History of Criminological Theories Research Paper: The First Rudimentary Ideas
History of Criminological Theories Research Paper: The First Rudimentary Ideas
History of Criminological Theories Research Paper: The First Rudimentary Ideas
Paper
This sample History of Criminological Theories Research Paper is published for educational
and informational purposes only. If you need help writing your assignment, please use our
research paper writing service and buy a paper on any topic at affordable price. Also check our
tips on how to write a research paper, see the lists of criminal justice research paper topics, and
browse research paper examples.
For thousands of years, mankind has developed and applied vague or specific thoughts about
what behavior should be labeled as criminal, who the criminals are, and what the causes of crime
may be. Philosophers in the eighteenth and early nineteenth century put forward the rational
criminal who is free to make choices and criticized the cruel and unjust criminal justice system.
The scientific study of crime started in the beginning of nineteenth century with mathematicians
searching for empirical regularities between countries and areas in administrative governmental
data. Sociologists, psychologists, and physicians introduced societal and individual causes of
crime in a comprehensive way. Later, new developments in other disciplines triggered new
thoughts on the causes of crime, notably strain, control, and cultural theories. These have been
dominant for about many decades, although critical theories emerged in the 1960s and 1970s of
the twentieth century. Recent developments of theoretical criminology point at including
disciplines that have not been very involved in the field of crime like neurosciences and
econometrics so far.
In the Middle Ages, the search for the causes of crime in Europe was more than ever regulated
by religious motives: those who committed crimes “must” offend God’s will and “therefore”
must be possessed by the devil. Draconic tortures were applied to release the man or the woman
from that evil. Until that time scientific knowledge as we have nowadays did not exist. Monks
copied unique written texts about religion or other affairs that were stored in monasteries.
Spiritual explanations were at that time the dominant thought about the causes of crime.
Desiderius Erasmus (1466 (?)–1536) was one of the first philosophers who wrote about the
“natural” causes of crime. He related the occurrence of criminal behavior not to the “spirit” of
the individual but to poverty in general (although without any empirical evidence). The physician
G. Battista della Porta (1535–1615) was the first who systematically was searching for
observable causes of crime. He opened the brains of hanged criminals in order to study
irregularities in their shape, color, and structure. In the age of Enlightenment, new vague
thoughts about crime were developed, of which the rudiments can still be found in
criminological theory of today. During Enlightenment intellectuals promoted the idea of ratio
and philosophy and in general the upcoming scientific work. They challenged widespread
superstition and abuses of rights of residents by those in power and by the church. Combined
with the upcoming Protestantism, the individual became more and more central in the new
philosophies (as they called science those days). This was an important period in the study of
crime, because many of our contemporary fundamental assumptions about the causes of crime
originated in the seventeenth, eighteenth, or nineteenth century (although the words may be
different and specifications may have been very well elaborated during the last 300 years).
Long before the name of criminology was introduced by Raffaele Garofalo in his 1885 published
Criminologia, researchers empirically studied crime under the label of moral statistics or
criminal science. The foundation of empirical criminology was laid in the beginning of that
century when Belgian, French, and British mathematicians and statisticians like Guerry (1832),
the astronomer and first PhD of mathematics of Ghent University in Belgium Que´telet (1984
[1831]), Ducpe´tiaux (1827), and Rawson (1839) and Fletcher (1848) of the Royal Statistical
Society of London tried to uncover regularities in the social and geographic distributions of
crime and criminals between counties, areas, cities, and countries (Beirne 1987; Weisburd et al.
2009). The publication of the Comptes Ge´ne´rales de l’Administration de la Justice Criminelle
en France in 1825/1826 inspired them to explore official data on crime in more detail (Weisburd
et al. 2009, p. 6). These scholars were the first who studied crime as a social phenomenon that is
unequally distributed over society (Guerry 1832, 1833, 1864). Que´telet (1847) discovered the
normal distribution in statistics in order to assess the “normality” of empirical regularities he
demonstrated in research. These scholars were inspired by the reform movement in their
countries of which fighting illiteracy and illness among the poor was the priority political issue.
