The Psychology of Religion 2019 2
The Psychology of Religion 2019 2
The Psychology of Religion 2019 2
CONTENTS
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 1
INTRODUCTION...................................................................................2
CONCLUSIONS ..................................................................................32
REFERENCE……………………………………………………… 34
1
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
It has been long since the last (and only) chapter on the psychology of religion appeared in
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 2
the Annual Review of Psychology (Gorsuch 1988). The psychology of religion as an
identifiable subfield of psychology has grown rapidly since then. The publication of an
increasing number of books on the topic, including several published by the American
Psychological Association (APA), testifies to the vibrancy of the field (Emmons 1999; Hill &
Hood 1999; Koenig 1998; Miller 1999; Pargament 1997; Richards & Bergin 1997, 2000;
Shafranske 1996). Whereas the more applied areas of psychology such as clinical,
counseling, and health have taken the lead in examining links between religion and
psychological, physical, and interpersonal functioning, basic subfields are also recognizing
that spiritual and religious influences may be profoundly important (e.g., Emmons &
McCullough 1999, Paloutzian & Kirkpatrick 1995). Because of this rapid growth, this work
cannot provide comprehensive coverage of recent developments for all topics in the
A literature search using the psychinfo database for the period 1988-2001 returned 1198
citations for the term religion and 777 citations for spirituality. This review, therefore, must
of necessity be selective rather than exhaustive. Because the clinical psychology of religion
has received a great deal of attention, we have chosen to highlight less-well-publicized areas
social psychology, which are new and not already documented by comprehensive summary
sources elsewhere. This work has several purposes: to document the various trajectories that
the psychology of religion has had during the previous century, to explain some of the
reasons for the trends that have been observed, to illustrate how all of the topics within the
psychology of religion are extensions of and feedback to the overall body of theory and the
database from general psychology, and to sketch the newest lines of emerging research that
2
show promise of contributing significantly to psychology during the next few years.
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 3
CHAPTER TWO
In psychology’s early days, at a time when all psychological thinking was fresh and new, and
3
when theory, research methods, statistical tools, and sub-disciplines within this now immense
and rich field were not even dreamed of in their modern form, those who were pioneering
this field (Hall 1904, 1917; James 1902; Starbuck 1899; see also Vande Kemp 1992) took it
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 4
as a serious part of their work to study the psychological aspects of human religiousness. The
challenge for the next century of psychologists (i.e., us) is to follow this example and do what
they began to do— come to an understanding of the psychological bases of religious belief,
experience, and behavior, with the goal of applying this knowledge for human good. The
attention to this topic by the generation of psychologists who came after those early pioneers
declined from approximately the mid 1920s until the mid 1960s. Several intradisciplinary
reasons for this have been suggested (Paloutzian 1996). These include but are not limited to
the establishment of scientific psychology after the model of physics, the separation of
psychology departments from their former home in philosophy departments and the tendency
by psychologists to stay away from “taboo” topics that might be considered too philosophical
or too theological. However, during this period there were writings by what might be called
the “grand theorists” of religion (Freud 1927, Jung 1938; see Wulff 1997 for a complete
presentation of these), but these writings did little to advance the psychology of religion in
the stricter, data-based sense. That is, these were overarching theories of human nature that
were attempts to explain everything, including religiousness. Although they are rich ideas
about what processes may underlie religiousness, they did little to feed the quantitative
The impressive flowering and maturing of the discipline as we know it today is embedded in
the co-occurrence of several factors. Most notably, the re-emergence of the field was partly
due to a generation effect, just as the early work was done by leaders who invested efforts in
this topic (Wulff 1998), a new group of psychologists emerged whose concerns included
issues of social relevance and whose view of psychology was expansive. The social
upheavals of the 1960s made them aware of the need to use their psychological training to
4
study real-life issues such as violence, aggression, prejudice, sexism—to tackle the big
problems (Hester 1998). Religion, among the most powerful of all social forces and here as
long as there have been human beings [e.g., it has been suggested that humans be thought of
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 5
as Homo religiosus because religion has been present as long as there have been Homo
sapiens (Albright & Ashbrook 2001)] and showing no sign of going away, is among them.
Following the lead of Gordon Allport, in which religiousness was found to be related in
important but non-obvious ways to racial prejudice (Allport 1954, Allport & Ross 1967), the
CHAPTER THREE
THE UPSURGE OF THE PAST QUARTER CENTURY
The psychology of religion re-emerged as a full-force, leading-edge research area that
5
contributes new knowledge, data, and professional activity to the rest of psychology and this
is apparent upon examination of the recent trends in the publication of textbooks and journal
religion, the establishment of new journals, books on clinical and health issues, and the
development of psychology of religion research that interfaces the theory and topics of the
mainstream discipline. These trends became visible after the establishment of APA Division
36, Psychology of Religion, in 1976. One of the most obvious evidences of the development
of an area of scholarship can be seen by examining the numbers and frequency of books that
are published in that area. Textbooks, in particular, are a benchmark source of data because
they serve the purpose of summarizing research and they reflect activity in a field. Prior to
1982 one could look far and wide for current books on the psychology of religion and come
up empty-handed. No recently published books existed. Things changed quickly during the
Batson & Ventis (1982), Paloutzian (1983), Spilka et al. (1985), and Brown (1987), and
Wulff’s (1991) book was in press before the decade was out. The 1990s saw this trend
replicate and expand. Some of the 1980s books came out in second editions, including
Batson et al. (1993), Paloutzian (1996), Hood et al. (1996), and Wulff (1997), and other
books were added to the list (Beit-Hallahmi & Argyle 1997, Pargament 1997, Spilka &
McIntosh 1997). This trend continues as the new century begins (Argyle 2000, Loewenthal
2000). Also, for the first time separate introductory chapters were included in general
psychology textbooks (Santrock & Paloutzian 1997, 2000). This flourishing of textbooks
feeds the increased teaching of psychology of religion (Hester 2002) and documents the
Textbooks do not make for major contributions to a field on their own, however. They
depend on the quality and visibility of the research on which they are based. During the past
25 years psychology of religion material has appeared with increasing frequency in high-end
6
journals. In addition, and added to the already existing psychology of religion journals such
as the Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion and the Review of Religious Research, new
for the Psychology of Religion (established in 1990) is published in the United States, and the
other, Mental Health, Religion, and Culture (established in 1998) is published in the United
Kingdom. To complement the function served by journals, the annual series Research in the
Social Scientific Study of Religion (JAI Press, Inc., established in 1990) and the topic’s first
Annual Review chapter (Gorsuch 1988) appeared, as did a chapter on religion and health
(Chatters 2000). Finally, special issues of leading journals are appearing that focus on
religious influences on personal and societal well-being (Paloutzian & Kirkpatrick 1995),
religion in the psychology of personality (Emmons & McCullough 1999), religion and the
family (Parke 2001), religion and adult development (Sinnott 2001), and religion as a
meaning system (Silberman 2003). These trends make it clear that individual researchers are
including religious dimensions in various aspects of their work and those journals of the
Closely related to this is the upsurge in the publication of specialized professional and
postgraduate-level books, both those that concern the religious aspects of applied work and
those that are handbooks on a specialized topic. For example, in 1996 the APA launched its
book series on religious issues in clinical practice and shortly thereafter published a lead
article on religion in the APA Monitor (Clay 1996). This so far has produced comprehensive
handbooks focusing on religion and clinical practice (Shafranske 1996), spiritual strategy for
counseling and psychotherapy (Richards & Bergin 1997), psychotherapy with religiously di-
verse people (Richards & Bergin 2000), and spirituality and treatment (Miller, 19996).
