Dreamed Movement Elicits Activation in T PDF
Dreamed Movement Elicits Activation in T PDF
Dreamed Movement Elicits Activation in T PDF
029
Report
Dreamed Movement Elicits Activation
in the Sensorimotor Cortex
Martin Dresler,1,5 Stefan P. Koch,2,5 Renate Wehrle,1,5 case, dreamed hand clenching. Predecided eye movements
Victor I. Spoormaker,1 Florian Holsboer,1 Axel Steiger,1 served as temporal markers for the onset of hand clenching
Philipp G. Sämann,1 Hellmuth Obrig,2,3,4 and for hand switching. Previous studies have shown that
and Michael Czisch1,* muscle atonia prevents the overt execution of dreamed hand
1Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Kraepelinstrasse 2-10, movements, which are visible as minor muscle twitches at
80804 Munich, Germany most [3, 9].
2Berlin Neuroimaging Center, Charité University Hospital, Subjects were instructed to make series of left and right hand
Humboldt University of Berlin, 10099 Berlin, Germany movements separated by sets of left-right-left-right (LRLR)
3Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain eye movements upon becoming lucid (see Experimental
Sciences, Stephanstrasse 1, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Procedures below; see also Supplemental Information avail-
4Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital Leipzig, able online). Out of six highly experienced lucid dreamers
04103 Leipzig, Germany who participated in the study, two subjects succeeded in
performing the task during lucid REM sleep under combined
electroencephalography (EEG)-fMRI or combined EEG-NIRS
Summary conditions, respectively. As a control condition, the subjects
additionally performed both an actually executed and an imag-
Since the discovery of the close association between rapid ined hand-clenching task during wakefulness. Increases in
eye movement (REM) sleep and dreaming, much effort has blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the sensori-
been devoted to link physiological signatures of REM sleep motor cortex contralateral to the indicated movement side
to the contents of associated dreams [1–4]. Due to the im- were found in the successful fMRI experiments during the
possibility of experimentally controlling spontaneous dream two lucid REM states in one subject (Figure 2). Given the short
activity, however, a direct demonstration of dream contents duration of the lucid dreams, the activation of the sensorimotor
by neuroimaging methods is lacking. By combining brain cortical regions was very specific compared to wakefulness:
imaging with polysomnography and exploiting the state of executed hand clenching during wakefulness (WE) elicited
‘‘lucid dreaming,’’ we show here that a predefined motor widespread activation clusters in the contralateral pre- and
task performed during dreaming elicits neuronal activation postcentral gyrus (pFWE < 0.005). Collection threshold for
in the sensorimotor cortex. In lucid dreams, the subject is both imagined hand movements during wakefulness (WI) and
aware of the dreaming state and capable of performing pre- dreamed hand movements (LD) were set at puncorr < 0.005,
defined actions while all standard polysomnographic criteria with voxel extent 50. Cluster significance was assessed in
of REM sleep are fulfilled [5, 6]. Using eye signals as temporal a volume of interest defined by the activation during executed
markers, neural activity measured by functional magnetic hand movement in wakefulness. The strength of the activation
resonance imaging (fMRI) and near-infrared spectroscopy was evaluated by analyzing the difference between the maxi-
(NIRS) was related to dreamed hand movements during lucid mum and minimum BOLD signal amplitude in the peak voxel’s
REM sleep. Though preliminary, we provide first evidence time course of the respective motor regions. Strongest BOLD
that specific contents of REM-associated dreaming can be signal fluctuations of the peak voxel’s time course can be
visualized by neuroimaging. seen during WE (left hand: 5.62%; right hand: 4.49%), whereas
WI and LD showed mean fluctuations of only 1.3%–1.4% and
Results 1.8%–2.5%, respectively (Figure 2).
