Investigating Print Awareness Skills of Preschool

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Education and Science

Vol 43 (2018) No 196 49-65

Investigating Print Awareness Skills of Preschool Children in Terms of


Child and Parent Variances *

Vedat Bayraktar 1

Abstract Keywords
The research was carried out with the aim of examining the print Print awareness
awareness skills of children attending preschool in terms of various Early literacy
variances. A total of 295 children were selected using the Reading-writing skills
convenience sampling method - 151 of them were male and 144 of Preschool
them were female. Data were obtained using the “Preschool Word
Preschool period
and Print Awareness Assessment Tool”. The data obtained from
the study were analyzed in the SPSS 20.0 package program. As a Article Info
result of the study, it was seen that there was no significant
Received: 12.30.2017
relationship between children’s print awareness skills and their
Accepted: 06.11.2018
gender, father’s occupation and father’s education status, while it
was found that there was a significant relationship between their Online Published: 10.11.2018
print awareness skills and mother’s occupation and education
status. DOI: 10.15390/EB.2018.7679

Introduction
Literacy is a multifaceted, multidimensional and sophisticated process that begins at birth and
continues throughout life (Brand & Donato, 2001; Decker & Decker, 2016; Erdoğan, 2013; Kamei-
Hannan & Ricci, 2015; Morrow, 2007; Whitehead, 2007). Literacy skills have enough power to shape the
children’s future life besides success of children in the school. (Niklas & Schneider, 2013). For this
reason, educators and scientists has been discussing on the topics of how children acquire literacy skills
and how they can acquire these skills better.
The experiences in the preschool period have great importance on the child to be a good literate
and have a positive attitude towards reading and writing (Black, 2013; Compean-Garcia, 2011; Fields,
Groth, & Spangler, 2007; Justice & Sofka, 2010; Piasta, Justice, McGint, & Kaderavek, 2012; Saracho, 2017;
Strickland & Riley-Ayers, 2007; Whitehead, 2007). Skills that children need to acquire in this period in
order to be able to be a good literate in the future are called early literacy skills. Early literacy skills are
the process that starts with the child’s learning native language, develops gradually, and includes the
preliminary knowledge, skills and attitudes that are necessary to become a good literate (Blake, 2014;
Griffith, Beach, Ruan, & Dunn, 2008; Trawick-Smith, 2014; Strickland & Schickedanz, 2009; Üstün, 2007).
Early literacy skills include verbal language, print awareness, alphabet knowledge, phonetic awareness
and writing attempts (Bayraktar & Temel, 2017; Bekir, 2017; Coe, 2009; Griffith et al., 2008; Jalongo, 2013;
Turan, 2017).

* This article is the extended version of the paper titled "Anasınıfına Devam Eden Altı Yaş Grubu Çocukların Yazı Farkındalığı

Becerilerinin Bazı Değişkenlere Göre İncelenmesi" and presented at the "5th International Early Childhood Education Congress".
1 Gazi University, Faculty of Education, Department of Elementary Education, Turkey, [email protected]

49
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Print awareness skills are one of the early literacy skills that children need to acquire in the pre-
school period (Bayraktar & Temel, 2017; Blake, 2014; Clay, 2000; Cabell, Justice, Kaderavek, Trunbull, &
Breit-Smith, 2009; Erdoğan, 2013; Justice & Sofka, 2010; Soderman, Gregory, & McCarty, 2005; Wang,
2015; Vacca, Vacca, Gove, Burkey, Lenhart, & McKean, 2012). Print awareness was first expressed by
Mary Clay in 1960. Print awareness is the child’s ability to recognize symbols and signs (letters, words,
punctuation marks, logos, cautions, warning signs, etc.) and to understand that letters constitute words,
and words constitute sentences; sentences begin with capital letters; a punctuation mark is put at the
end of each sentence; there is a gap between words; and texts are read from left to right and from top to
bottom. Print awareness is also the ability to understand which page is read first, how a book is held,
how pages are turned over, and what the relationship is between the written language and the spoken
language (Bayraktar & Temel, 2017; Blake, 2014; Clay, 2000; Cabell et al., 2009; Erdoğan, 2013; Justice &
Sofka, 2010; Soderman et al., 2005; Wang, 2015; Vacca et al., 2012). One of the early literacy skills that
children need to acquire in the pre-school period is the print awareness skill (Bayraktar & Temel, 2017;
Blake, 2014; Clay, 2000; Cabell et al., 2009; Erdoğan, 2013; Justice & Sofka, 2010; Soderman et al., 2005;
Wang, 2015; Vacca et al., 2012). Print awareness was first expressed by Mary Clay in 1960. Print
awareness is the child’s ability to recognize symbols and signs (letters, words, punctuation marks, logos,
cautions, warning signs, etc.) and to understand that letters constitute words, and words constitute
sentences; sentences begin with capital letters; a punctuation mark is put at the end of each sentence;
there is a gap between words; and texts are read from left to right and from top to bottom. Print
awareness is also the ability to understand which page is read first, how a book is held, how pages are
turned over, and what the relationship is between the written language and the spoken language
(Bayraktar & Temel, 2017; Blake, 2014; Clay, 2000; Cabell et al., 2009; Erdoğan, 2013; Justice & Sofka,
2010; Soderman et al., 2005; Wang, 2015; Vacca et al., 2012).
In the pre-school period, children often are able to notice symbols in their surroundings
(Beauchat, Blamey, & Walpole, 2010; Justice, Pence, Beckman, Skibbe, & Wiggins, 2005; Strickland &
Schickedanz, 2009; Whitehead, 2007; Wortham, 2009). A child’s symbol awareness is a process which
begins with the showing interest on pictures (Decker & Decker, 2016; Wang, 2015; Vukelich, Christie, &
Enz, 2012). A child’s beginning to notice symbols around himself or herself is considered to be
important for print awareness (Beauchat et al., 2010; Saracho, 2017; Strickland & Schickedanz, 2009;
Whitehead, 2007). Research shows that children can recognize texts, signs, logos and symbols in their
environment around the age of three (Trawick-Smith, 2014; Wortham, 2009). Skills in this period should
not be considered as real reading skills. But, it is accepted that learnings are prerequisite for the child’s
learning to read and write. Some children learn some of these skills by themselves, while some children
need adult support. Children should not be left alone to acquire by chance print awareness skills that
are essential for them to become literate. In order for children to be able to learn print awareness skills
in a complete and correct way, they need to be supported by adults in their regular environment,
because they can learn better in the regular environment. Adults’ task here is to make it easier for
children to learn these skills in a rich, stimulating environment (Beauchat et al., 2010; Israel, 2008;
Jalongo, 2013; Justice et al., 2005; Morrison, 2015; Trawick-Smith, 2014; Texas Education Agency [TEA],
2002; Üstün, 2007; Vukelich et al., 2012).
Adults encourage children to acquire print awareness skills and become models for them (Both-
de Vries & Bus, 2014; Decker & Decker, 2016; Justice et al., 2005). Books and other written and printed
materials play an important role in helping children achieve print awareness skills. Certain behaviors
support children’s print awareness skills, including adults’ reading books to children and interacting
about knowledge of written language while sharing books (to know how a book is held and how the
pages are turned, to distinguish the beginning and end of a book, also to distinguish the beginning,
middle and end of a page, and so on.). Such behaviors also include responding to questions about book-
related topics children are interested in and wonder about; introducing different written materials (such
as books, magazines, comics, newspapers, prescriptions, tickets, invitations, food menus and recipes);
preparing a shopping list with children; and taking children to the library or a bookstore (Brand &
Donato, 2001; Jackman, 2012; Rog, 2011; Rvachew, Rees, Carolan, & Nading, 2017; Vukelich et al., 2012;
Zucker & Grant, 2007). Also in the study conducted by Yıldız, Ataş, Aktaş, Yekeler, and Dönmez (2015),
it was found that the 5-year-old children of families with lower socioeconomic levels were in the stage

