reciprocal affirmation between client and therapist
should follow. In 78% of 32 findings, reciprocal affirmations were significantly
positive (figures derived primarily from the clients’ or external raters’ process
perspectives). Furthermore, Beutler, Crago, and Arizmendi (1986) found a
positive correlation between outcome and clients’ positive perceptions of
their therapists’ facilitative attitudes (empathy, genuineness, congruence,
nonpossessive warmth, and unconditional positive regard).
EXPERIENCING
Expressiveness
Since the year 2000, a good deal of research on client variables and out-
come has been conducted. One significant change in studying psychotherapy
outcomes has been the attempt to delve more deeply into what clients say
about the process. Looking at therapy from the client’s perspective consti-
tutes a paradigm shift in the field of psychotherapy research. Historically,
research has been more inclined to examine various therapist interventions
and how clients reacted in turn. Actually seeking in-depth data from clients
as to their perceptions about what was helpful or not is not commonplace in
prior research. Rennie (2002) did review a few studies done in the 1980s and
1990s that utilized qualitative methodology, but for the most part, researchers
have not sought client commentary about process and outcome until more
recently. In retrospect, it seems strange that researchers have not delved more
into client factors from the client’s perspective. There seems to have been a
kind of distrust of client input, perhaps reflective of the thinking that clients
would not be accurate in their descriptions because of transference issues or
not speaking the language of therapists. In fact, the term treatment carries
the implication of a client coming in for treatment to be administered by an
Client Perspectives
The second question attempted to gain information about how the change
came about. Specifically, Heatherington et al. (2012) asked clients, “If yes,
what do you believe took place during or between your therapy sessions that
led to such change (or changes)?” (pp. 166–167). One of the categories that
emerged as deeply informative was new experiential awareness. Heatherington
et al. described findings in this category as follows:
new experiential awareness . . . that a problems does exist (e.g., “I feel
resentment and anger that I was not aware of before”); new awareness of
patterns in interpersonal awareness (e.g., “I never really knew how much
anger scares me and reminds me of people who have hurt me in the past.
That is probably why I just clam up when I am angry. I’d rather make
excuses for other people’s bad behavior than let myself get angry”); and
new awareness of emotions (e.g., “I am more aware of my detachment in
everyday living”). (p. 166)
Further, Heatherington et al. (2012) noted that the shift involved an
uncovering of experience or feelings that clients typically described as having
occurred gradually; client quotes included such statements as “I have become
more aware of how often I blame myself for things and let people walk all over
me” (p. 182). New experiential awareness was the most frequently identified
corrective experience, accounting for approximately 30% to 40% of all mean-
ing units in four of the five samples analyzed. Heatherington et al. interpreted
this to mean than an association with a positive outcome occurred when
clients made “the shift from a state of not knowing to knowing something
Depth of Experiencing
Support for the importance of having a positive view of the self can
be found in a study by Halvorsen and Monsen (2007), who studied the pre-
treatment self-image of 233 patients using structural analysis of social behavior.
The patients were classified into four self-image groups (self-attack, self-control,
intermediate attack-control, and self-love). The more disturbed clients with the
self-attacking pattern needed longer treatment (M = 39 sessions) to overcome
their self-devaluating behaviors. The authors contended that the rigid forms of
self-hostility need numerous repetitions of corrective experiences before clients
can move toward more self-acceptance and a more positive view of the self.
There was a change toward a healthier self-image in the overall sample. In their
study, Halvorsen and Monsen found that the group with the largest change was
the hostile, self-attacking group.
Motivation
Resistance
Educating clients about the nature of therapy, the role of the therapist,
and the client’s role may be more important and useful than previously real-
ized (Swift & Greenberg, 2015). Most training programs and therapists likely
include a brief description of the nature of therapy and the expectations for
each. Indications are that this process should be a more considered one.
Constantino, Glass, Arnkoff, Ametrano, and Smith (2011) conducted
a meta-analysis of 46 studies that looked at the relationship between clients’
treatment expectations and outcome. Their analyses found a small but sig-
nificant effect size (r = .12) between expectations and outcome. Their review
included suggestions for assessing and modifying client expectations in order
to enhance treatment success.
In one illustrative study, Patterson, Uhlin, and Anderson (2008) exam-
ined the pretreatment expectations of 57 clients to determine the relation-
ship between expectations and the strength of the therapeutic alliance.
This study noted the importance of incorporating a more thorough assess-
ment of clients’ expectations at the beginning of therapy. One measure
utilized was the Expectations About Counseling–Brief Form (Tinsley &
Westcot, 1990). This measure included the Personal Commitment Factor,
which looked at clients’ expectations regarding attending counseling more
than a few sessions, the open expression of feelings in session, willingness
to take responsibility for therapy, and commitment to the work of ther-
apy. Following the third session, clients completed the Working Alliance
Inventory (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989), because alliance ratings from
Collaboration
Attachment Style
Client Involvement
Example:
Client: I wish I could figure out why I don’t stand up to my father.
I get so angry with myself for letting him treat me poorly. I
keep telling myself that I need to stand up for myself. I just
can’t do it. I can’t figure it out. I just don’t get it. Why do you
think I won’t stand up to my father?
Therapist: It sounds like you have thought about this a great deal. I
wonder what you have come up with so far.
New Awareness
REFERENCES
Bergin, A. E., & Garfield, S. L. (1994). Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change
(4th ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
Beutler, L. E., Crago, M., & Arizmendi, T. G. (1986). Therapist variables in psycho-
therapy process and outcome. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Hand-
book of psychotherapy and behavior change (3rd ed., pp. 257–310). New York, NY:
Wiley.
Beutler, L. E., Harwood, T. M., Michelson, A., Song, X., & Holman, J. (2011).
Reactance/resistance level. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships
that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp. 261–278). http://dx.doi.
org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199737208.003.0013
Binder, P. E., Holgersen, H., & Nielsen, G. H. (2010). What is a “good outcome” in
psychotherapy? A qualitative exploration of former patients’ point of view. Psycho
therapy Research, 20, 285–294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503300903376338
Bohart, A. C., & Greaves Wade, A. (2013). The client in psychotherapy. In M. J.
Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change
(6th ed., pp. 219–257). New York, NY: Wiley.
