Juruena 2017
Juruena 2017
Juruena 2017
PII: S0165-0327(17)30691-2
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.052
Reference: JAD9260
To appear in: Journal of Affective Disorders
Received date: 2 April 2017
Revised date: 11 July 2017
Accepted date: 26 September 2017
Cite this article as: Mario F. Juruena, Mariia Bocharova, Bruno Agustini and
Allan H. Young, Atypical Depression and Non-Atypical Depression: Is HPA
Axis Function a Biomarker? A Systematic Review, Journal of Affective
Disorders, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.052
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for
publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of
the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and
review of the resulting galley proof before it is published in its final citable form.
Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which
could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Atypical Depression and Non-Atypical Depression: Is HPA Axis Function a
*Correspondence to: PO72, De Crespigny Park-Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK.
[email protected]
Abstract
Background
The link between the abnormalities of the Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and
depression has been one of the most consistently reported findings in psychiatry. At the
same time, multiple studies have demonstrated a stronger association between the in-
creased activation of HPA-axis and melancholic, or endogenous depression subtype. This
association has not been confirmed for the atypical subtype, and some researchers have
suggested that as an antinomic depressive subtype, it may be associated with the opposite
type, i.e. hypo-function, of the HPA-axis, similarly to PTSD. The purpose of this systematic
review is to summarise existing studies addressing the abnormalities of the HPA-axis in
melancholic and/or atypical depression.
Method
We conducted a systematic review in the literature by searching MEDLINE, PsycINFO,
OvidSP and Embase databases until June 2017. The following search items were used:
"hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal" OR "HPA" OR "cortisol" OR "corticotropin releasing hor-
mone" OR "corticotropin releasing factor" OR "glucocorticoid*" OR "adrenocorticotropic hor-
mone" OR "ACTH" AND "atypical depression" OR "non-atypical depression" OR "melanchol-
ic depression" OR "non-melancholic depression" OR "endogenous depression" OR "en-
dogenomorphic depression" OR "non-endogenous depression". Search limits were set to
include papers in English or German language published in peer-reviewed journals at any
period. All studies were scrutinized to determine the main methodological characteristics,
and particularly possible sources of bias influencing the results reported.
Results
We selected 48 relevant studies. Detailed analysis of the methodologies used in the studies
revealed significant variability especially regarding the samples’ definition comparing the
HPA axis activity of melancholic patients to atypical depression, including healthy controls.
The results were subdivided into 4 sections: 1) 27 studies which compared melancholic OR
endogenous depression vs. non-melancholic or non-endogenous depression or controls; 2)
9 studies which compared atypical depression or atypical traits vs. non-atypical depression
or controls; 3) 7 studies which compared melancholic or endogenous and atypical depres-
sion subtypes and 4) 5 studies which used a longitudinal design, comparing the measures of
HPA-axis across two or more time points. While the majority of studies did confirm the asso-
ciation between melancholic depression and increased post-challenge cortisol levels, the
association with increases in basal cortisol and basal ACTH were less consistent. Some
studies, particularly those focusing on reversed vegetative symptoms, demonstrated a de-
crease in the activity of the HPA axis in atypical depression compared to controls, but the
majority did not distinguish it from healthy controls.
Conclusions
In conclusion, our findings indicate that there is a difference in the activity of the HPA-axis
between melancholic and atypical depressive subtypes. However, these are more likely ex-
plained by hypercortisolism in melancholia; and most often normal than decreased function
in atypical depression. Further research should seek to distinguish a particular subtype of
depression linked to HPA-axis abnormalities, based on symptom profile, with a focus on
vegetative symptoms, neuroendocrine probes, and the history of adverse childhood events.
New insights into the dichotomy addressed in this review might be obtained from genetic
and epigenetic studies of HPA-axis related genes in both subtypes, with an emphasis on the
presence of vegetative symptoms.
Introduction
The most robust and consistent finding in major depression so far has been its link to
the abnormalities of the stress response system. The stress response system is a
complex, multilevel mechanism largely dependent on feedback regulation. It relies
on two main elements - the autonomic stress response which exerts immediate ef-
fects when the organism is faced with physiological or psychological stressors; and
the impact on the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. A detailed account of
the structure and physiology of both components of the stress response system is
thoroughly given by Ulrich-Lai & Herman (2009) and is outside the scope of this re-
view. However, to understand the nature of the abnormalities which will be discussed
further, it is essential to highlight the key characteristics of the HPA-axis.
In response to stressful stimuli, the suppression of the subgenual prefrontal cortex
and the activation of amygdala lead to the stimulation of the autonomic sympathetic
axis, and the HPA axis (Dioro et al. 1993; Phelps and LeDoux, 2015; Gold, 2015).
The autonomic sympathetic axis is responsible for the most rapid response, and acts
via the secretion of epinephrine by the adrenal glands; the HPA axis is activated mi-
nutes after the epinephrine surge, and represents a cascade of events starting with
the secretion of the corticotropin releasing factor (CRF, also known as corticotropin-
releasing hormone, or CRH) from the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus
into the portal circulation, which stimulates the synthesis and release of adrenocorti-
cotropic hormone (ACTH) by the pituitary. The ACTH further stimulates the synthe-
sis and release of the glucocorticoid hormone cortisol by the adrenal cortex. Gluco-
corticoids are known to exert a range of functions, such as promoting gluconeogene-
sis, catabolic and antianabolic activity, suppression of innate immunity in immune
organs, insulin resistance and a prothrombotic state. The key role of glucocorticoids
consists in maintaining homoeostasis in response to stress (Juruena, 2014).
The HPA-axis exerts its feedback through two major types of receptors: the gluco-
corticoid (GR) and the mineralocorticoid (MR) receptors. MRs have higher affinity to
cortisol which results in their higher occupancy even at basal cortisol concentrations;
at the same time, they are less specific and bind with both cortisol and aldosterone.
GRs, on the other hand, bind more specifically to cortisol, yet respond to higher con-
centrations than MRs MR’s seem to be the ones that regulate cortisol feedback du-
ring acute or normal stress. However, during severe or prolonged stress, GR’s come
into action (De Kloet et al.,1998).
The abnormalities of stress response system in affective disorders have been im-
plied in several hundred studies (Stetler et al. 2013). However, accumulating evi-
dence suggests that the presence, and type of, HPA-axis abnormalities may vary
across various subtypes of depression (Gold et al. 2015; Porter and Gallagher
2006). Some studies have shown a robust association with HPA-axis overactivity
with more severe or endogenomorphic forms of depression such as melancholic de-
pression or psychotic depression (Nelson and Davis 1997).At the same time, in post-
traumatic stress disorder, an enhanced HPA negative feedback was described
(Yehuda et al. 1991). Some studies have hypothesised that atypical depression, un-
like the melancholic subtype and similar to PTSD, is characterised by hypocortiso-
lism and enhanced negative feedback. However, whether it is fair to claim there
exists such a dichotomy, is not clear at the moment.
The aim of the current article is to review existing literature addressing the function
of the HPA-axis in melancholic and atypical depressive subtypes. Evaluate whether:
a) there is a significant difference between the two subtypes in terms of the activity of
the HPA-axis; b) whether there is enough evidence to suggest that the HPA-axis is
overactive in depression with melancholic features; c) whether there is sufficient evi-
dence to indicate that the HPA-axis in depression with atypical features is hypoac-
tive.
Considering the studies which have addressed HPA-axis abnormalities in either of,
or both, melancholic and atypical subtypes, it is important to take into account that
over the recent decades, approaches to identify them have been changing, and even
today, appropriate criteria defining both subtypes remain a matter of debate.
A specifier introduced in DSM-III (1980), depression with melancholic features re-
presents a subtype of depression clinically characterized by a distinct pattern of low
mood, anhedonia, lack of reactivity to positive events, loss of appetite and weight,
insomnia, loss of libido and diurnal mood variations. Although depression with «me-
lancholic features» has been validated extensively (Shotte et al, 1997; Juruena et al
2011; Parker et al, 2015), there is still little agreement among researchers regarding
the particular set of features that define it (Maes et al., 1992; Leventhal et al, 2005 ;
Fink et al, 2007; Parker et al, 2013). Besides, the various diagnostic measures used
for identifying melancholic depression have shown a considerable degree of incon-
sistency, as exemplified by the comparison of Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC)
endogenous depression definition, Newcastle scale endogenous depression defini-
tion, and DSM-III and DSM-IV diagnoses of melancholic depression (Rush et al.,
1994 ; Coryell, 2007 Orsel et al., 2010).
