Anxiety in Spanish Efl University Lesson PDF

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 49/1, 2014

doi: 10.2478/stap-2014-0003

ANXIETY IN SPANISH EFL UNIVERSITY LESSONS:


CAUSES, RESPONSIBILITY ATTRIBUTION AND COPING

PATRICIA ARNAIZ AND JESSICA PÉREZ-LUZARDO

Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria

ABSTRACT

Few studies have focused on the anxiety experienced by learners of English as a foreign
language in the context of Spanish universities. This study reports on the findings of an
investigation into the sources of 216 Spanish university students’ anxiety and incorpo-
rates two underexplored aspects in this area, namely, the responsibility students attribute
to different agents and the perception students have of their own ability to cope during
an anxiety episode. A qualitative/quantitative design was used. Results indicated that
the primary source of anxiety related to the speaking skill. Quantitative analyses re-
vealed that learners place the main responsibility for their anxiety on themselves and
that women hold themselves responsible for their anxiety to a greater degree than men.
The perceived coping ability of men and women was ranked below ‘fair’. Theoretical
and practical implications are discussed.

Keywords: anxiety, foreign language, self-perceived responsibility, self-perceived cop-


ing ability, university context

1. Introduction

Language learning is a cognitive activity that involves encoding, storing and


retrieving information and anxiety can interfere with each of these processes.
Students may find themselves in a divided attention scenario which requires
them to centre on both the task and their reactions to it at the same time (MacIn-
tyre 1995: 96). This, in turn, has proved to have negative effects for the lan-
guage learning process, including lower course grades (Arnaiz & Guillén 2012;
Horwitz et al. 1986; Kondo & Yang 2003; MacIntyre et al. 1997; Saito &

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
58 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

Samimy 1996), having difficulty in producing language (MacIntyre & Gardner


1994), and communicating less with peers and teacher (Kondo & Yang 2003;
MacIntyre & Gardner 1991).
Several attempts have been made to find a coherent way to explain, examine
and present foreign language anxiety. Stroud and Wee (2006: 301) found two
main types: Competence-based anxiety and identity-based anxiety. Learners are
considered to experience competence-based anxiety when they do not feel con-
fident of their command of the target language, and worry that their perform-
ance will be assessed by the teacher or by their peers (Kondo & Yang 2003;
Krashen 1981, 1982; Spolsky 1989). When learners cannot resort to their usual
means of communication, they feel unable to express their true personality and
cannot demonstrate their intelligence to the full (Burden 2004). As Horwitz et
al. (1991) explain, language learners often have mature thoughts and ideas but
an immature second (L2) vocabulary with which to transmit them. The inability
to communicate language correctly and without obstacles causes disappoint-
ment and frustration. This can lead to unwillingness to communicate and may
activate what Dayhoff (2000: 27) refers to as an “anxiety feedback loop”, in
which anxiety is prompted by the worry of being assessed by others in a per-
formance situation. In these circumstances, learners may experience excessive
fear of being humiliated or judged negatively in learning situations. Conse-
quently, learners waste cognitive energy that could have been used for the task
assigned, and have difficulty in concentrating and processing information; fur-
thermore, the foreign language acquisition process slows down (Dewaele 2007).
Identity-based anxiety affects language learners when they are more worried
about their relationship with other people than with their own language abilities.
What makes these learners anxious is mainly their wish to be part of a particular
group and to feel accepted and safe (Norton 2000).
Other authors have focused on different factors to explain student anxiety.
Samimy (1994) and Aida (1994), for instance, studied factors related to the
instructor, such as a judgmental attitude or a rigid style of managing the class
group. Young (1990) presented a list of the activities that students find anxiety-
inducing: spontaneous role play, speaking and presenting a prepared dialogue in
front of the class; being corrected in front of the class and being evaluated nega-
tively by either the instructor or peers. To these anxiety-generating situa-
tions/activities, Williams and Andrade (2008: 186) added three: long silences
that occurred when they were trying to find the right word to express them-
selves in front of the whole class; having to employ simple or broken language;
and delivering a prepared speech by reading aloud.
But apart from students’ language competence, their worry about losing face
in front of peers and certain teachers’ classroom management techniques, as-
pects of self-perception and self-esteem have been identified as key factors in