Bohart, A. C., & Tallman, K. (1999). How clients make therapy work: The process of
active self-healing. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10323-000
Butler, J. M., Rice, L. N., & Wagstaff, A. K. (1962). On the naturalistic definition of
variables: An analogue of clinical analysis. In H. Strupp & L. Luborsky (Eds.),
Research in psychotherapy (Vol. 2, pp. 178–205). http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
10591-010
Combs, A. W. (1989). A theory of therapy: Guidelines for counseling practice. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Combs, A. W., & Gonzalez, D. M. (1994). Helping relationships: Basic concepts for the
helping professions (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Constantino, M., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., Ametrano, R. M., & Smith, J. Z.
(2011). Expectations. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work:
Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp. 354–376). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
acprof:oso/9780199737208.003.0018
Filak, J., Abeles, N., & Norquist, S. (1986). Clients’ pretherapy interpersonal attitudes
and psychotherapy outcome. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17,
217–222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.17.3.217
Fitzpatrick, M. R., Janzen, J., Chamodraka, M., & Park, J. (2006). Client criti-
cal incidents in the process of early alliance development: A positive emo-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14775-016
Humanistic Psychotherapies: Handbook of Research and Practice, Second Edition, D. J. Cain, K. Keenan,
and S. Rubin (Editors)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
485
The model proposed here builds on and expands an earlier synthesis of
mine (Cain, 2010) and another articulated by Bohart, O’Hara, and Leitner
(2004). The new synthesis attempts to bring together the best of what we know
so far, although it is still a tentative statement that I hope will be developed
further over time. My goal here is to identify the core premises of an optimal
practice of humanistic psychotherapy that provides guidelines for practice.
The proposed integration identifies the major humanistic variables that
affect the effective processes and outcome of humanistic psychotherapies. The
review covers research on humanistic psychotherapies over the past 75 years,
with an emphasis on those bodies of research that have been most compelling
over time. The proposed premises interweave therapist and client variables,
interactive variables, and guidelines regarding on what therapists should focus
to maximize the effects of therapy. It is hoped that the proposed model will
have wider applications in the larger field of psychotherapy, especially because
it has moved increasingly toward integrative models of practice.
Experienced Distress
Research
The dropout rate for clients varies from about 20% to as high as 67%
(e.g., Bohart & Wade, 2013; Clarkin & Levy, 2004), and the modal number
of sessions attended by clients is one (Miller, Duncan, & Hubble, 1997). In a
recent review of the literature on early termination, among the factors identi-
fied by Bohart and Wade (2013) were low motivation, an expectation that
therapy would be helpful more rapidly than it was, and that clients made less
progress than anticipated and felt disappointment in their therapists or with
the alliance. Perhaps most important, “unilateral terminators were more likely
to see their distress as lower when they terminated while their therapists were
more likely to see them as unchanged” (Bohart & Wade, 2013, p. 223).
Research
Presence Research
In recent years, evidence has been generated regarding the impact and
effectiveness of therapist presence. In an unpublished qualitative study of pres-
ence, Pemberton (1977, as cited in Geller & Greenberg, 2012) concluded that
therapists who had high levels of this quality also had awareness, acceptance,
and appreciation of who they were in relationships, were attuned to the present
moment (as opposed to focusing on the past or future), were accepting of imme-
diate experience, and were transparent and personal in their manner of sharing.
Therapists with strong presence were committed both to being themselves and
to being with their clients. They were focused, receptive to their clients’ selves,
and experiencing in an empathic and accepting manner. Other elements of
therapist presence included a sense of aliveness and integration that resulted
in the therapist being centered, authentic, clear, autonomous, and purposeful.
Fraelich (1989, as cited in Geller & Greenberg, 2012), in another
unpublished study, interviewed six therapists about the concept of presence
in a phenomenological study. He identified four themes: (a) immersion in the
moment, (b) spontaneous presence, (c) openness, and (d) living on the cut-
ting edge. Fraelich defined presence as “an intense and richly lived moment”
(Geller & Greenberg, 2012, p. 40), speculating that it contributed to a good
Being Engaged
Engagement Research
BEING EMPATHIC
Attuned Listening
First and primary, empathy creates a sanctuary that provides safety for
clients to be and explore themselves. Empathy is inherently nonjudgmental
because it addresses what is, not what should be. When heard without evalu-
ation, clients feel safe to disclose troublesome aspects of themselves openly
and nondefensively. Feeling understood also helps clients develop a close and
trusting bond with their therapists. When clients feel seen and heard, they
often experience mutual warmth and liking, feeling respected and valued,
and a sense of “we-ness” as they engage with their therapist.
Research
Research
Premise 5: The therapist and client are mutually accepting and affirming.
When clients come to see a new therapist, one of the main things they
hope for is that they will like and feel comfortable with that person. When
therapists and clients like each other, therapy is apt to proceed more naturally
and effectively. Conversely, clients do not work well with therapists they do
not like—nor do therapists work well with clients they do not like—because
the quality of the relationship is apt to be compromised or strained. As com-
mon sense, clinical wisdom, and research have indicated, client liking of
the therapist is a good predictor of outcome. Of course, therapy is likely to
proceed optimally if such feelings are mutual.
This premise expands Rogers’s (1961) belief about the constructive
effects of therapist unconditional positive regard by suggesting that mutual
liking, acceptance, and affirmation are more powerful than just the therapist’s
acceptance of the client.
When the client likes the therapist, and especially when this liking is
mutual, the client is more able to weather difficult moments in therapy and
strains in the relationship with the therapist. The mutual liking and affirma-
tion seem to create a bond that strengthens the therapeutic alliance, which is
also a good predictor of client progress. Because liking begets liking, allowing
oneself to genuinely like and prize one’s clients is almost inevitably therapeutic.
Research
Client affirmation, defined as respect and liking for the therapist, was
positively related to outcome in about 69% of 154 studies, and therapist
Research
RELATIONAL DEPTH
Research
Research
Premise 9: The therapist and the client work in a collaborative and coop-
erative manner.
The therapeutic alliance is strengthened by mutual collaboration and
cooperativeness between therapist and client. When clients and therapists
become partners and collaborate to assess the course of therapy and its effec-
tiveness, the client is likely to benefit by achieving better outcomes. This col-
laboration not only appears to involve the therapist’s ability to communicate
acceptance, warmth, and empathy but also requires the client and therapist to
come to a mutual agreement on the goals of treatment and how those goals will
be reached (Hatcher & Barends, 1996). One of the major changes in enhancing
the quality of psychotherapy and good outcome has come from therapists and
clients engaging in formal and informal assessment designed to review the cli-
ent’s progress periodically (e.g., Duncan, 2010; Lambert & Shimokawa, 2011).
