Anxiety in Spanish Efl University Lesson PDF
Anxiety in Spanish Efl University Lesson PDF
Anxiety in Spanish Efl University Lesson PDF
doi: 10.2478/stap-2014-0003
ABSTRACT
Few studies have focused on the anxiety experienced by learners of English as a foreign
language in the context of Spanish universities. This study reports on the findings of an
investigation into the sources of 216 Spanish university students’ anxiety and incorpo-
rates two underexplored aspects in this area, namely, the responsibility students attribute
to different agents and the perception students have of their own ability to cope during
an anxiety episode. A qualitative/quantitative design was used. Results indicated that
the primary source of anxiety related to the speaking skill. Quantitative analyses re-
vealed that learners place the main responsibility for their anxiety on themselves and
that women hold themselves responsible for their anxiety to a greater degree than men.
The perceived coping ability of men and women was ranked below ‘fair’. Theoretical
and practical implications are discussed.
1. Introduction
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
58 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 59
the development of foreign language anxiety (FLA) (Cheng 2002; Horwitz et al.
1986; Price 1991; Onwuegbuzie et al. 1999). Correlational studies have shown a
strong relationship between learners’ self-perceptions of L2 command and anxi-
ety. In fact, it has been demonstrated that learners’ self-evaluated competence is
a better predictor of anxiety level than actual L2 performance in language tests
(Cheng 2002; Clément et al. 1994; Gardner & MacIntyre 1993). This is consis-
tent with the cognitive self-evaluation theory of the causes of social anxieties,
which states that the main reason why people become anxious in social contexts
is not that they lack the necessary skills, but rather that they believe they lack
them and consider themselves unsuitable (Atkinson et al. 1994; Schlenker &
Leary 1985). If this theory is applied to language learning, a student may ex-
perience anxiety when using the target language because s/he thinks her/his L2
ability or competence is weak, thus expects to perform poorly and anticipates
negative results. Several empirical studies support this hypothesis: negative
correlations between self-perceived competence in the L2 and anxiety level
have been found; learners who see themselves as less competent tend to suffer
higher anxiety levels, while those with a more positive perception of their own
performance tend to score low (Clément et al. 1994; Kitano 2001).
Two other relevant aspects of self-perception come into play when address-
ing the study of anxiety episodes: the attribution of responsibility students make
regarding the anxiety experienced and the perceptions students have of their
coping abilities to act in such situations. None of these issues, however, has
been explored to any great extent in the English as a foreign language (EFL)
literature. Only the study conducted by Williams and Andrade (2008) with
Japanese university learners of English has directed its attention to both aspects.
The results revealed, on the one hand, that most students attributed the cause of
anxiety to the teacher or other people and that most of them felt frustrated and
helpless in such situations and, on the other hand, that more men than women
felt they could have a positive influence on the particular situation described.
Another study of interest in this regard was carried out by Matsumoto et al.
(1988: 279). The authors analyzed the responses of Japanese and American
university learners to a series of experiences concerning anger, disgust, fear,
guilt, joy, sadness, and shame. The aim of their research, which belongs to a
different field of study but the results of which are pertinent here, was to exam-
ine the ecology of emotional experience, the regulation and control of emotion,
the subjective evaluation of emotion-eliciting events, and the verbal, nonverbal,
and physiological reactions. Their results indicated that American participants
tended to assign the cause of the event to other people, and Japanese students
tended to put it down to chance or fate. The two groups also differed in their
view as to the need to act in emotion-eliciting actions: the number of students
who believed action could be taken was higher in the American group. There-
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
60 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
fore, as the authors explain, responsibility attribution and coping ability are
interconnected: if a person does not assume her/his responsibility for an event
or holds other agents responsible, the ability to cope may be limited.
Finally, it is important to bear in mind that in spite of the effort made by re-
searchers and educators to help learners tackle anxiety, a certain level of anxiety
is positive for the EFL class. In fact, it has been postulated that the function of
anxiety is bidirectional, that is, the concept can be split into facilitating and debili-
tating anxiety (Chastain 1975; Scovel 1978). The former motivates learners to
face the new learning task and stimulates them emotionally to take the most ap-
propriate decisions; the latter, by contrast, may motivate the learner to avoid a
particular learning task. This implies that a moderate level of anxiety can be a
motivating factor for students (Ewald 2007; Zhang 2001) and even enhance their
learning (Spielmann & Radnofsky 2001). However, when learners feel little or no
anxiety, they may not consider the tasks demanding enough, they may find them
boring or evade their responsibilities as language learners.
2. Present study
This research responds to Hewitt and Stephenson’s (2012: 170) suggestion that
“continued research into language anxiety matters” not only because it has been
proved to be a relevant variable in the foreign language learning process but also
because it is a variable that may allow for action on the part of the teachers. To
this end, this study investigates students’ perceptions of selected anxiety-related
aspects in the context of University English classes in Spain, thereby offering a
new perspective on a long-recognized problem. An open-ended question about
past anxiety episodes and two closed questions about attribution of responsibility
for their anxiety and coping abilities were answered by the group of students par-
ticipating in the study.
No research has been found in the literature recording the different situations
that cause anxiety to Spanish university learners of English from a qualitative per-
spective while also complementing such data with a quantitative analysis of student
attribution of responsibility for their anxiety to different agents together with stu-
dents’ self-perceived coping ability. A review of the literature reveals that only the
study by Williams and Andrade (2008), carried out with Japanese university stu-
dents of English, has directed its attention to similar aspects and has employed simi-
lar analyses. Other studies have focused on learners’ own experience with anxiety
but have addressed it from different perspectives and with varied student profiles.
For example, in Ewald’s research (2007), participants were university students of
Spanish; Fukai (2000) conducted his research with USA university students of
Japanese; Liu (2006) with Chinese university learners of English; and Zhang’s
(2001) research was carried out with Singaporean university learners of English.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 61
Although crucial for an understanding of anxiety and its role in foreign lan-
guage learning, the findings of these previous studies are not applicable to the
context of our investigation; it is therefore of the utmost importance to carry out
this type of research in the setting we describe in this paper. As Wesely (2012:
105) recommends, we need to be able to draw on different types of evidence if
our aim is to further knowledge about language learners and the language learn-
ing process. Since the diversity of learner characteristics, beliefs and percep-
tions is as broad as the contexts they belong to, it cannot be considered that the
situations of anxiety and perceptions detected in a certain group of students are
illustrative of students with other profiles (Liu 2006; Rifkin 2000).
The research revolved around the following research questions:
3. Method
3.1. Participants
The sample consisted of 216 Spanish university students, 120 females (55.5%)
and 96 males (44.5%). Their ages ranged from 18 to 39 (M = 20.39; SD = 3.69).
156 of the students were studying Teacher Training, while the other two groups
of students came from the Translation and Interpreting Faculty (30) and the
Computer Science Faculty (30).
Their language levels in English ranged from B1 to C1: 117 were B1-level
students, 87, B2-level and the remainder, 12, C1-level. English class groups at
university are organized according to these levels, established following The
Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning, Teach-
ing, Assessment (CEFR) (2001), a guideline used to describe foreign language
learners’ achievements across Europe1.
The participation of these individuals was voluntary and their written ap-
proval obtained.
1
The 2008 study by Martínez Baztán has addressed correspondence with the The American
Council on the Teaching of Foreign Languages (ACTFL) Guidelines. Thus, B1 would cor-
respond to Intermediate Mid and Intermediate High, B2 would correspond to Intermediate
High and Advanced Low, and C1, to Advanced Mid, Advanced High.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
62 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
The data elicitation tools used included an adapted version of the questionnaire
employed by Williams and Andrade (2008) in their study with Japanese univer-
sity EFL learners. The questionnaire included four questions which required ei-
ther free or structured responses. For the purposes of the research presented here,
the responses to three of the questions were analyzed. The first was an open-
ended question about a specific situation that had provoked anxiety in students –
this question was taken directly from Williams and Andrade’s paper. In the sec-
ond question, students had to indicate on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
(not at all) to 4 (a lot), the extent to which they perceived four agents to be re-
sponsible for the particular anxiety situation described – in Williams and
Andrade’s paper subjects were given nine options, but could not rank their per-
ceived responsibility. In the third question, participants were asked to rate their
ability to cope in that particular anxiety situation. A five-point Likert scale was
used on this occasion, ranging from 1 (very poor) to 5 (very good), while in Wil-
liams and Andrade’s students were given five statements regarding their ability to
cope on which they had to show their agreement or disagreement.
Furthermore, a background questionnaire, including some demographic in-
formation such as language level, gender and age was also included.
3.3. Procedure
Permission was requested from the deans of the three Faculties involved to con-
duct the survey.
Once the researchers had explained the purpose of the study, the participants
were asked to complete the background questionnaire. Subsequently, the stu-
dents were given the questionnaire and the researchers read the written instruc-
tions aloud, encouraging the participants to ask questions if they did not under-
stand the directions.
Subjects completed the questionnaire in the second semester of the academic
year during class time. Learners were assured that the results would not have
any effect whatsoever on their final mark or on their academic record. The re-
searchers also confirmed that the data gathered would be treated with complete
confidentiality.
Different analyses were conducted to analyze the three research questions selected.
A qualitative analysis was performed to assess the sources of anxiety for
university students during class time. This qualitative analysis was done in five
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 63
steps. Firstly, the descriptions given by students were read by the two authors of
this paper and organized into small units of basic ideas. Each basic idea unit,
together with participants’ original comments, was transferred to a table and
preliminary codes were allocated. A subsequent rereading of the original data
allowed patterns to be more rigorously identified, and answers in which similar
aspects were mentioned were grouped together. Thirdly, the themes previously
generated were converted to more abstract theoretical constructs, each of which
was then broken down into subcategories in order to examine students’ sources
of anxiety in a more detailed manner. Finally, a definitive coding scheme was
designed (Brown 2001). As two codes had to be assigned in some cases, contin-
gency analyses were performed.
As for the items relating to responsibility for anxiety feelings and self-
perceived ability to cope in the previously described anxiety situations, descrip-
tive statistics were calculated. Subsequently, the non-parametric Mann-Whitney
U test was performed to determine if there were any significant differences in
the distribution of responsibility attributions and self-perceived coping abilities
caused by gender. The variable level, taken into consideration for data collec-
tion, was not used in the statistical analysis. Although level has proved to be a
determining factor in learner’s anxiety levels (Arnaiz & Guillén 2012; Liu
2006), it was not within the scope of this research to correlate level and sources
of anxiety.
4. Results
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
64 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
2 contains the sources of anxiety related to agents. It can be seen from the data
in Table 1 that speaking was responsible for most of the anxiety suffered by
learners. In particular, oral presentations provoked the most anxiety, as shown
in the following quotation:
Just having to express themselves in the foreign language was also often con-
sidered a source of anxiety. The specific reason reported in six cases by students
was the lack of preparation time:
A second important source of anxiety for these students involved the teacher (as
shown in Table 2). This category comprised 23.1% of all the anxiety sources
identified and included all the decisions taken by teachers and their applications
in the classroom context. One student addressed the anxiety caused by the
teacher asking her/him to speak commenting:
Issues related to listening (20.8%) and feelings (14.4%) were the next most
frequently cited. With regard to listening (see Table 1), many students claimed
to feel anxious when they were not able to understand either the teacher or their
peers. One student pointed out:
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 65
In the feelings category (see Table 2), the comments made by the learners were
varied. The majority were related to their fear of falling behind, their high
expectations regarding their performance and their reluctance to being
observed, as shown in the following examples:
In their responses, some students mentioned more than one source of anxiety.
Contingency analyses enabled us to observe that of 118 students who mentioned
speaking as a source of anxiety, 27 incorporated the teacher in the description of
the situation and 13 added some type of feeling. In the example below we can
see a student describing a situation in which s/he has to perform an oral
presentation (speaking category) and makes explicit reference to the teacher
(teacher category):
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
66 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
In other cases (10), students identified a situation in which both their inability to
understand what the teacher says (listening) and their fear of falling behind
(feelings) are present as anxiety-generating. One student explained:
The last three situations considered by students to provoke anxiety were related
with reading aloud, with the language learning process itself and with peers.
Due to the appearance in some cases of more than one source of anxiety per
student, as explained earlier, the numbers in Tables 1 and 2 do not coincide with
the total number of students.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 67
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
68 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
responsible for the anxiety situation described to a greater extent than men.
Table 3 also gives information concerning the participants’ perceived ability to
cope. The mean rate participants attributed to themselves is 2.76. No significant
gender differences were detected.
5. Discussion
The purpose of this study was to explore, in the first place, the types of situation
that make students feel more anxious; secondly, to find out who the students
considered to be responsible for the anxiety they felt; thirdly, to examine how
respondents evaluated their ability to cope in those situations; and finally, the
researchers analyzed whether gender determined how students attributed the
responsibility for their anxiety and also the degree of self-perceived ability to
cope.
A number of important findings resulted from this study. Anxiety was most
often associated with speaking, also called output. This anxiety refers to the
apprehension students feel when they have to produce language. High anxiety at
this stage may interfere with students’ ability to speak the foreign language.
Anxiety related to speaking was mainly associated in this research with oral
presentations and speaking in L2 in class. These findings are consistent with
those reported by Frantzen and Magnan (2005) in their qualitative analysis of
the descriptions of the factors that increase anxiety written by 35 USA learners
of French and Spanish.
Furthermore, and also related to the speaking skill category, anxiety was as-
sociated by the students in this study with processing (mental planning), al-
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 69
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
70 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 71
tion. For example, teachers could place special emphasis on guiding learners in
the use of appropriate material that helps students prepare at home so that they
can come to class feeling confident.
Another element of interest in the present study is the fact that it was the big
group context that provoked student anxiety. The small-group context was
mentioned only once. Although students did not make explicit reference to class
arrangements, it can be assumed that the whole group was always present in
students’ minds when they described anxiety-generating situations. In this regard,
it is important to mention the results of Frantzen and Magnan’s (2005) qualitative
analysis with 35 USA learners of French and Spanish that highlight the particular
value of small group work or pair work to create a higher comfort level in class
and a sense of classroom community. The results obtained by Suwantarathip and
Wichadee (2010) in their study on the impact of cooperative learning on anxiety
and proficiency in an EFL class point in the same direction. These authors
interviewed six learners about their feelings when working with peers and all of
them found the small-group work context to be a supportive environment where
they did not feel anxious at all and did not worry about making mistakes
As for responsibility issues, a relevant finding for the language learning
process and for the language learning sessions was that participants gave the
highest score to themselves, and that in this particular case women considered
themselves significantly more responsible than men. The teacher ranked second.
These results point to a notable difference between our study and the study
carried out by Williams and Andrade (2008). In their case, the highest score was
given to the teacher while the students ranked second.
Peers appeared in third place. In line with the findings of Ewald (2007:
130), the learners in this study did not consider each other to be a serious cause
of anxiety, since they are all ‘in the same proverbial boat’.
The fact that these students did not attribute all the responsibility for their
anxiety to their teachers but admitted their own role makes teachers’ and
researchers’ tasks easier. It is definitely easier to help a learner who admits his
failings than to help a learner who does not.
In response to the question about their perceived ability to cope with the
anxiety-inducing situation, students indicated that their ability was slightly below
“fair”. No gender differences were detected on this occasion. In the study by
Williams and Andrade, results can be said to point in the same direction, although
to a greater extent: most students felt frustrated and bereft of resources.
Apart from Williams and Andrade’s work (2008), no other study that in-
cludes foreign language learners’ self-evaluations of coping strategies has been
found, but the relevance of L2-related beliefs has been emphasized by several
authors as potential contributors to FLA (Ganschow et al. 1994; Horwitz 1990,
1995; Young 1991). For example, in the study by Tóth (2007), results show that
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
72 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
6. Conclusion
This study sheds light on the complex issue of anxiety among university Spanish
learners of English. On the one hand, it provides teachers and researchers with
evidence of a wider and very detailed variety of anxiety situations in the
classroom. Although it is true that many of the responses were consistent with the
foreign language anxiety construct designed by Horwitz et al. (1986), it is also
true that the qualitative research presented in this paper permits a thorough
analysis of the situations that may make learners feel anxious and can provide
important insights into identifying other sources of anxiety. Certain issues re-
garding the teacher and the reading skill, for instance, are not included in Horwitz
et al.’s Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS). Furthermore, this
type of research encourages reflection to a greater extent than closed questions
and allows students the possibility of establishing connections between different
sources of anxiety. This is in line with Spielmann and Radnofsky (2001), who
argue that qualitative research strengthens statistical findings and makes the
peculiarities of the language learning experience easier to understand.
On the other hand, inviting learners to describe certain anxiety episodes
gives researchers and teachers access to their ‘no-concerns’. The scant number
of references to pronunciation is a valuable piece of information for teachers
and researchers. Similarly, it could be assumed that those classroom contexts
not mentioned are considered by students as ‘favorable experiences’, but this
cannot be asserted without further research data. Future investigation could
therefore focus on favorable or anxiety-free classroom situations in the context
of Spanish universities. This type of information would enable teachers to
design more realistic lesson plans for their sessions and adapt their classroom
management with the aim of facilitating oral performance and trying to
eliminate classroom practices that hinder success in the speaking of foreign
languages. Understanding university students’ sources of anxiety is an essential
part of understanding how to intervene.
In addition, this paper opens up a line of research in the context of Spanish
universities that could optimize outcomes for foreign language learners. Evalua-
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 73
REFERENCES
Aida, Yukie. 1994. Examination of Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope’s construct of foreign language
anxiety: The case of students of Japanese. The Modern Language Journal 78(2). 155-
168.
Arnaiz, Patricia & Félix Guillén. 2012. Foreign language anxiety in a Spanish university setting:
Interpersonal differences. Revista de Psicodidáctica 17(1). 5-26.
Arnaiz Castro, Patricia, Marcos Peñate Cabrera & Plácido Bazo Martínez. 2010. El efecto de la
planificación en la expresión oral de alumnos de primaria. Porta Linguarum 14. 181-
196.
Atkinson, Rita L., Richard C. Atkinson, Edward E. Smith, Daryl J. Bem & Susan Nolen-
Hoeksema. 1994. Pszichológia. Budapest: Osiris-Századvég.
Brown, James D. 2001. Using surveys in language programs. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Burden, Peter. 2004. The teacher as facilitator: Reducing anxiety in the EFL university classroom.
JALT Hokkaido Journal 8. 3-18.
Chastain, Kenneth. 1975. Affective and ability factors in second language acquisition. Language
Learning 25(1). 153-161.
Cheng, Yuh-show. 2002. Factors associated with foreign language writing anxiety. Foreign
Language Annals 35(5). 647-656.
Clément, Richard, Zoltán Dörnyei & Kimberly A. Noels. 1994. Motivation, self-confidence, and
group cohesion in the foreign language classroom. Language Learning 44(3). 417-
448.
Dayhoff, Signe. 2000. Diagonally-parked in a parallel universe. Working through social anxiety.
Placitas, New Mexico: Effectiveness-Plus Publications.
De Bot, Kees. 1996. Review article: The psycholinguistics of the output hypothesis. Language
Learning 46(3). 529-555.
Dewaele, Jean-Marc. 2007. Predicting language learners’ grades in the L1, L2, L3 and L4: The
effect of some psychological and sociocognitive variables. International Journal of
Multilingualism 4. 169-197.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
74 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 75
Martínez Baztán, Alfonso. 2008. La evaluación oral: Una equivalencia entre las guidelines de
ACTFL y algunas escalas del MCER. Granada. Universidad de Granada.