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 59

the development of foreign language anxiety (FLA) (Cheng 2002; Horwitz et al.
1986; Price 1991; Onwuegbuzie et al. 1999). Correlational studies have shown a
strong relationship between learners’ self-perceptions of L2 command and anxi-
ety. In fact, it has been demonstrated that learners’ self-evaluated competence is
a better predictor of anxiety level than actual L2 performance in language tests
(Cheng 2002; Clément et al. 1994; Gardner & MacIntyre 1993). This is consis-
tent with the cognitive self-evaluation theory of the causes of social anxieties,
which states that the main reason why people become anxious in social contexts
is not that they lack the necessary skills, but rather that they believe they lack
them and consider themselves unsuitable (Atkinson et al. 1994; Schlenker &
Leary 1985). If this theory is applied to language learning, a student may ex-
perience anxiety when using the target language because s/he thinks her/his L2
ability or competence is weak, thus expects to perform poorly and anticipates
negative results. Several empirical studies support this hypothesis: negative
correlations between self-perceived competence in the L2 and anxiety level
have been found; learners who see themselves as less competent tend to suffer
higher anxiety levels, while those with a more positive perception of their own
performance tend to score low (Clément et al. 1994; Kitano 2001).
Two other relevant aspects of self-perception come into play when address-
ing the study of anxiety episodes: the attribution of responsibility students make
regarding the anxiety experienced and the perceptions students have of their
coping abilities to act in such situations. None of these issues, however, has
been explored to any great extent in the English as a foreign language (EFL)
literature. Only the study conducted by Williams and Andrade (2008) with
Japanese university learners of English has directed its attention to both aspects.
The results revealed, on the one hand, that most students attributed the cause of
anxiety to the teacher or other people and that most of them felt frustrated and
helpless in such situations and, on the other hand, that more men than women
felt they could have a positive influence on the particular situation described.
Another study of interest in this regard was carried out by Matsumoto et al.
(1988: 279). The authors analyzed the responses of Japanese and American
university learners to a series of experiences concerning anger, disgust, fear,
guilt, joy, sadness, and shame. The aim of their research, which belongs to a
different field of study but the results of which are pertinent here, was to exam-
ine the ecology of emotional experience, the regulation and control of emotion,
the subjective evaluation of emotion-eliciting events, and the verbal, nonverbal,
and physiological reactions. Their results indicated that American participants
tended to assign the cause of the event to other people, and Japanese students
tended to put it down to chance or fate. The two groups also differed in their
view as to the need to act in emotion-eliciting actions: the number of students
who believed action could be taken was higher in the American group. There-

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
60 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

fore, as the authors explain, responsibility attribution and coping ability are
interconnected: if a person does not assume her/his responsibility for an event
or holds other agents responsible, the ability to cope may be limited.
Finally, it is important to bear in mind that in spite of the effort made by re-
searchers and educators to help learners tackle anxiety, a certain level of anxiety
is positive for the EFL class. In fact, it has been postulated that the function of
anxiety is bidirectional, that is, the concept can be split into facilitating and debili-
tating anxiety (Chastain 1975; Scovel 1978). The former motivates learners to
face the new learning task and stimulates them emotionally to take the most ap-
propriate decisions; the latter, by contrast, may motivate the learner to avoid a
particular learning task. This implies that a moderate level of anxiety can be a
motivating factor for students (Ewald 2007; Zhang 2001) and even enhance their
learning (Spielmann & Radnofsky 2001). However, when learners feel little or no
anxiety, they may not consider the tasks demanding enough, they may find them
boring or evade their responsibilities as language learners.

2. Present study

This research responds to Hewitt and Stephenson’s (2012: 170) suggestion that
“continued research into language anxiety matters” not only because it has been
proved to be a relevant variable in the foreign language learning process but also
because it is a variable that may allow for action on the part of the teachers. To
this end, this study investigates students’ perceptions of selected anxiety-related
aspects in the context of University English classes in Spain, thereby offering a
new perspective on a long-recognized problem. An open-ended question about
past anxiety episodes and two closed questions about attribution of responsibility
for their anxiety and coping abilities were answered by the group of students par-
ticipating in the study.
No research has been found in the literature recording the different situations
that cause anxiety to Spanish university learners of English from a qualitative per-
spective while also complementing such data with a quantitative analysis of student
attribution of responsibility for their anxiety to different agents together with stu-
dents’ self-perceived coping ability. A review of the literature reveals that only the
study by Williams and Andrade (2008), carried out with Japanese university stu-
dents of English, has directed its attention to similar aspects and has employed simi-
lar analyses. Other studies have focused on learners’ own experience with anxiety
but have addressed it from different perspectives and with varied student profiles.
For example, in Ewald’s research (2007), participants were university students of
Spanish; Fukai (2000) conducted his research with USA university students of
Japanese; Liu (2006) with Chinese university learners of English; and Zhang’s
(2001) research was carried out with Singaporean university learners of English.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 61

Although crucial for an understanding of anxiety and its role in foreign lan-
guage learning, the findings of these previous studies are not applicable to the
context of our investigation; it is therefore of the utmost importance to carry out
this type of research in the setting we describe in this paper. As Wesely (2012:
105) recommends, we need to be able to draw on different types of evidence if
our aim is to further knowledge about language learners and the language learn-
ing process. Since the diversity of learner characteristics, beliefs and percep-
tions is as broad as the contexts they belong to, it cannot be considered that the
situations of anxiety and perceptions detected in a certain group of students are
illustrative of students with other profiles (Liu 2006; Rifkin 2000).
The research revolved around the following research questions:

1. Which class situations provoke anxiety in Spanish university students of


English? (Excluding exam situations)
2. Who do students hold responsible for the anxiety episode they chose to de-
scribe? Is there any difference between women’s and men’s perceptions of
responsibility for anxiety?
3. How do students evaluate their ability to act in the particular anxiety situa-
tion described? Is there any difference between women’s and men’s percep-
tions of their coping abilities?

3. Method

3.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 216 Spanish university students, 120 females (55.5%)
and 96 males (44.5%). Their ages ranged from 18 to 39 (M = 20.39; SD = 3.69).
156 of the students were studying Teacher Training, while the other two groups
of students came from the Translation and Interpreting Faculty (30) and the
Computer Science Faculty (30).
Their language levels in English ranged from B1 to C1: 117 were B1-level
students, 87, B2-level and the remainder, 12, C1-level. English class groups at
university are organized according to these levels, established following The
Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning, Teach-
ing, Assessment (CEFR) (2001), a guideline used to describe foreign language
learners’ achievements across Europe1.
The participation of these individuals was voluntary and their written ap-
proval obtained.