Research
CLIENT INVOLVEMENT
Premise 10: The client is actively involved in and receptive to the thera-
peutic endeavor, participates cooperatively, has a positive expectation
that therapy will be helpful, and takes responsibility for change.
In recent decades, it has become clear that it is primarily the client
who makes therapy work. As veteran researchers Bergin and Garfield (1994)
concluded after reviewing decades of psychotherapy research,
It is the client more than the therapist who implements the change pro-
cess. If the client does not absorb, utilize and follow through on the facili-
tative efforts of the therapist, then nothing happens. Rather than argue
over whether or not “therapy works,” we could address ourselves to the
question of whether or not “the client works”! . . . Clients are not inert
objects upon whom techniques are administered. . . . People are agentic
beings who are effective forces in the complex of causal events. . . . As
therapists have depended more upon the client’s resources, more change
seems to occur. (p. 826)
Research
Research
Premise 12: The client participates in therapy with openness and recep-
tivity and in a nondefensive manner.
Client openness and nondefensiveness are among the very best predic-
tors of good outcome. Client openness represents a willingness to talk about
problems and to disclose and process unpleasant aspects of self and experience.
Conversely, client defensiveness impairs the therapeutic process because the
client avoids dealing with difficult issues, experiences, or disagreeable aspects
of the self. Clients who are open and participate cooperatively in their therapy
are more likely to be motivated to change and are receptive to multiple aspects
of the therapeutic process, including the willingness to look at themselves,
despite the possibility that doing so may be threatening to the self as currently
conceived.
Therapists, of course, contribute to clients’ openness by providing a safe,
trustworthy, and understanding environment in which clients feel accepted
as they are, including their flaws and limitations.
Research
Premise 13a: Clients attend to, express, and process their feelings and emo-
tion schemes with adequate depth to achieve clarity of personal meaning.
Client Expressiveness
Research
Premise 13b: The therapist focuses on and encourages the client to attend
to and process potent emotional experiences, with the intent to facilitate
adaptive client learning and more effective functioning.
A client’s willingness to self-explore and process emotion seems essential
to successful therapy. However, not every client who wants to explore will
necessarily be good at it. Clients’ capacity to access and process their feelings
varies from being almost entirely out of touch with their feelings to being
hypersensitive to them. As noted earlier, clients’ capacity for emotional expe-
riencing is an important element in determining whether or not they are likely
to benefit from therapy. Fortunately, as Gendlin (1996) has noted, clients can
learn to focus and experience their felt sense (the way the body experiences
reality) of problems and situations, and doing so often leads to fresh ways of
looking at self and others. Gendlin (1984) described the felt sense as “the
client inside us, a kind of self-response process” (p. 83). New ways of being and
behaving naturally emerge from seeing with fresh eyes and hearing with fresh
ears. Meaningful and substantive change is visceral, in one’s bones, as opposed
to being primarily cognitive. The fact that experiencing can be learned takes
on greater significance when one understands that clients do not, without
assistance, tend to deepen their experiencing. One way to conceive of client
experiencing is that it represents internal receptiveness and openness, because
it is a manner of listening and relating to oneself that is therapeutic. Clearly,
therapists’ ability to enable their clients to experience more fully is essential
to their intrapersonal and interpersonal learning.
Focusing-Oriented Research
Emotion-Focused Research
Premise 14: Clients develop and operate from an internal locus of control
and evaluation.
Locus of control (internal vs. external) refers to the extent to which peo-
ple believe that they can control events. In general, “internals” believe that
the outcomes of their actions result from their own abilities, and “externals”
attribute outcomes of events to external circumstances. Similarly, Bandura’s
(1997) concept of self-efficacy reflects people’s beliefs that they can succeed
in a particular activity. Locus of evaluation indicates whether people depend
on themselves (internal) or others (external) to make judgments regarding
their beliefs and behavioral choices.
From a humanistic perspective, developing and operating from an inter-
nal locus of control and evaluation is considered central to psychological
health; it is enhanced to the degree to which clients see themselves as cap-
tains of their ships and take responsibility for themselves and their actions.
As Rogers (1961) often stated, his goal was to free his clients to determine
the direction of their therapy and their lives and to empower them to make
Research
Premise 15: The therapist focuses on the self of the client with the inten-
tion to help the client gain clarity about the self, resolve discrepancies
in the self-concept, and increase self-efficacy.
Research
INDIVIDUALIZING PSYCHOTHERAPY
Premise 16: The therapist and client individualize therapy by being col-
laborative partners in the definition of the client’s problems, desired
goals, means to achieve those goals, and the definition and creation of
an optimal therapeutic relationship.
A compelling argument can be made that no one knows the client better
than the client. The belief that clients are the best experts on themselves is
critical for humanistic therapists to work effectively with them. Furthermore,
because each client is unique, clinical wisdom and many prominent therapists
(e.g., Lazarus, 2005) have pointed to the importance of individualizing therapy.
It is the therapist’s responsibility to adapt and accommodate in a manner that
works best for a given client. Therapists’ meta-communication about therapy
is vital in helping them know what fits and works for a specific client. Clients
need and benefit from different things at different times. Therefore, each course
of therapy needs to be cocreated by therapist and client as it evolves to increase
the likelihood of client benefit. Although clients take the lead in deciding what
problems are to be addressed and what their related goals are, therapists and cli-
ents work together in defining and creating an optimal therapeutic relationship
and course of therapy to achieve those goals. In this approach, the therapist is
cautious about defining a priori relational factors that are considered optimal.
If therapists take the role of learners in relationship to their clients, they are
likely to observe and inquire about what is needed or apt to be most fruitful in
specific situations regarding evolving goals and the therapeutic processes and
relational qualities most likely to achieve them.
Research
Premise 17: The client experiences in the therapist the relevant rela-
tional qualities, behaviors, and intents that facilitate change and growth
as defined by the client.
