Hunting and Gathering Societies 3-41
Hunting and Gathering Societies 3-41
Hunting and Gathering Societies 3-41
Introduction to Part II
A. Plan for next five chapters:
The chapters in Part II will follow closely the traditional division of societies into
technological types. We will emphasize the effects of environmental variation on the adap-
tations of human cultures, following Steward. For present purposes, we will take the basic
types of societies as historical givens. The last series of chapters in the course will return to
the problems of the evolutionary transformation of one kind of society into another after we
have considered evolutionary mechanisms and the nature of systemic interactions with en-
vironments in more detail.
The discussion will focus on five basic types of societies, defined initially in terms
of their basic subsistence technology (mode of production):
(a) Hunting and Gathering Societies. Those peoples whose technology is de-
signed to use primarily wild game and plant resources.
(b) Horticultural Societies. Societies that depend primarily on cultivated plants
for subsistence, but that lack the use of draft animals and the plow.
(c) Pastoral Societies. People who emphasize the raising of livestock.
(d) Agrarian Societies. Societies that depend mainly on plant cultivation, and
that use draft animals and plows.
(e) Commercial/Industrial Societies. Societies with a majority of the popula-
tion engaged in trade and manufacturing.
There are several more specialized types; e.g., Lenski and Lenski (1987) also list
fishing, and maritime societies. These are of minor importance generally, although mari-
time societies such as the Greek city-states of ancient times and Venice at the beginning of
the modern period do play an important role in theoretical discussions because they preco-
ciously resemble modern societies. Figure 3-1 reproduces a diagram from Lenski and Len-
ski’s (1987:82) that summarizes the most common, but not the only, evolutionary pathways
Industrial Societies
Level of
Technology
Agrarian Maritime
Societies Societies
Herding (?)
Horticultural Societies
Societies
Fishing Societies
Hunting and Gathering Societies
Type of Environment
The Big Question is: does human ecology provide a useful taxonomy? To the extent
that Steward’s concept of the culture core is useful, we expect to find a complex of associ-
ated traits that surround the technology and vary as technology and environment vary. In
essence, we are testing the utility of the ecological/evolutionary approach to human behav-
ior by organizing the grand sweep of the data on human diversity into a few categories us-
ing the culture core concept. If this exercise results in a compact, informative taxonomy,
there must be something to the idea.
With regard to cultural traits, aside from the toolkit itself, that are candidates for in-
clusion in the cultural core, we will focus on the following:
(a) Demographic variables, including average human density, settlement size,
and degree of mobility.
(b) Social and political organization, including patterns of relations between
individuals, degree of stratification, degree of occupational specialization, pat-
terns of leadership, institutions of social control and collective decision-mak-
Taxonomy is always a difficult business, and ecological (functional rather than lin-
eage-based) taxonomies are always very messy in detail. Let us agree to take our classifi-
cation system a bit lightly. There is no nice neat branching pattern such as is furnished by
organic evolution as it makes species. Humans are all the same species; races and cultures
fairly freely exchange ideas and genes. Mixed types, borderline cases, and the like are
bound to be common. The analogy between the historical and contemporary variants is es-
pecially likely to be rather imperfect. Ancient Rome and modern India are not exactly com-
parable, even if they are agrarian societies in our scheme. We will often have trouble
finding the best criterion to classify given examples at any level of a functional classifica-
tion scheme. To take a concrete example, how do we classify the African forest Pygmies?
They gain about half their subsistence hunting and gathering in the forest and about half
from horticultural crops obtained by working for Bantu horticulturalists. We can avoid end-
less terminological hassles here only by agreeing to use the classification as a means to an
end, not as an end in itself.
Does a culture core exist that varies as a function of environment, given technology?
If the Stewardian hypothesis is correct, and especially if the enlarged culture core of mod-
ern ecological anthropologists is correct, we expect to find strong statistical associations
with these variables and technology especially when the environmental variation within ba-
sic technical categories is taken into account. We also expect to be able to interpret much
of the variation in the culture core in adaptive terms. In the last lecture we did see that the
associations in Western North America were fairly strong.
Anatomically modern humans spread to Australia and America, the last major hab-
itable land areas of the Earth. The world was full of people, if rather thinly populated, by
the eve of the evolution of horticulture 10,000 years ago. The relatively recent shift from
B. Ethnographic Knowledge
The food foragers known from contemporary ethnographies (i.e., those that survived
long enough to receive reasonable description by professional anthropologists) are a poor-
ish sample of this variation. Most of the cases are peoples who live in, or recently lived in
environments so marginal the expanding farmers had not evicted them. The best known
studies of relatively unacculturated peoples are from desert dwellers like the Australian Ab-
origines, the African Bushmen, and tropical forest hunters from South America, Africa, and
SE Asia. North America had a great variety of hunters and gatherers until the mid 19th cen-
tury, many living in quite productive environments, but professional anthropology arrived
a little too late to observe them in a pristine state. The societies of aboriginal California are
an example. By judiciously combining explorers’ accounts, and the ethnographic and ar-
chaeological evidence, we can obtain some idea of earlier hunter-gatherer societies and
those from richer environments. But the story of late Pleistocene hunters in their full glory,
hunting Mammoths and Cave Bears, will always have a bit of the imaginative reconstruc-
tion about it! That style of life mostly disappeared with the climatic changes and waves of
big game extinctions about 10,000 years BP which we will discuss more in Chapters 22 &
23.
C. Mythologizing Hunters
Because hunting and gathering subsistence characterized humans as we evolved,
and because we have practiced this form of subsistence for more of our history than any
other, hunting and gathering peoples are prime candidates for mythologizing. This is Orig-
inal Man, wild and free, just as evolution fashioned him (sic). For a sweeping critique of
contemporary society, what more effective technique than to portray hunters and gatherers
as happy, healthy, peaceable, moral, wise, etc., and moderns as pale, corrupt shadows of
Natural Man and Woman? For a sweeping defense of modern society with its many petty
frustrations, what is more effective than to portray hunters and gatherers as ignorant, super-
stitious, violent, dirty, miserable wretches, barely more than animals? Neither portrayal
II. Technology
The toolkit of most hunter-gatherer peoples is quite simple. Light killing weapons,
spears, atlatls, bows and arrows, and simple choppers and knives for processing the carcass-
es are all that many groups use. Food collection equipment is often no more than a digging
stick and a slab of bark or a simple wooden bowl (Figure 3-2). Food preparation is likely
to be quite rudimentary, as simple as roasting a small animal by throwing it skin and all into
an open fire. Shelters are often very simple windbreaks or huts. Some toolkits were consid-
erably more elaborate than this minimum. For example, the Eskimo had sophisticated win-
ter clothing, kayaks, dog sleds, igloos, barbed harpoons, and other advanced items.
California Indians had basketry tight enough to hold and boil water (using red-hot stones
dropped into the basket). The more sophisticated toolkits were restricted to extreme envi-
ronments on the one hand, where nothing less would do (the arctic), or to more provident
environments for reasons we will see below (California).
Simple tools are used in sophisticated ways. To hunt and gather successful requires
considerable knowledge of natural history, and a good level of physical hardihood. Think
backpacking and living in a tent 365 days/year with no storebought food. Ethnoecologists
(students of the ecological knowledge of hunters and gatherers) have shown that they rec-
ognize a huge variety of plant and animal species and have an intimate understanding of
animal behavior and patterns of plant growth, flowering and fruiting. The tracking ability
of hunters is legendary. A human or animal can be followed on the basis of the most ob-
scure signs. Such skills are quite useful to people whose weapons are so weak that an out-
right kill is much less probable than a debilitating wound that may take hours or even days
to kill a large animal. Hunters will follow an injured large animal for days until it weakens
enough to be finished off.
In the most recent hunting and gathering societies, plant products supply the bulk of
the calories, while fish and game supply a major share of the protein. This is well demon-
strated in the case of the well-studied !Kung Bushmen1, where plant foods supply 60-80%
of calories (Tanaka, in Lee and DeVore, 1976). Plant collecting also requires fairly sophis-
ticated technical and natural-historical skill to find and process. For example, the acorn-
II. Demography
The single most important consequence of hunter-gatherer technology is that it or-
dinarily supports very low human population densities. It is not a very efficient system of
1. If you’re wondering what on earth !Kung represents, here’s the answer: the exclamation point is
used to represent the tongue click with which the !Kung San precede the word! Some languages use
several such unusual sounds.
2. In this process, mashed acorns are soaked in water and rinsed to remove the tannin. Tannin tastes
bitter and binds proteins, inhibityng their digestion, thus serving to discourage herbivores by mak-
ing acorns a less attractive food source.
Yellen (in Lee and DeVore, 1976) gives a travel diary for the Dobe group of !Kung,
showing 38 moves in a five and a half month period. The longest continuous stay in one
place was 26 days. However, about 2/5 of the total time was spent at their base camp (in 6
periods). The basic pattern was one of more or less brief excursions to distant areas fol-
lowed by a return to the base camp. The !Kung are in some ways less mobile than many
hunter-gatherers because, especially during the dry season, they are tied to the relatively
few permanent waterholes in the Kalahari.
Any substantial movement at all puts severe limits on the sophistication of toolkits if
all one’s belongings must be hand carried from place to place every few days. Imagine
what a Shoshone woman in the Great Basin, who might have had to walk several km. to a
new camp every few days, would do with a ceramic pot. Throw the fragile, heavy monster
away! It also means that food storage is difficult. Once a group has to move much at all,
long-term food storage is impossible. Food is heavy and bulky in quantity and left unguard-
ed it is vulnerable to pests and pilferage. Thus most lower latitude hunter-gatherers do not
even use simple food storage techniques such as making dried meat to any extent (Figure
3-3). Groups like the North-Western salmon fishermen and the Central California acorn
leachers who were sedentary enough to make accumulating stores worthwhile. Arctic peo-
ple can freeze meat, though the incidence of botulism fatalities from consuming putrid meat
is said to have been one of the hazards of their subsistence system.
The need to move frequently also puts a direct damper on population growth rates.
Movement on foot limits each woman to one dependent child at a time. !Kung San space
children deliberately so that they only have to carry one at a time, using infanticide or abor-
tion, if necessary, to space children about 4 years apart. N. Blurton-Jones at UCLA (cites
are in Chapter 9) has calculated from !Kung San data just how limiting it is to have to deal
with small children while moving camp and foraging. Anyone who has traveled with small
children under the best of modern conditions will have the dimmest inkling of what hiking
10 km with a toddler, a 5 year old, and all your worldly possessions would be like. Analysis
of the time budgets of Ache forest hunters show similar constraints (see, for example, Hill
et al. 1985). Thus, such constraints are probably common in mobile groups. Hunter-gathers
also probably tended to nurse children to the age of 4 or so, and lactation tends to suppress
ovulation. Hunter-gathers, at least tropical ones, lack the highly concentrated calories fats
and processed carbohydrates) that young children need to thrive.
Figure 3-2. Since foodstuffs, tools, and young children must be carried, the need of most hunter-gatherers for mobility is reflected in the many ways
in which they utilize their limited toolkit. (Source: R. B. Lee 1979:42-43.)
Hunting and Gathering Societies
Despite rather low birth rates and fairly high infant mortality rates, hunter-gather-
ing seems to be a fairly dependable mode of subsistence. Many low-quality wild foods re-
main to be exploited during droughts and bad winters. It doesn’t seem as if sedentary
agricultural or “rich” food foragers do much, if any, better in this respect in spite of their
ability to store food much more effectively. The greater densities and narrower specializa-
tion of richer peoples increase risks even while storage reduces them. As we will see in
Chapter 26, some scholars have hypothesized that human diets deteriorated after agricul-
ture was developed. The evidence is ambiguous; most ancient peoples seem to have suf-
fered at least seasonal or irregular bouts of poor nutrition during the worst time of year, and/
or in bad years.
Some hunting and gathering societies are exceptions to the generalization about the
simplicity of social and political organization. The Northwest Coast salmon fishing peo-
ples are the best known ethnographically. The Kwakiutl are a famous example. As we have
already noted, these people lived at higher densities because of the huge salmon resources
available in the big rivers of Puget Sound and similar areas. These groups were semi-sed-
entary, and stored dried salmon and other food products. They accumulated non-portable
possessions and lived in plank houses. Their political system was based on powerful
“chiefs,” actually “bigmen,” full time political specialists. Because this system is charac-
teristic of horticultural societies, we will investigate it in more detail in the next chapter.
Bigmen played a ritual role, redistributed surplus goods, conducted warfare, and the like.
Although dependent upon popular support, bigmen had quite a lot of formal authority.
Control of within-group conflict is not easy in the absence of real authority. Single
deviant individuals are ostracized, and being outside the sharing network will be fatal un-
less the culprit reforms. Larger scale disputes between families can often be solved by the
disaffected splitting to form their own band, if the party is large enough, or moving to a re-
lated group if small. Petty disputes are a serious problem; violence is typically not all that
uncommon at the level of feuds between families.