Nevertheless, it is of high importance for this review to point out that the sub-
sample identified by researchers as non-melancholic can be highly heterogeneous
and represent various diagnostic entities, such as neurotic/reactive, atypical, non-
differentiated depression, characterological depression (Fink and Taylor, 2007).
Therefore, although atypical depression is indeed greatly antithetic to melancholic
depression, it is not the same as non-melancholic depression, and for the purpose of
this review, unless specified as having features known to constitute the atypical sub-
type, non-melancholic subsamples of reviewed studies will not be not considered
atypical.
Atypical depression had been recognized by some researchers as a depressive
subtype since the 1960s. However, although addressed extensively in the context of
differential responsiveness to pharmacological treatment, it had not been included in
official DSM diagnostic criteria until 1994, following the formulation of Columbia aty-
pical depression criteria (Quitkin et al. 1993). The validity of the atypical specifier
has been demonstrated by some studies, that largely rests on data from psycho-
pharmacological research - an approach introduced by Klein and according to which,
the differential response to biological treatment represents different pathophysiology
(Klein, 1989) and genetic-epidemiological studies which indicated that atypical de-
pression subtype is genetically distinct from typical ones, which represents its etiolo-
gical insularity (Kendler et al., 1996; Sullivan et al..1998). At the same time, the pre-
cise definition of «atypical depression» remains a matter of debate. According to
DSM-IV diagnostic criteria (“atypical features” specifier), the disorder is primarily cha-
racterized by mood reactivity and 2 or more of the following symptoms as predomi-
nant features in patients with major depression or dysthymic disorder: overeating,
oversleeping, “leaden paralysis,” and interpersonal rejection sensitivity. The rele-
vance of mood reactivity to the subtype, as well as its relationship with other symp-
toms, has been questioned. The studies cited above as providing evidence for the
validity of the subtype, in fact, did not include the «mood reactivity» criteria. In a stu-
dy by Posternak et al. (2001) mood reactivity did not show any association with any
other psychopathological features of the proposed subtype. Similar results were re-
ported by a few other researchers (Parker et al., 2002). A much more robust asso-
ciation was shown for the component of the symptom definition described as «re-
versed vegetative symptoms», namely hyperphagia and hypersomnia, and some of
the studies selected for this review have focused on these characteristics rather than
the DSM-defined specifier (Benazzi, 2002). In two studies of treatment response,
Stewart et al. (2007) demonstrated a significant contribution of factors such as age
of onset (early vs late) and chronicity (chronic or remitting course) to the correlation
between atypical subtype and response to MAOI treatment.. Some authors proposed
a reappraisal of the DSM criteria for an atypical subtype to reduce the number of
symptoms besides mood reactivity to one, and to include the onset of dysphoria be-
fore 20 and chronic course as additional criteria. However, DSM-5 saw no amend-
ments to the subtype criteria (Stewart et al. 2007).
Therefore, the definition of the atypical subtype appears even more vague than that
of melancholic subtype, and data of all biological studies should be interpreted with
account of the characteristics of the phenotype. Besides, in the majority of studies,
as well as in the case with non-melancholic depression, the non-atypical descriptor
does not necessarily identify melancholic depression, therefore, unless specified
otherwise, non-atypical depression is considered as MDD not matching particular
subtype criteria.
Methods
The initial search yielded 9556 results in total, which was comprised of: 3192 results
in the Embase database, 2029 in all MEDLINE databases, 2344 in PsycINFO data-
base, and 1991 in Journals@ Ovid Full-Text database. Because the combined num-
ber of articles across all selected databases (9556) exceeded the 6000 limit allowing
for deduplication, Further deduplication limited the number of articles to 3061.
Next, the filters «Human», «Adult» and «Depression» were applied, leaving 570 arti-
cles for title/abstract screen. For the full-text screen, we included original studies as-
sessing the functional elements of the HPA-axis in samples including at least one of
the following phenotypes: melancholic depression, endogenous depression, en-
dogenomorphic depression, atypical depression, their characteristic traits (such as
«reversed neurovegetative symptoms»), and a comparison group.
After the title/abstract screen, 247 articles were retrieved for a full-text screen.
a) The inclusion of adult patients diagnosed with major depressive episode ac-
cording to DSM or ICD operational criteria;
c) The evaluation of the levels of basal and/or post-challenge cortisol in the blood,
saliva, urine or CSF, of basal and/or post-challenge ACTH in blood, and of basal
and/or post-challenge CRF in blood or CSF.
a) They had no comparison group (e.g. studies in a small sample of melancholic pa-
tients before and after TMS, however, studies which compared the subtypes be-
tween each other without healthy controls, were included)
Results
The final stage of the search process yielded 48 articles in general. The studies were
subdivided into four groups:
Group 1. Studies (n=27) which compared «melancholic» (or other definitions) de-
pression with depression not matching any of the definitions for melancholia, or a
control group
Group 2. Studies (n=9) which compared «atypical» (or other definitions) depression
with depressions not matching any of the definitions for atypicality, or a control
group.
Group 3. Studies (n=7) which compared «melancholic» (or other definitions) de-
pression with «atypical» (or other definitions) depression.
Group 4. Studies (n=5) which employed a longitudinal design.
The results for each section are summarized in Tables 1-4, respectively
Melancholic studies
Amsterdam (1989) attempted to assess several levels of the HPA axis and their ab-
normalities in hormonal response in depressive patients. In their paper, they referred
to 2 studies which used ACTH as a hormonal challenge. The first one compared 16
patients with a depressive disorder (of which, 9 had melancholic features) and 11
healthy controls. They found no difference in cortisol levels at baseline between
groups. However, after ACTH administration, the depressive group had a larger in-
crease in cortisol concentrations than controls (p<0,02). Further, the authors tried to
replicate these results in a larger sample of 72 depressed patients (of which 51 had
melancholic features) compared to 34 healthy controls and were unable to find the
same results. In contrast, they found that both at baseline and after ACTH admin-
istration, cortisol levels did not significantly differ between groups. The same article
cites a study with ovine CRH challenge. They administered oCRH to 26 depressed
patients (of which 14 had melancholic features) and compared to 11 healthy con-
trols. They found no difference between groups in cortisol levels at baseline and after
challenge; nonetheless, depressive patients as a group had lower ACTH response
compared to controls. The effect was larger in patients with melancholic features
(Joseph-Vanderpool et al 1991).
The study by Mitchell et al., (1990) tried a different challenge using the serotonin ag-
onist Fenfluramine 60 mg but found no difference between groups of “endogenous”
vs. “non-endogenous” depressive patients. The 30 patients in the study were evalu-
ated with four different types of criteria (DSM-III, ICD 9, RDC, Newcastle criteria) to
divide subtypes; however, they did not found differences in post-challenge cortisol
levels between any of the groups.
Another study that used a similar compound, in this case, d-Fenfluramine 30 mg, al-
so failed to show any significant difference. The study compared 23 depressive pa-
tients with 16 healthy controls and found that both groups had the same levels of
cortisol after challenge (O'Keane & Dinan, 1991).
In 2011, Paslakis (Paslakis et al., 2011) compared 3 ways of assessing HPA axis
and their relevance for detection of depression. One of those was the Dex/CRH test.
They found that the test had low levels of sensibility (30,8%) and moderate specifici-
ty (78,8%) being the worst marker between the three compared (DST, 24h cortisol).
Basal measurements
Basal cortisol
The search identified 19 studies which compared the basal cortisol levels (either as
part of further DST or separately) among patients diagnosed with melancholic de-
pression and matched controls and/or non-melancholic depressed patients.
Rubin et al. (1987) demonstrated that the elevation of basal cortisol when meas-
ured continuously over 26 hours correlated most significantly with the DST status of
the patients: i.e. patients that were non-suppressors had elevated 24-hour cortisol
and nocturnal cortisol nadir compared to both controls and patients who were sup-
pressors. This was shown in a sample of RDC-examined definite endogenous pa-
tients (Rubin et al., 1987).