Rogers (1961) helped us understand that, for therapy to be effective, the
core conditions needed to be perceived or experienced by the client to be effec-
tive. Both research and clinical experience have suggested that a large number
of therapist qualities, attitudes, and behaviors lead to constructive therapeutic
change in the client. However, because each client and course of therapy are
unique, it is essential that the therapist be aware that what is important for
each client to benefit optimally will vary. Furthermore, what clients benefit from
most will also vary over the course of therapy and even within sessions. What
matters most to a given client (e.g., feeling supported or encouraged) will vary;
thus, when the specific constellation of therapeutic qualities and behaviors
needed by the client to use therapy effectively are offered and perceived,
then therapeutic effectiveness will be increased. Because therapists are poor
judges of their impact on clients, it seems desirable for therapists to initiate
dialogue regarding what the client is experiencing in the therapy, especially
at moments within sessions or in various phases of therapy when the therapist
senses that something may be problematic.
Research
One of the core premises of Rogers’s (1961) theory and research was
that the client needed to experience the core conditions of empathy, positive
Premise 18: The therapist continuously monitors the quality of the thera-
peutic relationship, client progress, and any strains in the relationship,
collaborating with the client to make any needed or desirable adjustments.
Along with the client’s progress or lack thereof, one important area
to monitor is a strain or rupture in the therapeutic relationship or alliance.
Research from several sources has indicated that clients’ and therapists’ views
of the same therapy are often discrepant, sometimes substantially so. As men-
tioned earlier, studies have shown that therapists are not good judges of their
own behavior and impact on the client, often overestimating their effective-
ness (Bohart & Wade, 2013; Duncan, 2010; Rennie, 2002). Yet the responsi-
bility for monitoring the therapy falls to the therapist. Monitoring should be
an ongoing part of therapy that is done at frequent intervals. Careful atten-
tion to the quality of the therapeutic relationship should be a constant part of
an ongoing dialogue, especially because clients often withhold their feelings
Research
Rennie’s (2002) research showed that clients often did not share their
views of the therapist or therapy. Safran, Muran, and Samstag (1994) noted
that successful therapy was often characterized by a rupture–repair cycle in
the therapeutic alliance that may occur at any time in therapy. However,
it tended to occur when the therapist began to address maladaptive client
patterns. Such ruptures may also occur as a result of therapist criticism of,
indifference to, and dislike of the client. Safran et al.’s findings suggested that
therapists should focus on their clients’ feelings about the therapy, that thera-
pists are crucial in repairing ruptures, and that failure to address such ruptures
will likely lead to increased client negativity, termination, and unsuccessful
outcomes.
Lambert and Shimokawa’s (2011) research provided substantial and
compelling evidence that monitoring the client’s progress and providing
client feedback to therapists improved good outcome while decreasing treat-
ment failure. Shimokawa, Lambert, and Smart (2010) found that rates of
client deterioration could be reduced from a baseline of 20% to 13% when
therapists were informed of client progress status. Such interventions also fur-
ther reduced deterioration rates by 5.5% while doubling positive outcomes.
Although it may take some time, effort, and courage for therapists to assess
how therapy is going from the client’s perspective, such information is vital
in keeping the therapy on track and thriving (see also Chapter 2).
Premise 19: The therapist brings forth for the client’s consideration any
and all personal and professional resources that may be of value to the
client.
Clients want and deserve to have available to them all aspects of the
therapist’s personal qualities, skills, professional knowledge, and resources,
just as one should expect no less from one’s physician. When clients come to
therapy, they typically indicate that they hope the therapist will understand
them, be supportive and caring, help them understand themselves, and deal
effectively with their concerns. They often express a desire to develop some
strategies to help them cope and function better. In some cases, clients do not
Research
Research
In the past few decades, both researchers and practitioners have come to
understand that the primary agent of therapeutic change is the client. When
clients are active and involved participants in therapy, they are likely to
profit from what the therapist provides and the quality of their relationship.
Therefore, therapists need to do whatever they can to increase and maintain
client involvement. Clients who are receptive, open, and nondefensive are
apt to disclose and explore troublesome aspects of their selves and lives. They
are more likely to attend to, express, process, and learn from their emotions,
especially when their therapist is attuned to their emotions and supports and
guides them in doing so. When clients function from an internal locus of
control and evaluation, they are more likely to develop a sense of confi-
dence in themselves and their capacity to use sound judgment and engage in
healthy decision-making processes. As they focus on themselves and various
subselves or voices, they become clearer about and redefine who they are in
more positive and congruent ways, functioning in a more integrated manner.
Better connection and communication with and between clients’ subsystems
result and, consequently, they have more experiences of feeling whole and
efficient in their functioning and in relationships with self and others. Their
sense of self-efficacy also increases.
Clients, of course, engage in and benefit most from therapy when they
are motivated or sufficiently distressed in an area they deem personally rel-
evant, along with being desirous of change. That said, there are times when
clients need therapist assistance in identifying problematic areas, in finding
intrinsic motivation for change, and in becoming aware that some area of
their life is more in need of change than they had previously recognized.
Therapists, of course, play a crucial role in how well clients benefit
from therapy. An enormous and growing body of research has shown that
the quality of the therapist–client relationship serves as a foundation for
change. At times, it is sufficiently powerful to create transformative moments
or significant shifts in the way clients see themselves and others and, con-
sequently, in how they deal with their lives. Perhaps the quality that most
grounds therapy and lends power to a wide range of relational qualities is
presence. When experiencing high levels of presence, therapists are focused
and immersed in their clients, are for them and with them, and are invested
in their progress and well-being. Therapist presence contributes substantially
to the therapist’s and client’s capacity to meet at relational depth and engage
REFERENCES
Abramowitz, C. V., Abramowitz, S. I., Roback, H. B., & Jackson, C. (1974). Differ-
ential effectiveness of directive and nondirective group therapies as a function
of client internal-external control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
42, 849–853. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0037572
Adler, A. (1927). Understanding human nature. Greenwich, CT: Fawcett.
Angus, L., Watson, J. C., Elliott, R., Schneider, K., & Timulak, L. (2015). Human-
istic psychotherapy research 1990–2015: From methodological innovation to
evidence-supported treatment outcomes and beyond, Psychotherapy Research,
25, 330–347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2014.989290
Bachelor, A. (1988). How clients perceive therapist empathy: A content analysis
of “received” empathy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 25,
227–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085337
Baldwin, M. (1987). Interview with Carl Rogers on the use of self in therapy. In
M. Baldwin & V. Satir (Eds.), The use of self in therapy (pp. 45–52). http://dx.doi.
org/10.1300/J287v03n01_06
Baldwin, S. A., & Imel, Z. E. (2013). Therapist effects: Findings and methods. In
M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed.,
pp. 258–297). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Barrett, M. S., & Berman, J. S. (2001). Is psychotherapy more effective when thera-
pists disclose information about themselves? Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 69, 597–603.