That is the reason why they studied the empirical relationships between illiteracy and wealth and
personal and property crimes in France, England, and the low countries (now the Netherlands
and Belgium). They did not systematize their thoughts into structured theorizing. Instead, they
made up inventories of all kinds of correlates of possible causes of crime and criminals (good
examples of that kind of inventories are Afschaffenberg (1906) and Parmelee (1918/ 1919)).
Later in the nineteenth century, Charles Darwin formulated his evolution paradigm on the origins
of species (and like many original thinkers encountered conflicts with religious powers of his
time). Darwin’s revolutionary thoughts completely changed the human perspective on the roots
of mankind. The physician Cesare Lombroso (1835–1909) was fully impressed by Darwin’s
theory. He elaborated Darwin’s innovative way of thinking and applied it to the explanation of
crime (Lombroso 2006 [1878]). As physician he studied sentenced criminals as if they suffered
from a drawback in the evolutionary process, believing that “those” people can be recognized by
outer stigmata. Scholars in his time had an optimistic and naive view on science (positivism)
assuming that scientific knowledge could change the world in a better place to live in. Lombroso
later studied in his career many other topics as well (mostly social aspects of crime), among
others the spatial variations in the distribution of crime across areas, provinces, and countries
(Lombroso 2006 [1878]).
A landmark in the study of crime causation was the work of the sociologist Emile Durkheim
(1858–1917). He built on the analyses of social philosophers and the first sociologists who
observed in the nineteenth century macro-social processes in the speedy developing societies in
Europe (Bierstedt 1978). Saint-Simon (1760–1825) can be viewed as the first who envisaged the
new industrial society, and together with the younger Auguste Comte (1798–1857), he analyzed
the disruptive consequences of the many European political revolutions. The abrupt changes in
political structure and society that they observed led to a bigger gap between the state and the
families and individuals when intermediate institutions disappear. Also Georg Simmel (1858–
1918) was very critical at the new technical, urbanized materialistic culture that emerged in
Europe. Ferdinand To¨ nnies (1855–1936) focused more on the social changes in society. In his
view, societies transformed from Gemeinschaft into Gesellschaft, leaving behind the small-scale
agricultural villages with strong social cohesion and informal social control. In modern societies
of a Gesellschaft type, trade and industry took the lead; laborers recruited from rural areas and
ties between people were weakened. People in Europe became more individualized, and
sociologists feared the negative consequences for society and for crime rates. Durkheim built on
their thoughts and focused on the relationships between people and the impact of societal
changes on them (Durkheim 1951 [1897], 1964 [1893]). He argued that relationships between
people are based on cohesion, division of labor, religion, (scientific) knowledge, and morality,
and when societies become more heterogeneous, their collective conscience is diminished.
Anomic societies lose their power to regulate social relations between people, and increased
levels of crime are a consequence. People with less ties feel free to break the moral rules more
easily. Central is the idea that external forces from society (measured by structural features)
explain why people commit crime. Durkheim demonstrated empirically that in societies with
lower levels of social integration, people tend to commit more suicides and more crimes.
Durkheim on the other side inspired other influential criminologists to focus on the effects of
social control on crime. Travis Hirschi (1935–) is the best known protagonist of this perspective.
In line with Durkheim’s reasoning, Hirschi stated in Causes of Delinquency that if an individual
is not tied to society, he is free to deviate from the norms, e.g., free to commit crimes (Hirschi
2006 [1969]). That view was opposite to the mainstream and dominating cultural deviance
theories of his time. Hirschi was especially critical of the differential association theory, arguing
that individuals need a certain level of commitment to society and social ties with other people to
refrain them from crime. The family (especially parents) plays a central role in establishing ties.