50
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

of random letter formation, while the children of those with middle and upper socioeconomic levels
were in the writing phase by associating letters with sounds; that is, their writing perception was better.
In summary, the experience of children gained at their homes is important in the development of print
awareness skills (Fields et al., 2007; Justice & Sofka, 2010; Pena, 2009; Sim & Berthelsen, 2014). In this
process, children’s attaining early literacy, developing positive attitudes toward reading, and becoming
good literates depend on their ability to view reading as a fun activity in a rich, stimulating environment
(Blake, 2014; Sawyer, 2010).
What is aimed in the pre-school education is not to teach reading and writing to children. It is
a prerequisite for children to learn to read and write quickly and easily (Beauchat et al., 2010; Ministry
of National Education [MONE], 2013; Saracho, 2017; Strickland & Schickedanz, 2009; Whitehead, 2007).
Children’s literacy experience is directly proportional to language and early literacy skills (Dickinson &
McCabe, 2001; Speece, Ritchey, Cooper, Roth, & Schatschneider, 2004; Powell, 2012). Research shows
that children’s print awareness skills develop if such skills are supported in the preschool period
(Altınkaynak, 2014). Moreover, it has been determined that support for early literacy skills influences
children’s future reading achievement (Bayraktar & Temel, 2014; Cabell et al., 2011; Leopola, Poskiparta,
Laakkonen, & Niemi, 2005; Piasta et al., 2012; Puranik, Phillips, Lonigan, & Gibson, 2018).
Print awareness skills, which are one of early literacy skills, are the basic skills that children
need to acquire in order to be a good literate. It should not be left to chance that children can fully and
correctly acquire print awareness skills. It is also necessary for children to be supported by adults in
order to be able to acquire print awareness skills. Considering how stimuli, attitudes and behaviors
offered by parents to their children affect print awareness skills of children in the family, it is important
to determine how much the family contributes to their children’s print awareness skills and to resolve
identified deficiencies in a timely manner. Moreover, the pre-school education curriculum in Turkey
was updated in 2013 and the content of preparation studies for literacy was expanded for the first time
to include objectives and indicators about print awareness and reading awareness. Therefore, it is seen
that in Turkey, there is a limited number of studies on print awareness. It was aimed to increase the
number of research studies aimed at eliminating these identified needs, to determine the effect of
families on print awareness skills of children and to reveal deficiencies to be identified. In order to
achieve this aim, sub-problems of the study were determined as follows:
1. Is there a significant difference in print awareness skills of preschool children based on
gender?
2. Is there a significant difference in print awareness skills of preschool children based on
professions of their mothers?
3. Is there a significant difference in print awareness skills of preschool children based on
educational status of their mothers?
4. Is there a significant difference in print awareness skills of preschool children based on
professions of their fathers?
5. Is there a significant difference in print awareness skills of preschool children based on
educational status of their fathers?
Limitations of the Study
The study was limited to children aged 48–66 months who continued to a preschool institution
in the provincial center of Ankara in 2015–2016 school year and showed normal development. The
identification of the print awareness skills of the children in the sample group was limited to the
responses received during the administration of the “Preschool Word and Print Awareness Assessment
Tool”.

51
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Method
This is a survey research. The data of the study were obtained from children attending
preschool. The “Preschool Word and Print Awareness Assessment Tool” developed by Clay (1979) and
adapted to Turkish by Bayraktar (2013) was used as the data collection tool in the study.
Research Population and Sample
The research population consisted of 10,707 children aged 48–66 months who were attending
the public and private kindergartens and preschools in Çankaya district of Ankara province in the 2015–
2016 school year. From within the determined population, 371 children were identified in the 95%
confidence interval. Out of the 371 children included in the sampling, 295 were admitted into the study,
after 76 children as outliers were excluded.
The sample of the study consisted of a total of 295 children, 151 males and 144 females, selected
using the convenience sampling method from 16 institutions including 8 preschools in the primary
school, 4 public kindergartens and 4 private schools in Çankaya district center. 51.1% of the children
admitted to the sample were male, and 48.8% were female. 55% of the children were in preschools, 20%
in private kindergartens and 25% in formal kindergartens. Convenience sampling is the sampling
method based on data collection from nearby volunteer subjects that are in close proximity and are easy
to reach when a region is not in question (Yıldırım & Şimşek, 2016).
Data Collection Instrument
Personal Information Form: The form included questions such as child’s age and gender,
mother’s occupation and education status, father’s occupation and education status.
Preschool Word and Print Awareness Assessment Tool: The “Preschool Word and Print
Awareness Assessment Tool” developed by Clay (1979) and adapted to Turkish by Justice and Ezel
(2001) was re-adapted to Turkish, and validity and reliability studies were conducted by Bayraktar
(2013). The scale consisted of two sub-dimensions, print concepts (items such as “Show me the front of
the book” and “Show me the name of the book”) and word recognition (items such as “Show me a word
on this page”, “Show me the small words written on this page”). There were 14 items in the print
awareness sub-dimension and 12 items in the word recognition sub-dimension. The test retest reliability
level of the print concepts sub-dimension was 0.93, and its KR20 was 0.74; the test retest reliability level
of the word recognition sub-dimension was 0.92, and its KR20 was 0.71. The stories “Nine Ducks Nine”
by Hayes and “Spot Bakes A Cake” by Hill were used for the “Print Concepts” and “Word Recognition”
sub-tests, respectively. The story books were translated into Turkish by two experts who studied
English Language Literature and were examined by three experts of the field. After the necessary
corrections were made, these Turkish forms were used as books in the measurement process. In the
story book titled “Nine Ducks Nine,” the story of the adventure between nine ducks and a fox was told.
In the story titled “Spot Bakes a Cake,” the story of how Spot bakes a cake with his mother was told. In
the implementation of the scale, the child was asked various questions on almost every page of the story
as the story was read, and the story continued to be read. The child received 1 point for correct answers
and 0 points for incorrect answers. On the 4th, 5th, and 12th items of the Print Concepts sub-test, the
child could possibly get 0, 1 or 2 points according to the answers he or she gave. The highest score the
child could get from the Print Concepts sub-test was a total of 16 points; the highest score that could be
obtained from the Word Recognition sub-test was a total of 12 points.
Data Collection Process
The study was planned on the basis of volunteerism. The study was carried out in the 2015–
2016 school year in a total of 16 institutions consisting of 8 preschools, 4 pubic kindergartens and 4
private schools located in Çankaya district of Ankara province. Information on the purpose of the
research was given to the teachers and families of the sampled children. The scale was individually
administered to the children by the researcher himself. During the application, two story books were
used to ask children various questions about print awareness skills, and the answers given by children
were scored. It took approximately 10–20 minutes to administer the scale. The data were collected
between March 1, 2016 and May 30, 2016. The results of the study were shared as a report with both the
teachers and the parents.

