Cat Sense by Bradshaw John
Cat Sense by Bradshaw John
Cat Sense by Bradshaw John
CAT SENSE
How the New Feline Science
Can Make You a Better
Friend to Your Pet
John Bradshaw
BASIC BOOKS
A MEMBER OF THE PERSEUS BOOKS GROUP
New York
Copyright © 2013 by John Bradshaw
Published by Basic Books,
A Member of the Perseus Books Group
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any manner whatsoever without written permission, except in the case of brief quotations
embodied in critical articles and reviews. For information, please address Basic Books, 250 West 57th Street, 15th Floor, New York, NY 10107.
Books published by Basic Books are available at special discounts for bulk purchases in the United States by corporations, institutions, and other
organizations. For more information, please address the Special Markets Department at the Perseus Books Group, 2300 Chestnut Street, Suite 200,
Philadelphia, PA 19103, or call (800) 810-4145, ext. 5000, or email [email protected].
Designed by Trish Wilkinson
Set in 11.5 point Goudy Old Style
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Bradshaw, John, 1950–
Cat sense : how the new feline science can make you a better friend to your pet / John Bradshaw.
pagescm
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-465-04095-7 (e-book)1.Cats—Behavior.2.Cats—Psychology.3.Human-animal relationships.4.Cat owners.I.Title.
SF446.5.B725 2013
636.8—dc232013020749
10987654321
To Splodge
(1988–2004)
A Real Cat
Contents
Preface
Acknowledgments
Introduction
CHAPTER 1The Cat at the Threshold
CHAPTER 2The Cat Steps Out of the Wild
CHAPTER 3One Step Back, Two Steps Forward
CHAPTER 4Every Cat Has to Learn to Be Domestic
CHAPTER 5The World According to Cat
CHAPTER 6Thoughts and Feelings
CHAPTER 7Cats Together
CHAPTER 8Cats and Their People
CHAPTER 9Cats as Individuals
CHAPTER 10Cats and Wildlife
CHAPTER 11Cats of the Future
Further Reading
Notes
Index
Dogs look up to us: cats look down on us.
—WINSTON CHURCHILLWhen a man loves cats, I am his friend and comrade, without further introduction.
—MARK TWAIN
Splodge
Preface
W hat is a cat? Cats have intrigued people ever since they first came to live among us. Irish legend has it that “a cat’s
eyes are windows enabling us to see into another world”—but what a mysterious world that is! Most pet owners would
agree that dogs tend to be open and honest, revealing their intentions to anyone who will pay them attention. Cats, on the
other hand, are elusive: we accept them on their terms, but they in turn never quite reveal what those terms might be.
Winston Churchill, who referred to his cat Jock as his “special assistant,” famously once observed of Russian politics, “It
is a riddle, wrapped in a mystery, inside an enigma; but perhaps there is a key”; he might as well have been talking about
cats.
Is there a key? I’m convinced that there is, and that it can be found in science. I’ve shared my home with quite a few
cats—and have become aware that “ownership” is not the appropriate term for this relationship. I’ve witnessed the birth
of several litters of kittens, and nursed my elderly cats through their heartbreaking final declines into senility and ill
health. I’ve helped with the rescue and relocation of feral cats, animals that literally wanted to bite the hand that fed them.
Still, I don’t feel that, on its own, my personal involvement with cats has taught me very much about what they are really
like. Instead, the work of scientists—field biologists, archaeologists, developmental biologists, animal psychologists,
molecular biologists, and anthrozoologists such as myself—has provided me with the pieces that, once assembled, begin
to reveal the cat’s true nature. We are still missing some pieces, but the definitive picture is emerging. This is an
opportune moment to take stock of what we know, what is still to be discovered, and, most important, how we can use our
knowledge to improve cats’ daily lives.
Getting an idea of what cats are thinking does not detract from the pleasures of “owning” them. One theory holds that we
can enjoy our pets’ company only through pretending that they are “little people”—that we keep animals merely to project
our own thoughts and needs onto them, secure in the knowledge that they can’t tell us how far off the mark we are.
Taking this viewpoint to its logical conclusion, forcing us to concede that our cats neither understand nor care what we
say to them, we might suddenly find that we no longer love them. I do not subscribe to this idea. The human mind is
perfectly capable of simultaneously holding two apparently incompatible views about animals, without one canceling the
other out. The idea that animals are in some ways like and in others quite unlike humans lies behind the humor of
countless cartoons and greetings cards; these simply would not be funny if the two concepts negated each other. In fact,
quite the opposite: the more I learn about cats, both through my own studies and through other research, the more I
appreciate being able to share my life with them.
Cats have fascinated me since I was a child. We had no cats at home when I was growing up, nor did any of our
neighbors. The only cats I knew lived on the farm down the lane, and they weren’t pets, they were mousers. My brother
and I would occasionally catch intriguing glimpses of one of them running from barn to outhouse, but they were busy
animals and not over-friendly to people, especially small boys. Once, the farmer showed us a nest of kittens among the
hay bales, but he made no special effort to tame them: they were simply his insurance against vermin. At that age, I
thought that cats were just another farm animal, like the chickens that pecked around the yard or the cows that were driven
back to the barn every evening for milking.
The first pet cat I ever got to know was the polar opposite of these farm cats, a neurotic Burmese by the name of Kelly.
Kelly belonged to a friend of my mother’s who had bouts of illness, and no neighbor to feed her cat while she was
hospitalized. Kelly boarded with us; he could not be let out in case he tried to run back home, he yowled incessantly, he
would eat only boiled cod, and he was evidently used to receiving the undivided attention of his besotted owner. While he
was with us, he spent most of his time hiding behind the couch, but within a few seconds of the telephone ringing, he
would emerge, make sure that my mother’s attention was occupied by the person on the other end of the line, and then
sink his long Burmese canines deep into her calf. Regular callers became accustomed to the idea that twenty seconds in,
the conversation would be interrupted by a scream and then a muttered curse. Understandably, none of us became
particularly fond of Kelly, and we were always relieved when it was time for him to head back home.
Not until I had pets of my own did I begin to appreciate the pleasures of living with a normal cat—that is to say, a cat that
purrs when it is stroked and greets people by rubbing around their legs. These qualities were probably also appreciated by
the first people to give houseroom to cats thousands of years ago; such displays of affection are also the hallmark of
tamed individuals of the African wildcat, the domestic cat’s indirect ancestor. The emphasis placed on these qualities has
gradually increased over the centuries. While most of today’s cat owners value them for their affection above all else, for
most of their history, domestic cats have had to earn their keep as controllers of mice and rats.
As my experience with domestic cats grew, so did my appreciation of their utilitarian origins. Splodge, the fluffy black-
and-white kitten we bought for our daughter as compensation for having to relocate, quickly grew into a large, shaggy,
and rather bad-tempered hunter. Unlike many cats, he was fearless in the face of a rat, even an adult. He soon learned that
depositing a rat carcass on our kitchen floor for us to find when we came downstairs for breakfast was not appreciated,
and after that he kept his predatory activities private—without, I suspect, giving the rats themselves any respite.
However brave he was against a rat, Splodge usually kept away from other cats. Every now and then, we would hear the
cat-flap clatter as he arrived home in a tearing hurry, and a quick glance out the window would usually reveal one of the
older cats in the neighborhood, glaring in the general direction of our back door. He had a favorite hunting area in the park
nearby, but kept himself inconspicuous when traveling there and back. His diffidence toward other cats, especially males,
was not just typical of many cats; it also exemplified a weakness in social skills that is perhaps the greatest difference
between cats and dogs. Most dogs find it easy to get along with other dogs; cats generally find other cats a challenge. Yet
many of today’s owners expect their cats to accept other cats without question—either when they themselves wish to get a
second cat, or when they decide to move, depositing their unsuspecting cat into what another cat thinks is its territory.
For cats, a stable social environment is not enough; they rely on their owners to provide a stable physical environment as
well. Cats are fundamentally territorial animals that put down powerful roots in their surroundings. For some, their
owner’s home is all the territory they need. Lucy, another of my cats, showed no interest in hunting, despite being
Splodge’s great-niece; she barely strayed more than a dozen yards from the house—except when she came into season
and disappeared over the garden wall for hours on end. Libby, Lucy’s daughter and born in my home, was as brave a
hunter as Splodge had been, but preferred to call the tomcats to her rather than go to them. Even though they were all
related and all lived in the same house most of their lives, Splodge, Lucy, and Libby all had distinctive personalities, and
if I learned one thing from observing them it was that no cat is completely typical: cats have personalities, just as humans
do. This observation inspired me to study how such differences come about.
The transformation of the cat from resident exterminator to companion cohabiter is both recent and rapid, and—especially
from the cat’s perspective—evidently incomplete. Today’s owners demand a different set of qualities from their cats than
would have been the norm even a century ago. In some ways, cats are struggling with their newfound popularity. Most
owners would prefer that their cats did not kill cute little birds and mice, and those people who are more interested in
wildlife than in pets are becoming increasingly vocal in their opposition to the cat’s predatory urges. Indeed, cats now
probably face more hostility than at any time in the past two centuries. Can cats possibly shake off their legacy as
humankind’s vermin exterminator of choice, and in just a few generations?
Cats themselves are oblivious to the controversy caused by their predatory natures, but all too aware of the difficulties
they encounter in their dealings with other cats. Their independence, the quality that makes cats the ideal low-
maintenance pet, probably stems from their solitary origins, but it has left them poorly equipped to cope with many
owners’ assumptions that they should be as adaptable as dogs. Can cats become more flexible in their social needs, so that
they are unfazed by the proximity of other cats, without compromising their unique appeal?
One of my reasons for writing this book is to project what the typical cat might be like fifty years from now. I want people
to continue to enjoy the company of a delightful animal, but I’m not sure that the cat, as a species, is heading in the right
direction. The more I’ve studied cats, from the wildest feral to the most cosseted Siamese, the more I’ve become
convinced that we can no longer afford to take cats for granted: a more considered approach to cat keeping and cat
breeding is necessary if we are to ensure their future.
Acknowledgments
I began studying cat behavior more than thirty years ago, first at the Waltham Center for Pet Nutrition, later at the
University of Southampton, and now at the University of Bristol’s Anthrozoology Institute. Much of what I’ve learned
has come from painstaking observation of cats themselves: my own, my neighbors’, cats in adoption centers, the family of
cats that used to share the Anthrozoology Institute’s offices, and many ferals and farm cats.
Compared to the large number of canine scientists, rather few academics specialize in feline science, and even fewer make
the domestic cat the focus of their attention. Those I’ve had the privilege of working with and who’ve helped me to form
my ideas about how cats see the world include Christopher Thorne, David Macdonald, Ian Robinson, Sarah Brown, Sarah
Benge (née Lowe), Deborah Smith, Stuart Church, John Allen, Ruud van den Bos, Charlotte Cameron-Beaumont, Peter
Neville, Sarah Hall, Diane Sawyer, Suzanne Hall, Giles Horsfield, Fiona Smart, Rhiann Lovett, Rachel Casey, Kim
Hawkins, Christine Bolster, Elizabeth Paul, Carri Westgarth, Jenna Kiddie, Anne Seawright, Jane Murray, and others too
numerous to list.
I’ve also learned a great deal from discussions with colleagues both at home and abroad, including the late Professor Paul
Leyhausen, Dennis Turner, Gillian Kerby, Eugenia Natoli, Juliet Clutton-Brock, Sandra McCune, James Serpell, Lee
Zasloff, Margaret Roberts and her colleagues at Cats Protection, Diane Addie, Irene Rochlitz, Deborah Goodwin, Celia
Haddon, Sarah Heath, Graham Law, Claire Bessant, Irene Rochlitz, Patrick Pageat, Danielle Gunn-Moore, Paul Morris,
Kurt Kotrschal, Elly Hiby, Sarah Ellis, Britta Osthaus, Carlos Driscoll, Alan Wilson, and the late and much-missed Penny
Bernstein. My thanks also to the University of Bristol’s School of Veterinary Medicine, especially Professors Christine
Nicol and Mike Mendl, and Drs. David Main and Becky Whay, for nurturing the Anthrozoology Institute and its research.
My studies of cats have relied on the cooperation of many hundreds of volunteer cat owners (and their cats!), to whom I
will always be grateful. Much of our research would have been impossible without the unstinting assistance of the UK’s
re-homing charities, including the RSPCA, the Blue Cross, and St. Francis Animal Welfare, and I am especially grateful
to Cats Protection for two decades of practical and financial assistance.
Summarizing nearly thirty years of research on cat behavior into a form intended to be appreciated by the average cat
owner has not been an easy task. I have had expert guidance from Lara Heimert and Tom Penn, my editors at Basic and
Penguin respectively, and my indefatigable agent Patrick Walsh. Thank you all.
As in my previous books, I’ve turned to my dear friend Alan Peters to bring some of the animals to life in the illustrations,
and just as before, he’s done me more than proud.
Finally, I must thank my family for their forbearance for my enforced absences in what my granddaughter Beatrice calls
“Pops’ office.”
Introduction
T he domestic cat is the most popular pet in the world today. Across the globe, domestic cats outnumber “man’s best
friend,” the dog, by as many as three to one.1 As more of us have come to live in cities—environments for which dogs are
not ideally suited—cats have, for many, become the lifestyle pet of choice. About one-third of US households have one or
more cats, and they are found in more than a quarter of UK families. Even in Australia, where the domestic cat is
routinely demonized as a heartless killer of innocent endangered marsupials, about a fifth of households own cats. All
over the world, images of cats are used to advertise all kinds of consumer goods, from perfume to furniture to
confectionery. The cartoon cat “Hello Kitty” has appeared on more than 50,000 different branded products in more than
sixty countries, netting her creators billions of dollars in royalties. Even though a significant minority of people—perhaps
as many as one person in five—don’t like cats, the majority who do show no sign of relinquishing even a fraction of their
affection for their favorite animal.
Cats somehow manage to be simultaneously affectionate and self-reliant. Compared to dogs, cats are low-maintenance
pets. They do not need training. They groom themselves. They can be left alone all day without pining for their owners as
many dogs do, but they will nonetheless greet us affectionately when we get home (well—most will). Their mealtimes
have been transformed by today’s pet-food industry from a chore into a picnic. They remain unobtrusive most of the time,
yet seem delighted to receive our affection. In a word, they are convenient.
Yet despite their apparently effortless transformation into urban sophisticates, however, cats still have three out of four
feet firmly planted in their wild origins. The dog’s mind has been radically altered from that of its ancestor, the gray wolf;
cats, on the other hand, still think like wild hunters. Within a couple of generations, cats can revert back to the
independent way of life that was the exclusive preserve of their predecessors some ten thousand years ago. Even today,
many millions of cats worldwide are not pets but feral scavengers and hunters, living alongside people but inherently
distrustful of them. Due to the astonishing flexibility with which kittens learn the difference between friend and foe, cats
can move between these dramatically different lifestyles within a generation, and the offspring of a feral mother and feral
father can become indistinguishable from a cat descended from generations of pets. A pet that is abandoned by its owner
and cannot find another may turn to scavenging; a generation or two on, and its descendants will be indistinguishable
from the thousands of feral cats that live shadowy existences in our cities.
As cats become more popular and ever more numerous, those who revile them continue to raise their voices, but with
more venom now than for several centuries before. Cats have never shared the “unclean” tag foisted on the dog and the
pig, but despite cats’ superficially universal acceptance, a minority of people across all cultures finds cats disagreeable,
and as many as one in twenty say that they find them repulsive.2 When asked, few Westerners will admit that they don’t
like dogs: those who do usually turn out either not to like animals in general or can trace their aversion to a specific
experience, perhaps being bitten in childhood.3 Cat-phobia is more deeply seated and less widespread than common
phobias of snakes and spiders—phobias that have a logical basis in helping the sufferer to avoid poisonous varieties—but
is just as powerful an experience for those who suffer from it.4 Cat-phobics were likely at the forefront of the religious
persecution that led to the killing of millions of cats in medieval Europe, and cat-phobia was likely just as common then
as it is today. Thus, there can be no guarantee that the cat’s popularity will last. Indeed, without our intervention, the
twentieth century may turn out to have been the cat’s golden age.
Today, spurred on by confessed cat-haters, the cat is coming under attack on the specific grounds that it is a wanton and
unnecessary killer of “innocent” wildlife. These voices are most loudly raised in the Antipodes, but are becoming
increasingly strident in the UK and the United States. The anti-cat lobby, at its most extreme, demands that cats no longer
be allowed to hunt, that pet cats be kept indoors, and that feral cats should be exterminated. Owners of outdoor cats are
vilified for supporting an animal that is portrayed as laying waste to the wildlife around their homes. Veterinarians who
seek to manage the welfare of feral cats by neutering and vaccinating them, and then returning them to their original
territory, have come under attack from within their own profession, with some colleagues charging that this constitutes
(illegal) abandonment that benefits neither the cat nor the adjacent wildlife.5
Both sides in this debate admit that cats are “natural” hunters, but cannot agree on how this behavior might be managed.
In parts of Australia and New Zealand, cats are defined as “alien” predators introduced from the Northern hemisphere,
and are banned from some areas and subject to curfews or compulsory microchipping in others. Even in places where cats
have lived alongside native wildlife for hundreds of years, such as in the United States and the UK, their increasing
popularity as pets has prompted a vocal minority to press for similar restrictions. Cat owners point to a lack of scientific
evidence that pet cats contribute significantly to a population decline of any wild bird or mammal, which are caused
instead mainly by the recent proliferation of other pressures on wildlife, such as loss of habitat. Consequently, any
restrictions imposed on pet cats are unlikely to result in a resurgence of the species that they supposedly threaten.
Cats themselves are of course unaware that we no longer value their hunting prowess. Insofar as they are concerned, the
greatest threat to their subjective well-being comes not from people, but instead from other cats. In the same way that cats
are not born to love people—this is something they have to learn when they are kittens—they do not automatically love
other cats; indeed, their default position is to be suspicious, even fearful, of every cat they meet. Unlike the highly
sociable wolves that were forebears to modern dogs, the ancestors of cats were both solitary and territorial. As cats began
their association with humankind some 10,000 years ago, their tolerance for one another must have been forced to
improve so that they could live at the higher densities that man’s provision of food for them—at first accidental, then
deliberate—allowed.
Cats have yet to evolve the optimistic enthusiasm for contact with their own kind that characterizes dogs. As a result,
many cats spend their lives trying to avoid contact with one another. All the while, their owners inadvertently compel
them to live with cats they have no reason to trust—whether the neighbors’ cats, or the second cat obtained by the owner
to “keep him company.” As their popularity increases, so inevitably does the number of cats that each cat is forced into
contact with, thereby increasing the tensions that each experiences. Finding it ever harder to avoid social conflict, many
cats find it nearly impossible to relax; the stress they experience affects their behavior and even their health.
The well-being of many pet cats falls short of what it should be—perhaps because their welfare does not grab headlines in
the way that dogs’ welfare does, or perhaps because they tend to suffer in silence. In 2011, a UK veterinary charity
estimated that the average pet cat’s physical and social environment scored only 64 percent, with households that owned
more than one cat scoring even lower. Owners’ understanding of cat behavior scored little better, at 66 percent.6 Without a
doubt, if cat owners understood more about what makes their cats tick, many cats could live much happier lives.
Faced with such pressures, cats need not our immediate emotional reactions—irrespective of whether we find them
endearing or not—but instead a better understanding of what they want from us. Dogs are expressive; their wagging tails
and bouncy greetings tell us in no uncertain terms when they are happy, and they do not hesitate to let us know when they
are distressed. Cats, on the other hand, are undemonstrative; they keep feelings to themselves and rarely tell us what they
need, beyond asking for food when they’re hungry. Even purring, long assumed to be an unequivocal sign of contentment,
is now known to have more complex significance. Dogs certainly benefit from the knowledge of their true natures that can
come only from science, but for cats this comprehension is essential, for they rarely communicate their problems to us
until these have become too much to bear. Most of all, cats require our assistance when, as happens far too often, their
social lives run awry.
Cats desperately need the kind of research from which dogs have benefited, but unfortunately feline science has not seen
the explosion of activity that has recently occurred in canine science. Cats have simply not grabbed the attention of
scientists as dogs have. However, the past two decades have provided significant advances, profoundly affecting
scientists’ interpretations of how cats view the world, and what makes them “tick.” These new insights form the core of
this book, giving us the first indications on how to help cats adjust to the many demands we now put on them.
Cats have adapted to live alongside people while retaining much of their wild behavior. Apart from the minority that
belong to a breed, cats are not humankind’s creation in the sense that dogs are; rather, they have coevolved with us,
molding themselves into two niches that we have unintentionally provided for them. The first role for cats in human
society was that of pest controller: some 10,000 years ago, wild cats moved in to exploit the concentrations of rodents
provided by our first granaries, and adapted themselves to hunting there in preference to the surrounding countryside.
Realizing how beneficial this was—cats, after all, had no interest in eating grain and plant foods themselves—people must
have begun to encourage cats to stay by making available their occasional surpluses of animal products, such as milk and
offal. The cats’ second role, which undoubtedly followed hard on the heels of the first but whose origins are lost in
antiquity, is that of companion. The first good evidence that we have for pet cats comes from Egypt some 4,000 years ago,
but women and children in particular may have adopted kittens as pets long before this.
As far as humans are concerned, these dual roles of pest controller and companion have ceased to go hand-in-hand.
Although we treasured cats until recently for their prowess as hunters, few owners today express delight when their cat
deposits a dead mouse on their kitchen floor.
Cats carry the legacy of their primal pasts, and much of their behavior still reflects their wild instincts. To understand
why a cat behaves as it does, we must understand where it came from and the influences that have molded it into what it is
today. Therefore, the first three chapters of this book chart the cat’s evolution from wild, solitary hunter to high-rise
apartment-dweller. Unlike dogs, only a small minority of cats has ever been intentionally bred by people—and
furthermore, when there has been deliberate breeding, it has been exclusively for appearance. No one has bred cats to
guard houses, to herd livestock, or to accompany or assist hunters. Instead, cats have evolved to fill a niche brought about
by the development of agriculture, from its beginnings in the harvest and storage of wild grains, to today’s mechanized
agribusiness.
Of course, when the cat first infiltrated our settlements many thousands of years ago, its other qualities did not go
unnoticed. Its appealing features, its childlike face and eyes, the softness of its fur, and, crucially, its ability to learn how
to become affectionate toward us, led to its adoption as a pet. Subsequently, humankind’s passion for symbolism and
mysticism elevated the cat to iconic status. Popular attitudes toward cats have been profoundly influenced by such
connotations: extreme religious views toward cats have affected not only how they were treated, but their very biology—
both how they behave and how they look.
Cats have changed to live alongside humans, but we have very different ways of gathering information about and thereby
interpreting the physical world that we share. Chapters 4 through 6 examine those differences: humans and cats are both
mammals, but our senses and brains work in different ways. Cat owners often underappreciate these differences: we have
a natural tendency to assume that animals perceive the world around us in the same way as we do. Moreover, even in
today’s world of rationality and science, we still treat the world as if it were sentient, attributing intention to the weather,
the earth, and the movements of the stars in the sky. How easy, therefore, it is to fall into the trap of thinking that because
cats are communicative and affectionate, they must be, more or less, little furry humans.
Science, however, reveals that cats are anything but. Beginning with the way that every kitten constructs its own version
of the world, with consequences that will last its entire lifetime, this part of the book describes how the cat gathers
information about its surroundings, especially the way it uses its hypersensitive sense of smell; how its brain interprets
and uses that information; and how its emotions guide its responses to opportunities and challenges alike. In scientific
circles, it has only recently become acceptable to talk about animal emotions, and one school of thought still maintains
that emotions are a byproduct of consciousness, meaning that no animals except humans and possibly a few primates can
possibly possess them. However, common sense dictates that if an animal that shares our basic brain structure and
hormone systems looks frightened, it must be experiencing something very like fear—probably not in quite the same way
that we experience it, but fear nonetheless.
Most (but not everything) of what biology has revealed about the cat’s world fits the idea that cats have evolved as
predators first and foremost. Cats are social animals too; otherwise, they could never have become pets as well as hunters.
The demands of domestication—first of all, the need to cohabitate with other cats in human settlements, and then the
benefits of forming affectionate bonds with people—have extended cats’ social repertoires out of all recognition
compared to those of their wild ancestors. Chapters 7 through 9 explore these social connections in detail: how cats
conceive of and interact with other cats and with people, and why two cats may react very differently in the same
situation. In other words, we will examine the science of cat “personality.”
The book concludes with an examination of the cat’s current place in the world, and how this might evolve in the coming
decades. Cats are under pressure from many different interests, some well-meaning and others antagonistic. Pedigree cats
are still in a minority, and those who breed them are in a position to avoid the practices that have so adversely affected the
welfare of pedigree dogs over the past few decades.7 However, the growing fashion for hybrids between domestic cats and
other, wild, species of cat, resulting in “breeds” such as the Bengal, can have unintended consequences. We must also ask
whether the cat is being inadvertently and subtly altered by those who hold cat welfare closest to their hearts.
Paradoxically, the drive to neuter as many cats as possible, with its laudable aim of reducing the suffering of unwanted
kittens, may be gradually eliminating the characteristics of the very cats best suited to living in harmony with humankind:
many of the cats that avoid neutering are those that are most suspicious of people and the best at hunting. The friendliest,
most docile cats are nowadays neutered before leaving any descendants, while the wildest, meanest ferals are likely to
escape the attention of cat rescuers and breed at will, thus pushing the cat’s evolution away from, rather than toward,
better integration with human society.
We are in danger of demanding more from our cats than they can deliver. We expect that an animal that has been our pest
controller of choice for thousands of years should now give up that lifestyle because we have begun to find its
consequences distasteful or unacceptable. We also expect that we should be free to choose our cat’s companions and
neighbors without regard for their origins as solitary, territorial animals. Somehow, we presume that because dogs can be
flexible in their choice of canine companions, cats will be equally tolerant of whatever relationships we expect them to
develop, purely for our convenience.
Until about twenty or thirty years ago, cats kept pace with human demands, but they are now struggling to adapt to our
expectations, especially that they should no longer hunt, and no longer desire to roam away from home. In contrast to
almost every other domestic animal, whose breeding has been strictly controlled for many generations past, the cat’s
transition from wild to domestic has—with the exception of pedigree cats—been driven by natural selection. Cats
essentially evolved to fit opportunities that we provided. We allowed them to find their own mates, and those kittens that
were best suited to living alongside humans, in whatever capacity was required of them at the time, were the most likely
to thrive and produce the next generation.
Evolution is not going to produce a cat that has no urge to hunt and that is as socially tolerant as a dog—at least not within
a timescale that will be acceptable to the cat’s detractors. Ten thousand years of natural selection has provided the cat with
enough flexibility to fend for itself when, from time to time, its compact with man breaks down, but not enough to cope
with a demand that has grown from nowhere in just a few years. Even for such a prolific breeder as the cat, natural
selection would take many generations to move even a token step in this direction. Only deliberate, carefully considered
breeding can produce cats that are well suited to the demands of tomorrow’s owners, and that will be more acceptable to
cat haters.
We can do much to improve the lot of cats today. Better socialization of kittens, better understanding of what
environments cats really need, more deliberate intervention in teaching cats to cope with situations that they find
distressing—all these can help cats to adjust to the demands we now make of them, and can also deepen the bond between
cat and owner.
Cats are in many ways the ideal pet for the twenty-first century, but will they be able to adapt to the twenty-second? If
they are to continue to remain in our affections—and the persecution they have received in the past indicates that this is
hardly a given—then some consensus must emerge among cat welfare charities, conservationists, and cat fanciers on how
to produce a type of cat that checks all the boxes. These changes must be guided by science. Initially, the way forward
will be for cat owners and the general public alike to understand better where cats came from and why they behave as they
do. At the same time, owners can rehabilitate the cat’s fraying reputation by learning how to channel their cat’s behavior,
not only to discourage them from hunting, but also to make them happier in themselves. In the longer term, the emerging
science of behavioral genetics—the mechanics of how behavior and “personality” are inherited—will allow us to breed
cats that can better adapt to an ever-more crowded world.
As history shows, cats can fend for themselves in many ways. However, they cannot face what society now demands of
them without human assistance. Our understanding of cats must start with a healthy respect for their essential natures.
CHAPTER 1
Every member of the cat family, from the noble lion to the tiny black-footed cat, can trace its ancestry back to a medium-
sized catlike animal, Pseudaelurus, that roamed the steppes of central Asia some 11 million years ago. Pseudaelurus
eventually went extinct, but not before unusually low sea levels had allowed it to migrate across what is now the Red Sea
into Africa, where it evolved into several medium-sized cats, including those we know today as the caracal and the serval.
Other Pseudaelurus traveled east across the Bering land bridge into North America, where they eventually evolved into
the bobcat, lynx, and puma. Some 2 to 3 million years ago, following the formation of the Panama isthmus, the first cats
crossed into South America; here they evolved in isolation, forming several species not found anywhere else, including
the ocelot and Geoffroy’s cat. The big cats—lions, tigers, jaguars, and leopards—evolved in Asia and then spread into
both Europe and North America, their present-day distributions but a tiny relict of where they used to roam a few million
years ago. Remarkably, the distant ancestors of today’s domestic cats seem to have evolved in North America about 8
million years ago, and then migrated back into Asia some 2 million years later. About 3 million years ago, these began to
evolve into the species we know today, including the wildcat, the sand cat, and the jungle cat; a separate Asian lineage,
including Pallas’s cat and the fishing cat, also began to diverge at about this time.4
Jungle cat
Coeval with them were sand cats, Felis margarita, large-eared nocturnal animals that hunt by night, using their acute
hearing. They are, moreover, comparatively unafraid of humans, and hence might be thought good candidates for taming
and domestication. However, they are made for life in deserts—the pads on their feet are covered in thick fur to protect
them from the hot sand—so few would have found themselves near the first stores of grains: the Natufians generally built
their villages in wooded areas.
Sand cat
As civilization spread eastward through Asia, so it would have come into contact with other cat species. At Chanhudaro, a
town built by the Harappan civilization close to the Indus River in what is now Pakistan, archaeologists found a 5,000-
year-old mudbrick imprinted with a cat’s foot, overlapped by that of a dog. As the newly made brick was drying in the
sun, the cat appears to have run across it, closely followed by a dog, possibly in hot pursuit. The footprint is larger than
that of a domestic cat, and its webbed feet and extended claws identify it as a fishing cat, Felis viverrina, found today
from the Indus basin eastward and south to Sumatra in Indonesia (though not in the Fertile Crescent). As its name implies,
the fishing cat is a strong swimmer and specializes in catching fish and aquatic birds. Although it will also take small
rodents, it is difficult to see how it would switch to a diet consisting predominantly of mice, so it too is an unlikely
candidate for domestication.
Manul
Farther afield, we know of at least two other species of cat that came in out of the wild to prey on the vermin that plagued
humankind’s food stores. In Central Asia and ancient China, the local wildcat, the manul (or Pallas’s cat, named after the
German naturalist who first categorized it) was occasionally even tamed and deliberately kept as a rodent controller. The
manul has the shaggiest coat of any member of the cat family, so long that its hair almost completely obscures its ears. In
pre-Columbian Central America, meanwhile, an otter-like cat, the jaguarundi, was probably also kept as a semi-tame pest
controller. None of these species have ever become fully domesticated, neither are any of them included in the direct
ancestry of today’s house cats.
Jaguarundi
O ut of all these various wild cats, only one was successfully domesticated. This honor goes to the Arabian wildcat Felis
silvestris lybica, as confirmed by their DNA.5 In the past, both scientists and cat-fanciers have suggested that certain
breeds within the domestic cat family are hybrids with other species—for example, the Persian’s fluffy feet are
superficially similar to the sand cat’s, and its fine coat is somewhat like that of a manul. However, the DNA of all
domestic cats—random-bred, Siamese, or Persian—shows no trace of these other species, or indeed any other admixture.
Somehow, the Arabian wildcat alone was able to inveigle itself into human society, outcompeting all its rivals, and
eventually spreading throughout the world. Although the qualities that gave it this edge are not easy to pin down, they
probably occurred in combination only in the wildcats of the Middle East.
The wildcat Felis silvestris is currently found throughout Europe, Africa, and central Asia, as well as western Asia, the
area where it probably first evolved. Like many predators, such as the wolf, it is now found only in isolated and generally
remote areas where it can avoid persecution from man. This has not always been the case. Five thousand years ago,
wildcats were evidently regarded as delicacies in some areas; the rubbish pits left by the “lake dwellers” of Germany and
Switzerland contain many wildcat bones.6 The cats must have been abundant at the time; otherwise, they could hardly
have been trapped in such large numbers. Over the centuries they became less common, displaced by the felling of their
forest habitat for agriculture, and forced farther into the woods by development and loss of habitat. The invention of
firearms led to wildcats being hunted to extinction in many areas. During the nineteenth century, various European
countries, including the UK, Germany, and Switzerland, classified them as vermin, due to the harm they supposedly
caused both wildlife and livestock.7 Only recently, due to the establishment of wildlife reserves and a more informed
attitude to the important role that predators play in stabilizing ecosystems, are wildcats returning to areas such as Bavaria,
where they have not been seen for hundreds of years.
The wildcat is now divided into four subspecies or races. These are the European forest cat Felis silvestris silvestris, the
Arabian wildcat Felis silvestris lybica, the Southern African wildcat Felis silvestris cafra, and the Indian desert cat Felis
silvestris ornata.8 All these cats are rather similar in appearance, and all are capable of interbreeding where their ranges
overlap. A possible fifth subspecies is the very rare Chinese desert cat Felis bieti, which according to its DNA split off
from the main wildcat lineage about a quarter of a million years ago. It’s possible that these cats actually form a separate
species, as no hybrids are known to exist, but they live in such a small and inaccessible region—part of the Chinese
province of Sichuan—that this may be due to lack of opportunity rather than physical impossibility.
Wildcats from different parts of the world differ markedly in how easily they can be tamed. Domestication, moreover, can
start only with animals that are already tame enough to raise their young in the proximity of people. Those offspring that
are best suited to the company of humans and human environments are, perhaps unsurprisingly, more likely to stay and
breed there than those that are not; the latter will most likely revert to the wild. Over several generations, this repeated
“natural” selection will, even on its own, gradually change the genetic makeup of these animals so that they become better
adapted to life alongside people. It is also likely that, at the same time, humans will intensify that selection, by feeding the
more docile animals and driving away those prone to bite and scratch. This process cannot start without some genetic
basis for tameness existing beforehand, and in the case of wildcats, this is far from evenly distributed. Today, some parts
of the world have little raw material for domestication, while others seem more promising.
We also know that the mummified cats were about 15 percent larger than modern pet cats.23 In almost every other
domestic species—cattle, pigs, horses, and even dogs—the early domesticated forms are significantly smaller than their
wild counterparts, mainly because smaller individuals are easier to handle. However, this principle may not apply to the
cat, which was small relative to man to begin with. More surprising, the mummified cats were also 10 percent larger than
African wildcats are today. It may be that the Egyptians deliberately favored large wildcats because they were more
effective rodent controllers, and that domestic cats have subsequently become smaller as they gradually transformed from
full-time pest controllers into pets.
T he attitudes of the ancient Egyptians toward their cats seem paradoxical, almost unthinkable, to modern sensibilities. To
the Egyptians, some cats were revered pets, many more were simply pest controllers, used by rich and poor alike, but,
uniquely, many were bred specifically to be killed as sacrifices. Apart from the last, all this is not very different from the
way that cats were regarded in Europe and the United States in the first half of the twentieth century. Indeed, the Egyptian
habit of having elaborate coffins made for favorite pet cats mirrors today’s cat cemeteries.
Undoubtedly, the Egyptian association between cats and religion seems the most foreign to us. The worshipers who
bought ready-prepared mummies as offerings at the temple can hardly have been unaware of what the contents of those
mummies were, for the breeding premises and production line for mummies were both nearby, and would have been
evident from their smell alone. Presumably these sacrificial cats were regarded in some way as “different” than household
cats, even though they would have been genetically indistinguishable. Perhaps this was reinforced by their being bred in
purpose-built catteries. Cats would have been just as prolific breeders in those days as they are now, and mummy
manufacturers would have found trapping young feral cats easy. Since both law and custom forbade this, separate
breeding of cats must have been the only solution. It is possible that access to the premises used was prohibited to all but
the priests who looked after the “sacred” cats, which were never seen by the worshipers until the cats had been
mummified, thus maintaining a distinction between household and sacred cats, even though they were otherwise identical.
The mummies themselves show a paradoxical concern for welfare during life, but no trace of concern for life itself; the
procurers of these mummies evidently took great care of their cats, but then killed them in huge numbers. Judging from
the sizes of the cats, they were evidently well nourished. Finding sufficient high-quality meat and fish for such large
numbers of animals could not have been easy. Although it is not entirely clear how all the cats were killed, it seems most
likely that they were strangled in some kind of prescribed, ritual manner. Moreover, although the production of mummies
must have been a lucrative business, there seem to have been few attempts to cheat the worshipers; almost all the
mummies that were produced to look like cats actually do contain a complete cat skeleton, even though it would
presumably have been more profitable to wrap a bundle of reeds in linen and pass it off as a cat mummy. The entire
process seems to have been carried out according to strict rules. These rules protected not only the worshipers from the
purchase of fake mummies, but also the cats, which were well-fed and cared for, at least by the standards of the time, up
to the point when they were sacrificed.
The cats of ancient Egypt were likely the main ancestors of modern-day cats, as several of their qualities attest. We have
no credible evidence for large-scale domestication of the cat anywhere else in the world before the birth of Christ. These
domestic cats had the wildcat’s striped tabby coat, so would have been distinguishable from genuine wildcats only by
their affection for, rather than fear of, people. Some were pets, certainly in well-to-do households and most probably in
many others. Most would have been useful in keeping food stores and granaries reasonably free of rodent pests.
Household cats were venerated, at least during the last few hundred years of Egyptian civilization, as indicated by the
illegality of killing them, and the rituals performed when one died.
F rom about 2,500 years ago, keeping cats gradually became more widespread around the eastern and northern shores of
the Mediterranean, as Egypt came first under Greek and then Roman influence. Historians have traditionally ascribed the
slow northwards spread of the cat to laws that prohibited the export of cats from Egypt. Some accounts even tell of the
Egyptians sending out soldiers to retrieve and repatriate cats that had been taken abroad.24 However, these laws were
almost certainly symbolic, connected to cat worship. The cat’s independence, hunting ability, and rapid rate of
reproduction would have made it impossible for Egyptian authorities to prevent domesticated cats from spreading along
their trade routes.
As cats spread out of Egypt, they must have come across, and interbred with, wild and semi-domesticated cats in other
areas of the Eastern Mediterranean. As the Cyprus cats attest, there must have been tamed cats in other parts of the Middle
East for thousands of years before the Egyptians began to transform them into domesticated animals. Egyptian paintings
from about 3,500 years ago show cats onboard ships, and these cats could plausibly have been either immigrants or
emigrants. Between 3,200 and 2,800 years ago, trade in the eastern Mediterranean was dominated by the seafaring
Phoenicians (who may even have domesticated their own wildcats as pest controllers), operating out of several city-states
in what is now Lebanon and Syria. The Phoenicians probably introduced lybica cats, either tamed or partly domesticated,
to many of the Mediterranean islands and to mainland Italy and Spain. The spread of the cat was probably delayed not by
Egyptian laws, but more by the presence in Greece and Rome of rival rodent controllers: tamed weasels and polecats (the
latter becoming domesticated as the ferret).
The domestic cat’s migration north from Egypt toward Greece is not well documented. In the Akkadian language, spoken
in the eastern part of the Fertile Crescent, separate words for domestic cat and wildcat appear about 2,900 years ago, so
domestic cats had probably spread into what is now Iraq by this period.