Hirschi was certainly not the first to introduce the concept of social control as a condition for
crime. A number of predecessors can be mentioned. Edward Ross (1866–1951), one of the most
influential sociologists of the United States, was the mastermind of the concept of social control.
Around the turn of the twentieth century, he pointed at internal and external kinds of social
control that regulate criminal behavior (Ross 1918 [1901]). Walter Reckless (1899–1988)
focused later on social control as exercised by parents on their children: parental supervision
(Reckless 1940, 1961). When parents do not succeed in supervising their spouses, the children
will develop weak self-control and weak other internal controls. If they succeed, kids can resist
the worst neighborhood influences that normally lead to more crime (containment). Albert Reiss
Jr. (1922–2006) specified kinds of social control into personal and social control that both refrain
youngsters from criminal behavior (Reiss 1951); Jackson Toby introduced the concept of “stakes
in conformity” referring at how much a person has to lose when he breaks the law (Toby 1957),
later followed by F. Ivan Nye who distinguished four dimensions of social control (direct social
control, internalized social control, indirect social control, and access to legal means (Nye
1958)). All of their work was integrated by Hirschi and specified in testable propositions.
The idea of social control has been expanded in several new directions. Travis Hirschi,
collaborating with Michael Gottfredson, proposed a general theory of crime in which was stated
that human’s low self-control was the key explanatory mechanism in the explanation of crime
(Gottfredson and Hirschi 1990). Low self-control is developed by psychological deficits or by
parents in unorganized families by “inconsistent” parenting styles on their offspring. Blumstein
and his colleagues (1988) started more empirical research on criminal careers, putting aside static
views on offenders. The idea of a small group of active offenders was revealed by Wolfgang and
his colleagues in their Philadelphia study (Wolfgang et al. 1972). Later research found out that
offenders are influenced by the number and nature of risk factors they encounter during their life
course (Farrington 1988, 2002; Loeber and Wikstro¨ m 1993). This life-course perspective has
been developed by several scholars. Dan Nagin developed a method to analyze empirically
various trajectories of offending (Nagin 2005). John Laub and Robert Sampson (Laub and
Sampson 2003; Sampson and Laub 1993) initiated the developmental criminology and explored
the idea of varying social controls during the life course that may be responsible for the
variations in crime when people coming of age. They based their analysis on the data of two
matched groups of 500 delinquents and 500 nondelinquents collected by Sheldon and Eleanor
Glueck for their pioneering empirical study on juvenile crime in the 1950s (Glueck and Glueck
1950). Rolf Loeber in Pittsburgh (Loeber 1990), Marc LeBlanc in Montreal (Leblanc 1997), and
Farrington with his longitudinal study of male laborers in London (Farrington 2002, 2003) have
brought up new dynamic views on the causes of crime. Terry Moffitt’s distinction between
adolescent-limited and persistent criminals also inspired many present studies on criminal
careers, recidivism, and redemption (Moffitt 1993). Although her distinction between two groups
of offenders is probably too simple, she inspired may researchers to study criminal careers with a
variety of explanations for distinct career trajectories (Cullen 2011). In the 90s of the last
century, new promising horizons emerge from the idea that perhaps neurological defectives play
a role as cause of crime (Rafter 2008; Raine 1993). In the future, when scientists know more
about DNA and the human gnomes, new kinds of causal theories will be developed and tested.
Symbolic interactionism as developed by Mead was important not only for sociology but also for
criminology in two respects. Edwin Sutherland (1883–1947) built his famous differential
association theory in the 1930s and 1940s on the theoretical roots of symbolic interactionism
(Sutherland 1947). Criminal behavior is learned in a social process within social groups by an
excess of positive definitions to law violation above negative definitions towards law violation.
