CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Categorization
CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Categorization
CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Categorization
Barbara Loken
Department of Marketing, University of Minnesota, Carlson School of Management,
Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455; email: [email protected]
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 454
CONSUMER CATEGORIZATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
Similarity-Based Category Inferences to New Category Members . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
Assimilation and Contrast . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 456
The Influence of New Category Members on the Category . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 457
Goals as Organizing Frameworks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 458
Factors That Influence Category Expansion and Flexibility . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 458
Self as a Category . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 459
CONSUMER INFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 460
Inferences Based on Omitted Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 460
Singular Brand Versus Multiple Brand Inferences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 461
Inferences Based on Irrelevant Attributes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 461
Inferences Based on Experiential and Sensory Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 461
Conditional Inferences, Correlations, and Causal Reasoning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 463
Metacognitive Experiences and Knowledge . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 463
Brand Name Inferences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 464
Accessibility-Diagnosticity Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 464
Biases and Motivated Reasoning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 465
CONSUMER AFFECT, MOOD, FEELINGS, AND ATTITUDES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 465
0066-4308/06/0110-0453$20.00 453
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
454 LOKEN
INTRODUCTION
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
Research in the years 1994–1996 was reviewed previously (Jacoby et al. 1998),
and is included to the extent that it helps frame and clarify research on the topics
addressed. Research published outside the designated years or in publications
outside of the four journals noted is included selectively.
CONSUMER CATEGORIZATION
During the review period, research on brand categories (e.g., Healthy Choice prod-
ucts) and goal-derived categories (e.g., things to eat in my car on the way to work)
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
has increased relative to more traditional research on product categories (e.g., au-
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
456 LOKEN
extensions when the product category information of either the parent brand
(Meyvis & Janiszewski 2004) or the extension (Klink & Smith 2001) is the only
information accessible or available, and is less often used when the extension’s
individuating attribute information is available (Klink & Smith 2001). To the ex-
tent that both product category similarity and brand attribute similarity reflect a
common goal, they will both predict extension acceptance; if goals are incongru-
ent, other factors (e.g., only product category similarity or only brand attribute
similarity) may determine extension acceptance (Martin & Stewart 2001).
In addition to research on brand extensions, other consumer research replicates
earlier findings of a positive relationship between a category member’s typical-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
ity (or similarity to other category members) and the category member’s evalu-
ation. In general, more typical category members are better liked (Carpenter &
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
Nakamoto 1996; Folkes & Patrick 2003, study 3; Simonin & Ruth 1998; Veryzer
& Hutchinson 1998; Zhang & Sood 2002).
Another type of similarity-based comparison involves the alignability of at-
tributes of the new category stimulus and the existing category (e.g., Gentner &
Markman 1997, Gregan-Paxton 2001, Gregan-Paxton & John 1997, Moreau &
Markman 2001, Roehm & Sternthal 2001). Alignable differences (versus differ-
ences that are not alignable) are more memorable (Zhang & Markman 1998),
comparative ads are more effective than noncomparative ads when brands can be
compared along the same (versus different) attributes (Zhang et al. 2002; see also
Lurie 2004 for research on structural properties of attributes), and brand evaluations
are more prone to revision when counterattitudinal information can be compared
along the same attributes as accessible brand information (Pham & Muthukrishnan
2002).
A different perspective argues that resolving a moderate disparity produces pos-
itive affect, which is applied to the object evaluated (Peracchio & Meyers-Levy
1994). Per this view, an object that is moderately dissimilar from a category will
be better liked than either a similar or extremely dissimilar object. This moderate
incongruity effect was found for people with low prior knowledge about the cat-
egory, who required more effort to resolve the incongruity (Peracchio & Tybout
1996), and disappeared when people had low motivation to process information
(e.g., under high risk conditions, Campbell & Goodstein 2001).
for processing contextual information or when people are high in need for cog-
nition (Meyers-Levy & Tybout 1997), and when remembered information is re-
counted analytically rather than episodically (Bickart & Schwarz 2001). Others
argue that contrast effects are not due to more cognitive effort, but rather depend
on whether the accessible context information (the standard of comparison) is
well defined (Levin & Levin 2000) and is both distinctive and relevant (Stapel
et al. 1998). Finally, although assimilation responses are generally the default re-
sponse, in some situations contrast effects can be the default (Raghunathan & Irwin
2001).
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Information about new category members can also influence existing category
beliefs. For example, brand extensions (new category members) can have effects
on beliefs and attitudes about the parent brand category that are either negative
(dilution of the brand) or positive (enhancement). Beliefs about a well-known
brand were influenced negatively when information about a brand extension (John
et al. 1998) or information about a brand context (Buchanan et al. 1999) was
incongruent with beliefs about the brand (see also Milberg et al. 1997). People
processed the incongruent information thoughtfully and analytically (Buchanan
et al. 1999). Negative dilution, as well as positive enhancement, effects were repli-
cated under conditions of high motivation (Gurhan-Canli & Maheswaran 1998)
and when extension information was high in accessibility (Ahluwalia & Gurhan-
Canli 2000). Under low motivation, people used nonanalytic processing; more
(versus less) prototypical extensions modified parent brand evaluations (Gurhan-
Canli & Maheswaran 1998). That is, extremely atypical category members had
less impact on category beliefs than did moderately atypical category members.
When extension information was low in accessibility, diagnostic cues were used;
negative information (producing dilution effects) was more diagnostic for brand
evaluations when extensions were in similar, but not dissimilar, categories to the
parent brand, and positive information (producing enhancement effects) was more
diagnostic when extensions belonged to dissimilar, rather than similar, categories
(Ahluwalia & Gurhan-Canli 2000).
Negative brand extension information can also affect existing individual prod-
ucts of the parent brand if these products are not already strongly established in the
minds of consumers (John et al. 1998). Outside a laboratory setting, too, people
were found to update their perceptions of the parent brand and individual products
of the brand based on their experiences with brand extensions (Erdem 1998).