1
The 2008 study by Martínez Baztán has addressed correspondence with the The American
Council on the Teaching of Foreign Languages (ACTFL) Guidelines. Thus, B1 would cor-
respond to Intermediate Mid and Intermediate High, B2 would correspond to Intermediate
High and Advanced Low, and C1, to Advanced Mid, Advanced High.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
62 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

3.2. Data elicitation tools

The data elicitation tools used included an adapted version of the questionnaire
employed by Williams and Andrade (2008) in their study with Japanese univer-
sity EFL learners. The questionnaire included four questions which required ei-
ther free or structured responses. For the purposes of the research presented here,
the responses to three of the questions were analyzed. The first was an open-
ended question about a specific situation that had provoked anxiety in students –
this question was taken directly from Williams and Andrade’s paper. In the sec-
ond question, students had to indicate on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
(not at all) to 4 (a lot), the extent to which they perceived four agents to be re-
sponsible for the particular anxiety situation described – in Williams and
Andrade’s paper subjects were given nine options, but could not rank their per-
ceived responsibility. In the third question, participants were asked to rate their
ability to cope in that particular anxiety situation. A five-point Likert scale was
used on this occasion, ranging from 1 (very poor) to 5 (very good), while in Wil-
liams and Andrade’s students were given five statements regarding their ability to
cope on which they had to show their agreement or disagreement.
Furthermore, a background questionnaire, including some demographic in-
formation such as language level, gender and age was also included.

3.3. Procedure

Permission was requested from the deans of the three Faculties involved to con-
duct the survey.
Once the researchers had explained the purpose of the study, the participants
were asked to complete the background questionnaire. Subsequently, the stu-
dents were given the questionnaire and the researchers read the written instruc-
tions aloud, encouraging the participants to ask questions if they did not under-
stand the directions.
Subjects completed the questionnaire in the second semester of the academic
year during class time. Learners were assured that the results would not have
any effect whatsoever on their final mark or on their academic record. The re-
searchers also confirmed that the data gathered would be treated with complete
confidentiality.

3.4. Data analysis

Different analyses were conducted to analyze the three research questions selected.
A qualitative analysis was performed to assess the sources of anxiety for
university students during class time. This qualitative analysis was done in five

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 63

steps. Firstly, the descriptions given by students were read by the two authors of
this paper and organized into small units of basic ideas. Each basic idea unit,
together with participants’ original comments, was transferred to a table and
preliminary codes were allocated. A subsequent rereading of the original data
allowed patterns to be more rigorously identified, and answers in which similar
aspects were mentioned were grouped together. Thirdly, the themes previously
generated were converted to more abstract theoretical constructs, each of which
was then broken down into subcategories in order to examine students’ sources
of anxiety in a more detailed manner. Finally, a definitive coding scheme was
designed (Brown 2001). As two codes had to be assigned in some cases, contin-
gency analyses were performed.
As for the items relating to responsibility for anxiety feelings and self-
perceived ability to cope in the previously described anxiety situations, descrip-
tive statistics were calculated. Subsequently, the non-parametric Mann-Whitney
U test was performed to determine if there were any significant differences in
the distribution of responsibility attributions and self-perceived coping abilities
caused by gender. The variable level, taken into consideration for data collec-
tion, was not used in the statistical analysis. Although level has proved to be a
determining factor in learner’s anxiety levels (Arnaiz & Guillén 2012; Liu
2006), it was not within the scope of this research to correlate level and sources
of anxiety.

4. Results

4.1. Class situations that provoke anxiety

The qualitative analysis corresponding to the open-ended responses in which


students described situations that provoked anxiety revealed seven main thematic
affinities related to anxiety. Three of them were related to three language skills,
speaking, listening and reading. In these cases anxiety was generated in situations
that required having to produce oral language, having to listen to language, and
having to read. The fourth one was related to feelings; it was the fear of experi-
encing certain feelings rather than actually experiencing them that made students
feel anxious. The fifth thematic affinity concerned the teacher; these comments
referred to the classroom management techniques teachers used in class, their
philosophy and how they behaved and addressed the group. The sixth source of
anxiety reported by students was related to aspects of the language learning proc-
ess itself. Finally, peers constituted a further thematic affinity identified: these
comments referred to peers’ behavior and peer relationships.
The anxiety-triggering situations identified are summarized in Tables 1 and
2. Table 1 includes those sources of anxiety related to language skills and Table

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
64 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

2 contains the sources of anxiety related to agents. It can be seen from the data
in Table 1 that speaking was responsible for most of the anxiety suffered by
learners. In particular, oral presentations provoked the most anxiety, as shown
in the following quotation:

[307] Hablar en frente de la clase durante una presentación en inglés.


[Giving an oral presentation in front of the class.]

