Korean - Suffixes
Korean - Suffixes
Korean - Suffixes
Junghee Byun
(Seoul National University)
1. Introduction
2. Literature review
2.1 Significance of word stress in the English language
Noun Verb
INcrease inCREASE
CONtest conTEST
REcord reCORD
when stress is placed on the initial syllable, change their part of speech
into verbs when the stress shifts into the second syllable. It indicates the
function of English stress that can discriminate syntactic features of
words. Meanwhile, English stress also causes a major change in vowel
quality and quantity; for example, unstressed vowels become [ɪ] or [ə],
lax vowels. This phenomenon occurs particularly when affixes are
attached to word base.
The significane of stress in the English language is also supported by
the well-known view that stress is what mainly builds English to be a
stress-timed language. In a stress-timed language, stressed syllables
appear in the same duration, accompanied with trochaic foot (Strong-
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 3
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes
Weak) that reside in prosodic units such as word, phrase and sentence.
It is stressed syllables recurrent in the same intervals that create
rhythms in utterance. These stress and rhythm are often considered as a
key to distinguishing information which is important from which is not
in spoken communication (Baek, 2009).
For this reason, they can be recognized as one of the most important
aspects in acquiring L2 speaking fluency. Korean language, however, a
syllable-timed language, basically does not have a stress-rhythm system
like English, which causes Korean L2 learners to have tremendous
problem mastering English. In lack of practice and awareness of
Korean English learners in English stressed syllables and rhythm, we
are highly skeptical about how much they recognize stress shift and its
causes.
words
Percentage 94% 6% 17% 83%
Stress has an influence upon both quantity and quality of vowels and
the difference between stressed and unstressed syllables is greater in
English than in most other languages – with the possible exception of
German. The change of English stress is never simple and may be
brought about by various causes. One factor that influences stress
placement is the historical origin of a word. (Celce-Murcia et al, 1996)
2.2.2 Prefixes
2.2.3 Suffixes
neutral suffixes’. They may have no effect on the stress pattern of the
root word like the suffix ‘–ful’ in ‘beauty-beautiful’, -able, -al, -ence, -
hood and -ment etc. although exceptions to this exist as in admirable
and ignorance.
On the other hand, there are so-called ‘strong suffixes’ which bear
stress themselves. They may receive strong stress themselves like the
suffix ‘-ee’ in ‘interview-interviewee’. We call them ‘stress-carrying’
suffixes, for example, -ade, -aire, -ee, - esque and –eer and etc.
However, the following words have the same stress as those stress-
bearing suffixes though they are not made from derivation; veneer,
jamboree and brigade.
The other group of suffixes is what can move stress of the word base
(stem) to a different position: -ic, -cian, -tion, -y, –ity, and etc. They
may cause the stress pattern in the stem to shift from one syllable to
another like ‘–ic’ in ‘atom-atomic’. So to speak, they are ‘stress-
moving suffixes’. In these, suffix attachment brings about a change in
vowel quality as well as shift in stress (Celce-Murcia et al, 1996) ; for
example, a stressed vowel [o] in the word ‘PHOtograph’ becomes [ə]
due to neutralization as the suffix –‘y’ causes shift in stress like
‘phoTOGraphy’ when attached to the word base.
3. Method
3.1 Participants
They are randomly chosen 31 female students of the 1st grade of a high
school in Changwon, Kyungsangnamdo.
3.2. Procedure
The list of words including main words as the first row and the same
words with suffixes as the second row is prepared. Word samples were
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 7
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes
assumed to be the most difficult one to realize stress shift from the
word base, the form prior to suffix attachment. However, as shown in
Figure 1, it was quite unexpected that the word that had the lowest
success rate in shifting stress was comfortable, which has a stress-
neutral suffix –able at the end. Out of thirty one participants, only six
correctly put stress on the first syllable and the rest did on the second or
third syllable.
In particular, there were 18 people who put stress on the second syllable
when the suffix –able was added to the word base comfort. The number
was twice as many as those who put stress on the third syllable. Figure
1 also indicates that the word with a second lowest success rate to
realize is photography, a word of the stress-moving pattern. This word
did not show any significant tendency; out of thirty, twenty four people
almost gave stress evenly in different syllable positions. It may have
been affected by its word length. The third most difficult word to put
stress on is economic, a word with the stress- moving pattern. Out of
twenty nines, twenty two subjects did not succeed in stress shift with
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 9
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes
sixteen who did not change stress position. It may also have been
affected by lack of its phonological rule pertaining to stress shift . The
fourth most difficult word to put stress on is a word of stress-carrying
suffix pattern, engineer. Out of thirty, twenty people gave stress to the
second syllable. Lastly, the fifth word of difficulty is interviewee that
nineteen participants could not put stress on the suffix. To sum up,
these top five words came from all the three different suffix patterns.