Matsumoto, David, Tsutomu Kudoh, Klaus Scherer & Harald Wallbott. 1988. Antecedents of
and reactions to emotions in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology 19(3). 267-286.
Northcutt, Norvell. 1999. Qualitative analysis [Unpublished course supplement materials].
University of Texas, Austin.
Norton, Bonny. 2000. Identity and language learning: Gender, ethnicity and educational change.
Harlow: Longman/Pearson Education.
Onwuegbuzie, Anthony J., Philip Bailey & Christine E. Daley. 1999. Factors associated with
foreign language anxiety. Applied Psycholinguistics 20(2). 217-239.
Oxford, Rebecca. 1999. Anxiety and the language learner: New insights. In: Jane Arnold (ed.),
Affect in language learning, 58-67. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Price, Mary Lou. 1991. The subjective experience of foreign language anxiety: Interviews with
highly anxious students. In: Elaine K. Horwitz & Dolly J. Young (eds.), Language
anxiety: From theory and research to classroom implications, 101-108. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rifkin, Benjamin. 2000. Revising beliefs about foreign language learning. Foreign Language
Annals 33(4). 394-420.
Saito, Yoshiko & Keiko K. Samimy. 1996. Foreign language anxiety and language performance:
A study of learner anxiety in beginning, intermediate, and advanced-level college
students of Japanese. Foreign Language Annals 29(2). 239-249.
Samimy, Keiko K. 1994. Teaching Japanese: Consideration of learners’ affective variables.
Theory into Practice 33(1). 29-33.
Schlenker, Barry R. & Mark R. Leary. 1985. Social anxiety and communication about the self.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology 4(3-4). 171-192.
Scovel, Thomas. 1978. The effect of affect on foreign language learning: A review of the anxiety
research. Language Learning 28(1). 129-142.
Spielmann, Guy & Mary L. Radnofsky. 2001. Learning language under tension: New directions
from a qualitative study. The Modern Language Journal 85(2). 259-278.
Spolsky, Bernard. 1989. Conditions for second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Stroud, Cristopher & Lionel Wee. 2006. Anxiety and identity in the language classroom. RELC
Journal 37(3). 299-307.
Swain, Merryl. 1995. Three functions of output in second language learning. In: Guy Cook &
Barbara Seidlhofer (eds.), Principle and Practice in Applied Linguistics, 125-144.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Suwantarathip, Ornprapat & Saovapa Wichadee. 2010. The impacts of cooperative learning on
anxiety and proficiency in an EFL class. The Journal of College Teaching and
Learning 7(11). 51-57.
Tobias, Sheila. 1986. Anxiety and cognitive processing of instruction. In: R. Schwarzer (ed.),
Self-related cognition in anxiety and motivation, 35-53. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Tóth, Zsuzsa. 2007. Predictors of foreign-language anxiety: Examining the relationship between
anxiety and other individual learner variables. In: József Horváth & Marianne
Nikolov (eds.), UPRT 2007: Empirical studies in English applied linguistics, 123-
148. Pécs: Lingua Franca Csoport.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
76 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo
Wesely, Pamela M. 2012. Learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs in language learning.
Foreign Language Annals 45(1). 98-117.
Williams, Kenneth E. & Melvin R. Andrade. 2008. Foreign language learning anxiety in Japanese
EFL university classes: Causes, coping, and locus of control. Electronic Journal
of Foreign Language Teaching 5(2). 181-191.
Yan, Jackie X. & Elaine K. Horwitz. 2008. How anxiety interacts with other learner factors to
influence language achievement: A theoretical model. Language Learning 58(1).
151-183.
Young, Dolly J. 1990. An investigation of students’ perspectives on anxiety and speaking.
Foreign Language Annals 23(6). 539-553.
Young, Dolly J. 1991. Creating a low-anxiety classroom environment: What does language
anxiety research suggest? The Modern Language Journal 75(4). 426-437.
Zhang, Lawrence J. 2001. Exploring variability in language anxiety: Two groups of prc students
learning ESL in Singapore. RELC Journal 32. 73-91.
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM