The Effect of Severe Stress On Early Brain Development, Attachment, and Emotions
The Effect of Severe Stress On Early Brain Development, Attachment, and Emotions
The Effect of Severe Stress On Early Brain Development, Attachment, and Emotions
Ricardo M. Vela, MD
KEYWORDS
Child abuse Child neglect Limbic system development Emotions Attachment
Amygdala Psychoanatomical formulation
KEY POINTS
Child abuse and neglect are the most severe forms of stress experienced by children and
adolescents.
Child abuse and neglect has severe developmental consequences to the development of
the amygdala, septal nucleus, and anterior cingulate gyrus, which are rapidly developing,
especially in very early infancy.
The basic emotions of joy, surprise, sadness, anger, and fear develop in the first 6 months
of life. Embarrassment, envy, empathy, pride, shame, and guilt (which requires self-
consciousness) develop by 3 years of age.
Synaptic modification and consolidation is very vulnerable during the experience-
expectant, sensitive developmental periods. Child maltreatment may have severe conse-
quences, resulting in maladaptive cell assemblies and synaptic connections.
The psychoanatomical formulation is a theoretically based explanation used to conceptu-
alize a clinical case by correlating the disturbed neuroanatomy with behavioral and
emotional symptom expression. It provides the clinician with an added dimension in
understanding a clinical case.
Child neglect and abuse are the most extreme forms of stress in children, with severe
effects on social, emotional, interpersonal, and neuronal development. According to
the US Department of Health and Human Services, from data submitted by 49 states,
the District of Columbia, and the Commonwealth of Puerto Rico, there were 3,184,000
children who received child protective services in 2012. Overall, four-fifths (78.3%) of
victims were neglected, 18.3% were physically abused, 9.3% were sexually abused,
Disclosures: The author does not have any commercial conflicts to disclose.
Child and Family Services, North Suffolk Mental Health Association, Massachusetts General
Hospital, 301 Broadway, Chelsea, MA 02150, USA
E-mail address: [email protected]
Abbreviations
and 8.5% were psychologically maltreated.1 Child neglect is associated with adverse
psychological and educational outcomes and it is hypothesized that these outcomes
may be caused by adverse brain development.2 Early life trauma is associated with
persistent developmental brain changes that mediate the increased diathesis not
only to mood and anxiety disorders but also to depression, posttraumatic stress dis-
order (PTSD), schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder.3
Animal and human studies have expanded knowledge of the emotional or limbic
system, and provided insight into how normal and abnormal development unfolds.
This article introduces the concept of the psychoanatomical formulation, developed
by the author and defined as a theoretically based explanation used to conceptualize
a clinical case by correlating the disturbed neuroanatomy with behavioral and
emotional symptom expression. Following the vignette of a neglected 2.5-year-old
child, the development and function of limbic structures involved in emotions and
attachment is discussed, with emphasis on the first year of life. Next, the emergence
of basic emotions in children from birth to 3 years is reviewed, followed by a discus-
sion of basic principles of neural and synaptic development and their implications for
child abuse and neglect. In addition, the developmental neuroanatomy of child neglect
is brought together in the context of the psychoanatomical formulation of the case
vignette.
Medical evaluation did not show signs of physical or sexual abuse. There was no history of sexu-
alized play. However, because language was delayed, knowing fewer than 50 intelligible words,
she was referred for early intervention services. In a short period of time her speech showed
improvement. She started putting 2-word phrases together and repeated phrases. She looked
at people when they called her name, was affectionate with loved ones, and liked to be held
often. She played appropriately with toys. However, she had trouble adjusting to change and
continued to be aggressive toward other children, often hitting or biting them. She continued
to have no sense of danger, banging into objects and falling from furniture she climbed, even
though gross motor coordination was age appropriate. She continued to go up to unknown
people indiscriminately, in spite of repeated warnings by her adoptive mother and father.