Further evidence of the increase in basal cortisol levels between melancholic de-
pressed patients and controls/non-melancholic patients comes from studies by Gue-
chot et al. (1987), Wong et al. (2000), Paslakis et al. (2011), O’Keane and Di-
nan(1991). Guechot et al. performed a simple one-time salivary test of cortisol at 11
pm in patients diagnosed as «primary» depressive using DSM-III and Saint-Louis
criteria, compared to both secondary depressives (p<0.05) and controls (p<0.02).
They also reported high sensitivity (62,5%) and specificity (75% vs. secondary de-
pressives, 90% vs. controls) rates of the test for the identification of primary depres-
sive patients when the cut-off was set at 3.45 nmol/l (Guechot et al., 1987). A study
by Wong et al. (2000), addressed multiple measures including basal plasma cortisol
in patients with a DSM-II-R and RDC-defined melancholic subtype of depression and
controls, concluded that mean 30-h plasma cortisol levels in depressed patients
were significantly higher vs. controls. Paslakis et al. (2011), together with DST and
Dex/CRH, which will be discussed, further, performed a comparison of Basal cortisol
levels in melancholic patients defined using DSM-IV criteria vs. healthy controls.
They confirmed that diurnal basal cortisol secretion as measured throughout 24
hours, is significantly higher than in controls, as measured both by cortisol profile
graphs and area under the curve (p=0.001 and p<0.01, respectively). The maximum
of variation was observed at 11.30 and 14.00. In this study, basal cortisol was the
most sensitive marker for melancholic depression than DST, and at the time interval
from 10.00 to 12.00 yielded results with optimal sensitivity (83.3%) and specificity
(87.9%). O’Keane and Dinan (1991) study also reported elevated baseline cortisol in
patients diagnosed with DSM-III-R major depression and Newcastle scale criteria for
endogenicity. They showed a significant increase in basal cortisol in patients com-
pared to controls subjects. CORE measure was also used by Liu et al.(2016) in the
definition of melancholic depression. They performed an analysis of 228 blood meta-
bolic markers in 21 melancholic patients (as well as 58 patients matching criteria for
«anxious depression» defined by one or more comorbid anxiety disorders on M.I.N.I,
and 100 controls). One of the important outputs of this study was the confirmation
that melancholic depression can represent a more biologically distinct subtype of de-
pression. Regarding HPA-axis activity, increases in basal cortisol levels were signifi-
cant for the melancholic group, as well as increases in other metabolites in the hor-
mone biosynthesis pathway (androstenedione and corticosterone; Liu et al., 2016).
Michopoulos et al. (2008) studied whether elevated HPA-axis function was associat-
ed with executive dysfunction and memory deficits in melancholic (as defined by
DSM-IV-TR criteria) depressed patients. They also reported no significant difference
between plasma and salivary cortisol levels in melancholic vs. none- melancholic
groups. The only significant correlation between cortisol values and CANTAB tests,
either mnemonic or prefrontal, used for cognitive function assessment was an asso-
ciation between morning salivary cortisol and the ID/ED total errors.
In accordance, Mitchell et al. (1990) and Joyce et al. (2002) compared a range of
basal measures in patients with melancholic depression defined by several criteria.
Mitch-ell et al. compared such diagnostic systems as ICD-9, DSM-III, RDC and
Newcastle Scale melancholic/endogenous phenotypes, while Joyce et al. (2002)
compared DSM-IV based diagnose of melancholia with that focusing on CORE
measures. In the second study, there was a significant correlation with basal corti-
sol: in male patients defined as melancholic by CORE criteria.
A few studies mentioned above about DST results also assessed the association
of basal cortisol levels with particular symptoms. Casper et al. (1987) reported that
basal plasma cortisol was significantly associated with weight and appetite loss;
Kaestner et al. (2005) indicated that high baseline cortisol levels correlated with
HAM-D severity and the presence of weight loss.
Basal ACTH
There were three studies which addressed basal ACTH levels, of which none indi-
cated elevated plasma ACTH levels in melancholic patients.
Wong et al. (2000) who used both DSM-III-R and RDC criteria to define the me-
lancholic subtype failed to find a significant association in the melancho-
lic/endogenous group with elevated basal plasma ACTH; however, since they ob-
served elevated cortisol levels, they also reported that plasma cortisol-to-ACTH ratio
was significantly elevated in melancholic patients compared to controls. Similarly,
Joyce et al. (2002) failed to show elevated ACTH in melancholic patients when
applying either DSM-IV or CORE criteria. Finally, Gomez-Gil et al.(2010) got their
negative results when applying the NEDDI criteria.
Basal CRF
There were two studies which reported basal CRF levels. Wong et al. found that
melancholic patients did not differ from controls in their levels of basal CRF which
was disproportional about elevated basal cortisol. Similarly, Joyce et al. (2002) failed
to demonstrate elevated basal CRF using either DSM-IV or CORE definitions.
McGinn et al. (1996) studied patients from the same cohort as Asnis et al.(1995).
However, they stratified patients as having mood reactivity alone (n=29), having de-
pression with atypical features as defined by mood reactivity plus one of the four
atypical symptoms (n=33), and MDD not matching atypical criteria (n=52). The main
conclusion of the study was that AD patients, similar to the previous study, had a
significantly higher cortisol response to DMI.
Apart from DST, Stewart et al. used dextroamphetamine challenge in their sample.
The difference in post-dextroamphetamine cortisol levels did not reach statistical
significance although there was a trend for higher numbers in the early/chronic atypi-
cal group (Stewart et al. 2005).
Baseline measures
Baseline cortisol levels were assessed in seven studies in the group, while basal
ACTH was only mentioned in one study.
Both Asnis et al. (1995) and McGinn et al. (1996) who reported basal cortisol lev-
els in ADDS-assessed atypical depressive patients showed no difference be-tween
the patients and controls. Joseph-Vanderpool et al. (1991) reported a trend towards
lower basal cortisol in SAD patients vs. healthy controls which, however, was only
significant at 22.00.
Studies that applied DSM-IV criteria showed slightly differing results. Anisman et al.
(1999), compared 31 atypical MDD with 14 non-atypical MDD and 15 atypical dys-
thymic with 14 non-atypical dysthymic patients, assessing among other markers, on
basal cortisol and ACTH levels, and demonstrated significantly decreased basal cor-
tisol, but increased ACTH levels in atypical patients vs. controls.
Stewart et al. (2005) who also used DSM-IV criteria in their comparison of atypical
depression with various courses demonstrated that Patients with early/chronic atypi-
cal had significantly lower mean 3 h afternoon cortisol levels (Stewart et al, 2005).
Finally, Casper et al. (1988) and Levitan et al.(1997) used reversed vegetative symp-
toms as criteria for atypicality. The former showed no differences in any of the
measurements - i.e. plasma, CSF or urinary cortisol,and controls. At the same time,
Levitan et al. (1997) demonstrated a significant negative correlation between the
symptoms of hypersomnia and carbohydrate craving and basal cortisol values
Studies directly comparing the function of the HPA-axis in melancholic vs. atypical
depression are rather scarce. Practically, our search only yielded seven relevant ar-
ticles. Of them, the majority (n=5) focused on baseline cortisol measures. There
were two studies that assessed basal ACTH as well, one study that assessed DST;
and one study that applied Dex/CRH test. The results of the studies are summarised
in Table 3 below.
Challenge tests
The first study to compare the basal measures between the two subtypes was a
study by Elizabeth Young et al. (2001). Authors investigated the whether cortisol se-
cretion reflects a central CRF dysregulation or represents altered adrenal gland func-
tion. Over a period of 24 hours, ACTH and cortisol levels were assessed at 10-min
intervals in a sample of 25 premenopausal women and 25 healthy controls. Re-
garding depression subtyping, compared patients meeting RDC criteria for endoge-
nous depression (n=6) with patients meeting DSM-IV criteria for atypical depression.
They found no significant differences in either mean 24-hour plasma cortisol or uri-
nary cortisol secretion between groups, including between patients and controls, alt-
hough mean cortisol values tended to be higher in endogenous group vs. controls.
Regarding ACTH, the only significant findings were those regarding basal ACTH,
which was significantly increased in depressed patients in general vs. controls, and
so was the AUC for basal cortisol. No other significant differences were identified.
The obvious drawback of the study was essentially the small sample size which
comprised only 6 and 7 patients in phenotypes of interest.