Bergin, A. E., & Garfield, S. L. (Eds.). (1994). Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (4th ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
Beutler, L. (1999, June). The differential role of therapist relationship skills and techniques in
effective psychotherapy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International
Society for Psychotherapy Research, Braga, Portugal.
Beutler, L. E., Crago, S. B., & Arizmendi, T. G. (1986). Therapist variables in psycho-
therapy process. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy
and behavior change (3rd ed., pp. 257–310). New York, NY: Wiley.
537
Austin, S. B., 64, 429, 500 Berman, M. I., 236
Auszra, L., 465 Bernecker, S. L., 472
Authenticity, 31–36 Beutler, L. E., 227, 459, 469, 509, 521
defined, 31 Binder, P. E., 463
in existential therapies, 299 Binswanger, L., 14
measurement of, 32 Blanco, P., 395, 399
overview, 498–501 Blatt, S. J., 432
research on, 35–36 Bohart, A. C.
and social functioning, 33–35 and collaboration in therapy, 474, 477
and well-being/mental health, 32–33 and empathy, 116, 123, 424, 428, 494
Authenticity Inventory, 32 and optimal practices of humanistic
Authenticity Scale, 32 therapies, 486, 487, 504,
Autonomous motivation, 468–469 507–509, 517, 518
Autonomy, 12, 31, 191, 506, 507 and person-centered therapy, 187,
Aviram, A., 471 188, 192
Avoidant attachment, 473 and therapy outcomes, 456, 457
Awareness events, 98–99 Bolger, N., 21
Axline, Virginia, 388–389, 399 Bona fide treatments, 54, 199
Bordin, E., 425
Bachelor, A., 121, 516 Boritz, T. Z., 333
Badenoch, B., 404 Bostrom, A., 200
Baggerly, J., 401 Boudewyn, A. C., 200
Bahramabadi, M., 405 Boundary situations, 283
Baldwin, S. A., 427, 434, 503 Boutelle, K. N., 236
Baliousis, M., 32 Bowlby, J., 15, 356
Bandura, A., 513, 515 Bozarth, J. D., 195, 209
Barkham, M., 198 Bradley, B., 366
Barnes, M., 471 Branigan, C., 163
Barrett, M. S., 500–501 Bratton, S., 391–395, 397, 400, 406
Barrett-Lennard, G. T., 116, 119, 121, Breitbart, W., 295, 307
135–136, 191 British school of existential analysis,
Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory, 286, 296–297, 300, 303
63–64, 119, 121, 330 Brown, A. G., 401
Baumeister, R. F., 22 Brown, L. S., 91
Bayat, M., 397, 406 Buber, M., 14, 91, 202, 221, 290, 299, 305
Beauregard, A. M., 469, 506 Bugental, James, 295
Becker-Weidman, A., 369 Burkard, A. W., 468
Bedard, D. L., 154, 332 Burlingame, G., 402
Beebe, J., 460 Bushman, B. J., 156
Behavioral experimentation, 235, 242 Butler, J. M., 460, 509
Behavioral therapy, 150–151. See also Butollo, W., 233, 242
Cognitive–behavioral therapy
Beisser, A., 225 Cain, D. J., 12, 26, 188, 192
Bergen-Cico, D., 235–236 Callahan, J. L., 471, 517
Bergin, A. E., 51, 61, 422, 455, 505 Carryer, J. R., 156, 333
Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of CAS (“Clearing a Space”), 262–264,
Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, 266–269
197, 198, 523 Cassens, J., 460
Berkowitz, L., 155 Castonguay, L. G., 503
Berman, J. S., 121, 500–501 CBT. See Cognitive–behavioral therapy
index 539
Cognitive processing therapy (CPT), 234 Critical consciousness, 90–91
Cognitive psychology, 13 Critical–ideological approaches
Coherence, 323–324 (qualitative research), 88–90, 104
Collaboration, therapist-client, 14, Critical psychiatry, 190
471–472, 504–505 Crits-Christoph, P., 422
Combs, A. W., 476 Crook-Lyon, R. E., 468
Communicative attunement, 428–429 Crow, S. J., 236
Competence, 12 CST (Clinical Support Tool), 68
Competency, 401–402 CT (countertransference), 437–439
Complex trauma, 156–158 Cuijpers, P., 201
Conditioning, operant, 13 Cultural competence, 91
Conflict management skills, 362–363 Curtis, R., 429
Congruence
early research on, 64 Dalgleish, T. L., 367
and empathy, 120 Dalton, J., 364
overview, 425, 429–439, 498–501 Danger, S., 400
in person-centered therapy, 191 Daniel, T., 98
qualitative research on, 95 Daseinsanalysis, 285
Connor, J., 237 Davidson, L., 431
Constantino, M. J., 464, 469, 508 Day, L., 33–34
Constructivism, 323–324 DBT (dialectical behavior therapy),
Constructivist–interpretive qualitative 235, 237
research, 86–88, 104 Deception, 34
Contacting (Gestalt therapies), 220, Deci, E. L., 191
226, 242 Decision making, 4, 148
Contextual therapy, 285 Defensiveness, 459, 508–509
Cooper, M. De la Ronde, C., 34
and existential therapies, 194, 286, Delgato-Romero, E. A., 101
305 Del Re, A. C., 62, 434, 503
and humanistic psychotherapy with Denton, W. H., 364
children, 387, 388 Depression
and person-centered therapy, 192, and anger, 155–156
198, 202 counseling for, 209
and positive regard, 431 and emotional expression, 512
and relational connection, 14 emotion-focused therapy for, 327,
and relational depth, 501 328, 332–333
Cooperation, 504–505 person-centered therapy for, 200
Correia, E., 287, 289–291, 305–306 and self-criticism, 163
Counseling and Psychotherapy: and social support, 16
Newer Concepts in Practice Derkzen, D. M., 469, 506
(Carl Rogers), 186 Description, rule of, 221, 289
Counseling for depression, 209 Desensitization, 227
Countertransference (CT), 437–439 Dessaules, A., 512
Couple and family therapies. See DET (dialogical exposure therapy),
Humanistic couple and family 233–234
therapies Dewey, J., 255
CPT (cognitive processing therapy), 234 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Crago, M., 459 Mental Disorders, 60, 196, 229
Crago, S. B., 509 Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT),
Creaner, M., 64, 95, 335 235, 237
index 541
Emotions in psychotherapy, continued Existential analysis, 286
reflection on, 160–162 Existential analytical psychotherapy, 286
regulation and soothing of, 157–160 Existential experimentalism, 224–226
research on, 30–31 Existential–humanistic therapies,
and self-concealment, 27–28 295–296
and self-disclosure, 27 Existentialism
therapeutic strategies for getting in concepts in, 5
touch with, 151–155 loss of meaning in, 325
Emotion structure, 150 in person-centered therapy, 193–194
Empathic communication, 116 Existential person-centered therapy, 191
Empathic conjecture, 126 Existential–phenomenological therapy,
Empathic doubling responses, 126 286
Empathic evocations, 125 Existential psychotherapies, 283–310
Empathic exploration responses, 125 future directions for research on, 309
Empathic rapport, 428 general practices of, 286–293, 297–300
Empathic refocusing responses, 126 key branches of, 293–297
Empathic resonance, 116, 121 meaning-oriented practices in,
Empathic understanding responses, 125 306–308
Empathy, 115–136 outcome research on, 284, 300–304
affective, 116 prominence of, 6
affect-regulating function of, 135 scope of, 284–286
case study, 129–134 therapeutic relationships in, 305–306
cognitive, 116 EXIST project, 300–302
deconstructive function of, 135 Expectations, client, 470–471
definitions of, 115–116 Experiencing (therapy process), 459–461
early research on, 63–64, 117–118 defined, 255
importance of, 4, 6 in emotion-focused therapy, 331–332,
interpersonal function of, 134–135 511–512
overview, 424, 431–439, 492–495 and expressiveness, 460
perceived, 116 in Gestalt therapies, 220
person, 116, 429 in humanistic couple and family
in person-centered therapy, 190–191 therapies, 362
received, 116 and locus of control, 461
in relationship enhancement measurement of, 153–154
therapy, 355 overview, 459–468, 510–513
review of recent research on, 118–125 with process-guided focusing, 65
self-, 136 Experiencing Scale (EXP), 251, 260–262,
and strengthening of self, 135–136 320, 332, 362
types of therapist responses invoking, Experiential approaches
125–129 in emotion-focused therapy, 320
Empty-chair method, 65, 227, 327, in person-centered therapy, 191, 194
329, 334 Experiential–existential group psycho-
Epistemology, 83 therapy, 286, 296
Epoché, rule of, 221, 289 Experiential–existential therapy,
Epstein, L., 200 302–303
Eubanks-Carter, C., 435 Experiential–humanistic approaches.
Everall, R. D., 494–495 See Emotions in psychotherapy
Existence: A New Dimension in Psychiatry Experiential psychotherapy, 254. See
and Psychology (R. May, E. Angel, Focusing-oriented–experiential
& H. F. Ellenberger), 295 therapy
index 543
Generalized anxiety disorder, 58, 329. Gone, J. P., 89
See also Anxiety disorders Gonzalez, D. M., 476
Genuineness Goodkin, D. E., 200
importance of, 6, 91 Gormley, B., 473
overview, 425, 429–439, 498–501 Gottman, J. M., 357–358
and therapy effectiveness, 429 Gottman, J. S., 358
Gervaize, P. S., 512 Granger, D. A., 390–391
Gestalt therapies, 219–243 Grant, B., 187
chair work in, 193 Grawe, K., 424, 455–456, 491–492
clinical applications of research on, Greaves, A. L., 503
239–242 Greaves Wade, A., 477
conflict in, 173 Greenberg, L. S.
early research on, 226–227 and client variables, 457, 465–466
emotion in, 150 and emotion-focused couples therapy,
existential ideas in, 285 355–357, 365–368, 374
future directions for research on, 239 and emotion-focused family therapy,
Gestalt-consilient research, 235–238 359
Gestalt hybrid research, 233–235 and emotion-focused therapy,
Gestalt-specific research, 228–232 320–321, 331–334
integrative nature of, 220 and emotions in therapy, 148, 150,
intentionality in, 161 153, 155, 156, 164
play therapy, 405 and empathy, 116, 123, 124, 424, 494
prominence of, 6 and Gestalt therapies, 222, 234, 235,
theoretical background, 220–226 237, 238, 241
therapist–client relationship in, 14, and humanistic couple and family
221–222 therapies, 361
Giacomo, D., 461, 514 and humanistic therapy processes
Giblin, P., 360 and outcomes, 51–53, 62, 65
Ginott, Haim, 389 and optimal practices in humanistic
Giordano, M., 395 therapies, 511, 512
Glass, C. R., 469, 508, 521 and person-centered therapy, 197–199
Glass, G. V., 196 and philosophical principles of
Gleason, M. E. J., 21 humanistic therapies, 387
Golden, R. N., 364 and qualitative research, 85, 97
Goldfried, M. R., 503, 521 and therapeutic presence, 433, 491
Goldman, B. M., 32 and therapist factors, 428
Goldman, R. N. Greenman, P., 364
and client variables, 465–466 Grief, 154
and emotion-focused couples therapy, Grote, B., 517
356–357, 374 Grounded theory research
and emotion-focused therapy, 331, 335 contributions from, 102
and focusing-oriented–experiential early, 92–94
therapy, 261 humanistic psychologists’ involvement
and Gestalt therapies, 234 in, 81, 86, 87
and humanistic therapy processes on therapist factors, 431
and outcomes, 62 Group psychotherapy
and optimal practices of humanistic cognitive–existential, 303
psychotherapies, 512 existential–humanistic, 295–296
Goldstein, Kurt, 223 experiential–existential, 286, 296
Gomes-Schwartz, B., 458 meaning-centered, 295
index 545
Imel, Z. E., 427, 434, 503 Kelly, T., 230–231
Individualization of psychotherapy, Kennedy-Moore, E., 29
516–517 Kernis, M. H., 32
Insight, client Kifer, Y., 33
intentionality vs., 161 Ki Leung, T. Y., 230
qualitative research on, 98–99 Kilmann, P., 514
Instrumental emotions, 357 King, L. A., 33
Instrumental nondirectivity, 187 Kirschenbaum, H., 500