In spite of controversies and open criticism by some neuroscientists,4,5 the term limbic
system, which MacLean6 used to replace the old term rhinencephalon, has persisted
in the neurologic, psychiatric, and neurobiological literature, to denote the structures
that participate in generating emotions. The limbic system has become synonymous
with the emotional system.7 Five limbic structures have been posited to play a prin-
cipal role in emotional expression and attachment behavior in the first year of life:
hypothalamus, amygdala, septal nuclei, anterior cingulate gyrus, and hippocampus.8
These structures form a circuit involved in the hierarchical control of emotions, with the
anterior cingulate playing the role of an integrative cortical suprastructure, whereas, at
the lower end, the hypothalamus (and periaqueductal gray matter [PAG]) act as a fun-
nel through which all limbic-generated emotions are eventually expressed.9 We argue
later that child neglect and/or abuse lead to abnormal limbic development and, conse-
quently, to severe psychological disorder. Concepts and evidence discussed here are
used to provide a theoretic framework for the case using the psychoanatomical
formulation.
According to Kagan and Baird10 there are some correspondences between brain
maturation and the ontogeny of human psychological competencies from birth to
puberty, with significant maturational transitions that occur at specific ages. The first
maturational transition period occurs at 2 to 3 months of postnatal life and is charac-
terized by the disappearance of newborn reflexes, secondary to cortical inhibition of
brain stem neurons and appearance of new synaptic contacts. There is a growth of
inhibitory interneurons in the spinal cord that corresponds with the disappearance
of these reflexes. There is a concomitant reduction in crying and an increase in social
smiling that can probably be attributed to cortical inhibition of brain stem nuclei that
mediate crying, especially in the PAG. Together with these developments, the infant
shows the possibility of establishing visual expectations. This stage coincides with a
great increase in the growth velocity of the mossy cells of the dentate gyrus of the
hippocampus.10
Hypothalamus
The hypothalamus is almost fully developed at birth, and controls emotions in the
newborn infant. It is the central core through which all emotions derive their motive
force.11 Endocrine, hormonal, visceral, and autonomic functions controlled by the
hypothalamus are of utmost importance for the survival of the human infant. Paralle-
ling these biological functions, and crucially important for the emotional survival,
development, and attachment to the caregiver, are the emotional functions of the
hypothalamus. Through this structure the newborn infant is capable of expressing
4 Vela
aversion versus pleasure and rage versus quiescence. Vocalizations initially produced
by the hypothalamus elicit maternal behaviors that attend to the basic needs of the
infant, provide comfort, and are alerts to the need to be fed and cared for.
The hypothalamus consists of a complex number of nuclei, each with its subdivi-
sions. However, in their extensive work on the neuroanatomy of the hypothalamus,
Crosby and Woodbourne,12 Crosby and Showers,13 and Nauta and Haymaker14 indi-
cated that, for purposes of description, hypothalamic cell groups could be classified or
subdivided into 3 longitudinal zones: periventricular, medial, and lateral.
The periventricular area is intrinsically associated with the neurohormonal control of
the pituitary gland, and, as for stress, it plays a major role in the hypothalamic-
pituitary-adrenal axis and is critically involved in the adaptation to stressful changes.
From the point of view of the functional neuroanatomy of emotions, the medial and
lateral nuclei play antagonistic but complementary functions. Electrical brain stimula-
tion (EBS) with electrodes placed in the medial hypothalamic area in cats (and ventro-
lateral areas as well) resulted in what Flynn15 called “attack with rage.” Now called
affective aggression, it is manifested by rage display (eg, hissing and growling), auto-
nomic changes (eg, pupillary dilatation, piloerection), but at the same time with an
aversion component, because trained cats try to stop the apparently unpleasant stim-
ulation. A rat sharing the cage would not be hurt, as if the aggressive display was all for
show.9,15
In contrast with EBS to the medial hypothalamic area, electrical stimulation of the
lateral hypothalamic areas (specifically the dorsolateral area) elicited what Flynn15
called “quiet biting attacks,” which are now described in the scientific literature as
predatory aggression. Cats stimulated with brain electrodes in this area move swiftly,
with the nose close to the ground, stalking and going directly to the rat in the cage.
It may be useful clinically to conceptualize aggression under the subdivisions of
predatory versus affective, because clinical assessment of the nature of aggression
is often in one of these categories. Predatory aggression is goal oriented, planned,
and controlled. The perpetrators hide their aggressive acts. The aggressors can con-
trol their aggressive acts. At the end, the predatory aggressors are proud of having
been aggressive. This kind of aggression is seen in sociopathic behavior.