The same trend is occurring in psychiatry (Bhugra 1996, Boehnlein 2000) and from the
7
and psychodynamics (Malony & Spilka 1991). Finally, comprehensive handbooks and
monographs have appeared on religious experience (Hood 1995) and conversion (Malony &
Southard 1992, Rambo 1993), on religion and mental health (Koenig 1998) and physical
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 8
health (Koenig et al. 2001, Plante & Sherman 2001), cognitive science (Andresen 2001),
children’s religious cognition (Rosengren et al. 2000), emotion (Corrigan et al. 2000), and
spirituality in organizations (Giacalone & Jurkiewicz 2002). This impressive body of mate-
rial has emerged in less than a decade and documents the increasing attention to spirituality
CHAPTER FOUR
PROGRESS IN CONCEPTUALIZING RELIGION AND SPIRITUALITY
Our review of developments within substantive areas of the psychology of religion begins
8
with the swelling literature on various meanings that the terms religion and spirituality have
taken on. In order for progress to occur in a scientific discipline, there must be a minimum of
consensus concerning the meaning of core constructs and their measurement. Agreement on
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 9
the meaning of spirituality and religion is in short supply, as the religious landscape in the
broader culture and in psychology is changing with a new breed of spirituality that is often
distinct from traditional conceptions of religion (Hill 1999). Adding to the mayhem, religious
and spiritual variables are increasingly being included in experimental and epidemiological
studies. Yet how religion and spirituality are conceived and measured vary from study to
study.
Over the past decade, there has been arguably more printed devoted to conceptualizing
religion and spirituality than to any other topic in the psychology of religion. It has become
fashionable, both culturally and in the scientific literature, to differentiate between the
spiritual and the religious. Psychologists have exerted as much effort as anyone debating the
meaning of these terms. The noun “spirit” and the adjective “spiritual” are being used to refer
to an ever increasing range of experiences rather than being reserved for those occasions of
use that specifically imply the existence of nonmaterial forces or persons. Conceptions of
spirituality do not always have a transcendent reference point, a fact that has led to much
confusion over its meaning in research contexts. Most contemporary meanings of spirituality
“mysterious energy” and its ultimate aim is compassion. It is unclear how these qualities
this broad are even thematically in keeping with the origins of the term. Careful linguistic
9
2002).
There has also been no shortage of attempts to define religion. One of the best and simplest
as “a covenant faith community with teachings and narratives that enhance the search for the
sacred and encourage morality”. Religions are rooted in authoritative spiritual traditions that
transcend the person and point to larger realities within which the person is embedded.
Spiritualities may be contextualized within faith communities though they need not be.
Whereas some have argued that the movement toward spirituality represents a movement
away from traditional religion (Elkins 2001), others contend that the increased emphasis on
spirituality indicates an increased respect for the inner, contemplative practices of traditional
Zinnbauer et al. (1999) and Hill et al. (2000) systematically reviewed the evolving meanings
of the terms religion and spirituality. Achieving some degree of definitional clarity is
desirable, though not necessarily essential for scientific progress and the establishment of a
cumulative knowledge base. After all, many disciplines have failed to provide a core
consensual definition and have flourished in spite of definitional lacunae. The schism
between religion and spirituality is a recent occurrence (Hill et al. 2000) and the two
concepts are as much identified with their overlap as with what divides them. Zinnbauer et
al. (1999) posited “a search for the sacred” as the common ground between religion and
spirituality. They suggest that a dynamic view of spirituality and religion centered on a
search process offers considerable potential for understanding the influence of the spiritual
and religious realm in everyday life. The sacred core is what is central to both religious and
spiritual experience. Building upon this definition, Pargament (1999) has argued that
and responsibilities with sacredness opens new avenues for empirical exploration. For
example, Mahoney et al. (1999) found that when marital partners viewed their relationship as
10
imbued with divine qualities, they reported greater levels of marital satisfaction, more
constructive problem solving behaviors, decreased marital conflict, and greater commitment
to the relationship, than couples who did not see their marriage in a sacred light. Similarly,
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 11
Tarakeshwar et al. (2002) found that a strong belief that nature is sacred was associated with
greater pro- environmental beliefs and a greater willingness to protect the environment. This
finding is notable in that other studies have found conventional measures of religiousness to
One of the most important papers to appear on the topic is the review by Hill and associates
(Hill et al. 2000). On the basis of both historical considerations and a growing empirical
literature, the authors caution against viewing spirituality and religiousness as incompatible
and suggest that the common tendency to polarize the terms simply as individual versus
institutional or “good” versus “bad” is not fruitful for future research. Also cautioning
against the use of restrictive, narrow definitions or overly broad definitions that can rob
either construct of its distinctive characteristics, the authors propose a set of criteria that
recognizes the constructs’ conceptual similarities and dissimilarities. Both religion and
spirituality include the subjective feelings, thoughts, and behaviors that arise from a search
for the sacred. The term “search” refers to attempts to identify, articulate, maintain, or
transform. The term “sacred” refers to a divine being, divine object, ultimate reality, or
Ultimate Truth as perceived by the individual (p. 68). However, religion may or may not also
include the search for nonsacred goals such as social identity or health in the context the
search for the sacred, as well as prescribing rituals that facilitate a searchofthe sacred that are
validated and supported by a faith community. Hilletal. (2000) also reviewed a number of
spiritual (e.g., Zinnbauer et al. 1997). The general findings of these studies is that most
people describe themselves as both religious and spiritual, a finding that supports Hill et al.’s
claim that the recent emphasis on spirituality represents an expanding conception of religion
11
rather than a postmodern replacement of it.
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 12
CHAPTER FIVE
PROGRESS IN MEASURING SPIRITUAL AND RELIGIOUS CONSTRUCTS
Nearly some years ago the author of an influential article on the psychology of religion
12
contended that measurement is both the “boon” and “bane” of the psychology of religion
(Gorsuch 1984) and argued for a moratorium on new measures of religiousness. Textbooks
on psychometric theory state that the major problem in psychology is that of measurement.