Finally, priming a new brand extension can increase the accessibility of the
parent brand category, particularly when the parent brand category is not already
chronically accessible (Morrin 1999). Researchers have examined primes in a
variety of other contexts, too, including prosmoker and antismoker stereotypes
(Pechmann & Knight 2002), exemplars in television viewing (Shrum et al. 1998),
and product expensiveness judgments (Adaval & Monroe 2002).
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
458 LOKEN
2001). When goals conflict (i.e., a single product cannot meet all salient goals) or
when there is goal ambiguity (i.e., a lack of salient goals), consumers are more
likely to consider alternatives from different product categories (Ratneshwar et al.
1996). When people violated their goals, they showed decreased performance
on a subsequent task, as compared to people with no goals (Soman & Cheema
2004).
Motivational and self-regulatory approaches to assessing goals have increased
in interest (e.g., Ariely & Levav 2000, Bagozzi & Dholakia 1999, Baumeister
2002, Higgins 2002, Krishnan & Shapiro 1999). Researchers have compared con-
sumers who were promotion-focused and prevention-focused using a variety of
operationalizations (e.g., independent versus interdependent self-views, Aaker &
Lee 2001; ideals versus “oughts,” Pham & Avnet 2004). Consumers who were
promotion-focused (versus prevention-focused) were persuaded more by positive
(versus negative) outcomes (Aaker & Lee 2001), subjective affective responses
to an ad (versus message substance, Pham & Avnet 2004), hedonic, attractive,
performance-related attributes (versus utilitarian, unattractive, reliability-related
attributes, Chernov 2004a), and actions that departed from (versus preserved) the
status quo (Chernov 2004b).
When consumers were exposed five times (versus once) to information about a
brand category’s positive link to an incongruent brand extension, their perceptions
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
of extension consistency increased and they evaluated the extension more favor-
ably (Lane 2000). When a brand’s benefits were accessible, which occurred more
for brand categories with more (versus less) diverse members, new incongruent
category extensions were rated more positively (Meyvis & Janiszewski 2004).
Innovative consumers, who tend to be less risk averse, were more accepting of
incongruent category members (Klink & Smith 2001).
Ability and knowledge factors also increase flexibility. Experts (relative to
novices) were more likely to organize information by product subcategories, re-
trieve different brands for different usage occasions (Cowley & Mitchell 2003), and
store information about alternatives in a way that increased flexibility in evaluating
the same product across different usage occasions (Mitchell & Dacin 1996). Older
children, relative to younger children, define categories more by complex func-
tional (versus perceptual) cues (John 1999; see also Achenreiner & John 2003).
Owners (versus nonowners) of a brand (Kirmani et al. 1999) have broader, more
flexible categories when making judgments. Strategies taught to consumers to
break down frequency estimates into subcategories (e.g., unbundling credit card
expenses) can reduce errors and processing effort (Menon 1997, Srivastava &
Raghubir 2002).
Self as a Category
The self category has been described as flexible or malleable (Aaker 1999).
The same individual may retrieve and use different self-views, depending upon
the chronic and temporal accessibility of these inputs, in the form of cultural
views (Aaker & Lee 2001, Briley & Wyer 2002, Brumbaugh 2002, Forehand &
Deshpande 2001, Lau-Gesk 2003, Mandel 2003), social identities (Bolton & Reed
2004, Reed 2004), or personality traits (Aaker 1999). A social identity that is
salient, important to the self, and evaluatively diagnostic (versus one that is not) is
more likely to influence attitudes (Reed 2004), and thinking dominated by a strong
salient identity is more resistant to corrective procedures (Bolton & Reed 2004).
Self-appraisals regarding performance or reflections of what others might think
influenced more global self-definitions (Laverie et al. 2002). In comparison with
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
460 LOKEN
CONSUMER INFERENCES
Inferences Based on Omitted Conclusions
Consumers make inferences beyond what they read or see in the text of a message,
and these inferences can have an impact on judgments (Kardes et al. 2001, 2004b).
When ads omitted (versus included) a key element, recall was improved along
dimensions related to the element (Sengupta & Gorn 2002). When a compara-
tive ad stated a specific (versus vague) cost savings amount (relative to a named
comparison brand) for one service provided by the brand, consumers inferred that
the brand was also superior on other, missing, service price data, contributing to
suboptimal choices (Pechmann 1996). Greater motivation and ability increase the
likelihood that consumers will engage in spontaneous inferences. Consumers were
more likely to complete ambiguously cropped objects in ads under high than under
low motivation conditions (Peracchio & Meyers-Levy 1994) and to later falsely
recall the object as intact, although completing these objects did not necessarily
improve evaluations of the product in the ads. With regard to deceptive infer-
ences, highly motivated consumers were more likely to make invalid inferences
from one type of deceptive ad claim (incomplete comparison claims); however,
they were less likely to be deceived by ads that required detailed processing for
nondeception to occur (inconspicuous qualification claims, Johar 1995). When
cognitive capacity was high, consumers were also more likely to use product dis-
closures to correct or update their judgments about the product (Johar & Simmons
2000).
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
also show correction effects when they are made aware that relevant information
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
462 LOKEN
found to change in the direction of advertised information (presented after the prod-
uct experience) that provides a different interpretation of the experience (Braun
1999), although only when product familiarity is low (Cowley & Janis 2004).
Advertising was more likely to frame the interpretation of product experiences
for older than for younger children (Moore & Lutz 2000). Research shows other
fallibilities associated with experience data (Alba & Hutchinson 2000, Hoch 2002).
Although people like experience data, experience attribute claims (claims based
on attributes that the consumer can verify only through direct experience) are
generally regarded as less credible than search attributes, which are verifiable
without consumption (Jain et al. 2000), but will be used by consumers when these
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
attributes are the only data available and are accompanied by a high (versus low)
credibility source (Jain & Posavac 2001).
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
Sensory experience data aside from traditional product usage also have an im-
pact on judgment, and research on them has increased in emphasis during the review
period. When consumers experienced sound quality, and were provided criteria
with which to evaluate it (i.e., reducing its ambiguity and increasing its diagnostic-
ity), their memory of the information improved, they placed more weight on it, and
made better choices (Shapiro & Spence 2002). Other inferences based on auditory
systems include those based on the consumer’s formal writing system (phonetic
versus logographic; Tavassoli & Han 2001, Tavassoli & Lee 2003, Zhang & Schmitt
2004), the speed and pitch of an announcer’s voice (Chattopadhyay et al. 2003),
and the phonetic structure of brand names (Yorkston & Menon 2004). When expe-
riencing a product by touch, consumers’ confidence in judgments increased if they
were high (versus low) along a need-for-touch dimension (Peck & Childers 2003).
Ambient scent improved memory for familiar and unfamiliar brands (Morrin
& Ratneshwar 2003).
Visual elements, such as pictures (McQuarrie & Mick 2003), color as a back-
drop on Internet Web sites (Gorn et al. 2004, Mandel & Johnson 2002), product
shape (Folkes & Matta 2004, Raghubir & Krishna 1999, Wansink & Van Ittersum
2003), aesthetic design and unity (Veryzer & Hutchinson 1998), angle of vision,
cutting rate, camera motion (Larsen et al. 2004), typeface (Childers & Jass 2002,
Henderson et al. 2004), and numerical information (Viswanathan & Childers 1996),
have been found to convey meaning and influence processes such as information
search, elaborative processing, attitudes, and consumption. Visual elements can
also cause interference effects if ads for two different brands have similar pictures
(Kumar 2000, Kumar & Krishnan 2004).
A more direct link between perceptual (sensory-motor) data and cognition has
emerged. Features of an ad compete for available cognitive resources if they require
processing two elements simultaneously from the same (versus different) sensory
modality (Olsen 1997, Tavassoli & Lee 2003, Unnava et al. 1996). Features of an ad
can also be complementary and increase advertising effectiveness when perceptual
and other features (e.g., celebrity endorser, picture, brand name, scent, plot of
accompanying television show) are congruent with product attribute information
(Luna & Peracchio 2001, Mitchell et al. 1995, Russell 2002).
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
attribute correlational data when information load was low or when consumers
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
were motivated to elaborate (Kardes et al. 2004a), but were less prone to spillover
effects of negative information when they were highly committed to the brand
(Ahluwalia et al. 2001). While associative network theory continues to be a
prominent paradigm for examining belief interconnections and consumer learning
(Morrin 1999), others (van Osselaer & Janiszewski 2001) argue for a more dynamic
adaptive network theory in which association strengths are not learned indepen-
dently on the basis of the co-occurrence of two cues, but instead are updated and
evolve as cues interact. Attribution theory (Weiner 2000) and dialectical thinking
processes (Kahle et al. 2000) are useful for understanding the causal reasoning
strategies of consumers, and causal reasoning has been found to be shared across
cultural subgroups, which in turn influence the causal reasoning of individuals
(Sirsi et al. 1996).
464 LOKEN
whether, like other beliefs, they are easily accessible (Brown & Krishna 2004,
Campbell & Kirmani 2000, Shiv et al. 1997) or an ulterior motive by the marketer
is made salient (Warlop & Alba 2004).
to new product categories (Sen 1999), and focusing on a specific brand attribute
rather than the brand name can sometimes reduce consumers’ evaluations of brand
extensions (van Osselaer & Alba 2003).
When two brand names are combined (e.g., Slim Fast chocolate cake mix
by Godiva) the composite benefits more when the header brand (Slim Fast) is
combined with a complementary brand (Godiva) than when presented alone or
combined with a noncomplementary brand (e.g., Haagen-Dazs; Park et al. 1996).
In brand alliances, both brands are affected, but familiar (versus unfamiliar) brands
contribute more to brand evaluations (Siminon & Ruth 1998; see also Levin &
Levin 2000). When the ingredient in a brand extension is branded (e.g., Tide
with Irish Spring scent), more initial acceptance is found, but, unless the branded
ingredient represents a dissimilar attribute, using a new ingredient name (e.g.,
Tide with EverFresh scent) improves long-range expansion of the brand to new
categories (Desai & Keller 2002).
Accessibility-Diagnosticity Model
A number of psychological theories incorporate the constructs of accessibility
(salience) and diagnosticity (relevance). In consumer research, the framework of
Feldman & Lynch (1988), which argues that judgments are a function of the ac-
cessibility and the diagnosticity of information inputs relative to the accessibility
and diagnosticity of alternative inputs, has been used for understanding judgment
revision (Pham & Muthukrishnan 2002), behavioral frequency judgments (Menon
et al. 1995), car repurchase decisions (Fitzsimons & Morwitz 1996), the effects of
previously formed attitudes on choice (Baker 2001), cultural differences in the use
of cues (Aaker 2000), and brand extension information effects on family brand
evaluations (Ahluwalia & Gurhan-Canli 2000). A variant of the accessibility-
diagnosticity model, the mere-accessibility model, has been proposed to account
for conditions under which accessibility alone is sufficient to determine judg-
ments, for example when ease-of-retrieval occurs at a low awareness level and
functions as an heuristic in decision making, when the cognitive demands of the
task are too high, or when extra cognitive effort is unwarranted (Menon & Raghubir
2003).
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
(Jain & Maheswaran 2000) and impression and defensive goals (Ahluwalia 2002)
increase consumers’ selectively processing information in a biased manner. Other
research finds that biases were reduced when consumers had sufficient motivation
and ability to counter the bias, i.e., when accuracy in judgments was rewarded
(West 1996), when the evidence against the bias was strong, when the bias was
based on events not controllable, and when the judgments did not pertain to or
protect the view of self (Kamins et al. 1997, Lin et al. 2003, Menon & Johar 1997).
466 LOKEN
(goal-relevant) information (see Pham 2004). Some mood research suggests that
consumers were willing to make decision errors in order to maintain a positive
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
mood (Meloy 2000). But other research suggests that when the stakes are high
(e.g., life threatening), consumers will forego short-term mood maintenance for
longer-term gains (Keller et al. 2003), or will even maintain a negative mood if
it will improve task performance; that is, people will guide affect regulation in a
functional manner (Cohen & Andrade 2004).
Moods also affect type of processing. Some research supports the idea that
people in a positive mood engage in nonanalytic, top-down, creative processing
styles, and people in a negative mood engage in analytic, effortful processing
(Keller et al. 2002, Murry & Dacin 1996). Shiv & Fedorikhin (1999) find that
people will choose based on utilitarian considerations (e.g., fruit salad over cake)
unless their processing load is high, in which case they will choose based on
hedonic considerations (cake over fruit salad). Other findings suggest that people
in a positive mood are not less motivated to process information (Adaval 2003,
Pham et al. 2001), but instead rely more on strategic thinking (Lee & Sternthal
1999) or diagnostic inputs (Pham 1996), especially when the risks of not being
informed are very high (Keller et al. 2003).
Aside from testing these theories of affect, consumer research finds that emo-
tions are predictable and can be measured reliably. Consumers tend to provide
reliable measures of emotion (Richins 1997) and have a shared knowledge of
emotion categories (Ruth 2001), and negative emotions (which people prefer to
avoid) cause predictable shifts in choice patterns (Luce et al. 1999). Baumgartner
et al. (1997) showed how moment-to-moment emotional responses to an ad could
be integrated into an overall evaluation of the ad.
belief based. Using these criteria, some research (Fishbein & Middlestadt 1995)
finds that attitude toward an ad influenced attitude toward a behavior indirectly
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
through cognitive structure, whereas other research (Bodur et al. 2000) finds that
the connection between affect and attitude was direct and not mediated by cognitive
structure.
Most cognitive psychologists would agree that cognition includes an array of
conscious and unconscious processes and representations (cf. Barsalou 1999).
A growing number of consumer studies examine implicit brand attitudes, atti-
tudes that may be activated automatically and outside conscious awareness (Bargh
2002, Maison et al. 2004, Shapiro 1999). Increased emphasis is being placed on
development of appropriate measurement techniques to assess automaticity and
nonconscious processing (see Krishnan & Chakravarti 1999), such as the Implicit
Association Test (Brunel et al. 2004).
468 LOKEN
behavioral intention among collectivists, whereas the attitude factor was a stronger
determinant among individualists.
A meta-analysis (Notani 1998) of the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1991),
which is an extension of the theory of reasoned action, found that the construct
of perceived behavioral control (PBC) predicted behavior more when it reflected
control over factors primarily internal (versus external) to the individual, when
it was operationalized as a global measure (rather than as a set of beliefs), when
the behaviors were familiar (versus unfamiliar), and when the sample included
nonstudent (versus student) samples. The meta-analysis also found attitude toward
performing a behavior was a more consistent predictor than PBC of behavioral
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
intention, and intention was a stronger predictor than PBC of behavior, supporting
the original theory of reasoned action.
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
MODELS OF PERSUASION
Elaboration
The moderating role of elaborative thought on persuasion has been a key research
issue in the past decade. Amount of elaborative thought (high versus low) and
valence of thought (favorable or unfavorable) have been examined extensively.
Type of thought has also been studied, such as analysis of item-specific versus
relational thoughts (e.g., Malaviya et al. 1996), thoughts that vary as a function
of specific goals (e.g., Escales & Luce 2004, Sengupta & Johar 2000), thoughts
about the self (Meyers-Levy & Peracchio 1996), and metacognitive thoughts and
experiences (e.g., Schwarz 2004). More commonly, elaborative thought has been
examined as a function of how information is framed by goals or contextual factors.
Research on the amount of elaborative thought predicted by dual process models,
in particular, has been plentiful.
(Jain & Maheshwaran 2000), when brand extensions were incongruent with ex-
pectations regarding the parent brand (Gurhan-Canli & Maheswaran 1998), and
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
when message items were inconsistent with expectations, particularly for elderly
adults (Yoon 1997). Consumers elaborated more on messages from endorsers who
were untrustworthy (e.g., from endorsing too many products) than from endorsers
who were trustworthy (Priester & Petty 2003).
Consumers showed more heuristic processing when information was in a nar-
rative versus an unorganized list form, presumably because the narrative form is
structurally similar to daily life experiences (Adaval & Wyer 1998). Heuristic pro-
cessing was also found more for elderly (than for younger) adults (Yoon 1997),
who show reduced ability over time to remember detailed information.
Under low motivation conditions, people may also rely more on their subjective
accessibility experiences rather than content (Schwarz 2004). Children (versus
adults) focused more on surface than on deep cues (Zhang & Sood 2002; cf. John
1999), and younger children engaged in less elaboration than did older children
(Moore & Lutz 2000).
Cues that have been traditionally regarded as peripheral or heuristic (e.g., brand
names and endorser attractiveness) led to more attitude persistence (Sengupta et al.
1997) and were processed more centrally or systematically (and elaborated upon
more) when the cue was perceived to have relevance to the message arguments
(e.g., endorser attractiveness, Shavitt et al. 1994; source effects, Kirmani & Shiv
1998) or was perceived as diagnostic (e.g., Aaker & Maheswaran 1997). Other
variables, too, have been found to take on multiple roles in a persuasive message,
such as rhetorical questions (Ahluwalia & Burnkrant 2004), message framing
(Meyers-Levy & Maheswaran 2004), and color (Meyers-Levy & Peracchio 1995).
Also, both types of thought (heuristic and systematic) can occur concurrently
(Meyers-Levy & Maheswaran 2004).
Finally, even though high arousal is generally believed to reduce cognitive ca-
pacity, Pham (1996) found that when consumers were highly aroused, they selected
informative diagnostic cues over a peripheral cue (endorser status). People also
seemed to use different routes to persuasion because different types of information
were perceived as diagnostic under different routes rather than because they had
different desired levels of accuracy (Pham & Avnet 2004).
