Goffman Erving. Footing
Goffman Erving. Footing
Goffman Erving. Footing
You only need to read sections i, vi, and viii through xi (1, 6, 8-11).
Goffman is probably the most influential historical figure in the study of social
interaction in America. Not only was his research influential, but he was an influential
teacher, shaping the research of graduate students who went on to become influential
scholars and teachers themselves. In this article, Goffman addresses the ways in which
interlocutors build constantly shifting, variously embedded activities in their talk and
interaction. Shifting with these shifting activities are the projected perspectives and roles
of various participants in the social interaction.
We are reading this because it gives you perspectives and analytical categories that you
can use in your analyses of social interaction for your upcoming assignments. In your
next assignment, for example, you will be asked to isolate the boundaries of an activity in
talk and interaction. The boundaries of discrete activities in talk often coincide with
changes in footing.
1. What is "footing"?
3. What is a "coordinated task activity", p. 143, and what is distinctive about it, according
to Goffman, when compared to other forms of conversation?
5. What frame, or footing, was embedded in the news conference by Helen Thomas'
pirouette? (p. 156)
FOOTING
the right but not the obligation to initiate a little flurry of talk,
then relapse back into silence, all this with no apparent ritual
marking, as though adding but another interchange to a chronic
conversation in progress. Here something must be addressed that
is neither ratified participation nor bystanding, but a peculiar
condition between.
There remains to consider the dynamics of ratified participa-
tion. Plainly, a distinction must be drawn between opening or
closing an encounter, and joining or leaving an ongoing one;
conventional practices are to be found for distinguishably accom-
plishing both. And plainly, two differently manned encounters
can occur under conditions of mutual accessibility, each bystand-
ing the other.3 At point here, however, is another issue: the right
to leave and to join, taken together, imply circumstances in which
participants will shift from one encounter to another. At a
"higher" level, one must also consider the possibility of an en-
counter of four or more participants splitting, and of separate
encounters merging. And it appears that in some microecological
social circumstances these various changes are frequent. Thus, at
table during convivial dinners of eight or so participants, marked
instability of participation is often found. Here a speaker may
feel it necessary to police his listenership, not so much to guard
against eavesdroppers (for, indeed, at table overhearing hardly
needs to be concealed), as to bring back strays and encourage
incipient joiners. In such environments, interruption, pitch rais-
ing and trunk orientation seem to acquire a special function and
significance. (Note how a passenger sitting in the front seat of a
taxi can function as a pivot, now addressing his fellow passengers
in the back seat, now the driver, effectively trusting the driver to
determine whether to act as a nonperson or an addressee, and all
this without the driver's taking his eyes off the road or depending
on the content of the remark to provide participation instruc-
tions.) Another example of structural instability is to be observed
when couples meet. What had been two "withs" provide the
personnel for a momentarily inclusive encounter, which can then
~ . .
those who watch a play; but again the many ways in which these
two kinds of hearers are in the same position shouldn't blind one
to the very important ways in which their circumstances differ.
A town speaker's words are meant for his audience and are
spoken to them; were a reply to be made, it would have to come
from these listeners, and indeed, as suggested, signs of agreement
and disagreement are often in order. It is presumably because
there are so many persons in an audience that direct queries and
replies must be omitted, or at least postponed to a time when the
speech itself can be considered over. Should a member of the
audience assay to reply in words to something that a speaker in
midspeech says, the latter can elect to answer and, if he knows
what he's about, sustain the reality he is engaged in. But the
words addressed by one character in a play to another (at least
in modern Western dramaturgy) are eternally sealed off from the
audience, belonging entirely to a self-enclosed, make-believe
realm-although the actors who are performing these characters
(and who in a way are also cut off from the dramatic action) might
well appreciate signs of audience a t t e n t i ~ e n e s s . ~
I have suggested that orators and actors provide a ready
contrast to a conversation's speaker, the former having audiences,
the latter fellow conversationalists. But it must be borne in mind
that what goes on upon the platform is only incidentally-not
analytically-talk. Singing can occur there (this being another
way words can be uttered), and doings which don't centrally
involve words at all, such as instrument playing, hat tricks, jug-
gling, and all the other guileful acts that have done a turn in
vaudeville. The various kinds of audiences are not, analytically
speaking, a feature of speech events (to use Hymes's term), but
of stage events.
And from here one can go on to still more difficult cases.
There are, for example, church congregations of the revivalist
type wherein an active interchange is sustained of calls and an-
swers between minister and churchgoers. And there are lots of
4. Maintaining a rigid line between characters and audience is by no
means, of course, the only way to organize dramatic productions, Burmese
traditional theatre providing one example (Becker 1970), our own burlesqued
melodrama almost another.
Forms of Talk
the meaningful context and the relevant unit for analysis; the
words spoken, whether by one participant or two, are an integral
part of a mutually coordinated physical undertaking, not a talk.
Ritual is so often truncated in these settings because it is noncon-
versational work that is being done. It is the execution of this
work, not utterances, that will ordinarily be the chief concern of
the participants. And it is when a hitch occurs in what would
otherwise have been the routine interdigitation of their acts that
a verbal interchange between them is most likely.
A similar picture can be seen in extended service transac-
tions. Take, for example, mother-child pediatric consultations
in Scottish public health clinics, as recently reported by Strong
(1979, esp. chap. 6 ) . Here a mother's business with a doctor
(when she finally gets her turn) is apparently bracketed with
little small talk, very little by way of preplay and postplay, al-
though the child itself may be the recipient of a few ritual
solicitudes. The mother sits before the doctor's desk and briefly
answers such questions as he puts her, waiting patiently, qui-
etly, and attentively between questions. She is on immediate
call, poised to speak, but speaking only when spoken to, almost
as well behaved as the machine that is of so much use to airline
ticketers. The physician, for his part, intersperses his un-
ceremoniously addressed queries with notetaking, note-reading,
thoughtful musings, instruction to students, physical manipula-
tion of the child, verbal exchanges with his nurse and col-
leagues, and movements away from his desk to get at such
things as files and equipment-all af which actioiis ~ ~ p p e war-
ar
ranted by his institutional role if not by the current examina-
tion. The mother's answers will sometimes lead the doctor to
follow up with a next question, but often instead to some other
sort of act on his part. For his social and professional status
allows him to be very businesslike; he is running through the
phases of an examination, or checklist, not a conversation, and
only a scattering of items require a mother's verbal contribu-
tion. And indeed, the mother may not know with any specifi-
city what any of the doctor's acts are leading up to or getting at, -
her being "in on" the instrumentally meaningful sequence of
events in no way being necessary for her contribution to it. So
although she fits her turns at talk, and what she says, to the
Footing
Note that one deals in this case not so much with a body or mind
as with a person active in some particular social identity or role,
some special capacity as a member of a group, office, category,
relationship, association, or whatever, some socially based source
of self-identification. Often this will mean that the individual
speaks, explicitly or implicitly, in the name of "we," not "I" (but
not for the reasons Queen Victoria or Nixon felt they had), the
"we" including more than the self (Spiegelberg 1973:129-56;
Moerman 1968:153-69). And, of course, the same individual can
rapidly alter the social role in which he is active, even though his
capacity as animator and author remains constant-what in com-
mittee meetings is called "changing hats." (This, indeed, is what
occurs during a considerable amount of code switching, as
Gumperz has amply illustrated.) In thus introducing the name or
capacity in which he speaks, the speaker goes some distance in
establishing a corresponding reciprocal basis of identification for
those to whom this stand-taking is addressed. To a degree, then,
to select the capacity in which we are to be active is to select (or
to attempt to select) the capacity in which the recipients of our
action are present (Weinstein and Deutschberger 1963:454-66).
All of this work is consolidated by naming practices and, in many
languages, through choice among available second-person pro-
nouns.
The notions of animator, author, and principal, taken to-
gether, can be said to tell us about the "productiq~£ormat"
utterance.
,-----..- --
c
When one uses the term "speaker, ' one often implies that
I
of an
the following:
To the best of my recollection,
( I ) I think that
(2) I said
( 3 ) I once lived that sort of life.
where (I)reflects something that is currently true of the individ-
ual who animates (the "addressing self"), (2) an embedded
8. Some generative semanticists have argued that any unadorned utter-
ance implies a higher performative verb and a pronoun, e.g., "I say," "aver,"
"demand," etc., the implication being that all statements are made by figures
mentioned or implied, not by living individuals. See, for example, Ross 1970.
Forms of Talk
But one exception did shine through. The Queen and other members of the
Family occasionally reverted to telling family stories or personal experiences to
their interlocutor. The stories no doubt were carefully selected (as all stories
must be), but in the telling of them the royal personages could not but momen-
tarily slip into the unregal stance of storyteller, allowing their hearers the
momentary (relative) intimacy of story listeners. What could be conceived of
as "humanity" is thus practically inescapable. For there is a democracy implied
in narration; the lowest rank in that activity is not very low by society's stan-
dards-the right and obligation to listen to a story from a person to whom we
need not be in a position to tell one.
I2. Interestingly, the texts that folklorists and sociolinguists provide of
everyday stories often systematically omit the narrative frame breaks that very
likely occurred throughout the actual tellings. Here the student of stories has
tactfully accepted the teller's injunction that the shift in footing required to
introduce a correction or some other out-of-frame comment be omitted from the
official record. Often omitted, too, is any appreciation of the frequency with
Footing
which hearers change footing and inject in passing their own contribution to the
tale (Goodwin 1978, esp. chap. 3 and chap. 4, pt. 5).
153
apparently, marking her appreciation that to play her inquiry
straight would be directly to invoke a shared interest and compe-
tency, not a particularly masculine one, and hence a similarity her
neighbor might be disinclined to confront.) O r witness the way
in which the physical contact, focusing tone, and loving endear-
ments appropriate within the privacy of a courtship encounter
can be performed in fun to an unsuitable candidate as a set piece
to set into the focus of attention of a wider convivial circle. Or,
in the same sort of circle, how we can respond to what a speaker
says to an addressed recipient as though we weren't ratified
coparticipants, but bystanders engaged in irreverent sideplay. Or,
even when two individuals are quite alone together and cannot
possibly be overheard, how one may mark the confidential and
disclosive status of a bit of gossip by switching into a whisper
voice. I think there is no doubt that a considerable amount of
natural conversation is laminated in the manner these illustra-
tions suggest; in any case, conversation is certainly vulnerable to
such lamination. And each increase or decrease in layering-each
u
movement closer to or further from the "literal -carries with it
a change in footing.
Once it is seen that a participation framework can be paren-
thesized and set into an alien environment, it should be evident
that all the participation frameworks earlier described as occur-
ring outside of conversation-that is, arrangements involving an
audience or no official recipient at all-are themselves candidates
for this reframins process; they, too, can be reset into conversa-
;.
,~,,r;l ialk. Anci, oi course, with e a c i ~such crubedding a change
I have dealt till now with changes in footing as though the individ-
ual were involved merely in switching from one stance or align-
ment to another. But this image is itself too mechanical and too
easy. It is insufficiently responsive to the way embedding and
ritualization work. For often it seems that when we change voice
-whether to speak for another aspect of ourselves or for some-
one else, or to lighten our discourse with a darted enactment of
some alien interaction arrangement-we are not so much ter-
minating the prior alignment as holding it in abeyance with the
understanding that it will almost immediately be reengaged. So,
too, when we give up the floor in a conversation, thereby taking
up the footing of a recipient (addressed or otherwise), we can be
warranted in expecting to reenter the speaker role on the same
footing from which we left it. As suggested, this is clearly the case
when a narrator allows hearers to "chip in," but such perceivedly
temporary foregoing of one's position is also to be found when
storytelling isn't featured. So it must be allowed that we can hold
the same looting acres, se\r,ial of our turns at talk. And within
one alignment, another can be fully enclosed. In truth, in talk it
seems routine that, while firmly standing on two feet, we jump
up and down on another.
Which should prepare us for those institutional niches in
which a hard-pressed functionary is constrained to routinely sus-
t tain more than one state of talk simultaneously. Thus, through-
out an auction, an auctioneer may intersperse the utterances he
directs to the bidding audience with several streams of out-of-
frame communication-reports on each sale spoken through a
microphone to a recording clerk in another room, instructions to
assistants on the floor, and (less routinely) greetings to friends
and responses to individual buyers who approach with quiet
i
Forms of Talk
To end, let us return to the Nixon scene that formed the introduc-
tion to this paper. When Helen Thomas pirouetted for the presi-
dent, she was parenthesizing within her journalistic stance
another stance, that of a woman receiving comments on her ap-
pearance. No doubt the forces at work are sexism and presidents,
but the forces can work in this particular way because of our
general capacity to embed the fleeting enactment of one role in
the more extended performance of another.
When Helen Thomas pirouetted for the president, she was
employing a form of behavior indigenous to the environment of
the ballet, a form that has come, by conventional reframing, to
be a feature of female modeling in fashion shows, and she was
enacting it--of all places-in a news conference. No one present
apparently found this transplantation odd. That is how experi-
ence is laminated.
The news report of this conference itself does not tell us, but
from what is known about Nixon as a performer, a guess would
be that he switched from the high ritual of signing a bill to tKT
joshing of Ms. Thomas not merely as a bracketing device, a signal
that the substantive phase of the ceremony was over, but to show
he was a person of spirit, always capable of the common touch.
Footing
Abercrombie, David. 1965. Conversation and spoken prose; Studies in phonetics and
linguistics. London: Oxford University Press.
Argyle, Michael, and Dean, Janet. 1965. "Eye contact, distance and affiliation."
Sociornetry 28:289-304.
Becker, A. L. 1970. "Journey through the night: Notes on Burmese traditional
theatre." T ~Drnrna
P Review 15/3:83-87.
Blom, Jan-Peter, and Gumperz, John J. 1972. "Social meaning in linguistic
structure: Code-switching in Norway." In Direcfonr in S~riolin~uistics,
edited
by John J. Gumperz and Dell H. Hymes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston.
Cook-Gumperz, Jenny, and Gumperz, John. 1976. "Context in children's
speech." In Papers on Language and Context (Working Paper 46), by Jenny
Cook-Gumperz and John Gumperz. Berkeley: Language Behavior Re-
search Laboratory, University of California, Berkeley.
7he Euening Bulletin (Philadelphia), August 7, 1973.
Falk, Dean, "Language, handedness, and primate brains: Did the Australopithe-
cines sign? American Anthropo/ogist 82 (1980): 78.
Fisher, Lawrence E. 1976. "Dropping remarks and the Barbadian audience."
American Ethnologist 3 /2:22 7-42.
Goffman, Erving. 1974. Frame analysis. New York: Harper and Row.
. 1976. "Replies and responses." Language in Society 5:257-313.
. 1978. "Response cries." Language 54/4.
Goodwin, Marjorie. 1978. "Conversational practices in a peer group of urban
black children." Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
Gossen, Gary H. 1976. "Verbal dueling in Chamula." In Speech play, edited by
Barbara Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, pp. 121-46. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
Gumperz, John. 1976. "Social network and language shift." In Papers on Language
and Context (Working Paper 46), by Jenny Cook-Gumperz and John G u m F
erz. Berkeley: Language Behavior Research Laboratory, University of Cali-
fornia, Berkeley.
Hewes, Gordon W. 1973. "Primate communication and the gestural origin of
language." Current Anthropology 14:5-24.
Hockett, Charles. 1963. "The problem of universals in language." In Universals
of language, edited b y Joseph Greenberg, pp. 1-29. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Hymes, Dell H. 1974. Foundations in sociolinguistics. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
Laberge, Suzanne, and Sankoff, Gillian. 1979. "Anything you can do." In Dis-
course and syntax, edited by Talmy Givon and Charles Li. New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Moerman, Michael. 1968. "Being Lue: Uses and abuses of ethnic identification."
In Essays on the problem of tribe, edited by June Helm, pp. 153-69. Seattle:
University of Washington Press.
Polanyi, Livia. 1977. ''Not so false starts." Working Papers in Sociolinguistics, no. 41.
Austin, Texas: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory.
Ross, John Robert. 1970. "On declarative sentences." In Readings in English trans-
formationalgramrnar, edited by Roderick A. Jacobs and Peter S. Rosenbaum,
pp. 222-72. Waltham, Mass.: Ginn and Company.
Schieffelin, Bambi B. 1979. "Getting it together: An ethnographic approach to
the study of the development of communicative competence." In Studies in
developmental pragmatics, edited by Elinor 0. Keenan. New York: Academic
Press.
Spiegelberg, Herbert. 1973. "On the right to say 'we1: A linguistic and
phenomenological analysis." In Phenomenological sociology, edited by George
Psathas, pp. 129-56. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Strong, P. M. 1979. 7 l e ceremonial order ofthe clinic. London: Routledge and Kegan
Paul.
Tannen, Deborah, and Wallat, Cynthia. Forthcoming. "A sociolinguistic analy-
sis of multiple demands on the pediatrician in doctor/mother/child in-
teraction."
Weinstein, Eugene, and Deutschberger, Paul. 1963. "Some dimensions of alter-
casting." Sociometry 26/4:454-66.