He also combined the macro perspective of Chicago sociology on urbanization, social
disorganization, and social groups with the micro theory of Mead on how people learn attitudes
and behavior in intimate groups. Sutherland not only studied the crimes of young people in this
respect but applied his theory also on homeless people as well as professional thieves and
managers of powerful corporations (Sutherland 1937, 1983 [1949]). Sutherland had a great
number of students who dedicated their academic life in exploring, defending, and extending the
differential association theory (Cressey 1964, 1969; Glaser 1972). The differential association
theory inspired many empirical studies on the impact of peers on the criminal behavior of
adolescents.
Some may argue that this “wealth of ideas” is a blessing for the discipline; others would argue
that its presence demonstrates the impotence of theoretical criminology. There is no superior
theory (despite the claims of individual scholars) that can explain more and better than other
theories, let alone can predict that a person will commit a crime in the future given the conditions
of that theory. In the past, criminologists and others have also heavily and regularly criticized the
state of causal theory. In the 1930s of last century, the mathematicians Michael and Adler (1933)
critically evaluated criminology for the government and concluded that criminological theories
are no more than empirical generalizations that do not meet any mathematical criteria of theory
formation. Forty years later a German sociologist discussed the most important criminological
theories of that time and judged them as vague, with unclear concepts and unspecified relations
between them, without any formal structure and low explanatory power (Opp 1974). Wolfgang
and his colleagues drew similar conclusions after evaluating criminology between the years 1945
and 1972 and observing empirical progress but hardly any theoretical improvements (Wolfgang
et al. 1978). Bernard (1990) repeated earlier critics and questioned whether criminological
theories have any explanatory power. He stated, “Despite the enormous volume of quantitative
research, there has been no substantial progress in falsifying the criminology theories that existed
20 years ago” and that “None of these theories has been falsified in the past 20 years” (Bernard
1990, p. 147). Finally he concluded: “Current controversies in criminology indicate that there is
fundamental disagreement on even the most basic facts about crime” and “That being the case,
the failure to accumulate verified knowledge in the context of specific theories should not be
surprising. Even on simple correlations like those between age and crime, social class and crime
there is no agreement” (Bernard 1990, p. 329).
What might be going on with the discipline? Before condemning theoretical criminology, we
must first try to disentangle the existing causal theories whether they are rivals or not to assess
the state of a discipline (Lakatos 1970). However, that is a very difficult task to accomplish for
criminology. First, we have to acknowledge that not all criminological theories can be compared
because they are about different units of analysis. A majority of causal theories have individuals
as subject of analysis, but an increasing number of others are about geographical areas, like street
segments, neighborhoods, or countries, or social and ethnic groups, business corporations, states,
organized groups, institutions like prisons or probation systems, police, or laws. In order to
compare whether theories are rivals, they should at least have the same unit of analysis, or else
one is comparing apples with pears. Second, in case of theories about individuals, the dependent
variables (i.e., the behavior to be explained) differ too much prohibiting comparisons. Most
theories are about criminal behavior in general, others are about a specific kind of crime like
burglary or violence, some are about deviant behavior including mental disturbances or sexual or
psychological abnormalities, some include all aggressive behaviors, and some even do not
mention the kind of behavior that is aimed to be explained! Third, criminology is a product of
intellectual influences of many other disciplines, notably sociology and psychology but also from
medical sciences and law. As a consequence conceptual frameworks of these disciplines are
introduced in criminology, without further reference to other, already existing theoretical
concepts. The complex interdisciplinary discourse must be unraveled to assess whether we have
distinct independent causal factors or not. Fourth, theories of which the propensity to crime is the
object of explanation cannot be compared with theories in which criminal behavior is the
outcome. For instance, in Hirschi’s bonding theory, the outcome of the bonding process is that
the individual is free to deviate, not the frequency or seriousness of the criminal offense. Lastly,
most causal theories explain only little variance in the criminal behavior of humans as Weisburd
and Piquero recently demonstrated (Weisburd and Piquero 2008). They pointed at the
underdeveloped way of theory testing in criminology and the practice to use seldom primary data
for that purpose. But that is only part of the story. Probably the fact that we have to do with only
fragmented and limited explanations anyway is important as well.
Bibliography: