Korean - Suffixes

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Study on Korean EFL Learners'

Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns:


stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

Junghee Byun
(Seoul National University)

Byun, Junghee. 2014. Study on Korean EFL Learners’ Stress Shift


Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns: stress-moving, stress-carrying and
neutral suffixes. SNU Working Papers in English Linguistics and Language 12,
1-16. The study intends to emprically investigate Korean EFL learners’
realizations of stress shift where three patterns of suffix - stress-moving (e.g. -ity,
-ic, -ion, -ian), stress-carrying(e.g. -een, -ese, -ee, -eer) and neutral suffixes (e.g.
-al, -ment, -able, -hood) – are added to a word base. In order of difficulty, stress
placement with stress-carrying suffixes poses greatest challenge upon Korean
EFL learners, followed by stress-moving and lastly stress-neutral suffixes as the
least challenging stress pattern among the three. Despite unfamiliarity with the
words of the suffix-carrying pattern, the analysis of the three stress patterns
uniquely reveals that stress error of a derived form might be led by stress error in
the word base. To address this problem, explicit teaching of word stress patterns
should be further reinforeced as an integral part of the ESL/EFL instruction
particularly by clarifying the systematic patterns of stress placement in English
words. (Seoul National University)

Keywords: English stress, word stress shift through suffixes, stress-carying


suffix, stress-moving suffix, neutral suffix

1. Introduction

The greatest challenge that Korean English language learners encounter


in mastering pronunciation of speaking skills is generally attributed to
the fact that the English language is a stress-timed language, whereas
the Korean language is a syllable-timed language. Such different
phonological features of English from that of Korean entails the
significance of having a command of suprasegmentals when speakers
engage in spoken communication in English. Stress in English words
makes rhythms and pitch contours in larger units of utterance such as
2 Byun, Junghee

phrase and sentence, which is also called intonation. Thus, recognizing


and producing correct word stress is a fundamental that Korean English
learners should learn to empower their speaking skill. To accomplish
this, it is worthwhile that we study how much Korean EFL learners are
aware of English word stress and its major rules and how they realize
stress shift in actuality.

2. Literature review
2.1 Significance of word stress in the English language

As a rule, stressed syllables are defined as those syllables in utterance


that are longer, louder and higher in pitch. And such features of English
word stress constitute a crucial element in getting across meanings in
communication. Take xamples of the following words, which are nouns

Noun Verb
INcrease inCREASE
CONtest conTEST
REcord reCORD

when stress is placed on the initial syllable, change their part of speech
into verbs when the stress shifts into the second syllable. It indicates the
function of English stress that can discriminate syntactic features of
words. Meanwhile, English stress also causes a major change in vowel
quality and quantity; for example, unstressed vowels become [ɪ] or [ə],
lax vowels. This phenomenon occurs particularly when affixes are
attached to word base.
The significane of stress in the English language is also supported by
the well-known view that stress is what mainly builds English to be a
stress-timed language. In a stress-timed language, stressed syllables
appear in the same duration, accompanied with trochaic foot (Strong-
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 3
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

Weak) that reside in prosodic units such as word, phrase and sentence.
It is stressed syllables recurrent in the same intervals that create
rhythms in utterance. These stress and rhythm are often considered as a
key to distinguishing information which is important from which is not
in spoken communication (Baek, 2009).
For this reason, they can be recognized as one of the most important
aspects in acquiring L2 speaking fluency. Korean language, however, a
syllable-timed language, basically does not have a stress-rhythm system
like English, which causes Korean L2 learners to have tremendous
problem mastering English. In lack of practice and awareness of
Korean English learners in English stressed syllables and rhythm, we
are highly skeptical about how much they recognize stress shift and its
causes.

2. 2 English stress and word structure

Stress placement in English words is for the many parts predictable as a


rule-governed phenomenon. Dauer (1993) says that 90% of bisyllabic
nouns are stressed on word initial position while 60% of bisyllabic
verbs have primary stress on the second syllable. (as cited in Kim, 2012)
Kang’s study (2004) on stress position of bisyllabic verbs and nouns in
Table 1 reveals that among the basic 2,067 English vocabulary for the
high school English curriculum, 85.6% are mono or bisyllabic.
Recognizing this tendency can facilitate identifying stress position of a
large number of words in major parts of speech; nouns and verbs.

Table 1. Stress position and parts of speech of frequently-used


bisyllabic words (Kang, 2004)
Noun Verb
Stress
Strong-Weak Weak-Strong Strong-Weak Weak-Strong
position
Number of 367 23 35 166
4 Byun, Junghee

words
Percentage 94% 6% 17% 83%

2.2.1 English Word Stress with affixes

Stress has an influence upon both quantity and quality of vowels and
the difference between stressed and unstressed syllables is greater in
English than in most other languages – with the possible exception of
German. The change of English stress is never simple and may be
brought about by various causes. One factor that influences stress
placement is the historical origin of a word. (Celce-Murcia et al, 1996)

2.2.2 Prefixes

According to Celce-Murcia et al. (1996), at large, words containing


prefixes tend to be strongly stressed on the first syllable of the base or
root element, with the prefix either unstressed or lightly stressed. In
English, there are two categories of prefixes: prefixes of Germanic
origin and prefixes of Latinate origin. The Germanic category includes
a-(in award) be-(in believe), for-(in forgive), mis-(in mistake), and
un/under- (in understand). The second category is prefixes of Latinate
origin. These include a/ad-(in agree/adorn), com-(in command), de-(in
decide), dis-(in discuss), ex-(in exist), en-(in encourage), ob-(obtain),
pre-(in predict), pro-(in produce), re-(in reply) and sub-(in subside).

2.2.3 Suffixes

Suffixes in English are divided into two: inflectional and derivational


suffixes. (Lee, 2002) Inflectional suffixes such as –s/es (plural), -ed
(past tense) and –ing (present participle or gerund) do not affect stress.
Similarly, there are a group of derivational suffixes which does not
affect word stress, either, which are called ‘weak suffixes’ or ‘stress-
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 5
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

neutral suffixes’. They may have no effect on the stress pattern of the
root word like the suffix ‘–ful’ in ‘beauty-beautiful’, -able, -al, -ence, -
hood and -ment etc. although exceptions to this exist as in admirable
and ignorance.
On the other hand, there are so-called ‘strong suffixes’ which bear
stress themselves. They may receive strong stress themselves like the
suffix ‘-ee’ in ‘interview-interviewee’. We call them ‘stress-carrying’
suffixes, for example, -ade, -aire, -ee, - esque and –eer and etc.
However, the following words have the same stress as those stress-
bearing suffixes though they are not made from derivation; veneer,
jamboree and brigade.
The other group of suffixes is what can move stress of the word base
(stem) to a different position: -ic, -cian, -tion, -y, –ity, and etc. They
may cause the stress pattern in the stem to shift from one syllable to
another like ‘–ic’ in ‘atom-atomic’. So to speak, they are ‘stress-
moving suffixes’. In these, suffix attachment brings about a change in
vowel quality as well as shift in stress (Celce-Murcia et al, 1996) ; for
example, a stressed vowel [o] in the word ‘PHOtograph’ becomes [ə]
due to neutralization as the suffix –‘y’ causes shift in stress like
‘phoTOGraphy’ when attached to the word base.

2.3. EFL Learners’ awareness of stress suffixes

Ghorbany (2011) conducted a study on students’ awareness of stress


patterns (stress-moving and neutral suffixes) to examine the significant
difference in placing the two types of stress in terms of level of
difficulty. This study got subjects to read stress of a list of words and
the same words with suffixes attached. The Matched T-test result
showed that stress-moving suffixes are more difficult to learn than
neutral-suffixes, leading to making a suggestion that stress pattern in
the English language should be explicitly taught and practiced in the
6 Byun, Junghee

classroom by discovering more effective pedagogical tools or means for


the learners to promote their pronunciation.
Motivated by Ghorbany’s study (2011) to Iranian college students in
EFL context, similar to English learning context in Korea, the present
study seeks an extended version of experiment of Ghorbany to
investigate learners’ degree of awareness on the three stress patterns
suffixes can create. And it further attempts to explore the level of
difficulty for Korean EFL learners in placing stress when three different
types of suffixes are added to word stems; stress-neutral, stress-moving
and stress-carrying suffixes. The ultimate goal of this study will be to
cast insight into significance of teaching stress and provide pedagogical
implications on teaching English word stress and pronunciation.
Given that, this study is designed and conducted to answer the
following research questions.