In contrast, affective aggression is reactive, unplanned, and uncontrolled. The
aggressors may damage their own property with disregard to its value. They may
be aggressive and lose control in front of other people, oblivious of shame in the
heat of the moment. The aggressors expose themselves to physical harm. Children
may fight with bigger, stronger children, disregarding the risk of being hurt. Aggression
seems unplanned and to take place without an ultimate purpose. The aggressors may
express remorse after the aggressive act.
EBS studies have the limitation that electrical stimulation may stimulate hundreds of
thousands of emotions. A new, sophisticated technique developed by Karl Deisseroth
and Ed Boyden at Stanford University can selectively stimulate a specially selected
group of neurons (or their projection-specific dynamics) consisting of a promoter
gene to an opsin (light-sensitive receptor) gene, which forms a construct that is
expressed only on the specifically selected neuronal subpopulation (but not neigh-
boring cells) when light is delivered to the brain through a fiberoptic cable (for a review
see Tye and Deisseroth16 and Fennol and colleagues17).
Using a variation of this technique, it has been shown that optogenetic stimulation of
the ventromedial hypothalamus, ventrolateral part, causes mice to attack male and
female mice and inanimate objects.18 Optogenetic tools in the future will establish
areas of dysfunction in psychiatric disease that are impossible to establish by any
other means.19
Stress and Early Brain Development 5
According to Kagan and Baird’s10 ontogenetic model, the second transition occurs
in most healthy infants between the ages of 7 and 12 months. During this period the
infant is increasingly able to retrieve schemata (patterns of parents’ physical features)
and holds them along with current perceptions in a working memory circuit.10 Sepa-
ration fear occurs when the infant retrieves a schema of the mother’s former presence
held in a working memory circuit and tries to relate this to the discrepant perception of
her current absence. Growth and development of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex
are likely to be relevant to separation and stranger fear. Axonal projections from the
amygdala to the anterior cingulate cortex through the capsula interna myelinize
between postnatal months 7 and 10. This process coincides with the emergence of
fears of strangers of separation from a caretaker.10
Amygdala
Experiments producing selective amygdala lesions in stressed infant monkeys, start-
ing at 2 weeks of age, showed no difference in mother-infant interactions at 3 months
of age. However, the one consistent and robust finding in both animal and human
studies is that damage to the amygdala results in impairment in the danger-
detection systems.20–23
The amygdala therefore plays a modulating role in social behaviors. The role of the
amygdala as the detector of danger is to continuously evaluate the environment and
surroundings for potential threats. These functions extend to social situations and
generate appropriate physiologic/emotional responses.23 Expanding on this concept,
Buchanan and colleagues24 postulated that what influences amygdala function is the
unpredictable nature of social interactions. Damage to the amygdala results in inap-
propriate responses to ambiguous social cues.24 Neonatal amygdala damage results
in an inability to assess the degree of danger of a social situation and to modify
behavior appropriately.24
Receiving sensory inputs from different parts of the brain, the amygdala monitors
and abstracts the motivational significance from a wide array of multimodal sensory
stimuli, from food to discrete social-emotional nuances.11 It processes and funnels
emotionally relevant sensory information from multiple neocortical and limbic areas
and sends the processed data to the hypothalamus and brain stem, eliciting auto-
nomic, endocrine, and emotional responses.7 It responds mainly to danger/negative
stimuli that evoke fear or defensive reactions but also responds to positive stimuli
such as sexual attraction, positive emotional words, and appetizing foods.7 It is
responsive to somesthetic input and physical contact, both of which are necessary
for maternal-infant bonding and the normal development of the amygdala.8
In general, there is a lateral-to-medial unidirectional flow and processing of informa-
tion.4,25 The lateral nucleus receives information from the neocortex and projects it to
the basal and accessory basal nuclei, which in turn project it to the medial and central
nuclei and form an output to visceral and autonomic regions. The centromedial amyg-
daloid complex forms a macrostructure with cell columns through the substantia inno-
minata, including the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis portions of the nucleus
accumbens, all of which makes up the extended amygdala.26
The amygdala analyzes information and transfers it back to the neocortex, with
which it has extensive interconnections including the orbitofrontal and anterior cingu-
late gyrus. It projects to a much greater region of the neocortex than what it receives
from the neocortex.25 Hence, the normal amygdala is primarily wired to be able to
adaptively sense, react, analyze, and respond to dangerous stimuli, rather than
soothing and calming the individual. According to Amaral,27 the amygdala is a pro-
tective device, designed to detect and avoid danger. It evaluates objects in the
6 Vela
surroundings before interacting with them, and, based on this evaluation, coordinates
species-typical responses.