52
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Data Analysis
Data were evaluated using the “independent-samples t test,” which is a parametric test, when
the distributions were normal, and using the “Mann-Whitney U test” and the “Kruskal-Wallis H test,”
which are nonparametric tests, when the distributions were not normal. In the Kruskal-Wallis H test,
when there was a significant difference between the groups, the Mann-Whitney U Test was used to
investigate which group or groups differed.

Results
Independent samples t test, Mann-Whitney U test and Kruskal-Wallis H test were used to
evaluate the results of the test that was implemented to determine whether print awareness skills
acquired by the children differed according to the variables including gender, mother’s occupation and
education status, and father’s occupation and education status.

Table 1. The t Test Result for the Children’s Print Concepts Sub-dimension by Gender
Test Groups N �
𝑿𝑿 Std. Deviation sd t p
Male 151 8.8146 3,68131 292 -,881 ,379
Print Concepts
Female 143 9.1818 3,45726

When Table 1 was examined, it was seen that the mean scores that the children obtained from
the print concepts sub-dimension (t = -.881, p=.379˃05) did not show any significant difference according
to the gender of the children.

Table 2. The Mann-Whitney U Test Result for the Children’s Word Recognition Sub-dimension by
Gender
Gender N Mean Rank Total Rank U p
Word Male 151 142,91 21579,50
10103,500 ,338
Recognition Female 143 152,35 21785,50

When Table 2 was examined, it was seen that the mean scores-related mean ranks that the
children obtained from the word recognition sub-dimension (t=10103.500, p=.338˃05) did not show any
significant difference according to the gender of the children. The results of both Table 1 and Table 2
show that the children’s gender was not a significant variable in the print awareness skills in the pre-
school period.

Table 3. The Kruskal-Wallis H Results based on Occupation of Children’s Mothers


Mean Mann-Whitney
Mother’s Occupation N H (sd=3) P
Rank U Test
housewife 131 105,11
3>1, 4>1,
worker 14 85,75 5>1, 2<1
Print
retailer 18 161,53 17,684 ,001*
Concepts
public servant 19 139,66 3>2, 4>2,
5>2, 1>2
professional occupation 44 116,35
housewife 131 101,43
worker 14 90,00
Word 4>1, 5>1,
retailer 18 130,22 17,992 ,001*
Recognition 3>1, 2<1
public servant 19 147,82
professional occupation 44 135,26
*p<.05 Criteria: Housewife=1, worker=2, retailer=3, public servant=4, professional occupation=5

53
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

When Table 3 was examined, it was seen that there was a significant difference between the
mean scores-related mean ranks of the print concepts sub-dimension based on mother’s occupation
according to H=17.684, p=.001<.05. This significant difference was due to the fact that the print
awareness-related mean ranks of retailer mothers (161.53), public servant mothers (139.66) and mothers
with professional occupation (116.35) were greater than that of housewife mothers (105.11). It was also
due to the fact that the print awareness-related mean rank of housewife mothers (105.11) was greater
than that of worker mothers (85.75).
It was seen that there was a significant difference between the mean scores-related mean ranks
of the word recognition sub-dimension based on mother’s occupation according to H=17.792,
p=.000<.05. This significant difference was due to the fact that the print awareness-related mean rank of
public servant mothers (147.82), the word recognition-related mean ranks of retailer mothers (130.22)
and mothers with professional occupation (135.26) were greater than the word recognition -related
mean rank of housewife mothers (101.43). It was also due to the fact that the word recognition -related
mean rank of housewife mothers (101.43) was greater than that of worker mothers (90.00). These
findings show that the occupation status of children’s mothers was a significant variable in the pre-
school print awareness skills.

Table 4. The Kruskal-Wallis H Results based on Education Status of Children’s Mothers


Mann-
Mother’s Education Mean
N H (sd=3) P Whitney U
Status Rank
Test
primary school 80 98,64
Print middle school 62 120,58
18,161 ,000* 4˃1, 3˃1, 2˃1
Concepts high school 92 140,44
university or higher 68 141,46
primary school 80 104,04
Word middle school 12 92,63
16,966 ,001* 4˃1, 3˃1, 2˃1
Recognition high school 92 135,70
university or higher 68 146,46
*p<.05 Criteria: Primary school=1, middle school=2, high school= 3, university or higher=4

When Table 4 was examined, it was seen that there was a significant difference between the
mean scores-related mean ranks of the print concepts sub-dimension based on mother’s education
status according to H=18.161, p=.000<.05. This significant difference was due to the fact that the print
awareness-related mean ranks of mothers graduating from university or higher (141.46), mothers
graduating from high school (140.44) and mothers graduating from middle school (120.58) was greater
than that of mothers graduating from primary school (98.64).
It was seen that there was a significant difference between the mean scores-related mean ranks
of the word recognition sub-dimension based on mother’s education status according to H=16.966,
p=.001<.05. This significant difference was due to the fact that the word recognition-related mean ranks
of mothers graduating from university or higher (146.46) and mothers graduating from high school
(135.70) were greater than that of mothers graduating from primary school (104.04). It was also due to
the fact that the word recognition-related mean rank of mothers graduating from middle school (92.63)
was smaller than that of mothers graduating from primary school (98.64). These findings show that the
education status of children’s mothers was a significant variable in the pre-school print awareness skills.

54
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Table 5. The Kruskal-Wallis H Results based on Occupation of Children’s Fathers


Father’s Education Status N Mean Rank H (sd=3) P
worker 90 116,06
retailer 57 128,48
Print Concepts 2,244 ,523
public servant 42 131,74
professional occupation 60 130,38
worker 90 115,63
retailer 57 117,17
Word Recognition 5,412 ,144
public servant 42 137,74
professional occupation 60 137,58
*p<.05 Criteria: Worker=1, Retailer=2, Public Servant=3, and Professional Occupation=4

When Table 5 was examined, it was seen that there was no significant difference between the
mean scores-related mean ranks of the print awareness based on father’s occupation status according
to H=2.244, p=.523<.05. It was seen that there was no significant difference between the mean scores-
related mean ranks of the word recognition sub-dimension based on father’s occupation status
according to H=5.412, p=.144<.05. These findings show that the occupation status of children’s fathers
was not a significant variable in the pre-school print awareness skills.