RQ 1: What is the order of difficulty in pronouncing English words as


three suffix patterns are added to them?
RQ2: What is the stress rule on suffix attachment that Korean EFL
learners need to learn most?
RQ3: What are pedagogical implications made from this study?

3. Method
3.1 Participants

They are randomly chosen 31 female students of the 1st grade of a high
school in Changwon, Kyungsangnamdo.

3.2. Procedure

The list of words including main words as the first row and the same
words with suffixes as the second row is prepared. Word samples were
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 7
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

collected mostly from the vocabulary domain of middle school English


textbooks. There are 6 pairs of words for each suffix pattern. Stress-
neutral suffixes under examination include ‘-al’ ‘-ment’ ‘-able’ ‘-hood’
‘-ant’ and ‘-al’. Stress-moving suffixes under examination are ‘-ity’ ‘-
ic’(used for two words) ‘-ion’ ‘-ian’ and ‘-y’. Stress-carrying suffixes
for the experiment are ‘-een’ ‘-ese’ ‘-ee’ ‘-eer’ ‘-aire’ and ‘-esque’.
Then, the subjects are asked to speak words by giving stress to a vowel
position they believe to have stress. After that, their pronunciation is
recorded and scored in correct or incorrect. Acoustic analysis was
conducted. The test sheet is attached to the Appendix on the last page.

4. Results & Discussion

The result initially came out as expected. The order of difficulty in


stress pronunciation is as follows: stress-carrying 49%, stress-moving
62% and stress-neutral 66% (from the right to the left, most difficult to
least difficult). Thus, in average, words with stress-carrying suffixes are

Figure 1. Top 5 words of highest difficulty


8 Byun, Junghee

assumed to be the most difficult one to realize stress shift from the
word base, the form prior to suffix attachment. However, as shown in
Figure 1, it was quite unexpected that the word that had the lowest
success rate in shifting stress was comfortable, which has a stress-
neutral suffix –able at the end. Out of thirty one participants, only six
correctly put stress on the first syllable and the rest did on the second or
third syllable.

Figure 2. Accuracy of stress placement in neutral suffixes

In particular, there were 18 people who put stress on the second syllable
when the suffix –able was added to the word base comfort. The number
was twice as many as those who put stress on the third syllable. Figure
1 also indicates that the word with a second lowest success rate to
realize is photography, a word of the stress-moving pattern. This word
did not show any significant tendency; out of thirty, twenty four people
almost gave stress evenly in different syllable positions. It may have
been affected by its word length. The third most difficult word to put
stress on is economic, a word with the stress- moving pattern. Out of
twenty nines, twenty two subjects did not succeed in stress shift with
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 9
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

sixteen who did not change stress position. It may also have been
affected by lack of its phonological rule pertaining to stress shift . The
fourth most difficult word to put stress on is a word of stress-carrying
suffix pattern, engineer. Out of thirty, twenty people gave stress to the
second syllable. Lastly, the fifth word of difficulty is interviewee that
nineteen participants could not put stress on the suffix. To sum up,
these top five words came from all the three different suffix patterns.
Close examination made it possible to witness a huge discrepency in
the success rate of stress assignment among invidual words particularly,
those with stress-neutral and stress-moving suffixes. Except for
agreement and comfortable, which out of thirty one participants, less
than half succeeded in finding correct stress position, a majority of
them succeeded in giving stress to the rest words.
Regarding the failure rate to finding stress of agreement , the incorrect
stress assignment of the word base agree created the original problem.
Out of thirty one, twenty one, which are more than half, pronounced
agree with stress on the first syllable. Among them, fourteen put stress
on the same position when pronouncing agreement. And, adding the
nine subjects who correctly put stress on the second syllable in
prouncing both the word stem and its derived form, there are a total of
twenty three who did not attempt stress shift among thirty one. Thus,
what can be assumed from the data here is that many participants are
aware of the stress pattern ‘-ment’, which does not move stress, and that
it is too premature to conclude that the error was derived from their
ignorance on the shift causing suffix itself. Rather, this result implies
the necessity to teach explicitly and practice putting stress on the word
base, agree. To do so, it is worthy of speculating the cause of such
stress error as giving word-initial stress to agree.
On the other hand, out of thrity one, half of them put stress of comfort
on the second syllable. Among them, ten participants correctly
pronounced comfortable with stress on the second syllable. There are
only four of them who could correctly pronounce both comfort and
10 Byun, Junghee