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
470 LOKEN
certainty and their attitude-intention link, but only if the message was from an
expert source (Tormala & Petty 2004). Just as strong attitudes are more likely than
weaker attitudes to guide behavior, stronger beliefs (confidence in one’s thoughts)
are more likely to guide attitudes; increased confidence in positive thoughts in-
creased the effectiveness of ads, and increased confidence in negative thoughts
decreased their effectiveness (Brinol et al. 2004).
472 LOKEN
(as reported earlier; see also Cox & Cox 2001), it was often more persuasive
(Block & Keller 1995, Homer & Batra 1994, Meyers-Levy & Maheswaren 2004,
Shiv et al. 2004), especially when consumers were accuracy-motivated (Ahluwalia
2002) or perceived the negative tactic used as fair (Shiv et al. 1997). On the other
hand, consumers who processed narratives and imagined scenarios were more re-
sistant to incorporating negative (Adaval & Wyer 1998) and corrective information
(Bolton 2003), their inferences about a firm’s service providers were more resistant
to negative than positive information (Folkes & Patrick 2003), and the greater in-
fluence of negative (versus positive) information was attenuated when consumers
were impression-motivated but reversed when consumers were defense-motivated
(Ahluwalia 2002).
Exposure to both positive and negative information weakens attitudes if recon-
ciliation of evaluative items is not a salient goal or if issue elaboration is prevented.
Attitudes also weaken when people feel inconsistencies in preexisting beliefs and
attitudes or when they feel ambivalent, experiencing both positive and negative
affect toward an attitude object (Sengupta & Johar 2002). Conflicting affective or
emotional responses (Mick & Fournier 1998), that is, mixed emotions, may be
difficult to manage (Williams & Aaker 2002), but in some cases, conflict in emo-
tions may be a naturally occurring phenomenon. Ambivalent “weakened” attitudes
may not always reflect weakness; older (versus younger) and Asian (versus Anglo)
Americans have been found to be more adaptable in accepting mixed emotions
(Williams & Aaker 2002).
CONCLUSIONS
In the review period, general conclusions about psychological phenomena (e.g.,
dual processes of persuasion, relationship between category similarity and affect)
have been followed up through studies that examine (a) the effects in more di-
verse and numerous domains and contexts than found previously (demonstrating
the effects’ generalizability and importance), and (b) contingencies and excep-
tions to these effects. The difficult task of determining the size and nature of the
contingencies for many of these topic domains has not yet been accomplished.
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
with the bias or stereotypic thinking. To the extent that the effort of elaboration led
to positive outcomes (such as reduced uncertainty or feelings of successful resolu-
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author gratefully acknowledges the perspective and comments of Joan Meyers-
Levy, Deborah Roedder John, Durairaj Maheswaran, Frank Kardes, Rohini
Ahluwalia, Bob Wyer, and Curt Haugtvedt on an earlier draft of this article.
Preparation for this article was supported in part by a McKnight-BER research
grant from the Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota.
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
474 LOKEN
LITERATURE CITED
Aaker JL. 1999. The malleable self: the role of an accessibility-diagnosticity perspective. J.
self-expression in persuasion. J. Market. Res. Consum. Res. 27(3):371–81
36(1):45–58 Ahluwalia R, Unnava HR, Burnkrant RE. 2001.
Aaker JL. 2000. Accessibility or diagnosticity? The moderating role of commitment on the
Disentangling the influence of culture on per- spillover effect of marketing communica-
suasion processes and attitudes. J. Consum. tions. J. Market. Res. 38(4):458–70
Res. 26(4):340–57 Ajzen I. 1991. Attitudinal versus normative
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Aaker JL, Lee AY. 2001. “I” seek pleasures messages: an investigation of the differen-
and “we” avoid pains: the role of self- tial effects of persuasive communications on
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
Barone MJ, Miniard PW, Romeo J. 2000. The tensions? Empirical generalizations based on
influence of positive mood on brand exten- secondary analysis of eight studies. J. Mar-
sion evaluations. J. Consum. Res. 26(4):386– ket. Res. 38(4):494–500
400 Boush DM, Friestad M, Rose GM. 1994. Ado-
Barsalou LW. 1999. Perceptual symbol sys- lescent skepticism toward TV advertising
tems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22:577–660 and knowledge of advertiser tactics. J. Con-
Baumgartner H. 1995. On the utility of con- sum. Res. 21(1):165–75
sumers’ theories in judgments of covariation. Braun KA. 1999. Postexperience advertising ef-
J. Consum. Res. 21(4):634–43 fects on consumer memory. J. Consum. Res.
Baumgartner H. 2002. Toward a personology of 25(4):319–34
the consumer. J. Consum. Res. 29(2):286–92 Brendl CM, Markman AB, Messner C. 2003.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Baumgartner H, Sujan M, Padgett D. 1997. The devaluation effect: activating a need de-
Patterns of affective reactions to advertise- values unrelated objects. J. Consum. Res.
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
476 LOKEN
Buchanan L, Simmons CJ, Bickart BA. 1999. Chernov A. 2004b. Goal orientation and con-
Brand equity dilution: retailer display and sumer preference for the status quo. J. Con-
context brand effects. J. Market. Res. sum. Res. 31(3):557–65
36(3):345–55 Childers TL, Jass J. 2002. All dressed up with
Burnkrant RE, Unnava HR. 1995. Effects of something to say: effects of typeface se-
self-referencing on persuasion. J. Consum. mantic associations on brand perceptions
Res. 22(1):17–26 and consumer memory. J. Consum. Psychol.
Campbell MC, Goodstein RC. 2001. The mod- 12(2):93–106
erating effect of perceived risk on con- Cohen JB, Andrade EB. 2004. Affective intu-
sumers’ evaluations of product incongruity: ition and task-contingent affect regulation. J.
preference for the norm. J. Consum. Res. Consum. Res. 31(2):358–67
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
iarity and advertising repetition effects. J. Cooke ADJ, Sujan H, Sujan M, Weitz BA. 2002.