Brouwer et al.(2005), recruited a bigger total sample (n=113) of MDD patients. How-
ever, the numbers of patients meeting DSM-IV criteria for atypical or melanchol-ic
depression were relatively low (32 and 25 patients). The authors analyzed an ar-ray
of endocrine measures including serum and urinary cortisol levels. In subtype analy-
sis, only serum cortisol was significantly lower in atypical depressed patients vs.
those not matching either subtype.
Cizza et al. (2012), recruited 89 female patients from the POWER (Premeno-
pausal, Osteoporosis, Women, Alendronate, Depression) having reported a depres-
sive episode in the past 3 years. According to DSM-IV criteria, 51 patients had mel-
ancholic depression, 16 presented with atypical features, and in 22, no subtype crite-
ria were met. The 24-hour sampling of plasma ACTH and cortisol yielded no signifi-
cant difference in 24-hour cortisol plasma cortisol between groups. However, plasma
ACTH was significantly higher in the atypical subtype vs. the control group (F (1, 83)
= 4.01, p<0.05) vs. controls. A group by time interaction demonstrated that ACTH
was elevated in the atypical group only in the daytime, with greatest differences ob-
served from 10 AM to 5 PM. Besides, after adjustment for total body fat, the mean
24-hour adjusted log leptin value was elevated in the melancholic subgroup, as
compared with controls (Cizza et al. 2012).
The biggest patient sample analyzed so far was that recruited by Lamers et al.,
(2012), from the NESDA cohort. Authors compared some inflammatory, metabolic
markers, saliva cortisol awakening curves, and diurnal cortisol slope in 111 chronic
depressed patients with melancholic depression and 122 patients with atypical de-
pression. However, their labels did not refer to DSM classifiers, rather to results of a
latent class analysis performed using CIDI questionnaires, which showed no signifi-
cant effect of the measure of mood reactivity or interpersonal sensitivity, but a robust
effect of weight and sleep characteristics. In attempts to make the study more homo-
genous, the authors included only chronic severely depressed patients, as this ca-
tegory showed the most stable patterns of depressive symptoms. They demons-
trated that the atypical subtype differed from the melancholic subtype on a whole
range of symptoms, including area under the curve on the ground (AUCg) and diur-
nal cortsol slope measures (decreased in the atypical group) (Lamers et al., 2012).
Longitudinal studies
It is of note that due to a range of factors, and first of all the complexity of the
tests, there is a substantial lack of studies assessing the function of the HPA axis
across both phenotypes of interest longitudinally. Our search yielded only 5 articles
which used a longitudinal design. The first study found with the search terms was
that by R.G.Haskett et al. (2005), where the authors assessed the changes in the
DST response across several weeks in hospitalized patients. This methodology was
grounded in the previous observations that depressed patients who completed a
DST on day 2 of hospitalization had a higher frequency of cortisol nonsuppression
(71%) than depressed patients who were tested on days 3-6 (33%; Coccaro et al.,
1984). The study demonstrated that although there was an overall decrease in the
rates on non-suppression in both the patients with endogenous depression and con-
trol subjects (from 55 to 36%), this was mostly accounted for by the difference in the
control group, while the sensitivity of the test for the endogenous depressive patients
did not change significantly (Haskett et al., 1987).
In 1997, Steiger et al. performed a study assessing the levels of basal plasma cor-
tisol in 12 endogenous depressive patients diagnosed using RDC criteria on ad-
mission and post-treatment. They demonstrated that in ED patients, plasma cortisol
was significantly elevated on admission compared to healthy controls, and that it was
also significantly reduced after treatment, indicating that elevation was specific to
acute endogenous depression.
A study by Kaestner et al (2005) also employed a longitudinal design - pa-tients
were assessed on admission (t1) and after treatment, in remission (t2). The study
focused on assessing a range of factors, basal including basal cortisol and ba-sal
ACTH, in unmedicated, acutely depressed melancholic patients (n=37) compared
with 37 controls. They demonstrated that on admission, both cortisol and ACTH were
elevated in melancholic patients compared to controls, but not when compared with
non-melancholic patients. Plasma ACTH was still increased in melancholic patients
in remission compared to controls. At the same time, cortisol was elevated in acutely
depressed melancholic patients only, but not in remitted ones (Kaestner et al.,
2.005).
Pintor et al, 2013, measured ACTH and cortisol response using synthetic hu-
man CRF challenge in relapsing, non-relapsing and partially relapsing patients with
DSM- IV melancholic depression over a follow-up period of two years. In terms of the
overall comparison of depressed and healthy groups, significant differences were
observed for post-CRF ACTH, cortisol levels and for area under cortisol curve bet-
ween healthy controls and the three groups of melancholic patients.
Finally, the only article assessing the longitudinal course of HPA-axis abnormali-
ties in atypical depression is that by Geracioti et al., 1992, and represents a case
study of a female patient diagnosed with atypical depression across 6 months. The
results obtained by the authors correspond with the notion that atypical depression
has a different pattern of abnormalities: the patient was eucortisolemic in an acute
phase of depression, and further cortisol levels showed a negative correlation with a
deterioration of depressive symptoms.
Discussion
The key problem in diagnosis is the fact that elaborate classification systems that ex-
ist today are solely based on subjective descriptions of symptoms. Such detailed
phenomenology includes the description of multiple clinical subtypes; however, there
is no biological feature that distinguishes one subtype from another. Integrative ap-
proaches to understanding complex health issues can transcend disciplinary and
knowledge boundaries and provide opportunities to view phenomena from diverse
perspectives. A future diagnostic criteria system in which aetiology and pathophysi-
ology are essential in diagnostic decision-making would bring psychiatry closer to
other specialities of medicine (Juruena et al. 2007). Thus, the heterogeneity of clini-
cal conditions encompassed under the concept of major depression seems to be one
of the limiters of these advances. Therefore, the identification of distinct subtypes of
depression may allow advances in these areas by allowing the identification of more
homogeneous groups of patients, both in clinical aspects and in those related to the
aetiology and pathophysiology of the disorder presented. This depends on many fac-
tors like severity and type of depression, genotype, and history of exposure to stress,
temperament, and probably resilience (Mello et al., 2007)
The analysis of the articles focusing on the differences in the function of the HPA-
axis depending on depressive subtype has revealed a range of sufficient pitfalls in
research methodologies. In this sense, although the concept of a melancholic de-
pression subtype, equivalent to the concepts of endogenous or psychotic depres-
sion, has a long history of psychiatry and is well defined (Sullivan et al., 2002,
Baumeister & Parker, 2012). The atypical subtype, in turn, encompasses a hetero-
geneous group of patients and has only recently been introduced into the DSM-IV as
a specifier. Moreover, although the literature has given extensive support to the va-
lidity of depression with atypical features as distinct from melancholia and depres-
sion without atypical or melancholic features, there is still a certain degree of disa-
greement among researchers about which particular symptoms constitute this speci-
fier, and whether such factors as mood instability and interpersonal sensitivity have
the same weight in the dichotomy as biological reversed vegetative symptoms. Thus,
the lack of well-defined diagnostic criteria to characterize these subtypes of depres-
sion is reflected in the diversity of nomenclatures used in the literature to define
these subtypes.
In this sense, in this systematic review, we find a significant variation in the terms
used in the articles to define the melancholic and atypical subtypes. Besides the var-
iation in definition, a major complication is presented by the variation in approaches
to challenging tests (e.g. dosage, time of response measurement and threshold used
to defining non-suppression), and the variety of classifications used in order to define
«melancholic» or «atypical» subtypes complicate the task of arriving at a steady
conclusion.
Recently we have published a systematic review comparing the neuropsychologi-
cal performance of melancholic patients to non-melancholic depressive patients), in-
cluding atypical depressives, and healthy controls (Bosaipo et al. 2017). In this
study, the findings suggest that melancholic may have a distinct and impaired cogni-
tive performance compared to non-melancholic depressive patients on tasks involv-
ing verbal and visual memory, executive function, maintained attention and span, as
well as psychomotor speed, this last mainly when cognitive load is raised (Bosaipo et
al. 2017).