Domestic cats were likely common in Greece, at least among the aristocracy, some time before this. We know this from
coins minted for use in two Greek colonies. They were made about 2,400 years ago, one for Reggio di Calabria, on the
“toe” of Italy opposite Sicily, and the other for Taranto on the “heel,” and both depict their founders, some 300 years
previously. Although these were different people, the coins are remarkably similar and may both refer to the same legend.
Both show a man sitting on a chair, dangling a toy in front of a cat, which is reaching up with its forepaws. That this man
is shown with a cat rather than the more usual horse or dog suggests that pet cats were initially unusual in Greece,
possibly exotic imports from Egypt, and their possession an indicator of status. A bas-relief carved in Athens at about the
same time shows a cat and a dog about to fight, but the cat is leashed, implying a tamed rather than domestic animal.
In the Middle Ages, monasteries must have been particularly valuable to cats because of their fishponds, used to raise the
fish eaten during Lent, the liturgical season of the late winter and early spring when meat-eating was forbidden. Fish were
plentiful and great sources of protein, and livestock were slaughtered months before because little fodder was available to
feed them in the winter months. Cats, often pregnant during these months, would have been happy to turn from hunting
mice to scavenging fish scraps, thereby gaining nutrition essential to the well-being of their unborn kittens, and an
advantage over their heathen counterparts on nearby farms.
T he relationship between Church and cat became seriously antagonistic from the thirteenth to seventeenth centuries,
threatening the very survival of the domestic species in some parts of continental Europe. In 1233, the Catholic Church
began a concerted attempt to exterminate cats from continental Europe. On June 13 of that year, Pope Gregory IX’s
notorious Vox in Rama was published. In this papal Bull, cats—especially black cats—were identified specifically with
Satan. Over the next 300 years, millions of cats were tortured and killed, along with hundreds of thousands of their mainly
female owners, who were suspected of witchcraft. Urban populations of cats were decimated. The justification for this
barbarism was essentially the same as it had been in the fourth century—the extermination of cults that still included cats
in their worship, and the demonization of rival religions such as Islam—but now it was the cats themselves that bore the
brunt of the Church’s wrath.
Outside Western Europe, cats were generally better tolerated at this time. The Eastern (Greek) Orthodox Church appears
to have had little quarrel with cat-keeping. Islam has a tradition of kindness to cats, so cats continued to thrive in the
Middle East. In Cairo, the Sultan Baibars, ruler of Egypt and Syria, founded what was probably the first sanctuary for
homeless cats in 1280 CE.
Even within the reach of the Church of Rome, cats were not universally reviled. In Britain, cats feature in fourteenth-
century poetry, including Geoffrey Chaucer’s Canterbury Tales. If the “Steward’s Tale” is accurate, then cats were well
tended, as well as appreciated for their mouse-catching skills:
Let’s take a cat and raise him well with milk
And tender meat, and make his couch of silk,
Then let him see a mouse go by the wall—
At once he’ll leave the milk and meat and all,
And every dainty that is in the house,
Such appetite he has to eat a mouse.4
Moreover, it appears that cats were secretly popular even in ecclesiastical circles. Carvings of cats adorn the choir stalls of
medieval churches across Europe, including Britain, France, Switzerland, Belgium, Germany, and Spain. These cats are
depicted not as demons, but in natural or domestic situations—washing themselves, caring for kittens, and sitting by the
fireside. It may be that such carvings were deliberately placed out of view of the general congregation, since presumably
sermons sometimes referred to cats as demonic. Although cats and the women who doted on them were sporadically
persecuted, cats were generally tolerated for their usefulness—not least in rural areas, where the reach of the Church was
weakest and the services cats provided were most appreciated.
Did this reversal in its fortunes have any lasting impact on the cat as a domesticated animal? No physical trace of the
persecution of black cats in Western Europe remains: today, the black mutation is as common in Germany and France as
it is in Greece, Israel, or North Africa, all of which were outside the influence of the medieval Catholic Church. Sometime
during the Middle Ages, cats, which were significantly larger and more wildcat-like up until Roman times, became
smaller—in some locations, even smaller than the average cat of today. Although persecution might have influenced this,
it is difficult to pinpoint the time or place that the changes in size occurred. In part this is because it is rare for sufficiently
large numbers of cat bones to be found in one place to give an accurate picture of what the “average cat” might have
looked like: for example, researchers would presume that the remains of a single large cat found anywhere in Europe are
those of a wildcat, and an extra-small cat the product of malnutrition.
In Western Europe, the cat seems to have changed in size over the centuries, but not consistently. For example, in York in
the tenth and eleventh centuries, domestic cats were the same size they are today, but at the same period in Lincoln, a
scant eighty miles away, cats were mostly small by today’s standards. However, by the twelfth and thirteenth centuries,
York cats had become smaller than their counterparts of 200 years earlier. In Hedeby, Germany, cats of the ninth through
eleventh centuries were roughly the same size as they are today, but in Schleswig, also in Germany, some of the cats
recovered from eleventh-through fourteenth-century remains were tiny. A staggering and unexplained 70-percent
reduction in bone lengths seems to have occurred between the eleventh and fourteenth centuries: many of the cats found
in Schleswig, which seem to have left no descendants, would look tiny compared to a typical twenty-first-century cat.5
We might be tempted to ascribe this miniaturization to the persecution that began in the fourteenth century, but we have
no direct evidence that this was the case; indeed, the reduction in size in England predates the Papal proclamation. As
such, the cause of these shifts toward smaller cats remains a mystery, and we do not know when cats grew larger again.
Likewise, we might be tempted to link the persecution of cats to the Black Death, a pandemic of rat-borne bubonic plague
that swept from China to Britain from 1340 through 1350. More than a third of Europe’s human population died, along
with many of its cats. However, the plague was just as devastating in India, the Middle East, and North Africa, where cats
remained unpersecuted, as it was in Western Europe. Evidently the bacillus was simply too virulent to be contained, and
indeed plague continued to break out in Europe occasionally over the next 500 years. The last major epidemic in Britain
was the Great Plague of London of 1665–66, and this time cats, not rats, were blamed; 200,000 cats were slaughtered on
the Lord Mayor of London’s orders.6
Seventeenth-century Britain was not a good time and place to be a cat, nor were the new colonies in North America. As a
result of their link to the remnants of paganism in rural communities, cats—again, especially black cats—had become
associated with witchcraft. We see remnants of this association today in horror films and Halloween decorations. When
communities tried witches for their “crimes,” they often claimed that the witches could transform themselves into cats;
they also mentioned other animals, including dogs, moles, and frogs, but cats were the most common. Thus, the Church of
Rome gave its official sanction to cruelty toward cats. Anyone coming upon a cat after dark was justified in killing or
maiming it, on the grounds that it might be a witch in disguise. On the Scottish island of Mull, one black cat after another
was roasted alive for four days and four nights in an exorcism referred to as the Taigherm.7 Colonial leaders brought these
same prejudices to Massachusetts, culminating in the Salem Witch Trials of 1692–93.
Witches with their “familiars”—a cat, a rat, and an owl
The reputation of the cat began to improve in mid-eighteenth-century Europe. Louis XV, the great-grandson of the same
Louis XIV who had lit a Paris bonfire under a basket of cats, was at least tolerant of them, allowing his wife Maria and
her courtiers to indulge their pet cats’ every whim. It became fashionable for such pets to be incorporated into paintings of
French ladies of title, and special tombs were built for favored animals when they died. However, such attitudes were far
from universal: around the same time, the naturalist Georges Buffon wrote, in his definitive thirty-six-volume Histoire
Naturelle, Générale et Particulière: “The cat is an unfaithful domestic, and kept only through necessity to oppose to
another domestic which incommodes us still more, and which we cannot drive away.”8
Meanwhile in England, cats were growing in popularity. Eighteenth-century poets Christopher Smart and Samuel Johnson
not only valued the company of cats, but also wrote about them. Smart’s poem For I Will Consider My Cat Jeoffry begins:
For I will consider my Cat Jeoffry.
For he is the servant of the Living God duly and daily serving him.
For at the first glance of the glory of God in the East he worships in his way.
For this is done by wreathing his body seven times round with elegant quickness.
—implying that Smart saw no link between cats and devil worship: indeed, quite the opposite. Smart was also an acute
observer of cat behavior. The poem continues:
For first he looks upon his forepaws to see if they are clean.
For secondly he kicks up behind to clear away there.
For thirdly he works it upon stretch with the forepaws extended.
For fourthly he sharpens his paws by wood.
For fifthly he washes himself.
For sixthly he rolls upon wash.
For seventhly he fleas himself, that he may not be interrupted upon the beat.
For eighthly he rubs himself against a post.
For ninthly he looks up for his instructions.
For tenthly he goes in quest of food.
Likewise, Samuel Johnson adored his cats Hodge and Lily. His biographer James Boswell wrote, “I never shall forget the
indulgence with which he treated Hodge, his cat,” possibly referring to Johnson’s habit of feeding Hodge oysters—which,
it must be said, were not the luxury food then that they are now.
Toward the end of the nineteenth century, the cat completed its transformation to domestic status. In Britain, Queen
Victoria kept a succession of pet cats: her Angora, White Heather, was one of the consolations of her old age, surviving
her to become the pet of her son, Edward VII. In the United States, Mark Twain was not only a cat enthusiast but also,
like Smart, an acute observer of their nature:
By what right has the dog come to be regarded as a “noble” animal? The more brutal and cruel and unjust you are to him the more your fawning and
adoring slave he becomes; whereas, if you shamefully misuse a cat once she will always maintain a dignified reserve toward you afterward—you will
never get her full confidence again.
By the nineteenth century, cats became far more varied in appearance than their ancestors in Egypt. At different times
and in various places in Europe and the Middle East, new coat types appeared due to spontaneous genetic mutations.
Sometimes these must have disappeared after a few generations, particularly if cat owners regarded the mutations as
freakish. However, sometimes unusual-looking cats must have found sympathetic owners who treasured these unique
differences. If this preference came to be shared by local owners, and then spread to other places, the underlying mutation
might gradually spread through the general population, and thereby became part of the range of variation that we see in
today’s cats—different colors, different patterns, both long and short hair. Remarkably, we can still trace the origins and
spread of some of these changes, even in today’s multifarious cat populations.
Geneticists credit the comparative homogeneity of household cats across Europe and the Middle East to the local habit of
keeping cats on ships. For example, we can easily imagine a litter of kittens conceived in Lebanon being born two months
later in Marseilles after their mother had jumped ship. Seagoing Phoenicians, Greeks, and Romans spread cats around the
Mediterranean, and the genetics of cats in France still shows traces of the trade route up the river Rhône from the
Mediterranean and then down the Seine to the English Channel. The distribution of the orange tabby mutation in northern
Europe still shows the effects of its popularity with Viking invaders almost a thousand years ago.9
The first evidence for domestic cats that did not have striped coats comes not long after the beginning of the Christian era.
The Greeks first described black cats and white cats in the sixth century CE, but the black mutation, which occurs quite
regularly, may have been spreading through the domestic population for several centuries before. Many of the cats that
carry this mutation are not actually black. The black color is caused by an inherited inability to produce the normal pale
hair tips that give wildcats their brownish appearance, which is known technically as “agouti.” The hairs then revert to
their basic color, black, but only if the cat carries two copies of this mutation—one inherited from its mother and one from
its father. If it carries one normal copy and one mutant, then the normal copy is dominant and its coat will be the usual
tabby. Thus, a male and female pair of such cats, outwardly tabby, could produce a black kitten alongside their other,
tabby, kittens. The black mutation is widespread among today’s cats, wherever in the world they are found, suggesting
that it may have originated before the Phoenicians and Greeks began spreading domestic cats around Europe.
Black cats and white cats are not only opposites in terms of color; they are also opposites in terms of their relationship
with humankind. Cats can be all white either because they are albino, in which case their eyes will be pink, or because
they carry a mutation, “dominant white.” Both tend to be less healthy than normal cats; not only are they prone to skin
cancer, but blue-eyed “dominant white” cats are often deaf. Perhaps more importantly, unlike their striped counterparts,
all-white cats stand out against almost any background, so are unlikely to find it easy to catch enough food to sustain
themselves. Outside the pedigree population, in which dominant white coats have been deliberately bred into some
breeds, all-white cats are uncommon, rarely forming more than 3 percent of the free-ranging cat population.
By contrast, the black mutation is so common and widespread that it must not bring any major biological disadvantage to
the cats that carry it. In some places, more than 80 percent of cats carry this mutation—not that all or even most of these
cats will actually have black coats, since many will have only one copy of the mutation and will therefore appear striped.
Today, black is commonest in Britain and Ireland, in Utrecht in the Netherlands, in the city of Chiang Mai in northern
Thailand, in a few US cities such as Denton, Texas (holding the current record at almost 90 percent incorporation), in
Vancouver, and also in Morocco.
Since black has not always been everyone’s favorite color for a cat, this ubiquity is difficult to fathom. Since cultural
explanations appear not to fit, scientists have proposed a biological basis for the pervasiveness of the black mutation: that
possession of this mutation, even one invisible copy (the “heterozygote”), somehow makes the cat more friendly to people
and/or other cats, thereby giving the cats that carry it an advantage in high-density living situations or in prolonged,
unavoidable contact with people, such as onboard a ship.10
This hypothesis runs counter to a recent study of cats in Latin America.11 Here, roughly 72 percent of cats carry the black
mutation, similar to the proportion in Spain, from where most South American cats must have come. We can estimate the
number of long journeys that cats must have taken between Spain and each of the various Hispanic colonies by tracing
their paths along the trade routes. The ancestors of the cats of La Paz, 13,000 feet above sea level in the Andes, completed
many such journeys, but black cats are no more common here than in Spain; any cat that traveled this far must have been
remarkably tolerant of people. Thus, we cannot yet convincingly explain either the overall numbers of black cats or their
local variations.
Blotched and striped tabbies
The “classic” or blotched tabby pattern exhibits the most remarkable distribution, although the reasons for its prevalence
in some areas and not in others are not entirely clear. The cat’s wild ancestor had a striped, or “mackerel,” tabby coat, and
all domestic cats probably carried the genes for this pattern until at least 2,000 years ago, and possibly even more
recently.12 The mutation for the blotched tabby pattern probably first took hold sometime in the late Middle Ages, and
almost certainly in Britain, where it is the commonest pattern today.
Like the black mutation, blotched tabby is recessive: for a cat to have a blotched tabby coat, it must have two copies of the
blotched version of the gene, one inherited from its mother and one from its father. One striped, one blotched, and the cat
will have a striped coat. Despite this apparent handicap, in Britain and in many parts of the United States, blotched tabbies
outnumber striped tabbies about two to one, meaning that more than 80 percent of cats carry the blotched version of the
gene. In many parts of Asia, blotched tabbies are rare or even absent. The main exceptions are a few former British
colonies such as Hong Kong, which were presumably simultaneously colonized by British cats, either ships’ cats or the
pets of the settlers.
For a new version of a gene, especially a recessive gene, to spread through a population, it must provide some advantage.
Since (striped) cats had been in Britain since Roman times and probably earlier, blotched tabbies must have been rare to
begin with. They probably reached 10 percent of the population by about 1500 CE, and then increased year after year until
they reached their current near-ubiquity. In Britain, blotched tabby is sometimes referred to as “classic” tabby, as if the
striped version were the mutation, not the other way around.
The reason for the ascendancy of the blotched pattern is still unknown. British cat owners do not prefer the blotched to the
striped coat, at least not to the extent that this could account for their proportions among British pets; in fact, when asked,
they express a small preference for striped tabby, perhaps simply because it is (now) relatively unusual. Blotched tabby
does not seem to provide any better or worse camouflage than striped, at least in the countryside. There has been a
suggestion that the pollution caused by the Industrial Revolution, coating British cities in soot, favored darker cats—both
blotched tabby and black—because they were less conspicuous, but this has never been confirmed.13 Still, we know that
almost all genes have multiple effects, even though most are named for the most obvious change they bring about.
Therefore, the blotched version of the gene possibly produces some other advantage, nothing to do with the coat, that
somehow suited cats to life in Britain.
We see the rise of the blotched tabby gene in Britain reflected in the proportion of blotched tabbies in former British
colonies all around the world. In the Northeastern United States—New York, Philadelphia, and Boston—settled in by
Europeans in the 1650s, only about 45 percent of cats carry the blotched tabby gene, but this is considerably more than in
originally Spanish-settled areas, such as Texas, at around 30 percent—where cats look much like those in Spain today. As
shown in the nearby figure, the Atlantic Provinces of Canada, settled some 100 years later, have more blotched tabbies.
European colonies settled in the nineteenth century are more variable: Hong Kong in particular has fewer than it should,
probably because there was already a striped-tabby population of Chinese origin there, thus diluting the effect of British
immigration. Australia on the other hand has more than it should, possibly the result of later waves of British immigrants
in the twentieth century bringing their cats with them. The proportion in Britain was over 80 percent in the 1970s, and
may have continued to rise since then.
How the percentage of the blotched tabby allele varied between locations colonized from Britain between 1650 and 1900,
compared to England in 1950
We can explain this trend by making two assumptions. The first is that the proportion of blotched tabbies in Britain has
been rising steadily since about 1500, for some reason unique to that country—otherwise the same change should have
taken place in, for example, New York, Nova Scotia, Brisbane, and Hong Kong, resulting in blotched tabbies reaching 80
percent of the population today in those places as well. The second is that once a population of cats becomes established
in a particular place, the proportion of blotched to striped doesn’t change. The latter assumption holds for other places and
variations in coat color, too, so it may be universal (see box below, “Reconstructing the Origins of the Cats of Humboldt
County, California”): however, given that it seems not to have occurred anywhere else, the rise of the blotched tabby in
Britain becomes even more curious.
Relatively few of the domestic cats that colonized South America from Spain and Portugal were blotched tabbies, so
scientists have had to turn to other variations in appearance to trace their history. Here also, it seems that once a
population of cats becomes established, its genetics doesn’t change much, even over several centuries. It seems plausible
that the cat population is set up when the initial (human) colonists bring their cats with them, and subsequent immigrants
only adopt cats from the local population. For example, in the nineteenth century, Catalans from Barcelona established
several towns along the river Amazon, and the cats of at least two of those towns, Leticia-Tabatinga and Manaus, remain
similar to the cats of Barcelona more than a century later.16
As in Hispanic-settled parts of the United States, and in Spain itself, orange and tortoiseshell cats are more common in
South America than in most other places. The exceptions, where these colors are even more common, are Egypt—
possibly one origin of this mutation (see the box on page 41, “Why Ginger Cats Are Usually Boys”); the islands off the
north and west coasts of Scotland; and Iceland. Researchers have attributed these exceptions to a hypothetical preference
for orange cats among the Vikings, who colonized those places around the ninth century CE, but it is unclear whether the
Vikings first obtained their orange cats from the eastern Mediterranean, or whether orange appeared spontaneously and
independently somewhere in Norway and was then transported around the Norwegian Sea in Viking ships.
Today, cats come in many colors and coat types, and many of these variations are undoubtedly the result of human
preferences—even leaving aside pedigree cats, whose breeding is strictly under human control. The basic colors of cats—
black, tabby (striped or blotched), ginger—are well established in populations all around the world, albeit in slightly
different proportions. These colors persist in cats that have gone feral, so they do not seem to produce any major
advantage—or disadvantage—to their wearers. However, many other variations in appearance seem to persist mainly
because people like them. Cats with white feet and variable amounts of white on the body (the gene that controls this is
rather imprecise in its effects) are less well-camouflaged than their plainer counterparts, putting the cat at a disadvantage
when hunting. However, many people prefer their cats to carry white patches, especially the black-and-white “tuxedo”
cat. Some people also favor cats that carry a gene that dilutes their coat color, such that black cats become a fetching
shade of gray (often referred to as “blue”), and other colors are a few shades lighter than usual.
Some people like long-haired cats. Although it seems self-evident that these cats, which like all cats cannot sweat through
their coats, should be at a natural disadvantage in warm climates, a recent study of Latin American cats indicates that
human preference is a much more potent factor than the weather, although a thick coat, such as that of the Maine Coon
and the Norwegian Forest Cat, is undoubtedly beneficial for outdoor cats in cold climates.17 In temperate climates, the
main disadvantage of long hair is not that the cat overheats, but that its coat easily becomes matted, which, if left
unattended, leads to infection or infestation of the skin. Long-haired cats are rarely seen in feral colonies, testifying to
their unsuitability for a life without attention from humans.
The striped tabby pattern evidently suits wildcats the best, possibly because it provides the most effective camouflage for
hunting. Presumably, mutations that affect appearance have occurred from time to time, but the wildcats that happened to
carry them fared worse than their “normal” counterparts, so the mutation quickly died out. For a domestic cat, camouflage
is probably less critical, allowing other coat colors to spread through the population.
A similar variety of colors and coat types has, of course, also become a feature of many other types of domesticated
animal—dogs, horses, and cattle, for example. In the case of the cat, external appearance reflects two distinct factors, both
of which affect how many offspring a cat is likely to have, and therefore how common its coat color and type are likely to
be in the next generation. The first is whether these factors impede the cat’s hunting ability; this may not be so important
today, but it certainly was in the past. The second is how appealing the cat is to its owner. Because human tastes vary
from person to person and culture to culture, this second factor has produced many variations.
What seems to be missing here is any trace of each cat’s own preferences. Although never studied directly, we have no
evidence for “color prejudice” among cats. Tabby females do not seem to spurn black toms in favor of other tabbies: a
white bib and socks seems to be neither an advantage nor a disadvantage when it comes to finding a mate, except perhaps
indirectly, if the cat’s conspicuousness has resulted in it catching less prey and thus looking less healthy than its better-
camouflaged rival. Whatever criteria cats use to choose a mate, coat color seems to be well down the list.
Even today, most domestic cats exert a remarkable amount of control over their own lives, significantly more than other
domestic animals, such as dogs. If we leave the pedigree breeds to one side for a moment (and these are still in a
minority), most cats go where they please and choose their own mates (unless they are neutered—a relatively recent
phenomenon). For this reason alone, cats cannot be considered completely domesticated.18 Full domestication means that
humankind has complete control over what an animal eats, where it goes, and most crucially, which individuals are
allowed to breed and which are not.
We certainly provide domestic cats with most or all of their food, but in this respect, too, cats are an anomaly. Science
classifies them as obligate carnivores, animals that have to obtain a mostly meat-based diet if they are to thrive (see box
on page 71, “Cats Are the True Carnivores”). For a female cat to breed successfully, she must have a high proportion of
flesh in her diet, especially in late winter, when she is preparing to come into season, and subsequently while she is
pregnant. The domestication of such an animal takes some explaining; until recently, meat and even fish formed only a
minor part of most people’s diets, and were often only seasonally available. Most domesticated animals will thrive on
foods that we ourselves cannot eat, unlocking sources of nourishment that would otherwise be unavailable to us: cows
turn grass, which we can’t digest, into milk and meat, which we can. (Though classified as carnivores, even dogs are
actually omnivores; they may prefer meat, but cereal-based foods can, if necessary, give them all the nutrition they need.)
For most of their coexistence with humankind, cats were valued primarily for their skill as hunters. Since mice contain all
the nourishment a cat needs, a successful hunter automatically ate a balanced diet; starvation was always possible for the
less adept or the unlucky, but diseases due to specific nutritional deficiencies were unlikely. However, even historically,
few domestic cats would have lived by hunting alone, most being provided with at least some food by their owners,
supplementing their diet by scavenging. So long as some of their diet came from fresh meat, usually in the form of prey
they had killed themselves, scavenging would not have tipped them into nutritional imbalance; still, giving up hunting
entirely would have been risky.
Cats do not scavenge at random; they have some ability to make informed choices. They subsequently avoid foods that
make them feel seriously and instantly ill. When eating food that they have not killed for themselves, they also
deliberately seek out a varied diet, thereby avoiding a buildup of anything that might make them sick long-term, but might
slip beneath the radar of immediate malaise.
To demonstrate this behavior, I laid out individual pieces of dry cat food on a grid on the ground, some of one brand,
some of another, and then allowed rescued strays to forage over them, one at a time. In this way, I could record in
precisely what order each cat picked up and ate the pieces of the two foods. When there were equal numbers of each type
of food, the cats roamed across the grid, eating both foods but more of whichever food they liked better. However, when
one of the two foods made up 90 percent of the total, every cat, whatever its preference, stopped grazing indiscriminately
within a couple of minutes and started actively seeking out the rarer food. Thus, these cats demonstrated a primitive
“nutritional wisdom,” as if they assumed that eating a variety of food was more likely to produce a balanced diet than
simply eating the food that was easiest to find (even though both the foods offered were nutritionally complete).20 When I
gave similar choices to pet cats that had always had a balanced diet, few responded this way, most continuing to eat
whichever of the two foods they liked better from the outset, or was just easy to find. Thus, although all cats probably
have the capacity to deliberately vary their diets, it seems that this ability must be “awakened” by some experience of
having to scavenge for a living, as most of the stray cats in our original experiment must have done before they were
rescued.21
Most other animals have more varied diets than cats’. Rats, the best-studied example, are omnivores with extremely wide
tastes that suit them perfectly for a scavenging lifestyle. They employ several strategies that enable them to pick the right
foods from the wide but unpredictable choices available to them. New foods are only nibbled at until the rat is sure that
they’re not poisonous. As soon as each food starts to be digested, the rat’s gut sends information to its brain about its
energy, protein, and fat content, enabling the rat to switch to another food with a different nutritional content if necessary.
Cats are much less sophisticated in this regard, having traveled a different evolutionary path based on mainly eating fresh
prey, which is by definition nutritionally balanced.
Given their limited ability to subsist by scavenging alone, cats were locked into the hunting lifestyle until as recently as
the 1980s. Until science revealed all of their nutritional peculiarities, it would have been a matter of luck whether a cat
that wasn’t able to hunt obtained a nutritionally balanced diet, unless its owner was both willing and able to give it fresh
meat and fish every day. Although commercial cat foods have been available for over a century, there was initially little
understanding that cats had very different requirements to dogs, and much of this food must have been nutritionally
unbalanced. Commercial cat food that is guaranteed to be nutritionally complete has been widely available for only thirty-
five years or so—only 1 percent of the total time since domestication began.
“Clipnosis”
Unlike much infantile behavior, in some cats the “scruff” reflex mother cats use when carrying their kittens persists into
adulthood. For those individuals, scruffing can be used as a gentle and humane method of quieting a fearful cat. The cat is
simply grasped firmly by the skin on the back of its neck and, if the reflex is triggered, may go into what appears to be a
trance, enabling it to be picked up and carried; its weight must be supported by the other hand. Veterinary nurses
sometimes use a hands-free version, applying a line of several clothespins to the area of skin between the top of the head
and the shoulders. By doing so, the nurses can complete an examination of the cat without causing it too much stress.3
Many mother cats try to move their litters at least once before they wean them, but science has yet to find out why. In the
wild, a mother cat will inevitably carry a few fleas, and because she has to spend so much time in the nest, flea eggs
accumulate there; when they become adult fleas three or four weeks later, a single hop will take them onto a kitten.
Removing the kittens from such an obvious source of infestation seems a good strategy and may be one explanation for
nest-moving, but so far no evidence has been found that this reduces the number of fleas that kittens carry. Nest-moving
may simply be a response to a disturbance that made the mother anxious, or may sometimes be strategic, bringing the
kittens closer to the source of food onto which their mother intends to wean them. The kittens may suffer if the mother cat
moves them too early or chooses the wrong new nest site. Kittens are vulnerable to becoming chilled, especially in damp
weather that also favors the transmission of respiratory viruses; many feral kittens, particularly those born in the autumn,
succumb to cat flu.
As my own experience shows, not all cats are innately skilled at parenthood. My cat Libby, having a nervous disposition,
was not the best mother. As the time for her to have her litter approached, we tried to keep her in the same room where
she had herself been born, presuming she would find the surroundings reassuring. But she was restless, patrolling the
house and looking into every open cupboard and drawer, as if undecided as to the safest location. At least she showed no
inclination to give birth outdoors. Eventually, she decided to produce her kittens within a few feet of where she herself
had come into the world.
We should not have relaxed: a few days later, we discovered the kittens scattered all around the house. For a few hours
after the birth, Libby lay with her three kittens and allowed them to suckle, but after that she appeared to lose interest,
spending too much time away from them. Her mother Lucy was intrigued by the kittens, but at this stage played no part in
looking after them. When we checked the kittens’ weights, they seemed to be growing, so we were not unduly worried
until Libby began to try to pick them up in her mouth and carry them away from the den we had built for her. Her
inexperience as a first-time mother showed immediately, as she grasped the kittens rather roughly by the head, only
occasionally and apparently accidentally gripping them correctly by their scruffs. Once she had gotten the hang of
carrying them, she began to look for places to hide them.
Without our intervention, Libby’s kittens would surely have perished. She would find a secluded place to hide the first
kitten, and then return to the other two, pick up another, and take that one somewhere else, ignoring the cries of the first.
After taking the third to yet another location, Libby would wander off as if unsure what to do next. Each time this
happened, we searched out all the kittens and put them back in the original den. Once or twice, we tried constructing a
new den—in the same room, but with completely new bedding so that it smelled nothing like the first, hoping this might
fool Libby into believing she had successfully moved the whole litter herself. Still, the removals persisted. Finally,
grandmother Lucy, her maternal instincts aroused, began to retrieve the kittens herself. Libby, perhaps sensing that she
should follow her mother’s lead, gradually gave up trying to move the litter. She continued to feed them, and they grew
stronger, but from then on it was Lucy who groomed them and kept them together until they were ready to be weaned.
Lucy seemed to know the kittens by their appearance and by their smell, but first-time mothers can behave as if they do
not immediately “know” what a kitten is. Rather, they respond to a single powerful stimulus that ensures that they take
care of their newborns. This is the kitten’s high-pitched distress call, uttered if it is cold, hungry, or out of contact with its
littermates. When the mother cat hears this sound coming from outside the nest, indicating that a kitten has strayed away,
she should instantly begin a search and, when she finds the kitten, retrieve it by its scruff. If the kittens are all in the nest
and calling together, her instinct is to lie down and encircle them with her paws, drawing them onto her abdomen and
allowing them to suckle. Gradually, over the course of their first couple of weeks of life, she seems to recognize the
kittens as independent animals, though whether she ever comes to know them as individuals is unclear.
The development of the senses and other critical events in the life of a kitten
This “social buffering” is probably adaptive for cats in the wild. A mother that is absent for long periods is probably
having difficulty finding enough food for her litter or to replenish her milk, so the kittens must start to learn about the
world soon if they are to stand any chance of survival. Kittens whose mother is usually with them in the nest can delay
learning about any perils that may await them, because they can rely on their mother to protect them.
The personalities of cats are powerfully affected by what they learn when they’re tiny kittens. Most kittens born in houses
will be looked after both by their mothers and by their mothers’ owners, but those unfortunate enough to undergo
prolonged stress in their first few weeks of life may grow up to have enduring emotional and cognitive problems. For
example, kittens that are abandoned by their mothers and are then hand-raised can become excessively attention-seeking
toward their first owners, though some subsequently seem to grow out of this. Based on what we know about other
mammals in similar situations, we can assume that after the mother’s departure, the kittens’ brains endure high levels of
stress hormones. These consistently high levels cause permanent changes in their developing brains and stress hormone
systems, such that they may overreact to unsettling events later in life.
Such cats may not make particularly satisfying pets, but this is by no means to say that they are mentally defective.
Rather, their apparently abnormal behavior is an evolved adaptation. A mother that has struggled to raise her kittens was
likely affected by some difficulty that has made food hard to find. A kitten whose mother has struggled to raise it
therefore expects to emerge into an uncertain world, in which it may have to live on its wits and thereby outcompete its
littermates and any other kittens born in the vicinity. The kittens of a relaxed, well-fed mother are likely to be able to
depend on a more stable world, one in which they will have the time to hone their social skills and reproduce several
times over a period of years. Such kittens, of course, are likely to make better pets than kittens that have been stressed in
early life.
As a kitten begins its third week, it embarks on the most crucial six weeks of its life, as far as its development is
concerned (see box on page 87, “Stages of Development”). From this point onward, its eyes, ears, and legs begin to
function reliably, and, guided by its hormones, it begins to make decisions about whom and what it should interact with,
and whom and what it should keep away from. At the same time, its brain is growing rapidly, every day adding thousands
of new nerve cells and millions of new connections among them, establishing the framework for storing all the new
knowledge it accumulates. Its mother is still a crucial influence during this period, but from now on a kitten gradually
becomes able to distinguish its littermates from one another, and to learn about the other animals around it, including
people. Kittens born in the wild also begin to learn how to hunt, for within a few short weeks they will need to feed
themselves.
Stages of Development
The way a cat reacts to the world around it develops for at least the first year of its life, but most of the crucial changes
take place in the first three or four months. Biologists divide this into four periods, each of which has a different
significance for the growing kitten.
During the prenatal period, especially the second month of the queen’s pregnancy, the kitten is largely—but not
entirely—isolated from the outside world. The composition of the amniotic fluid and the blood in the placenta both reflect
the mother’s environment. For example, if the mother eats a strongly flavored food during this period, the kittens may
prefer to be weaned onto a food with the same flavor, showing that they gain the ability to learn well before they emerge
into the world. Female kittens that are adjacent to male kittens in the womb absorb some of their testosterone and are
briefly more aggressive in their social play than kittens in all-female litters. Such inclinations are likely to be short-lived,
but more far-reaching changes are also possible. Based on what we know of other mammals, if the mother is highly
stressed during her pregnancy, her stress hormones may cross the placenta and impair the development of her kittens’
brains and endocrine systems.
During most of the neonatal period, from birth to about two and a half weeks of age, the kitten is deaf and blind, relying
on its senses of smell and touch to bond to its mother.
The socialization period begins as the eyes and ears open and begin to function in the third week of life, enabling the
kitten to start learning about the world around it, including the people that are caring for it and its mother. At the same
time, it learns to walk and then to run. When they are not sleeping, kittens spend much of this period engaged in play,
initially with one another and then increasingly with objects.
The beginning of the juvenile period, at eight weeks of age, coincides with the customary time to rehome a kitten (except
pedigree kittens, traditionally homed at thirteen weeks). By this point, the sensitive period for socialization is virtually
over. The juvenile period ends at sexual maturity, sometime between seven months and one year of age; many pet cats
will of course be neutered before the end of the first year.
Most of the kittens’ interactions with one another are playful, and for the first half of the socialization period, most of
their play is directed at other kittens. However, we do not know if early on each kitten recognizes that what it is playing
with is another kitten; most of the actions performed are similar to those used later toward objects. Bouts of play are short
and disorganized, and it may be that the movements of the other kitten trigger each attempt at play. By the time they are
six weeks old, though, kittens will play on their own with the objects around them, poking, pouncing, chasing, batting
them with their paws, and tossing them in the air. These are all actions adult cats use when they capture prey, so biologists
have looked for an elusive link between the amount of play that kittens perform and their hunting ability when they grow
up. While play with objects hones the kittens’ general coordination, it’s probably not the most important factor in
determining whether a cat grows up to be capable of catching enough prey to keep itself fed.
For feral kittens, their mother enables them to learn how to fend for themselves. As soon as they are old enough, she
brings recently killed prey back to the nest; as they become better coordinated, she brings back prey that is still alive. This
gives the kittens the opportunity both to handle prey and to find out what it tastes like. The mother doesn’t seem actively
to teach them how to deal with prey; rather, she simply places it in front of them and allows their predatory instincts to
take over. If they show no interest, she may draw attention to the prey by starting to feed on it herself, stopping when the
kittens begin to join in. Of course, this process rarely happens with owned cats, unless the mother happens to be an
accomplished and habitual hunter herself, in which case small, gory “presents” may find their way into the nest.
Whether a cat is destined to be a hunter or (more likely) not, among the most important events in a kitten’s life is when its
mother decides to start weaning it. This typically occurs in the fourth or fifth week of the kitten’s life, but may be earlier if
the litter is large—six or more kittens—or if the mother is unwell or stressed. Whatever the circumstances, the mother cat
drives this process; kittens rarely if ever decide to wean themselves. At the decisive moment, the mother starts to spend
time away from the kittens or simply blocks access to her milk by lying or crouching with her abdomen pressed firmly to
the ground. Not surprisingly, the kittens begin to get hungry, and for a few days their weight gain, steady since birth,
slows down or even stops. Hunger drives them to become much more inquisitive about other possible sources of food.
In the home, with human owners supplying the grub, the new source of food should be special kitten food. In the wild,
mother cats bring prey back to the nest and dissect it, making it easier for small mouths to chew. The kittens continue to
pester her for milk, but for the next couple of weeks or so she will ration them, to force them to develop their ability to
eat—and digest—meat. As their eating habits change, so do their insides. Meat takes longer to digest than milk, so
kittens’ intestines become lined with villi, small finger-shaped projections that increase the amount of nutrients that can be
absorbed. Lactase, the enzyme that breaks down milk sugar, is permanently replaced by sucrase to break down the sugars
in muscle, such that many adult cats find milk indigestible. The mother cat is simply being cruel to be kind: when the
kittens become fully weaned at about eight weeks of age, she may spontaneously allow them to suckle occasionally,
possibly as a way of reinforcing family ties—although in a domestic situation, they may no longer be with her.
Scientists have sometimes portrayed the weaning process as a conflict between mother and offspring. One theory holds
that an animal such as a cat that can have several litters in a lifetime should behave in a way that balances out the survival
of each of her litters. For example, the demands of an extra-large litter might become excessive, jeopardizing her own
health and therefore her chance of having any more litters. In some mammals, including mice and prairie dogs, mothers
with large litters may kill one or two of the weakest members, presumably to ensure the survival of the rest. However, this
tactic has never been recorded with cats, although a sickly kitten may simply be ignored by its mother, presumably
because it does not produce the right signals to induce her to care for it.
Each kitten must do its best to survive, regardless of whether it is its mother’s favorite; if it does not survive to maturity, it
will never leave any offspring of its own. Given that female cats tend toward promiscuity, no kitten can be sure that it is
closely related even to its littermates, let alone to any of the members of its mother’s next litter.8 It therefore has to put its
own interests first, even more so than if it could be sure that it shared both parents with its littermates. It therefore has no
incentive to give up suckling, even to the point of weakening its mother.
A mother cat cannot afford to be too hardhearted toward her offspring; after all, she cannot be at all certain that she will
have the chance to breed again. She therefore carefully assesses their needs, keeping them hungry enough to want to try
meat while not significantly compromising their health. For example, if her milk temporarily dries up before she’s
finished weaning them, she will start nursing them again before weaning is over, as soon as her milk comes in again; this
ensures that that they don’t miss out on any essential nutrients. Likewise, the kittens themselves cannot be too aggressive
in demanding milk from their mother, for (in the wild) they need to keep her on their side for several weeks longer, as she
teaches them essential hunting skills. Furthermore, if her milk supply starts to fail early, kittens increase the amount of
time they spend playing, presumably to prepare themselves for learning how to hunt. While most young animals—mice,
for example—play less when they’re hungry, presumably to conserve energy, for kittens play is preparation for hunting
behavior. Responding to their mother’s predicament, such kittens are thereby preparing themselves for a premature
independence.
Play prepares kittens not just for hunting, but also for getting along with other cats. If domestic cats were as solitary as
their ancestors, they would have little need for social graces. For animals whose individual contact is restricted to brief
courtship and mating, and then the raising of each litter by its mother, social play is unsophisticated and brief. For
domestic kittens, play with littermates becomes increasingly sophisticated as they get older, and no longer revolves
around the elements of hunting behavior.
By about six to eight weeks of age, kittens begin to use specific signals aimed at persuading their littermates to play with
them, such as rolling onto their backs (see “Belly-Up” in the drawing on page 92), placing their mouth over another
kitten’s neck (“Stand-Up”), or rearing onto their hind legs (“Vertical Stance”). By ten weeks—assuming the litter is still
together, since many kittens are homed at eight weeks old—each kitten will have come to learn the “correct” response to
each of these—Belly-Up for Stand-Up (and vice versa), and Belly-Up for Vertical Stance. As the kittens get older, play
tends to get rougher, and occasionally one of the kittens gets hurt. To avoid any confusion between play and a real fight,
kittens will use a “Play Face” to indicate their friendly intentions, particularly when in the vulnerable Belly-Up position.