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 13
religion, like other fields of scientific inquiry, will progress neither slower nor faster than
allowed by current measurement instruments. The 1980s and 1990s saw an explosion of new
inventories in the psychology of religion. With this rapid growth, the need for an
authoritative guide to their use has become more important than ever. In recent years private
foundations and governmental agencies have commissioned panels of experts to identify the
measurement in basic and applied research. The objective of these efforts, and this chapter, is
to make researchers and mental health professionals aware of the existence of pertinent
measures as they design their studies and interventions. Only then will needless duplication
of scales be avoided, and more importantly, progress will accelerate as cumulative databases
A published authoritative reference volume (Hill & Hood 1999) provides detailed
information on over 100 standardized measures of religiousness. These are grouped into 17
major clusters including religious beliefs and practices, religious attitudes, religious values,
Whereas familiar measures often employed in social scientific research on religion are
included (e.g., intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity), so too are less widely accessible scales that
tap constructs of interest in and of themselves (e.g., images of God, spiritual maturity, and
attitudes toward death), and that may also have a bearing on the well-being and health
outcomes being increasingly studied by scientists and health professionals. Work is already
13
well under way on a companion volume focused primarily on measures of spirituality.
Slater et al. (2001) described recent developments in the measurement of spiritual and
appear in the Hill & Hood compendium. They advocate research that examines the
theoretical frameworks. A good example of this is the article by MacDonald (2000), who
CHAPTER SIX
The connection between religion and emotion is a long and intimate one. Religion has always
14
been a source of profound emotional experience. Jonathan Edwards described the function of
(1746/1959). Edwards was so struck by the evidentiary force of emotion that he made it a
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 15
cornerstone of his theology. Love, gratitude, and thankful joy displayed toward God were
among the signs of genuine spiritual experience, according to Edwards. A review of his
contributions (Hutch 1978) suggests that he can still be read with profit.
Watts (1996), Hill (1999), and Hill & Hood (1999) trace historical developments on the
association between religious experience and feeling states. Both Watts and Hill have been
vociferous in calling for a greater awareness of the intimate and reciprocal relationships
between the psychology of religion and the psychology of emotion and identify several
fruitful areas of research that can inform and enrich both fields. An important but rarely cited
book on religious ways of knowing (Watts & Williams 1988) devotes an entire chapter to
documents Christian and Jewish teachings on the mastery of envy, anger, pride and other
potentially destructive emotions. Averill (1996) suggests that fruitful dialogue might involve
Watts (1996) distinguishes between two main notions about the role of emotions in religious
life: The charismatic movement stresses the cultivation of intense positive emotions and their
importance in religious experience and collective religious rituals (see also McCauley 2001),
whereas the contemplative tradition stresses a calming of the passions and the development
the ascetic view (Allen, 1996), which links religion with greater awareness of emotion
(possible emotional intelligence, to use a contemporary term) and the creative expression of
emotion. Emotional regulation techniques that have their rationales in religious traditions can
modulate everyday emotional experience (Schimmel 1997, Watts 1996), providing spiritual
15
rationales and methods for handling problematic emotions such as anger, guilt, and
depression. Positive emotional benefits have been reported for Zen meditation (Gillani &
Smith 2001) and the cultivation of transpersonal states long associated with spiritual and
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 16
religious traditions (McCraty et al. 1998). The literature on emotion regulation in adulthood
(e.g., Gross 2002) might be mined to see what it offers the psychology of religion;
conversely, the field of emotion research might profit from a greater awareness of spiritual
and religious influences on felt emotions (Hill 1999). Silberman (2003) suggests three ways
in which religion as a meaning system affects emotions. First, religion prescribes appropriate
emotions and their level of intensity. Second, beliefs about the nature and attributes of God
may affect emotional well-being, and third, religion offers the opportunity to experience a
A debatable issue continues to be the uniqueness of emotions that are labeled as religious.
Are these a separate class of emotions or simply ordinary emotions felt in religious contexts
or elicited through religious rituals such as prayer and worship? Advances in philosophy of
mind might be helpful here. Murphy (1998) recently argued that religious experience
religious emotion religious are ordinary felt emotions under circumstances that make it
apparent to the person that God or a higher power is involved. In d’Aquili & Newberg’s
(1999) neurotheological approach, experiences of the sacred are partially mediated by the
experience has tended to focused on extraordinary spiritual experiences rather than more
routine religious experiences (Brown & Mathew 2001), so relatively little is known about the
brain’s role in everyday religious emotions. Advances in the affective sciences will likely
provide new ways of thinking about religious emotions and might eventually impact
Unfortunately, empirical work on emotion within a religious or spiritual context has lagged
16
behind theoretical writings of a largely speculative nature. There are only a handful of
studies examining emotion and religion/spirituality, and none speak to the thorny issue of
distinguishing religious from nonreligious emotions. Samuels & Lester (1985) found that, in
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 17
a small sample of Catholic nuns and priests, out of 50 emotions, love and gratitude were the
most frequently experienced toward God. Another study employed a very different
methodology in studying the relationship between emotion and religion. Mayer (1994)
classified emotion terms in the books of the Hebrew Bible and examined changes in the
frequency of occurrence over the eight-century period during which the books were written.
The primary finding was that over time, references to happiness increased; no other emotions
alternative hypotheses, Mayer suggests that this finding can be taken as evidence of the
McCullough et al. (2002) found that people who reported high levels of spirituality reported
more gratitude in their daily moods, as did people higher in religious interest, general
religious orientation and quest-seeking religious orientation were not significantly correlated
with the amount of gratitude in daily mood. These findings suggest that people high in
organizing principle (i.e., people high in intrinsic religiousness) and people who report high
levels of spiritual transcendence, experience more gratitude in their daily moods than do their
less religious/spiritual counterparts. The authors suggest that the presence of gratitude may
religiously engaged people. This study is one of few attempts to examine the daily emotional
17
religiousness here as a person characteristic, as a belief and meaning system that is stable
over time and manifested across diverse situations. Research is just beginning to unravel the
complex causal mechanisms responsible for these relationships between religiousness and
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 18
health endpoints. One particularly promising explanation might involve the experience of
religiously engendered emotions such as hope, love, forgiveness, and gratitude (Ellison &
Levin 1998). Given that expressions of praise and thanksgiving are key components of
religious worship, the physiological effects of gratitude hold promise for understanding
religion’s impact on health, perhaps even as a mediator of the robust association between
religiousness and physical health. George et al. (2000) state that a high priority for future
(p. 113) and contend that spiritual experience is the most- ignored dimension of spirituality.