comfortable. It suggests the explicit teaching and practice of the word


base form comfort , similar to what needs to be done for the pair of
agree and agreement. Additionally, further examination needs to be
made on the tendency of stress on the second syllable of comfort and
comfortable.
Some speculations can be made in accouting for the stress errors. First,
it is possible that the cause is traced back to the dialect in their native
language. Kim (2012) in his study on the stress realization of
Kyungsang dialect speakers states that if the word final is a light
syllable, stress is accompanied with high tone in the penultimate; for
example, [akási] for ‘lady’ and [mikuráči] for ‘mudfish’. It may have
affected the stress error that many subjects commiteed in pronouncing
comfort, which they could take as a three-syllable word [컴포트] in
syllabic structure of the Korean language. Another explanation to this
error may be that the heavy syllable of –fort could attract stress more
than com-.
Moreover, it is salient that not a small number of participants (seven out
of thirty one) divided the word comfortable into the following syllabic
structure, com-for-table, pronoucing the last syllable [teɪbl] as in ‘table’.
This recalls another easily noticeable error among EFL learners
pronoucing –mine to be [maɪn] in ‘determine’. It can be considered as a
strategy to make up a gap in pronoucing a confusing word with the
word they already know. One clear thing is that they tend to identify the
content word able with the suffix –able and such confusion in
pronunciation seems to be mainly due to the same spelling of the two
words.
All in all, in the stress-neutral pattern, except the suffix –able, the
participants generally can put stress on the right position of the words
with the suffixes -al, -ant, -ment and –hood .
Meanwhile, as shown in Figure 3, pronunciation of the words with
stress-moving suffixes are mostly scored high except photography and
economic. More specifically, the word base economy was pronounced
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 11
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

correctly by most of the participants but, there were only seven who
correctly made stress shift. Similarly, there were merely four subjects
who were able to move stress to the second syllable when the word
base photograph was followed by the suffix –y.
It was quite mindboggling that the result of stress realization of the two

Figure 3. Accuracy in stress placement of words with stress-


moving suffixes

words with the same suffix –ic in atomic and economic was drastically
different with 80% to 17% in ratio. This data can support that word
length, in other words, the number of syllables in a word, is a strong
factor to find the stress position of a word. That is, when a word is
longer than another with the same suffix attached to them, one can
experience more difficulty in finding stress position in a longer word.
In summary, it was found that the suffixes -ity, -ian and -tion impose
relatively less challenge on participants in stress assignment of derived
words while the suffixes -y in photography and -ic in economic need to
be paid more attention to.
Among individual words with the stress-carrying suffixes in Table 4,
discrepency in the degree of difficulty of stress placement was not as
12 Byun, Junghee

great as the other two suffix patterns although overall, participants


found stress placement more challenging than in pronouncing words
with the other two suffix patterns. As most of the stress-carrying
suffixes used for this study such as -teen, -ese, -aire, and –esque are in
need of additional practice, learning the more frequently-used suffixes
like –ee in interviewee and -eer in engineer needs to be prioritized. It
was apparent that they did not know those suffixes bear stress

Figure 4. Accuracy in stress placement of words with stree-


carrying suffixes

themselves demonstrating stress shift from a stressed (word-initial)


vowel of the word base to its adjacent vowel instead of placing stress
on the suffixes.
So far, we have looked into whether Korean high school students are
aware of the suffixes that affect stress assignment as well as vowel
quantity and quality in pronouncing words with stress. The foremost
findings about them in this study are first the lack of awareness on
English as a stress-timed language, second, the lack of knowledge on
the relation between suffixes and stress shift, third, the lack of
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 13
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