Table 6. The Kruskal-Wallis H Results based on Education Status of Children’s Fathers


Mann-
Father’s Education Mean
N H (sd=3) P Whitney U
Status Rank
Test
primary school 43 98,92
middle school 25 100,48
Print Concepts 11,210 ,011 4˃1
high school 99 132,17
university or higher 80 134,73
primary school 43 95,94
middle school 25 111,76
Word Recognition 10,344 ,016 4˃1
high school 99 129,64
university or higher 80 135,93
*p<.05 Criteria: Primary school=1, middle school=2, high school= 3, university or higher=4

When Table 6 was examined, it was seen that there was a significant difference between the
mean scores-related mean ranks of the print concepts sub-dimension based on father’s education status
according to H=11.210, p=.011<.05. This significant difference was due to the fact that the print
awareness-related mean ranks of fathers graduating from university and higher (134.73) was greater
than that of fathers graduating from primary school (98.92).
It was seen that there was a significant difference between the mean scores-related mean ranks
of the word recognition sub-dimension based on father’s education status according to H=10.344,
p=.016<.05’e. This significant difference was due to the fact that the print awareness-related mean ranks
of fathers graduating from university and higher (135,93) was greater than that of fathers graduating
from primary school (95,94). These findings show that the education status of children’s fathers was a
significant variable in the pre-school print awareness skills.

55
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Discussion, Conclusion and Suggestions


In this study, the print awareness skills of 48 months and 66 months old children attending
preschool were examined and compared in terms of different variables. According to the findings
obtained from the study, while there was a statistically significant difference in the children’s print
awareness skills based on the variables of mother’s occupation and education status, there was no
statistically significant difference according to the variables of children’s gender, father’s occupation
and education status.
The experiences a child gains in the preschool period have great importance for the child to
become a good literate and form a positive attitude towards reading and writing (Black, 2013; Compean-
Garcia, 2011; Justice & Sofka, 2010; Piasta et al., 2012; Saracho, 2017). What is aimed in pre-school
education is not to teach children reading and writing. The aim is to support early literacy skills so that
children can learn to read easily. One of the early literacy skills is the print awareness skills (Griffith et
al., 2008; Justice et al., 2005; Machado, 2003; Soderman et al., 2005). According to Machado (2003),
children’s acquiring print awareness skills is the basis for reading. These skills evolve progressively
until the actual reading takes place. In print awareness, the first stage is the child’s understanding of the
writing symbols. Research shows that children can recognize texts around them (writings on packaging
of food that they like, the texts and logos of cartoons on television, etc.) beginning from the age of three
(Justice et al., 2005; Wortham, 2009).
Adults encourage children to acquire print awareness skills and become models for them (Both-
de Vries & Bus, 2014; Decker & Decker, 2016). In a study by Byrne et al. (2006), it has been found that
the child’s interaction with his surroundings is important in the development of the print awareness
skill. Children’s print awareness skills begin to develop before they start kindergarten, and they learn
better in natural settings, revealing the importance of parents. Kuby and Aldridge have found in their
studies conducted in different years (1991, 2006) that children’s print awareness skills can be improved
better through play in the natural settings. The task of the adult here is to provide opportunities for
children to learn these skills in a rich, stimulating environment (Jalongo, 2013; Justice et al., 2005;
Morrison, 2015; Trawick-Smith, 2014; Üstün, 2007; Vukelich et al., 2012). It is seen in studies that these
skills are improved in children who are supported for their print awareness skills in the preschool
period (Altınkaynak & Akman, 2016; Bayraktar, 2013; Compton-Lilly, 2012; Goodrich, Lonigan, &
Farver, 2017; Ihmeideh, 2014; Mckenzie, 2015; Neumann, 2014; Padlick-Field, 2011; Puranik et al., 2018;
Rvachew et al., 2017; Wesseling, Christmann, & Lancmann, 2017). The results of these earlier studies
can be said to support the results obtained in this study.
According to the findings of the study, the print awareness skills the children acquired did not
differ according to the gender of the children. Research findings of Harper and Pelletier (2008), Gürocak
(2007), Karaman (2006), Kelman (2007), Renee Harell (2003), Matthews, Ponitz, and Morrison (2009),
Tafa (2009) and Yıldırım (2008) also show that early literacy skills of children do not differ according to
the gender of children. The research findings are consistent with the findings of this study, which
showed that the print awareness skills of the children did not differ according to the gender of the
children. Unlike these studies, there are also studies showing that girls are more successful than boys in
print awareness skills according to the gender variable. Research conducted by Karaman (2013), Lee
and Otaiba (2015), Lundberg, Larsman, and Strid (2012), Marjanovic-Umek and Fekonaja-Peklaj (2017);
Moss and Washbrook (2016); Niklas and Schneider (2013), Sigmundsson, Eriksen, Ofteland, and Haga
(2017) found significant differences between the variables of early literacy skills and gender of children,
in favor of girls. For early literacy skills, children need adult support (Jalongo, 2013; Justice et al., 2005;
Morrison, 2015; Trawick-Smith, 2014; Üstün, 2007; Vukelich et al., 2012). What is important here is the
interference that parents apply. These interventions constitute the communication that parents establish
with their children — that is, how much they talk to their children — and the stimuli they offer to them.
Dodici, Draper, and Petersom (2003), Gustafsson, Hansen, and Rosen (2011), Huttenlocher, Height,
Bryk, Seltzer, and Lyons (1991), Karacan (2000) found that these types of activities were found to be
carried out more commonly with girls than with boys. This is thought to lead girls to develop language

56
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

skills and early literacy skills earlier than boys. In the study of Johnson (2008); Leslie (2012); Prendiville
and Toye (2007) and Warrington et al., (2006), it was found that boys’ language skills developed when
they were supported through activities (drama, role playing, improvisation, etc.). In another study by
Byrne et al. (2006), it was found that the environment was more effective in the development of print
awareness skills than genetic factors. The findings of this study together with the research findings
mentioned above, show us that the gender variable is not important in the development of children’s
print awareness skills; what is important is how well the child’s early literacy skills are supported.
There was a significant difference in the sub-dimensions of both the print concepts and the word
recognition according to the mother’s occupation. This difference was in favor of the retailer mothers,
mothers with professional occupation and mothers who were public servants. The mothers who were
retailers, who had professional occupations and who were public servants were considered to have
higher socioeconomic levels than the mothers who were workers and housewives, and were thought to
offer richer stimuli to their children. Parents’ education levels and occupations are considered as criteria
for the socio-economic level (Alexsander, 2016; Berk & Meyers, 2015, Kalaycıoğlu, Çelik, Çelen, &
Türkyılmaz, 2010). Research conducted by Arnold and Doctoroff (2003), Ayaz (2015), Hartas (2011),
Husain, Choo, and Singh (2011), Mckenzie (2015) and Slavan (2011) found that as families’
socioeconomic status increased, they read more books to their children, communicated more verbally,
encouraged children in other ways, and provided children with more freedom and stimuli to explore
the environment; it was also seen that the success of such children rose in a direct proportion. It has
been determined that children of families with low socioeconomic status have low literacy and language
experiences, which causes such children to have low early literacy skills (Foster & Miller, 2007; Wang,
2015). Gürocak (2007) and Karaman (2006) also found significant differences between the variables of
children’s achievement and their mother’s occupation. It can be said that research supports the finding
of this study that children differ in terms of their print awareness skills based on their mother’s
occupation.
Another finding of the study was that there was a significant difference in the sub-dimensions
of both the print concepts and the word recognition according to the mother’s education status. This
difference favored the mothers who graduated from university or higher, high school, and middle
school. As the learning level of the mothers increased, their children’s scores also increased in direct
proportion. This is thought to be due to the nature of the mothers’ attitude towards reading and writing
and the time they spent with their children. That is to say, it is thought that as the education level of the
mothers increased, they supported and encouraged their children more in ways such as communicating
more with their children, reading those books more frequently, giving them the opportunity to express
themselves or providing more stimulating materials. It is also stated in the literature that children’s
attitudes and achievements towards literacy are related to how their families approach to reading and
writing and to the materials they present to their children (Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Powell, 2012;
Schick, 2014; Speece et al., 2004). Research by Ersoy and Bayraktar (2015); Beyazova (2006); Hartas (2011)
and Saban and Altınkamış (2014) has also shown that as the education level of mothers increases, the
frequency of reading books to their children increases. There are many studies that have found that
reading books to children supports children’s print awareness skills (Altınkaynak, 2014; Bayraktar,
2013; Bayraktar & Temel, 2014; Horner, 2004; Lovelace & Steward, 2007; Lefebvre, Trudeau, & Sutton,
2011; McGinity, Smith, Xitao, Justice, & Kaderavek, 2011; Swain, Brooks, & Bosley, 2014; Weigel, Martin,
& Bennett, 2006).When the research on how education status of mothers affects children’s early literacy
skills was investigated, it was found that education status of mothers supports children’s print
awareness skills positively (Altıparmak, 2010; Curenton & Justice, 2008; Gürocak, 2007; Karaman, 2013;
Kelman, 2007; Lynch, Anderson, Anderson, & Shapiro, 2007, 2008; PISA, 2009; Taylor, Greenberg, &
Teryy, 2016; Yıldırım, 2008). Research shows consistency with the finding of this study, which revealed
that the print awareness skills acquired by the children differed based on the mother’s education status
— there was a difference in favor of the mothers graduating from university or higher compared to the
mothers graduating from primary school, middle school and high school.

57
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Considering the finding of the study related to the father variable, there was no significant
difference in the children’s print concepts and word recognition sub-dimensions in terms of occupations
of their fathers. This is thought to be related to the role of fathers in the society. In the literature, it is
stated that fathers who have a traditional father role spend less time with their children and undertake
less child care (Çatıkkaş, 2008; Feldman, Nash, & Aschenbrenner, 1983). It is stated in the literature that
the nature and content of the the time fathers spend with their children are much more important than
the span of the time (Department for Children, Schools and Families [DCSF], 2008). Moreover, studies
by Ersoy and Bayraktar (2015) and Ayaz (2015) have shown that fathers read books to children at a
lower rate than their mothers. Those studies appear to support the research finding of this study, which
showed that there was no significant difference between the print awareness skills of children and the
father’s occupation.
Considering another finding of the study related to the father variable, there was a significant
difference in the children’s print concepts and word recognition sub-dimensions in terms of educational
status of their fathers. This differentiation favored the fathers who graduated from a university and
higher institution. As the learning level of the fathers increased, their children’s scores also increased in
direct proportion. This is thought to be related to the fathers’ points of views about reading and writing
and to their spending quality time together with their children. It was also seen in studies done by
Altıparmak (2010), Gürocak (2007) and Karaman (2006) that there was a significant difference in print
awareness skills in terms of the father’s educational status variable. This appears to support the research
finding that there was a significant difference between the print awareness skills of children in terms of
their father’s educational status.
In conclusion, it was seen that while there was no significant difference in print awareness skills
of children in terms of gender and father’s occupation variables, there was a significant difference in
terms of the variables of mother’s occupation, mother’s educational level and father’s educational level.
According to the findings obtained from the study, it can be said that the variables of mother’s
educational level, mother’s occupation and father’s educational level are influential on children’s print
awareness skills.
Suggestions
Investigations can be done to determine the reasons for the difference between the children’s
print awareness skills based on the mother’s education status. Based on the needs of families, teachers
can organize family education activities on awareness about and the importance of early literacy skills,
and how the families can support their children in their regular environment. Teachers can encourage
families with low socioeconomic status to have their children benefit from environments such as
libraries, which are rich in terms of quality books and where children are served by expert staff. Family
education events can be organized in Public Education Centers to raise awareness of and develop
knowledge, skills and attitudes of families with low socioeconomic status pertaining to early literacy.
Research can be conducted to determine the reasons why the father’s education status and occupation
did not have any effect on the child’s print awareness skills. Family education activities can be organized
in Public Education Centers to raise awareness of families about the importance of father’s participation
in his child’s education. Moreover, universities, Ministry of National Education and non-governmental
organizations can develop projects, with a multidisciplinary approach, for fathers’ participation in the
education of their children.

58
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

References
Alexsander, S. J. (2016). Sociology (1st ed.). New York: Rosen Publishing.
Altınkaynak, Ş. Ö., & Akman, B. (2016). Aile temelli okuma yazmaya hazırlık programının çocukların
okuma yazmaya hazırlık becerilerine etkisi. Eğitim ve Bilim, 41(186), 185-204.
doi:10.15390/EB.2016.6711
Altınkaynak, Ş.Ö. (2014). Aile temelli okuma yazmaya hazırlık programının çocukların okuma yazmaya hazırlık
becerilerine etkisi (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Hacettepe University, Institute of
Educational Sciences, Ankara.
Altıparmak, S. (2010). Erken çocukluk döneminde ebeveynlerin okuma-yazmaya hazırlık konusundaki görüşleri
(Unpublised master’s thesis). Middle East Technical University, Institute of Social Sciences,
Ankara.
Arnold, D. H. & Doctoroff, G. L. (2003). The early education of socioeconomically disadvantaged
children. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 517-545. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.111301.145442
Ayaz, C.Ö. (2015). Ailelerin, okul öncesi dönemdeki çocuklarının okuryazarlık becerilerini desteklemek için
kullandıkları okuryazarlık uygulamalarının incelenmesi: Tekirdağ ili örneği (Unpublised master’s thesis).
Çanakkale On sekiz Mart Üniversitesi, Çanakkale.
Bayraktar, V. (2013). Okuma yazmaya hazırlık eğitim programının anasınıfına devam eden 6 yaş grubu
çocukların yazı farkındalığı becerilerine ve ilkokul birinci sınıftaki ses farkındalığı ve okuma yazma
becerilerine etkisinin incelenmesi (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Gazi University, Institute of
Educational Sciences, Ankara.
Bayraktar, V., & Temel, F. (2017). Yazı farkındalığı becerileri. In F. Temel (Ed.), Dil ve erken okuryazarlık
(pp. 63-88). Ankara: Hedef CS.
Bayraktar, V., & Temel, F. (2014). Okuma-yazmaya hazırlık eğitim programının çocukların okuma
yazma becerisine etkisi. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 29(3), 08-22.
Beauchat, K. A., Blamey, K. L., & Walpole, S. (2010). The building blocks of preschool success. New York:
The Guilford Press.
Bekir, H. (2017). İki dilli çocuklarda erken okuryazarlık yazı farkındalığı becerileri. In F. Temel (Ed.),
Dil ve erken okuryazarlık (pp. 187-214). Ankara: Hedef CS.
Berk, L. E., & Meyers, A. B. (2015). Infants and children: Prenatal through middle childhood (8th ed.). London:
Pearson
Beyazova, U. (2006). Kitap çocuk ilişkisi. In II. Ulusal Çocuk ve Gençlik Edebiyatı Sempozyumu (pp. 533-
553). Ankara.
Black, S. (2013). Learning what print means: Print awareness in school, home, and community. B.
Culatta, K. Hall-Kenyon, & S. Black (Eds.), Systematic and engaging early literacy. San Diego: Plural
Publishing, Inc.
Blake, C. (2014). Defining emergent literacy: Developing lifelong readers. Retrieved from
https://online.cune.edu/defining emergent-literacy/.
Both-de Vries, A. C., & Bus, A. G. (2014). Visual processing of pictures and letters in alphabet books and
the implications for letter learning. Contemp. Educ. Psychol., 39(2), 156-163. doi:
10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.03.005
Brand, S. T., & Donato, J. M. (2001). Storytelling in emergent literacy: Fostering multiple intelligences. Albany,
NY: Delmar-Thomson.
Byrne, B., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., & Willcutt, E. (2006).
Genetic and environmental ınfluences on early literacy. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(1), 33-49.
doi.10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00291.x
Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Trunbull, K. P., & Breit-Smith, A. (2009). Emergent literacy.
San Diego: Plural Publishing, Inc.

59
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Konold, T. R., & McGinty, A. S. (2011). Profiles of Emergent literacy skills
among preschool children who are at risk for academic difficulties. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 26(1), 1-14. Retrieved from 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.05.003.
Çatıkkaş, K. T. (2008). Okul Öncesi eğitime babaların katılım düzeyleri ile ilgili değişkenlerin incelenmesi
(Unpublised master’s thesis). Yeditepe University, Institute of Social Sciences, İstanbul.
Clay, M. M. (1979). The early detection of reading difficulties: A diagnostic survey with recovery procedures (2st
ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
Clay, M. M. (2000). Concepts about print. Singapore: Heinemann
Coe, G. (2009). Tools of the mind-literacy activities for young children. In Uluslararası Çocuk, Aile ve Okul
Bağlamında Okul Öncesi Eğitim Kongresi (pp. 117 121). Trabzon: Trabzon Valiliği Kültür Yayınları.
Compean-Garcia, N. (2011). Early literacy experiences in the home: A parent’s perspective. Journal of
Border Educational Research, 9, 23-36.
Compton-Lilly, C. (2012). Reading time: the literate lives of urban secondary students and their families. New
York, NY: Teacher College Press.
Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L. M. (2008). Children’s preliteracy skills: ınfluence of mothers’ education
and beliefs about shared-reading interactions, Early Education and Development, 19(2), 261 283.
Decker, C. A., & Decker, J. R. (2016). Planning and administering early childhood programs (11st ed.). New
Jersey: Pearson Education, Inc.
Department for Children, Schools and Families. (2008). The impact of parental involvement on children’s
education.
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: the multiple orgins, skills, and
environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(4), 186 202.
Retrieved from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/0938-8982.00019/epdf
Dodici, B. J., Draper, D. C., & Petersom, C. A. (2003). Early parent-child interactions and early literacy
development. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23(3), 124-136. Retrieved from
http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/02711214030230030301
Erdoğan, T. (2013). Okul öncesi dönemde okuma yazmaya hazırlık. In T. Erdoğan (Ed.), İlkokula
(ilköğretim) programları (pp. 109-131).Ankara: Eğiten Kitap
Ersoy, Ö., & Bayraktar, V. (2015). Annelerin öğrenim durumlarına göre çocuklarını kitapla buluşturma
konusundaki durumlarının incelenmesi (Çalışma Kapsamı.57 İl). İlköğretim Online, 14 (4), 1406
1415. doi.org/10.17051/io.2015.42378
Feldman, S. S., Nash, S. C., & Aschenbrenner, B. C. (1983). Antecedents of fathering. Child Development,
54(6), 1628-1636. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1129826.pdf
Fields, M. V., Groth, L. A., & Spangler, K. L. (2007). Let’s begin reading right: A developmental approach to
emergent literacy. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc.
Foster, W. A., & Miller, M. (2007). Development of the literacy achievement gap: A longitudinal study
of kindergarten through third grade. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 173-181.
doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2007/018)
Goodrich, J. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Farver, J. A. M. (2017). Impacts of literacy-focused preschool
curriculum on the early literay skills of language-minority children. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 40(3), 13-14. doi:10.1016/J.ecresq.2017.02.001
Griffith, P. L., Beach, S. A., Ruan, J., & Dunn, L. (2008). Literacy for young children. London: Corwn Press.
Gürocak, S. Ü. (2007). Anasınıfına devam eden 60-72 ay çocukların dil gelişimi ve ince motor gelişimi açısından
değerlendirilmesi (Unpublised master’s thesis). Abant İzzet Baysal University, Institute of Social
Sciences, Bolu.