Presumably this would include an analysis of the frequency and intensity of religious
emotions in daily life. In this vein another promising research program has begun to explore
the emotion of “awe” in both its religious and nonreligious contexts (Keltner & Haidt 2002).
CHAPTER SEVEN
THE RETURN TO VIRTUE
The study of virtue is making a comeback in psychology and is at the nexus of the
18
emotion (Hill 1999, Snyder & McCullough 2000). The positive psychology movement
(Seligman& Csikszentmihalyi 2000) has sought to systematically classify these strengths and
as forgiveness, love, hope, humility, gratitude, self-control, and wisdom appear as highly
prized human dispositions in Jewish, Christian, Muslim, Buddhist, and Hindu thought and
are affirmed universal principles in world philosophies and ethical systems. Basic research as
well as interventions to cultivate these virtues are well under way and have been yielding
fruit. Forgiveness has been an especially vigorous research area, and recent research is
reviewed below. A special issue of the Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology
(McCullough & Snyder 2000) was devoted to a contemporary appraisal of several virtues
highlighting their links to physical and psychological well-being. The capstone article by
Schimmel (2000) explains how those virtues (and their corresponding vices) were
Sandage & Hill (2001) recently articulated an outline of the construct of virtue by drawing
on moral philosophy and recent social science research related to virtue. They suggest six
dimensions for the definition of virtue. These include the understanding that virtues: (a)
integrate ethics and health; (b) are embodied traits of character; (c) are sources of human
strength and resilience; (d) are embedded within a cultural context and community; (e)
contribute to a sense of meaningful life purpose; and (f) are grounded in the cognitive
capacity for wisdom. Perhaps the most significant point of tension is whether virtues are
construed as universal or culturally embedded. These categories are not necessarily mutually
exclusive. It seems possible that a particular virtue (e.g., forgiveness) might be universally
valued but still locally embedded in specific cultural institutions and rituals. This would
mean that forgiveness might be expressed or even defined differently in various cultural
19
Individualistic models of forgiveness would tend to construe forgiveness as a personal
literature on repentance (Exline & Baumeister 2000) and seeking forgiveness from others
(Sandage et al. 2000). Research programs have been rapidly developing around several
specific virtues. We focus on three that have roots in each the major religions of the world:
GRATITUDE Gratitude has been defined as “the willingness to recognize the unearned
increments of value in one’s experience” (Bertocci & Millard 1963, p. 389) and “an estimate
of gain coupled with the judgment that someone else is responsible for that gain” (Solomon
1977, p. 316). At its core, gratitude is an emotional response to a gift. It is the appreciation
felt after one has been the beneficiary of an altruistic act. Some of the most profound
reported experiences of gratitude can be religiously based or associated with reverent wonder
toward an acknowledgment of the universe (Goodenough 1998) including the perception that
life itself is a gift. In the great monotheistic religions of the world the concept of gratitude
permeates texts, prayers, and teachings. Worship with gratitude to God for the many gifts
and mercies are common themes, and believers are urged to develop this quality. A religious
framework thus provides the backdrop for experiences and expressions of gratitude.
McCullough and colleagues (McCullough et al. 2001) reviewed the classical moral writings
on gratitude and synthesized them with contemporary empirical findings. They suggest that
the positive emotion of gratitude has three moral functions: It serves as (a) a moral barometer
(an affective readout that is sensitive to a particular type of change in one’s social
relationships, the provision of a benefit by another moral agent that enhances one’s well-
being), (b) a moral motivator (prompting grateful people to behave prosocially), and (c) a
moral reinforcer (which increases the likelihood of future benevolent actions). McCullough
20
et al. (2002) found that measures of gratitude as a disposition were positively correlated with
nearly all of the measures of spirituality and religiousness, including spiritual transcendence,
related to measures of spiritual and religious tendencies. Although these correlations were
not large (i.e., few of them exceeded r = 0.30), they suggest that spiritually or religiously
inclined people have a stronger disposition to experience gratitude than do their less
gratitude, but it is also conceivable that gratitude facilitates the development of religious and
spiritual interests (Allport et al. 1948) or that the association of gratitude and spiritual-
ity/religiousness is caused by extraneous variables yet to be identified. The fact that the
correlations of gratitude with these affective, prosocial, and spiritual variables were obtained
using both self-reports and peer reports of the grateful disposition suggests that these
associations are substantive and not simply the product of one- method biases in
measurement. This study may be also be useful for explaining why religiously involved
people are at a lower risk for depressive symptoms and other mental health difficulties.
theological inquiry for thousands of years. Indeed, most religious accounts of optimal human
functioning include the capacity to seek forgiveness and grant forgiveness as key elements of
social science construct has also generated popular and clinical interest and for the most part
has been considered apart from the psychology of religion by mainstream psychology (for
reviews, see Enright & Fitzgibbons 2000, Fincham 2000, McCullough et al. 2000, Witvliet et
al. 2001). The scientific literature on forgiveness is growing rapidly across a number of areas
developing measures of dispositional forgiveness (Berry et al. 2001, Brown et al. 2001), (b)
21
(Berry & Worthington 2001, Farrow etal. 2001, Seybold et al. 2001, Toussaint et al. 2001,
Witvliet et al. 2001), (c) exploring the dispositional and situational correlates of forgiveness
(Maltby et al. 2001, McCullough et al. 1997, Mullet et al. 2002, Sandage et al. 2000), and (d)
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 22
examining the mental health and interpersonal benefits of forgiveness (e.g., Coyle & Enright
1997, Huang & Enright 2000). Few studies have examined links between religion and
forgiveness (for exceptions see Edwards et al. 2002, McCullough & Worthington 1999,
contexts. The chapter by Rye and associates in the McCullough et al. 2000 volume is a
There has been limited empirical research on forgiveness published to date that has
investigated: (a) forgiveness in non-Western contexts (for valuable exceptions, see Huang &
Enright 2000, Park & Enright 1997) or (b) ethnic or cultural variables related to forgiveness.
.