Knauft (1988) has recently emphasized that the murder rate is remarkably high in all
the well-known politically very simple societies due to the use of self-help violence. (This
category includes some tropical horticulturalists, and most contemporary hunter-gatherer
groups, that are characterized by very small settlements, low population density, and
strongly egalitarian social systems.) These groups tend to have strongly peaceful norms,
but a statistically high rate of individual murder. He cites rates of ca. 300 to 800 murders/
100,000 population/yr. in several such groups. This compares to 142/100,000/yr. for Cleve-
land Black Males 1969-74 and 0.5/100,000/yr for Great Britain in 1959, to place such num-
bers in context. This picture contrasts very sharply with the conventional wisdom about
such societies. Anthropologists have apparently been mislead by the peaceful norms of
such groups, and by the fact that even high rates of murder do not result in very many dead
bodies per year per small group. However, in the group Knauft studied, the Gebusi of New
Guinea, these deaths added up. In a very unhealthful environment, homicide accounted for
1/3 of all adult deaths. His interpretation of this pattern is that in politically very simple so-
The scale and duration of warfare would, however, tend to be limited by the lack of
effective political institutions (a war chief with the power to command, or even lead by the
example of his prestige), and the logistical limitations of conducting war in the face of a
daily need to forage. Also, the lack of much stored food or material possessions among
hunters means that the one motivation for predatory raiding, booty, is greatly reduced. It is
notable that richer and more sedentary peoples seemed to have warred more. The North-
west Coast peoples had quite well organized warfare in contact times, and possibly before,
over resources and trade routes, and to acquire slaves.
Small societies without role specialists and writing may be limited in the number of
cultural traits they can keep in a culture. Those things that only a few happen to learn are
likely to be lost by accident. There is some evidence from Tasmania to suggest that the
small, isolated population there lost a number of traits they brought over from Australia due
to this process (Diamond, 1978). If this line of argument is correct, the sheer size of a cul-
ture will have important effects on its sophistication.
V. Ideology
Typical mobile food foragers have relatively simple ritual practices and religious be-
liefs. Formalization is relatively weak. More sedentary groups, like the California people,
seem to have had more elaborate and formal ones. However, the Australians are noted for
their very elaborate ritual and spiritual beliefs, while Bushmen, living in relatively similar
environments, seem more “secular”. The Bushmen do have a series of dance disciplines
that induce mystical experiences (Katz in Lee and DeVore, 1976). A case might be made
on the basis of this contrast that ideological variables are outside the culture core. All for-
agers for the last 35,000 years have had some art objects, often quite elegant ones. Clearly,
sedentarism at least allows these to be multiplied (e.g. Bushmen don’t make totem poles,
but sedentary fishermen of the Northwest Coast did). Score a point for Kroeber’s possi-
bilism here.
Jorgensen (1980) notes that correlation between environment and social organiza-
tion is imperfect for Western North American hunters and gatherers, the gross fit with
Stewardian expectations notwithstanding. Part of the failure to fit is due to the relatively
rapid ongoing evolution in this region. In the Northwest, politically more centralized
groups seemed to have been spreading at the expense of less centralized ones. The ongoing
intensification of production using harder-to-process foods like acorns and grass seed was
ongoing at contact. The spread of intensive plant using strategies into the Great Basin oc-
curred only about 500-700 years ago according to Bettinger and Baumhoff. Maize agricul-
ture was on the move in the Southwest. Thus, a significant element of historical variation
overlays the environment-technology-culture core pattern.
Whether the hunting and gathering way of life is as rosy as some students of this type
of society have sometimes thought is debatable. Just in the last 20 years, opinion has fluc-
tuated between the “nasty brutish, and short” and “civilization was the big mistake” schools
of thought. We used to champion the latter, but Knauft’s paper and other neo-Hobbesian
observations have shaken our faith. It is good to beware of mythologizing! Keep you hy-
potheses multiple and don’t get overly fond of any one!
V. Bibliographic Notes
References:
Bettinger, R.L. and M.A. Baumhoff. 1982. The Numic spread. Great Basin cultures in com-
petition. Amer. Antiquity. 47: 485-503.
Binford, L.R. 2001. Constructing frames of reference : an analytical method for archaeo-
logical theory building using hunter-gatherer and environmental data sets. Berkeley
: University of California Press
Diamond, J. 1978. The Tasmanians: The longest isolation, the simplest technology. Nature
273: 185-6.