Besides, although the literature is increasingly demonstrating distinct differences in
clinical, biological, anatomical and response to treatment characteristics be-tween
these two subtypes of depression, this debate is still ongoing. In this sense, the HPA
axis play a vital role in the distinction between these subtypes, since stressful life
events play a major role in the pathogenesis and onset of depressive episodes
(Kendler et al., 2002). According to some authors, stress could lead to the onset of
the first depressive episode in genetically vulnerable individuals, making them even
more sensitive to stress in a fast forwarding fashion, compatible with the kindling hy-
pothesis by Post (1992). With this, the individual would need less stress to trigger
new crises, and it would become more vulnerable to the reprint of new depressive
episodes before different, sometimes milder, stressors (Post, 1992). Also, adverse
experiences in early life have been associated with significant increases in the risk of
developing depression in adulthood, particularly in response to additional stressors
(Tofoli et al. 2011; Cohen et al., 2001; Juruena, 2014). Thus, as the HPA axis is acti-
vated in response to stressors, changes in the functioning of this axis, at any level of
its components, and its regulations may play a pivotal etiological role in the onset of
depressive disorders (Holsboer, 2000; Tyrka et al., 2008).
Among the studies included in this systematic review that evaluated patients with
melancholic depression, most (n=17) studies focused on DST response. Of them,
the vast majority did indicate significantly elevated degrees of non-suppression in
melancholic patients.
Nevertheless, there have been two studies, which did not demonstrate this signifi-
cant association. The one Hubain et al. (1996), who performed DST in a large sam-
ple of 155 Newcastle Endogenous Depression Diagnostic Index (NEDDI) - de-fined
endogenous patients vs. a similar number of non-endogenous patients, in fact, did
initially show was a statistically significant difference in the dexamethasone suppres-
sion test response at 1600 h, but when the effects of age and severity of depression
were controlled, those differences disappeared. In a study by Berger et al.(1984), the
majority of comparison groups were other psychiatric patients, which somewhat
complicates drawing conclusions about the melancholic-nonmelancholic dichotomy,
however, this study showed the importance of biological symptoms such as weight
loss as a factor in non-suppression, confirming the notion that research may need to
focus more on the vegetative symptoms of subtypes of interest.
The inconsistence of results may partly be influenced by different dexamethasone
doses and suppression thresholds that were used. However, our review has demon-
strated that the most dramatic differences lie between studies that used different ap-
proaches to defining melancholic depression. So, when RDC was used as a defini-
tion scale, those having a diagnosis of endogenous depression showed higher non-
suppression rates than those with probable endogeneicity. Patients with DSM-
defined melancholic features tended to show higher non-suppression in melancholia,
too. It is also of note that strong support of elevated post-dex cortisol in melancholic
patients comes from the studies which either focused on particular symptoms which
are characteristic of the melancholic subtype or considered patients were meeting
more than one diagnostic scale criteria (e.g. both DSM-IV and RDC) or used their
operational criteria of endogeneicity. Another suggestion made by a few authors is
that depression characterised both by melancholic features and DST-non-
suppression is, in fact, a distinct form of depression. This inference stems from the
observations that melancholic patients who are non-suppressors exhibit higher basal
cortisol levels as well compared to melancholic patients normally responding to DST.
Also, this increase seems to be associated mainly with melancholic depression with
psychotic symptoms (Contreras et al., 2007).
Evidence of elevated basal cortisol and basal ACTH in melancholic patients is
much less consistent. Approximately half of the selected studies failed to demon-
strate differing levels of cortisol in melancholic patients compared to non-melancholic
ones or controls. This may be due to differences in methodology, or differences in
the diagnosis of melancholia (e.g. RDC definite endogenous criteria showed a more
consistent association than DSM-III melancholic criteria). Regarding ACTH, none of
the studies showed alteration in this measurement compared to controls.
However, when studies of melancholia focused on particular biological symptoms
such as weight loss and appetite loss/insomnia or used their operational criteria for
endogeneity mainly focusing on vital symptoms, they reported significant increases
in basal cortisol. This means that there may be a stronger association with biological
symptoms rather than subtypes as a whole.
When atypical studies were evaluated, importantly, the majority of those, re-
regardless of the outcome measures, did not show a difference between atypical pa-
tients and control subjects, although there was a significant difference between atyp-
ical and melancholic patients. There were indeed studies (Levitan et al., 2002; Anis-
man et al., 1999), which showed significantly decreased post-DST and basal cortisol
in atypical patients. However, it is important to consider that while Levitan et al.
(2002) compared atypical patients to healthy controls and their results indeed may
suggest hypoactive HPA-axis; Anisman et al. (1999) compared patients to non-
atypical depressed patients, which rather indicates the difference with another sub-
type.
The studies directly comparing the function of the HPA-axis between melancholic
and atypical patient groups are scarce and difficult for analysis since their methodol-
ogies vary largely. In particular, of the 7 studies comparing HPA axis functioning be-
tween patients with melancholic and atypical depression, only 2 used challenge tests
(Fountoulakis et al., 2004; Heizmann et al., 2014.). Among them, only the one by
Heinzmann et al.,(2014) despite a very different design from the rest of DST studies
observed in the whole review, showed a significant difference between sub-types,
There was no consensus in the studies assessing basal cortisol, while Brouwer et
al. (2005) and Lamers et al. (2012) did show decreased cortisol levels in atypical pa-
tients vs. controls and also underline the difference be-tween melancholic and atypi-
cal groups), others only indicated that they were not different between atypical and
controls. Notably, the design employed by Lamers et al. (2012) showed no signifi-
cant effect of the measure of mood reactivity or interpersonal sensitivity, but a robust
effect of weight and sleep characteristics. At the same time, the authors only recruit-
ed severely depressed atypical patients, which may also have contributed to the
strength of association.
This systematic review also considered a separate group including only longitudinal
studies. Although these studies are just a few and are also different in methodologi-
cal aspects, it could be suggested that in melancholic depression, elevated HPA-axis
function is a state rather than a trait characteristic, i.e. that remit-ted patients have
lower basal and post-challenge cortisol levels compared to acutely depressed pa-
tients. At the same time, the case study - and the only longitudinal study of the atypi-
cal subtype - showed a negative correlation between the severity of depressive
symptoms and cortisol levels, thus supporting the idea of the dichotomy.
Thus, although data in the literature seem to confirm that there are distinct pat-
terns of HPA axis functioning between the melancholic and atypical depression sub-
types, further studies with refining and homogeneous methodology are needed to
characterize this pattern better. These may be attributed to methodological differ-
ences, including varying challenges and doses and non-suppression thresholds, var-
ying availability of cortisol in urine, blood, saliva, or CSF. However, mainly the heter-
ogeneity of clinical conditions assessing the same endophenotype and incorporated
under the concept of major depression seems to be one of the limiters of these ad-
vances.
Novel advances in the methodology may shed light on the dichotomy in a more
precise manner. In particular, there are currently no published studies evaluating hair
and nail cortisol levels between the subtypes, but a few are underway. Be-sides,
speaking of challenge tests, among the articles included in this review, the majority
used the Dexamethasone Suppression Test. However, although the Dexa-
methasone Suppression Test remains widely used and widely studied as a biological
marker in psychiatry, this test has some limitations because of the pharmacokinetic
and pharmacodynamic characteristics of Dexamethasone that are very different from
cortisol. Unlike cortisol, Dexamethasone has low affinity to MR receptors. Therefore,
these studies allow us to investigate only the functioning of GR receptors in the sub-
types of depression (Pariante et al., 2002; Juruena et al. 2006). Future studies might
use different challenges, like MR antagonists, such as Spironolactone, MR agonists,
such as Fludrocortisone, the Prednisolone suppression test, which appears to bind
to MR as well as GR. MR function, and perhaps more important, MR/GR ratio, re-
mains understudied in depressed populations, and it seems like an interesting pro-
spect in this area (Juruena et al. 2013).
In general, this review has provided a rather convincing support for the presence
of a difference in HPA-axis activity between the two subtypes, melancholic and atyp-
ical depression, regardless of the classification. However, it is much more difficult to
conclude whether atypicality is associated with hypofunctional HPA-axis and en-
hanced negative feedback (such as implied in PTSD which was confirmed in a study
by Halbreich et al..1989), or simply is not different from controls. It may be that the
severity and the course of atypical depression, as well as the presence of particular
vegetative symptoms (hypersomnia, weight gain as opposed to interpersonal sensi-
tivity), are stronger predictors of decreased basal and post-challenge cortisol levels.
However, this is yet to be established in studies employing a more unified efficient
methodology.
The assessment of other factors potentially interfering with the dichotomy is out-
side the scope of this review. However, it should be noted that in the same studies
which showed conflicting results regarding hypocortisolism in atypical depression,
there was a much stronger association with elevated inflammatory factors (Lamers et
al., 2012). This has driven a novel appraisal of the two subtypes, suggesting that
while "typical"(or melancholic) depression has core pathophysiological features of
overactive HPA-axis, what we call "atypical" depression may rather be comprehend-
ed as immuno-metabolic depression (Penninx et al., 2016). The precise interaction
of potentially decreased activity of the HPA-axis with immune and metabolic abnor-
malities in the atypical subtype remains to be investigated.
CONCLUSION
Conflicts of Interest:
MF Juruena has within the last year received honoraria for speaking from GSK,
Lundbeck and Pfizer. AH Young received honoraria for lectures and advisory boards
for all major pharmaceutical companies with drugs used in affective and related dis-
orders. Investigator-initiated studies from AZ, Eli Lilly and Lundbeck. Dr MB and BA
has no conflicts of interest to declare.
REFERENCES
Amsterdam, J. D., Maislin, G., Gold, P., & Winokur, A. (1989). The assessment of abnormalities
in hormonal responsiveness at multiple levels of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenocortical
axis in depressive illness. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14(1– 2), 43–62.
Anisman, H., Ravindran, a V, Griffiths, J., & Merali, Z. (1999). Endocrine and cytokine corre-
lates of major depression and dysthymia with typical or atypical features. Molecular
Psychiatry, 4(2), 182–8.
Asnis, G. M., McGinn, L. K., & Sanderson, W. C. (1995). Atypical depression: Clinical aspects
and noradrenergic function. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(1), 31–36.
Banki, C. M., Arato, M., et al. (1986). Associations among dexamethasone non-suppression
and TRH-induced hormonal responses: increased specificity for melancholia. Psychoneuro-
endocrinology, 11(2), 205–211.
Barocka, A., Pichl, J., Beck, G., & Rupprecht, R. (1987). Factors interfering with the 1 mg
dexamethasone suppression test in depression. Pharmacopsychiatry, 20(6), 258–261.
Baumeister, H., Parker, G. (2012). Meta-review of depressive subtyping models. Journal of
Affective Disorders 139:126–140.
Benazzi, F. (2002). Can only reversed vegetative symptoms define atypical depression? Eur
Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. Dec;252(6):288-93
Berger, M., Pirke, K. M., et al. (1984). The limited utility of the dexamethasone suppression test
for the diagnostic process in psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 372–382.
Bosaipo, NB, Foss, MP, Young, AH, Juruena, MF Neuropsychological Changes in Melancholic
and Atypical Depression: A Systematic Review Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, Feb
2017, 73:309-25
Brouwer, J. P., Appelhof, B. C., et al. (2005). Thyroid and adrenal axis in major depression: A
controlled study in outpatients. European Journal of Endocrinology, 152(2), 185–191.
Carroll, B.J., Feinberg, M., et al. (1981). A specific laboratory test for the diagnosis of melan-
cholia. Standartization, validation, and clinical utility. Arch Gen Psychiatry, Vol. 38.
Carroll, B. J., et al. (1982). The Dexamethasone Suppression Test for Melancholia.
Brit.J.Psychiat. (140) 292–305.
Casper, R. C., Swann, A. C., Stokes, P. E., Chang, S., Katz, M. M., & Garver, D. (1987).
Weight loss, cortisol levels, and dexamethasone suppression in major depressive disorder.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 75(3), 243–250.
Casper, R. C., Kocsis, J., et al. (1988). Cortisol measures in primary major depressive disorder
with hypersomnia or appetite increase. Journal of Affective Disorders, 15(2), 131–140.
Cizza, G., Ronsaville, D. S., et al. (2012). Clinical subtypes of depression are associated with
specific metabolic parameters and circadian endocrine profiles in women: The power study.
PLoS ONE, 7(1).
Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ. (1994) Neuropsychopharmacologic challenge in biological
psychiatry. Clin Chem. 1994 Feb;40(2):319-27.
Cohen, P., Brown, J., and Smailes, E. (2001). Child abuse and neglect and the development of
mental disorders in the general population. Developmentand Psychopathology.981-99.
Contreras F, Menchon JM, Urretavizcaya M, Navarro MA, Vallejo J, Parker G. Hormonal differ-
ences between psychotic and non-psychotic melancholic depression. J Affect Disord.
2007:100:65-73.
Coryell, W. (2007), The facets of melancholia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 115: 31–36.
De Kloet et al.(1998) Brain Corticosteroid Receptor Balance in Health and Disease. Endocrine
Reviews 19(3): 269–01
Diorio D, Viau V, Meaney MJ. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the
regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. J Neurosci 1993; 13: 3839–
3847.
Evans, D.L., Nemeroff, C.B. (1987): The clinical use of the Dexamethasone Suppression test in
DSM-III affective disorders: Correlation with the severe depressive subtypes of melancholia
and psychosis. J Psychiat Res 21: 185
Fink, M. and Taylor, M. A. (2007), Resurrecting melancholia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
115: 14–20.
Fountolakis, K.N., Iacovides, A. et al. (2004). Relationship among Dexamethasone Suppres-
sion Test, personality disorders and stressful life events in clinical subtypes of major depres-
sion: An exploratory study Annals of General Hospital Psychiatry. 3:15
Geracioti, T.D., Orth, D.N., et al. (1992). Serial cerebrospinal fluid corticotrophin-releasing
hormone concentrations in healthy and depressed humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 74:
1325–1330.
Gold, P.W. (2015). The organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in depressive
illness. Molecular Psychiatry; 20, 32–47; doi:10.1038/mp.2014.163.
Gómez-Gil, E., Navinés, R., et al. (2010) Hormonal responses to the 5-HT1A agonist buspi-
rone in remitted endogenous depressive patients after long-term imipramine treatment. Psy-
choneuroendocrinology. 35(4):481-9
Guechot, J., Lepine, J. P., et al. (1987). Simple laboratory test of neuroendocrine disturbance
in depression: 11 p.m. saliva cortisol. Neuropsychobiology, 18, 1–4.
Halbreich, U., Olympia, J., et al. (1989). Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal activity in endogenously
depressed post-traumatic stress disorder patients. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14(5), 365–
370.
Haskett, R. F., Carroll, B. J., & Lohr, E. (1989). Comparison of Early and Delayed Inpatient
dexamethasone suppression tests.pdf
Heinzmann, J. M., Kloiber, S., et al. (2014). Mice selected for extremes in stress reactivity
reveal key endophenotypes of major depression: A translational approach. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology, 49(1), 229–243.
Holsboer, F. (2000). The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychophar-
macology. 23:477-501.
Hubain, P., Van Veeren, et al. (1996). Neuroendocrine and sleep variables in major depressed
inpatients: Role of severity. Psychiatry Research, 63(1), 83–92.
Joseph-Vanderpool, J. R., Rosenthal, N. E.,et al. (1991). Abnormal pituitary-adrenal responses
to corticotropin-releasing hormone in patients with seasonal affective disorder: clinical and
pathophysiological implications. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 72(6),
1382–1387.
Joyce, P.R., Mulder, R.T., et al. (2002). Melancholia: Definitions, Risk Factors, Personality,
Neuroendocrine Markers and Differential Antidepressant Response. Australian & New Zea-
land Journal of Psychiatry. Vol 36, Issue 3, pp. 376 - 383
Juruena MF, Cleare AJ, Papadopoulos AS, Poon L, Lightman S, Pariante CM. “Different re-
sponses to Dex and prednisolone in the same depressed patients”. Psychopharmacology
2006; 189 (2): 225-35.
Juruena, MF; Marques, AH; Mello, AF; Mello, MF. (2007). A paradigm for understanding and
treating psychiatric illness. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 29(Suppl. 1), s1-s2.
Juruena, MF, Pariante, CM; Papadopoulos, AS; Poon, L; Lightman, S, Cleare, AJ (2009).
Prednisolone suppression test in depression: a prospective study of the role of HPA axis
dysfunction in treatment resistance. British Journal of Psychiatry, 194, 342-49.
Juruena MF, Cleare AJ. Overlap between atypical depression, seasonal affective disorder and
chronic fatigue syndrome. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2007: 29(Suppl I):S19-26
Juruena, M. F., Cleare, A. J., Papadopoulos, A. S., Poon, L., Lightman, S., & Pariante, C. M.
(2010). The prednisolone suppression test in depression: Dose—response and changes
with antidepressant treatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(10), 1486–1491.
Juruena MF, Calil HM, Fleck MP, Del Porto JA. Melancholia in Latin American studies: a dis-
tinct mood disorder for the ICD-11. Rev Bras de Psiquiatr 2011:33(Suppl I):S48-58.
Juruena, MF. Early-life stress and HPA axis trigger recurrent adulthood depression Epilepsy &
Behavior, 38, 148-59, 2014.
Kaestner, F., Hettich, M., et al. (2005). Different activation patterns of proinflammatory cyto-
kines in melancholic and non-melancholic major depression are associated with HPA axis
activity. Journal of Affective Disorders, 87(2–3), 305–311.
Karlo i , D., Serretti, A., et al. (2012). Serum concentrations of CRP, IL-6, TNF-α and cortisol
in major depressive disorder with melancholic or atypical features. Psychiatry Research,
198(1), 74–80.
Kendler, K.S., Eaves, L.J., et al. (1996). The identification and validation of distinct depressive
syndromes in a population-based sample of female twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry; 53(5):391-9.
Kendler, K.S., Sheth, K., et al. (2002). Childhood parental loss and risk or first-onset of major
depression and alcohol dependence: the time-decay of risk and sex differences. Psychol
Med. 32(7):1187-94.
Klein, D.F., Davis, J.M. Diagnosis and drug treatment of psychiatric disorders. Williams &
Wilkins, Baltimore, 1969.
Klein DF. The pharmacological validation of psychiatric diagnosis. In: RobinsL, BarrettJ, eds.
Validity of psychiatric diagnosis. New York: Raven, 1989, 203–216.
Lamers, F., Vogelzangs, N., et al. (2012). Evidence for a differential role of HPA-axis function,
inflammation and metabolic syndrome in melancholic versus atypical depression. Molecular
Psychiatry, 18(6), 692–699.
Leventhal., A. M., Rehn, L.P. (2005). The empirical status of melancholia: Implications for
psychology. Clinical Psychology Review 25; 25 – 44
Levitan, R. D., Kaplan, A. S., et al. (1997). Low plasma cortisol in bulimia nervosa patients with
reversed neurovegetative symptoms of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 41(3), 366–368.
Levitan, R. D., Vaccarino, F. J., et al. (2002). Low-dose dexamethasone challenge in women
with atypical major depression: Pilot study. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 27(1),
47–51.
Liu, Y., Yieh, L., et al. (2016). Metabolomic biosignature differentiates melancholic depressive
patients from healthy controls. BMC Genomics, 17, 669.
Maes, M., Schotte, C., et al. (1990). Clinical subtypes of unipolar depression: Part II. Quantita-
tive and qualitative clinical differences between the vital and nonvital depression groups.
Psychiatry Research, 34(1), 43–57.
Maes, M., Maes, L., et al. (1992). A clinical and biological validation of the DSM-III melancholia
diagnosis in men: results of pattern recognition methods. J Psychiatr Res. 26: 183-196.
Marques-deak, A. H., Neto, et al. (2007). Cytokine profiles in women with different subtypes of
major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 41, 152–159.
McGinn, L., Asnis, G., & Rubinson, E. (1996). Biological and clinical validation of atypical
depression. Psychiatry Research, 60, 191–198.
Mello AF, Juruena MF, Pariante CM, Tyrka AR, Price LH, Carpenter LL, Porto JA. Depression
and stress: is there an endophenotype?. Rev Bras.Psiquiatr. 2007 29:s13-8.
Michopoulos, I., Zervas, I. M., et al. (2008) Neuropsychological and hypothalamic-pituitary-axis
function in female patients with melancholic and non-melancholic depression. European Ar-
chives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258(4), 217–225.
Miller, K. B., & Nelson, J. C. (1987). Does the dexamethasone suppression test relate to sub-
types, factors, symptoms, or severity? Archives of General Psychiatry, 44(9), 769–74.
Mitchell, P., Smythe, G., et al. (1990). Hormonal responses to fenfluramine in depressive
subtypes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157(OCT.), 551–557.
Mulder, R. T., Porter, R. J., & Joyce, P. R. (2003). The prolactin response to fenfluramine in
depression: Effects of melancholia and baseline cortisol. Journal of Psychopharmacology,
17(1), 97–102.
Nelson JC, Davis JM. (1997) DST studies in psychotic depression: a meta-analysis. Am J
Psychiatry; 154: 1497–1503.
O’Keane, V. and Dinan, T.G. (1991) Prolactin and cortisol responses to d-fenfluramine in major
depression: Evidence for diminished responsivity of central serotoninergic function.
Am.J.Psychiatry 148, 1009-1015.
Orsel, S., Karadag, H., et al. (2016).Diagnosis and Classification Subtyping of Depressive
Disorders: Comparison of Three Methods, Klinik Psikofarmakoloji Bülteni-Bulletin of Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 20:1,57-65
Orsel S, Karadag H, Turkcapar H & Kahilogullari A K (2010) Diagnosis and Classification
Subtyping of Depressive Disorders: Comparison of Three Methods, Klinik Psikofarmakoloji
Bülteni-Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20:1, 57-65
Parker, G., Roy, K., et al. (2002). Atypical depression: a reappraisal. Am J Psychiatry 159:
1470–1479.
Parker G, Paterson A, Hadzi-Pavlovic D. (2015). Cleaving depressive diseases from depres-
sive disorders and non-clinical states. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 131(6):426-33
Parker, G., McCrawa, S., et al. (2013). Validation of a new prototypic measure of melancholia.
Compr Psychiatry. 54(7):835-41.
Paslakis, G., Krumm, B.,et al. (2011). Discrimination between patients with melancholic de-
pression and healthy controls: Comparison between 24-h cortisol profiles, the DST and the
Dex/CRH test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(5), 691–698.
Penninx, B. W., Milaneschi, Y., Lamers, F., & Vogelzangs, N. (2013). Understanding the somat-
ic consequences of depression: biological mechanisms and the role of depression symptom
profile. BMC Medicine, 11, 129.
Peselow, E.D., & Fieve, R.R. Depressive Attributional Style and the Dexamethasone Suppres-
sion Test: Relationship to the Endogenous/Melancholic Distinction and to Each Other. Psy-
chopathology 1992;25:173–182
Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal
models to human behavior. Neuron 2005; 48: 175–187.
Pintor L, Torres X, Navarro V, Martinez de Osaba MA, Matrai S, Gastó C. Corticotropin-
releasing factor test in melancholic patients in depressed state versus recovery: a compara-
tive study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2007:31(5):1027-1033.
Porter, R., & Gallagher, P. (2006). Abnormalities of the HPA axis in affective disorders: Clinical
subtypes and potential treatments. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 18 (5), 193-209.
Post RM. (1992). Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affec-
tive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 149:999-1010.
Posternak, M.A., Zimmerman, M. (2001). Symptoms of atypical depression. Psychiatry
Res;104(2):175-81.
Quitkin, F.M., Stewart, J.W., et al. (1993) Columbia atypical depression: a subgroup of depres-
sives with better response to MAOI than to tricyclic antidepressants or placebo. Br J Psychi-
atry Suppl. 21:30–34.
Rubin, R. T., Poland, R. E., et al. (1987). Neuroendocrine aspects of primary endogenous
depression. Arch.Gen.Psychiatry, 44, 328–336.
Rush AJ, Weissenburger JE. Melancholic symptom features and DSM-IV. Am J Psychiatry
1994;151:489–498.
Schotte, C.K., Maes, M., et al. (1997). Cluster analytic validation of the DSM melancholic
depression. The threshold model: integration of quantitative and qualitative distinctions be-
tween unipolar depressive subtypes. Psychiatry Res. 71: 181-195.
Steiger, A., & Holsboer, F. (1997). Nocturnal secretion of prolactin and cortisol and the sleep
EEG in patients with major endogenous depression during an acute episode and after full
remission. Psychiatry Research, 72(2), 81–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-
1781(97)00097-8
Stetler, C., et al. (2013). Depression and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Activation: A Quantita-
tive Summary of Four Decades of Research. Psychosomatic Medicine 73:114–126
Stewart, J. W., Quitkin, F. M., et al. (2005). Defining the boundaries of atypical depression:
Evidence from the HPA axis supports course of illness distinctions. Journal of Affective Dis-
orders, 86(2–3), 161–167.
Stewart, J. W., McGrath, P. J., et al. (2007), Atypical depression: current status and relevance
to melancholia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 115: 58–71.
Sullivan, P.F., Kessler, R.C. (1998). Latent class analysis of lifetime depressive symptoms in
the national comorbidity survey. Am J Psychiatry; 155(10):1398-406.
Sullivan, P.F., Prescott, C.A., Kendler, K.S. (2002). The subtypes of major depression in a twin
registry. J. Affect. Disord. 68: 273–284.
Thase, E., Ph, D., Jarrett, B., Kupfer, J., & Mallinger, G. (1989). E. Thase, (March), 329–333.
Tofoli, S.M.C., Baes, C.W., et al. (2011). Early life stress, HPA axis, and depression Psycholo-
gy & Neuroscience. 4, 2, 229 - 234
Tyrka, A.R., Wie,r L., et al. (2008). Childhood parental loss and adult hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenal function. Biol Psychiatry. 63:1147-154.
Ulrich-Lai & Herman (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 397-409
Valdivieso, S., Duval, F., et al. (1996). Growth hormone response to clonidine and the cortisol
response to dexamethasone in depressive patients. Psychiatry Research, 60(1), 23– 32.
WHO(2017). Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders. Global Health Estimates. WHO
reference number: WHO/MSD/MER/2017.2
Winokur, G., Black, D. W., & Nasrallah, A. (1987). DST nonsuppressor status: Relationship to
specific aspects of the depressive syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, 22(3), 360–368.
Wong, M. L., Kling, M. A., et al. (2000). Pronounced and sustained central hypernoradrenergic
function in major depression with melancholic features: relation to hypercortisolism and cor-
ticotropin-releasing hormone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Unit-
ed States of America, 97(1), 325–30.
Yehuda R, Giller EL, Southwick SM, Lowy MT, Mason JW. (1991) Hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenal dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 15;30(10):1031-48.
Young, E. A., Carlson, N. E., & Brown, M. B. (2001). Twenty-four-hour ACTH and cortisol
pulsatility in depressed women. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(2), 267–276.
Thre
Dos shol
e d
Meas-
(For (for
Type of ure- Subtype
Au- chal- № of sub- Comparison
meas- ment Results
thor, leng chal- jects group
ure- time definition
year e leng
ment (s)
tests e
) tests
)
loss of sexual
interest;
weight loss
Ru- 1 7 am 3,5 RDC «de- n = 40 n = 40 38% NS in
bin et mg μg/dl finite» healthy sub- the endoge-
3 pm endoge- nous group
al., jects
nous vs 12% in
1987 11 pm
controls
Weight
loss: con-
tinuous
severity
measures
from 1lb
or more
Wino 1 8 am "Strong DSM-III n = 423 no healthy Melancholia
kur et mg suppres melancho- MDD controls significantly
al., and/o pres- associated
lic MDD patients
r4 sors": with high-
1987
pm cortisol degree
equal non-
to or suppression
lower
than (24% vs 9%,
1.5 p=0.01).
μg/dl,
n = 163
High
non-
suppres
pres-
sors:
(corti-
sol
equal
to or
greater
than 6
μg/dl,
n=164)
Lower basal
cortisol and
higher dex
suppression
rates in
PTSD-ED vs
MDD-ED
ACTH Amster- 250u 8:30 3 baseline Quanti- HDRS 1st: 16 1st: 1st: No
ster- g measures; after tative (9 with 11 differ-
dam, (ACT ACTH compar- melan- healt ence be-
1989 H) 30,60,90,120,1 ison cholic hy tween
80,240 min fea- con- groups
tures); trols at base-
line.
2nd: 72 Larger
(51 with increas-
melan- es in
nd
cholic 2 : cortisol
features, 37 in the
21 healt MDD
without) hy group
con- after
trols ACTH
(p<0,02)
;
2nd: No
differ-
ence at
baseline;
no sta-
tistically
signifi-
cant dif-
ferences
between
groups,
but a
trend to
higher
cortisol
levels in
the mel-
ancholic
group
(P=0,31)
.
oCRH Amster- 1,0ug 8:30. 3 (every HDRS 26 11 No dif-
ster- /kg 15 minutes) (14 healt ference
dam, baseline melan- hy in corti-
1989 measures for cholic con- sol lev-
ACTH and cor- fea- trols els after
tisol levels; tures) chal-
after oCRH:0, lenge;
30,60,90,120,1 depres-
80, 240 min sive pa-
tients
had
lower
ACTH
response
com-
pared to
controls.
(p<0,05)
The ef-
fect was
larger in
melan-
cholic
features
(p<0,04)
Dex/CR Paslak- 100u 3 pm of day 2 Quanti- HDRS 26 33 Low
H test is, 2010 g (1 day after tative 21 moder- healt specific-
hCR DST) compar- ate to hy ity and
H ison severe con- low sen-
melan- trols sibility
cholic for the
depres- CRH/De
sion x test
(78.8%
and
30,8%
respec-
tively)
Basal Measures
DSM-III melan-
Orsel, S. Basal plasma n = 38 (DSM- DSM-III Signifi-
cholic depres-
et al., cortisol 8 am III criteria) criteria: cantly ele-
sion+cluster anal-
vated basal
2010 ysis which n = 40 «endo- n = 40 non-
cortisol in
identi- genous» cluster melancholic
the
fied«endogenous» (included both MDD, incl.
«ëndoge-
and «non- DSM mel and
n = 27 nous» clus-
endogenous» non-mel pa-
«simple» ter
subtype tients)
MDD
Discriminators: n = 4 BD1
• early morning n = 2 Dys-
awakening thymia
• distinct quality of n = 5 De-
mood pressive
• feelings of guilt, disorder
non-reactivity NOS
• suicidal ideation n = 2 Ad-
• psychomotor justment
disorders disorder
«Non-
endogenous
» cluster:
(included 8
mel pa-
tients) n =
38
Liu et al., cortisol as part The CORE scale n = 21 n = 58 an- Increased
2016 of 228 meta- for melancholic xious de- basal corti-
bolites depression; pression sol in me-
lancholia
Anxious depres- n = 100
vs healthy
sion defined as healthy
controls;
controls
number of comor-
bid anxiety disor-
ders on the
M.I.N.I. Interna-
tional Neuropsy-
chiatric Inter-
view > 0
Time/sourc
Type of Authors, Control
e of Subtype Sample Results
measure- year measure- definition size group
ment
ment
3.No signifi-
cant differ-
ences in CSF
cortisol
Similar in
AD and
controls.
Definition
of
Type Definition
melanchol
of Authors, Measureme of atypical Comparis
ic Results
measureme year nt time subtype/ on group
subtype/
nt sample size
sample
size
Lamers et Saliva corti- CIDI + CIDI + pre- n = 543 Lower
al., 2012 sol awaken- previous vious latent healthy AUCg,
ing curves latent class class analy- controls lower di-
(CAR 30, 45 analysis sis (own urnal cor-
and 60 (own oper- operational tisol slope
minutes lat- ational cri- criteria) in atypical
er. Addi- teria) of of patients vs melan-
tional sam- patients with persis- cholic pa-
ple at 11am. with per- tent chronic tients and
Diurnal cor- sistent depression vs controls
tisol slope chronic n = 111
depression
n = 122
hHR: 219
hLR:219
hIR: 219
Type of
Subtype
Au- Subtype Comparison
measurement
thors, Design + Results
(incl.dose
year definition group
sample size
and timing
specifications)
HIGHLIGHTS
Different depressive subtype classification criteria may influence severity and
treatment.
Depressive subtype is an important factor influencing Hypothalamus-Pituitary-
Adrenal (HPA) axis activity
Melancholic patients has increased post-challenge cortisol levels than Atypical
Depressive patients
Studies focusing on reversed vegetative symptoms, demonstrated a decrease in the
activity of the HPA axis in atypical depressives compared to controls, but the ma-
jority did not distinguish it from healthy controls.
The correct definition of depression subtypes remains a cornerstone in biological
research in affective disorders.
Future studies should consider epigenetic studies of HPA-axis related to both sub-
types, with an emphasis on vegetative symptom and standardized methodologies.