McCullough (2001) makes several recommendations for future research and theory on
forgiveness. These include the need for a greater focus on psychological mechanisms that
allow dispositionally inclined people to forgive transgressions against them, the need to
examine contextualized goals and strivings (Emmons, 2000), and appraisals of these goals,
and the need for more sophisticated theorizing on the place of forgiveness within broader
models of the person. Important clues might be gleaned from the self-regulation literature,
particularly with regard to how religious ideologies that emphasize forgiveness can become
22
translated into effective thought-action sequences and then protected from competing
intentions (Emmons etal. 1993). With regard to the place of forgiveness in personality,
traits that underlie prosocial tendencies and can account for individual differences in the
HUMILITY. Since medieval times, pride has been one of the deadly sins, and some have
argued that pride is the parent of all the vices (Schimmel 1997). Humility, as the antidote to
pride, is the realistic appraisal of one’s strengths and weaknesses— neither overestimating
one’s strengths and weaknesses, thinking oneself no better or no worse than others, and being
There is little direct research on humility. Researchers who have advocated for the benefits of
literature Baumeister (1998, Baumeister et al. 1996) found that when people focus directly on
enhancing how they see themselves, an artificially heightened and “dark side” of self-esteem
emerges. Bushman & Baumeister (1998) found that people who had the highest opinion of
themselves were also the most aggressive after being criticized for the poor quality of a
written essay. Those with high self-esteem are also more likely to be antagonistic if their
view of themselves is threatened, such as being told that one has failed an aptitude test
(Heatherton & Vohs 2000). In response to such findings, Baumeister (1998) and Baumeister
et al. (1996) concluded that, the claims that enhancing self-esteem was the cure to many
There is an urgent need for tools to measure humility and for studies that examine its real-
world consequences. Too much of what is assumed about humility is inferred from research
23
on related constructs. Tangney (2000) described the challenges in developing self-report
measures of humility. In a cleverly designed experiment Exline et al. (2000) found that
writing about a time in which they felt humble enabled participants to delay defecting in a
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 24
Prisoner’s Dilemma game, relative to both a pride and a control condition. Rowatt et al.
themselves on positive attributes and their evaluations of others on those same attributes.
Evidence was found for a “holier than thou effect”: Participants rated themselves to be more
adherent to biblical commandments than others, a tendency that was positively correlated
with intrinsic religiousness. This general evaluation bias also held for nonreligious attributes,
leading the authors to conclude that religious individuals may not necessarily adhere to the
CHAPTER EIGHT
Personality psychology has had a longstanding relationship with the psychology of religion.
Kirkpatrick (1999) noted that personality psychology provides a natural home for the study
of religion and spirituality in that a concernwith the transcendent is an inherent part of what it
means to be human. Emmons (1999) argued that personality theory and theology ought to be
natural allies; both are concerned, ultimately, with what it means to be a human being. Much
24
progress has been made at the interface of personality psychology and the psychology of
religion, as personality researchers from diverse theoretical positions have begun to view
religion as a fruitful topic for empirical study. A recent special issue of the Journal
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 25
ofPersonality (Emmons & McCullough 1999) highlighted diverse ways in which religious
and spiritual issues impact personality traits and processes and vice-versa.
Evidence is accruing that spirituality may represent a heretofore unacknowledged sixth major
dimension of personality (MacDonald 2000, Piedmont 1999a). Other recent research has
similarly noted that spirituality and religiousness are omitted from structural models of
personality that are developed around the five factor model (FFM) (Saucier & Goldberg
1998). Piedmont (1999b) demonstrated the value of the FFM for advancing the scientific
study of religion. He suggests that the FFM can provide an empirical reference point for
evaluating the development of new measures of religiousness and for evaluating the meaning
of existing measures. Ozer & Reise (1994) advise that personality researchers routinely
correlate their particular measure with the FFM. Given the proliferation of measurement
instruments in the psychology of religion, researchers would do well to heed this advice.
Religion and the ffm The FFM offers a starting point for exploring the relationship between
(see Digman 1990, McCrae & Costa 1999). There is a growing literature on the Big Five
traits and religiousness. MacDonald (2000) found somewhat different patterns of correlations
across the Big Five depending upon the domain of spirituality examined. A factor labeled
extraversion and openness only. A recent meta-analytic review (Saroglou 2002) reported that
low psychoticism (in Eysenck’s model), whereas it is unrelated to the other Big Five traits.
25
One other generalization that appears warranted is that openness tends to be negatively
correlated with more fundamentalist measures of religiousness. McCrae & Costa’s (1999)
model of personality may prove useful for understanding how basic trait tendencies are
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 26
channeled into characteristic adaptations that include culturally conditioned religious and
CHAPER NINE
Personality psychology can introduce new units of analysis for empirically examining
religiousness and spirituality in people’s lives and here we describe two recent examples.
to stand outside of their immediate sense of time and place and to view life from a larger,
more objective perspective. This transcendent perspective is one in which a person sees a
fundamental unity underlying the diverse strivings of nature” (Piedmont 1999a, p. 988). In
developing the spiritual transcendence scale (STS) a consortium of theological experts from
26
diverse faith traditions including Buddhism, Hinduism, Quakerism, Lutheranism,
Catholicism, and Judaism was assembled. This focus group identified aspects of spirituality
The STS manifested a single overall factor comprised of three “facet” scales: prayer
fulfillment, a feeling of joy and contentment that results from personal encounters with a
transcendent reality (e.g., “I find inner strength and/or peace from my prayers or
meditations”); universality, abelief in the unitive nature of life (e.g., “I feel that on a higher
level all of us share a common bond”); and connectedness, a belief that one is part of a larger
human reality that cuts across generations and across groups (e.g., “I am concerned about
those who will come after me in life”). The STS evidenced incremental validity by
social support, interpersonal style) even after the predictive effects of personality were
removed (Piedmont 1999a). For the STS to be shown to capture a universal aspect of
spirituality, it would be necessary to evidence that the instrument remains reliable and valid
Piedmont & Leach (2002) have already documented the utility of the STS in a sample of
Indian Hindus, Muslims, and Christians. Support was found for two of the facet scales and
the overall domain (connectedness was not found to be reliable). The STS was presented in
. English, a second language for these participants. This may have created difficulties in
understanding the terminology or the exemplars used, as items lacked relevance in this
culture. Nonetheless, these data highlight the value of cross-cultural research on spirituality
and show the STS to reflect spiritual qualities relevant across very different religious
traditions. However, Moberg (2002) doubts that valid, universal measures of spirituality can
27
occupy psychologists of religion for some time to come.