Just having to express themselves in the foreign language was also often con-
sidered a source of anxiety. The specific reason reported in six cases by students
was the lack of preparation time:

[273] En clase de inglés he llegado a sentir ansiedad cuando sé que


tengo que contestar a algo sobre la marcha, porque necesito pensar lo
que quiero decir. [I have experienced anxiety in the language class
when I have had to give an answer straight away, because I need to
think about what I want to say]

A second important source of anxiety for these students involved the teacher (as
shown in Table 2). This category comprised 23.1% of all the anxiety sources
identified and included all the decisions taken by teachers and their applications
in the classroom context. One student addressed the anxiety caused by the
teacher asking her/him to speak commenting:

[184] Cuando el profesor me pide que hable en inglés en público o que


explique un ejercicio, porque me pongo muy nerviosa. [When the
teacher asks me to speak in English in class or to explain an exercise,
because I get very nervous]
[121] Cuando el profesor escoge al azar al alumno para que salga a la
pizarra o explique algo delante de la clase. [When the teacher chooses
somebody at random to go to the blackboard or explain something to
the whole group]
[122] Cuando me han hecho una pregunta en alto. [When I have been
asked a question out loud (in the big group)]

Issues related to listening (20.8%) and feelings (14.4%) were the next most
frequently cited. With regard to listening (see Table 1), many students claimed
to feel anxious when they were not able to understand either the teacher or their
peers. One student pointed out:

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 65

[252] Cuando me preguntan y no entiendo la pregunta, aunque me la


repitan, y todo el mundo es capaz de entenderla menos yo. [When I’m
asked a question and I don’t understand the question, even if it is
repeated, and everybody but me can understand it]

In the feelings category (see Table 2), the comments made by the learners were
varied. The majority were related to their fear of falling behind, their high
expectations regarding their performance and their reluctance to being
observed, as shown in the following examples:

[141] Cuando veo que mis compañeros se defienden mejor que yo en la


materia. [When I see that my peers do better than me in the subject]
[172] Varias veces que creo que podría hacer mucho más o dar mucho
más de mí misma y no lo hago. [Very often I think that I could do much
more or make a greater effort and I don’t]
[262] Sentirme observada en una exposición oral, no tener claro el
guión que debo exponer. [Feeling observed in an oral presentation, not
being clear about what I’m going to present]

In their responses, some students mentioned more than one source of anxiety.
Contingency analyses enabled us to observe that of 118 students who mentioned
speaking as a source of anxiety, 27 incorporated the teacher in the description of
the situation and 13 added some type of feeling. In the example below we can
see a student describing a situation in which s/he has to perform an oral
presentation (speaking category) and makes explicit reference to the teacher
(teacher category):

[231] Durante la exposición del libro en tercer curso, ya que al no


conocer demasiado al profesor, no me inspiraba confianza y sabía que
no lo estaba haciendo todo lo bien que podía. [In the oral presentation
on the book, in the third year, as I did not know my teacher very well, I
didn’t feel very confident and I knew I was not doing my best]

One student addressed the anxiety involved in producing language (speaking


category) and the fear of making mistakes in the English class (feelings
category), commenting:

[284] Cuando he tenido que hablar en inglés y por miedo a equivocarme


he sentido ansiedad. [When I have had to speak in English in class, and
for fear of getting it wrong, I have felt anxious]

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
66 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

In other cases (10), students identified a situation in which both their inability to
understand what the teacher says (listening) and their fear of falling behind
(feelings) are present as anxiety-generating. One student explained:

[112] Cuando la profesora da una explicación en inglés y no me he


enterado de nada, ves que todo el mundo entiende, menos yo, y sólo
puedo esperar a que alguien me lo explique [When the teacher explains
something in English and I can’t understand a word, I see everybody
but me understands, I have no choice but to wait till somebody explains
it to me]

The last three situations considered by students to provoke anxiety were related
with reading aloud, with the language learning process itself and with peers.
Due to the appearance in some cases of more than one source of anxiety per
student, as explained earlier, the numbers in Tables 1 and 2 do not coincide with
the total number of students.

Table 1. Sources of anxiety: language skills


(N = 216) n % (N = 216) n %

SPEAKING in L2 118 54.6 LISTENING 45 20.8

Oral presentation 26 12.1 Not being able to 27 12.5


understand what the teacher
says

Oral presentation 3 1.4 Not being able to 11 5.1


without time for understand
planning
Speaking in L2 24 11.1 Not being able to 3 1.4
understand because of the
speaker’s speed

About a topic I’m not 3 1.4 OTHERS 4 1.9


sure about
Without time for 6 2.8
planning
Not being able to 16 7.4 READING 16 7.4
express myself
Big group 11 5.1 Reading aloud in L2 8 3.7

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 67

Not being able to give 12 5.5 Reading aloud because of 2 0.9


an answer my bad
pronunciation
Lack of 10 4.6 Not being able to 3 1.4
communicative understand what I read.
competence [Understanding]
OTHERS 7 3.3 Own work 3 1.4

Table 2. Sources of anxiety: agents


(N = 216) n % (N = 216) n %

FEELINGS 31 14.4 TEACHER 50 23.1

Fear of falling behind 18 8.3 Classroom 29 13.5


and high expectations management
Feeling observed + 9 4.1 Teacher’s presence 5 2.3
being afraid of being
laughed at
I feel offended 1 0.5 Unfair treatment 4 1.9

Fear of making 2 0.9 Using new language 3 1.4


mistakes
Fear of lack of 1 0.5 Evaluation 3 1.4
knowledge
PEERS 6 2.8 Bad teachers 3 1.4

Noisy classmates 4 1.9 Teacher upset because of 2 0.9


student’s lack of competence
Poor relationship with 2 0.9
peers No support or 1 0.5
encouragement
LANGUAGE 9 4.2
LEARNING
PROCESS

Table 3 provides information on students’ perception as to who they consider


responsible for their anxiety. Some interesting outcomes appeared in relation to
this question. Participants give themselves the highest score, followed by the
teacher. The non-parametric Mann-Whitney U test found significant male-
female differences on responsibility attribution: women consider themselves

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
68 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

responsible for the anxiety situation described to a greater extent than men.
Table 3 also gives information concerning the participants’ perceived ability to
cope. The mean rate participants attributed to themselves is 2.76. No significant
gender differences were detected.