Consum. Res. 30(2):292–304 Marketing the unfamiliar: the role of con-
Campbell MC, Kirmani A. 2000. Consumer’s text and item-specific information in elec-
use of persuasion knowledge: the effects of tronic agent recommendations. J. Market.
accessibility and cognitive capacity on per- Res. 39(4):488–98
ceptions of an influence agent. J. Consum. Cowley E, Janis E. 2004. Not necessarily bet-
Res. 27(1):69–83 ter, but certainly different: a limit to the ad-
Carpenter GS, Glazer R, Nakamoto K. 1994. vertising misinformation effect on memory.
Meaningful brands from meaningless differ- J. Consum. Res. 31(1):229–35
entiation: the dependence on irrelevant at- Cowley E, Mitchell AA. 2003. The moderating
tributes. J. Market. Res. 31(3):339–50 effect of product knowledge on the learning
Carpenter GS, Nakamoto K. 1996. Impact of and organization of product information. J.
consumer preference formation on market- Consum. Res. 30(3):443–54
ing objectives and second mover strategies. Cox D, Cox AD. 2001. Communicating the
J. Consum. Psychol. 5(4):325–58 consequences of early detection: the role of
Chaiken S. 1980. Heuristic versus systematic evidence and framing. J. Market. 65(3):91–
information processing and the use of source 103
versus message cues in persuasion. J. Per- Desai KK, Keller KL. 2002. The effects of in-
sonal. Soc. Psychol. 39:752–66 gredient branding strategies on host brand ex-
Chakravarti A, Janiszewski C. 2003. The influ- tendibility. J. Market. 66(1):73–93
ence of macro-level motives on consideration Erdem T. 1998. An empirical analysis of um-
set composition in novel purchase situations. brella branding. J. Market. Res. 35(3):339–
J. Consum. Res. 30(2):244–58 51
Chandon P, Morwitz VG, Reinartz WJ. 2004. Escales JE, Luce MF. 2004. Understanding the
The short- and long-term effects of measur- effects of process-focused versus outcome-
ing intent to repurchase. J. Consum. Res. focused thought in response to advertising.
31(3):566–72 J. Consum. Res. 31(2):274–85
Chattopadhyay A, Dahl DW, Ritchie RJB, Feldman JM, Lynch JG. 1998. Self-generated
Shahin KN. 2003. Hearing voices: the im- validity and other effects of measuring be-
pact of announcer speech characteristics on lief, attitude, intention, and behavior. J. Appl.
consumer response to broadcast advertising. Psychol. 73(3):421–35
J. Consum. Psychol. 13(3):198–204 Fishbein M, Ajzen I. 1975. Belief, Attitude,
Chernov A. 2004a. Goal-attribute compatibil- Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction
ity in consumer choice. J. Consum. Psychol. to Theory and Research. Reading, MA:
13(1,2):141–50 Addison-Wesley
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
Fishbein M, Middlestadt SB. 1995. Noncog- screen color affects time perception. J. Mar-
nitive effects on attitude formation and ket. Res. 41(2):215–25
change: fact or artifact? J. Consum. Psychol. Gorn GJ, Pham MT, Sin LY. 2001. When
4(2):181–202 arousal influences ad evaluation and valence
Fishbein M, Middlestadt SB. 1997. A strik- does not (and vice versa). J. Consum. Psy-
ing lack of evidence for nonbelief-based chol. 11(1):43–55
attitude formation and change: a response Grant SJ, Malaviya P, Sternthal B. 2004. The
to five commentaries. J. Consum. Psychol. influence of negation on product evaluations.
6(1):107–15 J. Consum. Res. 31(3):583–91
Fiske ST, Neuberg SL. 1990. A continuum of Gregan-Paxton J. 2001. The role of abstract and
impression formation, from category-based specific knowledge in the formation of prod-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
478 LOKEN
Herr PM, Page CM. 2004. Asymmetric asso- Janiszewski C, Noel H, Sawyer AG. 2003. A
ciation of liking and disliking judgments: meta-analysis of the spacing effect in ver-
So what’s not to like? J. Consum. Res. bal learning: implications for research on ad-
30(4):588–601 vertising repetition and consumer memory.
Higgins ET. 2002. How self-regulation creates J. Consum. Res. 30(1):138–49
distinct values: the case of promotion and Jarvis WB, Petty RE. 1996. The need to evalu-
prevention decision making. J. Consum. Psy- ate. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 70:172–94
chol. 12(3):177–91 Johar GV. 1995. Consumer involvement and
Hoch SJ. 2002. Product experience is seductive. deception from implied advertising claims.
J. Consum. Res. 29(3):448–54 J. Market. Res. 32(3):267–79
Homer PM, Batra R. 1994. Attitudinal effects Johar GV, Simmons CJ. 2000. The use of con-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Keller PA, Block LG. 1996. Increasing the per- Kumar A, Krishnan S. 2004. Memory interfer-
suasiveness of fear appeals: the effect of ence in advertising: a replication and exten-
arousal and elaboration. J. Consum. Res. sion. J. Consum. Res. 30(4):602–11
22(4):448–59 Lane VR. 2000. The impact of ad repetition and
Keller PA, Block LG. 1997. Vividness effects: ad content on consumer perceptions of in-
a resource-matching perspective. J. Consum. congruent extensions. J. Market. 64(2):80–
Res. 24(3):295–304 91
Keller PA, Lipkus IM, Rimer BK. 2002. De- Larsen V, Luna D, Peracchio LA. 2004. Point of
pressive realism and health risk accuracy: the view and pieces of time: a taxonomy of image
negative consequences of positive mood. J. attributes. J. Consum. Res. 31(1):102–11
Consum. Res. 29(1):57–69 Lau-Gesk LG. 2003. Activating culture through
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Keller PA, Lipkus IM, Rimer BK. 2003. Af- persuasion appeals: an examination of the
fect, framing, and persuasion. J. Market. Res. bicultural consumer. J. Consum. Psychol.
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
40(1):54–65 13(3):301–15
Kempf DAS, Smith RE. 1998. Consumer pro- Laverie DA, Kleine RE III, Kleine SS. 2002.
cessing of product trial and the effects of prior Reexamination and extension of Kleine,
advertising: a structural modeling approach. Kleine, and Kernan’s social identity model
J. Market. Res. 35(3):325–38 of mundane consumption: the mediating role
Kirmani A, Shiv B. 1998. Effects of source of the appraisal process. J. Consum. Res.
congruity on brand attitudes and beliefs: the 28(4):659–69
moderating role of issue-relevant elabora- Law S, Hawkins SA, Craik FIM. 1998.
tion. J. Consum. Psychol. 7(1):25–47 Repetition-induced belief in the elderly: re-
Kirmani A, Sood S, Bridges S. 1999. The own- habilitation age-related memory deficits. J.
ership effect in consumer responses to brand Consum. Res. 25(2):91–107
line stretches. J. Market. 63(1):88–101 Lee AY. 2004. The prevalence of metacogni-
Klink RR, Smith DC. 2001. Threats to the ex- tive routes to judgments. J. Consum. Psychol.
ternal validity of brand extension research. J. 14(4):349–55
Market. Res. 38(3):326–426 Lee AY, Sternthal B. 1999. The effects of pos-
Krishnamurthy P, Sivaraman A. 2002. Counter- itive mood on memory. J. Consum. Res.
factual thinking and advertising responses. J. 26(2):115–27
Consum. Res. 28(4):650–58 Lee JA. 2000. Adapting Triandis’s model of
Krishnamurthy P, Sujan M. 1999. Retrospec- subjective culture and social behavior rela-
tion versus anticipation: the role of the ad tions to consumer behavior. J. Consum. Psy-
under retrospective and anticipatory self- chol. 9(2):117–26
referencing. J. Consum. Res. 26(1):55–69 Levin IP, Levin AM. 2000. Modeling the role of
Krishnan HS, Chakravarti D. 1999. Memory brand alliances in the assimilation of product
measures for pretesting advertisements: an evaluations. J. Consum. Psychol. 9(1):43–
integrative conceptual framework and a diag- 52
nostic template. J. Consum. Psychol. 8(1):1– Lin Y, Lin C, Raghubir P. 2003. Avoiding
37 anxiety, being in denial, or simply stroking
Krishnan HS, Shapiro S. 1999. Prospective and self-esteem: Why self-positivity? J. Consum.
retrospective memory for intentions: a two- Psychol. 13(4):464–77
component approach. J. Consum. Psychol. Loken B, Joiner C, Peck J. 2002. Category atti-
8(2):141–66 tude measures: exemplars as inputs. J. Con-
Kumar A. 2000. Interference effects of contex- sum. Psychol. 12(2):149–62
tual cues in advertisements on memory for Loken B, Ward J. 1990. Alternative approaches
ad content. J. Consum. Psychol. 9(3):155– to understanding the determinants of typical-
66 ity. J. Consum. Res. 17:111–26
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
480 LOKEN
Luce MF, Payne JW, Bettman JR. 1999. Emo- Meloy MG. 2000. Mood-driven distortion
tional trade-off difficulty and choice. J. Mar- of product information. J. Consum. Res.
ket. Res. 36(2):143–59 27(3):345–59
Luna D, Peracchio LA. 2001. Moderators of Menon G. 1997. Are the parts better than the
language effects in advertising to bilinguals: whole? The effects of decompositional ques-
a psycholinguistic approach. J. Consum. Res. tions on judgments of frequent behaviors. J.
28(2):284–95 Market. Res. 32:335–46
Lurie NH. 2004. Decision making in informa- Menon G, Block LG, Ramanathan S. 2002.
tion rich environments: the role of informa- We’re at as much risk as we are led to be-
tion structure. J. Consum. Res. 30(4):473–86 lieve: effects of message cues on judgments
Maison D, Greenwald AG, Bruin RH. 2004. of health risk. J. Consum. Res. 28(4):533–49
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Predictive validity of the implicit associa- Menon G, Johar GV. 1997. Antecedents of pos-
tion test in studies of brands, consumer at- itivity effects in social versus nonsocial judg-
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
and coping strategies. J. Consum. Res. tive experience. J. Consum. Res. 28(1):89–
25(2):123–43 104
Milberg S, Park CW, McCarthy M. 1997. Man- Muthukrishnan AV, Ramaswami S. 1999. Con-
aging negative feedback effects associated textual effects on the revision of evaluative
with brand extension: the impact of alterna- judgments: an extension of the omission-
tive branding strategies. J. Consum. Psychol. detection framework. J. Consum. Res. 26(1):
6(2):119–40 70–84
Miniard PW, Barone MJ. 1997. The case for Notani AS. 1998. Moderators of perceived be-
noncognitive determinants of attitude: a cri- havioral control’s predictiveness in the the-
tique of Fishbein and Middlestadt. J. Con- ory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis. J.
sum. Psychol. 6(1):77–93 Consum. Psychol. 7(3):247–71
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
Mitchell A, Dacin PA. 1996. The assessment of Olsen DG. 1997. The impact of interstimulus
alternative measures of consumer expertise. interval and background silence on recall. J.
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
482 LOKEN
Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Priester JR, Nayakankuppam D, Fleming MA,
Springer-Verlag Godek J. 2004. The A2 SC2 model: the in-
Pham MT. 1996. Cue representation and selec- fluence of attitudes and attitude strength on
tion effects of arousal on persuasion. J. Con- consideration and choice. J. Consum. Res.
sum. Res. 22(4):373–87 30(4):574–87
Pham MT. 1998. Representatives, relevance, Priester JR, Petty RE. 2003. The influence
and the use of feelings in decision making. J. of spokesperson trustworthiness on message
Consum. Res. 25(2):144–57 elaboration, attitude strength, and advertising
Pham MT. 2004. The logic of feeling. J. Con- effectiveness. J. Consum. Psychol. 13:408–
sum. Psychol. 14(4):360–9 21
Pham MT, Avnet T. 2004. Ideals and oughts Raghubir P, Krishna A. 1999. Vital dimen-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
and the reliance on affect versus substance in sions in volume perception: Can the eye
persuasion. J. Consum. Res. 30(4):503–18 fool the stomach? J. Market. Res. 36(3):313–
by University of Minnesota- Law Library on 08/29/06. For personal use only.
Russell CA. 2002. Investigating the effective- yond our conscious control: perceptual and
ness of product placements in television conceptual fluency effects caused by inciden-
shows: the role of modality and plot con- tal ad exposure. J. Consum. Res. 26(1):16–
nection congruence on brand memory and 36
attitude. J. Consum. Res. 29(3):306–18 Shapiro S, MacInnis DJ, Heckler SE. 1997. The
Russo JE, Meloy MG, Medvec VH. 1998. Pre- effects of incidental ad exposure on the for-
decisional distortion of product information. mation of consideration sets. J. Consum. Res.
J. Market. Res. 35(4):438–52 24(1):94–104
Ruth JA. 2001. Promoting a brand’s emotion Shapiro S, Spence MT. 2002. Factors affecting
benefits: the influence of emotion categoriza- encoding, retrieval, and alignment of sensory
tion processes on consumer evaluations. J. attributes in a memory-based brand choice
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
cussion about a product: Rehearsing what to 1994. The interaction of endorser attractive-
say can affect your judgments. J. Consum. ness and involvement in persuasion depends
Res. 29(1):101–15 on the goal that guides message processing.
Schwarz N. 1997. Moods and attitude judg- J. Consum. Psychol. 3(2):137–62
ments: a comment on Fishbein and Mid- Shiv B, Britton JAE, Payne JW. 2004.
dlestadt. J. Consum. Psychol. 6(1):93–98 Does elaboration increase or decrease the
Schwarz N. 2004. Metacognitive experiences in effectiveness of negatively versus posi-
consumer judgment and decision making. J. tively framed messages? J. Consum. Res.
Consum. Psychol. 14(4):332–48 31(1):199–208
Sen S. 1999. The effects of brand name sugges- Shiv B, Edell JA, Payne JW. 1997. Factors af-
tiveness and decision goal on the develop- fecting the impact of negatively and posi-
ment of brand knowledge. J. Consum. Psy- tively framed ad messages. J. Consum. Res.
chol. 8(4):431–55 24(3):285–94
Sengupta J, Fitzsimons GJ. 2000. The effects Shiv B, Fedorikhin A. 1999. Heart and mind
of analyzing reasons for brand preferences: in conflict: the interplay of affect and cog-
disruption or reinforcement. J. Market. Res. nition in consumer decision making. J. Con-
37(3):318–30 sum. Res. 26(3):278–92
Sengupta J, Fitzsimons GJ. 2004. The effect of Shrum LJ, Wyer RS, O’Guinn TC. 1998. The
analyzing reasons on the stability of brand effects of television consumption on social
attitudes: a reconciliation of opposing pre- perceptions: the use of priming procedures to
dictions. J. Consum. Res. 31(3):705–11 investigate psychological processes. J. Con-
Sengupta J, Goodstein RC, Boninger DS. 1997. sum. Res. 24(4):447–58
All cues are not created equal: obtaining atti- Siminon BL, Ruth JA. 1998. Is a company
tude persistence under low-involvement con- known by the company it keeps? Assess-
ditions. J. Consum. Res. 23(4):351–61 ing the spillover effects of brand alliances
Sengupta J, Gorn GJ. 2002. Absence makes the on consumer brand attitudes. J. Market. Res.
mind grow sharper: effects of element omis- 35(1):30–42
sion on subsequent recall. J. Market. Res. Simonson I, Carmon Z, Dhar R, Drolet A,
39(2):186–202 Nowles S. 2001. Consumer research: in
Sengupta J, Johar GV. 2002. Effects of incon- search of identity. Annu. Rev. Psychol.
sistent attribute information on the predic- 52:249–75
tive value of product attitudes: toward a res- Sirsi AK, Ward JC, Reingen PH. 1996. Micro-
olution of opposing perspectives. J. Consum. cultural analysis of variation in sharing of
Res. 29(1):39–56 causal reasoning about behavior. J. Consum.
Shapiro S. 1999. When an ad’s influence is be- Res. 22(4):345–72
25 Oct 2005 16:33 AR ANRV264-PS57-17.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: KUV
484 LOKEN
Soman D, Cheema A. 2004. When goals responses to new product designs. J. Consum.
are counterproductive: the effects of vio- Res. 24(4):374–94
lation of a behavioral goal on subsequent Viswanathan M, Childers TL. 1996. Processing
performance. J. Consum. Res. 31(1):52– of numerical and verbal product information.
62 J. Consum. Psychol. 5(4):359–85
Spangenberg ER, Greenwald AG. 1999. So- Viswanathan M, Childers TL. 1999. Under-
cial influence by requesting self-prophecy. J. standing how product attributes influence
Consum. Psychol. 8(1):61–89 product categorization: development and val-
Srivastava J, Raghubir P. 2002. Debiasing using idation of fuzzy set-based measures of grad-
decomposition: the case of memory-based edness in product categories. J. Market. Res.
credit card expense estimates. J. Consum. 36(1):75–94
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
CONTENTS
Frontispiece—Herbert C. Kelman xvi
PREFATORY
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
viii CONTENTS
FAMILY/MARITAL THERAPY
Current Status and Future Directions in Couple Therapy,
Douglas K. Snyder, Angela M. Castellani, and Mark A. Whisman 317
ATTITUDE CHANGE AND PERSUASION
Attitudes and Persuasion, William D. Crano and Radmila Prislin 345
BARGAINING, NEGOTIATION, CONFLICT, SOCIAL JUSTICE
Psychological Perspectives on Legitimacy and Legitimation, Tom R. Tyler 375
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND ASSESSMENT
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006.57:453-485. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
INDEXES
Subject Index 613
Cumulative Index of Contributing Authors, Volumes 47–57 637
Cumulative Index of Chapter Titles, Volumes 47–57 642
ERRATA
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Psychology chapters
may be found at http://psych.annualreviews.org/errata.shtml