knowledge on the effects that stress shift can produce. Lastly, there is a
high demand on practice to pronounce syllables both stressed and
unstressed, a source of creating rhythms.
Having said that, I would like to propose some approaches to address
pronunciation problems related to stress shift. Study on word origin
might help understanding of its complexity. As mentioned in literature
review, affixes of Latin and Germanic origin maintain particular stress
positions of their own. For example, in case of comfort and agree, their
background on origin can be borrowed to disentangle stress
irregularities that many participants in this study may have felt.
The words, comfort and agree take Latinate roots which avoid stress if
you look up an etymology dictionary source (http://www. etymonlin
e.com/index.php) though a- and com- are now subsumed into the word
stem so that they are hardly recognized as separate affixes. Use of word
origin can facilitate meaningful learning on English stress rules so as to
reinforce memory duration.
Also, more fundamentally, the background on the syllabic structure of
English word and comparison with that of Korean language needs to be
highlighted in speaking class. Knowledge on syllable initial/final
consonant clusters of English and light/heavy syllable are necessary to
present in teaching pronunciation. In particular, syllable final consonant
clusters do not exist in the Korean language (Kim, 1993) and syllable
initial consonant clusters are possible only if a glide is followed. Thus,
syllable initial consonant clusters like CCV or CCCV in English do not
exist in Korean. The notion of heavy and light syllables is something to
mention in relation with English stress.
As Korean language does not have the stress system like English which
stressed syllables and the rhythm occur repeatedly in regular intervals,
presenting some basic concepts or rules on English stress might also be
a great support for learners to recognize the regularity of English stress,
taking its unpredictability into account at the same time; long vowels
and diphthongs attract stress even though there are exceptions to this.
14 Byun, Junghee

By associating stress with spelling, we can address the simple but


substantial stress errors of agree and engineer that a large number of
subjects gave stress on the first syllable in this study by explaining that
a long vowel identified as the spelling ‘-ee’ attracts stress, whereas ‘a-
’in agree is a light syllable, being reduced to a lax vowel [ə] without
being stressed. In short, it should be an integral part of teaching
pronunciation that Korean EFL learners raise awareness on the prosodic
features of English and the differences in the syllabic structure between
the two languages.

5. Conclusion

The study aims at looking into the degree of awareness of the suffixes
that affect English stress. It was found that words with the stress-
carrying suffix pattern gives Korean EFL learners in this study hardest
task in stress placement and second the stress-moving suffix and lastly,
the stress-neutral suffix pattern. It may be due to unfamiliarity with the
words of the suffix-carrying pattern. However, it was commonly found
from the data of the three patterns that the stress error of the derived
forms led to the stress error in the word stem. To address this problem,
teachers can ultimately minimize students’ frustration by comparisons
of prosodic features between the two languages and to encourage them
to raise consciousness continuously by clarifying the systematic
patterns of stress placement in English words. Also, teachers should
develop strategies on how to present word stress to students with
careful presentations, various practices from controlled to
communicative, and consistent reinforcement. Explicit teaching of
word stress patterns should be a part of the ESL/EFL pronunciation
curriculum.
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 15
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes

References

Baek, S. J. (2009). An analysis of english stress shift realizations by


Korean learners of english (Master’s thesis). Retrieved from
http://www.riss.kr/search/detail/DetailView.do?p_mat_type=be54d
9b8bc7cdb09&control_no=b0b1a648acb53abbffe0bdc3ef48d419&
outLink=N#redirect
Celce-Murcia, M., Briton, D. M. & Goodwin, J. M. (1996). Teaching
pronunciation: A reference for teachers of English to speakers of
other languages. NY: Cambridge University Press.
Ghorbany. Z. (2011). EFL learners’ awareness of stress-moving .vs.
neutral suffixes. English Language Teaching. doi: 10.5539/elt.
v4n4p146
Harper, D. (2013). Online etymology dictionary. http://www.
etymonline.com/index.php.
Kang, Y. S. (2004). Yeng-e kangsey-uy ihay [Understanding English
stress]. Korea, Seoul: Kyengjin Munhwasa.
Kim, H. W. (2012). The effect of Kyungsang dialect on the English
word stress acquisition (Master’s thesis). Retrieved from http://
www.riss.kr/search/ detail/ Detail View. do?p_mat_type= be54d9b
8bc7cdb09& control_no =413b7ff7 7582 a4
c3ffe0bdc3ef48d419#redirect
Kim, S. H. (1993). A contrastive analysis of the syllable structures
(Master’s thesis). Retrieved from http://www.riss.kr /search /detail/
Detail View.do?p _mat_type= be54d9 b8bc7 cdb09 & control_no
=53cdcf37 b147a292 #redirect
Lee, Y. G. (2002). Yeng-e kangseylon [English stress theory]. Korea,
Chonman: Chonnam University Press.

Junghee Byun
[email protected]
16 Byun, Junghee

Appendix

You might also like