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of responsibility attribution and coping


perception and results of Mann-Whitney U Test according to gender.
General Women Men
(N = 216) (n = 120) (n = 96)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) U Z p
Myself 2.85 3.03 2.64 4865.00 –2.07 .039*
(1.13) (0.97) (1.28)
Teachers 1.97 2.00 1.92 5587.00 –.39 .693
(1.05) (0.99) (1.13)
Responsibility

Peers 1.75 1.85 1.63 5149.00 –1.41 .157


(1.02) (1.02) (1.01)
Others 1.03 1.05 1.02 5367.50 –1.55 .121
(0.59) (0.46) (0.71)
Coping 2.76 2.78 2.75 5640.00 –.28 .780
(0.96) (0.88) (1.05)
* p < .050

5. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to explore, in the first place, the types of situation
that make students feel more anxious; secondly, to find out who the students
considered to be responsible for the anxiety they felt; thirdly, to examine how
respondents evaluated their ability to cope in those situations; and finally, the
researchers analyzed whether gender determined how students attributed the
responsibility for their anxiety and also the degree of self-perceived ability to
cope.
A number of important findings resulted from this study. Anxiety was most
often associated with speaking, also called output. This anxiety refers to the
apprehension students feel when they have to produce language. High anxiety at
this stage may interfere with students’ ability to speak the foreign language.
Anxiety related to speaking was mainly associated in this research with oral
presentations and speaking in L2 in class. These findings are consistent with
those reported by Frantzen and Magnan (2005) in their qualitative analysis of
the descriptions of the factors that increase anxiety written by 35 USA learners
of French and Spanish.
Furthermore, and also related to the speaking skill category, anxiety was as-
sociated by the students in this study with processing (mental planning), al-

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 69

though to a lesser degree. At the processing stage, students experience feelings


of apprehension when they carry out cognitive tasks as a reaction to external
stimuli. The degree of anxiety experienced in this phase seems to be determined
by the difficulty of the stimuli, the extent to which memory is required and the
way the material is presented (Tobias 1986). This evidence confirms what other
studies have already described about the relevance of providing students with
time to gather their thoughts and prepare their discourse before they speak (Ar-
naiz Castro et al. 2010; Burden 2004; Williams & Andrade 2008).
It is worth noting that only 2 students considered pronunciation to be a
reason for feeling anxious. This result differs from those of Williams and
Andrade’s study (2008), which found that poor pronunciation was a common
source of discomfort. Taking into account that students’ anxiety in language
learning is generally linked to their weaknesses and failures, this result requires
careful examination. The absence of pronunciation as an anxiety-generating
factor in our study can be interpreted in two different ways: it can either be
assumed that students believe their pronunciation to be acceptable or it can be
understood that students, although aware of this limitation in the target
language, do not consider it important. Whichever interpretation we choose,
what is clear is that measures should be taken in this respect. On the one hand,
and as has been demonstrated, Spanish people do not have a very good
command of the English language nor very good pronunciation or intonation
(see EF, English Proficiency Index 2012), therefore not being conscious of their
weakness in this facet of the language is a problem. As stated by some authors
(de Bot 1996; Swain 1995), becoming aware of one’s weaknesses is not the
same as solving them, but is surely a starting point from which to take measures
and pave the way to improvement. On the other hand, consciously ignoring the
importance of pronunciation and intonation when communicating reflects total
ignorance of what being competent in a foreign language involves.
After the speaking skill, the aspects that generated the two other most
common sources of discomfort were the teacher and the listening skill. In many
cases it was being forced to speak that students considered most anxiety-
generating. These results corroborate those obtained by Burden (2004) in his
analysis of 289 questionnaires corresponding to first-year students during
English conversation classes at a university in Western Japan and by Ewald
(2007) in his research with 21 advanced students of Spanish. Williams and
Andrade (2008) have suggested two possible solutions to this problem. One
would be to provide prompts to produce a successful response; another
technique would be to teach learners a variety of utterances such as “I don’t
know the answer to your question, maybe one of my classmates can help me’ or
‘could you help me?” These conversational strategies would perhaps help
students feel a bit more in control of the conversation and would reduce their
anxiety levels.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
70 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

As regards the anxiety caused by the inability to understand a new word or


phrase, the results obtained are in line with those in Burden’s study (2004).
Students’ ability to pay attention to, to focus on and to encode external stimuli is
decisive; a high level of anxiety may reduce the amount of stimuli processed by
the memory or subsequently retrieved and this, consequently, hinders learning.
Another common source of anxiety revolved around students’ own feelings.
Some students found it hard to accept that they would make mistakes when they
spoke and they constantly felt that they were falling behind. Learners used peers
as a benchmark and part of their perceived failure was attributed to the belief
that their classmates’ knowledge of English and ability exceeded their own.
These findings are consistent with the results obtained in other studies. In the
research carried out by Burden (2004), forty six percent of students believed
that their classmates did better in English than them. Likewise, in the study by
Yan and Horwitz (2008), some students referred to the difference in language
abilities between themselves and their peers as a source of anxiety. As indicated
by Yan and Horwitz (2008), this comparison with peers can be beneficial for
the language learning process, provided it encourages learners to work harder.
This fear of making mistakes, together with maintaining high expectations
regarding their class performance and the fear of being laughed at can all be
said to be a result of a desire for perfectionism when producing language. This
connection between perfectionism and anxiety was highlighted by Gregersen
and Horwitz (2002) in their research. Perfectionist students set themselves
unreasonably high performance standards and the impression they make on
others, both academically and personally, is one of their main concerns. One
possible approach to help learners solve this problem would be to introduce
whole-group discussions about their feelings and their expectations. It is the
teachers’ task to help students understand that the class is not only a place for
“demonstrating knowledge and skill, but also for gaining it” (Gregersen &
Horwitz 2002: 570). The acceptance of mistakes by both the teacher and
learners should be a key principle in the foreign language learning process.
Learners should be continuously reassured that although they may be nervous,
these feelings should not make them feel unhappy about their learning. As
Oxford indicated (1999), students would benefit from verbalizing and reflecting
on what they feel to be anxiety-generating situations and observing that their
peers often experience the same feelings. Encouraging students to identify their
fears and assimilate that anxiety situations are transitory may help them to face
those situations more realistically.
Furthermore, as Stroud and Wee (2006: 301) point out, much of the frustra-
tion students feel when they speak could perhaps be alleviated with more time
for rehearsal. Language teachers must effectively communicate to students that
learning a foreign language implies constant repetition and plenty of prepara-

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 71

tion. For example, teachers could place special emphasis on guiding learners in
the use of appropriate material that helps students prepare at home so that they
can come to class feeling confident.
Another element of interest in the present study is the fact that it was the big
group context that provoked student anxiety. The small-group context was
mentioned only once. Although students did not make explicit reference to class
arrangements, it can be assumed that the whole group was always present in
students’ minds when they described anxiety-generating situations. In this regard,
it is important to mention the results of Frantzen and Magnan’s (2005) qualitative
analysis with 35 USA learners of French and Spanish that highlight the particular
value of small group work or pair work to create a higher comfort level in class
and a sense of classroom community. The results obtained by Suwantarathip and
Wichadee (2010) in their study on the impact of cooperative learning on anxiety
and proficiency in an EFL class point in the same direction. These authors
interviewed six learners about their feelings when working with peers and all of
them found the small-group work context to be a supportive environment where
they did not feel anxious at all and did not worry about making mistakes
As for responsibility issues, a relevant finding for the language learning
process and for the language learning sessions was that participants gave the
highest score to themselves, and that in this particular case women considered
themselves significantly more responsible than men. The teacher ranked second.
These results point to a notable difference between our study and the study
carried out by Williams and Andrade (2008). In their case, the highest score was
given to the teacher while the students ranked second.
Peers appeared in third place. In line with the findings of Ewald (2007:
130), the learners in this study did not consider each other to be a serious cause
of anxiety, since they are all ‘in the same proverbial boat’.
The fact that these students did not attribute all the responsibility for their
anxiety to their teachers but admitted their own role makes teachers’ and
researchers’ tasks easier. It is definitely easier to help a learner who admits his
failings than to help a learner who does not.
In response to the question about their perceived ability to cope with the
anxiety-inducing situation, students indicated that their ability was slightly below
“fair”. No gender differences were detected on this occasion. In the study by
Williams and Andrade, results can be said to point in the same direction, although
to a greater extent: most students felt frustrated and bereft of resources.
Apart from Williams and Andrade’s work (2008), no other study that in-
cludes foreign language learners’ self-evaluations of coping strategies has been
found, but the relevance of L2-related beliefs has been emphasized by several
authors as potential contributors to FLA (Ganschow et al. 1994; Horwitz 1990,
1995; Young 1991). For example, in the study by Tóth (2007), results show that

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
72 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

underestimated proficiency may have contributed to a lower L2 self-image and


higher anxiety whereas overestimated proficiency may have led to an increase
in students’ L2 self-image and a decrease in their anxiety levels. This is consis-
tent with the cognitive self-evaluation theory of the reasons for social anxiety
described at the beginning of this paper, which explains that the main reason
why people become anxious in social meetings is not that they lack the neces-
sary strategies, but rather that they believe they lack them and consider them-
selves inadequate (Schlenker & Leary 1985).

6. Conclusion

This study sheds light on the complex issue of anxiety among university Spanish
learners of English. On the one hand, it provides teachers and researchers with
evidence of a wider and very detailed variety of anxiety situations in the
classroom. Although it is true that many of the responses were consistent with the
foreign language anxiety construct designed by Horwitz et al. (1986), it is also
true that the qualitative research presented in this paper permits a thorough
analysis of the situations that may make learners feel anxious and can provide
important insights into identifying other sources of anxiety. Certain issues re-
garding the teacher and the reading skill, for instance, are not included in Horwitz
et al.’s Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS). Furthermore, this
type of research encourages reflection to a greater extent than closed questions
and allows students the possibility of establishing connections between different
sources of anxiety. This is in line with Spielmann and Radnofsky (2001), who
argue that qualitative research strengthens statistical findings and makes the
peculiarities of the language learning experience easier to understand.
On the other hand, inviting learners to describe certain anxiety episodes
gives researchers and teachers access to their ‘no-concerns’. The scant number
of references to pronunciation is a valuable piece of information for teachers
and researchers. Similarly, it could be assumed that those classroom contexts
not mentioned are considered by students as ‘favorable experiences’, but this
cannot be asserted without further research data. Future investigation could
therefore focus on favorable or anxiety-free classroom situations in the context
of Spanish universities. This type of information would enable teachers to
design more realistic lesson plans for their sessions and adapt their classroom
management with the aim of facilitating oral performance and trying to
eliminate classroom practices that hinder success in the speaking of foreign
languages. Understanding university students’ sources of anxiety is an essential
part of understanding how to intervene.
In addition, this paper opens up a line of research in the context of Spanish
universities that could optimize outcomes for foreign language learners. Evalua-