The complexity of the small, almond-shaped amygdala cannot be overstated. It is
heterogeneous in neurochemical organization, with rich intrinsic and extrinsic chemi-
cal neuroanatomy. The amygdala has the highest density of gamma-aminobutyric
acid A receptors; a rich distribution of opiate receptors; and expresses as many
neurotransmitters, peptides, and calcium-binding proteins as do the neurons in the
neocortex.25 Two major fiber systems, the stria terminalis and the ventral amygdalo-
fugal pathway, connect the amygdala with the hypothalamus and other limbic struc-
tures involved in emotional expression.28 In contrast with the hypothalamus, which
can be immediately turned on and off, stimulation of the amygdala produces
longer-lasting mood states after cessation of electrical stimulation.11
In the monkey, the amygdala receives input from area TE located in the anterior
portion of the inferior temporal cortex. Area TE carries information from primary
visual area V1, and comes at the end of the ventral stream of hierarchical visual pro-
cessing. It is most responsive to complex visual objects like faces. It terminates in
the lateral nucleus of the amygdala, which projects to the adjacent basal nucleus.25
In the human brain, it is the fusiform gyrus that carries information of faces to the
amygdala for processing. Social recognition requires the ability to recognize and
remember other people before forming social relationships.29 Lesions in the fusi-
form gyrus can abolish the ability to recognize faces. The right amygdala has a crit-
ical role in processing the emotional content of stimuli, including fearful facial
expressions.24,25,30
The amygdala has a well-documented primary involvement in conditioned fear4,31
and is prominently activated in response to fearful faces in subjects with PTSD
compared with normal controls.32
Bilateral damage to the amygdala can result in an extreme lack of fear of dangerous
stimuli and social situations. The case of S.M., a 44-year-old woman with bilateral
amygdala damage as a result of lipoid proteinosis (Urbach-Wiethe disease), has
been extensively studied.33 She showed no fear of snakes and poisonous spiders
when taken to an exotic pet shop. She was oblivious of danger when attacked with
a knife in a park. She had an excessive degree of approach behavior and had a
compulsive desire to touch and poke dangerous snakes, finding all of this interesting
and amusing.33
Septal Nuclei
The septal nucleus attains greatest evolutionary development in humans. It is involved
in emotional functioning and, like the amygdala and cingulate gyrus, it is capable of
producing emotional vocalizations that may elicit care. It maintains a counterbalancing
relationship with the amygdala, with antagonistic influence on the hypothalamus. The
septal nucleus facilitates actions of the medial hypothalamus (whereas the amygdala
mainly activates the lateral hypothalamus). It reduces extremes in emotionality and
arousal and maintains a state of quiescence and readiness. It counters and inhibits
aggressive behavior and suppresses expression of rage reactions following hypotha-
lamic stimulations.11 The septal nucleus exerts inhibitory influences on the amygdala,
whereas the amygdala acts to facilitate or inhibit septal functions.11 Development of
the septal nuclei and anterior cingulate gyrus enable human infants to slowly develop
a stable and selective loving attachment and, at around 6 months to 1 year of age, to
show expressions of anger, joy, and fear. Normal, intact septal nuclei act to promote
selective social attachments that are strengthened through positive reinforcement and
caring parent-child interactions.