(Emmons et al. 1998, Emmons 1999). Emmons (1999), following Tillich (1957) among
others, argued that both religion and spirituality deal with people’s ultimate concerns and
developed a research program to identify ultimate concerns and their role in human
personality and subjective well-being. A religious perspective can illuminate the origins of
some of the most profound human strivings. Religions, as authoritative faith traditions, are
systems of information that provide individuals with knowledge and resources for living a
life of purpose and direction. Religion and goals are intertwined in human experience. One of
the functions of a religious belief system and a religious world view is to provide “an
ultimate vision of what people should be striving for in their lives” (Pargament & Park 1995,
p. 15) and the strategies to reach those ends. Religions recommend the ultimate goal of
binding with the sacred and prescribe rituals for its realization. Emmons et al. (1998) found
that not only is it possible to reliably assess the search for the sacred in personal goals, but
that individual differences in sacred goals predicted well-being more strongly than any other
category of striving that has been studied, exceeding those for intimacy, power, or
generativity goals. Emmons (1999) argues for a more inclusive role of religion and
As our review suggests, researchers have made substantial progress in uncovering some
basic facts about the personality contours of religiousness. Missing from much of the
underlying mechanisms responsible for the observed associations. We do not yet know
whether personality influences the development of religiousness (e.g., the tendency to strive
for the sacred, to ask existential questions about one’s place in the cosmos), whether
28
personality and religiousness share common genetic or environmental causes. Longitudinal
data on the relationship between personality and religiousness are sorely needed (McFadden
1999), as are research methodologies for studying the direction of causation between religion
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 29
and personality. We eventually need to get beyond correlating lists of personality traits with
traits that predicted entry into and duration of involvement in an evangelical “disciple-
making training course.” Disciple makers scored significantly higher on the higher-order
factor of constraint (reflecting high harm avoidance, high traditionalism, and high self-
control); this factor correlates highly with Big Five conscientiousness. Furthermore, after 24
months, participants with low harm avoidance and low aggression scores were likely to
remain in the program, leading the authors to conclude that “personality is a powerful
although participants high in harm avoidance initially were attracted to the program, those
low in the trait were more likely to remain over time. This dynamic trend would not have
been observed by simply examining the correlation between personality and religious
CHAPTER TEN
SPIRITUAL TRANSFORMATION
The effects of religious conversion on personality change, was one of the first topics studied
with empirical research methods when psychology emerged as a science over one hundred
years ago (James 1902, Starbuck 1899). The term spiritual transformation is used here to
experience.” Some researchers have begun to use the term “quantum change” to highlight the
profound nature of this religious experience (Miller & C’de Baca 2001). However, it must be
29
noted that one’s spiritual transformation can be profound whether it occurs gradually or via a
sudden experience. Much of the contemporary scientific psychological research examines the
relationship between the self or personality and spiritual transformation. For example,
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 30
Zinnbauer & Pargament (1998) gave a group of spiritual converts, a group who experienced
gradual religious change, and a group of religious adherents who reported no religious
change (all subjects were Christian undergraduate students), measures of stress, life events,
motivation for change, and sense of self. The authors posited that spiritual conversion should
lead to radical personal change. However, they found that self definition changed markedly
for both spiritual and gradual converts. In addition, the spiritual converts reported more pre-
conversion stress and perception of personal inadequacy, more improvement in their personal
Kirkpatrick (1997, 1998) has published two longitudinal studies of religious conversion. In
his 1997 study, 146 women readers of the Denver Post were surveyed approximately 4 years
apart about a variety of religious commitments. Of concern was whether different adult
attachment styles predicted religious commitment. He found that when religion at time T1
attachment style were more likely than those with a secure attachment style to report finding
a new relationship with God by time T2. Insecure-anxious subjects were more likely than
those who had secure or ambivalent attachments to report having had a religious experience
or a religious conversion during this time period. These results were interpreted as supporting
figure for those having difficulty forming human bonds. These findings were replicated and
extended in a follow-up study (Kirkpatrick 1998) in which college students were assessed for
Based upon their systematic review of the literature, Paloutzian et al. (1999) argue that
30
functions of personality (the Big Five). However, they suggest that spiritually transforming
experiences can result in profound life changes at the mid-level functions of personality such
as goals, feelings, attitudes, and behaviors. Indeed, these researchers posit that “self-
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 31
defining” personality functions (such as identity, life meaning) do change dramatically after a
spiritual transformation. Few studies published to date are immune from methodological
shortcomings such as reliance on cross-sectional, retrospective designs and near total reliance
CHAPTER ELEVEN
CONCLUSIONS
Although it is clear that the psychology of religion is alive, well, and growing, two questions
linger: First, is the rest of psychology embracing its knowledge and data and drawing the
relevant connections to its own material as this review would suggest? Second, what is the
psychology of religion’s current paradigm and what paradigm would have to describe it if its
contribution is to unfold to the fullest degree? The response to the first question depends
upon the receptiveness of psychologists in other areas. Emmons (1999) examined this
31
one (Megargee 1997) bemoans this very neglect of the
topic. Nor does the Handbook of Social Psychology
(Gilbert et al. 1997:12) devote any space whatsoever to
religion and social behavior. Out of a total of over 3,000
pages in these three presumably comprehensive
handbooks, less than 1 page discussesPSYCHOLOGY religious OF RELIGION 32
influences on personal and social behavior.
It appears that although there is research in the psychology of religion that is tied to almost
every area of research in general psychology, only a portion of the field especially that
concerned with clinical applications and health psychology, has begun to incorporate the
knowledge from the psychology of religion into its own information base and practices. As to
the paradigm issue, Gorsuch (1988) seemingly reluctantly concluded that even as late as the
mid 1980s, when other areas of psychology had gone beyond trying to find the purest
measure for a concept, the psychology of religion was still in a measurement paradigm.
published (Hill & Hood 1999) and new measures of important variables continue to appear
from time to time [e.g., religious maturity (Leak & Fish 1999), faith development (Leak et al.
1999), spiritual strivings (Emmons 1999)], the evidence indicates that the field has now gone
beyond focusing on measurement as its primary concern. The psychology of religion has
undergone a paradigm shift. It has emerged as a strong research enterprise whose topics
interface almost all areas of psychology, whose scholars produce an impressive body of
research, whose research will further develop internationally and cross-culturally, and whose
importance is only going to increase. There is also much to be gained from an increasing
dialogue and collaboration between psychologists who specialize in religion and our
science, so that developments in the psychology of religion take into account and build upon
advances in these related scientific disciplines. This will need to be accomplished non-
reductively, echoing our concerns raised above. A single disciplinary approach is incapable
32
cognitive science of religion (Andresen 2001, Barrett 1998, Woolley 2000), the neurobiology
of religious experience (Brown et al. 1998, McNamara 2001, Newberg et al. 2001), the
evolutionary psychology of religion (Boyer 2001, Kirkpatrick 1999), and behavior genetics
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 33
(D’Onofrio et al. 1999). With a few exceptions, however, these approaches currently provide
promissory notes, and considerably more research is needed before their full con-
tributioncanbe evaluated. We are sanguine that these developments will ultimately anchor the
psychology of religion as strongly in the biological sciences as in the social and clinical
sciences and will yield new and scientific ways to talk about the human spirit.
We think, therefore, that the field has changed to such a degree since Gorsuch’s (1988)
chapter that a new concept is needed to guide it. We call it the multilevel interdisciplinary paradigm.
This paradigm recognizes the value of data at multiple levels of analysis while making non-
reductive assumptions concerning the value of spiritual and religious phenomena. It is the
implementation of this paradigm that will carry the day, and this hinges on the interaction
between those who study the psychology of religion and their counterparts in the rest of
psychology and allied sciences. The field has made great strides in its efforts to say
something important to the rest of psychology, and we think what has come before is only a
REFERENCE
Albright CR, Ashbrook JB. 2001. Where God Lives in the Human Brain. Naperville, IL:
Sourcebooks
. Allen D. 1997. Ascetic theology and psychology. In Limning the Psyche: Explorations in
Christian Psychology., ed. RC Roberts, MR Talbot, pp. 297-316. Grand Rapids, MI:
Eerdmans
Allport GW, Ross JW. 1967. Personal religious orientation and prejudice. J. Abnorm. Soc.
Psychol. 5:432-43
Allport GW. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison Allport GW,
Gillespie JM, Young J. 1948.
Andresen J, ed. 2001. Religion in Mind: Cognitive Perspectives on Religious Belief, Ritual,
and Experience. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press
Appleton Hall GS. 1917. Jesus, the Christ, in the Light of Psychology., 2 Vols. New York:
Appleton Heatherton TF,
Argyle M. 2000. Psychology and Religion: An Introduction. London: Routledge
33
Ashton MC, Lee K. 2001. A theoretic basis for the major dimensions of personality. Eur. J.
Personal.
Averill JR. 1996. Emotions: here and now, then and there. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 6:8994
Barrett JL. 1998. Cognitive constraints on Hindu concepts of the divine. J. Sci. Stud. Relig.
37:608-19 Batson CD, PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 34
Batson CD, Ventis WL. 1982. The Religious Experience: A Social-Psychological Perspec-
tive. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
Baumeister RF, Smart L, Boden JM. 1996. Relation of threatened egotism to violence and
aggression: the dark side of high self-esteem. Psychol. Rev.
BaumeisterRF. 1998. The self. See Gilbert etal. 1998.
Beit-HallahmiB,ArgyleM. 1997. ThePsychol- ogy of Religious Behaviour, Belief and Expe-
rience. London: Routledge
Berry JW, Worthington EL Jr, Parrott L III, O’Connor LE, Wade NG. 2001. Dispositional
forgivingness: development and construct validity of the Transgression Narrative Test of
Forgivingness (TNTF). Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
Berry JW, Worthington EL Jr. 2001. Forgivingness, relationship quality, stress while imag-
ining relationship events, and physical and mental health. J. Couns. Psychol. 48:44755
Bertocci PA, Millard RM. 1963. Personality and the Good. Philadelphia: Mckay Bhugra D,
ed. 1996. Psychiatry and Religion: Context, Consensus and Controversies. London:
Routledge
Boehnlein JK, ed. 2000. Psychiatry and Religion: The Convergence of Mind and Spirit.
Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatric Press
Boyer P. 2001. Religion Explained. New York: Basic Books Brown LB. 1987. Psychology of
Religious Belief. London: Academic
Brown SW, Gorsuch R, Rosik CH, Ridley CR. 2001. The development of a scale to measure
forgiveness. J. Psychol. Christ.
Brown WS, Mathew RJ. 2001. Neuroscience of Religion and Religious Experiences:
Perspective and State of the Research. Univ. Press
Brown WS, Murphy N, Malony HN. 1998. Whatever Happened to the Soul?: Scientific and
Theological Portraits ofHuman Nature. Minneapolis, MN: Fortress
Bushman BJ, Baumeister R. 1998. Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct
and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? J. Personal. Soc.
Psychol.
Buskist. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Hill PC. 1999. Giving religion away: what the study of
religion offers psychology. Int. J. Psychol. Relig.
Cambridge, MA: Addison Allport GW, Gillespie JM, Young J. 1948. The religion of the
post-war college student. J. Psychol. 25:3-33
Chatters LM. 2000. Religion and health: public health research and practice. Annu. Rev.
Public Health .
Clay RA. 1996. Psychologists’ faith in religion begins to grow. APA Monitor 27(8):1, 3-5
Corrigan J, Crump E, Kloos J. 2000. Emotion and Religion: A Critical Assessment and An-
notated Bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood
Coyle CT, Enright RD. 1997. Forgiveness intervention with post-abortion men. J. Consult.
Clin. Psychol. 65:1042-46 d’Aquili E,
D’Onofrio BM, Eaves LJ, Murrelle L, Maes HH, Spilka B. 1999. Understanding biological
and social influences on religious affiliation, attitudes, and behaviors: a behavior genetic
34
perspective. J. Personal.
Digman M. 1990. Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annu. Rev.
Psychol.
Dollahite DC. 1998. Fathering, faith, and spirituality. J. Men’s Stud.
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 35
Edwards J. 1959. Religious Affections. Ser., Vol. 2, ed. JE Smith. New Haven, CT: Yale
Edwards LM, Lapp-Rincker RH, Magyar-Moe JL, Rehfeldt JD, Ryder JA, et al. 2002. A
positive relationship between religious faith and forgiveness: faith in the absence ofdata?
Pastor. Psychol.
Elkins DN. 2001. Beyond religion: toward a humanistic spirituality.
Ellison CG, Levin JS. 1998. The religion-health connection: evidence, theory, and future di-
rections. Health Educ. Behav. 25:700-20
Emmons RA, Cheung C, Tehrani K. 1998. Assessing spirituality through personal goals:
implications for research on religion and subjective well-being. Soc. Indic. Res.
Emmons RA, King LA, Sheldon K. 1993. Goal conflict and the self-regulation of action. In
Handbook ofMental Control, ed. DM Weg- ner, JW Pennebaker, pp. 528-51.
Emmons RA, Larson DB. 2001. Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychol. Bull.
Emmons RA. 1999. The Psychology of Ultimate Concerns: Motivation and Spirituality in
what we know, what we need to know. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 19:102-16 Giacalone RA,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Emmons RA, McCullough ME, eds. 1999. Religion in
the Psychology of Personality. J. Personal. 67(6):whole issue
Enright RD, Fitzgibbons RP. 2000. Helping Clients Forgive: An Empirical Guide for Re-
solving Anger and Restoring Hope. Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
Enright RD. 2000. Forgiveness and anger-related emotions in Taiwan: implications for
therapy. Psychotherapy 37:7179
Exline JJ, Baumeister RF. 2000. Expressing forgiveness and repentance: benefits and barri-
ers. In Forgiveness: Theory, Research, and Practice, ed. ME McCullough, KI Pargament,
C Thoresen, . New York: Guilford
Exline JJ, Bushman B, Faber J, Phillips C. 2000. Pride gets in the way: Self-protection works
against forgiveness. Symp. Soc. Personality Soc. Psychol, Nashville, TN.
Farrow TFD, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JFW, et al. 2001. Investigating the
functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. NeuroReport
Fincham FD. 2000. The kiss ofthe porcupines: from attributing responsibility to forgiving.
Pers. Relat.