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 73

tion on the part of the learners of self-perceived responsibility and self-


perceived coping abilities should be incorporated in future studies so as to fur-
ther explore the link between these aspects and anxiety levels.
Finally, one main limitation must be acknowledged in this paper. Although
strictly systematic procedures for collecting and examining data were followed
in this research, any interpretation of verbal data is susceptible to subjectivity
and bias. Incorporating additional raters in the analytic process (see Northcutt
1999) would reduce this problem. Despite this limitation we consider that the
data presented here provide a satisfactory account of the role of anxiety in
foreign language learning for these students because they were strictly based on
their own experiences and in their specific language learning context.

REFERENCES

Aida, Yukie. 1994. Examination of Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope’s construct of foreign language
anxiety: The case of students of Japanese. The Modern Language Journal 78(2). 155-
168.
Arnaiz, Patricia & Félix Guillén. 2012. Foreign language anxiety in a Spanish university setting:
Interpersonal differences. Revista de Psicodidáctica 17(1). 5-26.
Arnaiz Castro, Patricia, Marcos Peñate Cabrera & Plácido Bazo Martínez. 2010. El efecto de la
planificación en la expresión oral de alumnos de primaria. Porta Linguarum 14. 181-
196.
Atkinson, Rita L., Richard C. Atkinson, Edward E. Smith, Daryl J. Bem & Susan Nolen-
Hoeksema. 1994. Pszichológia. Budapest: Osiris-Századvég.
Brown, James D. 2001. Using surveys in language programs. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Burden, Peter. 2004. The teacher as facilitator: Reducing anxiety in the EFL university classroom.
JALT Hokkaido Journal 8. 3-18.
Chastain, Kenneth. 1975. Affective and ability factors in second language acquisition. Language
Learning 25(1). 153-161.
Cheng, Yuh-show. 2002. Factors associated with foreign language writing anxiety. Foreign
Language Annals 35(5). 647-656.
Clément, Richard, Zoltán Dörnyei & Kimberly A. Noels. 1994. Motivation, self-confidence, and
group cohesion in the foreign language classroom. Language Learning 44(3). 417-
448.
Dayhoff, Signe. 2000. Diagonally-parked in a parallel universe. Working through social anxiety.
Placitas, New Mexico: Effectiveness-Plus Publications.
De Bot, Kees. 1996. Review article: The psycholinguistics of the output hypothesis. Language
Learning 46(3). 529-555.
Dewaele, Jean-Marc. 2007. Predicting language learners’ grades in the L1, L2, L3 and L4: The
effect of some psychological and sociocognitive variables. International Journal of
Multilingualism 4. 169-197.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
74 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

English Proficiency Index. 2012. EF Education First Limited. http://www.ef.com/epi/.


Ewald, Jennifer D. 2007. Foreign language learning anxiety in upper-level classes: Involving
students as researchers. Foreign Language Annals 40. 122-142.
Frantzen, Diana & Sally S. Magnan. 2005. Anxiety and the true beginner dynamic in beginning
French and Spanish classes. Foreign Language Annals 38(2). 171-186.
Fukai, Miyuki. 2000. Foreign language anxiety and perspectives of American college students of
Japanese in the United States: An exploratory study. Japanese-Language Education
around the Globe 10. 21-41.
Ganschow, Leonore, Richard Sparks, Reed Anderson, James Javorshy, Sue Skinner & Jon
Patton. 1994. Differences in language performance among high-, average-, and low-
anxious college foreign language learners. The Modern Language Journal 78. 41-55.
Gardner, Robert C. & Peter D. MacIntyre. 1993. A student’s contributions to second language
learning. Part II: Affective variables. Language Teaching 26(1). 1-11.
Gregersen, Tammy & Elaine K. Horwitz. 2002. Language learning and perfectionism: Anxious
and non-anxious language learners’ reactions to their own oral performance. The
Modern Language Journal 86. 562-570.
Hewitt, Elaine & Jean Stephenson. 2012. Foreign language anxiety and oral performance:
A replication of Phillips’s MLJ study. The Modern Language Journal 96(2). 170-189.
Horwitz, Elaine K. 1990. Attending to the affective domain in the foreign language classroom, in:
S. Magnan (ed.). Shifting the instructional focus to the learner, 15-33. Middlebury,
VT: Northeast Conference on the Teaching of Foreign Languages,
Horwitz, Elaine K. 1995. Student affective reactions and the teaching and learning of foreign
languages. International Journal of Educational Research 23(7). 573-579.
Horwitz, Elaine K., Michael B. Horwitz & Joann Cope. 1986. Foreign language classroom
anxiety. The Modern Language Journal 70(2). 125-132.
Horwitz, Elaine K., Michael B. Horwitz & Joann Cope. 1991. Foreign language classroom
anxiety. In: Elaine K. Horwitz & Dolly J. Young (eds.), Language Anxiety from
Theory and Research to classroom implications, 27-33. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Kitano, Kazu. 2001. Anxiety in the college Japanese language classroom. The Modern Language
Journal 85(4). 549-566.
Kondo, David S. & Ying-Ling Yang. 2003. The English language classroom anxiety scale: Test
construction, reliability, and validity. JALT Journal 25(2). 593-598.
Krashen, Stephen D. 1981. Second language acquisition and second language learning. Oxford:
Pergamon Press Inc.
Krashen, Stephen D. 1982. Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford:
Pergamon Press Inc.
Liu, Meihua. 2006. Anxiety in Chinese EFL at different proficiency levels. System 34. 301-316.
MacIntyre, Peter D. 1995. How does anxiety affect second language learning? A reply to Sparks
and Ganschow. The Modern Language Journal 79(1). 90-99.
MacIntyre, Peter D. & Robert C. Gardner. 1991. Investigating language class anxiety using the
focused essay technique. The Modern Language Journal 75(3). 296-304.
MacIntyre, Peter D. & Robert C. Gardner. 1994. The subtle effects of language anxiety on
cognitive processing in the second language. Language Learning 44(2). 283-305.
MacIntyre, Peter D., Kimberly A. Noels & Richard Clément. 1997. Biases in self-ratings of
second language proficiency: The role of language anxiety. Language Learning
47(2). 265-287.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
Anxiety in Spanish EFL university lessons:… 75

Martínez Baztán, Alfonso. 2008. La evaluación oral: Una equivalencia entre las guidelines de
ACTFL y algunas escalas del MCER. Granada. Universidad de Granada.
Matsumoto, David, Tsutomu Kudoh, Klaus Scherer & Harald Wallbott. 1988. Antecedents of
and reactions to emotions in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology 19(3). 267-286.
Northcutt, Norvell. 1999. Qualitative analysis [Unpublished course supplement materials].
University of Texas, Austin.
Norton, Bonny. 2000. Identity and language learning: Gender, ethnicity and educational change.
Harlow: Longman/Pearson Education.
Onwuegbuzie, Anthony J., Philip Bailey & Christine E. Daley. 1999. Factors associated with
foreign language anxiety. Applied Psycholinguistics 20(2). 217-239.
Oxford, Rebecca. 1999. Anxiety and the language learner: New insights. In: Jane Arnold (ed.),
Affect in language learning, 58-67. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Price, Mary Lou. 1991. The subjective experience of foreign language anxiety: Interviews with
highly anxious students. In: Elaine K. Horwitz & Dolly J. Young (eds.), Language
anxiety: From theory and research to classroom implications, 101-108. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rifkin, Benjamin. 2000. Revising beliefs about foreign language learning. Foreign Language
Annals 33(4). 394-420.
Saito, Yoshiko & Keiko K. Samimy. 1996. Foreign language anxiety and language performance:
A study of learner anxiety in beginning, intermediate, and advanced-level college
students of Japanese. Foreign Language Annals 29(2). 239-249.
Samimy, Keiko K. 1994. Teaching Japanese: Consideration of learners’ affective variables.
Theory into Practice 33(1). 29-33.
Schlenker, Barry R. & Mark R. Leary. 1985. Social anxiety and communication about the self.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology 4(3-4). 171-192.
Scovel, Thomas. 1978. The effect of affect on foreign language learning: A review of the anxiety
research. Language Learning 28(1). 129-142.
Spielmann, Guy & Mary L. Radnofsky. 2001. Learning language under tension: New directions
from a qualitative study. The Modern Language Journal 85(2). 259-278.
Spolsky, Bernard. 1989. Conditions for second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Stroud, Cristopher & Lionel Wee. 2006. Anxiety and identity in the language classroom. RELC
Journal 37(3). 299-307.
Swain, Merryl. 1995. Three functions of output in second language learning. In: Guy Cook &
Barbara Seidlhofer (eds.), Principle and Practice in Applied Linguistics, 125-144.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Suwantarathip, Ornprapat & Saovapa Wichadee. 2010. The impacts of cooperative learning on
anxiety and proficiency in an EFL class. The Journal of College Teaching and
Learning 7(11). 51-57.
Tobias, Sheila. 1986. Anxiety and cognitive processing of instruction. In: R. Schwarzer (ed.),
Self-related cognition in anxiety and motivation, 35-53. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Tóth, Zsuzsa. 2007. Predictors of foreign-language anxiety: Examining the relationship between
anxiety and other individual learner variables. In: József Horváth & Marianne
Nikolov (eds.), UPRT 2007: Empirical studies in English applied linguistics, 123-
148. Pécs: Lingua Franca Csoport.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM
76 P. Arnaiz and J. Pérez-Luzardo

Wesely, Pamela M. 2012. Learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs in language learning.
Foreign Language Annals 45(1). 98-117.
Williams, Kenneth E. & Melvin R. Andrade. 2008. Foreign language learning anxiety in Japanese
EFL university classes: Causes, coping, and locus of control. Electronic Journal
of Foreign Language Teaching 5(2). 181-191.
Yan, Jackie X. & Elaine K. Horwitz. 2008. How anxiety interacts with other learner factors to
influence language achievement: A theoretical model. Language Learning 58(1).
151-183.
Young, Dolly J. 1990. An investigation of students’ perspectives on anxiety and speaking.
Foreign Language Annals 23(6). 539-553.
Young, Dolly J. 1991. Creating a low-anxiety classroom environment: What does language
anxiety research suggest? The Modern Language Journal 75(4). 426-437.
Zhang, Lawrence J. 2001. Exploring variability in language anxiety: Two groups of prc students
learning ESL in Singapore. RELC Journal 32. 73-91.

Unauthenticated
Download Date | 2/2/15 11:47 AM

You might also like