60
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Gustafsson, J. E., Hansen, K. Y., & Rosen, M. (2011). Effects of background on student achievement in
reading, mathematics, and science at the fourth grade. Timss and Pirls International Study Center
2011 report. Retrieved from https://timssandpirls.bc.edu/timsspirls2011/downloads/TP11_
Chapter_4.pdf
Harper, S. N., & Pelletier, J. P. (2008). Gender and language ıssues in assessing early literacy: Group
differences in children’s performance on the test of early reading ability. Journal of
Psychoeducational, 26(2), 185-194. Retrieved from
http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0734282908314105
Hartas, D. (2011). Families’ social backgrounds matter: Socio-economic factors, home learning and
young children’s language, literacy and social outcomes. British Educational Research Journal, 37(6),
893-914. doi:10.1080/01411926.2010.506945
Horner, S. L. (2004). Observational learning during shared book reading: The effects on preschoolers'
attention to print and letter knowledge. Reading Psychology, 25, 167-188.
Husain, F. M., Choo, J. C. S., & Singh, M. K. M. (2011). Malaysian mothers’ beliefs in developing
emergent literacy through reading. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 846-855. Retrieved
from https://ac.els cdn.com/S1877042811027741/1-s2.0-S1877042811027741 main.pdf?
Huttenlocher, H. W., Height, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, J. (1991). Early vocabulary growth:
Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236-248.
doi:10.1037/00121649.27.2.236
Ihmeideh, F. M. (2014). The effect of electronic books on enhancing emergent literacy skills of pre-school
children. Computers & Education, 79, 40-48. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.07.008.
Israel, S. E. (2008). Early reading first and beyond. London: Corwin Press.
Jackman, H. L. (2012). Early education curriculum: A child’s connection to the world (5th ed.).United States
of America: Thomson Delmar.
Jalongo, M. R. (2013). Early childhood language arts (6th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
Johnson, D.P. (2008). Contemporary sociology theory: An integrated multi-level approach. USA: Springer.
Justice L. M. & Ezell, K. E. (2001). Word and print awareness’ in 4- year- old children. Child Language
Teaching and Therapy, 13, 207- 225.
Justice, L. M., & Sofka, A. E. (2010). Engaging children with print: building early literacy skills through quality
read-alouds. New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.
Justice, L. M., Pence, K. L., Beckman, A. R., Skibbe, L. E., & Wiggins, A. K. (2005). Scaffolding with
stırybooks: A guide for enhancing young children’s language and literacy achievement. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Kalaycıoğlu, S., Çelik, K., Çelen, Ü., & Türkyılmaz, S. (2010). Temsili bir örneklemde sosyo-ekonomik
statü (SES) ölçüm aracı geliştirilmesi: Ankara kent merkezi örneği. Journal of Sociological Research,
13(1), 182-220.
Kamei-Hannan, C., & Ricci, L. A. (2015). Reading connections: strategies for teaching students with visual
impairments. New York: Pearson Education, Inc.
Karacan, E. (2000). Çocuklarda Dil Gelişimini Etkileyen Faktörler. Sürekli Tıp Eğitimi Dergisi, 9(7).
Retrieved from http://www.ttb.org.tr/STED/sted0700/6.html
Karaman, G. (2006). Anasınıfına devam eden farklı sosyo-kültürel seviyedeki çocukların fonolojik
duyarlılıklarının incelenmesi (Unpublised master’s thesis). Hacettepe University, Institute of Social
Sciences, Ankara.
Karaman, G. (2013). Erken okuryazarlık becerilerini değerlendirme aracı’nın geliştirilmesi, geçerlik ve güvenirlik
çalışması (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Gazi University, Institute of Educational Sciences,
Ankara.

61
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Kelman, M. E. (2007). An investigation of preschool children's primary literacy skills. Dissertation


Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 68(5-B), 3004.
Kuby, P., & Aldridge, J. (1991). The impact of environmental print instruction on early reading ability.
Journal of Instructional Psychology, 31(2), 106-114. Retrieved from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ774068
Kuby, P., & Aldridge, J. (2006). Direct versus indirect environmental print instruction and on early
reading ability in kindergarten children. Reading Psychology, 18(2), 91-104.
doi:10.1080/02271970180201
Lee, J. A. C., & Otaiba, S. A. (2015). Socioeconomic and gender group differences in early literacy skills:
A multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis approach. US NAtionl Library of Medicine Nationa
Institudes of Health, 21(1), 40-59. doi:10.1080/13803611.2015.1010545
Lefebvre, P., Trudeau, N., & Sutton, A. (2011). Enhancing vocabulary, print awareness and phonological
awareness through shared storybook reading with low-income preschoolers. Journal of Early
Childhood Literacy, 11(4), 453-479. doi: 10.1177/1468798411416581
Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and Relationship Between
Phonological and Motivational Processes and Naming Speed in Predicting Word Recognition in
Grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(4), 367–399. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0904_3
Leslie, J. (2012). The gender agenda-boys and literac y in the early years. BA childood Practice. Retrieved
from
http://workforcesolutions.sssc.uk.com/new/docs/research/The_Gender_Agenda_Boys_and_Litera
cy_in_the_Early_Years.pdf
Lovelace, S. & Stewart, S. R. (2007). Increasing print awareness in preschoolers with language
impairment using non-evocative print referencing. American Speech Language-Hearing Association.
38, 16-30.
Lundberg, I., Larsman, P., & Strid, A. (2012). Development of phonological awareness during the
preschool year: The influence of gender and socioeconomic status. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 305-
320. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9269-4
Lynch, J., Anderson, J., Anderson, A., & Shapiro, J. (2007). Parents’ beliefs about young children’s
literacy development and parents’ literacy behaviors. Reading Psyhology, 27(1), 1-20.
doi:10.1080/02702710500468708
Lynch, J., Anderson, J., Anderson, A., & Shapiro, J. (2008). Parents and preschool children interacting
with storybook: Children’s early literacy achievement. Reading Horizons, 48(4), 227-242. Retrieved
from http://scholarworks.wmich.edu/reading_horizons.
Machado, J. M. (2003). Early childhood experience in language arts emerging literacy. Canada: Delmar
Learning.
Marjanovic-Umek, L., & Fekonaja-Peklaj, U. (2017). Gender differences in children’s language: A
metaanalysis of slovenian studies. C.E.P.S. Journal, 7(2), 97-111. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1145869.pdf
Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender differences in self-regulation and
academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 689-704. doi: 10.1037/a0014240.
McGinity, A. S., Smith, A. B., Xitao, F., Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2011). Does intensity matter?
Preschoolers’ print knowledge development with in a classroom-based intervention. Early
Childhood Research Quartely, 26(3), 255-267. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.02.002
Mckenzie, S. (2015). Socioeconomic factors that affect children’s literacy experiences. Education and
Human Development Master's Theses, 550. Retrieved from
http://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/ehd_theses/550
Ministry of National Education. (2013). Okul öncesi eğitim programı. Retrieved from
http://tegm.meb.gov.tr/www/okul oncesi-egitim programi-ve-kurul-karari/icerik/54
Morrison, G. S. (2015). Early childhood education today (13rd ed.). New Jersey: Pearson Education, Inc.