Close examination made it possible to witness a huge discrepency in
the success rate of stress assignment among invidual words particularly,
those with stress-neutral and stress-moving suffixes. Except for
agreement and comfortable, which out of thirty one participants, less
than half succeeded in finding correct stress position, a majority of
them succeeded in giving stress to the rest words.
Regarding the failure rate to finding stress of agreement , the incorrect
stress assignment of the word base agree created the original problem.
Out of thirty one, twenty one, which are more than half, pronounced
agree with stress on the first syllable. Among them, fourteen put stress
on the same position when pronouncing agreement. And, adding the
nine subjects who correctly put stress on the second syllable in
prouncing both the word stem and its derived form, there are a total of
twenty three who did not attempt stress shift among thirty one. Thus,
what can be assumed from the data here is that many participants are
aware of the stress pattern ‘-ment’, which does not move stress, and that
it is too premature to conclude that the error was derived from their
ignorance on the shift causing suffix itself. Rather, this result implies
the necessity to teach explicitly and practice putting stress on the word
base, agree. To do so, it is worthy of speculating the cause of such
stress error as giving word-initial stress to agree.
On the other hand, out of thrity one, half of them put stress of comfort
on the second syllable. Among them, ten participants correctly
pronounced comfortable with stress on the second syllable. There are
only four of them who could correctly pronounce both comfort and
10 Byun, Junghee
correctly by most of the participants but, there were only seven who
correctly made stress shift. Similarly, there were merely four subjects
who were able to move stress to the second syllable when the word
base photograph was followed by the suffix –y.
It was quite mindboggling that the result of stress realization of the two
words with the same suffix –ic in atomic and economic was drastically
different with 80% to 17% in ratio. This data can support that word
length, in other words, the number of syllables in a word, is a strong
factor to find the stress position of a word. That is, when a word is
longer than another with the same suffix attached to them, one can
experience more difficulty in finding stress position in a longer word.
In summary, it was found that the suffixes -ity, -ian and -tion impose
relatively less challenge on participants in stress assignment of derived
words while the suffixes -y in photography and -ic in economic need to
be paid more attention to.
Among individual words with the stress-carrying suffixes in Table 4,
discrepency in the degree of difficulty of stress placement was not as
12 Byun, Junghee
knowledge on the effects that stress shift can produce. Lastly, there is a
high demand on practice to pronounce syllables both stressed and
unstressed, a source of creating rhythms.
Having said that, I would like to propose some approaches to address
pronunciation problems related to stress shift. Study on word origin
might help understanding of its complexity. As mentioned in literature
review, affixes of Latin and Germanic origin maintain particular stress
positions of their own. For example, in case of comfort and agree, their
background on origin can be borrowed to disentangle stress
irregularities that many participants in this study may have felt.
The words, comfort and agree take Latinate roots which avoid stress if
you look up an etymology dictionary source (http://www. etymonlin
e.com/index.php) though a- and com- are now subsumed into the word
stem so that they are hardly recognized as separate affixes. Use of word
origin can facilitate meaningful learning on English stress rules so as to
reinforce memory duration.
Also, more fundamentally, the background on the syllabic structure of
English word and comparison with that of Korean language needs to be
highlighted in speaking class. Knowledge on syllable initial/final
consonant clusters of English and light/heavy syllable are necessary to
present in teaching pronunciation. In particular, syllable final consonant
clusters do not exist in the Korean language (Kim, 1993) and syllable
initial consonant clusters are possible only if a glide is followed. Thus,
syllable initial consonant clusters like CCV or CCCV in English do not
exist in Korean. The notion of heavy and light syllables is something to
mention in relation with English stress.
As Korean language does not have the stress system like English which
stressed syllables and the rhythm occur repeatedly in regular intervals,
presenting some basic concepts or rules on English stress might also be
a great support for learners to recognize the regularity of English stress,
taking its unpredictability into account at the same time; long vowels
and diphthongs attract stress even though there are exceptions to this.
14 Byun, Junghee
5. Conclusion
The study aims at looking into the degree of awareness of the suffixes
that affect English stress. It was found that words with the stress-
carrying suffix pattern gives Korean EFL learners in this study hardest
task in stress placement and second the stress-moving suffix and lastly,
the stress-neutral suffix pattern. It may be due to unfamiliarity with the
words of the suffix-carrying pattern. However, it was commonly found
from the data of the three patterns that the stress error of the derived
forms led to the stress error in the word stem. To address this problem,
teachers can ultimately minimize students’ frustration by comparisons
of prosodic features between the two languages and to encourage them
to raise consciousness continuously by clarifying the systematic
patterns of stress placement in English words. Also, teachers should
develop strategies on how to present word stress to students with
careful presentations, various practices from controlled to
communicative, and consistent reinforcement. Explicit teaching of
word stress patterns should be a part of the ESL/EFL pronunciation
curriculum.
Study on Korean EFL learners’ Stress Shift Realizations in Three Suffix Patterns 15
stress-moving, stress-carrying and neutral suffixes
References
Junghee Byun
[email protected]
16 Byun, Junghee
Appendix