FMRI results were confirmed by an independent imaging
Lucid dreaming is a rare but robust state of sleep that can method in a second subject: NIRS data showed a typical
be trained [5]. Phenomenologically, it comprises features of hemodynamic response pattern of increased contralateral
both waking and dreaming [7]: in lucid dreams, the sleeping oxygenation over the sensorimotor region during successful
subject becomes aware of his or her dreaming state, has full task performance in lucid REM sleep (Figure 3; Figure 4).
access to memory, and is able to volitionally control dreamed Notably, during dreaming, the hemodynamic responses were
actions [6]. Although all standard polysomnographic criteria of smaller in the sensorimotor cortex but of similar amplitude in
rapid eye movement (REM) sleep [8] are maintained and REM the supplementary motor area (SMA) when compared to overt
sleep muscle atonia prevents overt motor behavior, lucid motor performance during wakefulness.
dreamers are able to communicate their state by predefined
volitional eye movements [6], clearly discernable in the electro- Discussion
oculogram (EOG) (Figure 1). Combining the techniques of lucid
dreaming, polysomnography, and brain imaging via functional The discovery of the close relationship between REM sleep
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) or near-infrared spectros- and dream reports [10, 11] boosted decades of neuroscientific
copy (NIRS), we demonstrate the possibility to investigate research into the brain state potentially evoking vivid and in-
the neural underpinnings of specific dream contents—in this tense dreams. Neurophysiological studies suggest that during
REM sleep, the brain functions as a closed loop system, in
which activation is triggered in pontine regions while sensory
5These authors contributed equally to this work input is gated by enhanced thalamic inhibition and motor
*Correspondence: [email protected] output is suppressed by atonia generated at the brain stem
Current Biology Vol 21 No 21
1834
plane resolution 3.125 3 3.125 mm2, TR 2 s, TE 40 ms) and NIRS were per-
formed in the early morning hours during which REM sleep incidence is
highest. The NIRS device was a custom-built monitor with 22 measuring
channels unilaterally covering right central sensorimotor areas. Five of the
six subjects underwent NIRS measurements with concurrent polysomno-
graphic recordings during sleep for 1–3 nights, and four of the six subjects
were assessed with fMRI with concurrent polysomnographic recordings for
2–6 nights. Subjects were instructed to signal the onset of a lucid dream by
left-right-left-right (LRLR) eye movements and to immediately start clench-
ing their left hand in their dream. They were further instructed to repeat the
LRLR signal after ten clenches and start to clench the right hand. The alter-
nating hand-clenching task was repeated for as long as possible, with the
LRLR signal indicating each changeover. Whereas a flat electromyogram
(EMG) indicated REM sleep atonia, eye signals were clearly visible and
distinguishable from spontaneous REMs in the EOG, providing unequivocal
temporal markers for imaging analysis (Figure 1). Polysomnographic data
were monitored online by a trained experimenter throughout the recording
session. Immediately after participants awakened, a short dream report was
prompted to verify lucidity and task compliance. Task performance was
Figure 3. Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Topography
considered successful when the following three conditions were concur-
Concentration changes of oxygenated (D[HbO], upper panel) and deoxy- rently fulfilled: (1) the subject was in REM sleep, (2) EOG signals showed
genated hemoglobin (D[HbR], lower panel) during executed (WE) and imag- at least four sets of LRLR eye signals, and (3) both lucidity and dreamed
ined (WI) hand clenching in the awake state and dreamed hand clenching hand clenching were confirmed by the subject in the subsequent verbal
(LD). The optical probe array covered an area of w7.5 3 12.5 cm2 over the report. When the EEG showed clear signs of awakening after a LRLR signal,
right sensorimotor area. The solid box indicates the ROI over the right one of the investigators entered the scanner or NIRS room and inquired
sensorimotor cortex with near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS)-channels sur- about task performance. Alternatively, when LRLR signals were followed
rounding the C4-EEG electrode position. NIRS channels located centrally by a period of about 40 s without any further signals, one of the investigators
over midline and more anterior compared to sensorimotor ROI were chosen entered the scanner or NIRS room and inquired about task performance,
as ROI for the supplementary motor area (SMA, dotted box). thereby definitely awakening a potentially sleeping participant.
During fMRI, two subjects signaled and reported lucid dreams of suffi-
cient length, i.e., four sets of LRLR eye signals and hand clenchings. Poly-
activity over frontal and frontolateral areas when comparing
somnographic evaluation, however, revealed that one of the two subjects
lucid against nonlucid REM sleep, suggesting a neural corre- showed increased EMG amplitudes, which could indicate a transition to
late of this subjectively experienced cognitive control [28]. wakefulness. This subject was excluded from the analysis. The remaining
Hence, it cannot be excluded that neural activity associated subject showed two lucid dreams during which the task was correctly per-
with nonlucid dreams differs from that of dream content delib- formed, verified by polysomnographic data matching the dream report. Two
erately induced by a lucid dreamer. To assess regular dream further subjects reported lucidity during fMRI; however, one was not able to
perform the signaling or task as a result of sudden dream termination,
mentation in more detail, lucid dreamers might reenact certain
whereas the EEG of the other was compromised by strong sweating arte-
simple dream reports and the accordant activation patterns of facts, thereby making any objective evaluation of sleep stage, lucidity, or
lucid and nonlucid dreams could be compared. Although the task performance impossible. During NIRS, two subjects signaled and
neuroimaging of lucid dreaming is currently hampered by the reported lucidity, but only one subject was able to successfully conduct
scarcity of the phenomenon even in trained subjects, pro- the hand-clenching task (in two different dreams). We compared the activa-
mising attempts exist to enhance lucid dreaming frequency, tion pattern of lucid hand movements to executed and imagined hand
movements, collected during wakefulness with both neuroimaging modali-
e.g., by transcranial direct current stimulation [27]. ties (see Supplemental Information for details).
In summary, we provide the first demonstration of imaging
of specific dream contents by using the technique of lucid Supplemental Information
dreaming. This technique could be used to inversely infer
specific dream content from its underlying neural activity, Supplemental Information includes two tables, Supplemental Results,
allowing for true ‘‘dream reading.’’ Similar to first ‘‘brain Supplemental Experimental Procedures, and Supplemental Discussion
reading’’ experiments in the wake state [29], we chose a simple and can be found with this article online at doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.029.
hand-clenching task because motor activity can be reliably
Acknowledgments
located by both imaging modalities applied. The combination
of lucid dreaming with neuroimaging and polysomnography We thank J. Allan Hobson for his advice and discussions on this topic.
is a promising technique to also transfer more sophisticated
‘‘brain reading’’ tasks [30] to the dreaming state. Future appli- Received: February 9, 2011
cations might therefore lead to the imaging of prototypical Revised: August 31, 2011
dream content like visual dream imagery or dreamed emotions. Accepted: September 15, 2011
Published online: October 27, 2011
Six healthy male subjects (aged 21–38 years, mean age 29.6 years) who had 1. Dement, W., and Wolpert, E.A. (1958). The relation of eye movements,
trained and practiced lucid dreaming for several years participated in the body motility, and external stimuli to dream content. J. Exp. Psychol.
study. Whole-brain BOLD-fMRI (1.5 Tesla, 25 slices, 4 mm thickness, in- 55, 543–553.
respective activation map during executed hand movement (A), thresholded at whole-brain corrected pFWE < 0.005, cluster extent >50 voxels, and served as
a ROI for analysis of the WI and LD conditions in (B) and (C), respectively. T values are color-coded as indicated. The time course of the peak voxel inside the
ROI is depicted (black) along with the predicted hemodynamic response based on the external pacing (A and B) or the predefined LRLR-eye signals during
(C). The maximal difference in activation of the peak voxel between conditions is indicated as percentage of BOLD signal fluctuations of the predicted time
course (gray).
Dreamed Movement Activates the Sensorimotor Cortex
1837
2. Roffwarg, H.P., Dement, W.C., Muzio, J.N., and Fisher, C. (1962). Dream 21. Shima, K., and Tanji, J. (1998). Both supplementary and presupplemen-
imagery: relationship to rapid eye movements of sleep. Arch. Gen. tary motor areas are crucial for the temporal organization of multiple
Psychiatry 7, 235–258. movements. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 3247–3260.
3. Gardner, R., Jr., Grossman, W.I., Roffwarg, H.P., and Weiner, H. (1975). 22. Tanji, J., and Mushiake, H. (1996). Comparison of neuronal activity in the
The relationship of small limb movements during REM sleep to dreamed supplementary motor area and primary motor cortex. Brain Res. Cogn.
limb action. Psychosom. Med. 37, 147–159. Brain Res. 3, 143–150.
4. Hobson, J.A., and Pace-Schott, E.F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience 23. Roland, P.E., Larsen, B., Lassen, N.A., and Skinhøj, E. (1980).
of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat. Rev. Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of
Neurosci. 3, 679–693. voluntary movements in man. J. Neurophysiol. 43, 118–136.
5. LaBerge, S.P. (1980). Lucid dreaming as a learnable skill: A case study. 24. Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self
Percept. Mot. Skills 51, 1039–1042. (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
6. LaBerge, S., Nagel, L., Dement, W.C., and Zarcone, V. (1981). Lucid 25. Dechent, P., Merboldt, K.-D., and Frahm, J. (2004). Is the human primary
dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM sleep. motor cortex involved in motor imagery? Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 138–144.
Percept. Mot. Skills 52, 727–732. 26. Munzert, J., Lorey, B., and Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor
7. Hobson, J.A. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of pro- processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representa-
toconsciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 803–813. tions. Brain Res. Rev. 60, 306–326.
8. Rechtschaffen, A., and Kales, A. (1968). A Manual of Standardized 27. Noreika, V., Windt, J.M., and Lenggenhager, B. (2010). New perspec-
Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of tives fort he study of lucid dreaming: From brain stimulation to philo-
Human Subjects (Washington, DC: Public Health Service, US sophical theories of self-consciousness. Int. J. Dream Res. 3, 36–45.
Government Printing Office). 28. Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Tuin, I., and Hobson, J.A. (2009). Lucid
9. Erlacher, D., and Schredl, M. (2008). Do REM (lucid) dreamed and dreaming: a state of consciousness with features of both waking and
executed actions share the same neural substrate? Int. J. Dream. Res. non-lucid dreaming. Sleep 32, 1191–1200.
1, 7–14. 29. Dehaene, S., Le Clec’H, G., Cohen, L., Poline, J.B., van de Moortele,
10. Aserinsky, E., and Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of P.F., and Le Bihan, D. (1998). Inferring behavior from functional brain
eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science 118, images. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 549–550.
273–274. 30. Haynes, J.D., and Rees, G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain
11. Dement, W., and Kleitman, N. (1957). The relation of eye movements activity in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 523–534.
during sleep to dream activity: an objective method for the study of
dreaming. J. Exp. Psychol. 53, 339–346.
12. Wehrle, R., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T.C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D.P.,
Pollmächer, T., and Czisch, M. (2007). Functional microstates within
human REM sleep: first evidence from fMRI of a thalamocortical
network specific for phasic REM periods. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 863–871.
13. Hobson, J.A., and McCarley, R.W. (1977). The brain as a dream state
generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process.
Am. J. Psychiatry 134, 1335–1348.
14. Maquet, P., Péters, J., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A.,
and Franck, G. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-
movement sleep and dreaming. Nature 383, 163–166.
15. Braun, A.R., Balkin, T.J., Wesensten, N.J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R.E.,
Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G., and Herscovitch, P. (1998).
Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections
during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science 279, 91–95.
16. Schwartz, S., and Maquet, P. (2002). Sleep imaging and the neuro-
psychological assessment of dreams. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 23–30.
17. Hong, C.C., Harris, J.C., Pearlson, G.D., Kim, J.S., Calhoun, V.D., Fallon,
J.H., Golay, X., Gillen, J.S., Simmonds, D.J., van Zijl, P.C., et al. (2009).
fMRI evidence for multisensory recruitment associated with rapid eye
movements during sleep. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1705–1722.
18. Boeve, B.F. (2010). REM sleep behaviour disorder. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci.
1184, 15–54.
19. Wamsley, E.J., Tucker, M., Payne, J.D., Benavides, J.A., and Stickgold,
R. (2010). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced
sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Curr. Biol. 20, 850–855.
20. Erlacher, D., Schredl, M., and LaBerge, S. (2003). Motor area activation
during dreamed hand clenching: A pilot study on EEG alpha band. Sleep
Hypn. 5, 182–187.