Fish S. 1999. Development and initial validation of a measure of religious maturity. Int. J.
Psychol. Relig.
Freud S. 1927. The Future of an Illusion. Transl. J. Strachey, 1961. New York: Norton
George
Gilbert DT, Pinel EC, Wilson TD, Blumberg SJ, Wheatley TP. 1998. Immune neglect: a
source ofdurability bias in affective forecasting. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
Gillani NB, Smith JC. 2001. Zen meditation and ABC relaxation theory: an exploration of
relaxation states, beliefs, dispositions, and motivations. J. Clin. Psychol.
Goodenough U. 1998. The Sacred Depths of Nature. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
Gorsuch RL. 1988. Psychology of religion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 39:201-21
35
GorsuchRL. 1984. Measurement: the boon and bane of investigating religion. Am. Psychol.
Gross JJ. 2002. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psy-
chophysiology
Guilford Emmons RA. 2000. Is spirituality an intelligence? Motivation,
PSYCHOLOGY cognition,
OF RELIGION 36 and the
psychology of ultimate concern. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 10:3-26
Hall GS. 1904. Adolescence: Its Psychology and Relations to Physiology, Anthropology,
Sociology,, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education, 2 Vols. New York:
Hester M. 1998. The status of the psychology ofreligion: aninterviewwithR.F. Paloutzian.
Teach. Psychol. 25:303-6
Hester M. 2002. Psychology of religion: then and now. In The Teaching of Psychology: A
Tribute to Wilbert McKeachie and Charles Brewer, ed. S Davis, W
Hill PC, Hood RW Jr. 1999. Measures ofRe- ligiosity. Birmingham, AL: Religious Educ.
Press
Hill PC, Pargament KI, Wood RW Jr, McCullough ME,
Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs S, eds. 1997. Handbook of Personality Psychology. San Diego,
CA: Academic
Hood RW Jr, Spilka B, Hunsberger B, Gorsuch RI. 1996. The Psychology of Religion: An
Empirical Approach. New York: Guilford Huang ST,
Hood RW Jr. 1995. Handbook of Religious Experience. Birmingham, AL: Religious Educ.
Press
Hutch RA. 1978. Jonathan Edwards’ analysis of religious experience. J. Psychol. Theol.
James W. 1902. The Varieties of Religious Experience. New York: Longmans Jung CG.
1938. Psychology and Religion. New Haven: Yale Univ. Press
Jurkiewicz CL, eds. 2002. The Handbook of Workplace Spirituality and Organizational
Performance. Armonk, NY: Sharpe. In press Gilbert DT, Fiske ST,
Kanagy CL, Willits FK. 1993. A “greening” of religion? Some evidence from a Pennsylva-
nia sample. Soc. Sci. Q. 74:674-83 Personality. New York:
Keltner D, Haidt J. 2002. Approaching awe: a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn.
Emot. In press
Kirkpatrick LA. 1997. A longitudinal study of changes in religious belief and behavior as a
function of individual differences in adult attachment style. J. Sci. Stud. Relig.
Kirkpatrick LA. 1998. God as a substitute attachment figure: a longitudinal study of adult
attachment style and religious change in college students. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
Kirkpatrick LA. 1999. Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality.
J.Pers. 67:921-52 Koenig HG, ed. 1998. Handbook of Religion and Mental Health. San
Diego, CA: Academic
Koenig HG, McCullough ME, Larson DB. 2001. Handbook of Religion and Health. New
York: Oxford Univ. Press Leak G,
Leak GK, Loucks AA, Bowlin P. 1999. Development and initial validation of an objective
measure of faith development. Int. J. Psychol. Relig.
Lindzey G, eds. 1997. Handbook of Social Psychology. Boston: McGraw-Hill. 4th ed.
LK, Larson DB, Koenig HK, McCullough ME. 2000. Spirituality and health:Fuller Theol.
Semin.,Pasadena, CA. Unpubl. manuscr.
Loewenthal KN. 2000. The Psychology of Religion: A Short Introduction. Oxford: Oneworld
36
MacDonald DA. 2000. Spirituality: description, measurement, and relation to the five factor
model of personality. J. Pers.
Mahoney A, Pargament KI, Jewell T, Swank AB, Scott E, et al. 1999. Marriage and the
spiritual realm: the role of proximal and distal religious constructs in marital functioning. J.
Fam. Psychol.
PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION 37
Malony HN, Southard S, eds. 1992. Handbook of Religious Conversion. Birmingham, AL:
Religious Educ. Press Malony HN,
Maltby J, Macaskill A, Day L. 2001. Failure to forgive self and others: a replication and
extension of the relationship between forgiveness, personality, social desirability and
general health. Personal. Individ. Differ.
Mayer JD. 1994. Emotion over time within a religious culture: a lexical analysis of the Old
Testament. J. Psychohis.
McCauley RN. 2001. Ritual, memory, and emotion: comparing two cognitive hypotheses
See Andresen 2001.
McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. 1999. A Five-Factor theory of personality. In Handbook of Per-
sonality: Theory and Research, ed. LA Per- vin, OP John. New York: Guilford. 2nd ed.
McCraty R, Barrios-Choplin B, Rozman D, Atkinson M, Watkins AD. 1998. The impact of a
new emotional self-management program on stress, emotions, heart rate variability, DHEA
and cortisol. Integr. Physiol. Behav. Sci.
McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J. 2002. The grateful disposition: a conceptual and
empirical topography. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82:112-27 McCullough ME, Kilpatrick SD,
McCullough ME, Pargament KI, Thoresen CE, eds. 2000. Forgiveness: Theory, Practice
and Research. New York: Guilford McCullough ME,
McCullough ME, Worthington EL Jr, Rachal KC. 1997. Interpersonal forgiving in close
relationships. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
McCullough ME, Worthington EL Jr. 1999. Religion and the forgiving personality. J. Pers.
McCullough ME. 2001. Forgiveness: Who does it and how do they do it? Am. Psychol. Soc.
McFadden SH. 1999. Religion, personality and aging: a life span perspective. J. Pers.
Newberg AB. 1999. The Mystical Mind: Probing the Biology of Religious Experience.
Minneapolis, MN: Augsburg Fortress
Schoenrade P, Ventis WL. 1993. Religion and the Individual: A Social-Psychological
Perspective. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
Snyder CR. 2000. Classical source of human strength: revisiting an old home and building a
new one. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.
Spilka B, eds. 1991. Religion in Psychodynamic Perspective: The Contributions of Paul W
Pruyser. New York, Oxford Univ. Press
Swyers JP, et al. 2000. Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: points of commonality,
points of departure. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 30:51-77
Vohs KD. 2000. Interpersonal evaluations following threats to self: role of self-esteem. J.
Personal. Soc. Psychol.
37