They may also use special movements of their tails to signal playfulness, but so far no scientist has been able to decode
these. Older kittens also have a special signal showing when they want to stop playing: they arch their back, curl their tail
upward, and then leap off the ground.
If a litter is left together after the normal age for homing, social play occupies more and more of the kittens’ time, peaking
somewhere between nine and fourteen weeks of age. All this sophistication confirms that domestic kittens are designed to
become social adults, a process that begins when they are just a few weeks old and, unless interrupted, continues for
several months.
Surprisingly, we know little about the optimum time for cats to learn how to interact with other cats. Experiments
pinpointing the sensitive period for socialization to people have yet to be repeated to uncover the same for cat interaction,
but we can assume that there is more than one sensitive period, each tailored to the social environment in which the kitten
finds itself. The first period spans the kitten’s first two weeks, when kittens form their attachment to their mother, based
on olfaction.
Rolling on catnip
The behavior released by catnip is a bizarre mixture of play, feeding, and female sexual behavior, whether the cat itself is
male or female. Cats may first play with a catnip toy as if they think it is a small item of prey, but they quickly switch into
bouts of a seemingly ecstatic combination of face-rubbing and body-rolling, reminiscent of a female cat in season. Most
cats also drool and attempt to lick the catnip. This behavior may continue for several minutes at a time, until the cat
eventually recovers and walks away—but if the toy is left where it is, the cat may repeat the whole sequence, albeit with
less intensity, twenty or thirty minutes later.
A few other plants elicit the same response, notably the Japanese cat shrub or silver vine, and the roots of the kiwifruit
vine, which despite its name originated in southern China. In the 1970s, the first growers of kiwi vines in France learned
this when they found to their distress that cats had excavated and chewed their seedlings. All three plants contain similar
fragrant chemicals, thought to be responsible for the cats’ responses.
By some accident of evolution, these chemicals likely stimulate the cat’s nose to trigger circuits in the brain that would
never normally be activated at the same time, somehow bypassing the normal mechanisms that ensure that cats don’t
perform two incompatible actions at once. A cat in the throes of catnip-induced oblivion would seem vulnerable to attack,
and since cats presumably don’t get any lasting benefit from their experience, evolution should have weeded out the gene
responsible. Most species in the cat family, from lions to domestic cats, respond to these plants the same way, so the gene
must have evolved several million years ago. Why it did so remains a mystery.
Finding prey from the odors it produces can be difficult. Scent marks rarely indicate the current position of the animal that
left them, only where it was when it made the mark, perhaps hours earlier. In the sand experiment, the urine samples
continued to attract cats for at least a couple of days. It’s possible that cats, as sit-and-wait hunters, use the scent marks to
see whether they will attract other members of the same species that made them. Mice use urine marks to signal to other
mice, and the marks contain a great deal of useful information about the mouse that produced them. Thus, the scentmarker
doesn’t put itself at risk, so much as other members of the same species that show up to examine the mark.
In addition, odors spread out from their sources in ways that are not predictable. We know intuitively that light travels in
straight lines but not around obstacles, and that sound travels in all directions, including around obstacles. However,
because we rely so little on odor to give us directional information, it’s not immediately obvious what problems animals
face when determining where an odor is emanating from. Of course, odors outdoors are carried by the air—downwind,
not upwind—but air movements close to the ground, where cats operate, are usually highly complex. While the wind may
be blowing in a consistent direction a few yards above the ground, friction caused by its contact with the ground and
especially with vegetation causes it to break up into eddies of various sizes. These carry “pockets” of odor away from the
source, so that a cat somewhere downwind of a mouse’s nest will get intermittent bursts of mouse smell.
Tracing these bursts to their source, especially in thick cover, is likely to require diligent searching and possibly some
backtracking. Once a cat has located a source of mouse odor, it is potentially able to use the fact that odors don’t travel
upwind to position itself downwind of the odor source so its own smell can’t be detected by the mouse, and then wait for
mice to turn up. While sit-and-wait is a well-documented feline hunting method, we do not know whether cats routinely
prefer patrolling the downwind sides of, for example, hedgerows to avoid their own scent betraying their presence.
However, it seems likely that a predator as smart as a cat could quickly learn this tactic, even if it is not instinctive.
Cats possess a second olfactory apparatus, which humans lack: the vomeronasal organ (VNO; also called the Jacobson’s
organ).7 A pair of tubes, the nasopalatine canals, run from the roof of the cat’s mouth, just behind the upper incisors, up to
the nostrils; connected roughly halfway up each one of these tubes is a sac, the VNO itself, filled with chemical receptors.
Unlike the nose, the entire VNO is full of fluid, so odors must be dissolved in saliva before they can be detected.
Moreover, the ducts connecting the VNOs to the canals are only about one-hundredth of an inch wide, so thin that the
odors must be pumped in and out of the sacs by a dedicated set of tiny muscles.8 This gives the cat precise control over
when it uses the VNO—unlike the nose, which automatically receives odor every time the cat breathes. Thus, the VNO’s
function lies somewhere between our senses of smell and taste. Appreciating how cats make use of this faculty requires a
great leap of imagination.
Cats, unlike dogs, perform an obvious facial contortion when bringing the VNO into play. They pull their top lip upward
slightly, uncovering the top teeth, while the mouth is held partially open. This pose is usually held for several seconds: it’s
sometimes known as the “gape” response, although it’s usually referred to by its German name, “Flehmen.” Researchers
theorize that during this pose, the tongue is squeezing saliva up into the canals, from where the pumping mechanism
delivers it to the VNO.
Cats perform Flehmen exclusively in social situations, so by implication they must use their VNO to detect the smells of
other cats.9 Male cats perform it after sniffing urine marks left by females, including during courtship, and female cats
will do the same toward urine marks left by tomcats, although usually only if the tom is not present.
The cat’s VNO can probably detect and analyze a wide range of “smells,” since it contains at least thirty different kinds of
receptors; more than the dog’s, which has only nine. These receptors are distinct from those found in the nose, and are
connected to their own dedicated area of the brain, known as the accessory olfactory bulb.
Cat displaying “Flehmen”—using its vomeronasal organ to detect the odor left by a cat that has cheek-rubbed the twig
Why do cats—and, indeed, most mammals, apart from primates—need two olfactory systems? The answer seems to
change from species to species. Mice have highly sophisticated VNOs, with several hundred receptor types and two
distinct connections to the accessory olfactory bulb, rather than the cat’s one; the odorants mice pick up regulate
reproduction, as well as enabling recognition of every other mouse in the neighborhood from its unique odor
“fingerprint.” In many species, some odor communication takes place through the VNO and some through the nose. For
example, in rabbits, chemical communication between adults involves the VNO, but the scent emanating from the mother
that stimulates her kits to suckle is picked up by their noses. Sometimes the balance between the two changes as the
animal matures: the VNO and the nose are used in tandem during a guinea pig’s first breeding season, but the following
year its nose alone will suffice. Although cats have not been studied in this amount of detail, it’s feasible that they also
interpret olfactory information in a flexible way.
If we accept that the VNO is primarily designed to analyze odors coming from other members of the same species, then
the fact that dogs are generally more sociable than cats doesn’t really fit the fact that their VNOs are less discriminating.
Dogs, descended from social ancestors, conduct much of their relationships with other dogs face to face, and thus use
visual cues to confirm who another dog is and what it is likely to do next, so their VNOs may not be required very often.
The domestic cat’s solitary ancestors only rarely had the opportunity to meet one another; the exceptions are males when
they are courting females, and females interacting with their litters for a few months. In the wild, much of the social life of
cats must be conducted through scent marks, which can be deposited for another cat to sniff days, sometimes weeks later.
Since wild cats rarely get to meet other members of their own species, any information they can pick up from scent marks
is crucial for making decisions on how to act when they do encounter each other. Most critically for the survival of her
offspring, a female cat must assess her various male suitors, who themselves have been attracted by the change in her own
scent as she comes into season. She may already have gained useful information about each of them by sniffing the scent
marks they have left as they roamed through her territory, information she can use to supplement what she can see of their
condition and behavior when she finally meets them. She may also be able to distinguish those that are unrelated to her
from those that are—perhaps a son who has roamed away and then happened to return to the area a few years later—
thereby avoiding inbreeding. Scientists have not yet studied any of these possibilities in cats, but we know them to occur
in other species.
Cats’ sense of smell has evolved not purely for hunting, but also for social purposes. Successful hunting was, until
domestication, crucial to the survival of every individual cat. However, it cannot in itself ensure the survival of an
individual cat’s genes; that also requires an effective mating strategy. Each female cat tries to select the best male for her
purposes every time she mates to ensure that her genes pass down to successive generations. Ideally, she should take a
long-term view, trying to gauge not only how likely her own offspring are to survive, but also how successful they are
likely to be when they in turn become old enough to breed. If she picks the strongest and healthiest male(s) to mate with,
then her male kittens are likely to be strong and healthy also when they are old enough to mate. She will of course be able
to make a judgment based on her suitors’ appearance, but she may be able to obtain a better idea of how healthy they are
based on their odor. Her sense of smell may thus provide her with extra information to make these crucial mating
decisions.
Cats probably perceive most of the scents that have social meaning using the nose and the VNO in tandem. While both
may be needed the first time a particular smell is encountered—for example, the first time a young male detects a female
in estrus—on subsequent occasions either one will do the job, the brain presumably using memories of previous
encounters to “fill in” the missing input.
Like dogs, cats pay a great deal of attention to scent marks left by other cats, both those carried in urine and those that
they rub onto prominent objects, using the glands around the mouth. To distinguish it from the rubbing that cats perform
on people and on one another, which is mainly a tactile display, this is sometimes referred to as “bunting,” a term whose
origins are obscure. Cats’ faces have numerous scent-producing glands, one under the chin, one at each corner of the
mouth, and one beneath the areas of sparse fur between eye and ear, while the pinna itself produces a characteristic odor.
We know little about how cats use these scent marks, but they certainly display an interest in the scents other cats
produce. For example, male cats can distinguish between females at different stages in their estrus cycle based on their
facial gland secretions alone. Each gland produces a unique blend of chemicals, some of which have even been used in
commercial products that can have a positive effect on reducing stress in anxious cats.10
Apart from the social role of the VNO, and to a lesser extent the nose, all the cat’s other senses are exquisitely tuned to
the hunting lifestyle of their ancestors. They have quite an arsenal at their disposal: they can locate prey visually, their
eyes effective in the half-light of early dawn and late dusk; aurally, detecting high-pitched squeaks and rustles; or
olfactorily, through detecting the odors that rodents leave in scent marks. As they approach their prey, cats’ exquisite
sense of balance and the sensory hairs on their cheeks and elbows allow them to do so silently and stealthily. As they
pounce, the whiskers on their faces sweep forward to act as a short-range radar, guiding the mouth and teeth to precisely
the right place to deliver the killing bite. Cats evolved as hunters, something domestication has done little to change.
What the cat senses is only half the story. Their brains have to make sense of the vast amounts of information that their
eyes, ears, balance organs, noses, and whiskers produce, and then turn that information into action, whether correcting the
cat’s balance as it tiptoes along the top of a fence, deciding on the precise moment to pounce on a mouse, or checking the
yard for the scent of cats that have visited during the night. The sheer volume of data that each sense organ generates has
to be filtered every waking second. An analogy might be the vast banks of TV screens and monitors at NASA
headquarters during the launch of a spacecraft: at any one moment, only a minute fraction of what they display is
important, and it takes a highly trained observer to know which ones to watch and which can safely be ignored.
Unfortunately at present we know much less about how sensory information is processed than how it is generated.
The size and organization of the cat’s brain can give us some clues as to their priorities in life. The basic form of the felid
brain, as shown by the shape of the skull, evolved at least 5 million years ago. Some parts of the brain, especially the
cerebellum, are disproportionally geared to processing information relating to balance and movement, reflecting cats’
prowess as athletes. While this is apparently contradicted by the occasions that cats get stuck up trees, the problem here is
not their intelligence or their sense of balance, but rather that their claws all face forward, so they cannot be used as brakes
when descending. The part of the cortex that deals with hearing is well-developed; so too, as we have seen, are the
olfactory bulbs.
In cats, the parts of the brain that seem to be important in regulating social interactions are also less well-developed than
they are in the most social members of the Carnivora, such as the wolf and the African hunting dog. This is unsurprising,
given the solitary lifestyle of the domestic cat’s immediate ancestors. Nevertheless, domestic cats are remarkably
adaptable in their social arrangements; some form deep attachments with people, and others remain in a colony with other
cats for their entire lives, their only interactions with humans consisting of running away and hiding. Once made, these
choices cannot be reversed, since they are set during socialization: the cat as a species can adapt to a number of social
environments, but individual cats generally cannot. This lack of flexibility must ultimately lie in the way that their brains
are constructed, and in particular, the parts of their brains that process social information. Science has yet to unravel the
factors that lie behind these constraints, so that today’s cats have limited options when faced with changes in their social
milieu.
CHAPTER 6
Cats can recognize outlines even when they are broken up or unusual. They know the difference between pictures that
produce an illusion, like the “falling square” in the three pictures at top left, and those that don’t, like the three at bottom
left. They also recognize the negative image as a bird.
Cats can also make sophisticated judgments on how large or small something is. If they are trained to pick out the smallest
or largest of three objects, they continue to pick out the smallest when all three objects are made smaller, so that what was
originally the small object is no longer the smallest of the three. Prey will appear larger or smaller depending on how far
away it is, so making a judgment about relative size is important in deciding whether to run away (from a large rat, still
some distance away) or attack (a small rat, close by). Mysteriously, cats also seem to classify shapes according to whether
they are closed—for example, a filled circle or square—or open—for example, an uppercase I or U. We do not know why
this skill evolved as it did, since its contribution to cats’ survival is obscure.
That all these examples relate to vision is a consequence of our own biases: because we are a visual species, scientists
tend to focus on an animal’s visual abilities to get an idea of how their brains work. Cats must also be able to classify
what they hear, and although we don’t know how they categorize sounds, we can guess from their hunting behavior that
they probably have categories for each of the sounds made by the various species they prey upon. Presumably they also
have categories for the odors that they pick up with their noses and their vomeronasal organs, but with our comparatively
poor sense of smell, we have trouble imagining how such a system might work.
Humans also categorize events by when they happened, but cats probably do not. We know little about cats’ conception of
time, but they are definitely much better at judging short durations than long ones. Cats have been successfully trained to
discriminate sounds that last four seconds from those that last five, and also to delay their response to a cue for a few
seconds (because they only get the reward if they wait for the correct time).4 However, cats are poor at discriminating
longer periods of time, and their perception is likely limited to the few seconds provided by their working memory. We
have no evidence to suggest that cats can spontaneously recall memories and place those events as having happened a few
days ago, as opposed to a few hours or weeks previous—something we find easy to do.
Cats have a general sense of the rhythm of the day. They have a free-running daily rhythm that is reset every day by the
onset of daylight, and they also take other cues from their environment about what time of day it is. Some are natural,
such as the sun rising and setting, and some are learned, such as their owner feeding them at roughly the same time every
day. Still, they don’t seem to think about time passing, in the way that we do.
O nce a cat has worked out what it is observing through sight, smell, or hearing, it must work out what to do next. If its
survival might be threatened, the cat may need to act first and think later. When a cat is startled, say by a sudden loud
noise, it instantly prepares itself for action through a set of preprogrammed and coordinated reflexes. It crouches, ready to
run if necessary. Its pupils dilate while its eyes quickly focus as closely as possible, regardless of whether there is
anything there to focus on; this presumably maximizes the chances of pinpointing the threat if it is close by. If the threat is
still far away, then the cat has less urgency to identify what it is.
Almost every other reaction a cat makes changes with experience: over time, its reactions change. Even the startle reflex
gradually wanes and may eventually disappear, say if the same loud noise is repeated over and over again. In this process,
habituation, something initially excites the cat, but then becomes progressively less interesting until it eventually evokes
no reaction at all.
For example, cats are renowned for quickly getting “bored” with toys. Intrigued as to why this should be, in 1992 I set up
a research project at the University of Southampton to look into cats’ motivation for playing with objects. Do they literally
“play” for the sheer fun of it, as a child might, or are their intentions more “serious”? The manner in which cats play with
toys is highly reminiscent of the way that they attack prey, so we designed our experiments with the presumption that
whatever was going on in their heads, it was probably related to their hunting instincts. My graduate student Sarah Hall
and I found that habituation is the main underlying reason for this apparent boredom. We presented cats with toys—
mouse-sized, fake-fur-covered “pillows” tied to a piece of cord—and at first they usually played intensely, appearing to
treat the toy as if it was indeed a mouse. However, many cats stopped playing within a matter of a couple minutes. When
we took the toys away for a while and then presented them again, most of the cats started playing again, but neither as
intensely nor for as long as the first time. By the third presentation, many of the cats would scarcely even begin to play.
They clearly became “bored” with the toy.
If we switched the toy for a slightly different one—a different color (say, black to white, since cats’ perception of colors is
different from ours), texture, or odor—almost all of the cats would start playing again. Thus, they were “bored” not by the
game, but by the toy itself. In fact, the frustration of being offered the same toy repeatedly actually increased their desire
to play. If the interval between the last game with the original toy and the first game with the new toy was about five
minutes, they attacked the second toy with even more vigor than they did the first one.5
To understand why playing with a toy would make a cat frustrated, we considered what might motivate cats to play in the
first place. Kittens sometimes play with toys as if they were fellow kittens, but adult cats invariably treat toys as if they
were prey: they chase, bite, claw, and pounce on toys just as if the toys were mice or rats. To test the idea that cats think
of toys in the same way they think of prey, we tried different kinds of toys to see which ones cats prefer. Our findings
showed that, unsurprisingly, they like mouse-sized toys that are furry, feathered, or multi-legged—toy spiders, for
example. Even indoor cats that had never hunted showed these preferences, so they must be hardwired in the cat’s brain.
The cats played with rat-sized toys covered in fake fur in a subtly different way from the mouse-sized toys. Instead of
holding them in their front paws and biting them, most cats would hold the rat-sized toys at arm’s length and rake them
with their hind claws—just as hunting cats do with real rats. The cats were apparently thinking of their toys as if they
were real animals, and as if their size, texture, and any simulated movement (such as our pulling on the toy’s string) had
triggered hunting instincts.
We then examined whether a cat’s appetite has similar effects on the way it hunts and the way it plays with toys. If cats
play with toys just for their own amusement, as many people assume they do, then they should be less inclined to play
when they are hungry, since their minds should be focused instead on how to get something to eat. Conversely, as a
hunting cat gets hungrier, it will hunt more intensely and become more inclined to take on larger prey than usual. We
found exactly the latter when we offered toys to our cats. If their first meal of the day had been delayed, they played more
intensely than usual with a mouse-sized toy—for example, biting it more frequently. Moreover, many of the cats that
normally refused to play with a rat-sized toy at all were now prepared to attack it.6 This convinced us that adult cats do
think that they are hunting when they’re playing with toys.
Cats don’t easily get “bored” with hunting, so we were still puzzled as to why our cats stopped playing with most toys so
quickly. Indeed, they appeared to get “bored” with most commercially available toys and with the kinds of toys we made
for our first experiments. The few toys that sustained our cats’ interest all shared one quality: they fell apart as the cat was
playing with them.7 Although we had to abandon experiments that involved these toys, which came apart at the seams as
our cats batted them about, we noticed that several of the cats were extremely reluctant to give them up. We then realized
that our original swapping experiments mimicked one aspect of what happens when a cat rips a toy apart: when we
exchanged the toy for a slightly different one, the cat’s senses told it that the toy had changed. It didn’t seem to matter to
the cat that it had not caused the change itself; what was important was that a change seemed to have occurred.
We deduced that not only do cats think they are hunting when they’re playing with toys, but their behavior is being
controlled by the same four mechanisms whether they’re hunting or playing. One of these mechanisms is affected by
hunger, and the same one that makes a cat more likely to play with a toy makes it likely to make a kill when it’s hungry.8
The second is triggered by the appearance—and presumably the smell and sound—of prey, and certain specific features,
such as fur, feathers, and legs, that the cat recognizes instinctively are likely to belong to prey animals. The third
mechanism is affected by the size of the toy or prey. Attacking a mouse puts the cat in much less danger than attacking a
rat, so the cat attacks the rat much more carefully; likewise, cats treat large toys much more circumspectly than small toys,
as if they were capable of fighting back. Even though cats should quickly learn that the toys are unlikely to retaliate, most
cats don’t seem to do so. The fourth mechanism is the source of the cat’s apparent frustration: if all that biting and
clawing doesn’t seem to have any effect on its target, then either the target wasn’t a meal, or if it is prey, then it’s proving
difficult to subdue. A toy that starts to disintegrate, or is taken away but looks different when it comes back (as in our
original experiment), mimics the early stages of a kill, thus encouraging the cat to persist.
Overall, many of the cat’s hunting tactics can be explained in terms of simple reflexes, modified by emotion—
specifically, the fear of getting hurt by large prey animals—and habituation, which ensures that the cat continues to
grapple with its prey only if it is likely to end up with a meal. However, these are only the basic building blocks of
hunting behavior; cats undoubtedly perfect their hunting skills through practice, by learning how to assemble the various
elements in the most productive ways.
H abituation can explain many short-term changes in a cat’s behavior, but its effects wear off after a few minutes; longer-
term, more permanent changes in the way a cat reacts require a different explanation. These must be based on learning
and memory.
Fundamentally, cats learn the same way as dogs, even though dogs are self-evidently much easier to train. Two factors lie
behind this difference between cats and dogs. First, most cats do not find human attention rewarding in its own right,
whereas dogs do; we therefore train cats using food as a reward, rather than affection. Second, dogs instinctively behave
in ways we can easily shape into something useful: for example, the herding behavior of a sheepdog is composed of
elements from the hunting behavior of the wolf, the dog’s ancestor. Cat behavior features little that we can usefully refine
by training, except for our own amusement. Obviously, we have benefited from the cat’s hunting abilities for ages.
However, we usually leave cats to their own devices: they will seek out the mice that invade our grain stores regardless of
whether we want them to do so. Dogs, on the other hand, are specialized for cooperative hunting of much larger prey.
They are a nuisance when unsupervised and useful only when trained. We take on the responsibility of gearing their
attention toward particular prey, when that is what we need from them.
Much of what cats learn is based on two fundamental psychological processes: classical and operant conditioning. Both of
these involve new associations forming in the cat’s mind. The first involves two events that regularly occur closely
together in time; the second involves something the cat does or does not do, and a predictable consequence of that action,
which may be good for the cat (a reward), or bad (a punishment). Because cats seem to have little or no instinctive
appreciation either of how humans behave or the best ways to interact with them, virtually all their dealings with us are
built up through this sort of learning.
Classical conditioning is also known as Pavlovian conditioning, after Ivan Pavlov, the first scientist to map out how such
learning works in a series of experiments with dogs in the 1890s. In fact, his principles apply equally well to cats.9 A
hungry cat that smells food will instinctively seek it out and then eat it. For a wild cat, food is the result of a successful
hunting expedition; for a pet cat, the owner makes this trip unnecessary by buying food at the supermarket and presenting
it to the cat. Cats don’t need to learn that food can appear without being preceded by hunting, because this is precisely
what happens when, in the wild, their mother brings food back to the nest. What they do learn, via classical conditioning,
are the cues that indicate that food is on its way—for example, the sound of a can opener. In psychologist’s jargon, this
action by the owner is the conditioned stimulus, which becomes associated in the cat’s mind with the unconditioned
(“instinctive”) stimulus, the smell of the food. Nothing in the cat’s evolution has prepared it to respond automatically to
the sound of a can opener: the association is something that every cat would have to learn for itself. Of course, this is
hardly a difficult lesson, nor is the underlying process complex; scientists have found such behavior even in bees and
caterpillars. Nevertheless, classical conditioning is the main way that cats find out how the world around them is
constructed: which parts occur in predictable sequences, and which do not.
The result—the unconditioned response—doesn’t have to be a “reward,” such as food. In fact, learning occurs more
quickly when it helps the animal to avoid something unpleasant or painful. A cat that is attacked by another, larger cat
will certainly experience fear and possibly pain, and will instinctively try to run away. It will also probably remember
what the attacking cat looked like, associating its appearance with the unpleasant feelings it experienced at the time.10 The
next time it sees that cat, it will feel the fear before any attack takes place—and may immediately run away, as it
ultimately did the first time they met. However, relatively sophisticated animals such as cats can respond flexibly: they do
not automatically have to perform the original response simply because stimuli are similar. In this way, the cat may not
run away immediately, but instead “freeze,” hoping to avoid detection, having previously learned that running away can
invite a chase.
This simple learning has one major constraint: the events that a cat associates together must occur either at precisely the
same moment or no more than a second or two apart. Say a cat has done something that its owner doesn’t like—for
example, depositing a dead mouse on the floor. The cat’s owner finds the mouse several minutes after the cat has left it
there and shouts at the cat. In this case, classical conditioning does not link the two events together: rather, it links the
unpleasantness of being shouted at with whatever happened immediately before the shout—probably the owner’s arrival
in the room.
This rule has one exception: if a cat eats something that makes it feel ill, it will thereafter avoid foods with the same
flavor. Moreover, forging this association requires only one such experience. This food-aversion learning differs from
classical conditioning both in the speed with which the lesson is learned and in the delay between the sensation—the odor
of the food—and the consequence—the upset stomach. It is obviously in the cat’s best interest to avoid repeating any
action that could kill it, which accounts for the irreversibility of this learning. Likewise, there would be little point in the
cat associating its first feelings of sickness with something else that occurred at the same time—the problem food would
have been eaten many minutes, even hours, before. Nevertheless, this is still classical conditioning, except that the “rule”
for the time frame has been both extended and made much more specific, to the flavor of the last food that the cat ate
before feeling ill; other cues, such as the characteristics of the room where it ate the meal, are ignored as irrelevant. Of
course, feeling nauseous can also be a symptom of an infection unconnected with something the cat ate, so this
mechanism occasionally has unexpected consequences: a cat that succumbs to a virus may then go off its regular food
even after it has recovered, because it has incorrectly associated the illness with the meal that happened to precede it.
Cats can also learn spontaneously, when there is no obvious reward or penalty involved. This becomes especially useful
when they are building up a mental map of their surroundings. A cat will learn that a particular shrub it passes every day
has a particular smell. If the cat sees a similar shrub elsewhere, it will expect that shrub to have the same odor as the first
one. If it turns out not to—perhaps because an unfamiliar animal has scent-marked it—the cat will give it an especially
thorough inspection. Such “behaviorally silent” learning can be explained by classical Pavlovian learning—that is, if the
cat spontaneously feels rewarded by the information it has gained. In other words, cats are programmed to enjoy their
explorations; otherwise, they wouldn’t learn anything from them.
This kind of learning allows cats to relax in what must be, for them, the highly artificial indoor environments we provide
for them. Domesticated cats are happy once they have been able to set up a complete set of associations between what
each feature of that environment looks, sounds, and smells like. This explains why cats immediately pay attention to
anything that changes—move a piece of furniture from one side of the room to the other, and your cat, finding that its
predictable set of associations have been broken, will feel compelled to inspect it carefully before it can settle down again.
To cope with such changes, cats can gradually unlearn associations that no longer work, a process known technically as
extinction.
All animals make some associations more easily than others; evolution has built in certain responses that are difficult to
overwrite. For cats, one of these is the likelihood that high-pitched sounds are likely to come from prey. In one
experiment, two Hungarian scientists taught some rats that whenever a high-pitched clicking sound came from a
loudspeaker at one end of a corridor, a piece of food would appear at the other end, about seven feet away. The rats had to
run quickly from the speaker to the food dispenser, otherwise the food would disappear. They learned this quickly and
reliably, such that they would sit and wait for the clicking noise, and then on hearing it, run in the correct direction. To the
experimenters’ surprise, cats took a great deal longer to learn the same exercise: many of the cats required hundreds of
repetitions before they would perform reliably. They quickly learned that food was available in the corridor when the
clicking sound occurred, but almost invariably started running toward the sound rather than away from it in the direction
of the food. While they were still learning the task, some of the cats appeared to become so conflicted that they refused to
eat the food even if they reached it in time. Even when they had completely learned the task, they often glanced briefly at
where the sound was coming from before proceeding toward the food, something the rats had given up doing at an early
stage.11
The difference between the cats’ and the rats’ performance in this task does not mean that rats are smarter than cats.
Rather, for a hungry cat, high-pitched noises are too important to ignore. For the rats, the clicking noise was an arbitrary
cue, just as the sound of a can opener is for a cat; such a sound only comes to have meaning because of its association
with the arrival of food. For the cats, the clicking noise instinctively meant “Food is likely to be over here,” something
they had great difficulty in learning to ignore.
Like other animals, cats can learn to perform a particular action every time a particular situation occurs. This forms the
basis for much of animal training, and is technically known as operant conditioning. Contrary to popular myth, cats can be
trained, but few people bother, apart from the professionals who produce performing cats for movies and TV. Cats are
much more difficult to train than dogs are for at least three reasons. First, their behavior shows less intrinsic variety than
that of dogs, so there is less raw material with which to work. Training any animal to do something that it would never do
naturally is a difficult process; most training consists of changing the cues that lead to a piece of normal behavior, rather
than inventing a piece of behavior the animal has never performed in its life. Second, and perhaps most important, cats are
less naturally attentive toward people than dogs are. Domestic dogs have evolved to be exceptionally observant of what
people want from them, because virtually every use that humankind has ever had for them has favored dogs that could
interpret human behavior from those that could not. Cats are surprisingly good at following simple pointing gestures, but
when they encounter a problem they cannot solve, they tend not to look to their owners for help—something that dogs
automatically do.12 Third, although dogs are powerfully rewarded by simple physical contact from their owner, few cats
are: professional cat trainers generally have to rely on food rewards. These trainers also use secondary reinforcers
extensively, rewards that are initially arbitrary but that become rewarding through association with the arrival of food.
Nowadays, cat owners can emulate this using training aids such as clickers (see nearby box, “Clicker Training”).
Cats can be trained to perform normal behavior out of its usual context. In the process known as shaping, the cat is
initially rewarded for any behavior approximating the desired result that it performs immediately after the cue given by its
trainer. Only behavior that is close to the result is rewarded, until finally only exactly the right response gets the reward.
To take a simple example, most cats will (sensibly!) not jump over an obstacle if they can walk around it. To train a cat to
jump on command, the trainer rewards it for walking over a stick that is lying on the ground, and then for stepping over it
when it is raised slightly. Then if the trainer raises the stick further, she rewards the cat only for actual jumps. Once the
habit is established, trainers can ingrain it further by rewarding only some successful performances and not others. This
may seem counterintuitive, but animals usually concentrate harder when they know that the outcome is slightly uncertain
than when it is guaranteed. Humans show this same quality under some circumstances—behavior that is perfectly
exploited in the payout schedule of slot machines.
Clicker Training
Like most animals (with the notable exception of dogs), cats can be trained only with food rewards. However, delivering
the reward to the cat at exactly the right moment to reinforce the desired piece of behavior can be tricky; also, the cat may
be distracted from what it is supposed to be learning by the smell of the food “concealed” in the trainer’s hand. Cats can
be trained much more easily if a secondary reinforcer is used—a distinctive cue that signals to the cat that a piece of food
is on its way, and instantly making it feel good, thereby reinforcing the performance of whatever it was doing when the
cue appeared.
Although in principle almost anything could be used as a secondary reinforcer, in practice distinctive sounds are the most
convenient and practical, partly because they can be timed very precisely, and partly because the cat cannot avoid
perceiving them even if it is some distance away and looking in the opposite direction. Animal trainers used to use
whistles, but nowadays the cue of choice is often the clicker, a tensioned piece of metal in a plastic case that makes a
distinctive click-clack sound when pressed and released.
Clicker-training a cat
Cats must be taught to like the sound of the clicker, which has little or no instinctive significance for them, by classical
conditioning. This is simply done by attracting the cat’s attention with a handful of its favorite treats when it’s hungry,
and then offering the treats one at a time, each preceded with one click-clack from the clicker (Some cats are
hypersensitive to metallic sounds—either hold the clicker away from the cat, or use a quieter sound, for example the
plunger on a retractable ballpoint pen). After a few sessions, the sound of the clicker will have become firmly associated
in the cat’s mind with something pleasant, and this sound will gradually become pleasant in its own right.
Once this has become established, the clicker can be used to reward other pieces of behavior. For example, many cats can
be trained to come consistently when called, initially by clicking when they turn and start to approach, but then gradually
delaying the click until the cat arrives at the trainer’s feet. Once the click has been established as a reward, it doesn’t have
to be followed by food every single time, although if the cat hears the sound over and over again without food appearing,
the association may start to be lost. It is therefore usually most effective to intersperse sessions of training to come when
called (when it is impossible to deliver the reward immediately after the click, because the cat is too far away) with repeat
sessions of the original click-treat pairings that reestablish the link.
Instructions on how to clicker-train cats can be found at www.humanesociety.org/news/magazines/2011/05-
06/it_all_clicks_together_join.html.
More complex tricks and “performances” are usually taught piece by piece, each step becoming linked together in the
cat’s mind through chaining. The easiest way to put a sequence together is to start at the end with the final action and its
reward, and progressively add the preceding steps—backward chaining. For example, to train a cat to turn around once
and then offer its paw to be shaken, the paw-shake is shaped first, and once that is perfected, the turn is shaped to precede
it. Although it would seem more logical to train the first action first—forward chaining—most animals, including cats,
find this much more difficult, indicating that their abilities to think ahead are limited.
Operant conditioning is not confined to deliberate training; it is one way that cats learn how to deal with whatever
surroundings they find themselves in. Cats have not (yet) evolved to live in apartments; their instinctive behavior is still
tuned to hunting in the open air. The fact that they can adapt to indoor living is testament to their learning abilities. Not
only can they make sense of their surroundings by means of associations built up by classical conditioning, they are also
able to learn how to manipulate objects around them to get what they want.
For example, many cats learn how to open a door fitted with a lever-type handle by jumping up and grabbing it with their
forepaws. A superficially “clever” trick such as this one can be explained by operant conditioning. Of course, doors that
unlatch with a handle and swing open on hinges do not form part of the world in which cats have done most of their
evolving, so the final, successful, version of this behavior cannot be natural. However, it most likely starts with something
that cats will do instinctively when they are unable to get somewhere they want to go, which is to jump up onto a vantage
point to see whether there is an alternative route. If the cat tries to jump up onto the lever, which from the ground will
look like a fixed platform, it will find that when the lever moves, not only does it lose its footing, but also that the door
may swing open. The cat then gets the reward of being able to explore the room on the other side of the door—and cats, as
territorial animals, find exploration of novel areas rewarding in itself. The cat will remember the association between its
action and the reward. After trying various alternative actions on the handle, it will progressively arrive at the most
efficient solution, which is to raise a single paw and gently pull the lever downward.
Pet cats learn how to use the same techniques on their owners. Even the most ardent cat-lovers sometimes describe them
as manipulative, but much of a cat’s so-called manipulative behavior is built up by operant conditioning. Feral cats are
remarkably silent compared to domestic cats (except during fighting and courtship, notoriously noisy activities); in
particular, such cats rarely meow at one another, whereas the meow is the pet cat’s best-known call. The meow is usually
directed at people, so rather than being an evolved signal it’s more likely to have been shaped by some kind of reward.
Cats need to meow because we humans are generally so unobservant. Cats constantly monitor their surroundings (except
when they’re asleep, of course) but we often fix our gaze on newspapers and books, TVs and computer screens. We do,
however, reliably look up when we hear something unusual, and cats quickly learn that a meow will grab our attention.
For a few cats this may be rewarding in itself, but the meow will often also produce the reaction that the cat is hoping for,
such as a bowl of food or an opened door. Some cats then shape their own behavior to increase the precision of their
request. Some will deliver the meow at specific locations—by the door means “Let me out,” and in the middle of the
kitchen means “Feed me.” Others find that different intonations lead to different results, and so “train” themselves to
produce a whole range of different meows. These are generally different for every cat, and can be reliably interpreted only
by the cat’s owner, showing that each meow is an arbitrary, learned, attention-seeking sound rather than some universal
cat-human “language.”13 Thus, a secret code of meows and other vocalizations develops between each cat and its owner,
unique to that cat alone and meaning little to outsiders.
C lassical and operant conditioning are not the only feasible explanations for why cats behave as they do, but they are
often the simplest. Still, cats are undoubtedly much more than mere stimulus-response machines. Disentangling cat
intelligence is a challenge, partially because we tend to believe willingly that much cat behavior is driven by rational
thought. Even the earliest animal psychologists recognized this tendency. In his 1898 book Animal Intelligence, Edward
L. Thorndike wrote, with his tongue firmly in his cheek:
Thousands of cats on thousands of occasions sit helplessly yowling, and no one takes thought of it or writes to his friend, the professor; but let one cat
claw at the knob of a door supposedly as a signal to be let out, and straightway this cat becomes the representative of the cat-mind in all the books.14
We also lack scientific research on cat intelligence: the past decade has seen an explosion in studies of the mental abilities
of dogs, but cats, popular subjects for such studies in the 1960s and 1970s, have subsequently been eclipsed by “man’s
best friend”—simply because dogs are easier to train.
Some recent studies have focused on cats’ understanding of the way the world around them works—their grasp of physics
and engineering, if you will. Cats may seem entirely au fait with their surroundings, but their capacity to translate the
phenomena they encounter into mental pictures evolved when they were wild animals; it has not caught up with
humankind’s manipulations. I realized this firsthand when I noticed that my cat Splodge always inspected the fenders of
cars parked outside my house. Sometimes after sniffing he looked around nervously, and I guessed that he must have
picked up a scent mark from another cat, probably deposited a few hours previously when the car had been parked
elsewhere, miles away. This happened day after day, month after month, but Splodge, who was otherwise a perfectly
intelligent cat, never seemed to understand the possibility that the scent marks might have arrived already on the car: he
always seemed to presume that they belonged to an unfamiliar cat that must have just arrived in our neighborhood. In
nature, scent marks stay where they’ve been left, so there would be no need to evolve an understanding that scent marks
might move with objects on which they’ve been deposited.
We have little research on cats’ comprehension of physics, but one recent experiment has confirmed that it may be
extremely rudimentary. Scientists trained pet cats to retrieve a food treat from beneath a mesh cover by pulling on a
handle connected to the treat by a string (see illustration on page 142). Many of them learned to do this quite easily,
giving the impression that they “understood,” as you or I would without thinking twice, that the handle was connected to
the food by the string. However, we can also explain this behavior by simple operant conditioning—pull the handle and a
food reward arrives—in which the handle-pulling is, so far as the cat is concerned, just an arbitrary action. The
researchers exposed the cats’ lack of understanding of the connection by adding another string and handle alongside the
first one; the crucial difference, which the cats could easily have seen, was that only the first handle was connected to the
food. Although the cats could easily have seen that this was the case, they continued to pull on the handles, but were
unable to predict which handle would produce the reward, showing that for them, the handle was an arbitrary object, not
physically linked to the piece of food. Unsurprisingly, the cats performed equally badly when the strings were crossed
over.15 Extrapolating from this experiment, it seems likely that cats, unlike crows or apes, are mentally incapable of
learning to use tools.
Cats appear not to understand that one string has food on the end, and the other doesn’t
Their inability to understand that a piece of string can physically link two other things together highlights just how
different cats’ minds are to our own. We not only find such an idea obvious, we routinely extrapolate it to other situations
in which the physical connection is much less obvious, such as the electronic connection between the cursor on the screen
in front of me and the (computer) mouse in my hand. Cats fail at the first hurdle, the comprehension of a physical
connection between three objects, and their inability to perform such manipulations is not, as some would jokingly have
it, simply due to their lack of opposable thumbs.