Stress and Early Brain Development 7
The septal nuclei undergo a more protracted rate of development than the cingulate
gyrus. They do not reach adult levels of development until 3 years of age and continue
developing into puberty.8
Hippocampus
The hippocampus plays a major role in the storage and consolidation of information
into long-term memory. It is of utmost importance in learning and memory encoding,
providing long-term storage and retrieval of newly learned information.7,11 The hippo-
campus has an intimate relationship with septal nuclei with which it is connected by
fibers from the precommissural fornix. In concert with the medial hypothalamus and
8 Vela
Emotions are almost fully developed by the age of 3 years, although further refinement
and elaboration of emotions may take over after that time.36 Thus, the first 3 years of
life represent the major developmental leap in the emergence of human emotions.36
As previously discussed, the hypothalamus is almost fully developed at birth and is
responsible for generating primitive emotions. At birth, children show what has been
called a bipolar emotional life36 (here, the term bipolar is not used to describe a
mood disorder, but the manifestation of 2 distinct emotional poles). Although the
newborn can show general distress manifested by crying and irritability (the negative
pole), there is also pleasure reflected by satiation, attention, and responsivity to the
environment, which represents the positive pole of this bipolar dichotomy.
The development of the social smile by the age of 3 months marks the emergence of
joy in human infants. They show happiness and excitement (active and wiggling) when
exposed to familiar or unfamiliar faces or even a cardboard Halloween mask when
frontally presented.37 During the same time, sadness emerges. Three-month-old
infants react with sadness when their mothers stop interacting with them or on with-
drawal of positive-stimulus events.36 Infants react with joy and smiling when posi-
tioned in front of their mother’s face. On the mother’s presentation of a still face,
8-month-old babies first try to engage mothers by gesturing, making noises, and other
maneuvers to attract the mother’s attention. If the mother reengages in the interaction,
the child’s joy returns. However, if mothers are not available to reassume interactions
with their infants, the results can severely impair the child’s emotional development.
Distaste, the precursor of disgust, also appears around this age and is manifested
by spitting out and getting rid of unpleasant tastes in the mouth. Although some clini-
cians consider this to be evidence of disgust,36 others have argued that true disgust
does not appear as a separate emotion until the child is 4 to 8 years old.38,39
Stress and Early Brain Development 9
Anger emerges between 4 and 6 months of age. Anger is the adaptive action pattern
that has evolved to enable humans to overcome a barrier to a goal. Anger thus results
when the goal is blocked. Experiments with 4-month-old infants have shown that
anger is manifested when children are frustrated (eg, when their hands are tied
down and prevented from moving).36 Anger expressions are targeted at other people
by 7 months.40
Fearfulness emerges at around the age of 8 to 9 months. In order to develop fear,
infants have to have the capacity to compare the event that frightens them with
another event. For example, in the case of stranger anxiety, infants have to compare
the face of the stranger with the mental representation of familiar faces. Fear results
when there is a discrepancy between the new (stranger) face and the mental represen-
tations of familiar faces. Before developing this cognitive capacity, infants do not fear
stranger faces, or even, as mentioned earlier, masks of human faces.
Surprise is another emotion that appears in the first 6 months. Children show a
surprise response either when there is a violation of expected events or as a response
to discovery. For example, when infants see a midget walking toward them, they show
interest and surprise (rather than joy or fear), as a result of the discrepancy of seeing a
small adult.36
The emergence of self-awareness during the second half of the second year of life
gives rise to self-conscious emotions. These emotions include embarrassment,
empathy, and envy. The emergence of embarrassment only takes place after children
develop self-recognition. Embarrassment is measured by nervous touching, smiling,
gaze aversion, and return behaviors.36 Shame is closely related to embarrassment,
but it is a more intense emotion accompanied by the wish to hide, disappear, or die
and is a highly and painful emotional state that is difficult to dissipate.41 Shame requires
self-evaluation, which is a higher cognitive level than self-recognition or the conscious-
ness that is required for the emotion of embarrassment. True empathy or veridical
empathy develops at the end of the second year into the third year, when children
become more aware that others can have their own thoughts, feelings, and desires.42
Self-conscious evaluative emotions require children to have the capacity to evaluate
their behavior against a standard. This cognitive capacity emerges between 2 and
3 years of age. Besides shame, these complex social-evaluative emotions include
pride and guilt. Evaluating self-conscious emotions of failure may result in shame,
guilt, or regret, whereas self-perceived success gives rise to feelings of pride.