Integrative approaches, 5–6, 191–193, Klagsbrun, J., 263, 268
519–522 Klein, M., 460
Integrative person-centered therapy, 191 Klein, M. H., 64, 425, 429, 460, 500
Intentionality, 161 Klein, M. J., 462
Internal coherence, 160 Knaan-Kostman, I., 429
Internal focus, 167 Knez, R., 231
Internalizing disorders, 397–399 Knox, R., 299
International Children’s Focusing Knox, S., 468, 501
Conference, 270 Kohut, H., 116
Interpersonal connection. See Kolden, G. G., 64, 425, 429, 500
Relational connection König, J., 233
Intersubjective theory, 293 Korman, L. M., 511
Intimacy, 14, 34 Koshikawa, Y., 267
Intuitive inquiry, 86 Krebs, P. M., 469, 488
I–Thou concept Krieger, K., 67
and authenticity, 14 Krupnick, J. L., 503
and existential therapies, 290, Kubota, S., 261
299, 305
and Gestalt therapies, 221–222 Laing, R. D., 14, 296, 297
and relational depth theory, 91, 202 Lakey, C. E., 33
Lambert, M. J., 226
Jackson, C., 514 and client variables, 455
Jadda, D., 265 and humanistic therapy processes
Janzen, J., 462, 503 and outcomes, 61, 62, 66–67
Jarry, J. L., 65 and optimal practices in humanistic
Jenkins, A. H., 100 therapies, 505, 507, 517,
Jewell, C., 431 518, 521
Johnson, S. M., 321, 355–357, 361, 364, and therapist factors, 423
365, 367, 374 Landreth, G., 390, 395, 400
Johnson, W. R., 227 Lane, J. S., 424
Jones, L., 392 Längle, Alfried, 6, 294
Joseph, S., 32, 191 Language problems, 399–400
Jourard, S. M., 262 Lashani, L., 229
Layton, J. B., 17
Kalff, D. M., 261 Lazarus, A. A., 521
Karl, R., 233 Leary, M. R., 22
Karver, M., 404 LeBlanc, M., 392
Katonah, D. G., 262, 263, 269, 275 Lee, K. R., 403
Keating, E., 335 Lee, V., 307
Keisler, D. J., 460 Leijssen, M., 269, 510
Kelly, A. C., 432 Leitner, L. M., 486
Kelly, A. E., 28 Lennox, S., 263
index 547
Nadler, W. P., 512 Optimal practices of humanistic
Narrative qualitative research, 81, 87 psychotherapies, 485–527. See
Narratives also specific main headings
coherence of, 149 acceptance, 495–497
emotional reflection with, 160 affirmation, 495–497
National Health Service, 209 client depth of experiencing, 510–513
National Institute for Clinical client distress and readiness for
Excellence, 209 change, 486–489
National Institute of Mental Health client experience of therapeutic
Treatment of Depression relationship, 517–518
Collaborative Research client involvement, 505–506
Program, 503 client locus of control and
National Institutes of Health, 92 evaluation, 513–514
Navelski, L., 395 client positive expectations and
NDST (nondirective supportive therapy), therapist optimism, 506–509
197–201 collaboration and cooperation,
Neff, K. D., 34 504–505
Neuroimaging, 365 congruence, genuineness, and
Nguyen, P., 234 authenticity, 498–501
Nielsen, G. H., 463 empathy, 492–495
individualization of psychotherapy,
Nondefensiveness, 508–509
516–517
Nondirective supportive therapy
mutual affirmation and liking, 497–498
(NDST), 197–201
overview of, 524–527
Nondirectivity, 187, 195, 203–204
positive regard, 495–497
Nonverbal behaviors, 117–118
pragmatism, pluralism, and
Norcross, J. C.
integration, 519–522
and client variables, 469
relational depth, 501–502
and empathy, 118
relationship monitoring, 518–519
and existential therapies, 285, 287,
self and self-concept, 514–516
289, 308 therapeutic alliance, 502–504
and humanistic therapies and therapist investment in client
outcomes, 62 well-being, 522–524
and optimal practices for humanistic therapist presence and engagement,
therapies, 488 489–492
Norquist, S., 458 Optimism, 4, 506–509
No-treatment control studies, 55 Orlinsky, D. E.
and client variables, 455–456,
O’Donnell, P., 66–67 458–460, 491–492, 496
Ogles, B. M., 521 and principles of humanistic
O’Hara, M. M., 486 therapies, 504–506, 508–509
O’Leary, E., 227 and therapist factors, 424
O’Leary, K. D., 120 Orr, S. P., 124
On Becoming a Better Therapist Outcome research on humanistic
(Barry Duncan), 520 psychotherapies. See also
On Becoming a Person (Carl Rogers), Research on humanistic
207 psychotherapies
Ontology, 83 and emotions. See Emotions in
Openness, 459, 508–509 psychotherapy
Operant conditioning, 13 and empathy, 119–120
index 549
Process research on humanistic Relatedness, 12
psychotherapies. See also Relational connection, 13–20
Research on humanistic and emotions in psychotherapy,
psychotherapies 168–169
and emotions. See Emotions in in humanistic psychotherapy, 14–20
psychotherapy humanistic therapy’s influence on, 6
on existential therapies, 284, 304, 309 overview of, 13–14
measurement technologies in, 90 in person-centered therapy, 207–208
on nondirectivity, 195 research on, 17–20
Prochaska, J. O., 469, 488 Relational depth
Prosser, M. C., 119–121, 154, 332 defined, 14
Psychiatry, critical, 190 overview, 387–388, 501–502
Psychoanalysis, 219 in person-centered therapy, 201–202,
Psychodrama, 285 207–208
Psychodynamic therapy, 90, 198 Relational Depth Inventory, 202
Psychoeducation, 57, 306–307 Relational skills, 291
Psychotherapy (journal), 6 Relational trauma, 152–153
Psychotherapy Process Q-Sort Manual, 287 Relationship enhancement (RE) therapy,
Psychotherapy research, 11. See also 354–355, 360–363, 369–374
specific headings Relationship monitoring, 518–519
PTSD. See Posttraumatic stress disorder Rennie, D. L., 82, 87, 92, 94, 95, 461,
Purkey, W. W., 515 516–519
Purton, C., 259 Research on humanistic psychotherapies,
49–74. See also Qualitative
Qualitative research, 81–105 research
characteristics of, 82 and alternative treatments, 56–59
clinical applications of, 101–104 applications of, 68–73
constructivist–interpretive current body of, 53–55
approaches to, 86–88, 104 early, 50–53
convergence between humanistic no-treatment and wait-list control
worldview and, 90–92 studies, 55
critical–ideological approaches to, overview of, 5–7
88–90, 104 and research on other therapies,
on existential therapies, 284 60–65
with grounded theory, 81, 86, 87, and routine monitoring, 66–68
92–94, 102 on therapeutic processes, 65–66
postpositivist approach to, 83–86 Resistance, 120, 469–470
recent reviews on psychotherapy Responsiveness, 14
research, 94–101 Responsivity, 22
RE (relationship enhancement) therapy,
Raimy, V., 515 354–355, 360–363, 369–374
Ralston, M., 65 Rhine, T., 392
Rapport, empathic, 428 Rice, K. G., 34
Raskin, N. J., 186–187, 514 Rice, L. N., 162, 320, 460, 509
Raue, P. J., 503 Richmond, C. J., 473
Ray, D. C., 391–393, 395–397, 399–403 Riess, H., 121
Reactance, 469 Rimes, K., 236
Received empathy, 116 Ritchie, M., 392
Received support, 20–26 Roback, H. B., 514
Redecision theory, 227 Robinson, Virginia, 186
index 551
Shirk, S., 404 Summers, L., 263
Shlien, J., 207 Supportive–expressive therapy, 286,
Shrout, P. E., 21 295, 302
Siegel, D., 404 Surace, F. I., 94
Silence in therapy, 85, 102–104 Sveško-Visentin, H., 231
Silverman, W. K., 403 Swan, K., 395, 400
Skillfulness, 22 Swann, W. B., Jr., 34
Skinner, B. F., 13 Swift, J. K., 471, 517
Smart, D. W., 67 Symbolization, 159, 168
Smith, E. W. L., 227 Symonds, D., 62, 434, 503
Smith, J. Z., 469, 508 Systematic evocative unfolding, 193
Smith, M. L., 196, 395
Smith, S. K., 363 Taft, Jessie, 186
Smith, T. B., 17 Tallman, K., 456, 457, 474, 507, 517
Smith, T. W., 488 Tausch, R., 195
Snyder, W. U., 195 Technical eclectic approach to therapy,
Social functioning, 33–34 521
Social support. See also Relational Tennessee Self-Concept Scale, 265
connection Terminal illness, 295–296
and emotional expression, 26 TF-CBT (trauma-focused cognitive–
importance of, 15–16 behavioral therapy), 403
Softening events, 366 Thematic analysis, 87
Somatic problems, 170 Theoretical integration approach to
Song, X., 469 therapy, 521
Soteria approach, 285 Therapeutic alliance
Sotskova, A., 120 capacity to repair, 298
Sousa, Alegria, 289 client’s role in, 458
Sousa, D., 287, 289, 293 conceptualizations of, 425–426
Spiegel, D., 295 definitions of, 62
Spinelli, Ernesto, 297 in emotion-focused therapy, 193,
Sprenkle, D., 360 322, 331–332
Standard view of person-centered empathy’s role in, 134–135
therapy, 259 and motivation, 468–469
Stanley, P. H., 515 overview, 502–504
Starling, M., 402 quantitative research on, 84
Steckley, P. L., 119 and therapeutic presence, 433
Sterner, L., 512 Therapeutic presence, 235, 489–492
Stevens, C., 228 Therapeutic Presence Inventory (TPI),
Stiles, W. B., 198 238, 491
Stoler, N., 498 Therapist–client relationship. See also
Stolorow, Robert, 224 Therapeutic alliance
Strauss, J. L., 435 client experience of, 517–518
Stringfellow, J., 228 concordance in, 124–125
Strümpfel, Goldman, 226–227 in existential therapies, 298–299
Strupp, H. H., 458 in Gestalt therapies, 221–222
Stuart, J., 494–495 importance of, 4, 14, 387–388
Stulmaker, H. L., 403 overview, 425–426, 434–439
Subjectivity of client, 4, 135, 285 qualitative research on, 93
Suicidality, 16–17 quantitative research on, 84
Suizzo, M. A., 34 and research outcome variance, 61–63
index 553
Wexler, D. A., 320, 509 Working at Relational Depth in Counseling
Wheeler, Gordon, 219 and Psychotherapy (D. Mearns &
Wholeness (humanistic value), 322 M. Cooper), 501
Wiggins, S., 202, 299, 502 Wormith, J. S., 469, 506
Williams, P. G., 488 Worry, 167
Willutzki, U., 505 Worsley, R., 193
Wingrove, J., 236
Winograd, G., 471–472 Yalom, I. D., 160, 295–296, 523
Wisconsin Project, 191–192, 196 Yamaguchi, S., 261
Wiser, S., 503 Yerymenko, N., 153
Withdrawal emotions, 163 Yip, J. J., 28
Wittenborn, A. K., 364 York I study, 327
Wolff, M. C., 120 York II study, 328–329
Wong, P. T. P., 294 Yousefi, N., 229
Wood, A. M., 32–34
Woodin, E. M., 120 Zimring, F. M., 195
Working alliance, 117, 330. See also Zuccarini, D., 367
Therapeutic alliance Zuroff, D. C., 432, 434, 468
Kevin Keenan, PhD, received his MS and PhD in clinical psychology from
the University of Kentucky. He is a core faculty member of the Michigan
School of Professional Psychology. Dr. Keenan is on the executive board of
the American Psychological Association Division 32 (Society for Humanistic
Psychology [SHP]) and a fellow of the Michigan Psychological Association.
He is an associate editor for the Journal of Humanistic Psychology and coeditor
of SHP’s newsletter. Dr. Keenan has a private practice in Farmington Hills,
555
Michigan, and has special interests in recovery, spirituality, life-span develop-
ment, supervision, and relational depth psychotherapy.
Shawn Rubin, PsyD, was trained by Clark Moustakas, PhD, and carries
on the work of his mentor by presenting and training internationally on human-
istic approaches to play therapy, parent guidance and support, the heuristic
research method, clinical supervision, and the experiential teaching and train-
ing of graduate students across the helping professions. He is chair of the
School of Clinical Psychology at Saybrook University and director of both
the PhD and PsyD programs in clinical psychology. Dr. Rubin serves as editor
in chief of the Journal of Humanistic Psychology and board member of American
Psychological Association Division 32 (Society for Humanistic Psychology).