Yet cats do have a sophisticated understanding of three-dimensional space, as we would expect of an opportunistic hunter.
They probably have little need for this when indoors, instead relying on what are known as egocentric cues—“This is
where I turn left,” “This is where I turn right,” “This is where I jump up.” However, when outdoors and in familiar
territory, cats can take shortcuts, showing that during their earlier explorations they have constructed a mental map—“The
last time I caught a rat, I first went to the oak tree and then turned left down the hedge, so this time I will go diagonally
across the field and through the hedge; I already know that the rats’ hole is just beyond the hedge.” They are capable of
using this information efficiently; given a choice of routes to a destination that cannot be seen from where they are, cats
will pick the shortest one. Likewise, as many people also do, they prefer a route that starts out in roughly the right
direction; a slightly shorter route that involves initially walking in the wrong direction is usually shunned.
As hunters, cats should be able to work out where objects that have gone out of sight are likely to be. No wild cat would
give up hunting a mouse immediately after it disappeared from sight under the mistaken impression that it had ceased to
exist. As expected, cats do seem to remember where prey has disappeared, although they store this information for only a
few seconds, in “working” memory; not until the cat actually makes contact with the prey does the memory become
longer-lasting. Presumably, it is not worthwhile for a cat to continue to search a particular location for highly mobile prey
for much longer than this; by that point, the prey has either made its escape or gone to ground.
Scientists recently demonstrated that cats do indeed remember the last place they saw a mouse, rather than merely keeping
their eyes fixed on it or even just heading in that general direction. In the apparatus illustrated, scientists allowed the cat to
watch a food treat being pulled behind a small barrier on a piece of cord, through the transparent section of the screen.
The cat was then allowed to walk into the apparatus, but—as the remainder of the screen was opaque—this action
temporarily blocked the cat’s view of the location of the food. Nevertheless, the cat usually picked the right place to look
for the food. Interestingly, many of the cats tested sometimes went the long way around—they set off and entered the
apparatus from the “wrong” side, but then immediately crossed to the correct side to retrieve the food. This is similar to a
common hunting tactic: if cats are in hot pursuit of a mouse or rat, they will often briefly take a more roundabout route,
possibly to make their prey think that they have mistaken where it has hidden.16
Cats sometimes take the most direct route to the place where food has disappeared (left), but other times they seem to
deliberately take a more indirect route (right), as if they were hunting and wanted to confuse their prey.
The cat’s mental abilities are specifically tuned to their hunting lifestyle, rather than being part of a more general spatial
intelligence. Cats perform remarkably badly in tests designed to track the development of such abilities in human infants.
Many children of eighteen months can understand that if an object is hidden in a container, and then the container itself is
hidden, the object ought to be in the container when it reappears. If they then find that it isn’t, they look next in the place
where the container disappeared. They not only understand that an object they have seen must still be somewhere even
when they can’t see it, but can also use their imagination to guess where it might be. Cats cannot do this at all, probably
because it is not a situation that their ancestors encountered while hunting. Mice hide, certainly, but they do not hide
inside objects that are themselves capable of moving around.
The cat’s ability to reason seems limited, especially when determining cause and effect. They rely on simple associations
built up through conditioning, and can easily be “fooled” by our manipulations of their surroundings, which must seem
arbitrary in their view of the world (as if they were thinking “How did that bag arrive in the middle of the kitchen floor?”
or “Why does my owner talk into that thing in her hand?”). Nevertheless, it is entirely possible that scientists have yet to
design experiments that allow cats to demonstrate their true abilities. It could be that the small number of situations in
which cats have been tested happen to be those in which their evolution has favored reliance on simple learning and short-
term, rather than long-term, memory. Scientists studying canine intelligence, which has received considerable attention
over the past two decades, have only recently begun to find ways of testing dogs that fit their particular way of
interpreting the world. Cats, with their enigmatic reputation, may still be hiding the true extent of their brainpower.
Cats are masters at concealing their thoughts, and are even better at hiding their emotions. Several cartoons show an
array of cats with identical expressions, each labeled with a different emotion, ranging from “Frisky” to “Content” to
“Sad,” and in one instance, with irony, “Alive.” One version that I treasure, by British cartoonist Steven Appleby, has no
less than thirty cat faces, of which twenty-nine have identical expressions (the captions range from “About to do nothing
at all” to “Slightly irritated but concealing it well”); the thirtieth, “Asleep” differs from the others only by its closed
eyes.17
Biology provides good reasons why most animals keep their emotions to themselves. Dog owners may find this idea
absurd, since both dogs and humans express their emotions spontaneously. Indeed, we must often suppress our feelings
when social mores dictate. Nevertheless, we humans have evolved a very sophisticated ability to detect tiny flickers of
emotion in others, telltale signs that help us predict what that person is likely to do next. Dogs, in their way, are similar to
us in this respect; not only have they evolved the ability to guess our intentions from our body language, but they also
express their emotions openly, partly because we generally respond in ways that are beneficial to them—backing away
when they growl, or giving them a pat if they’re wagging their tails. Dogs and humans are both social species that usually
live in stable groups, and such stability means that emotional honesty is unlikely to be penalized.
Cats are descended from a species with a solitary lifestyle, and therefore much of their behavior is guided by the need to
compete, not to collaborate. In the wild, a male cat will live alone. The only way he can be sure of leaving any
descendants is first to convince a female to accept him as a mate, and second to convince any rival males to back off.
Macho behavior, laced with a generous portion of bluff, is therefore essential to their success. Although female domestic
cats do cooperate when raising kittens, this habit, which may have evolved during domestication, seems to have had little
effect on their capacity to express their emotions.
Historically, scientists have changed their mind several times over whether animals’ emotions should be used when
discussing their behavior. In the nineteenth century, researchers often ascribed human emotions to cats. For example, in
his 1886 book Animal Intelligence, physiologist George S. Romanes wrote:
The only other feature in the emotional life of cats which calls for special notice is that which leads to their universal and proverbial treatment of
helpless prey. The feelings that prompt a cat to torture a captured mouse can only, I think, be assigned to the category to which by common consent they
are ascribed—delight in torturing for torture’s sake.18
By the beginning of the twentieth century, such anthropomorphism had been abandoned, and the guiding principle in
animal psychology was Morgan’s canon: “In no case may we interpret an action as the outcome of the exercise of a higher
psychical faculty, if it can be interpreted as the outcome of the exercise of one which stands lower [that is, simpler] in the
psychological scale.”19 For a while, indeed, scientists conceived of animals as if they were robotic stimulus-response
machines, leaving no room for any consideration of emotion. Recently, however, we have come to realize that it is
difficult to explain some animal behavior without invoking the idea of emotion. In addition, MRI scanning has enabled us
to see where in the human brain emotions are generated, and the simpler, “gut-feeling” emotions occur in parts of our
brains that we share with other mammals, including cats.
The current view holds that emotions are a necessary component of the mechanisms that drive animal behavior—and
indeed our behavior as well. They can be prompts to shortcuts, enabling our brains to choose the best response to a
situation when fast action is called for. In this, cats are no different from ourselves. A cat that sees another, larger,
unfamiliar cat approaching will immediately become alert, crouch down, and prepare to flee. The anxiety it feels at seeing
a potential adversary enables it to take these actions immediately, without having to think the situation through and
evaluate all possible strategies and outcomes.
Emotions also explain spontaneous and apparently functionless behavior. Kittens engage in play during most of their
waking moments, and explaining why they do so is not straightforward. In the wild, play is a mildly risky activity,
exposing the kittens to danger and possibly attracting the attention of a predator—surely it seems safer for kittens to stay
quietly in their nest and wait for their mother to return with food. Furthermore, kittens do sometimes nip each other when
playing, but this doesn’t seem to put them off playing with that same kitten again, which it should do if they are simple
stimulus/response machines.
The simplest explanation for why kittens feel they ought to play, and why they continue to play even following a slight
mishap, is that play is fun. Neuroscientists have found in young rats that when they play, the neurohormone profile of the
brain changes. Moreover, these changes are not the consequences of play, they appear to be its cause: they occur as soon
as the rats are given the signal that it’s time to play. As such, the mere sight of a sibling ready to play is likely enough to
make a kitten want to join in, because the brain signals “fun” before the playing has even begun.
Of course, hormones are not the same as emotions, but changes in certain hormones are often a sign that emotions are
being experienced. We are all aware of the racing heartbeat, hyperventilation, hyperalertness, and sweaty palms induced
by adrenalin or epinephrine, the “fight-or-flight” hormone associated with feelings of fear and panic. Some of us are
familiar with the elation we sometimes feel after strenuous exercise, caused by the release of endorphins and other
hormones into the brain. Although not all hormones are so closely connected to emotions, many are, and can provide an
indicator of immediate emotion or underlying mood.
We can therefore conceive of animal emotions as manifestations of the brain and the nervous system and their associated
hormones, sometimes enabling decisions to be made quickly, other times directing learning. Sometimes, information
coming into the brain from the cat’s senses has to trigger an immediate reaction. A cat that slips while walking along the
top of a fence must correct its balance immediately: the emotional panic that almost certainly follows will help train the
cat to be more careful next time. On other occasions, the emotion triggers the behavior. Indeed, the sight of its owner
coming home will cause a cat to feel affectionate, and as a consequence it will raise its tail upright and begin to walk
toward her.
Some people who don’t like cats—and even some who do—would claim that love, especially towards its human owner—
is not part of the cat’s behavioral repertoire: as the popular adage goes, “Dogs have masters, cats have staff.” True, the
average cat does not outwardly demonstrate love for its owner in the same way as, say, a Labrador retriever would. Still,
that tells us little about what is going on inside the cat’s head.
In the animal world, lavish outward displays of emotion are usually manipulative. Consider, for example, the incessant
chirping of baby birds in a nest, each essentially calling out, “Feed me first, feed me first!” Evolution has ensured that
where such displays occur, they work: the baby bird that makes the least fuss is ignored by its parents, which often
produce more offspring than they can comfortably feed, and it may perish as a result. Wildcats are solitary and self-
sufficient except for a few brief weeks at the beginning of their lives, and so have little need for sophisticated signals.
Although domestic cats now depend on us for food, shelter, and protection, they have not done so for long enough to have
evolved the average dog’s effusive greeting. That doesn’t mean that cats are incapable of love, merely that their ways of
showing love are somewhat limited.
Cats become extravagantly demonstrative only when they are angry or afraid. A fearful cat either will make itself look as
small as possible by crouching, and then slink away, or, if it judges that running away may provoke a chase, will make
itself look as large as possible, arching and raising the fur on its back. The raw emotion does not therefore provoke an
automatic and invariable reaction; instead, the brain selects the more appropriate response based on other information
available to it.
The angry cat will not only try to look as large as possible, but will also stand head-on toward the threat (usually another
cat) with its ears forward, either yowling or growling loudly and lashing its tail side to side. We may be inclined to read
these postures as expressions of emotion, but they are fundamentally expressions of intention, as well as attempts to
manipulate the animal the cat is confronting.
Even when a cat is demonstrably trying to manipulate his opponent’s behavior, such manipulation need not be conscious.
We can explain the cat’s behavior by its adhering to a set of rules that have served its ancestors well; which have, in the
past, achieved the best possible outcome from an aggressive encounter. We must not forget, however, that the bluff is
being directed at an animal with similar brainpower, and therefore evolution has also favored animals capable of
“mindreading” the other’s intentions. One cat confronting another will expect one of two possible reactions: either a sign
that the other cat is fearful and will probably back down, or that it is not fearful and so a fight is imminent. In this way, the
behavior, whether indicating fear or anger, becomes ritualized: any cat that adopts neither posture, or does something
different, is likely to be attacked.
To explain their behavior, we must thus allow that cats feel joy, love, anger, and fear. What other emotions do they
possess? Can they feel the full gamut of emotions that we can? To answer these questions, we must consider which are
likely the product of human consciousness, and therefore unknown to animals.
People do not agree on the range of emotions their cats can feel. A 2008 survey of British cat owners revealed that almost
all think their cat could feel affection, joy, and fear.20 Nearly one-fifth of these owners—perhaps those who own very
timid cats—were unsure whether anger is within the cat’s repertoire.
We all know the old saw “Curiosity killed the cat,” and indeed, most owners acknowledge the cat’s characteristic
nosiness. The original version of this proverb, from its first appearance in the sixteenth century until the end of the
nineteenth, was “Care killed the cat”—“care” in the sense of worry, anxiety, or sorrow. Apparently, the idea that cats
could become so anxious that they could even die from it was once common (and is now being revisited by veterinarians).
Despite this, about a quarter of the owners surveyed in 2008 thought their cat incapable of feeling anxiety or sadness.
If asked, scientists would now agree that the old version of the proverb contained a germ of truth: anxiety does constitute
a serious and real affliction for many cats. Anxiety, if simply defined as a fear of something that is not currently
happening, has a reliable basis in physiology. Some anti-anxiety drugs developed for humans have also been found to
reduce symptoms of anxiety in cats, so although we cannot be sure that cats experience anxiety in precisely the same way
we do, we know that they feel something similar.
Owners’ opinions about their cats’ emotional capabilities
The most common cause of anxiety in cats is probably the worry that their territory is likely to be invaded by other cats in
the neighborhood, or even by another cat in the same household. When I surveyed ninety cat owners in suburban
Hampshire and rural Devon in 2000, they reported that almost half of their cats regularly fought with other cats, and two
out of five were fearful of cats in general. My colleague Rachel Casey, a veterinary surgeon specializing in cat behavioral
disorders, regularly diagnoses anxiety and fear as the main factors driving cats to urinate and defecate indoors, outside the
litter tray. Some cats spray the walls or furniture with urine, possibly to deter other cats from entering their owner’s house
believing it to be cat-free; others find the point in the house farthest from the cat-flap and urinate there, seemingly terrified
of attracting the attention of any other cat. Some will defecate on the bedsheets, desperately trying to mingle their own
odor with their owner’s to establish “ownership” of the core of the house. When the conflict is between two cats that live
in the same house, one may spend much of its time hiding, or obsessively groom itself until its coat becomes patchy.21
The stress of being forced to live with cats it does not trust can often be severe enough to affect the cat’s health. One
illness now known to be closely linked to psychological stress is cystitis, referred to by veterinarians as idiopathic cystitis
because no disease or other medical cause is apparent. As many as two-thirds of cats taken to vets for urination
problems—blood in the urine, difficult or painful urination, urinating in inappropriate places—have no obvious medical
problems, other than inflammation of the bladder and intermittent blockage of the urethra by mucus thereby displaced
from the bladder wall. The factors triggering such episodes of cystitis are therefore psychological, and research has
identified conflict with other cats living in the same household as being perhaps the most important of these. Less easy to
quantify, but possibly just as important, are conflicts with neighbors’ cats: certainly cats prone to cystitis usually run away
from cats they meet in their own gardens, rather than standing up to them, which suggests that they find contact with other
cats particularly stressful. Idiopathic cystitis is less common in female cats than in males: the conventional medical
explanation is that the tube leading out of the bladder, the urethra, is generally narrower in males and therefore more
prone to blockage. However, male cats are generally more territorial and less sociable than females, so the latter may find
it easier to resolve or avoid conflicts with other cats before stress affects their health.
Colleagues of mine at Bristol University Veterinarian School documented one case involving a five-year-old male cat that
was having great difficulty urinating, and when he did, his urine was bloody. He also groomed his abdomen excessively,
but otherwise was entirely healthy. The cat was one of six within the household, but was friendly with none of the other
cats. Moreover, cats from neighboring households had recently attacked him. His symptoms gradually disappeared once
his owners had implemented the changes recommended by the clinic: his own exclusive area within the house, his own
food bowl, and his own litter tray that the other cats could not access. At the same time, his owners blocked his view of
the garden by covering over the glass at the bottom of the windows in his part of the house, so he could not see any cats
that were coming into the garden. Six months later his symptoms returned, but on investigation, it turned out he had been
accidentally shut in with the other cats a couple days previously. His owner vowed never to allow this to happen again,
and the cat soon recovered.22 Anxiety, a useful emotion if experienced for a few minutes, can become the bane of a cat’s
existence if prolonged for weeks or months, leading to chronically elevated levels of stress hormones, (presumably) a
nagging and ever-present sense of dread, and eventually to deterioration in health.
In the same survey, cat owners were also asked about their cats’ more complex emotions—jealousy, pride, shame, guilt,
and grief. Almost two-thirds believed that their cat could feel jealous or proud. Only shame, guilt, and grief were ruled out
by a majority of the owners.
Basic emotions such as anger, affection, joy, fear, and anxiety are “gut feelings” that appear spontaneously. The most
primitive part of the cat’s brain produces these emotions—the same part that evolved hundreds of millions of years ago
before there were any mammals, let alone cats. More complex emotions, such as jealousy, empathy, and grief, require the
cat to have some understanding of the mental processes of animals other than themselves, and hence psychologists
sometimes refer to them as relational emotions.
Take jealousy, for example. When we experience jealousy, we are not only aware that whoever we are feeling jealous of
is another human being, we can also guess what that other person is feeling; we have what psychologists call theory of
mind, the idea that other humans have their own thoughts that may be different from ours. We are also capable of
becoming obsessively jealous, thinking about the incident that triggered the original feeling long afterward, even when the
person we were jealous of is no longer present. We have little evidence that cats have either the brainpower or the
imagination to do either of these.
Cats undoubtedly recognize other cats as cats, and can evidently react to what they see them doing. However, even dogs,
which are much more highly evolved socially than cats, show no evidence of understanding what other dogs are thinking,
so it’s unlikely that cats can either. Moreover, cats seem to live in the present, neither reflecting on the past nor planning
for the future. Still, at its heart, jealousy is an emotion first experienced in the here and now; it does not require the cat to
understand what its rival is thinking, or even that it is capable of thinking at all. All that jealousy requires is that the cat
merely perceive that another cat is getting more of something than it should. Thus, cats are almost certainly capable of
feeling jealous, even if not quite as demonstratively, or as commonly, as dogs. Although not something that my cats have
ever indulged in, countless owners have regaled me with stories of how one of their cats would always intervene when
they tried to stroke the other.
Many people think that cats are capable of grief, because they behave oddly when another cat that they have known
disappears. What they actually feel is probably a temporary anxiety, which disappears once all traces of the missing cat
have disappeared. A mother cat may search for her kitten for a day or two after it has been homed. She probably has a
memory of that kitten, and may even count the remaining kittens to check that one is missing. This behavior would be the
same if that kitten had temporarily gotten lost; in the wild, it would be in the mother cat’s interest to seek it out and
continue to look after it until it was old enough to become independent of her. She cannot “know” that it has gone to a
good home where it will be well cared for, as nothing in her evolution will have prepared her to embrace that concept. For
a few days, the mother is reminded of that particular kitten by the lingering traces of its individual odor, the kind of cue
that is often meaningless to us. We know that the kitten has gone because we can no longer see or hear it. Once the
kitten’s odor has faded below her threshold, the mother cat probably forgets all about the departed kitten. While she can
still smell it, she may well feel an anxiety that drives her to continue to look for it. This, however, is not the same as grief.
Emotions such as guilt and pride would require cats to possess a further level of cognitive sophistication, the ability to
compare their actions with a set of rules or standards that they have worked out for themselves. When we feel guilty, we
compare the memory of something we’ve just done with our sense of what is wrong. Such feelings are sometimes referred
to as self-conscious emotions, because they require a degree of self-awareness to be experienced. So far, science has yet to
reveal any evidence for self-awareness in cats, or even in dogs. Dogs are widely believed to display a “guilty look” when
their owners discover that they’ve done something that’s forbidden, but a clever experiment has shown that this is all in
the owners’ imagination.23 The researcher asked the owners to command their dogs not to touch a tempting food treat, and
then leave the room. Then, unbeknown to the owners, she encouraged some of the dogs, but not others, to eat the treat.
When the owners came back into the room, they were all told that their dog had stolen the treat—whereon all of the dogs
immediately began to look guilty, whether or not they actually had something to feel guilty about. The “guilty look” was
nothing more than each dog’s reaction to their owners’ body language, which had changed subtly as soon as they had
been told of the dog’s misdeed, whether real or invented. If dogs’ “guilty looks” are a figment of their owners’
imaginations, it follows that they—and by extension, cats—are incapable of feeling guilt. The same is probably true for
pride, but no scientist seems to have studied it in any relevant animal.
Cats’ emotional lives are more elaborate than their detractors would have us think, but not quite as sophisticated as the
most ardent cat-lover would probably like to believe. Unlike dogs, cats hide their emotions—not primarily from us but
from one another, a legacy of their evolutionary history as solitary, competitive animals. We have every reason to believe
that they possess the basic range of emotions, the gut feelings, shared by all mammals, because having these empowers
them to make quick decisions, whether that is to run away (fear), play with a ball of string (joy) or curl up on their
owner’s lap (love). However, cats are not as socially sophisticated as dogs are: they are undoubtedly intelligent, but much
of that intelligence relates to obtaining food and defending territory. Emotions that relate to relationships, such as
jealousy, grief and guilt, are probably beyond their reach, as is the ability to comprehend social relationships with any
great sophistication. This leaves them ill-equipped for the demands of living closely with other cats, as domestication has
progressively required them to do.
CHAPTER 7
Cats Together
C ats can be very affectionate, but they are choosy about the objects of their affection. This apparent fastidiousness
stems from the cat’s evolutionary past: wildcats, especially males, live much of their lives with no adult company, and
regard most other members of their species as rivals rather than as potential colleagues. Domestication has inhibited not
only the wildcat’s intrinsic distrust of people, but has also tempered some of their wariness of other cats.
The bond between cat and owner must have its origins in the bond between cat and cat; such behavior has no other
plausible evolutionary source. Although the cat’s immediate ancestor, the wildcat, is not a social animal, adult felids of
other species, such as lions, do cooperate. As such, cats of any species could possibly become more sociable in the right
conditions. We may find the source of the domestic cat’s affection for its owners through a brief survey of the social life
of the entire cat family.
Both male and female tigers are solitary, exemplifying the pattern that almost all members of the cat family, big and
small alike, live alone. The females hold non-overlapping territories, which they defend from one another; each territory is
large enough to provide food not just for that female, but also for the litters of cubs she rears. Young males are usually
nomadic, and when they become mature, they try to set up their own large territories. These will contain far more prey
than the male will ever need to satisfy his hunger, but that is not its purpose. The male is trying to achieve exclusive
access to as many females as possible: especially successful males may hold territories that overlap those of up to seven
females.
Cheetah males, especially brothers, are a bit more sociable than tiger males. Female cheetahs are as solitary as female
tigers, but male cheetahs sometimes band together in twos or threes to seduce females, many of which are migratory, as
they pass nearby. Even though only one of the brothers will be the father of the resulting litter, biologists have shown that
over his lifetime each brother will father more cubs than if he had tried to attract females on his own. Male cheetahs do
occasionally try to hunt together as well, but are rarely successful, apparently lacking the skills to coordinate their efforts.
The best-known exception to the standard felid pattern is the lion, the only member of the cat family to have several males
and several females living together. In Africa, the lion pride is usually constructed from one family of female lions, with
the males originating from a different family (thereby preventing inbreeding). While still young, related males band
together, sometimes adding unrelated males to swell their ranks, until their numbers are sufficient to challenge and eject
the resident males from a pride. Once they have taken over the pride, they may kill all the cubs, and by doing so bring all
the females into season within a few months. The males must then keep control of the pride until the females have given
birth to their progeny and raised them to independence. For their part, the females not only have to raise the cubs, but they
also do most of the hunting, while the males do little but protect the females from other bands of males. Thus, the
superficial image of lion society as generally harmonious is something of a myth; rather, it is the result of tension between
the benefits of cooperation and competition, with each individual lion employing tactics that maximize its own breeding
success.
Biologists still debate why lions live in such groups. In India, lions are often solitary animals, males and females meeting
only for mating, so members of this species can evidently choose whether or not to live together. Although the females in
a pride generally hunt together, they do not cooperate as selflessly as their “brave” image suggests: if the prey is large and
potentially dangerous, the more experienced females usually hold back and let the younger, more impetuous lionesses
take the risks. The main benefit of numbers, and especially the presence of the fierce males, may come after the kill, when
the valuable meat must be defended from other animals, particularly hyenas.
Scientists once considered lions and cheetahs the only social cats, and added the domestic cat to that exclusive list only
recently. It had long been clear that wherever there was a regular source of suitable food, a group of feral cats would
spring up, but these groups were originally considered mere aggregations of individuals that had somehow agreed to
tolerate one another, as happens when animals of many species come together to drink at a waterhole. Cat breeders also
knew that their queens would sometimes suckle one another’s litters, but scientists dismissed this behavior as the result of
the artificial conditions under which humans usually keep pedigree cats. In the late 1970s, however, David Macdonald’s
documentary about cats on a farm in Devon, England, showed that this was in fact natural behavior—that free-living
females, especially related females, will spontaneously cooperate to raise their kittens together.1
At the start of the study, the colony consisted of just four cats: a female, Smudge; her daughters, Pickle and Domino; and
their father, Tom. When not in the farmyard, they kept to themselves—domestic cats, unlike lions, do not hunt together—
but when their visits coincided, they usually curled up together in apparent contentment. They evidently regarded the
farmyard and the food and shelter it provided as “theirs,” since the three females would join together to drive away three
other cats that lived nearby—a female, Whitetip, her son Shadow, and her daughter Tab. Tom, though, was aggressive
only toward Shadow, presumably regarding him as a potential rival, whereas he might need to court the two females
should he lose his own “pride” at some point in the future.
Pickle and Domino were the first to reveal how female cats come together to help one another. Early in May, Pickle
produced three kittens in a nest in a straw stack. For the first couple weeks, she looked after her kittens on her own, just as
any other mother cat would. Then, suddenly, her sister Domino appeared in the nest and gave birth to five more kittens—
ably assisted by Pickle, who helped with their delivery and with cleaning them up. Then, despite their difference in ages,
all eight kittens settled down together and were nursed and cared for indiscriminately by both their mothers. Sadly, all
eight kittens subsequently succumbed to cat flu, a common scourge of litters born outdoors in the UK. However, when
their grandmother Smudge produced a single male kitten a few weeks later, the aptly named Lucky, both Domino and
Pickle helped with caring for him, including playing with him and bringing back mice they had caught, to save Smudge
leaving him alone while she went out hunting for herself.
The turntable used to test cats’ ability to learn from other cats. The cat can get the food by gradually rotating the table
until the bowl passes through the gap.
Whether young cats ever learn much from cats that are not members of their family is doubtful. Underlying distrust
probably focuses their minds exclusively on staying out of trouble, overriding any curiosity about what another cat is
doing. However, within a close-knit family group, younger members may benefit from watching how older, more
experienced members of the group solve everyday problems. Because cats hunt alone, this is most likely to occur when
the cats are in their shared core territory, perhaps when scavenging for food or interacting with people.
Although we may logically assume that cats began to live in family groups only since they started to associate with
humans, wildcats also may have (or may have had) this ability. The formation of a group of cats seems to have only one
essential requirement: a reliable source of food that can feed more than one cat and her litter of kittens. The only felid that
has consistently achieved this leap is the lion, which has adopted group hunting as a way to prey on large animals.
However, other felids might once have lived in small groups even without developing this additional skill.
Before man’s domination of the environment in the twentieth century led to the depletion of the small cats and their
favorite prey, wildcats might occasionally have lived in colonies. Several accounts left by early twentieth-century
European explorers of Africa give tantalizing glimpses of this possibility. Willoughby Prescott Lowe, one of the last noted
collectors of animals for the British Natural History Museum, describes a specimen he took from near Darfur in the Sudan
in 1921:
I trapped an interesting cat near Fasher. Something like a domestic cat—but v. different in coloration. The curious thing is that they live in colonies in
holes in the open plain—all the holes are close together—just like a rabbit warren. I’m told they are v. local—Anyhow a cat with these habits was quite
new to me! They feed on gerbils which swarm everywhere and the ground is always a mass of holes.8
Ten years later, on an expedition in the Ahaggar Mountains in the center of the Sahara desert, Lowe again recorded
colonies of wildcats, living in burrows previously dug by fennec foxes.
Both these cats looked like typical African wildcats, but their sociability need not have come from their wildcat ancestry.
The DNA of apparent wildcats from farther south in Africa, and also from the Middle East, reveals extensive
interbreeding between domestic and wild cats. What Lowe saw may have been colonies of hybrids, which had retained the
domestic cat’s ability to live in family groups while outwardly appearing to be wildcats. That social groups of these Felis
lybica have been recorded so infrequently suggests that, when they do occur, their social skills may have originated in
previous interbreeding with domestic cats.
The switch from solitary animal to social living requires a quantum leap in communication skills. For an animal as well-
armored and suspicious as a cat, a simple tiff between sisters might well escalate into a family bust-up—unless, that is, a
system of signaling evolved that allowed each cat to assess the others’ moods and intentions. And this, it seems, is
precisely what happened.
For domestic cats, my own research has shown that the key signal is the familiar straight-up tail. In cat colonies, when two
cats are working out whether to approach each other, one usually raises its tail vertically; if the other is happy to
reciprocate, it usually raises its tail also, and the two will walk up to each other.9 If the second cat does not raise its tail
and the first is feeling especially bold, it may approach nevertheless, but obliquely. If the second cat then turns away, the
first cat occasionally meows to attract its attention—among the very few occasions when feral cats meow. Otherwise, the
first cat lowers its tail and heads off in another direction, presumably judging that the other is not in the mood to be
friendly. Hesitation can be risky. My research team documented instances when a cat moving in the wrong direction, even
with its tail upright, was chased off by another, usually larger, cat determined to be left alone.
Observations such as these do not prove conclusively that the upright tail is a signal; it could conceivably be something
that happens when two friendly cats meet, with no meaning for either. To isolate the raised tail from everything else that a
real cat might do to indicate its intentions, we cut life-size silhouettes of cats from black paper and stuck them to the
baseboard of cat owners’ houses. When the resident cat saw an upright-tail silhouette, it usually approached and sniffed it;
when the silhouette had a horizontal tail, the cat backed away.10
The tail-up signal has almost certainly evolved since domestication, arising from a posture kittens use when greeting their
mothers. Adults of other cat species raise their tails only when they are about to spray urine, simply for hygiene. A few
individuals of Felis lybica in zoos do raise their tails when about to rub on their keepers’ legs, but these, of course, could
have some domestic cat in their ancestry. Adults of the other races of Felis silvestris do not raise their tails in greeting, but
their kittens do hold their tails upright when approaching their mother; no one has tested whether this holds any
significance for the mother, so we do not know whether kittens do this as a signal or whether it’s purely incidental.
Therefore, it seems most plausible that the upright tail evolved from a posture into a signal during the early stages of
domestication. This would have required two changes in how cats organize their behavior: one, for adult cats to perform
the kitten’s raised tail posture when approaching other cats, and two, for other cats to recognize instinctively that a cat
with its tail raised is not a threat.11 Once both these changes had occurred, the posture would have evolved into a signal,
one that enabled adult cats to live in close proximity to each other with less risk of quarreling.12
Once an exchange of tail-ups has established that both cats are happy to approach each other, one of two things occurs;
which of the two seems to be influenced by both the cats’ moods and the relationship they have with each other. If the cats
are in the middle of doing something else, and/or one cat is significantly older or larger than the other, they usually walk
up to or alongside each other. Then, keeping their tails upright, they come into physical contact and rub their heads,
flanks, or tails—or a combination of all of them—on each other, before separating and walking on. Any two cats from the
same group will perform this occasionally, but it is typically performed by female cats greeting males, and young cats of
either sex greeting females.
The precise significance of this rubbing ritual is still unclear. The physical contact in itself may reinforce the friendship
between the two participants, and thereby keep the group together—a ritual that counteracts the natural tendency of cats to
regard others as rivals, not allies. The act of rubbing together also inevitably transfers scent from one cat to the other, so
repeated rubbing could cause a “family odor” to build up. We know that some of the cat’s carnivore relatives exchange
scent via rubbing rituals: for example, badgers from the same sett create a “clan odor” by rubbing their back ends
together, exchanging scent between their subcaudal glands, wax-filled pockets of skin that lie just beneath their tails.13
Cats may not deliberately exchange odor when they rub on one another; if they were doing so, they would likely
concentrate their rubs on scent-producing areas of their bodies, such as the glands at the corners of their mouths, which
they use to scent-mark prominent objects in their territories—but they generally don’t. As such, the rubbing ritual may be
mainly tactile, a reaffirmation of trust between two animals, which by accumulation reduces the likelihood of the group
splintering apart.
The other social exchange that can follow the tail-up signal is mutual licking, or allogrooming. Cats spend much time
licking their coats, so it is hardly surprising that when two cats lie down side by side, they often lick each other.
Moreover, they tend to groom the top of the other cat’s head and between the shoulders. These are areas that the supplest
of cats finds hardest to groom for itself—not impossible, of course, since cats with no grooming buddy use their wrists to
wipe those areas, and then in turn lick their wrists—and all cats use this method to clean their mouths after eating.
One interpretation of allogrooming holds that it is entirely accidental: two cats sitting together groom those areas that
smell the least clean, oblivious that those areas belong to another cat. However, we know that allogrooming has a
profound social significance in many other animals, especially in primates, in which it has been linked with pair-bonding,
the building of coalitions, and in reconciliations between family members who have recently quarreled. In cats,
allogrooming likely performs the same function as mutual rubbing: cementing an amicable relationship. Consistent with
this is the observation that in large groups of cats consisting of more than one family, most allogrooming takes place
between relatives.14
Tomcat spraying
Bluff and Bluster
When two rival cats meet, each will try to persuade the other to back down without either having to resort to physical
contact. Cats are too well armed to risk fighting unless this is unavoidable; they therefore resort to adopting postures that
attempt to persuade their opponent that they are bigger than they really are.
Each cat will draw itself up to its full height, turn partially sideways, and make its hair stand on end, all designed to make
its profile seem as big as possible. Of course, since both cats do this, neither gains any advantage, but that also means that
neither can risk not performing this display to its maximum extent. The only clue that such a cat may be less than
confident about winning is when it pulls its ears toward the back of its head: the ears are very vulnerable to being
damaged in a fight, as testified by how ragged they can become even when they belong to the most successful of toms.
At the same time, each cat tries to add to the general effect by uttering a variety of calls, each designed to enhance the
general impression that it is not to be trifled with. These include guttural yowls and snarls, violent spitting, and especially
low-pitched growls—the lower the sound, the larger the voice box must be, and therefore by implication, the larger the
cat. These vocalizations usually continue even if the visual posturing fails and actual fighting begins.
Cats, lacking the dog’s rich repertoire of visual signals, find it difficult to signal an intention to back down. Fights usually
end with one cat fleeing, with the victor in hot pursuit. If neither cat wishes to fight, one will gradually adopt a much less
threatening posture, with its body crouched and its ears flattened, and then attempt to creep slowly away, frequently
looking over its shoulder to check that the other cat is not about to launch an attack.
Because most owners have their cats neutered, mature tomcats are something of a rarity in western society. Few people
keep them as pets, and many of those who try to do so are subsequently discouraged by the pungency of the urine tomcats
spray around the garden (or, worse, the house); by the wounds they receive from stronger, more experienced tomcats in
the neighborhood; and by their weeklong absences as they journey off in search of receptive females. Most owners of
male cats never get to this point, taking the advice of their veterinarian to have their kitten neutered before testosterone
starts to kick in at about six months of age. Male cats that have been neutered during their first year behave much more
like females than males, and are usually as sociable toward other cats as a female would be under the same circumstances;
that is, most will remain friendly toward other cats that they have known since birth (usually, but not necessarily, their
blood relations), and a few will be even more outgoing.
Tomcat behavior gives us insight on how the cat may be evolving today. A tomcat’s main goal is to compete for the
attention of as many females as possible. As a consequence, wildcat toms evolved to be 15 to 40 percent heavier than
females. This is also true of domestic cats today; tomcats’ physicality appears to have been affected little by
domestication.
By definition, half the genes in each new generation of kittens comes from their fathers. Because successful tomcats can
mate with many females over the course of a lifetime, those tomcats that leave the most offspring have a disproportionate
effect on the next generation. Most owners of unneutered female cats allow them to mate with any tomcat in the
neighborhood, so the decision over which tomcats leave the most offspring is usually determined by the cats themselves,
and not by people.
The tactics tomcats employ to maximize their chances of successfully mating are affected by how many females live
nearby. Where females are widely dispersed, as with wildcats and rural domestic cats, males try to defend large territories
that overlap with those of as many females as possible, typically three or four. Even allowing that tomcats are larger than
females, the amount of available food in such large territories is far more than they need, but their primary goal is access
to females, not nourishment. Inevitably, there are not enough females for all males to monopolize more than one female—
male and female cats are born in roughly equal numbers—so some tomcats, usually the younger ones, must adopt a
different strategy, that of roaming around, trying to find unclaimed females while avoiding bumping into those males that
are established territory-holders.
This overtly competitive system inevitably breaks down when large colonies of cats form around an abundant source of
food. Although tomcats’ tactics seem to have evolved prior to domestication, when all females were solitary and lived in
separate territories, there appears to have been no great need for males to change their behavior when females began to
live in small family groups, attracted by the fairly modest concentrations of food stemming from human activity. This
arrangement continues to this day, for example on farms that can support just a handful of cats.
In places where scores of females are concentrated into one area—fishing ports, towns with many outdoor restaurants, or
where several feral cat-feeders operate together—no single male, however strong and fierce, could succeed in
monopolizing several females, or even one. In these situations, there is a buildup of large colonies that include both males
and females, some of which act on their own, and others cooperating in family groups. Each male is essentially in
competition with all the others for the attention of the females, but, as we have seen, they somehow manage mostly to
avoid the overt fighting that occurs around smaller colonies. Moreover, males in these large groups are often even less
aggressive when some of the females become receptive to mating, almost as if they knew they needed to be on their best
behavior for a female to accept them as a mate.
Most of the time, female cats avoid contact with males, particularly those they don’t know well, presumably for fear of
being attacked. There are some exceptions. In the small, single-family colony that David Macdonald studied, all the
females behaved affectionately toward the resident tomcat, possibly hoping that he would defend their kittens against any
marauding males attempting to take over the group. Of course, this antipathy changes as the female begins to come into
season. When she is in the pro-estrus phase, the few days prior to actual mating, she becomes both more attractive to
males and more tolerant of them, although at this stage she will not allow more than fleeting contact. She becomes more
restless than usual, and repeatedly deposits scent by rubbing against prominent objects in her territory. If there are no
males in the vicinity, she begins to roam away from her usual haunts, scent marking and uttering a characteristic guttural
cry as she goes. She probably also advertises her imminent willingness to mate by changes in her scent, that may be
detected by males as far away as several miles downwind. Then, as estrus approaches, she begins to roll over and over on
the ground, purring all the while, interspersed with bouts of stretching, kneading the ground with her claws, and more
restless scent marking. By this time, several males are usually in attendance. While she now lets them approach, she does
not allow them to mount her, fighting off any that try with her claws and teeth.
As each male attempts to maximize the numbers of kittens they sire, each female tries to maximize the genetic quality of
the limited number of kittens she can produce in a lifetime; she uses her courtship period to select between the males that
she has attracted. She may already know something about them from the scent marks they have deposited in her territory
(see box on page 180, “That Catty Smell”), but she can make a more balanced assessment from observing their behavior
toward one another and toward other females. She may then decide—or be forced—to accept more than one for mating.
As her hormones take her into full estrus, the female’s behavior changes abruptly. In between bouts of rolling, she
crouches with her head close to the ground, treading with hind legs partially extended, and holding her tail to one side,
inviting the males to copulate. The boldest of those in attendance then mounts her, grasping the scruff of her neck in his
teeth. A few seconds later, in apparent contradiction to her invitation, she screams in pain, turns on him, and drives him
away, spitting and scratching. This abrupt change of mood is brought about by the pain she undoubtedly experiences
during copulation: the male cat’s penis is equipped with 120 to 150 sharp spines, designed to trigger her ovulation (cats,
unlike humans, do not ovulate spontaneously, but require this stimulus). Happily, she appears to forget this discomfort
quickly; within a few minutes, she displays herself to the males all over again—a cycle that continues at a gradually
decreasing rate for a day or two. Her receptive period over, she removes herself from the area and the males disperse. If
she has not become pregnant, she comes into season again every couple of weeks, until she successfully conceives.
Felinine
In tomcat urine, the signal is probably not the protein (cauxin) but the felinine, which is made from one of two amino
acids, cysteine and methionine, both of which contain the sulfur atom necessary for the eventual generation of the pungent
odor. Cats cannot make either of these amino acids for themselves, meaning that the amount of felinine they can make is
determined by the amount of high-quality protein in their diets. In a wild cat, this is in turn determined by how successful
a hunter that cat is. Thus, the smellier the urine mark is, the more felinine it contains, indicating that the cat that made the
mark must be good at obtaining food.
The urine mark must also contain information about the identity of the cat that produced it; otherwise, a female could not
know which of the several males in front of her was the best hunter. So far, scientists have not investigated this part of the
message, but it is presumably other than the pungent sulfur compounds. Other species use the vomeronasal organ to detect
individual odor “signatures.”
However repellent to us, the cat-urine odor is a genuine badge of quality. A sickly or incompetent male is simply unable
to get enough food to make its urine pungent. The evolution of this signal was therefore probably driven by the females,
which selected males based on how smelly their urine was: males unable to make felinine, however good their diet, would
not have been favored by females. Using these criteria, tomcats whose owners feed them high-quality commercial food
are technically cheats, but this has happened too recently to have had any effect on female behavior—and anyway, I guess
nobody has told them yet.
Much of this complex ritual evolved long before domestication, when every female cat lived in her own territory, perhaps
several miles away from the nearest male. Scientists think that the female’s delayed ovulation evolved to give her plenty
of time to locate a mate, since wildcats, unlike many carnivores, do not usually form pair bonds. This drawn-out courtship
would be unnecessary if only one male was attracted at a time. Instead, it seems designed for the female first to attract
several males, presuming that there are a few good ones in the area, and then to observe them for many hours or even
days, so she can gauge which is likely to provide the best genes for her kittens—which is all she can hope to get from
their father, since paternal care is unknown in cats.
The optimum strategy for males probably changed once man began to provide cats with food, at the beginning of the
journey to domestication. Female territories became smaller and reliably focused on prime hunting areas around granaries,
rich areas for scavenging, and any places where people deliberately provided food. It then became efficient for males to
compete all year for territories that encompassed those of several females, and to monopolize those females when they
came into season. Males that adopted the “old-fashioned” tactic of roaming widely, expecting to pick up the scent of
receptive females by chance, were likely less successful. The few studies that have investigated male mating patterns
show that the most successful males are often those that combine these tactics, staying “at home” when their own females
were likely coming into season, but also making brief forays to other groups and solitary females nearby in the hope of
achieving successful mating there also.20 Such a no-holds-barred lifestyle takes its toll, however: males are rarely strong
or experienced enough to compete effectively until they are three years old, and many do not survive beyond six or seven,
the victims of road traffic accidents or infections spreading from wounds incurred in fights.
When many cats of both sexes all live in one small area, tomcats are forced to change their tactics. Monopolizing even
one female risks becoming unprofitable, since while his back is turned to deal with one challenger, another male may
sneak in and successfully mate with “his” female. Under these circumstances, the toms appear to accept that each female
will inevitably copulate with several males; instead of attempting to attach themselves to any one or two individuals, they
try to mate with as many females as possible.
This strategy probably did not evolve specifically to allow cats to live at such very high densities. Indeed, such high-
density cat colonies probably occur rarely in nature, although they attract a disproportionate amount of attention from
scientists where they do, given how convenient they are to study. Instead, males that grow up in such large groups
probably learn which tactics work best—or, perhaps more likely, which tactics should be avoided because they risk
injury—from a combination of personal experience and observation of how older males in the colony conduct themselves.
They may also use this knowledge as the older males become too infirm to compete: in the few large colonies that have
been studied, young males rarely emigrate, the opposite of the situation in smaller colonies. This situation may result in
some inbreeding, given the enhanced risk that a male that remains in his family group will mate with a close female
relative. Still, immigration by males and females from outside, attracted by abundant resources—usually a “cluster” of
dedicated cat-feeders—prevents such potential inbreeding from becoming debilitating.
For some time, scientists have known that the coat colors of the kittens in some litters can be accounted for only if some
had one father and some another. If a female attracts several males, she will sometimes refuse all but one; often, however,
she will choose to mate with two or even more. Multiple paternity within a single litter is therefore always a possibility,
but we cannot reliably detect it by coat color alone, especially in large colonies where the males may all look similar.
DNA testing has enabled much more reliable examination of how females’ opportunities for choices at mating translate
into actual choices of fathers for her kittens.
In small colonies with one resident male, females do not appear to have exerted much choice at the time of mating: only
about one litter in five exhibits DNA from any other male. The females may, however, have settled on their choice of
male long beforehand, when they allowed him to live alongside them. Alternatively, he may simply have imposed himself
on them, giving them little option but to wait for another, larger male to turn up if they had doubts about the quality of his
genes. At present, we have little scientific evidence either way.
In larger colonies, females not only mate with several males in sequence, but the large majority of their litters also contain
the DNA of more than one of these. The females may be exerting some choice—they sometimes show a preference for
males from outside their group, thus preventing inbreeding, and also for those males from their own group that are able to
defend the largest territories.
The female’s practice of offering herself to several males one after the other may have another purpose: protecting her
future offspring from infanticide. Each male has observed her being mated by others, but cannot know which of her
kittens are his and which not. Likewise, since each of the larger males has mated with several females, none has any
incentive to kill any litters.
Most urban males today are faced with a new and different problem: how to locate females that are capable of breeding.
Nowadays, an increasing proportion of pet cats are neutered before they are old enough to reproduce. Animal welfare
charities not only promote the spaying of all females before they have even one litter, they also attempt to seek out and
neuter the feral colonies that remain.21 The urban tomcat is unlikely to locate, let alone defend, a harem of reproductive
females. In built-up environments, most tomcats probably adopt the same roaming lifestyle as their wild forebears, hoping
to stumble on a young female whose owners have delayed spaying either because they want her to have a litter or simply
because they are unaware that today’s reliable nutrition enables females to mature more quickly than ever before and can
come into season when they are as young as six months old.
Judging by my admittedly casual observations, tomcats seem unable to distinguish between neutered females that form
the large majority in many towns and cities, and the few, mostly young, females that are still reproductively intact.
Roaming toms still visited annually in late winter to check out my two females ten years after they had been neutered
(having produced one and three litters respectively). Spayed females may be difficult to distinguish from intact females
between seasons, certainly not by the way they behave and possibly not even by their odor, to which tomcats are
presumably very sensitive.
Urban toms thus face a needle-in-a-haystack problem: they are surrounded by hundreds of females, only an otherwise
indistinguishable few of which will ever present them with the opportunity to sire any kittens. The tomcats must therefore
roam as widely as possible, endlessly straining their senses for the yowl and odor of the rare female that is coming into
season. Such toms are shadowy animals; some are theoretically “owned”—though their owners rarely see them—and
many feral. Because they make themselves inconspicuous except when they have located a prize female, there are
probably far more of them than most people realize. When it first became possible to obtain a cat’s DNA fingerprint from
just a few hairs, my research team attempted to locate every litter born in homes in a couple of districts of Southampton,
UK. From what we’d read, we expected to find that just a few “dominant” tomcats had sired most of the litters in each
district; instead, we found that out of more than seventy kittens, virtually all litters had different fathers, only one of which
we were able to locate. We found no evidence for littermates having different fathers, which implied that most estrous
females had attracted only one male. Apparently, by inadvertently “hiding” the few reproductive queens that remain in a
sea of spayed females, the widespread adoption of neutering is making it difficult for even the fiercest, strongest tomcat to
do much more than search at random, thereby giving all the males in the area an even chance of reproducing.
C ats of both sexes have shown remarkable flexibility in adapting their sexual behavior to the various scenarios that we
humans have, over the course of time, imposed on them. One reason that they have coped so well is simply that female
domestic cats are very fecund, capable of producing as many as a dozen kittens per year. Even when conditions for
reproduction are difficult, most free-living female cats will leave at least two or three descendants, enough to maintain the
population even if many of her litters do not survive to adulthood.
Cat society has even adapted to life at high densities, despite origins in territorial rivalry. Logically, that heritage should
result in mayhem, totally unsuitable for raising defenseless infants. Females seem to have resolved this problem by
accepting the advances of several males each time they come into season, thereby making each male uncertain which
kittens are his, and so banishing all thoughts of infanticide. When cat colonies are smaller, females choose to live in small
family groups, and may bond with only one male, whom they accept as being both the most suitable father for their
kittens and the most effective at driving away marauding rivals. When they are deprived of the company of their female
relatives, either because food is insufficient for more than one cat, or due to human intervention, females are equally
capable of bringing up litters without any assistance whatsoever. This remarkable flexibility must have contributed to the
cat adapting to such a wide range of niches.
Male cats have also adapted remarkably well, though in a different way to females. Since in theory one male cat can sire
many hundreds of kittens, the number of males in any one area is never likely to be the factor that limits the size of the cat
population. Nevertheless, each individual male has no interest in the survival of his species, only in producing as many
offspring of his own as he can. When breeding females are few and far between, a male must search as widely as possible
for mating opportunities, pausing only to grab enough food to keep him on the move. If he can be the first and ideally
only male to reach a female at the crucial moment, she is unlikely to be choosy about his quality. If he happens to be in an
area where there are several females, and some of these live in groups, it may benefit him to form a pair bond or harem—
although this seems not to inhibit him from searching for additional females elsewhere, when he judges that he can get
away with it. He will also keep an eye out for other groups of females that he might try to take over in the future, and it is
most likely males that are in this frame of mind that commit the occasional act of infanticide. However, if he finds himself
living in a vast colony with many potential rivals as well as many potential mates, he will learn to curb his natural
aggression, this being the only sensible strategy that will allow him to father some kittens without sustaining mortal
injury.
These tales of sex and violence seems far removed from the cozy world of cat and devoted owner, but these are of course
the ways in which the next generation of cats is created. Only about 15 percent of cats in the Western world come from
planned mating, the great majority arising from liaisons initiated by the cats themselves. Since most pets are now neutered
before they become sexually mature, most cat owners are only dimly aware of the shenanigans that undoctored cats get up
to; spayed females behave as if permanently stuck between cycles of reproductive activity, and males that are neutered
young never develop characteristic tomcat behavior, instead behaving more like neutered females. Tolerance of other cats
is improved by neutering, but only up to a point, and family bonds established between brothers and sisters, or mothers
and their offspring, are still apparent if the cats continue to live in the same house. Unrelated cats that are brought together
by their owners to live in the same household, or that meet on the boundaries between adjacent gardens, still often display
the natural antipathy that they have inherited from their wild ancestors. However, unlike those ancestors, today’s domestic
cats can also establish close bonds with humans, bringing a new dimension to their social networks.
CHAPTER 8
Sonagram of purring: reading from left to right, the in-breaths are shown by the dense “spikes,” with quieter out-breaths
in between
The rumbling sound is produced as the vocal chords are vibrated by a special set of muscles, like a low-pitched hum. The
difference from a hum is that the basic sound is not produced by air passing across the vocal cords and making them
vibrate, but rather by the vocal cords themselves banging together, like an old-fashioned football rattle, ratchet, or
gragger. Often the cat will also hum at the same time, but this is possible only on the out-breaths, reinforcing the sound of
the purr and giving it a more rhythmic quality.
Some cats can add a further meow-like sound to their purr, making it seem more urgent to the human listener.6 These cats
often use their “urgent purr” when soliciting food from their owners, reverting to the normal purr when they are more
content. This version of the purr has not yet been recorded from kittens or from ownerless cats, so it may be that, like the
meow, this is something individual cats learn as an effective way to get something they want.
Purring therefore seems to convey a general request: “Please settle down next to me.” In the gentlest possible way, the
purring cat is asking someone else, whether cat or human, to do something for it. When they purr, kittens are asking their
mother to lie still for long enough to allow them to feed—something they cannot take for granted. As in the wild, where
purring evolved, there may come a time when she will feel too hungry and exhausted to stay with them, and she may have
to choose between feeding them and going hunting to feed herself. Although scientists have not studied this
systematically, when adult cats purr to one another, they are presumably asking the other cat to remain still. I have heard
my cat Libby purring while grooming her mother, Lucy, in a rather aggressive manner, sometimes placing one paw across
her mother’s neck as if to hold her down.
Purring conveys information to those with an ear for it, but not necessarily the emotional state of the cat. Of course, it may
occur when the cat is happy; indeed, this may be the norm. But often, a purring cat—whether a kitten suckling from its
mother, or a pet enjoying being stroked by its owner—purrs not to show that it is contented, but instead to prolong the
circumstances that are making it so. On other occasions, the purring cat may be hungry or mildly anxious because it is
unsure how its owner or another cat is going to react; it may even be experiencing fear or pain. Under all these
circumstances, the cat instinctively uses the purr to try to change the situation to its advantage.
Mark Twain lightheartedly acknowledged that a purring cat might be conning him when he observed, “I simply can’t
resist a cat, particularly a purring one. They are the cleanest, cunningest, and most intelligent things I know . . .”7 To say
that cats are cunning probably overstates their mental abilities: they do not deliberately and consciously deceive their
owners and each other through purring. Rather, each cat simply learns that purring under certain circumstances makes its
life run more smoothly.
With the purr in mind, we can see that the way our cats behave toward us can be widely misinterpreted. Science has
demonstrated that a signal many owners interpret as a sign of love may sometimes mean something else. Insofar as we
know, purring is not central to affectionate relationships in cat society, apart from its original role in the bond between a
mother cat and her kittens. However, other signals that we tend to overlook may in fact be more genuine displays of
affection. Relationships between adult cats seem to be sustained mainly through mutual licking and rubbing, so we should
examine whether these also indicate affection when our cats direct such behavior toward us.
Many cats lick their owners on a regular basis, but scientists have not yet investigated why this should be. Cats that do
not lick their owners may have been put off because their owners resisted being licked in the past; the cat’s tongue is
covered with backward-facing spines that work well at disentangling fur, but can feel harsh on human skin. Conceivably,
some cats may lick their owners because they like the taste: some researchers speculate that they lick us for the salt on our
skins, but cats don’t seem to have a strong preference for salty flavors.8 The most likely explanation is a social one, that
the cat is trying to convey something to its owner about their relationship. The question is, what that something is. It may
vary from one cat to another, as also seems to be the case when one cat grooms another (allogrooming).
The reason must be basically affectionate, because two cats that don’t like each other never groom each other, although
grooming can apparently reunite two cats that have recently quarreled. Cats licking their owners may sometimes be
attempting to “apologize” for something the cat thinks it has done wrong—possibly something the owner has not even
noticed, but that has some significance for the cat. However, a cat that licks its owner’s hand with one paw placed over
her wrist may be attempting to exert some sort of control over her. Until we know more about why cats groom one
another, we can only speculate on why they groom us.
Cat owners also engage in a tactile ritual with their cats, of course, when they stroke them. Most owners stroke their cats
simply because it gives them pleasure, and because the cat also seems to enjoy it, but stroking may also have symbolic
meaning for the cat, possibly substituting for allogrooming in some and possibly for rubbing in others. Most cats prefer to
be stroked on their heads than any other part of the body, precisely the area toward which cats direct their grooming;
studies show that fewer than one in ten cats likes to be stroked on the belly or around the tail.
Many cats do not simply accept their owner’s stroking passively; rather, they regularly invite their owners to stroke them,
perhaps by jumping on their laps or rolling over in front of them. These rituals may not have any underlying significance,
simply being mutually agreed exchanges that particular cats and their owners have learned will lead to pleasurable
interactions. But while the owner has to initiate the actual stroking, most cats then indicate precisely where they wish to
be stroked by offering that part of the body, or by shifting their position to place it under the owner’s stroking hand.9 By
accepting our petting, cats are doing more than enjoying themselves: in their minds, they are almost certainly engaging in
a social ritual that is reinforcing the bond with their owner.
Some scientists have speculated further, that the cat is also deliberately inviting its owner to take up its scent. Cats may
prefer to be stroked around the cheeks and ears because those areas are equipped with skin glands that emit perfumes that
appeal to other cats, and the cat wishes the owner to take on the smell of these specific glands.10 Cats have similar glands
on areas of skin that they don’t generally like their owners to touch, such as at the base of the tail, so this theory implies
that the cat does not want its owner to smell of these other areas. The subtle smell of the cat, virtually undetectable to our
noses, will inevitably be transferred onto the owner’s hand by the act of stroking, but this exchange may not have much
social significance for the cat. If it did, cats would presumably be constantly sniffing our hands; while this does
sometimes happen, cats do not do so obsessively. More likely, the primary function of the stroking ritual lies in its tactile
component.
While touch is very important to cats, a common visual signal, the upright tail, is probably the clearest way cats show
their affection for us. In the same way that an upright tail is a sign of friendly intentions between two cats, so it must be
when directed at a person. When cats raise their tails to another cat, they usually wait to see whether the other will
reciprocate before approaching, but this is obviously impossible when the recipient is human. Presumably each cat learns
enough about their owner’s body language to be able to work out, first, whether they’ve been noticed—they tend not to
raise their tails until they have been—and second, whether the owner is ready to interact. Or at least, most cats do: my
long-haired cat Splodge sometimes startled me by approaching while my back was turned, and jumping up to rub his head
against the side of my knee.
Since this tail-up signal seems unique to the domestic cat, we do not know whether it evolved first as a signal to be
directed at people, and then became useful to maintain amicable relationships with other cats, or vice versa. The latter
seems more likely, however: because the raised tail has its origins in kitten-mother interactions, all cats are presumably
born with an innate sense of its significance, and adults are therefore able to extend its use for interactions with other cats.
The alternative explanation seems more far-fetched, since we would have to assume that the first people to domesticate
the cat found this gesture so appealing that they deliberately favored cats that did it every time they met them.
As when two cats meet, a cat approaching its owner with tail raised will often rub on its owner’s legs. The form that the
rub takes seems to vary from cat to cat, and despite years of research I am still uncertain whether there is any significance
in which part of the body the cat uses to rub. Some rub just with the side of their head, others continue the rub down their
flank, and some routinely make contact with head, flank, and tail. Many simply walk past without making any contact at
all. A few, like Splodge, jump as they initiate the rub, so that the side of the head makes contact with the owner’s knee,
and the flank caresses the owner’s calf.
Some more nervous cats often prefer to perform their rubs on a physical object nearby, such as a chair leg or the edge of a
door. Splodge’s great-great niece Libby was a classic example. Even confident cats will sometimes do this when they
don’t know the person well, even though they are perfectly happy to rub on their owner’s leg. Indeed, most times this
happens, the cat is probably just redirecting its rub onto an object because it is confident that the object, unlike a familiar
person, will not push it away. However, sometimes when this happens it looks as though the cat is also scent marking the
object with the glands on the side of its head. Scent is certainly deposited: when I’ve invited such cats to rub on posts
covered in paper, those pieces of paper excite a great deal of interest when presented to cats in other households.
However, the redirected rubbing behavior is not performed in precisely the same way as when a cat is deliberately scent
marking an object. The difference can easily be seen if the same cat is presented at head height with the blunt end of a
pencil, mimicking the protruding twigs that are many cats’ favorite targets for “bunting” with their heads.
Rubbing can only be a sign of affection. Because many cats rub most intensely when they are about to be fed, they have
been accused of showing nothing more than what we British call cupboard love. However, few cats confine their rubbing
to occasions when they are expecting to receive a tangible reward. When two cats rub on each other, they exchange no
food or any other currency; after the rub, each usually continues with what it was doing before. Such an exchange of rubs
is a declaration of affection between the two animals—nothing more, nothing less.
Cats also rub on animals other than cats and humans, even animals that do not understand the significance of the ritual and
are unlikely to give anything in return. Splodge used to perform the tail-up/rubbing ritual on our Labrador retriever,
Bruno. Bruno was already a couple of years old when Splodge arrived in our house as an eight-week-old kitten; although
he had not been brought up with cats, Bruno was too laid back to think about chasing them, so Splodge regarded him as a
friend from the very start. Of course, Bruno never fed Splodge—quite the opposite: as a typical Lab he was all too eager
to finish up any uneaten cat food—so the cupboard love explanation cannot possibly apply. Again, the only plausible
significance must be social.
Scent must become transferred from cat to owner during rubbing, but this does not seem to hold any particular
significance for the cat. Most owners certainly seem oblivious to this possibility—although apparently not Mark Twain,
who once observed, “That cat will write her autograph all over your leg if you let her.”11 If the primary motivation for
rubbing was to leave scent behind, cats should constantly try to sniff people’s legs to discover if any other cat had left its
scent there. Of course, our habit of changing our clothes regularly cannot help, but the resulting confusion might lead to
more sniffing rather than less. All the evidence points to rubbing, like stroking, as a primarily tactile display.
When two cats rub each other, they don’t do so in equal measures, and a similar asymmetry seems to apply when cats rub
on humans and other animals. When two cats of a different size approach each other, the smaller cat usually rubs on the
larger, which usually doesn’t reciprocate. When Splodge rubbed on Bruno, who was substantially larger, Splodge’s
instincts probably told him not to count on any particular response. Likewise, when our cats rub on us, our greater height,
along with their knowledge that we are in control of most of the resources in the house, probably lead them to expect the
rub to be one-sided. They are showing their affection for us in a way that doesn’t demand a response—which is just as
well, because unless we bend down and stroke them, we generally don’t reply, at least not at the time.
Cats presumably find rubbing on us rewarding in its own right—if they didn’t, they’d probably give up doing it—and like
most of their attempts to communicate with us, they learn how to do it gradually. Kittens spontaneously rub on older cats
with which they are friendly, and continue to do this as they get older. However, after they arrive in their new homes at
around eight weeks of age, young cats (especially females) may take several weeks or even months to start rubbing on
their new owners, as if they need time to work out how best to use this behavior to cement the relationship. Once the habit
is formed, however, it seems to become fixed.12
Cats are more than intelligent enough to learn how to get our attention when they need to. Many use purring to persuade
us to do something they want us to do, and a few invent their own personal rituals, such as jumping onto laps, or walking
along the mantelpiece dangerously close to valued ornaments. However, the meow comes nearest to being their universal
method of attracting our attention.
Purring is too quiet and low-pitched to be of much use for this sort of summons. Cats also have a greeting call that they
use toward one another, a brief, soft “chirrup” sound; for example, mothers use this when returning to their kittens.13
Some cats will also use this chirrup to greet their owners: my cat Splodge used to greet me with this sound when he came
back from a roam around the garden. Knowing a bit about cat behavior, I would try to chirrup back at him—something he
evidently appreciated, as this exchange became something of a ritual between us.
The meow is part of the cat’s natural repertoire, but they rarely use it to communicate with other cats, and its meaning in
cat society is somewhat obscure. Feral cats occasionally meow when one is following another, perhaps to get the cat in
front to stop and participate in a friendly exchange of rubs. However, feral cats are generally rather silent animals,
nowhere near as vocal as their domestic counterparts. While all cats are apparently born knowing how to meow, each has
to learn how to use it to communicate most effectively.
The meow is much the same wherever the cat happens to live, confirming that it is instinctive. Every human language has
a linguistic representation of this type of call:
The English cat “mews,” the Indian cat “myaus,” the Chinese cat says “mio,” the Arabian cat “naoua” and the Egyptian cat “mau.” To illustrate how
difficult it is to interpret the cat’s language, her “mew” is spelled in thirty-one different ways [in English alone], five examples being maeow, me-ow,
mieaou, mouw, and murr-raow.14
Domestication appears to have subtly modified the sound of the cat’s meow. All Felis silvestris wildcats can make a
meow sound, wherever they live, whether the north of Scotland or South Africa. Meows performed by southern African
wildcats, Felis silvestris cafra, are lower-pitched and more drawn out than typical domestic cat meows. When researchers
played recordings of these wildcat meows to cat owners, the owners rated them significantly less pleasant than domestic
cat meows.15 During the course of domestication, humans may have selected for a cat with a meow that is easier on our
ears. However, it’s equally possible that the meow of the direct ancestor of the domestic cat, Felis (silvestris) lybica, was
different from that of its southern African cousin.
Feral cat meows are not as guttural as wildcat meows, but not as sweet-sounding as those of pet cats. Keep in mind that
feral cats are genetically almost identical to pet cats, which suggests that their calls—as much else in cat behavior—are
profoundly affected by each individual’s early experiences of people. Pet kittens don’t start meowing until after they’re
weaned, so as they become old enough to meow, they likely try out a range of meows on their owners, quickly finding
that higher-pitched versions produce a more positive reaction. As with so much of cat behavior, differences between the
wild meow and the domestic meow seem to be partly genetic and partly learned; domestication has enhanced cats’ ability
to learn how to use their meow, but may have also altered its basic sound.
Cats can also modify their meows to suit different situations: some are coaxing, and others more urgent and demanding.
They do this by altering their pitch and duration, or by combining the meow with another of their calls, perhaps a chirrup
or a growl. Owners often say that they know what their cats want from the tone of their meow. However, when scientists
recorded meows from twelve cats and then asked owners to guess the circumstances under which the meow had been
uttered, few guessed correctly. Angry meows had a characteristic tone, as did affectionate meows, but meows requesting
food, asking for a door to be opened, and appealing for help were not identifiable as such, even though they made sense to
each cat’s owner in the context in which they were uttered.16 Therefore, once cats have learned that their owners respond
to meows, many likely develop a repertoire of different meows that, by trial and error, they learn are effective in specific
circumstances. How this unfolds will depend on which meows get rewarded by the owner, through achieving what the cat
wants—a bowl of food, a rub on the head, opening a door. Each cat and its owner gradually develop an individual
“language” that they both understand, but that is not shared by other cats or other owners. This is, of course, a form of
training; but unlike the formalities of dog training, the cat and the owner are unwittingly training each other.
If we can decode them, the meows that we inadvertently train each of our cats to use may provide us with a window into
their emotional lives. Our universal recognition of the “angry meow” and the “affectionate meow” suggests that each has
an underlying and invariant emotional component, as the names I’ve used for them imply. The cat uses the others, the
“request meows,” simply to attract its owner’s attention. The context within which they occur provides clues on what the
cat wants—whether it is sitting beside a closed door, or walking around the kitchen gazing at the cupboard where the food
is kept. The meows themselves are probably emotionally neutral.
Signs that Cats in a Household Do or Don’t Get Along with One Another23
Cats that see themselves as part of the same social group generally
•hold their tails upright when they see one another
•rub on one another, either when walking past or alongside one another
•regularly sleep in contact with one another
•play gentle, “mock-fighting” games with one another
•share their toys
Cats that have set up separate territories within the house will tend to
•chase or run away from one another
•hiss or spit when they meet
•avoid contact with one another: one cat may always leave the room when another enters
•sleep in widely separated places; often, one will sleep high up, perhaps on a shelf, to avoid another
•sleep defensively; the cat has its eyes closed and looks as if it is asleep, but its posture is tense and its ears may twitch
•apparently restrict one another’s movements on purpose—for example, one cat sitting for hours by the cat-flap, or at the top
of the stairs
•watch one another intently
•look unusually tense when they’re in the same room
•interact separately with their owner—for example, they may sit either side of the owner to avoid physical contact with one
another
Surprisingly few owners seem particularly bothered about such conflicts until they begin to affect their cat’s health: a bite
can turn into an abscess requiring veterinary treatment. More ominously, the cat may become so stressed, or its
movements so restricted, that it begins to urinate or defecate in the house (see box below, “Signs that a Cat Is Failing to
Establish a Territory Outside Its Owner’s House”). Even if the various cats in an area eventually arrive at a truce, owners
may inadvertently reignite the conflict by temporarily removing their cat, perhaps boarding it elsewhere for a couple of
weeks while they go on holiday. Encouraged by the signs that the cat may have left for good, such as fading scent-marks,
absence of sightings, one or more of the neighbors’ cats may start to encroach into what was previously that cat’s
territory. When that cat returns, it may have to reestablish its rights all over again.
Signs that a Cat Is Failing to Establish a Territory Outside Its Owner’s House24
•Not leaving the house even when encouraged to do so
•Waiting to be let out by the owner rather than using its cat-flap (because there might be a rival cat ready to ambush it on the
other side)
•Neighbor’s cats entering the house through the cat-flap
•Leaving the house only if the owner is in the garden
•Excessive time watching out of windows
•Running away from windows and hiding when another cat is spotted in the garden
•Running into the house and immediately to a place of safety, far from the access point
•Tense interaction with the owner, including rough play
•Urinating and defecating in the house, by a cat that usually goes outside to do this but feels too insecure to do so
•Spraying urine (scent-marking) in the house, especially if near to access points such as doors and cat-flaps (more likely in
male cats than females)
•Other signs of psychological stress, such as excessive grooming
Increasingly, cat owners are avoiding such problems by keeping their cats indoors, although their motivation may be more
to protect their pet from traffic, disease, or potential thieves (especially if it is a valuable pedigree animal) than from social
stress. Restricting the cat to one relatively small area for its entire life can induce stresses of its own. Although the
practice of keeping cats indoors has been common among apartment-dwellers for more than thirty years, we have little
systematic research into whether domestic cats find this confinement stressful. To see how we should expect indoor cats
to behave if they were stressed by confinement, we must therefore look further afield, to their wild ancestors.
Wild felids often react badly to being confined. Both “big cats” such as lions and “small cats,” such as jungle and leopard
cats, are prone to the habit, once commonplace in zoos all over the world, of pacing to and fro in their cages.25 Of other
types of animal, only bears are as badly affected, and like most of the cat family, they are also solitary territorial
carnivores. We do not fully understand why these animals pace, but the reasons probably arise from a mixture of
frustration at not having access to a large enough hunting territory, even though their nutritional needs are being more
than satisfied, and “boredom”: well-fed carnivores do instinctively sleep for much of the time, but many seem to crave
mental stimulation when they are awake. Changing the way the wild cat is fed can provide the latter: rather than simply
providing one daily meal that can be wolfed down in seconds, zookeepers now provide food several times each day, and
the cats must make an effort to get at least some of it—for example, by feeding bones that have to be cracked open, or
placing the food in puzzle feeders that the cat has to work at for an extended period of time.
When considering whether domestic cats kept indoors are likely to suffer, we might first examine whether they show
signs of objecting to being spatially restricted, and whether they show signs of “boredom.” Repetitive pacing is
surprisingly rare in domestic cats, considering that this is the most common abnormal behavior observed in cats kept in
zoos. This difference may have already evolved prior to domestication; when in captivity, the domestic cat’s wild ancestor
(Felis silvestris) is more prone to “apathetic resting” (taking no notice of its surroundings) than to pacing. The difference
could also be a consequence of domestication. Whichever the culprit, domestic cats seem to have lost much of their
ancestors’ spontaneous “drive” to roam. We do not know why this loss would have benefited cats: for most of their
10,000-year history, they have had to hunt for their living.
Domestication may have given the cat a much greater flexibility in its territorial behavior. Wild felids generally feed on
prey dispersed over the landscape around them, and therefore have always needed a large territory: they may need to
venture farther afield if food is scarce, but in all their evolutionary history, they would never have encountered a situation
where food was so locally plentiful that they could afford not to go out foraging day after day. Domestic cats, by contrast,
have adapted to hunting in and around what are, by comparison, very small areas—human settlements—while retaining
the ability to expand their hunting ranges quickly if food becomes scarce, provided that other cats don’t prevent them
from doing so.
Feral domestic cats can thus have territories 10,000 times larger than those of some owned cats that are allowed twenty-
four-hour outdoor access. However, just because the species as a whole shows this flexibility does not mean that
individual cats are so adaptable. Much depends on their previous lifestyle and what expectations they have acquired. A
feral cat accustomed to hunting across fifty acres and then suddenly confined to a pen will be almost as distressed as its
wildcat counterpart. A pet cat that has never had to hunt to survive would most likely perish if abandoned somewhere
remote.
Domestic cats have probably become so flexible in their demands for space that they can, under the right circumstances,
adapt adequately to indoor living. Very few cats that are allowed outdoors voluntarily restrict themselves to an area as
small as even the most spacious apartment, and those that do would probably venture farther away if they were not
anxious about meeting other cats. However, the additional restriction does not appear to cause undue stress. Cats that have
grown up wandering wherever they choose will almost certainly be stressed by being suddenly confined indoors, even
when this becomes necessary to protect the cat’s health. Cats that are destined for a life indoors should probably never be
allowed outdoors, so that they can’t miss what they’ve never had.
Space that is restricted needs to be quality space. It’s unlikely that wild cats value open space for the simple joy of the
view into the distance; rather, they presumably gain satisfaction from how many places they can see that might be
concealing their next meal. Zookeepers have tried giving big cats access to wide open spaces, but this usually had no
effect on their habitual pacing, and in fact they rarely visit the additional territory. However, making the same amount of
space more interesting was a much more successful strategy; zoos adapted the enclosures so that the cat could not see the
entire area from any one place, requiring the cat to move around.
An indoor cat must also be kept busy, since it cannot experience the variety automatically provided by the outdoors—nor,
admittedly, the anxiety of being ambushed by another cat. For the owner, this requires extra effort (see box on page 217,
“Keeping an Indoor Cat Happy”), which must be balanced against the relative ease of allowing a cat to seek much of its
mental stimulation outdoors. In particular, owners should allow the cat to perform as much of its “natural” behavior as
possible. Although there is no specific scientific evidence to support such a recommendation for the domestic cat, it is one
of the guiding principles of animal welfare that have been established for vertebrate animals in general.
The owner can provide the cat with social behavior either by spending time with it or by keeping two compatible cats
together. The easiest way to achieve the latter is probably to obtain two kittens from the same litter, although even this is
not guaranteed to be successful—sibling rivalry is not unheard of among cats. Hunting behavior can be simulated by
giving the cat a view of some “natural” space, through “play” with prey-sized toys (since cats react to these as if they
really are prey), and feeding dry cat food in a device that requires the cat to perform predatory behavior to release each
piece (simulation of hunting behavior has been successfully used to restore normal behavior in captive wildcats, so it
should also benefit indoor domestic cats).26 None of these may be entirely satisfactory substitutes, but any stress that the
cat feels at being restricted or not allowed to carry out all its natural behavior may be counterbalanced by its being
removed from the stress of being “bullied” by other cats in the neighborhood.
Considering how little time has elapsed since they were first domesticated, cats demonstrate remarkable flexibility when
it comes to the amount of space they need. However, we must be careful not to provide them with too little space, or space
that contains incipient threats. Pet cats have no practical need to maintain a hunting territory any more, but it is too soon
for evolution to have removed the desire to do so. Unfortunately for the cats, this perceived need to roam can bring them
into daily conflict with other cats, each with the same agenda. Cats, having a relatively unsophisticated repertoire for
communicating with other cats, take time to “negotiate” their territory boundaries, time that increasingly we no longer
provide them with.
Cats face great pressure to change their ways—and not only to adapt to modern urban lifestyles. The conservation lobby,
from Australasia to the United States to Great Britain, increasingly objects to their maintaining any kind of hunting
territory. To change, cats must evolve new ways to organize their behavior, at an unprecedented speed. Evolution requires
variation: cats must differ from one another in the way they perceive and react to their environment, both social and
physical. We still see great variation among cats in their personalities, and somewhere among these, we might find the
combination of characteristics for the ideal twenty-first-century cat.
CHAPTER 9
Cats as Individuals
C ats have much in common with one another; as a species, they are highly distinctive, so what is true of one cat is
also likely true of another. But cats are also unquestionably individuals, both in appearance and—more significantly for
their relationship with their owner, and for their future—in the way they behave. Even scientists now talk freely of cats
having their own personalities. The existence of many different personality types among today’s cats gives us hope that
they, as a species, have the potential to adapt to the demands of the twenty-first century and beyond. Somewhere hidden
among the cats that live around us today are the genes that will enable their offspring to evolve into a slightly different
kind of cat; for example, one that is better adapted to living indoors.
Of course, genes alone cannot drive such changes; the environment in which a cat finds itself plays a powerful role in
guiding the development of its personality. Furthermore, cats do not have to make these changes on their own; we as
owners have a wealth of strategies we can employ to help them to lead happier lives. If cats’ genes were as unvarying as
those of some pedigree dog breeds—many of which contain little more variation in total than does the average human
extended family—then no amount of breeding for temperament would achieve much: the only way forward would be for
cat owners to change the way they relate to their cats. But knowing that cats are so genetically variable, even today,
provides us with two complementary ways to help them adapt to our world.
The crucial question is, how much influence do genes have? Much of the cat’s personality depends on other factors. For
example, whether a cat will tolerate people depends on whether it has had contact—and the right kind of contact—with
people during the first eight weeks of its life. Cats that do receive such contact nevertheless vary greatly in how friendly
they are to people in general, and even toward their owner. How much of this variation, not fully explained by the basic
process of socialization, is inherited? Is each cat’s ability to tolerate other cats due simply to whether it grows up with
other cats, or are some born to be more adaptable than others?
Decoding the inheritance of personality is nowhere near as simple as the inheritance of the color or length of a cat’s coat.
We can track most of those visible differences among cats to twenty or so well-defined genes that operate in a highly
predictable manner. If a cat’s parents both have black coats, then the cat will also be black; this is not affected by whether
it is born in a hedge or in a kitchen.
Genes and environment can interact in complex ways, however. Even coat colors can be affected by the environment: for
example, the darker “points” on a Siamese cat’s face, paws, and ears come from a temperature-sensitive mutation that
prevents the hairs from taking up their usual color at normal body temperature. As newborns, these cats are whitish all
over because their mother’s womb is uniformly warm. As they grow and the extremities of the body become cooler, the
hair there grows darker, producing the characteristic “pointed” coat. Finally, as the cat enters old age and the circulation
of blood in its skin deteriorates slightly, it gradually turns brown all over.
The relationship between genetics and environment is evident in personality as well. Cat personality is influenced by
hundreds of genes and a lifetime of experience, interacting together to produce the cats we see today.
T o search for evidence that personality can be inherited, we might start with pedigree cats. Unlike dogs, which have been
bred for different functions for many centuries, pedigree cats have been bred mainly for their looks. Deliberate selection is
probably not to blame for any consistent differences in behavior between different cat breeds; we cannot expect to find
differences as great as those between, say, a border collie and a Labrador retriever. However, because all pedigree cats are
raised by breeders, and, at least within each country, in much the same way, any consistent behavioral differences
between them are likely due to genetics.
The breeding of pedigree or “show” cats is regulated by standards laid down by the individual “breed clubs,” and the best
cats from each breed compete in cat shows run by organizations such as the Cat Fanciers’ Association and the
International Cat Association in the United States, and the Governing Council of the Cat Fancy in the UK. Well-known
breeds or groups of breeds include the Persian or “Exotic” breeds, stocky cats with long hair and flat faces; the “Foreign”
breeds, fine-boned, long-limbed cats such as the Siamese, Burmese, and Abyssinian; and the Domestic breeds, which as
their name implies were originally derived from ordinary domestic cats local to the British Isles. Some individual breeds
can be defined by a single mutation, such as the short, wavy coat of the Cornish Rex, the downy hair of the Sphynx, and
the short tail of the Manx.
Many of the newer breeds of cat are simply color variations on existing breeds. For example, the Havana Brown is
genetically indistinguishable from the Siamese, except that it lacks the mutation that causes most of the Siamese’s coat to
stay cream-colored. Some of the longer-established breeds claim ancient ancestry—for example, the Siamese breed is
apparently described in the “Cat-Book Poems” written in the ancient Siamese city of Ayutthaya sometime between 1350
and 1750—but their DNA shows that the breeds have become separate entities only during the past 150 years or so.1
This recent evidence separates the breeds into roughly six groups, each seemingly derived from—or possibly allowed to
interbreed with—local street cats. The DNA of the Siamese, Havana Brown, Singapura, Burmese, Korat, and Birman
shows not only that they are closely related, but that they are also genetically similar to the street cats of Southeast Asia
from which they were undoubtedly derived. The Bobtail, a traditional Japanese breed, is genetically close to random-bred
cats in Korea, China, and Singapore (and, presumably those of Japan, which were not included in the study). The Turkish
Van cat, as its name implies, is related to non-pedigree cats from Turkey, as well as Italy, Israel, and Egypt. The Siberian
and Norwegian Forest cats are derived from longhaired northern European random-bred cats, while the superficially
similar Maine Coon finds its closest non-pedigree relatives in New York state. Most of the stockier breeds—the American
and British Shorthairs, the Chartreux, the Russian Blue, and surprisingly the Persian and Exotic breeds, are all closely
related and are presumably derived from Western European stock. The modern Persian, even if some of its distant
ancestors did come from the Middle East, seems to have lost most traces of its origins, possibly due to recent breeding to
produce the flat (brachycephalic) face preferred by its devotees.
The various breed clubs usually describe typical personalities for their cats. For example, the Governing Council of the
Cat Fancy (GCCF) describes the Ocicat, an American breed derived from Abyssinian, Siamese, and American Shorthair
lines, as follows:
Many owners remark on the almost doglike tendencies of the Breed, in that they are devoted to people, are easily trained and respond well to the voice,
but retain their independence as a proper cat should, and are very intelligent. Because of their adaptability they are a joy to be with, they are not
demanding in any way and seem to take life in their stride. Ocicats are reasonably vocal and do not like being left alone for long periods, but do make
ideal companions for households with other pets, and are confident with children.2
Although such formal recognition permeates the world of cat enthusiasts, scientists have devoted little attention to
investigating whether cat breeds have distinctive personalities. The line-breeding necessary to develop cats that breed
“true”—that is, where the offspring look the same as their parents—has led to some behavioral abnormalities that have a
genetic basis (see box below, “Fabric Eating in Pedigree Oriental Cats”). Because these are essentially pathologies, and
isolated within a single breed or group of breeds, scientists do not classify these abnormalities as aspects of “personality.”
Turning to more universal cat behavior, Siamese and other Oriental cats are remarkably vocal; many develop so many
variations on the meow that they seem to “talk” to their owners. Longhaired cats, especially Persians, have a reputation
for being lethargic and not terribly fond of close contact with people, perhaps because these cats overheat easily. Beyond
such self-evident differences, we have little hard information on precisely how breeds differ in personality, and how those
differences arise. Most of the information we have is based on surveys of experts—veterinarians or cat-show judges, for
example—who tend to see most cats when they are away from their normal territories, and therefore may not always get a
complete picture of their behavior.
One small-scale study conducted in Norway confirmed that Siamese and Persian cats do indeed behave in characteristic
ways in their owners’ homes.3 Although the cats’ personalities were recorded by the owners themselves (which in itself
could have introduced some biases) rather than through direct observation, the Siamese were reported to be more contact-
seeking, more vocal, and more playful and active than standard house cats. One in ten Siamese was regularly aggressive
toward people, compared to one in twenty house cats and one in sixty Persians. Persians were generally less active than
other cats, and apparently more tolerant of unfamiliar people and cats—although their apparent laziness might have
simply made them disinclined to run away.
It’s highly implausible that every single variation in cat personality will be traced to a different gene. Rather, breed
characteristics seem to emerge during kittenhood as general tendencies, such as when making the choice whether to
explore or to move away from novel objects or situations. In turn, these tendencies profoundly affect what each kitten
learns, and thus how its behavior develops: tactics it learns are useful in one particular circumstance may become general
strategies, used in many situations. The underlying processes have scarcely been investigated in pedigree cats, but in one
study, researchers found that the ability of Norwegian Forest kittens to remember novel situations develops more slowly
than those of other pedigree cats (Oriental breeds and Abyssinians), whose brains may develop somewhat more quickly
than those of ordinary house cats.4 Such slowing-down and speeding-up of the rates at which different parts of the brain
grow might have long-term effects on cat personality. Many of the self-evident differences in behavior between dog
breeds result from changes in the speed at which different areas of the brain develop: for example, Siberian Huskies
display a full range of wolf-type behavior, while breeds with “baby faces,” such as bulldogs, signal to one another in a
similar way to wolf cubs of just a few weeks old.5 However, scientists have not yet documented any such link for cats.
Differences among breeds provide useful insight into whether cat behavior might be influenced by genetics, and pedigree
breeds are useful in this regard because each cat’s parentage is documented. Popular males can sire many kittens, yet
rarely even see any of them, so their influence on their offspring must be genetic. In the Norwegian study, playfulness,
fearfulness, and confidence in encounters with unfamiliar people were all distinctively different between the offspring of
different fathers, although some other traits, such as aggression to cats or people, were not. Because this study was small-
scale and carried out in only one country, its details may not apply everywhere; still, the principle that some aspects of a
cat’s behavior are influenced by its father’s genes seems likely to stand.6
Non-pedigree cats also vary greatly in their “personalities,” spurring on the myth that a cat’s temperament and its coat
color are inextricably linked.7 The British refer to tortoiseshell cats as “naughty torties”; likewise, blotched tabbies are
“real homebodies,” mackerel tabbies are “independent,” and white patches on a cat’s coat have a “calming effect” on the
animal. It seems part of human nature to link outward appearance and inner character, and to continue to see those links
even when evidence is to the contrary. Some scientists have speculated that the specific biochemistry that generates
different coat colors also somehow affects the way a cat’s brain works, showing a genetic effect referred to as pleitropy,
but little evidence has been found to support this idea in cats.8
Links between coat color and personality do occasionally occur among pedigree cats, and these do provide an opportunity
for proper investigation because the family trees are available. The relatively restricted gene pool for each color within
each breed does result in certain temperaments accidentally becoming associated with particular coat colors. At any one
time, only a limited number of high-quality tomcats within each breed are available to produce the desired color; as a
result, the temperament of the most popular of those tomcats—or at least those aspects affected by genetics—tends to
become predominant within that section of the breed. For example, twenty years ago, Scotland’s British Shorthair cats
with tortoiseshell, cream, and especially red (a rare, un-patterned version of orange) coats were relatively difficult to
handle; scientists traced this characteristic back to one male with a particularly difficult temperament.9 Likewise, cats with
dark “points” on their paws and ears, even if not pedigree Siamese, are likely unusually vocal, because the gene that
causes the points to appear is very rare in any cat without at least one Siamese in its recent ancestry.
Coat color and some aspect of personality can also become linked if the gene that controls the color and a gene affecting
the way the brain develops happen to occur very close together on the same chromosome. Because genes are grouped
together on chromosomes—cats have thirty-eight: eighteen pairs, plus two sex chromosomes—not all combinations are
passed on randomly from one generation to the next. If two genes occur on different chromosomes, then the chances that a
kitten will receive any particular combination of the two are essentially random. However, two genes that occur on the
same chromosome tend to be inherited together. This is not inevitable, because matching pairs of chromosomes do
occasionally swap sections between each other, by a mechanism known as crossing over; if the swap happens in between
the two genes in question, they are then inherited separately. Such exchanges rarely occur between genes that are sited
close together on the same chromosome. For example, the gene that causes a white coat (“dominant white,” that is;
different from albino) is situated on the same chromosome and close to another gene that causes both the eyes to be blue
and the cat to be deaf, a rare example of one gene affecting both appearance and (indirectly) behavior. Blue-eyed, white
cats are thus almost invariably deaf.10 In the case of the ginger cats in rural France, the gene that suits those cats to the
feral lifestyle might simply be very close to the O(range)-gene (on the X-chromosome), rather than being a direct effect of
the cat being orange.
M aking assumptions about a cat’s personality based solely on its appearance is often misleading, but cats undoubtedly
do behave in individual ways, irrespective of their color. Until about twenty years ago, most scientists considered that
only humans could have “personalities,” yet now this concept is widely applied to animals—and not just to domestic
animals. Even wild animals behave in consistently different ways that reflect different ways of reacting to the world
around them: over the past few years, the concept of “personality” has been applied to animals as diverse as lizards,
crickets, bees, chimpanzees, and geese. Some individuals may be particularly bold, and therefore the first to exploit a new
food source, whereas others are particularly shy and therefore less likely to run headlong into dangerous situations. The
success of each strategy is likely to vary depending on what the environment is like, and as that changes, so sometimes
bold individuals will do best, other times they will be the ones who perish first. In this way, the genes that influence both
types persist in the species.
Some of the most complex effects of personality occur in social situations. Sticklebacks, fish that sometimes swim in
shoals, can be classed as either bold or shy. When a fish has a choice of shoals consisting entirely of bold or shy
individuals, it will choose to join the bold shoal, irrespective of whether it is bold or shy itself. Bold shoals usually find
more food, and a shy fish will find the middle of a shoal of bold fish a good place to hide. However, to keep up with the
bold shoal, it must swim faster than usual, so it temporarily starts behaving more like a bold fish. Although we don’t yet
know much about social effects on cat personality, such observations raise the fascinating possibility that each cat may be
able to adjust its personality to fit in with those of the other animals—human, feline, and canine—in the household in
which it finds itself.
We have two broad approaches to studying cat personality: watching the cats, or asking their owners. Because owners are
likely to be biased, observing the cat’s behavior is the only way an impression of its personality can be gained. For this
reason, most of my own studies have involved recording cats’ behavior. To ensure that outdoor cats would be home, I
chose to observe them just before and just after their usual feeding time.11 Since many cats interact most intensely with
their owners when expecting food, and since hunger usually affects the way they interact, this arrangement had the
additional advantage that all the cats would have been hungry when the observation started, and sated at the end.
While their food was being prepared, the thirty-six cats in the study acted as cats usually do when expecting to be fed:
walking around the kitchen with their tails upright, meowing, and rubbing on their owners’ legs. After the meal, some
went straight outdoors, while others sat and groomed themselves; some interacted with their owners again, while others
investigated the unfamiliar human in the room—that is, the person making the observations. So far, so obvious; but the
first objective of our study was to find out whether each cat behaved in a characteristic way every time. We repeated these
visits once a week for eight weeks, and found that indeed they were indeed fairly consistent—so what we had measured
was probably a reflection of the cat’s personality, or at least its “personal style.”
Minimizing the impact of feral cats on wildlife is much more difficult when they live near pet cats—which, on the
mainland, is almost everywhere. We have few reliable estimates on how much damage such feral cats do, mainly because
they are rarely the only predator present, competing with both their native equivalents and also introduced species such as
the red fox and the rat. Feral cats, even those that obtain some of their food from handouts or scavenging, are of necessity
much more “serious” hunters than the vast majority of pet cats, and so per capita must be responsible for more damage to
wildlife.
In many locations, human activity has reduced areas of conservation interest (“biodiversity value”) into small “islands,”
albeit islands surrounded by concrete rather than water. For example, urbanization has broken the once-contiguous
heathland habitat of the sand lizard on the south coast of England into fragments, making each isolated population highly
vulnerable to extinction by wildfires. In other, similarly fragmented habitats feral cats could potentially cause
considerable damage, although well-documented examples remain scarce. Eradication of feral cats from areas where they
coexist with pet cats is both problematic and ultimately unproductive. Unless all pet cats are curfewed, or kept
permanently indoors, or compulsorily registered and microchipped, it is virtually impossible to be certain that a cat that
has been trapped is feral, particularly if it is somewhat socialized to people. Even if local eradication was achieved, the
niche formerly occupied by the feral cats would still exist, and would soon be filled by stray cats or by ferals migrating
from other areas.
Although they rarely say so outright, it is difficult to avoid the impression that conservationists and wildlife enthusiasts
would like to exterminate all feral cats. This would account for their vehement objection to Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR)
schemes, in which feral cats are, for welfare reasons, neutered and then returned to the site where they were originally
trapped. Although such schemes might, in theory, eventually lead to the disappearance of the feral cat population in the
locality in question due to reduced breeding, this has rarely actually been achieved. Undoctored cats migrate into the area
to which the neutered cats have been returned, and soon replace the original breeding capacity. Indeed, such schemes can
inadvertently generate “hotspots” for the abandonment of unwanted cats, their owners believing, possibly mistakenly, that
the cat will join the feral colony and be allowed to share its food. Even where the site is fairly well isolated and trapping
and neutering is maintained for years, the feral cats rarely disappear completely.
I studied one such colony that had formed around a partly derelict hospital in the south of England, originally built in the
nineteenth century as an insane asylum, and—as such places usually were—located several miles from the nearest village.
Prior to the introduction of TNR, the colony had consisted of several hundred cats and kittens; several years later,
numbers had reduced to about eighty cats. Many of these were members of the original colony, now neutered and
gradually becoming elderly, but at least one tomcat and several females had evaded capture and were continuing to breed.
In addition, pregnant females would appear periodically, presumably having been “dumped” there—although, being well
socialized, these were easy enough to trap and re-home via a humane charity. Overall, the colony stayed the same size,
residual breeding and immigration replacing those of the original group that were dying of old age, sustained by the food
provided by the dwindling band of remaining long-stay patients.
Supporters of TNR maintain that once a colony has been neutered and its food supply stabilized, its impact on wildlife
should reduce. However, little reliable evidence has been found to support this position. The cats presumably continue to
hunt: better nutrition may result in less consumption of prey items, but they, having caught the habit of hunting every day,
nevertheless continue to kill and harass wildlife. However, the neutered cats do at least occupy space that, had they been
euthanized rather than neutered and returned, would have soon become occupied by other cats. From the perspective of
assigning finite resources to wildlife conservation, it may be better for conservationists to allow cat enthusiasts to assist in
managing a cat population, if it is not causing catastrophic damage to wildlife. The much-touted alternative, the
extermination of feral cats on a regular basis, is likely to alienate those of their supporters who care equally for cats and
wildlife.
Neutered feral cats at the hospital
Since feral cats present such a slippery target where they coexist with free-roaming pets, conservationists tend to
concentrate their efforts on restricting owned cats—this, despite an almost complete lack of hard evidence that pet cats are
causing significant and lasting damage to wildlife. In an apparent attempt to fill the loophole caused by this lack of
evidence, scientists at the UK’s University of Sheffield have proposed a “fear effect” hypothesis: that pet cats suppress
breeding in bird populations, their very presence triggering fear responses in the birds that inhibit foraging and depress
fertility.23 However, this theory discounts that urban birds seem to have evolved strategies to overcome the impact of cats.
Furthermore, the mere presence of one lazy and ineffective predator must surely have less impact than the fear
engendered by rats, magpies, crows, and other “serious” predators of small birds and their young.
When targeting pet cats for criticism, bird enthusiasts often fail to mention that other predators may make much more of
an impact on bird numbers than cats do. Magpies, major predators of songbird nests, have tripled in number in the UK
since 1970 to between 1 and 2 million individuals—with most of the increase occurring in towns where, coincidentally,
cats have also increased. The Royal Society for the Protection of Birds—pledged to protect magpies, as well as their
prey—investigated this increase in case it was tied to reductions in songbird populations over the same period. They
concluded:
The study . . . found no evidence that increased numbers of magpies have caused declines in songbirds and confirms that populations of prey species are
not determined by the numbers of their predators. [Presumably, these include domestic cats, although they don’t say so specifically.] Availability of food
and suitable nesting sites are probably the main factors limiting songbird populations. . . . We discovered that the loss of food and habitats caused by
intensive farming have played a major role in songbird declines.24
Magpie killing a blackbird chick
Cats rarely catch magpies, but they may inadvertently assist smaller birds by suppressing the populations of some of their
other enemies. The UK has at least ten brown rats for every cat, and while rats are omnivores, their impact on bird and
small mammal populations around the world has been well documented. Moreover, since young brown rats are among
cats’ favorite prey items, if cats suppress rat populations in towns, they may indirectly be helping birds.25 Cat owners may
therefore be able to do more for wildlife by improving habitats for small birds (and mammals other than rats) in their
gardens than by confining their cats indoors (see box on page 251, “How Can We Prevent Cats from Hunting?”).
Such precautions may not be enough to silence the cat’s most vocal critics. Furthermore, most of today’s owners revile or
endure, rather than admire, their cat’s hunting prowess. Unlike their ancestors, pet cats no longer need to hunt to stay
healthy, so a reduction in their desire to hunt will do them no harm. Ideally, the cat of the future will have less inclination
to hunt than the cat of today.
CHAPTER 11
A peke-faced Persian
Other breeds face health concerns as a result of breeding: surprisingly, one, the Manx, has been exhibited in cat shows for
more than a century. The gene that gives the Manx cat its stumpy tail is essentially a defect, often lethal: a kitten that has
inherited two copies of this gene, one from its father and one from its mother, will likely die before birth. Cats with one
copy of the gene grow tails of varying lengths, and some with partial tails are prone to arthritis that may produce severe
pain. The gene can also affect the growth of not just the tail but also the back, damaging the spinal cord and causing a
form of spina bifida. Manx cats are also prone to bowel disease.
A different skeletal malformation characterizes the “squitten” or “twisty cat” (not a recognized breed), which has
incomplete development of the long bone in the front legs, resulting in the paw being twisted and attached at the shoulder,
a deformity that has been likened to the effects of Thalidomide on human infants. Such cats cannot walk, run or dig
properly, and cannot defend themselves; they do, however, sit upright in a “cute” manner, which presumably accounts for
their “twisted” appeal.
In other breeds, problems caused by breeding for appearance are less obvious. For example, the gene that gives the
Scottish Fold its characteristic lop-eared appearance also causes malformations of the cartilage elsewhere in the body, and
as a result many of these cats develop severely painful joints at a relatively early age.
The second type of problem arises as a side effect of so-called line breeding, which is effectively inbreeding. The quest
for the perfect specimen can result in the perpetuation of genes that disadvantage the cats that carry them—genes that
would be quickly selected out if they appeared in alley cats, because they must impede hunting. For example, many
Siamese cats have poor stereoscopic vision due to a lack of nerves in their brains to compare signals from the left and
right eyes. As a result, they may see double, or one eye may shut down completely, sometimes causing a squint to
develop. Another separate malformation in the retina leads to their vision blurring every time they move their heads. In
wishing to sustain the Siamese cat’s distinctive appearance, breeders inadvertently allowed this defect to continue from
generation to generation.
A reduced motivation to hunt and kill prey is just one of several factors that will enable cats to adapt better to twenty-
first-century living. Allowing a little anthropomorphism: if cats could write themselves a wish list for self-improvement, a
set of goals to allow them to adapt to the demands we place on them, it might look something like this:
•To get along better with other cats, so that social encounters are no longer a source of anxiety.
•To understand human behavior better, so that encounters with unfamiliar people no longer feel like a threat.
•To overcome the compulsion to hunt even on a full stomach.
The corresponding requests from owners:
•I’d like to have more than one cat at a time, and for my cats to be company—not just for me, but also for one another.
•I wish my cat didn’t disappear into the bedroom to urinate on the carpet every time I have visitors.
•I wish my cat didn’t bring gory “presents” through the cat-flap.
We currently know of two ways to achieve these goals. First, we can train individual cats to change how they interpret
and react to their surroundings. The advantage of this approach is that its effects would be immediate; the disadvantage is
that it must be repeated for each successive generation of cats. Second, because the cat’s genome is not yet fully
domesticated, there is still scope to genetically adapt their behavior and personalities to twenty-first-century lifestyles.
The benefits of selective breeding toward our goals will become apparent only after several decades, but these changes
would be permanent.
Cats are intelligent, and (up to a point) adaptable animals, so we can achieve some of these goals through directing cats’
learning—providing them with the right experiences to enable them to conform to the demands placed upon them. This
will almost certainly involve a certain amount of formal “training.” Although most dog owners know that they must train
their dogs to make them socially acceptable, such a thought scarcely crosses cat owners’ minds, or if it does, they reject it
as only appropriate for “performing cats.” Providing the right sort of experiences during the first few months of a kitten’s
life likely produces long-lasting effects, considering that this is the time when cats’ personalities are forged, but more
research is needed into precisely what those experiences should be.
The genetic basis for the cat’s behavior must have changed as it adapted to living alongside man, even though the details
are now lost in prehistory and ultimately impossible to trace. The average cat’s personality is likely still changing, as
some personality types fit modern conditions better than others. However, such haphazard change will not come about
quickly enough to keep up with the pace of change that we now demand of our pets, as our own lifestyles change, so more
direct intervention will be needed if the cat is to adapt at an acceptable pace.
We value our cats for their affectionate behavior, yet this trait has rarely been deliberately bred for—and then, only as an
afterthought. This trait must have been accidentally selected for in the past, as loving cats get the best food!4 Nevertheless,
“unfriendly” tendencies have persisted even among pet cats: in the 1980s experiments that defined the cat’s socialization
period, the experimenters noted “a small but constant percentage of the cats (about 15 percent) seem to have a
temperament that is resistant to socialization.”5 Even today, cats seem to possess a large range of variation in the genes
that underpin temperament, learning, and behavior, providing the raw material for work on selective breeding for behavior
and “personality,” rather than just appearance.
As cat owners, we have various resources available to help individual cats adapt to today’s crowded conditions, but
many people seem unaware of many of these. Our challenge is to use these tools early in the cat’s life, when it is still
learning about its surroundings. The second and third months of life are the crucial period, during which the growing
kitten learns how to interact socially—with other cats, with people, and with other household animals. As we have seen, it
is during this period of its life that the cat learns both how to identify its social partners, and how to behave toward them
in a way that will produce the desired outcome—a friendly tail-up, a lick behind the ear, a bowl of food, or a cuddle. More
generally, this is also the time when cats learn how to cope with the unexpected—whether to be curious, accepting the risk
of approaching and inspecting new things, or whether to play it safe and run away. Research shows that a cat’s capacity to
take risks can be subject to a strong genetic influence, but learning must also play a part.
A cat that has had only limited exposure to different kinds of people during its second and third months of life may
become timid, retreating to its safe place whenever people it regards as unfamiliar come to the house. Without exposure to
people before eight weeks of age a cat will become fearful of humans in general. However this is not the end of the
process of socialization, just the necessary beginning: kittens must have the opportunity to make their own connections
with different types of humans.
Many kittens homed at eight weeks may miss out on useful socialization experience with other kittens. The third month of
life is when play with other kittens peaks, and feral kittens maintain strong social links with their peer group until they are
about six months old. Veterinarians often (sensibly) advise that kittens should be kept indoors for the first few weeks after
homing, to prevent them from straying: however, if there are no other cats in the house, they may miss out on a crucial
phase in the development of their social skills.
Individual cats adopt different strategies when encountering the unfamiliar. Many withdraw, hide, or climb to a safe
vantage point. A minority may become aggressive, perhaps those that have not been given the opportunity to retreat on
previous occasions. Their owner may have run after them and scooped them up rather than allow them to withdraw. These
cats quickly learn that the stress of an undesired encounter can be prevented by scratching, hissing, and biting. Some of
these cats develop this tactic even further, preemptively striking out at people they don’t know or who have previously
been forced on them.
Cats also develop their own preferred tactics for dealing with other cats they do not know. When making their first social
encounters as kittens, some simply flee; others attempt to stand their ground, and often get a swipe or worse for their
efforts. Few attempt to engage in a friendly greeting, and even fewer find such a greeting reciprocated. Flight or fight thus
often becomes the young cat’s default response when meeting unfamiliar cats.
Owners who wish to add another cat to their household have an opportunity to manage the introduction to have a positive
outcome for all concerned. We cannot take for granted that cats will immediately like one another. The new cat will likely
feel stressed at being suddenly uprooted from its familiar surroundings and dropped into what it perceives as another cat’s
territory, and the resident cat will probably resent the intrusion. Therefore, it is usually best to start by keeping the new cat
in a part of the house that the resident cat rarely uses, allowing it to establish a small “territory” of its own and getting to
know its new owners before facing the challenge of meeting the resident cat face to face. The two cats will undoubtedly
be aware of each other’s presence, if only by smell, but this will be less stressful at this early stage than being able to see
each other. Owners can build up some degree of familiarity between the two cats before a meeting takes place by
periodically taking toys and bedding from each of the two cats and introducing them to the other while rewarding that cat
with food treats or a game. This builds up a positive emotional link with the other cat’s odor. The actual introduction
should wait until after both cats no longer show any adverse reaction to the other’s smell, and should be carried out in
stages, starting with allowing the cats to be together for just a few minutes.6
Because cats carry a reputation for being untrainable, most owners are unaware that training can reduce the stress that cats
can feel in situations where they would much rather run away. For example, owners can use clicker training (see chapter
6) to entice a cat to walk into its cat carrier, rather than forcing it in.7 Similar training could help cats overcome their
initial fear of other potentially stressful situations—for example, encounters with new people. In general, cats need
persuasion, not force, if they are to adopt a calm approach to new situations. If more owners understood the value of
training, a great deal of stress could be avoided—for the cats, certainly, but also for the owner, if the cat’s stress results in
deposits of urine or feces around the house.
Training can also help cats to adapt to indoor living. Training a cat is a one-on-one activity that is mentally stimulating for
the cat, and that also enhances the bond between cat and owner. It may also be useful in reducing some of the negatives of
keeping a cat indoors. Cats need to express their natural behavior, and many owners understandably object to the damage
to household furnishings that their cats unwittingly cause, for instance when sharpening their claws. In some countries,
veterinarians remove the cat’s claws surgically, but this may not be in the cat’s best interest, and in some countries this
intervention is illegal (see box on page 267, “Declawing”). A cat without claws may not only experience phantom pains
from its missing toes, but is also unable to defend itself if attacked by other cats. Training the cat to claw only in specific
places is a far more humane and straightforward alternative, especially if the cat has not yet developed a preference for
soft furnishings.8
Although untested so far, training might also be useful in reducing cats’ desire to hunt—or at least curtail their
effectiveness as predators of wildlife. We know that when cats appear to be playing with toys, they think they’re hunting,
but we have no information on whether playing in this way reduces—or, just conceivably, enhances—their desire to hunt
for real. If play does have an effect on this desire, how long does the effect last? Would a daily “hunting” game between
owner and cat save the lives of garden birds and mammals? Is it possible to train a cat to inhibit its pounce?
We also know little about how experience affects the hunting habit in general. Cats vary enormously in how keen they are
to go out hunting. That the basis for this is mainly genetic is unlikely, since only a few generations have elapsed since all
cats had to hunt to obtain the right kind of food. Anecdotally, one of the arguments for allowing a female house cat to
have one litter is that this is usually born in the spring, distracting the cat (provided its owners feed it well) from going out
hunting and thereby learning its trade. Is there a “sensitive period” for perfecting predatory skills, after which the desire to
hunt is unlikely to develop fully? Further study of this might not only save animals’ lives, but also a great deal of
aggravation between cat and wildlife enthusiasts.
Declawing
Cats instinctively scratch objects with their front claws. Perhaps they do this to leave behind an odor or a visible sign of
their presence, to alert other cats. They may also scratch because their claws are itchy: periodically, the whole of the
outside of the claw detaches, revealing a new, sharp claw within. If this is not shed, maybe because the cat is arthritic and
finds scratching painful, the whole claw may overgrow and cause the pad to become infected.
Some owners who object to scratch marks on their furniture seek to have the cat’s front claws (and occasionally, too, its
back claws) removed. Few veterinary procedures excite as much controversy as declawing (known technically as
onychectomy). This is regarded as routine in the United States and the Far East, but is illegal in many places, including
the European Union, Brazil, and Australia.
Declawing is a surgical procedure that involves amputation of the first joint of the cat’s toes. The initial pain resulting
from the procedure may be controlled with analgesics, but we do not know whether cats subsequently feel phantom pain
due to the nerves that have been severed. However, cats and humans have nearly identical mechanisms for feeling pain,
and four out of five people who have fingers amputated have phantom pain, so cats likely do as well. (I myself
experienced phantom pain for more than ten years after most of the nerves in one fingertip were severed in an accident. I
learned to ignore the pain because I knew it was meaningless—something cats are unlikely to be able to do.) Declawed
cats are more likely to urinate outside their litter boxes than other indoor cats, possibly because of the stress of this
phantom pain.
Claws are an essential defense mechanism for cats. While owners of indoor cats will argue that their cat never meets other
cats, and so should never need their claws, a declawed cat that is picked up roughly by a person may resort to biting,
unable to scratch to indicate its discomfort, and thereby cause a much more significant wound.9
T oday’s cats find themselves in a delicate situation. On one hand, they must adapt to meet our changing needs; on the
other, they have a reputation for being a pet that is easy to maintain. Persuading many cat owners to train their cats, to
spend that extra time and effort to change their pets’ behavior, may be difficult. As such, we must focus on a genetic shift
as well, taking the cat further down the road toward full domestication.
Ideally, cats that are predisposed to adapt to modern living conditions—to achieve the three goals outlined above—should
be identified and then prioritized for breeding. This will not be entirely straightforward, since cats’ personalities continue
to develop after the normal age of neutering, so before any such breeding program can begin, research will be needed to
separate the effects of the desired genes from those of the cat’s social environment. Moreover, there is probably no single
“perfect” set of genes that will enable cats to fit all the lifestyles that humankind will demand of them. The ideal indoor
cat will almost certainly be genetically distinct from the ideal outdoor cat, since, among other differences, the owner can
have far more influence on relationships with other cats if the cat is confined indoors.
We have three potential sources for such genes: house cats, pedigree cats, and hybrids. Conventional pedigree cats have
been produced almost exclusively for their looks, not their behavior, so they are unlikely to be a rich source of new
behavior traits.10 Pedigree cats are derived entirely from cats that have only ever had two functions: to hunt vermin and to
be good companions. In most breeds, there seems to have been little direct selection for behavior, only looks. There are
however a few interesting exceptions.
The Ragdoll is a semi-longhaired breed that was originally named for its extremely placid temperament. The first
examples to be exhibited, bred in the early 1990s, went limp when picked up, almost as if the scruffing reflex was
triggered by touch anywhere on the body, not just the back of the neck. It was once rumored that these cats were
insensitive to pain, and animal welfare organizations raised concerns that people might be tempted to toss these cats
around like cushions. The breed no longer shows such extreme behavior, but is still renowned for its easygoing
temperament. A similar breed derived from the same original stock, the RagaMuffin, is described thus: “The only extreme
allowed in this breed is its friendly, sociable and intelligent nature. These cats love people and are extremely
affectionate.”11 The genetic basis for these cats’ relaxed sociality, at least toward people, is unknown, but could
potentially be transferred to other cats by crossbreeding. Unfortunately for their welfare, ragdoll-type cats are reputedly
vulnerable to attack from neighborhood cats, perhaps because they are simply too trusting, and for this reason many
breeders advise prospective owners to keep them indoors.
Hybrids, crosses between Felis catus and other species, while initially produced mainly for their “exotic” appearance,
have brought new genetic material into the domain of the domestic cat. Their behavior is often quite distinctive, so
hybrids may potentially provide a source of genes that influence behavior that are not currently found in ordinary
domestic cats.
The most widespread of these hybrids, the Bengal cat, may not offer any solution to the adapting the domestic cat to the
twenty-first century, since its personality appears to have headed back toward that of its wild ancestors. The Bengal is a
hybrid between domestic cats and the Asian leopard cat Prionailurus bengalensis. The latter species is separated by more
than 6 million years of evolution from the domestic cat, and has never been domesticated in its own right; therefore, it
would seem an unlikely starting point for a new breed of cat, were it not for its attractive spotted coat (referred to as
“rosettes”). Domestic cats and Asian leopard cats will mate with one another if given no other option, but the resulting
offspring are essentially untamable. During the 1970s, repeated breeding between these hybrids and domestic cats
produced some offspring that retained the leopard cat’s spots on the back and flanks, creating the current Bengal “breed.”
Unfortunately, many Bengals possess not only wild-type coats, but also wild-type behavior, as this information from the
Bengal Cat Rescue website confirms:
This breed has a strong and sometimes dominant personality and although affectionate, lots are not simple lap cats. They can respond aggressively to
discipline and to being handled . . . Their commonest problems are aggression and spraying. . . . Also hardly a week goes by when someone doesn’t
contact me about having bought or adopted a pair that are trying to kill each other. . . . Bengals enjoy climbing and this includes your clothes and
curtains. They like exploring and are no respecter of ornaments or photographs. Often cat-aggressive, many will terrorise not just their own household
but can actively seek out neighbors’ cats and enter their homes to hurt them. They are not playing, they mean it.12
From the perspective of producing a docile pet, the Asian leopard cat was never going to be a good candidate for
hybridization. This species is one of the few small wild cats that is not threatened with extinction; nevertheless, many
zoos keep one or two specimens. These animals are, however, virtually untamable: zookeepers report that they are
impossible to approach, let alone touch.13
From the perspective of seeking genes that could be useful for changing the domestic cat, some of the smaller South
American cats could be better candidates for hybridization. In particular, Geoffroy’s cat, similar in size to the domestic
cat, and the slightly larger margay, are often friendly to their keepers when kept in zoos, and might therefore provide
genetic material useful to the continued evolution of the domestic cat. The South American cats lost one pair of
chromosomes soon after they diverged from the rest of the cat family some eight million years ago; this should mean that
the offspring of a domestic cat and a South American cat is sterile, but surprisingly, hybrids with Geoffroy’s cat can be
fertile. The resulting “breed,” usually known as the “Safari,” was first created in the 1970s, but is still rare: it is usually
still produced by mating the two original species, and the kittens, which grow into extra-large cats, fetch thousands of
dollars. Breeding between these first crosses and ordinary domestic cats, the method used to produce the Bengal, seems to
produce fertile offspring, but breeders apparently chose not to pursue this option. Hybrids with the margay, once referred
to as the “Bristol,” suffer from fertility problems and are apparently no longer bred. The margay is a tree-dwelling cat
with double-jointed ankles, enabling it to climb down tree trunks as easily as other cats climb upward, to hang one-footed
from branches like a monkey, and to leap twelve feet from one perch to another: such agility, if passed onto its hybrids,
might be appealing, but also excessive for most domestic purposes.
Bengal cat (above) and Safari cat (beneath)
Several other types of cat have been produced through hybridization with other felids, but all have been bred for their
“wild” looks, and none seem to offer more than curiosity value to the domestic cat’s genome. These include the Chausie,
a domestic cat crossed with the jungle cat Felis chaus, and the Savannah (serval Leptailurus serval), as well as many other
oddities of doubtful provenance. Some are classified as wild animals rather than as pets, much as wolf-dog hybrids are.14
These various hybrid breeds appear more of a side issue than a potential source of new genetic material to enrich the
genome of Felis catus. The most promising of the species in behavioral terms, the more docile of the South American
cats, are genetically incompatible with the domestic cat. The Old-World cats that are better matched genetically produce
hybrids that are wilder, not calmer, than today’s alley cats, so have little to offer.
Existing variation within Felis catus seems to be the best starting point for the completion of the cat’s domestication.
Plenty of modern cats combine an easygoing nature with a disinclination to hunt. Research has not yet indicated precisely
how much of this variation is underpinned by genetics, but a significant proportion must be. Our goals should be to
identify those individual cats with the best temperaments, and to ensure that their progeny are available to become
tomorrow’s pets.
One potential source of genetic variation has only recently emerged. Although house cats the world over are superficially
similar, their DNA reveals that they are genetically distinct—as different under the skin as, say, a Siamese and a Persian
are on the surface. Interbreeding between, for example, ordinary pet cats from China with their counterparts from the UK
or the United States might produce some novel temperaments, some of which might be better suited to indoor life, or be
more sociable, than any of today’s cats.
Alley cats from the Far East (left) and Western Europe (right)
Selection for the right temperament among house cats requires deliberate intervention; natural evolutionary processes,
which have served the cat well so far, will not be enough. One obstacle is the increasingly widespread practice of
neutering cats before they breed. With so many unwanted cats euthanized every year, arguing against the widespread use
of this procedure is difficult. Still, if taken too far, widespread neutering likely favors unfriendly cats over friendly.
Encouraging owners not to allow their cats to produce any offspring whatsoever removes all the genes that those cats
carry from being passed on to the next generation. Some of those genes have contributed to making those cats into valued
pets.
When almost every pet house cat has been neutered—a situation that already applies in some parts of the UK—then we
must fear for the next generations of cats. These will then mainly be the offspring of those that live on the fringes of
human society—feral males, stray females, as well as those female cats owned by people who either do not care whether
their cat is neutered or not, or have a moral objection to neutering. The qualities that allow most such cats to thrive and
produce offspring are, unfortunately, those same behaviors we want to eliminate: wariness of people and effectiveness at
self-sustaining hunting.
These cats will undoubtedly adapt their behavior to cope with whatever situations they find themselves in, but they are
also likely to be genetically slightly “wilder” than the average pet cat—therefore, distinctly different from the “ideal” pet
cat. Initially, the difference will probably be small, since some of the breeding cats will be strays that are genetically
similar to pet cats. But as the decades pass, as fewer and fewer reproductively intact cats are available to stray, most of the
kittens born each year will come from a long line of semi-wild cats—since these are the only cats that are able to breed
freely. Thus, the widespread adoption of early neutering by the most responsible cat owners risks pushing the domestic
cat’s genetics back gradually toward the wild, away from their current domesticated state.
A study that I conducted in 1999 suggests that such extrapolation cannot be dismissed as science fiction.15 In one area of
Southampton (UK), we found that more than 98 percent of pet cat population had been neutered. So few kittens were
being born that potential cat owners had to travel outside the city to obtain their cats. This situation had clearly existed for
some time: from talking to the owners of the older cats, we calculated that the cat population in that area had last been
self-sustaining some ten years previously, in the late 1980s. We located ten female pets in the area that were still being
allowed to breed and tested the temperament of their kittens after homing, when the kittens were six months old. Our
hypothesis was that feral males must have fathered many of these kittens, since so few intact males were being kept as
pets in the area, and all of these were young and unlikely to compete effectively with the more wily ferals. We found that
on average, the kittens in those ten litters were much less willing to settle on their owners’ laps than kittens born in
another area of the city that still had a significant number of undoctored pet tomcats. There was no systematic difference
in the way these two groups of kittens had been socialized, and the mother cats in the two areas were indistinguishable in
temperament. We therefore deduced that even if only one of the two parents comes from a long line of ferals, the kittens
will be less easy to socialize than if both parents are pets. The study was too small to draw any firm conclusions, but in the
years since it was carried out, blanket neutering has become more widespread, and so the cumulative effects of this on the
temperament of kittens should be becoming more obvious.
Neutering is an extremely powerful selection pressure, the effects of which have been given little consideration. At
present, it is the only humane way of ensuring that there are as few unwanted cats as possible, and it is unlikely ever to
become so widely adopted that the house cat population begins to shrink. However, over time it will likely have
unintended consequences. Consider a hypothetical situation. A century or more ago, when feline surgery was still crude,
society generally accepted that most cats would reproduce. Imagine that a highly contagious parasite had appeared that
sterilized cats of both sexes while they were still kittens, but otherwise left them unaffected, so that they lived as long as
an unaffected cat. Any cats that happened to be resistant to that parasite would be the only cats able to breed—so, within a
few years the parasite, deprived of susceptible hosts, would die out.
The only significant difference between such a hypothetical parasite and neutering is that the latter does not require a host
(a cat) to continue: it lives as an idea, and so is detached from its effects.16 Because neutering inevitably targets those cats
that are being best cared for, it must logically hand the reproductive advantage to those cats that are least attached to
people, many of which are genetically predisposed to remain unsocialized. We must consider the long-term effects of
neutering carefully: for example, it might be better for the cats of the future as a whole if neutering programs were
targeted more at ferals, which are both the unfriendliest cats and also those most likely to damage wildlife populations.
We need a fresh approach to cat breeding. Pedigree cats are bred largely for looks, not with the primary goal of ensuring
an optimal temperament—although an adequate temperament is, of course, taken into account in the majority of breeds.
Random-bred cats are under siege from neutering; even if this widespread practice is not making each successive
generation a little wilder than the previous, it is highly unlikely to be having the opposite effect. So while cats’ looks and
welfare both have their champions, the cat’s future has none.
Then again, why should it? Cats have always outnumbered potential owners. Why should that situation change? Cats have
become more popular, not less, so there should be more homes available for them, not fewer, than there were a few
decades ago. Apart from the (significant) minority of cat-haters, the general public is more tolerant of cats than of dogs.
We cannot guarantee, however, that these apparent givens will continue.
Recent decades have witnessed immense changes to the way dogs are kept, especially in urban areas, with a proliferation
of “clean up after your dog” regulations, no-dog parks, and legislation aimed at protecting the public from dog bites. We
expect dogs to behave in a much more controlled and civilized way than they did half a century ago. Are similar
restrictions on cat-keeping just around the corner? Will gardeners and wildlife enthusiasts unite to produce legislation that
restricts cats to their owners’ property? If such pressures do appear, they will be easier to head off if cat-lovers are already
taking steps toward producing a more socially acceptable cat. At the same time, the cats themselves will benefit if they
find it easier to cope with the vagaries of their pets’ social lives.
Ultimately, the future of cats lies in the hands of those who breed them—not those whose eye is primarily on success in
the show ring, but those who can be persuaded that an improved temperament, not good looks, should be the goal. The
genetic material is available, although more science is needed to devise the temperament tests that will locate which
individual cats carry it; many cats that appear well-adapted to life with people will have received an optimal upbringing,
rather than being anything special genetically. The relevant genes are probably scattered all over the globe, so ideally we
need collaboration between cat enthusiasts in different parts of the world.
Such human-friendly cats, however cute, are unlikely ever to command much of a price. Expectations that non-pedigree
kittens should be free, or nearly so, will take a long time to die out. Commercial breeding of non-pedigree cats may never
be viable. Well-adjusted kittens need a wealth of early experience that even some pedigree breeders struggle to provide.
Providing this level of care is cost-effective only if kittens are bred in people’s homes, the very type of environment into
which they will move when they become pets.
Meanwhile, the way that cats are socialized has much room for improvement. Both breeders and owners can play a part in
this, since kittens adapt to their surroundings throughout their second and third months of life. In this context, the
continuing policy of some cat breeders’ associations to prohibit homing until a kitten is twelve weeks of age demands
careful scrutiny: it may provide extra socialization with littermates, but often at the cost of learning about different kinds
of people, and the development of a robust strategy for dealing with the unfamiliar. For adult cats, training, both in the
general sense of providing the right learning experiences as well as teaching them how to behave calmly in specific
situations, could improve each cat’s lot considerably, if only its value was more widely appreciated.
Finally, we must continue to research why some cats are strongly motivated to hunt, while the majority are content to
doze in their beds. Science has not yet revealed to what extent such differences are due to early experience, and how much
to genetics, but ultimately it should be possible to breed cats that are unlikely to feel the need to become predators, now
that we can easily provide them with all the nutrition they need.
Cats need our understanding—both as individual animals that need our help to adjust to our ever-increasing demands, and
also as a species that is still in transition between the wild and the truly domestic. If we can agree to support them in both
these ways, cats will be assured a future in which they are not only popular and populous, but are also more relaxed, and
affectionate, than they are today.
Further Reading
M ost of my source material for this book has consisted of papers in academic journals, which are often difficult to
access for those without a university affiliation. I’ve included references to the most important of these in the notes, with
Web links if they appear to be in the public domain. For those readers who wish to take their study of cats further without
first requiring a degree in biology, I can recommend the following books, most of them written by knowledgeable
academics but with a more general audience in mind.
The Domestic Cat: the Biology of Its Behaviour, edited by Professors Dennis Turner and Patrick Bateson, is now available
in three editions; all are published by Cambridge University Press, the most recent in 2013. These books consist of
chapters written by experts in different aspects of cat behavior.
My own The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, 2nd edition (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012), coauthored by
Drs. Sarah L. Brown and Rachel Casey, provides an integrated introduction to the science of cat behavior, aimed at an
advanced undergraduate audience. Feline Behavior: A Guide for Veterinarians by Bonnie V. Beaver (St. Louis, MO:
Saunders, 2003) is, as its title indicates, aimed at veterinary surgeons and veterinary students.
For the various stages in the history of the cat’s life with humankind, Jaromir Malek’s The Cat in Ancient Egypt (London:
British Museum Press, 2006), Donald Engel’s Classical Cats (London: Routledge, 1999), and Carl Van Vechten’s The
Tiger in the House (New York: New York Review of Books, 2006) provide specialist accounts.
Carrots and Sticks: Principles of Animal Training (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2008) by Professors Paul
McGreevy and Bob Boakes from the University of Sydney, Australia, is a fascinating book of two halves: the first
explains learning theory in accessible language, and the second contains fifty case histories of animals (including cats)
trained for specific purposes, ranging from film work to bomb detection, each illustrated with color photographs of the
animals and how they were trained.
For cat owners seeking guidance on a problem cat, there is often no substitute for a one-to-one consultation with a
genuine expert, but these can be hard to find. The advice given in books by Sarah Heath, Vicky Halls, or Pam Johnson-
Bennett may be helpful. Celia Haddon’s books may also provide some light relief.
Notes
All Web addresses mentioned in the Notes are active as of April 2013.
Introduction
1. This ratio takes many millions of unowned animals into account, and also incorporates a guess as to the numbers in Muslim countries where dogs are
rare.
2. The Prophet Muhammad is said to have loved his cat Muezza so much that “he would do without his cloak rather than disturb one that was sleeping
on it.” Minou Reeves, Muhammad in Europe (New York: NYU Press, 2000), 52.
3. Rose M. Perrine and Hannah L. Osbourne, “Personality Characteristics of Dog and Cat Persons.” Anthrozoös: A Multidisciplinary Journal of the
Interactions of People & Animals 11 (1998): 33–40.
4. A recognized medical condition, referred to as “ailurophobia.”
5. David A. Jessup, “The Welfare of Feral Cats and Wildlife.” Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 225 (2004): 1377–83; available
online at www.avma.org/News/Journals/Collections/Documents/javma_225_9_1377.pdf.
6. The People’s Dispensary for Sick Animals, “The State of Our Pet Nation . . . : The PDSA Animal Wellbeing (PAW) Report 2011.” Shropshire, UK:
2011; available online at tinyurl.com/b4jgzjk. Dogs scored a little better for social and physical environments (71 percent) but worse for behavior (55
percent).
7. The situation for pedigree dogs in the UK has been summarized in several expert reports, including those commissioned by the Royal Society for the
Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (www.rspca.org.uk/allaboutanimals/pets/dogs/health/pedigreedogs/report), the Associate Parliamentary Group for
Animal Welfare (www.apgaw.org/images/stories/PDFs/Dog-Breeding-Report-2012.pdf), and the UK Kennel Club in partnership with the re-homing
charity DogsTrust (breedinginquiry.files.wordpress.com/2010/01/final-dog-inquiry-120110.pdf).
Chapter 1
1. Darcy F. Morey, Dogs: Domestication and the Development of a Social Bond (New York: Cambridge University Press, 2010).
2. Quoted in C. A. W. Guggisberg, Wild Cats of the World (New York: Taplinger, 1975), 33–34.
3. These cats are now extinct on Cyprus, displaced by the red fox, another introduction, which is now the only land-based carnivorous mammal on the
island.
4. For a more detailed account of these migrations, see Stephen O’Brien and Warren Johnson’s “The Evolution of Cats,” Scientific American 297
(2007): 68–75.
5. The spelling lybica should more correctly be libyca, “from Libya,” but most modern accounts use the original (incorrect) version.
6. These “lake dwellers” built villages on sites that now lie beneath the margins of lakes, but were probably fertile dry land at the time.
7. Frances Pitt (see note 9 below) claimed that the Scottish wildcat would have joined its English and Welsh counterparts in extinction, had it not been
for the call-up of the younger gamekeepers to fight in the Great War.
8. Carlos A. Driscoll, Juliet Clutton-Brock, Andrew C. Kitchener, and Stephen J. O’Brien, “The Taming of the Cat,” Scientific American 300 (2009):
68–75; available online at tinyurl.com/akxyn9c.
9. From The Romance of Nature: Wild Life of the British Isles in Picture and Story, vol. 2, ed. Frances Pitt (London: Country Life Press, 1936). Pitt
(1888–1964) was a pioneering wildlife photographer who lived near Bridgnorth in Shropshire.
10. Mike Tomkies, My Wilderness Wildcats (London: Macdonald and Jane’s, 1977).
11. This and the following two quotations are from Reay H. N. Smithers’s “Cat of the Pharaohs: The African Wild Cat from Past to Present,” Animal
Kingdom 61 (1968): 16–23.
12. Charlotte Cameron-Beaumont, Sarah E. Lowe, and John W. S. Bradshaw, “Evidence Suggesting Preadaptation to Domestication throughout the
Small Felidae,” Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 75 (2002): 361–66; available online at www.neiu.edu/~jkasmer/Biol498R/Readings/essay1-
06.pdf. In this paper, which came before Carlos Driscoll’s DNA study making cafra a separate subspecies, the Southern African cats are listed as Felis
silvestris libyca.
13. Carlos Driscoll et al., “The Near Eastern Origin of Cat Domestication,” Science (Washington) 317 (2007): 519–23; available online at
www.mobot.org/plantscience/resbot/Repr/Add/DomesticCat_Driscoll2007.pdf. The data discussed can be found in the online Supplemental Information,
Figure S1.
14. David Macdonald, Orin Courtenay, Scott Forbes, and Paul Honess, “African Wildcats in Saudi Arabia” in The Wild CRU Review: The Tenth
Anniversary Report of the Wildlife Conservation Research Unit at Oxford University, ed. David Macdonald and Françoise Tattersall (Oxford: University
of Oxford Department of Zoology, 1996), 42.
15. The estimate of fifteen to twenty comes from Carlos Driscoll of the Laboratory of Genomic Diversity at the National Cancer Institute in Frederick,
Maryland, who is currently working to pinpoint where these genes lie on the cat’s chromosomes, and how they may work.
16. See note 13 above.
17. O. Bar-Yosef, “Pleistocene Connexions between Africa and Southwest Asia: An Archaeological Perspective,” The African Archaeological Review 5
(1987), 29–38.
18. Carlos Driscoll and his colleagues have discovered five distinct types of mitochondrial DNA in today’s domestic cats; mitochondrial DNA is
inherited only through the maternal line. The common maternal ancestor of these five cats lived about 130,000 years ago; over the next 120,000 years,
her descendants gradually moved around the Middle East and North Africa, their mitochondrial DNA mutating slightly from time, before a few of them
happened to become the ancestors of today’s pet cats.
Chapter 2
1. J.-D. Vigne, J. Guilane, K. Debue, L. Haye, and P. Gérard, “Early Taming of the Cat in Cyprus,” Science 304 (2004): 259.
2. James Serpell, In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-Animal Relationships, Canto ed. (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1996);
Stefan Seitz, “Game, Pets and Animal Husbandry among Penan and Punan Groups” in Beyond the Green Myth: Borneo’s Hunter-Gatherers in the
Twenty-first Century, ed. Peter G. Sercombe and Bernard Sellato (Copenhagen: NIAS Press, 2007).
3. Veerle Linseele, Wim Van Neer, and Stan Hendrickx, “Evidence for Early Cat Taming in Egypt,” Journal of Archaeological Science 34 (2007):
2081–90 and 35 (2008): 2672–73; available online at tinyurl.com/aotk2e8.
4. Jaromír Málek, The Cat in Ancient Egypt (London, British Museum Press, 2006).
5. This stone coffin is now in the Museum of Egyptian Antiquities in Cairo. On its side, alongside pictures of Ta-Miaut herself and the deities Nephthys
and Isis, are several inscriptions. Words spoken by Osiris: “Ta-Miaut is not tired, nor weary is the body of Ta-Miaut, justified before the Great God.”
Words spoken by Isis: “I embrace you in my arms, Osiris.” Words spoken by Nephthys: “I envelop my brother, Osiris Ta-Miaut, the Triumphant.” See
www.mafdet.org/tA-miaut.html.
6. At the same time, cats may have been implicated in the first outbreaks of bubonic plague. Although this disease was later spread into Europe by black
rats, its natural host is apparently the Nile rat. Although the disease is usually transmitted from the Nile rat to humans via the rat flea, cat fleas are also
sometimes responsible. See Eva Panagiotakopulu, “Pharaonic Egypt and the Origins of Plague,” Journal of Biogeography 31 (2004): 269–75; available
online at tinyurl.com/ba52zuv.
7. Both genets and mongooses are occasionally kept as domestic pets, but these are genetically unaltered from their wild ancestors, not domesticated
animals, and consequently difficult to keep.
8. From The Historical Library of Diodorus the Sicilian, Vol. 1, Chap. VI, trans. G. Booth (London: Military Chronicle Office, 1814), 87.
9. Frank J. Yurko, “The Cat and Ancient Egypt,” Field Museum of Natural History Bulletin 61 (March–April 1990): 15–23.
10. Mongooses have been introduced to many parts of the world in an attempt to control snakes, especially islands such as Hawaii and Fiji, which lack
other predators of snakes.
11. Angela von den Driesch and Joachim Boessneck, “A Roman Cat Skeleton from Quseir on the Red Sea Coast,” Journal of Archaeological Science 10
(1983): 205–11.
12. Herodotus, The Histories (Euterpe) 2:60, trans. G. C. Macaulay (London & New York: MacMillan & Co., 1890).
13. Herodotus, Histories, 2:66.
14. From The Historical Library of Diodorus the Sicilian, Vol. 1, Chap. VI, 84, trans. G. Booth (London: Military Chronicle Office, 1814), 84.
15. Herodotus, Histories (Euterpe), 2:66.
16. Elizabeth Marshall Thomas, The Tribe of Tiger: Cats and Their Culture (New York: Simon & Schuster, 1994), 100–01.
17. Paul Armitage and Juliet Clutton-Brock, “A Radiological and Histological Investigation into the Mummification of Cats from Ancient Egypt,”
Journal of Archaeological Science 8 (1981): 185–96.
18. Stephen Buckley, Katherine Clark, and Richard Evershed, “Complex Organic Chemical Balms of Pharaonic Animal Mummies,” Nature 431 (2004):
294–99.
19. Armitage and Clutton-Brock.
20. The “black panther,” a melanistic form of the leopard, is common in the rainforests of southern Asia. Presumably so little light penetrates to the
forest floor that camouflage is not as much of an issue as for a normally spotted leopard hunting in the African bush.
21. Neil B. Todd, who collected this data, instead suggests that the orange mutation first arose in Asia Minor (roughly, modern Turkey), even though it
is less common there than in Alexandria. “Cats and Commerce,” Scientific American 237 (1977): 100–07.
22. Dominique Pontier, Nathalie Rioux, and Annie Heizmann, “Evidence of Selection on the Orange Allele in the Domestic Cat Felis catus: The Role of
Social Structure.” Oikos 73 (1995): 299–308.
23. Terence Morrison-Scott, “The Mummified Cats of Ancient Egypt,” Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 121 (1952): 861–67.
24. See chapter 16 of Frederick Everard Zeuner’s A History of Domesticated Animals (New York: Harper & Row, 1963).
25. This rapid acceptance of cats contrasts with the Japanese refusal to admit dogs for thousands of years after they had become widely adopted in
China.
26. Monika Lipinski et al., “The Ascent of Cat Breeds: Genetic Evaluations of Breeds and Worldwide Random-bred Populations,” Genomics 91 (2008):
12–21; available online at tinyurl.com/cdop2op.
27. Cleia Detry, Nuno Bicho, Hermenegildo Fernandes, and Carlos Fernandes, “The Emirate of Córdoba (756–929 AD) and the Introduction of the
Egyptian Mongoose (Herpestes ichneumon) in Iberia: The Remains from Muge, Portugal,” Journal of Archaeological Science 38 (2011): 3518–23. The
related Indian mongoose has been introduced into many parts of the world in an attempt to control snakes, especially islands such as Hawaii and Fiji,
which lack other snake predators.
28. Lyudmila N. Trut, “Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment,” American Scientist 87 (1999): 160–69; available online at
www.terrierman.com/russianfoxfarmstudy.pdf.
Chapter 3
1. Perhaps fortunately for the domestic cat’s popularity, the brown rat Rattus norvegicus, much larger and formidable than the black rat, did not spread
across Europe until the late Middle Ages. As it advanced, it gradually displaced the plague-carrying black rat from the towns and cities; now, black rats
are generally found only in warmer places. Most cats are not powerful or skillful enough to take on a full-grown brown rat, although they can be an
effective deterrent to brown rat colonization or recolonization. See Charles Elton, “The Use of Cats in Farm Rat Control,” British Journal of Animal
Behaviour 1 (1953), 151–55.
2. Researchers have found numerous examples of this in Britain, France, and Spain, so this superstition must have been widespread. A mummified
cat/rat pair were even discovered in Dublin’s Christ Church Cathedral, although the official story holds that they were trapped there by accident.
3. Translation by Eavan Boland; see homepages.wmich.edu/~cooneys/poems/pangur.ban.html. Bán means “white” in Old Irish, so presumably that was
the color of the writer’s cat.
4. Ronald L. Ecker and Eugene J. Crook, Geoffrey Chaucer: The Canterbury Tales—A Complete Translation into Modern English (Online Edition;
Palatka, FL: Hodge & Braddock, 1993); english.fsu.edu/canterbury.
5. Tom P. O’Connor, “Wild or Domestic? Biometric Variation in the Cat Felis silvestris Schreber,” International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 17
(2007): 581–95; available online at eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/3700/1/OConnor_Cats-IJOA-submitted.pdf.
6. At this time, cats were also considered generally detrimental to good health. The French physician Ambroise Paré described the cat as “a venomous
animal which infects through its hair, its breath and its brains,” and in 1607 the English cleric Edward Topsell wrote, “It is most certain that the breath
and savour of cats . . . destroy the lungs.”
7. Carl Van Vechten, The Tiger in the House, 3rd ed. (New York: Dorset Press, 1952), 100.
8. J. S. Barr: Buffon’s Natural History, Vol. VI, translated from the French (1797), 1.
9. Neil Todd, “Cats and Commerce,” Scientific American 237 (May 1977): 100–07.
10. Ibid.
11. Manuel Ruiz-García and Diana Alvarez, “A Biogeographical Population Genetics Perspective of the Colonization of Cats in Latin America and
Temporal Genetic Changes in Brazilian Cat Populations,” Genetics and Molecular Biology 31 (2008): 772–82.
12. Even black cats carry the genes for one or other of the “tabby” patterns, but because the hairs that should be brown at the tips are black, the pattern
doesn’t show—at least, not until the cat gets old, when the hairs that would be brown if they weren’t black fade to a dark, rusty color. A tabby pattern
can also be just discernible in black kittens for a few weeks. Another variation of the tabby gene, “Abyssinian,” restricts the black stripes to the head,
tail, and legs, while the body is covered in brown-tipped hairs; this is quite rare except in the pedigree cats of the same name.
13. See Todd, footnote 9.
14. Bennett Blumenberg, “Historical Population Genetics of Felis catus in Humboldt County, California,” Genetica 68 (1986): 81–86.
15. Andrew T. Lloyd, “Pussy Cat, Pussy Cat, Where Have You Been?” Natural History 95 (1986): 46–53.
16. Ruiz-García and Alvarez, note 11.
17. Manuel Ruiz-García, “Is There Really Natural Selection Affecting the L Frequencies (Long Hair) in the Brazilian Cat Populations?” Journal of
Heredity 91 (2000): 49–57.
18. Juliet Clutton-Brock, formerly of the British Museum of Natural History in London, pointed this out in her 1987 book A Natural History of
Domesticated Mammals (New York: Cambridge University Press). Indian elephants, camels, and reindeer are among other domestic animals that exist,
like the domestic cat, somewhere between wildness and full domestication.
19. For more detail on cat nutrition and how it interacts with their lifestyles, see Debra L. Zoran and C. A. T. Buffington, “Effects of Nutrition Choices
and Lifestyle Changes on the Well-being of Cats, a Carnivore that Has Moved Indoors,” Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 239
(2011): 596–606.
20. The idea of “nutritional wisdom” comes from Chicago-based pediatrician Clara Marie Davis’s classic 1933 experiment, which showed that human
infants, allowed to choose from thirty-three “natural” foodstuffs, would choose a balanced diet, even though each infant preferred a different
combination of foods.
21. Stuart C. Church, John A. Allen and John W. S. Bradshaw, “Frequency-Dependent Food Selection by Domestic Cats: A Comparative Study,”
Ethology 102 (1996): 495–509.
Chapter 4
1. I suspect—and this is only conjecture—that it is no coincidence that both cat and dog are members of the Carnivora.
2. Eileen Karsh, The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behavior, 2nd ed., Dennis C. Turner and Patrick Bateson (New York: Cambridge University
Press, 1988), 164. Professor Karsh and her team carried out their study at Temple University in Philadelphia. Remarkably, this revolutionary work has
never been published in peer-reviewed journals; however, no one since has fundamentally disagreed with its conclusions.
3. M. E. Pozza, J. L. Stella, A.-C. Chappuis-Gagnon, S. O. Wagner, and C. A. T. Buffington, “Pinch-induced Behavioral Inhibition (‘Clipnosis’) in
Domestic Cats,” Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 10 (2008): 82–87.
4. John M. Deag, Aubrey Manning, and Candace E. Lawrence, “Factors Influencing the Mother-Kitten Relationship,” in The Domestic Cat, 23–39.
5. Jay S. Rosenblatt, “Suckling and Home Orientation in the Kitten: A Comparative Developmental Study,” in The Biopsychology of Development, ed.
Ethel Tobach, Lester R. Aronson, and Evelyn Shaw (New York: Academic Press, 1971), 345–410.
6. R. Hudson, G. Raihani, D. González, A. Bautista, and H. Distel, “Nipple Preference and Contests in Suckling Kittens of the Domestic Cat Are
Unrelated to Presumed Nipple Quality,” Developmental Psychobiology 51 (2009): 322–32.
7. St. Francis Animal Welfare in Fair Oak, Hampshire, UK.
8. Female cats sometimes mate with several males in succession, such that the members of a litter may be half-siblings. See the chapter by Olof Liberg,
Mikael Sandell, Dominique Pontier, and Eugenia Natoli in The Domestic Cat, 119–147.
9. Hand-reared kittens often end up spending their entire lives with the person that hand-reared them. Whether this is because the kittens are difficult to
home or whether their human foster parents cannot bear to give them away seems to be unknown.
10. John Bradshaw and Suzanne L. Hall, “Affiliative Behaviour of Related and Unrelated Pairs of Cats in Catteries: A Preliminary Report,” Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 63 (1999): 251–55.
11. Roberta R. Collard, “Fear of Strangers and Play Behavior in Kittens with Varied Social Experience,” Child Development 38 (1967): 877–91.
12. Karsh, The Domestic Cat, note 2.
13. We measured the closeness of the relationship by asking the owners how likely they would be to turn to their cat for emotional support in each of
nine situations—for example, after a bad day at work, or when they were feeling lonely. See Rachel A. Casey and John Bradshaw, “The Effects of
Additional Socialisation for Kittens in a Rescue Centre on Their Behaviour and Suitability as a Pet,” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114 (2008):
196–205.
14. Practical steps for minimizing the stress of being moved to a new house—for both kittens and cats—can be found on the Cats Protection website:
www.cats.org.uk/uploads/documents/cat_care_leaflets/EG02-Welcomehome.pdf.
15. Although purring is not always a reliable indicator of how friendly a kitten is, it probably was in this instance.
16. Perhaps surprisingly, the “owners” of these half-wild cats didn’t seem to mind—some people seem to value cats for their wildness, and may even
deliberately choose a cat with a personality to match.
Chapter 5
1. Birds, much more visual creatures than cats, see four colors, including ultraviolet, invisible to mammals of all kinds.
2. At least we know that people who are red-green color-blind see colors this way. A very small number of people also have one normal eye and one
red-green color-blind eye, and can develop a normal vocabulary for color using their “good” eye, and then using that vocabulary to report what they see
with only their color-blind eye open.
3. You can try this by placing a finger on the page of this book, and then moving the finger a little way toward your nose while continuing to focus on
the print. We can choose to fix our gaze on either the print or the finger, but if we had a cat’s eyes, at this distance, we would be unable to fix on the
finger.
4. David McVea and Keir Pearson, “Stepping of the Forelegs over Obstacles Establishes Long-lasting Memories in Cats,” Current Biology 17 (2007):
R621–23.
5. See also the animation at en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cat_righting_reflex.
6. Nelika K. Hughes, Catherine J. Price, and Peter B. Banks, “Predators Are Attracted to the Olfactory Signals of Prey,” PLoS One 5 (2010): e13114,
doi: 10.1371.
7. A vestigial Jacobson’s organ can be detected in the human fetus, but it never develops functional nerve connections.
8. Ignacio Salazar, Pablo Sanchez Quinteiro, Jose Manuel Cifuentes, and Tomas Garcia Caballero, “The Vomeronasal Organ of the Cat,” Journal of
Anatomy 188 (1996): 445–54.
9. Whereas mammals seem to use their VNOs exclusively for social and especially sexual functions, reptiles use them more diversely. Snakes use their
forked tongues, which do not have taste buds, to deliver different samples of odorants to their left and right VNOs, useful when they are tracking prey or
a snake of the opposite sex.
10. See Patrick Pageat and Emmanuel Gaultier, “Current Research in Canine and Feline Pheromones,” The Veterinary Clinics: North American Small
Animal Practice 33 (2003): 187–211.
Chapter 6
1. However, science has recently revealed that some of our emotions never surface into consciousness but nevertheless affect the way we behave; for
example, images and emotions that we never become aware of affect the way we drive our cars. See Ben Lewis-Evans, Dick de Waard, Jacob Jolij, and
Karel A. Brookhuis, “What You May Not See Might Slow You Down Anyway: Masked Images and Driving,” PLoS One 7 (2012): e29857, doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0029857.
2. Leonard Trelawny Hobhouse, Mind in Evolution, 2nd ed. (London: Macmillan & Co., 1915).
3. For details of the experiments, see M. Bravo, R. Blake, and S. Morrison, “Cats See Subjective Contours,” Vision Research 18 (1988): 861–65; F.
Wilkinson, “Visual Texture Segmentation in Cats,” Behavioural Brain Research 19 (1986): 71–82.
4. Further details of the discriminatory abilities of cats can be found in John W. S. Bradshaw, Rachel A. Casey, and Sarah L. Brown, The Behaviour of
the Domestic Cat, 2nd ed. (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012), chap. 3.
5. Sarah L. Hall, John W. S. Bradshaw, and Ian Robinson, “Object Play in Adult Domestic Cats: The Roles of Habituation and Disinhibition,” Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 79 (2002): 263–71. Compared to “classic” habituation as studied in laboratory rats, the timescale over which cats remain
habituated to toys is very long—minutes, rather than seconds. We subsequently found that the same applies to dogs.
6. Sarah L. Hall and John W. S. Bradshaw, “The Influence of Hunger on Object Play by Adult Domestic Cats,” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58
(1998): 143–50.
7. Commercially available toys don’t come apart for a good reason: occasionally a cat can choke on a piece of toy, or get fragments lodged in its gut.
8. Although cats will go out hunting whether they’re hungry or not, they’re more likely to make a kill if they’re hungry; see chapter 10.
9. For a (very) alternative view, look online for comedian Eddie Izzard’s “Pavlov’s Cat,” currently at tinyurl.com/dce6lb.
10. Psychologists generally classify pain as a feeling rather than an emotion, but there is no doubt that both feelings and emotions are equally involved in
how animals learn about the world.
11. Endre Grastyán and Lajos Vereczkei, “Effects of Spatial Separation of the Conditioned Signal from the Reinforcement: A Demonstration of the
Conditioned Character of the Orienting Response or the Orientational Character of Conditioning,” Behavioral Biology 10 (1974): 121–46.
12. Ádam Miklósi, Péter Pongrácz, Gabriella Lakatos, József Topál, and Vilmos Csányi, “A Comparative Study of the Use of Visual Communicative
Signals in Interactions Between Dogs (Canis familiaris) and Humans and Cats (Felis catus) and Humans,” Journal of Comparative Psychology 119
(2005): 179–86; available online at www.mtapi.hu/userdirs/26/Publikaciok_Topal/Miklosietal2005JCP.pdf.
13. Nicholas Nicastro and Michael J. Owren, “Classification of Domestic Cat (Felis catus) Vocalizations by Naive and Experienced Human Listeners,”
Journal of Comparative Psychology 117 (2003): 44–52.
14. Edward L. Thorndike, Animal Intelligence: An Experimental Study of the Associative Processes in Animals, Chapter 2 (New York: Columbia
University Press, 1898); available online at tinyurl.com/c4bl6do.
15. Emma Whitt, Marie Douglas, Britta Osthaus, and Ian Hocking, “Domestic Cats (Felis catus) Do Not Show Causal Understanding in a String-pulling
Task,” Animal Cognition 12 (2009): 739–43. The same scientists had earlier shown that the crossed-strings arrangement defeats most dogs, even those
which, unlike the cats, had earlier successfully solved the parallel-strings problem—thus, dogs’ understanding of physics seems to be better than cats’.
16. Claude Dumas, “Object Permanence in Cats (Felis catus): An Ecological Approach to the Study of Invisible Displacements,” Journal of
Comparative Psychology 106 (1992): 404–10; Dumas, “Flexible Search Behavior in Domestic Cats (Felis catus): A Case Study of Predator-Prey
Interaction,” Journal of Comparative Psychology 114 (2000): 232–38.
17. For other works by this cartoonist, visit www.stevenappleby.com.
18. George S. Romanes, Animal Intelligence (New York: D. Appleton & Co., 1886); available online at www.gutenberg.org/files/40459/40459-h/40459-
h.htm.
19. C. Lloyd Morgan, An Introduction to Comparative Psychology (New York: Scribner, 1896); available online at tinyurl.com/crehpj9.
20. Paul H. Morris, Christine Doe, and Emma Godsell, “Secondary Emotions in Non-primate Species? Behavioural Reports and Subjective Claims by
Animal Owners,” Cognition and Emotion 22 (2008): 3–20.
21. More detail of the causes of such problematic behavior can be found in chapters 11 and 12 of my book, The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, 2nd ed.,
coauthored with Rachel Casey and Sarah Brown (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012).
22. Anne Seawright et al., “A Case of Recurrent Feline Idiopathic Cystitis: The Control of Clinical Signs with Behavior Therapy,” Journal of Veterinary
Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 3 (2008): 32–38. For background information on feline cystitis, see the Feline Advisory Bureau’s website,
www.fabcats.org/owners/flutd/info.html.
23. Alexandra Horowitz, a professor of cognitive psychology at New York’s Barnard College, performed this study. See her paper “Disambiguating the
‘Guilty Look’: Salient Prompts to a Familiar Dog Behaviour” in Behavioural Processes 81 (2009): 447–52, and her book Inside of a Dog: What Dogs
See, Smell, and Know (New York: Scribner, 2009).
Chapter 7
1. “The Curious Cat,” filmed for the BBC’s The World About Us series (1979). A delightful account of the making of this film is included in its
companion volume of the same title, written by Michael Allaby and Peter Crawford (London: M. Joseph, 1982). Similar studies were being conducted at
roughly the same time by Jane Dards in Portsmouth dockyard (UK), Olof Liberg in Sweden, and Masako Izawa in Japan.
2. Strictly speaking, the term “gene” refers to a single location on a particular chromosome, and the competing versions of the same gene are alleles; one
example already discussed is the blotched and striped alleles of the “tabby” gene.
3. This is likely true even though we know little about how such genes might work, since almost all cooperation between animals occurs between
members of the same family. Genes code for proteins, and it is difficult to imagine a protein that could promote family loyalty as such. Rather, many
genes must be involved, each contributing a small piece to the whole: for example, one might reduce the threshold for aggression toward other cats in
general, while another enables the recognition of odors characteristic of family members, through changes in the accessory olfactory bulb, the part of the
brain that processes information coming from the vomeronasal organ.
4. Christopher N. Johnson, Joanne L. Isaac, and Diana O. Fisher, “Rarity of a Top Predator Triggers Continent-wide Collapse of Mammal Prey: Dingoes
and Marsupials in Australia,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B 274 (2007): 341–46.
5. Dominique Pontier and Eugenia Natoli, “Infanticide in Rural Male Cats (Felis catus L.) as a Reproductive Mating Tactic,” Aggressive Behavior 25
(1999): 445–49.
6. Phyllis Chesler, “Maternal Influence in Learning by Observation in Kittens,” Science 166 (1969): 901–03.
7. Marvin J. Herbert and Charles M. Harsh, “Observational Learning by Cats,” Journal of Comparative Psychology 31 (1944): 81–95.
8. Transcribed from the original letter in the British Museum (Natural History) collections.
9. These studies were mainly conducted by my colleagues Sarah Brown and Charlotte Cameron Beaumont. For more detail, see chapter 8 of my book,
The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, 2nd ed., coauthored by Rachel Casey and Sarah Brown (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012).
10. This silhouette trick fooled most cats only once; the second one they came across elicited almost no reaction at all.
11. Researchers think that the retention of juvenile characteristics into adulthood, referred to as neoteny, was a major factor in the domestication of many
animals, especially the dog. For example, at first sight the skull of a Pekinese is nothing like that of its ancestor the wolf, but in fact it is roughly the
same shape as the skull of a wolf fetus. Although the domestic cat’s body has not been neotenized, some of its behavior may have been, including the
upright tail and some other social signals.
12. A fuller account of the evolution of signaling in the cat family can be found in John W. S. Bradshaw and Charlotte Cameron-Beaumont, “The
Signalling Repertoire of the Domestic Cat and Its Undomesticated Relatives,” in Dennis Turner and Patrick Bateson, eds., The Domestic Cat: The
Biology of Its Behaviour, 2nd ed. (Cambridge: Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000), 67–93.
13. Christina D. Buesching, Pavel Stopka, and David W. Macdonald, “The Social Function of Allo-marking in the European Badger (Meles meles),”
Behaviour 140 (2003): 965–80.
14. Terry Marie Curtis, Rebecca Knowles, and Sharon Crowell-Davis, “Influence of Familiarity and Relatedness on Proximity and Allogrooming in
Domestic Cats (Felis catus),” American Journal of Veterinary Research 64 (2003): 1151–54. See also Ruud van den Bos, “The Function of
Allogrooming in Domestic Cats (Felis silvestris catus): A Study in a Group of Cats Living in Confinement,” Journal of Ethology 16 (1998): 1–13.
15. See my book Dog Sense: How the New Science of Dog Behavior Can Make You a Better Friend to Your Pet (New York: Basic Books, 2011).
16. Feral dogs, the direct descendants of wolves, show little of their ancestors’ social sophistication. Although groups of males and females band
together to form packs that share common territory, all the adult females attempt to breed, and most of the puppies are cared for only by their mother—
although occasionally, two litters may be pooled, and some records show fathers bringing food to their litters.
17. One well-researched instance of explosive speciation is the cichlid fish of Lake Victoria, which despite now being the world’s largest tropical lake
was dry land only 15,000 years ago. Today it contains many hundreds of cichlid species, none of which are found in any of the other African great lakes,
and most of which have evolved in the 14,000 years since the lake last filled with water. See, for example, Walter Salzburger, Tanja Mack, Erik
Verheyen, and Axel Meyer, “Out of Tanganyika: Genesis, Explosive Speciation, Key-innovations and Phylogeography of the Haplochromine Cichlid
Fishes,” BMC Evolutionary Biology 5 (2005): 17.
18. Kipling, Just So Stories for Little Children (New York: Doubleday, Page & Co., 1902); available online at www.boop.org/jan/justso/cat.htm.
19. Our noses are particularly sensitive to thiols: minute traces are added to natural gas, which in itself is completely odorless, to make it easier to detect
leaks.
20. Ludovic Say and Dominique Pontier, “Spacing Pattern in a Social Group of Stray Cats: Effects on Male Reproductive Success,” Animal Behaviour
68 (2004): 175–80.
21. See, for example, the policies of Cats Protection, which is as of 2011 “the largest single cat neutering group in the world”: www.cats.org.uk/what-
we-do/neutering/.
Chapter 8
1. Gary D. Sherman, Jonathan Haidt, and James A. Coan, “Viewing Cute Images Increases Behavioral Carefulness,” Emotion 9 (2009): 282–86;
available online at tinyurl.com/bxqg2u6.
2. Robert A. Hinde and Les A. Barden, “The Evolution of the Teddy Bear,” Animal Behaviour 33 (1985): 1371–73.
3. See www.wwf.org.uk/how_you_can_help/the_panda_made_me_do_it/.
4. Kathy Carlstead, Janine L. Brown, Steven L. Monfort, Richard Killens, and David E. Wildt, “Urinary Monitoring of Adrenal Responses to
Psychological Stressors in Domestic and Nondomestic Felids,” Zoo Biology 11 (1992): 165–76.
5. Susan Soennichsen and Arnold S. Chamove, “Responses of Cats to Petting by Humans,” Anthrozoös: A Multidisciplinary Journal of the Interactions
of People & Animals 15 (2002): 258–65.
6. Karen McComb, Anna M. Taylor, Christian Wilson, and Benjamin D. Charlton, “The Cry Embedded within the Purr,” Current Biology 19 (2009):
R507–08.
7. Henry W. Fisher, Abroad with Mark Twain and Eugene Field: Tales They Told to a Fellow Correspondent (New York: Nicolas L. Brown, 1922), 102.
It’s worth noting the end of the quotation as well: “ . . . outside of the girl you love, of course.”
8. Some manufacturers add salt to dry cat food, but not for its taste: it’s mainly there to stimulate cats to drink, thereby minimizing their risk of
developing bladder stones.
9. The late Penny Bernstein conducted a detailed study of stroking, details of which sadly remained unpublished when she died in 2012. For a summary,
see Tracy Vogel’s “Petting Your Cat—Something to Purr About” at www.pets.ca/cats/articles/petting-a-cat/, and Bernstein’s own review, “The Human-
Cat Relationship,” in The Welfare of Cats, ed. Irene Rochlitz (Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer, 2005), 47–89.
10. Soennichsen and Chamove, “Responses of Cats,” note 5.
11. Mary Louise Howden, “Mark Twain as His Secretary at Stormfield Remembers Him: Anecdotes of the Author Untold until Now,” New York
Herald, December 13, 1925, 1–4; available online at www.twainquotes.com/howden.html.
12. Sarah Lowe and John W. S. Bradshaw, “Ontogeny of Individuality in the Domestic Cat in the Home Environment,” Animal Behaviour 61 (2001):
231–37.
13. To hear two Bengal cats yowling and chirruping to each other, go to tinyurl.com/crb5ycj. The noise that many cats make when they see birds
through a window is sometimes referred to as “chirping,” but is more correctly “chattering”; see tinyurl.com/cny83rd.
14. Mildred Moelk, “Vocalizing in the House-cat: A Phonetic and Functional Study,” American Journal of Psychology 57 (1944): 184–205.
15. Nicholas Nicastro, “Perceptual and Acoustic Evidence for Species-level Differences in Meow Vocalizations by Domestic Cats (Felis catus) and
African Wild Cats (Felis silvestris lybica),” Journal of Comparative Psychology 118 (2004): 287–96. When this paper was published, it was common
practice to refer to all African wildcats as lybica; however, these southern African wildcats, now known as cafra, are not particularly closely related to
domestic cats, having diverged from the Middle-Eastern/North-African lybica more than 150,000 years ago.
16. Nicholas Nicastro and Michael J. Owren, “Classification of Domestic Cat (Felis catus) Vocalizations by Naive and Experienced Human Listeners,”
Journal of Comparative Psychology 117 (2003): 44–52.
17. Dennis C. Turner, “The Ethology of the Human-Cat Relationship,” Swiss Archive for Veterinary Medicine 133 (1991): 63–70.