Therefore, by 3 years of age, normal children have formed the basis for an elaborate
and complex emotional system that will expand in the years to come.36 However, this
normal development can be disrupted by excessive stress in infants or children as a
result of neglect, physical or sexual abuse, or witnessing domestic violence. Indi-
viduals with poor attachment histories display empathy disorders: limited capacity
to perceive the emotional state of others.43
The effect of child abuse and neglect on the limbic brain development at the cellular
level can be conceptualized and organized under 4 closely interrelated developmental
principles:
1. Hebbian synaptic modification
2. Experience-expectant learning
3. Sensitive periods of development
4. Self-organizing brain development
10 Vela
During early postnatal development, the brain expects to receive visual stimuli,
including faces that are to become familiar, to develop language receptivity and the
anxiety necessary for detecting danger, which will promote survival, and to establish
the facial recognition necessary for bonding and so forth.
Limbic system circuit development requires extensive environmental stimulation.
These circuits cannot achieve full functional potential under impoverished or adverse
environmental conditions.46 Experience-expectant emotional experiences during
early infancy are crucial for interlimbic and prefrontolimbic pathway development
during critical sensitive periods, and shape their function in adulthood. Neglected
infants are particularly prone to this. Dysfunctional infant attachment may predispose
the individuals to develop lasting, irreversibly impaired emotionality and social
behavior later in life.46
The third developmental principle is that certain aspects of early brain development
only take place during sensitive periods of development. Sensitive periods are devel-
opmental windows in which maturation and specific experiences interact to produce
differential long-term effects on the brain and behavior.47 During this time window a
biological event develops more easily under the influence of an environmental stimulus.
After this sensitive period has ended, learning is more difficult and less efficient. Exam-
ples of this are vision and language acquisition. Hubel and Weisel48 performed exper-
iments temporarily suturing the eyelids of kittens from birth to 3 months of age. These
cats not only never fully developed vision through the blindfolded eye but also neurons
in the occipital cortex receiving inputs from that eye greatly decreased their space
compared with the increased cell growth of the cortex corresponding with the seeing
eye. The deprived eye never develops normal vision later in life.
Some investigators distinguish between sensitive periods and critical periods. Sensi-
tive periods start and end gradually and are particularly sensitive to certain types of stim-
uli. After the sensitive periods end, the individual can learn, but learning is more difficult,
takes longer, and may require intensive interventions. An example of this is language
acquisition. By contrast, critical periods are finite, compulsory, and are associated
with a heightened sensitivity to a specific environmental stimulus that allows the devel-
opment of a certain skill, after which acquisition is difficult or impossible. The example of
vision fits this concept. Limbic and higher neocortical structures are open; that is, they
change with experience during development. Lower structures like the hypothalamus
and PAG are closed and change little or not at all.9 However, limbic structures complete
myelination in the second decade of life, much earlier than the neocortex.34
The principle of sensitive periods and critical periods has important implications for
neglected or maltreated infants. Infants who do not regain adequate care after pro-
longed deprivation during the first year of life may be emotionally damaged for life.37
Finally, the fourth developmental principle conceptualizes brain development as a
process of self-organization. Drawing from neurobiological research, Lewis35 incorpo-
rated these data to formulate the principles and mechanisms of self-organization, with
emphasis on the role of emotion in self-organizing neural systems.35
Brain development can be conceptualized as a process of self-organization,
evolving in unpredictable and indeterminate ways through the repeated modification
of synaptic systems.35 Neural development organizes itself to achieve forms that
were initially indeterminate. Self-organizing synaptic sculpting consists of 2 forces:
1. Synaptic elaboration (ie, proliferation and strengthening)
2. Synaptic pruning
Synaptic elaboration favors activity of some synapses rather than others. Pruning
gets rid of underused synapses and consolidates synaptic stability. Neuronal
12 Vela
Amy fulfills the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
(DSM-V) diagnostic criteria for disinhibited social engagement disorder50 (this cor-
responds with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth
Edition [DSM-IV] diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder of infancy and early child-
hood, disinhibited type). This disorder is characterized by a pattern of behavior in
which a child actively approaches and interacts with unfamiliar adults, and in Amy’s
case this was manifested by reduced or absent reticence in approaching and interact-
ing with unfamiliar adults. She also displayed overly friendly, familiar behavior. She
showed willingness to go off with an unfamiliar adult with minimal or no hesitation,
as when she tried to leave the house with strangers who came to the door. Amy
also experienced a pattern of extremes of insufficient care, as shown by social neglect
and deprivation in the form of a persistent lack of having emotional needs met for com-
fort, stimulation, and affection by caregiving adults. She also had repeated changes of
primary caregivers (mother, relatives, and mother’s friend), which limited opportunities
to form stable attachments.
However, this diagnostic formulation does not provide insight into the underlying
developmental/neuroanatomic mechanisms that are playing an important part in the
manifestation of emotions and behavior in the patient. This author has developed
the concept of psychoanatomical formulation, which is defined as the assessment/
analysis of a clinical case based on the disturbed functional neuroanatomy underlying
behavioral and emotional symptom expression. The earlier discussion of limbic nuclei
development and function, development of emotions, and developmental principles
as they relate to child neglect and abuse serves as a background for better under-
standing this formulation. This psychoanatomical formulation comes closer to
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Director Thomas Insel and colleagues’51
efforts to develop research domain criteria that conceptualize mental disorders as dis-
orders of brain circuits.
Amy’s neglect continued through the second half of her first year of life, with the changing of
caregivers. It can be inferred that neglect and lack of positive reinforcement from caring parent-
child interactions during this time period interfered with the normal development of the septal
nuclei and anterior cingulate gyrus, which are involved in the development of stable, selective,
loving attachments. Because of the lack of normal development of the anterior cingulate gyrus
and its synaptic connections, she did not develop the ability to be discriminating in interactions
with others and to form real, specific attachments. Although not available by history, she prob-
ably never developed stranger and separation anxiety during the ages of 7 to 12 months. She
was unable to form long-term attachment because of her exposure to neglect, the change of
caregivers, and the resulting poorly developed anterior cingulate gyrus.
As the result of the stress caused by neglect and social impoverishment, Amy most likely estab-
lished, strengthened, and consolidated the connections necessary for her immediate survival;
however, now that she has been adopted into a caring and nurturing environment, these Heb-
bian modifications are maladaptive. She is not able to respond appropriately to her new envi-
ronment and express positive emotions. Synapses that would provide healthy adaptation,
learning, and positive emotions were not properly developed, ending up retracted and most
likely eliminated.
Amy was not exposed to the normal environmental stimuli that a neonatal brain is pro-
grammed to receive in order to attain optimal development. She did not receive experience-
expectant emotional experiences during the 20 months of life that are crucial for limbic and
prefrontolimbic circuit development. Self-organizing brain development and synaptic sculp-
turing most likely proliferated and strengthened maladaptive connections, with synapses pro-
moting positive emotions and secure attachments being eliminated. In addition, all of this
neglect occurred very early in life, during the sensitive periods of emotional development. Re-
covery from the severe consequences of this neglect will be difficult, and most probably require
intensive, long-term interventions. To her advantage is that she is still at a young age in which
many limbic circuits have not finished myelinating. In addition, basic research has shown that
afferents and new synaptic contacts from the amygdala to the medial prefrontal cortex increase
significantly during the rat age equivalent to adolescence and early adulthood.52 The implica-
tion here is that there is a second chance during adolescence for development of increased
emotional control. It is thus possible that Amy may have another opportunity to improve
emotional connections.
SUMMARY
Severe stress in the form of child abuse or neglect during early infancy may have
serious, long-lasting effects on a person’s brain development, affecting future mani-
festations of negative emotions, maladaptive behaviors, and conflictual attachments.
As a result, individuals thus affected operate in a survival mode, rather than learning to
flexibly adapt to environmental demands. More research is needed in order to under-
stand the genetic and developmental protective factors that enable some persons to
be less vulnerable and even more resilient to these extreme stressors, and for clini-
cians to learn to treat these patients more effectively.
The analysis and the correlation of external traumatic events with resulting structural
brain changes provide a deeper, more detailed understanding of the classic nature-
versus-nurture paradigm. This article is intended to stimulate psychiatrists and other
mental health professionals to expand and incorporate their knowledge of neuro-
anatomy to conceptualize psychoanatomical formulations for patients with a variety
of psychological disorders along the lifespan.
REFERENCES
22. Bauman MD, Lavenex P, Mason WA, et al. The development of social behavior
following amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys. J Cogn Neurosci 2004;16(8):
1388–411.