62
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Morrow, L. M. (2007). Developing literacy in preschool. New York. London: The Guilforf Press.
Moss, G., & Washbrook, L. (2016). Understanding the gender gap in literacy and language development.
Bristol Working Papers in Education Series. Retrieved from
https://www.bristol.ac.uk/education/research/publications
Neumann, M. M. (2014). Using environmental print to foster litearcy in children from a Low-SES
community. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(3), 310-318 doi: 10.1016/J.ecresq.2014.03.005
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Home literacy environment and the beginning of reading and
spelling. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(1), 40-50. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.10.001
Padlick-Field, K. (2011). The Impact of the Environment on Children’s Attitudues toward Literacy. M.S.
Literacy Education, School of Arts Sciences St. John Fisher College. Retrieved from
http://reflectivepractitioner.pbworks.com/f/Padlick-FieldCapstone-Final.pdf
Pena, A. C. (2009). Effects of family literacy ınterventions on children’s acquisition of reading. New York:
Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Piasta, S. B., Justice, L. M., McGint, A. S., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2012). Increasing young children’s contact
with print during shared reading: Longitudinal effects on literacy achievement. Child Development,
83(3), 810-820. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012. 01754. X
PISA. (2009). Uluslararası Öğrenci Değerlendirme Programı, Ulusal Ön Raporu. Milli Eğitim Bakanlığı
Eğitimi Araştırma ve Geliştirme Dairesi Başkanlığı. Retrieved from
http://pisa.meb.gov.tr/wpcontent/uploads/2013/07/PISA-2009 Ulusal-On Rapor.pdf
Powell, D. R. (2012). Parenting education in family literacy programs. B. H. Wasik (Ed.), Handbook of
family literacy programs (2nd ed.) New York: Routledge.
Prendiville, F. & Toye, N. (2007). Speaking and listening through drama. London: Paul Chapman
Publishing.
Puranik, C. S., Phillips, B. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Gibson, E. (2018). Home literacy practices and preschool
children’s emergent writing skills: An inital investigation. Early Childhood Resh Quarterly, 40(1), 228-
238. doi:10.1016/J.ecresq.2017.10.004earc
Renee Harrell, E. (2003). Sex differences in preschool children’s phonological awareness (Unpublished
undergratuate thesis). Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Rog, L. J. (2011). Read, write, play, and learn: Literacy ınstruction in today’s kindergarten. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Rvachew, S., Rees, K., Carolan, E., & Nading, A. (2017). Improving emergent literacy with achool-based
shared reading: Paper versus ebooks. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 12, 24-29.
doi:10.1016/j.iJcci.2017.01.002
Saban, A. İ., & Altınkamış, N. F. (2014). Okul öncesi çağda çocuğu olan ebeveynlerin okuma inançlarının
incelenmesi. Journal of Human Sciences, 11(1), 317-337. doi:10.14687/ijhs.v11i1.2761
Saracho, O. N. (2017). Literacy and language: New developments in research, theory, and practice. Early
Child Development and Care, 187(3-4), 299-304. doi:10.1080/03004430.2017.1282235
Sawyer, A. (2010). The Home Environment and Its Impact on Literacy Development. M.S. Literacy
Education. School of Arts Sciences St, John Fisher College. Retrieved from
http://reflectivepractitioner.pbworks.com/f/SawyerFinalCapstone.pdf
Schick, A. R. (2014). Home-school literacy experiences of Latino preschoolers: Does continuity predict
positive child outcomes? Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 370-380.
doi:10.1016/J.appdev.2014.05.006
Sigmundsson, H., Eriksen, A. D., Ofteland, G. S., & Haga, M. (2017). Letter-sound knowledge: Exploring
gender differences in children when they start school regarding knowledge of lage letters, small
letters, sound large letters, and sound small letters. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(8), 1-6.
doi:10.3389/psyg.2017.01539

63
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Sim, S., & Berthelsen, D. (2014). Shared book reading by parents with young children: evidence-based
practice. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 39(1), 50-55. Retrieved from
https://search.informit.com.au/documentSummary;dn=192581574559362
Slavan, R. (2011). Educational psychology: Theory and practice (10th ed.) Boston: Pearson Education.
Soderman, A. K., Gregory, K. M., & McCarty, L. T. (2005). Scaffolding emergent literacy. United States of
America: Pearson Education.
Speece, D. L., Ritchey, K. D., Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., & Schatschneider, C. (2004). Growth in early
reading skills from kindergarten to third grade. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(3), 312 332.
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2003.07.001
Strickland, D. S., & Riley-Ayers, S. (2007). Literacy leadership in early childhood: The essential guide. New
York: Teachers College Press.
Strickland, D. S., & Schickedanz, J. A. (2009). Learning about print in preschool. Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Swain, J., Brooks, G., & Bosley, S. (2014). The benefits of family literacy provision for parents in England.
Journal of Early Childhood Research, 12(1), 77-91. Retrieved from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1019443
Tafa, E. (2009). The standardization of the concept about print into Greek. Literacy Teaching and Learning,
13(1, 2), 1-24. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ867298.pdf
Taylor, N. A., Greenberg, D., & Terry, N. P. (2016). The relationship between parents’ skills and their
preschool children’s emergent literacy skills. Journal of Research Ann Practice for Adult Literacy,
Secondary, and Basic Education, 5(2), 5-16. Retrieved from
http://files.constantcontact.com/94655a8e201/ee92b702-c5e4-42e6-bb39-621be51d32a6.pdf
Texas Education Agency. (2002). Guidelines for examining phonics & word recognition programs. Retrieved
from https://buildingrti.utexas.org/sites/default/files/booklets/redbk3.pdf
Trawick-Smith, J. (2014). Early childhood development: A multicultural perspective (6th ed.). London:
Pearson Education.
Turan, F. (2017). Fonolojik farkındalık becerileri. In F. Temel (Ed.), Dil ve erken okuryazarlık (pp. 89-103).
Ankara: Hedef CS.
Üstün, E. (2007). Okul öncesi çocuklarının okuma yazma becerilerinin gelişimi. İstanbul: Morpa Kültür
Publishing.
Vacca, J. A. L., Vacca, R. T., Gove, M. K., Burkey, L. C., Lenhart, L. A., & McKeon, C. A. (2012). Reading
and learning to read (8th ed.) Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
Vukelich, C., Christie, J., & Enz, B. (2012). Helping young children learn language and literacy (3rd ed.).
Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
Wang, X.L. (2015). Understanding language and literacy development: Diverse learners in the classroom. West
Sussex: John Wiley&Sons, Inc.
Warrington, M., Younger, M. & Bearne, E. (2006) Raising Boys achievement in primary schools. London:
Open University Press.
Weigel, D., Martin, S., & Bennett, K. (2006). Contributions of the home literacy environment to
preschool-aged children's emerging literacy and language skills. Early Child Development and Care,
176, 357-378.
Wesseling, P. B. C., Christmann, C. A., & Lancmann, T. (2017). Shared book reading promotes not only
language development, but also grapheme awareness in german kindergarten children. Frontiers
Psychol, 8(364), 1-13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00364
Whitehead, M. R. (2007). Developing language and literacy with young children. London: Paul Chapman
Publishing.

64
Education and Science 2018, Vol 43, No 196, 49-65 V. Bayraktar

Wortham, S. C. (2009). Early childhood curriculum: Developmental bases for learning and teaching. (5th ed.).
New Jersey, Columbus, Ohio: Pearson Merill Prentice Hall.
Yıldırım, A. (2008). Okul öncesi eğitim kurumlarından yararlanmayan 4-5 yaş çocuklarının dil gelişiminin
etkileyen faktörlerin incelenmesi (Unpublised master’s thesis). Selçuk University, Konya.
Yıldırım, A. ve Şimşek, H. (2016). Sosyal bilimlerde nitel araştırma yöntemleri (10th ed.). Ankara: Seçkin
Yayıncılık.
Yıldız, M., Ataş, M., Aktaş, N., Yekeler, A. D., & Dönmez, T. (2015). Çocuklar ne yazıyor? Okul öncesi
dönemde yazı algısının gelişimi. Turkish Studies, 10(3), 1121-1142. doi:10.7827/TurkishStudies.7844
Zucker, T. A., & Grant, S. L. (2007). Assessing home supports for literacy. K. L. Pence (Ed.), Assessment
in emergent literacy. San Diego: Plural Publishing, Inc.

65

You might also like