18. Desmond Morris, Catwatching: Why Cats Purr and Other Feline Mysteries Explained (New York: Three Rivers Press, 1993).
19. Of course, many cats do groom their owners, but cats also groom other adult cats.
20. Maggie Lilith, Michael Calver, and Mark Garkaklis, “Roaming Habits of Pet Cats on the Suburban Fringe in Perth, Western Australia: What Size
Buffer Zone Is Needed to Protect Wildlife in Reserves?” in Daniel Lunney, Adam Munn, and Will Meikle, eds., Too Close for Comfort: Contentious
Issues in Human-Wildlife Encounters (Mosman, NSW, Australia: Royal Zoological Society of New South Wales, 2008), 65–72. See also Roland W.
Kays and Amielle A. DeWan, “Ecological Impact of Inside/Outside House Cats around a Suburban Nature Preserve,” Animal Conservation 7 (2004):
273–83; available online at www.nysm.nysed.gov/staffpubs/docs/15128.pdf.
21. The transmitter and its battery could be carried by a bird the size of a thrush, so it was extremely light. These radio collars emit “beeps” every few
seconds that are picked up on a portable antenna and receiver; the aerial is directional, producing the strongest signal when pointed directly at the
animal. When used for tracking wildlife, the operator keeps at a distance once a reasonably strong signal has been picked up, to avoid disturbing the
animal, taking several recordings from different angles to map the animal’s exact location. With a pet cat, it’s easier to simply walk toward the radio
source until the cat is sighted.
22. This feline unfaithfulness was exposed by the University of Georgia’s Kitty Cam project: fifty-five cats in Athens, Georgia wore lightweight video
recorders, revealing that four often visited second households where they received food and/or affection. See the website for The National Geographic &
University of Georgia Kitty Cams (Crittercam) Project: A Window into the World of Free-roaming Cats (2011), www.kittycams.uga.edu/research.html.
23. Adapted from material provided by Rachel Casey. See John W. S. Bradshaw, Rachel Casey, and Sarah Brown, The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat,
2nd ed. (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012), chapter 11.
24. Ibid.
25. Ronald R. Swaisgood and David J. Shepherdson, “Scientific Approaches to Enrichment and Stereotypes in Zoo Animals: What’s Been Done and
Where Should We Go Next?” Zoo Biology 24 (2005): 499–518.
26. Marianne Hartmann-Furter, “A Species-Specific Feeding Technique Designed for European Wildcats (Felis s. silvestris) in Captivity,”
Säugetierkundliche Informationen 4 (2000): 567–75.
27. Adapted from the RSPCA webpage “Keeping Cats Indoors” (2013), www.rspca.org.uk/allaboutanimals/pets/cats/environment/indoors.
28. See Cat Behavior Associates, The Benefits of Using Puzzle Feeders for Cats (2013), www.catbehaviorassociates.com/the-benefits-of-using-puzzle-
feeders-for-cats/.
Chapter 9
1. Monika Lipinski et al., “The Ascent of Cat Breeds: Genetic Evaluations of Breeds and Worldwide Random-bred Populations,” Genomics 91 (2008):
12–21.
2. See www.gccfcats.org/breeds/oci.html.
3. Bjarne O. Braastad, I. Westbye, and Morten Bakken, “Frequencies of Behaviour Problems and Heritability of Behaviour Traits in Breeds of Domestic
Cat,” in Knut Bøe, Morten Bakken, and Bjarne Braastad, eds., Proceedings of the 33rd International Congress of the International Society for Applied
Ethology, Lillehammer, Norway (Ås: Agricultural University of Norway, 1999), 85.
4. Paola Marchei et al., “Breed Differences in Behavioural Response to Challenging Situations in Kittens,” Physiology & Behavior 102 (2011): 276–84.
5. See chapter 11 of my book Dog Sense (New York: Basic Books, 2011).
6. Obviously, the mother cat makes genetic contributions to the kittens as well, but she can also influence the development of her kittens’ personalities
through the way she raises them. Thus, the effects of maternal genetics, while undoubtedly as strong as those of the father, are harder to pin down.
7. For a more detailed discussion, see Sarah Hartwell, “Is Coat Colour Linked to Temperament?” (2001), www.messybeast.com/colour-tempment.htm.
8. Michael Mendl and Robert Harcourt, “Individuality in the Domestic Cat: Origins, Development and Stability,” in The Domestic Cat, 47–64.
9. Rebecca Ledger and Valerie O’Farrell, “Factors Influencing the Reactions of Cats to Humans and Novel Objects,” in Ian Duncan, Tina Widowski,
and Derek Haley, eds., Proceedings of the 30th International Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology (Guelph: Col. K. L. Campbell
Centre for the Study of Animal Welfare, 1996), 112.
10. Caroline Geigy, Silvia Heid, Frank Steffen, Kristen Danielson, André Jaggy, and Claude Gaillard, “Does a Pleiotropic Gene Explain Deafness and
Blue Irises in White Cats?” The Veterinary Journal 173 (2007): 548–53.
11. John W. S. Bradshaw and Sarah Cook, “Patterns of Pet Cat Behaviour at Feeding Occasions,” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47 (1996): 61–74.
12. For a review, see Michael Mendl and Robert Harcourt, note 8.
13. Sandra McCune, “The Impact of Paternity and Early Socialisation on the Development of Cats’ Behaviour to People and Novel Objects,” Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 45 (1995): 109–24.
14. Sarah E. Lowe and John W. S. Bradshaw, “Responses of Pet Cats to Being Held by an Unfamiliar Person, from Weaning to Three Years of Age,”
Anthrozoös 15 (2002): 69–79.
15. The technical term for an inveterate cat-hater is an ailurophobe. I carried out my research here with the assistance of a former colleague, Dr. Deborah
Goodwin.
16. Kurt Kotrschal, Jon Day, and Manuela Wedl, “Human and Cat Personalities: Putting Them Together,” in Dennis C. Turner and Patrick Bateson,
eds., The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour, 3rd ed. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2014).
17. Jill Mellen, “Effects of Early Rearing Experience on Subsequent Adult Sexual Behavior Using Domestic Cats (Felis catus) as a Model for Exotic
Small Felids,” Zoo Biology 11 (1992): 17–32.
18. Nigel Langham, “Feral Cats (Felis catus L.) on New Zealand Farmland. II. Seasonal Activity,” Wildlife Research 19 (1992): 707–20.
19. Julie Feaver, Michael Mendl, and Patrick Bateson, “A Method for Rating the Individual Distinctiveness of Domestic Cats,” Animal Behaviour 34
(1986): 1016–25.
20. See Patrick Bateson, “Behavioural Development in the Cat,” in Turner and Bateson, eds., The Domestic Cat, 2nd ed., 9–22.
Chapter 10
1. Michael C. Calver, Jacky Grayson, Maggie Lilith, and Christopher R. Dickman, “Applying the Precautionary Principle to the Issue of Impacts by Pet
Cats on Urban Wildlife,” Biological Conservation 144 (2011): 1895–901.
2. Michael Woods, Robbie Mcdonald, and Stephen Harris, “Predation of Wildlife by Domestic Cats Felis catus in Great Britain,” Mammal Review 33
(2003): 174–88; available online at tinyurl.com/ah6552e. This paper does not note that the survey was largely completed by children, nor does it provide
any information about the format of the questionnaire used.
3. See, for example, Britta Tschanz, Daniel Hegglin, Sandra Gloor, and Fabio Bontadina, “Hunters and Non-hunters: Skewed Predation Rate by
Domestic Cats in a Rural Village,” European Journal of Wildlife Research 57 (2011): 597–602. The University of Georgia Kitty Cam project recorded
30 percent of outdoor cats capturing and killing prey; see www.wildlifeextra.com/go/news/domestic-cat-camera.html. This reduced to 15 percent if
indoor-only cats were included.
4. See tinyurl.com/ak8c4ne.
5. Keen gardeners also seem to detest cats; a 2003 survey commissioned by the UK’s Mammal Society as further grist for their campaign against cat
ownership found that cats were rated alongside rats and moles as the mammals gardeners least like to see in their gardens.
6. Natalie Anglier, “That Cuddly Kitty Is Deadlier than You Think,” New York Times, January 29, 2013, tinyurl.com/bb4nmpb; and Annalee Newitz,
“Domestic Cats Are Destroying the Planet,” io9, January 29, 2013, tinyurl.com/adhczar.
7. Ross Galbreath and Derek Brown, “The Tale of the Lighthouse-keeper’s Cat: Discovery and Extinction of the Stephens Island Wren (Traversia
lyalli),” Notornis 51 (2004): 193–200; available online at notornis.osnz.org.nz/system/files/Notornis_51_4_193.pdf.
8. B. J. Karl and H. A. Best, “Feral Cats on Stewart Island: Their Foods, and Their Effects on Kakapo,” New Zealand Journal of Zoology 9 (1982): 287–
93. Despite this study, the cats on Stewart Island were subsequently exterminated, but (as predicted from the study) the kakapo continued to decline, and
eventually scientists moved all the survivors to another, predator-free island.
9. Scott R. Loss, Tom Will, and Peter P. Marra, “The Impact of Free-ranging Domestic Cats on Wildlife of the United States,” Nature Communications
(2013): DOI: 10.1038/ncomms2380.
10. Cats may not be a recent introduction to Australia: it has been suggested that feral cats actually spread there several thousand years ago from
Southeast Asia, following the same route as the dingo, the Australian feral dog. See Jonica Newby, The Pact for Survival: Humans and Their
Companion Animals (Sydney, Australia: ABC Books, 1997), 193.
11. Christopher R. Dickman, “House Cats as Predators in the Australian Environment: Impacts and Management,” Human–Wildlife Conflicts 3 (2009):
41–48.
12. Ibid.
13. Maggie Lilith, Michael Calver, and Mark Garkaklis, “Do Cat Restrictions Lead to Increased Species Diversity or Abundance of Small and Medium-
sized Mammals in Remnant Urban Bushland?” Pacific Conservation Biology 16 (2010): 162–72.
14. James Fair, “The Hunter of Suburbia,” BBC Wildlife, November 2010, 68–72; available online at www.discoverwildlife.com/british-wildlife/cats-
and-wildlife-hunter-suburbia. The study Fair reports on was conducted by Rebecca Thomas at the University of Reading.
15. Loss, Will, and Marra, note 9.
16. Ibid.
17. Philip J. Baker, Susie E. Molony, Emma Stone, Innes C. Cuthill, and Stephen Harris, “Cats about Town: Is Predation by Free-ranging Pet Cats Felis
catus Likely to Affect Urban Bird Populations?” Ibis 150, Suppl. 1 (2008): 86–99.
18. Andreas A. P. Møller and Juan D. Ibáñez-Álamo, “Escape Behaviour of Birds Provides Evidence of Predation Being Involved in Urbanization,”
Animal Behaviour 84 (2012): 341–48.
19. Eduardo A. Silva-Rodríguez and Kathryn E. Sieving, “Influence of Care of Domestic Carnivores on Their Predation on Vertebrates,” Conservation
Biology 25 (2011): 808–15. The cat and rat experiment was conducted in the early 1970s, when the ethics of animal experimentation were different than
they are today; Robert E. Adamec, “The Interaction of Hunger and Preying in the Domestic Cat (Felis catus): An Adaptive Hierarchy?” Behavioral
Biology 18 (1976): 263–72.
20. See the video at “Cat’s Bibs Stop Them Killing Wildlife,” Reuters, May 29, 2007; tinyurl.com/c9jfn36.
21. For more detailed advice, see www.rspb.org.uk/advice/gardening/unwantedvisitors/cats/birdfriendly.aspx.
22. David Cameron Duffy and Paula Capece, “Biology and Impacts of Pacific Island Invasive Species. 7. The Domestic Cat (Felis catus),” Pacific
Science 66 (2012): 173–212.
23. Andrew P. Beckerman, Michael Boots and Kevin J. Gaston, “Urban Bird Declines and the Fear of Cats,” Animal Conservation 10 (2007): 320–325.
24. See www.rspb.org.uk/wildlife/birdguide/name/m/magpie/effect_on_songbirds.aspx.
25. James Childs, “Size-dependent Predation on Rats (Rattus norvegicus) by House Cats (Felis catus) in an Urban Setting,” Journal of Mammalogy 67
(1986): 196–99.
Chapter 11
1. John W. S. Bradshaw, Rachel Casey, and Sarah Brown, The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, 2nd ed. (Wallingford, UK: CAB International, 2012),
chapter 11.
2. Darcy Spears, “Contact 13 Investigates: Teens Accused of Drowning Kitten Appear in Court,” June 28, 2012,
www.ktnv.com/news/local/160764205.html.
3. Summaries are available in several expert reports, including those commissioned by the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, the
Associate Parliamentary Group for Animal Welfare, and the UK Kennel Club in partnership with the rehoming charity Dogs Trust. See
www.rspca.org.uk/allaboutanimals/pets/dogs/health/pedigreedogs.
4. Domestic dogs have, of course, been selected for this trait throughout their association with man, since it is primarily their affection for us that makes
them trainable.
5. Eileen Karsh, “Factors Influencing the Socialization of Cats to People,” in The Pet Connection: Its Influence on Our Health and Quality of Life, ed.
Robert K. Anderson, Benjamin L. Hart, and Lynette A. Hart (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1984), 207–15.
6. You can find additional details of the introduction procedure on the Cats Protection website, www.cats.org.uk/cat-care/cat-care-faqs.
7. See the box in chapter 6 on clicker training. You can view a video of Dr. Sarah Ellis using clicker training to persuade a cat to walk into its cat carrier
at www.fabcats.org/behaviour/training/videos.html (icatcare.org).
8. See Vicky Halls’s article, for example, on the Feline Advisory Bureau website, www.fabcats.org/behaviour/scratching/article.html (icatcare.org).
9. For more information on declawing, written by a veterinarian, see “A Rational Look at Declawing from Jean Hofve, DVM” (2002),
declaw.lisaviolet.com/declawdrjean2.html.
10. This constraint does not apply so much to dogs. Most pedigree dog breeds were originally derived from working types—terriers, herding dogs,
guard-dogs, pack-hounds, and so on—and although the show-ring has diluted out much of their characteristic behavior, some still remains. Moreover,
some working breed clubs have deliberately kept their lines separate to perpetuate the genes that enable their dogs to perform their traditional functions.
11. The Governing Council of the Cat Fancy, “The Story of the RagaMuffin Cat” (2012), www.gccfcats.org/breeds/ragamuffin.html.
12. Debbie Connolly, “Bengals as Pets” (2003), www.bengalcathelpline.co.uk/bengalsaspets.htm.
13. Charlotte Cameron-Beaumont, Sarah Lowe, and John Bradshaw, “Evidence Suggesting Preadaptation to Domestication throughout the Small
Felidae,” Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 75 (2002): 361–66. This study included sixteen leopard cats and six Geoffroy’s cats.
14. Susan Saulny, “What’s Up, Pussycat? Whoa!” New York Times, May 12, 2005, www.nytimes.com/2005/05/12/fashion/thursdaystyles/12cats.html.
15. John W. S. Bradshaw, Giles F. Horsfield, John A. Allen, and Ian H. Robinson, “Feral Cats: Their Role in the Population Dynamics of Felis catus,”
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65 (1999): 273–83.
16. In this context, neutering can be conceived of as a “meme,” a concept that spreads, rather like a virus, from one human brain to another, producing
biological consequences. See Susan J. Blackmore, The Meme Machine (New York: Oxford University Press, 1999).
Index
Abandonment, xxi, 86, 215, 253
Abyssinian cats, 221, 225
Adrenalin, 148
Affection, display of, xiii, 148–149, 190–191
licking, 197
rubbing, 196, 197–198, 199–200, 200–201, 204–205
straight-up tail, 169–170, 174, 175, 198–199, 212, 230
Africa, cats of, 8, 12, 15, 16, 21. See also Egypt
Aggressive behavior, 225, 226, 264–265, 270
Agouti, 61
Agriculture, and domestication of cats, xxiv, 5, 6, 7, 22, 32–33, 47–48
Allogrooming, 171–173, 174, 196, 197
Anger, display of, 149–150
Angora, 260
Animal Intelligence (Romanes), 146–147
Animal Intelligence (Thorndike), 140–141
Anthropomorphism, xii, 124, 146–147, 188–189, 262
Anxiety, 150, 151, 152–153, 257–258
Apathetic resting, 215
Appleby, Steven, 145–146
Arabian wildcat (Felis silvestris lybica), 11, 12, 18–19, 19 (fig.), 40, 47
Archeological record, of cats, 2, 26–27, 28
Art, cats in, 2, 24, 28, 29 (fig.), 30, 31, 32 (fig.), 44, 45–46
Asia, cats of, 8, 12, 15, 47, 64, 221–222. See also Burmese cats; Siamese cats
Asian leopard cat (Prionailurus bengalensis), 269, 270
Australia, proposed restrictions on cats, 241, 245–246
Avena sativa (cat grass), 217
Baby-faced animals, 188–189
Balance, sense of, 109–111, 110 (fig.), 121
Bastet (Egyptian cat goddess), 18, 35, 50, 54
Beezlebina (European wildcat), 14
Belgium, cat worship in, 51, 53, 54
Belly-Up (play signal), 91, 92 (fig.)
Bengal cats (hybrids), xxvi, 269–270, 271 (fig.), 272
Birds, endangered, 241–245, 248, 255
Black cats, 58, 61, 62, 63, 75
Black Death, 53, 57
Blumenberg, Bennett, 66–67
Book of Kells, 54
Boston, Massachusetts, cats of, 67
Boswell, James, 60
Brain, cat, 121–122
Breeding, of cats. See also Mating, cat
and abnormal behavior, 223–224
dangers of breeding for extremes, 259–261
for the future, 263, 269–277
hybridization, with domestics, 269–272
pedigree, xxvi, 221, 259, 259–261, 268–269
and personality, 222–223, 224–225, 226–227
for personality, 263, 272–273
prolific nature of, 258–259
Bristol University Veterinary School, 152–153
Britain. See also United Kingdom
first cats in, 51–52
Middle Ages, 55–56, 57
tabby cats, 64–65, 65 (fig.)
British Natural History Museum, 168
Bubonic plague, 53, 57
Bunting, 120
Burial, of cats, 2, 26–27, 28, 30, 31, 37–37, 39 (fig.), 40, 42, 43, 49
Burmese cats, 47, 221, 223–224
Burrows, cat, 168
Caffre cats, 15, 17, 18, 202
California, cats of, 66–67, 244
Calming a cat, 79
Canada, cats of, 65, 67
Canterbury Tales (Chaucer), 55–56
Caracal, 8, 15, 34
Carnivore, cat as obligatory, 70–73
Cartoons, cat, xix, 145–146, 189
Casey, Rachel, 97, 151
Cat Fanciers’ Association, 221
Cat grass (Avena sativa), 217
“Cat-Book Poems,” 221
Catholic Church, 45–55, 56, 58
Catnip, 115
Cats Protection (charity), 99, 287, 291
Catteries, 49, 94
China, cats of, 10–11, 12, 46, 47, 222
Chromosomes, 227
Churchill, Winston, xi
Classical conditioning (Pavlovian), 132–135
Clicker training, 136, 137–138, 266
Clipnosis, 79
Cognitive processes, 123–156
and learning, 131–145
memory, 124, 125, 127, 143
sense of time, 127
testing, 145, 166–167
Collars, safety, 251
Colonies, cat, 83–84, 161–169
artificial, in shelters, 172–173
and dominance hierarchies, 173
feral, 161–166, 253
and mating, 178–179, 182–183
matriarchal nature, 173–174
Color blindness, 104–105, 128–129
Color variations, among cats, 40, 41, 42, 60–70, 220, 226–227
Conditioning
classical, 132–135
operant, 135–139, 139, 141–142
Cornish Rex, 221
Coronados Island, Gulf of California, 244
Cruelty, laws against animal, 258
Cupboard love, 199, 200
Curiosity, of cats, 139, 150
Cyprus, 2–3, 26–27, 28
Cystitis, urinary, 152–153
Dark Ages, 52–53, 54
Deafness, in cats, 62, 227
Declawing, 266, 267
Defecating, outside litterbox, 152, 213
Diana, Roman goddess, 46, 50, 51, 54
Dick Whittington’s Cat, 31
Dickman, Christopher R., 245
Diet, of cat, 70, 71–72, 73–75, 89
commercial food, 74–75
and hunting instinct, 250
and nutritional wisdom, 73, 74 (fig.)
and obesity, 217
Deities, ancient Egyptian, 18, 34–36, 50, 54
Dingo (wild dog), 28, 164, 245
Dislike, of cats, xix, xv, xx–xxi, 242
cats’ reaction to, 234–235
DNA
of domestic cats, 19, 19–20, 21, 273
and hybridization, 17–18, 168–169
of modern cats, 1, 11
of pedigree cats, 221–222
of wildcats, 15–16, 19–20, 20
Dogs, and cats, 96, 164
Domestication of cats, xxiv, 1–5, 11–17, 42, 47–48, 49, 50, 76, 190, 202–203, 218
Dominance, 173
Ear structure, cat, 107–108, 109, 110
Egypt, ancient, 28–36, 37–40, 42–44
cats in art, 28, 29 (fig.), 30, 31, 44
cults and religion, xxiv, 48, 49, 50
sacrificial cats, 37, 38–40, 42–43
snakes in, 33
wildcats, attempts to tame, 7
Emotions, xxiv, 145–156. See also Stress
affection, 148–149, 190–191
anger, 149–150
anxiety, 150, 151–153, 257–258
cats as unexpressive, 146, 192
fear, 149–150
grief, 154–155
guilt, 155, 156
and hormones, 148
and hunting, 146–147
jealousy, 153–154, 156
pride, 155
relational, 153–155, 156
survey, of United Kingdom cat owners, 151 (fig.)
Endangered species, 244
Endorphins, 148
Epinephrine, 148
Estrus, of female, 179
Europe, 12
cat sacrifice, 53–54
cats of, 12, 14–15, 52
Dark Ages, 52–53, 54
Middle Ages, 46, 54–55, 57
spread of cats, to Western, 51–52, 61
European forest wildcat (Felis silvestris silvestris), 52
Evolution
cultural, 102
of urban birds, 248, 255
Evolution, and explosive speciation, 174–175
Evolution, of cats, 8
and domestication, 190, 218
genetic, 102–103
and influence of human contact, 174
and persecution, 56
and personality, 263
and sacrificial catteries, 49
Explosive speciation, 174–175
Extinct species, 243, 245
Eye color, 227
Eye structure, cats’, 103–105
Fabcats.org (icatcare.org) website, 251
Fabric eating, in pedigree oriental cats, 223–224
Facial features, cat, 188–189
Falls, reaction to, 110–111
Farm cats, 37, 159–160, 236–237
Fear, display of, 149–150, 152
Fear effect hypothesis, 255
Feeders, puzzle, 217
Felinine, 180, 181
Felis silvestris (wildcats). See Wildcats (Felis silvestris)
Feral cats, 254 (fig.)
colonies, 161–166, 190
eradications, 252–253, 254–255
foraging in garbage, 249 (fig.)
handling feral kitten, 77 (fig.)
homing, 98–101
hunting, 249–250
kittens, 77 (fig.), 88, 94–95
neutering, xxi, 253–254
population, 244–245
relocating, 83–84
and socialization, 94–95, 98–101, 122
and trauma, 100–101
vocalizations, 201–202, 202
and wildlife predation, 243, 251–252
Ferrets, 45, 48, 188
Fertile Crescent, 2, 5, 6, 45
political unrest, 21
wildcats of, 7–8
Fertility, cat, 185–186
Fighting, cat, 176, 212–213
Fish, personalities in, 228
Fishing cat, 15
Fishing cat (Felis viverrina), 8, 10
Flehmen, 117, 118 (fig.)
Food, commercial cat, 74–75
Food, for cats, 70–75, 238, 217, 250
For I Will Consider My Cat Jeoffry (Smart), 59–60
France, cats of, 19–20, 53, 59, 61
Fur, cat, 54
Furniture, scratching, 266
Gape response, 117
Genet, 33
Genetic defects, and breeding, 260–261
Genetic homogeneity, 61
Genetics
and color, 40, 41, 42, 60–62, 64, 66–67, 220, 226–227
and domestication, 13–15, 48
and evolution, 102
and fabric eating, 223–224
and hunting skills, 239
and mechanisms of inheritance, 227
and personality, 101, 219–222, 225–227, 231–233, 234, 236, 237–238, 239–240
polydactyly, 67
and response to catnip, 115
Geoffroy’s cat, 8, 270–271
Germany, wildcats of, 12
Ginger cats, 40, 41, 42, 68, 227
Governing Council of the Cat Fancy (GCCF), 98, 221, 222
Grain stores, ancient, 5
Graves, cat, 2, 30, 31, 37, 97–98
Abydos cemetery, Upper Egypt, 30
ancient Egypt. See Egypt, ancient, sacrificial cats
Shillourokambos (Neolithic archeological site), 26–27, 28
Greece, ancient, 45–46, 53
Gregory IX, Pope, Vox in Rama, 55
Grief, 154–155, 156
Grooming, excessive, 152, 213
Grooming, mutual, 171–173, 174, 196, 197
Guilt, 155, 156
Habituation, and behavior, 131
Hand-raised cats, 93, 236
Harappan civilization, 10, 15
Hearing, cat, 106–108, 121, 127, 134–135
Hello Kitty, xix, 189
Herodotus, 35, 36
Hobhouse, Leonard Trelawny, 124
Homing, of kittens, 98–101, 201
age for, 98–99, 264
feral, 99–101
littermates homed together, 94, 216
and personality, 233
Hong Kong, cats of, 64, 65, 65 (fig.)
Hormones, 5, 84, 85, 86, 148, 191–192
Hughes, Rebecca, 247
Humane Society website, 138
Humans
body language, 234
cats’ attachment to, 190–191
cats’ perception of, 204–207, 234
Humboldt County, California, cats of, 66–67
Hunter-gatherers, and pets, 27, 28
Hunting
and cat color, 69
and emotions, 146–147
and hearing, 106–108, 121, 127, 134–135
and hunger, 130, 207, 208, 209
and object permanence, 143–144, 145
and personality, 238–239
preventing cats from, 251, 262, 266
and sense of smell, 114, 116–118, 121
and sense of touch, 108–109
and size of prey, 130
skills, and genetics, 239, 240
triggers for, 130–131
and vision, 106, 116, 121, 126
Hunting instinct, 75, 88, 218, 266, 268, 277
and wildlife protection, 249–250
Hybrid cats, xxvi, 16–18, 18, 19–20, 21, 22–24, 168–169
breeding, 269–272
Imprinting, kitten, 83
India, arrival of cats, 46
Indian desert cat (Felis silvestris ornata), 12, 15, 17, 47
Indoor cats, 214, 215–218, 258, 266
Industrial Revolution, and cat color, 64
Infanticide, of kittens, 36–37, 164–165, 187
Institute of Wildlife Research, Sydney, Australia, 245
Intelligence, cat, 140–142, 145
International Cat Association, 221
Islam, and cats, 55
Israel, 2, 5, 6, 21
Italy, 44, 46, 51
Jaguar, 8, 40
Jaguarundi, 11
Japan, cats of, 46, 222
Japanese cat shrub, 115
Jealousy, 153–154, 156
Johnson, Samuel, 60
Jordan, 2, 6, 21
Jungle cats, 8, 9 (fig.), 15, 28, 214
Kin selection, and cooperation, 163
Kipling, Rudyard, “The cat that walked by himself,” 175
Kittens, 82 (fig.)
birth and postnatal period, 79–83, 84, 87
diet of, 89
group mothering, 83–84, 159–161
hand-raised, 93, 236
homing, 94, 98–101, 201, 216, 233
multiple paternity, in litter, 183, 184, 186
neurological, 86, 87
nursing, 83, 84
personality, 230–234
and play, 90–93, 93 (fig.), 147–148
and primary socialization period, 77, 87, 88, 101, 220, 264
sensory development, 84, 85, 85 (fig.), 87
socialization of, 231–234
and stress hormones, 84, 85, 86
sexual maturity, 87
weaning, 89–90
Kittens, feral, 77 (fig.), 88, 94–95, 98–100
Kittens with a dog, 96 (fig.)
Kitty Cams, 247
Korat cat, 47, 221
Latin America, cats of, 62, 63–64
Laws, animal cruelty, 258
Learning
cat-to-cat, 165–168
and classical (Pavlovian) conditioning, 132–135, 137–138
clicker training, 136, 137–138
and imitation, 165–166
and operant conditioning, 135–139, 141–142
and rewards, 131, 132–133, 136
Leopard, 8
Leopard cat, 191, 214, 269–70
Libby (author’s cat), xiv, 80–82, 82 (fig.), 161, 172 (fig.), 195, 199
Licking, mutual, 171–173, 196, 197
Licking, of humans, 196–197
Lions, 8, 115, 158–159, 164–165, 168, 214
Literature, cats in, 54, 55–56, 59–60, 221
Litterbox, not using, 151–152, 213
Littermates, homed together, 94, 216
Long-haired cats, 69, 98, 223, 224
Los Angeles Times, 242
Lost cats, 210–211
Lowe, Willoughby Prescott, 168, 169
Loyalty, 190, 191
Lucy (author’s cat), xiv, 80–82, 82 (fig.), 161, 172 (fig.), 195
Macdonald, David, 18–19, 159, 178–179
Magpie, as predator, 255–256, 256 (fig.)
Maine Coon cat, 69, 222
Male cats, vs. female, 175, 177–178, 182, 184–185, 186–187
Mammal Society, United Kingdom, 242, 247
Manipulation, of owners by cats, 139–140, 201, 204–205
Manul (Pallas’s cat), 10 (fig.), 11
Manx cats, 221, 260–261
Margay cat, 270, 272
Massachusetts, cats of, 67
Mating, cat, 177–187. See also Breeding, of cats
copulation, 179–180, 181
and domestication, 182–183
estrus, of female, 179
and infanticide, 165
and multiple paternity, 183, 184, 186
and scent signals, 117–118, 119, 120, 179, 193
in urban environments, 184–185
Mediterranean islands, 2–5, 27, 28, 44
Memory, 124, 125, 127, 143, 145
Meowing, 139–140, 169, 201–204, 230
“Messybeast” website, 294
Mice, 3–4, 5–7, 14, 32, 48, 116, 118
Middle Ages, xx, xxiv, 47, 53–57
Middle East, cats of, 2, 11, 20, 21–22
Migrating, to another owner, 210–211
Mongolia, 15, 17, 46
Mousers, cats as, xii, xxiii–xxiv, 7–8, 24, 32–33, 48, 51, 52–53, 56, 72
Mull (Scottish island), 58
Multiple cats, in household, 211–213, 217, 218, 265
Mummified cats, 37–38, 39 (fig.), 40, 42, 43, 49
Munchkin cats, 259
Natufians, 6–7, 9, 22
Natural History Museum, Paris, 25–27
Naughty torties, 226
Neoprene bib, 138
Nest-moving, new mother, 79–81
Neutering, 184–185
and evolution, xxvi, 273–275
of feral cats (Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR), xxi, 253–254
genetic consequences, xxvi, 273–275
and improved social tolerance, 177, 187
and territoriality, 208
Nevada, animal cruelty law, 258
New York Times, 242
New Zealand, xxi, 236, 244–245
Norwegian Forest cat, 68, 69, 225
Nursing kittens, 83, 84
Nutritional wisdom, 73
Obesity, 217
Object permanence, 143–144, 145
Odor, cat, 180–181
Olfactory bulb, 112
Olfactory sense. See Smell, sense of
Onychectomy (declawing), 266, 267
Operant conditioning, 135–139, 139, 141–142
Oriental cats, 11, 15, 47, 98, 223–225
Oxytocin, in mother cats, 84
Pacing, 214
Pallas’s cat (Manul), 8, 10 (fig.), 11
“Pangur Bán” (poem), 54
Parachuting, during fall, 110
Pavlov, Ivan, 132
Pavlovian conditioning (classical), 132–134
and extinction, 134
Pedigree cats, xxv–xxvi, 220, 221. See also individual breeds
breeding, 221, 259, 259–261, 268–269
dangers of breeding for extremes, 259–261
DNA of, 221–222
and personality, 226
Persecution of cats, xx, xxiv, 46, 51, 54–55, 56, 57, 58, 75
Persian cats, 11, 98, 221, 223, 224, 225, 260
Personality, 219–240
bold vs. shy, 228, 229 (fig.), 231–232, 235, 236, 237
and breed, 223, 224–225, 226–227
and breeding, 222–223
breeding for, 263, 269, 272–274
and cat-cat interactions, 236–237
and color, 226–227
concept of, 227–228
and environment, 219, 220, 239
and genetics, 101, 219–222, 225–227, 231–233, 234, 236, 237–238, 239–240
and homing of kittens, 233
and hunting skills, 238–239, 240
and reaction to cat phobics, 234–235
and socialization, 231–233, 235–236
studying, cat, 228–234
Petting, 197–198
Phobia, of cats, xx–xxi, 242
cats’ reactions, 234–235
Phoenicians, 44, 52, 61
Pica, 223–224
Pitt, Frances, 14–15
Plague, the, 53, 57
Play, and kitten development, 90–93, 93 (fig.)
and hormones, 147–148, 148
Play, of adult cats, 128–131
Play-Face, 91, 92 (fig.)
Pleitropy, 226
Polydactyly, 67
Pregnancy, cat, 87
Prey, brought home, 205–206
presenting a dead vole, 206 (fig.)
Price, of cat, Dark Ages, 52–53
Pride, feeling of, 155, 156
Primary socialization period, 77, 87, 88, 101, 220
Purring, 193–196, 201
Qadan culture, 22
Radio collar, for tracking, 209–210
RagaMuffin cats, 269
Ragdoll cats, 269
Rats, 33–34, 48, 243
and bubonic plague, 53
and wildlife predation, 243–244, 245, 255, 256
Reading University, United Kingdom, 247
Reciprocal altruism, 163
Relational emotions, 153–155, 156
Religion, cats’ role in, 34–36, 37, 42–43, 46, 50–51, 55
Rodents. See Mice, Rats
Romanes, George S., Animal Intelligence, 146–147
Rome, ancient, 36, 45, 46
Royal Society for the Protection of Birds, 242, 255–256
Rubbing
on humans, xiii, 196, 197–198, 199, 200, 200–201, 204, 230
mutual, 171, 174, 196, 200, 204, 212
on other animals, 200
Russian fur-fox experiment, 49
Russian-American Trading Company, 67
Sacrificial cats, 37, 38–40, 42–43, 49, 53–54
Safari cat (hybrid), 271, 271 (fig.)
Sand cat (Felis margarita), 8, 9 (fig.)
Satan (European wildcat), 14–15
Saudi Arabia, 21, 18–19
Scent glands, locations of, 120, 197–198
Scent marks
rubbed on furniture, 199
rubbed on humans, 197–198, 200
rubbed on other cats, 170–171, 174
spraying urine, 175 (fig.), 177, 180, 213
Scent signals
and hunting, 114, 116, 121
and mating 117–118, 119, 120, 179, 193
Schweinfurth, Georg, 4, 18
Scottish Fold cat, 261
Scratching, furniture, 266, 267
Scruffing reflex, 79, 269
Sensory perception, of cats. See Balance, Hearing, Smell, Touch, Vision
Serval, 8, 272
Shelters, kittens born in, 97–98
Shillourokambos, Cyprus, 26–27, 28
Ships, cats on, 51, 52, 61, 72
Siamese cat, 11, 15, 47, 98, 220, 223 (fig.), 223–224, 225, 227, 261
Smart, Christopher, For I Will Consider My Cat Jeoffry (poem), 59–60
Smell, of cats, 180–181
Smell, sense of, 103, 112–114, 115–119
Smithers, Reay, 16–17
Smithsonian Institute, 242, 247
Snakes, as pests, 33, 48
Sociability, and personality, 230–231, 236–237
Socialization period, primary, 77, 87, 88, 101, 220, 264
Society, cat, 236–237
domestic, 211–213, 217, 218, 265
and individual personality, 230, 231, 236–237
signs that cats do or don’t get along, 212
South Africa, cats of, 15, 16, 17, 163
South America, cats of, 8, 68, 69, 270–271
Spain, cats of, 44, 62, 68
Speciation, explosive, 174–175
Sphynx cat, 221
Splodge (author’s cat), xiii–xiv, 191, 199, 200, 201
radio collar for, 209–210
Spraying, of urine, 177, 180, 213, 270
tomcat spraying, 175 (fig.)
Squitten (twisty cat), 261
Stand Up, 91
Startle reflex, 128
Stress, 214–214, 223–224
and social conflict, xxii, 151–153, 213, 257, 265
Stress hormones, 84, 86, 87, 191–192
Syria, 2, 5, 21, 44, 55
Tabby cats, 63 (fig.), 63–66, 69, 226
Taigherm, 58
Tail, straight-up, 169–170, 174, 175, 198–199, 212, 230
Territoriality, of cats, xiv, 191–192, 207–210, 211, 218
and domestication, 215
feral cats, 208, 215
and fighting, 210, 211, 212–213
signs of failing to establish territory, 213
wildcats, 214
Testing cats’ cognitive processes, 145, 166–167
Thorndike, Edward L., Animal Intelligence, 140–141
Thumamah reserve, Saudi Arabia, 18–19
Tigers, 8, 157–158
Toes, extra, 67
Tomcats, 175, 177–178, 182, 184–185, 186–187
neutered, 187, 208
Tomkies, Mike, 15
Tortoiseshell cats, 41, 67, 68
as “naughty torties,” 226
Touch, sense of, 108–109, 121
Toys, for cats, 128–130, 217, 251
Training, 131–142, 262–266
chaining, 138–139
clicker, 136, 137–138, 266
dogs vs. cats, 131, 135–136
and furniture scratching, 266
for indoor living, 266
and operant conditioning, 141–142
of owners by cats, 139–140
and rewards, 131, 132–133, 136
for social acceptability, 262–263, 263–264
and stressful situations, 266
and wildlife protection, 266
Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR), 253–254
Trauma, feral cats, 100–101
Travel, and cat carrier, 266
Treed cats, 121–122
Turkish Angora, 260
Turkish Van cat, 222
Turntable, for learning experiment, 167 (fig.)
Twain, Mark, 60, 196, 200
Twisty cat (squitten), 261
United Kingdom, 12, 17, 98, 221, 242, 247. See also Britain
United States, 17, 221, 244–245
University of Georgia’s Kitty Cams, 242
Urban environment, xix, 184–185, 252–253, 255
Urinary problems, 151–153, 175 (fig.), 177, 180, 213, 267, 270
Urine, composition of male, 180
Van cat, 222
Vermin. See also Mice, Rats
cats as, 12, 20
cats as controllers of, xii, xxiii–xxiv, 34, 44, 46, 48, 51, 52–53, 56, 72, 244, 245
Vertical Stance, 91, 93 (fig.)
Vision, cat, 103–106, 121, 125–129, 261
Vocalizations, 139–140, 169, 176, 201–202, 223, 227
Vomeronasal organ (VNO), 117–120
cat displaying use of, 118 (fig.)
and cat urine, 181
and mating, 117–118, 119, 120
other animals, 118–119
Vox in Rama (Pope Gregory IX), 55
Weaning, of kittens, 89–90
Whiskers, cat, 109, 121
White cats, 61–62, 227
Whitfield, Paul, 242
Wildcats (Felis silvestris), 7–8, 12–17, 19–20, 21, 24, 168–169, 202, 214–215
interbreeding, with domestics. See Hybrid cats
Wildlife protection, xix, xxi, 218, 240, 241–256
and bird evolution, 248
and cat training, 266
cats’ impact, 241–243, 245–248, 251–252
and fear effect hypothesis, 255
and habitat loss, 255, 256
and neutering programs, 253–254
and other predators, 248, 255–256
and restrictions on cats, 241, 245–246, 255, 258
in urban environments, 252–253
Worship, cat, 34–36, 37–38, 42–43, 46, 50–51, 53, 54, 55
Zoos, 17, 214–215, 216