23. Schumann CM, Amaral DG. The human amygdala in autism. In: Whalen PJ,
Phelps EA, editors. The human amygdala. New York: Guilford; 2009. p. 362–81.
24. Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Adolphs R. The human amygdala in human function. In:
Whalen PJ, Phelps EA, editors. The human amygdala. New York: Guilford; 2009.
p. 289–318.
25. Emery NJ, Amaral DG. The role of the amygdala in primate social cognition. In:
Lane RD, Nadel L, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York:
Oxford University Press; 2000. p. 156–91.
26. Heimer L. A neuroanatomical framework for neuropsychiatric disorder and drug
abuse. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:1726–39.
27. Amaral DG. The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior:
implications for understanding social anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:11–7.
28. Nieuwenhuys R, Voogd J, van Huijzen C. The central nervous system. 4th
edition. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2008. p. 917–46.
29. Lim MM, Young LJ. Neurobiology of the social brain. In: Beach SR,
Wamboldt MZ, Kaslow NJ, et al, editors. Relational process and DSM-V:
neuroscience, assessment, prevention and treatment. Arlington (VA): American
Psychiatric Publishing; 2006. p. 21–37.
30. Clark DL, Boutrus NN, Mendez MF. The brain and behavior: an introduction
to behavioral neuroanatomy. 3rd edition. Cambridge (United Kingdom):
Cambridge University Press; 2010.
31. Le Doux J. Cognitive-emotional interactions: listen to the brain. In: Lane RD,
Nadel L, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York: Oxford University
Press; 2000. p. 129–55.
32. Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, et al. A functional magnetic resonance
imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly
presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry
2005;62:273–81.
33. Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, et al. The human amygdala and the induc-
tion and experience of fear. Curr Biol 2011;21:34–8.
34. Yakovlev PI, Lecours AR. The myologenetic cycles of regional maturation in the
brain. In: Minkowski A, editor. Regional development of the brain in early life.
Oxford (United Kingdom): Blackwell; 1967. p. 3–70.
35. Lewis MD. Self-organizing individual differences in brain development. Dev Rev
2005;25:252–77.
36. Lewis M. The emergence of human emotions. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM,
Barett LF, editors. Handbook of emotions. 3rd edition. New York: Guilford; 2008.
p. 304–19.
37. Spitz RA. The first year of life: a psychoanalytic study of normal and deviant
development of object relations. New York: International Universities Press; 1965.
38. Ekman P. Emotions revealed: recognizing faces, and feelings to improve
communication and emotional life. New York: Henry Holt; 2003. p. 174.
39. Rozin P, Haidt J, McCanley CR. Disgust. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM,
Barret LF, editors. Handbook of emotions. 3rd edition. New York: Guilford;
2008. p. 757–76.
40. Lemerise EA, Dodge KA. The development of anger and hostile interactions. In:
Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barett LF, editors. Handbook of emotions. 3rd
edition. New York: Guilford; 2008. p. 730–41.
16 Vela
41. Lewis M. Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride, shame and guilt. In:
Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barett LF, editors. Handbook of emotions. 3rd
edition. New York: Guilford; 2008. p. 742–56.
42. Hoffmann ML. Empathy and prosocial behavior. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM,
Barett LF, editors. Handbook of emotions. 3rd edition. New York: Guilford; 2008.
p. 440–55.
43. Schore AN. Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: Norton; 2003.
44. Hebb DO. The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York:
Wiley; 1949.
45. Bear MF, Connors BW, Paradiso MA, editors. Neuroscience: exploring the brain.
Baltimore (MD): Williams & Wilkins; 1996.
46. Braun K. The prefrontal-limbic system: development, neuroanatomy, function,
and implications for the socioemotional development. Clin Perinatol 2011;
38(4):685–702.
47. Penhune V, de Villers-Sidani E. Time for new thinking about sensitive periods.
Front Syst Neurosci 2014;8:1–2.
48. Hubel DH, Weisel TN. Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with
artificial squint. J Neurophysiol 1995;26:994–1002.
49. Schore AN. Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York: Norton; 2003.
50. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders. 5th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
51. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a
new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry
2010;167:748–51.
52. Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM. Amygdalo-cortical sprouting
continues into early adulthood: implications for the development of normal
and abnormal function during adolescence. J Comp Neurol 2000;453:116–30.